international journal of language & communication disorders · sli could reflect neurological...

30
This article was downloaded by:[University College Dublin] [University College Dublin] On: 22 March 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 769440815] Publisher: Informa Healthcare Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t713393930 Verbal deficits in Down's syndrome and specific language impairment: a comparison Glynis Laws a ; Dorothy V. M. Bishop a a Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford. Oxford. UK To link to this article: DOI: 10.1080/13682820410001681207 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13682820410001681207 Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf This article maybe used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material. © Taylor and Francis 2007

Upload: others

Post on 25-Mar-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

This article was downloaded by:[University College Dublin][University College Dublin]

On: 22 March 2007Access Details: [subscription number 769440815]Publisher: Informa HealthcareInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

International Journal of Language &Communication DisordersPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t713393930

Verbal deficits in Down's syndrome and specificlanguage impairment: a comparisonGlynis Laws a; Dorothy V. M. Bishop aa Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford. Oxford. UK

To link to this article: DOI: 10.1080/13682820410001681207URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13682820410001681207

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdfThis article maybe used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction,re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expresslyforbidden.The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will becomplete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should beindependently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings,demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with orarising out of the use of this material.© Taylor and Francis 2007

Page 2: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

Verbal deficits in Down’s syndrome andspecific language impairment: a comparison

Glynis Laws and Dorothy V. M. Bishop

Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK

(Received 12 September 2003; accepted 13 February 2004)

Abstract

Background: Down’s syndrome is a chromosome disorder characterized by arange of physical and psychological conditions, including language impairment.The severity of impairment is variable, and some components of the languagesystem appear to be more affected than others. This description could alsobe applied to typically developing children diagnosed with specific languageimpairment.Aims: To compare findings from the largely separate research literatures onthese conditions, and to address the questions about whether the languagepathology associated with Down’s syndrome could be the same as thatdescribed as specific language impairment in typical development, and whetherthe two conditions could have similar causes.Main Contribution: Research studies suggest similar patterns of languageimpairment in the two populations, and some similarities in underlyingprocessing deficits.Conclusions: Future research should consider whether similarities in the languagebehaviours associated with Down’s syndrome and specific language impairmentcould be related to similarities at other levels of analysis, including neurologicaldevelopment and genetics.

Keywords: Down’s syndrome, specific language impairment, language disorders.

Introduction

Down’s syndrome (DS) is the most common biological cause of developmentaldelay with an incidence rate of around 1:1000 live births (Steele 1996). In 95–98%of cases, DS is caused by a trisomy resulting from non-disjunction of chromosome21 at meiosis. More rarely, it is the result of the translocation of chromosomematerial, or is due to mosaicism where an error has occurred during cell division inembryogenesis so that only a proportion of an individual’s cells is affected by thetrisomy. As well as various medical problems and moderate-to-severe levels oflearning disability, most people with DS face the challenge of language impairment.The present review explores the notion that this impairment mirrors that

International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders

ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online # 2004 Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists

http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals

DOI: 10.1080/13682820410001681207

Address correspondence to: Glynis Laws, Department of Experimental Psychology, University ofOxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3DU, UK; e-mail: [email protected]

INT. J. LANG. COMM. DIS., OCTOBER–DECEMBER 2004,

VOL. 39, NO. 4, 423–451

Page 3: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

encountered in otherwise typically developing individuals diagnosed with specificlanguage impairment (SLI), and considers whether the language disorders couldhave similar causes.

At first glance, the differences between SLI and DS are far more evident thanthe similarities. Children with SLI, by definition, have no general cognitive delayand no obvious signs of neurological impairment. The diagnosis is made when achild has selective difficulties with language development that cannot be accountedfor in terms of low intelligence, hearing impairment, physical disabilities orabnormalities of the language-learning environment. Two-to-three times as manymales than females appear to be affected (Robinson 1991). Although SLI can occurin the absence of speech impairment, a proportion of children have both speechand language difficulties (e.g. Newcomer and Hammill 1988, Bishop et al. 1995,Conti-Ramsden and Botting 1999, Shriberg et al. 1999, Tomblin and Zhang 1999).

SLI has been regarded as evidence for the modularity of the developing cognitivesystem, as it demonstrates that one can have selective and severe impairments in thedomain of language despite apparently adequate non-verbal intellectual development. Agreat deal of research effort has focused on documenting the underlying nature oflanguage deficits in SLI, with researchers divided between those who attribute theproblems to impairment of a specialized language-learning module, and those whopropose impairments of more general perceptual or learning systems as the primarycause (Bishop 1997, Leonard 1998). In contrast, language impairment in DS hasattracted less research attention. It might be speculated that it was assumed that theverbal difficulties are an inevitable consequence of the physical characteristics andcognitive delay associated with this syndrome. However, relatively small contributions tolanguage variation in DS have been attributed to cognitive ability, hearing status, speechdifficulties, home environment or socio-economic factors (Wulpert et al. 1975, Miller1988, Chapman et al. 1991, Rondal 1995). Also, other syndromes that lead to equivalentlevels of non-verbal cognitive delay are not characterized by such severe languagedeficits—Williams syndrome is the clearest example, with several studies drawingexplicit comparisons with DS (e.g. Harris et al. 1997, Mervis and Bertrand 1997, Kleinand Mervis 1999, Mervis and Robinson 2000, also Lenneberg 1967, Tew 1979, Bellugiet al. 1988, Rondal 1995, Rondal and Edwards 1997, Abbeduto et al. 2001b). It isreasonable to conclude that language deficits in DS are not attributable to the learningdisability.

In explaining the reasons for language problems in DS, it is important toconsider the range as well as the average level of language skills. There is substantialunexplained variation in language development in DS, so that the chromosomeabnormality is insufficient in itself to account for language impairment. It isnecessary to look beyond the diagnosis of DS to establish the biological andenvironmental determinants of language difficulties in this population.

The review begins by presenting evidence for parallels in the language deficitsseen in DS and SLI. Many researchers have argued that the study of individualsfollowing atypical pathways of development can inform one’s understanding ofnormal development (e.g. Lenneberg 1967, Cicchetti 1984, Rondal and Edwards1997, Nadel 1999, Abbeduto et al. 2001a). The usual approach is to focus on thedissociation of functions in groups with different genetic syndromes (Tager-Flusberg 1999, Dykens and Hodapp 2001). It is argued that it could be informativeto extend this approach to a comparison of language pathology in otherwise normaldevelopment with what could be the same pathology combined with a known

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop424

Page 4: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

genetic disorder such as DS. Observation of connections between different aspectsof the language system, or between language and other domains, where these aredeveloping under different conditions, could help clarify the nature of functionalrelationships. It could also provide more compelling evidence for causalconnections between language impairment and underlying capacities or processesthan can be provided by observations under just one condition.

A further motivation for comparing the nature of language impairment in thesetwo populations is the question of whether phenotypic similarities between DS andSLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between thesedisorders. Over the past decade there has been mounting evidence for a substantialgenetic contribution to SLI (for a review, see Bishop 2001a), but little is knownabout which genes are implicated and how they influence brain development. It isplausible that different genetic mechanisms in DS and SLI lead to similarneurological consequences, and hence affect language learning. A more radicalpossibility is that trisomy in DS affects the expression of genes that normallyinfluence rate of language acquisition, and thus by studying genetic variationassociated with language ability in DS one might gain understanding of geneticinfluence on SLI. This is an important question for the future, but before it can beaddressed, it is one that first requires careful comparison of the languagephenotypes associated with these conditions.

Language in DS and SLI: a comparison

Before embarking on a comparative review of these disorders, it is necessary tograpple with the issue of heterogeneity in SLI. Unlike DS, which can be defined interms of objective physical criteria, SLI is diagnosed purely in terms of behaviouralcharacteristics. Furthermore, this is largely a diagnosis by exclusion: many of thedefining characteristics state what is not wrong with the child, rather thandescribing positive diagnostic features. It is perhaps not surprising to find thatchildren who are included under the umbrella of SLI can be quite diverse inpresentation. Before one can ask whether the language characteristics of SLI aresimilar to those in DS, it needs to be stated what kind of SLI is of concern. This iscomplicated by the fact that although most experts agree that SLI is heterogeneous,there is little consensus about how it should be subclassified. In general, in arguingfor similarities between SLI and DS, focus will be on what might be regarded as‘typical’ SLI, in which the most obvious difficulties experienced by the child arewith structural aspects of language, i.e. phonology and syntax (Conti-Ramsdenand Botting 1999). While accepting that boundaries between subtypes of SLIare difficult to draw, and most research samples contain a mixture of children,conclusions are not expected to apply to those children who have disproportionatedifficulties with social use of language (so-called pragmatic language impairment;Bishop 2000).

Table 1 lists four characteristics of typical SLI. The following sections describeeach of these and consider how far each applies also to DS.

Language delay in relation to non-verbal ability

Poor language skill in relation to non-verbal ability is a defining feature of SLI.Because children with DS function at an overall level comparable with that of

Down’s syndrome and SLI 425

Page 5: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

a much younger child, it was for many years not appreciated that they usuallyhad disproportionate problems with language. However, several studies clearlydemonstrate that when abilities are considered in terms of developmental level (i.e.the age at which a normally developing child would achieve that skill), spokenlanguage lags behind non-verbal ability (e.g. Chapman et al. 1991, 1998). Expressivelanguage is commonly assessed by mean length of utterance (MLU), a measurebased on the total number of words, or number of words plus additionalmorphemes (e.g. verb inflections), produced in a conversational speech sample ornarrative sample (Brown 1973). Expressive language deficits are indicated in DSand in SLI by lower MLUs than would be expected based on chronological age(CA) or mental age (MA). The MLUs of individuals with DS are also significantlylower than those of MA-matched controls with other intellectual impairments(Rosin et al. 1988).

Implicit in the term ‘language delay’ is the notion that language acquisition is ona normal developmental trajectory, but proceeding at an unusually slow rate. If onetakes ‘delay’ literally, it might be expected that the same endpoint of competencewould be reached, but at a later age. Although some language experts have cited DSas exemplifying delayed rather than deviant language development (e.g. Lenneberg1967), systematic studies suggest that neither SLI nor DS can be so simplyconceptualized. Delayed onset of language is a consistent feature of SLI (Leonard1998), but outcomes for children are variable. In some cases, language does seemto be simply delayed, and problems resolve over time (Bishop and Edmundson1987a, Bishop and Adams 1990, Thal et al. 1991, Whitehurst et al. 1991b, Paul1993), particularly those predominantly affecting speech (Griffiths 1969, Hall andTomblin 1978, Bishop and Adams 1990). However, there are poorer outcomes forchildren with more severe problems, especially when these include comprehensiondifficulties (Bishop and Adams 1990), or where there is a family history of languageimpairment (Whitehurst et al. 1991a). These difficulties can continue into teenageyears (Aram et al. 1984, Johnson et al. 1999) and early adulthood (Tomblin et al.1992). Even when overt language problems have resolved, residual evidence forthem can persist as phonological memory deficits (Bishop et al. 1996a), poorphonological awareness (Goulandris et al. 2000) or reading disability (Stothard et al.1998, Bishop 2001b).

There is also delayed onset of language for many children with DS (Fowler1990), although some children start to talk at the time expected for typical children(Chapman 1995, Berglund et al. 2001). Some research shows that rates of progressare uneven and often interrupted in middle childhood by developmental plateaux(Fowler 1988). These might apply more to grammar than to vocabulary acquisition(e.g. Laws and Gunn 2004). However, although language status can remain fixedfor some time, other evidence points to further progress in language developmentin late adolescence or early adulthood (Chapman et al. 2002, Fowler 1995). There is

Table 1. General descriptions applicable to SLI

Language acquisition is delayed in relation to non-verbal abilityLanguage impairment cannot be accounted for by known medical or environmental causesExpressive language is more severely affected than receptive languageGrammatical components of the language system tend to be most vulnerable to delays and difficulties

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop426

Page 6: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

wide variation in individual language progress and the levels of language skilleventually reached. Some exceptional individuals with DS achieve almost normallanguage (Rondal 1995), but Fowler (1990) reports that the majority achieve levelsof language production that would be expected of children under three years. Thereis a somewhat brighter picture for language comprehension, which is less severelyaffected than expressive language (see below). However, although receptivevocabulary is often commensurate with, or in excess of non-verbal abilities,grammar understanding is below the level that would be expected based on non-verbal ability.

Another implication of ‘delay’ is that the profile of language skills is equivalentto that of a younger, normally developing child. In other words, the onset oflanguage may be delayed but thereafter follows the same developmental pathway.Although many linguistic features of the language produced by children with SLIare consistent with MLU (Leonard 1998), some aspects of language differ incomparison to MLU-matched controls. For example, a more limited range of verbsmay be used (Watkins et al. 1993), and there may be significant differences in thecorrect use of certain grammatical morphemes (Rice and Wexler 1996). Theselinguistic features illustrate an essential difference between children with SLI andyounger children with the same MLU, suggesting that the former are not simplyfollowing a slower version of the normal developmental pathway. A similar pictureemerges from research on DS, with limited use of verbs (Hesketh and Chapman2000), and evidence for specific morphological deficits (e.g. Chapman et al. 1998,Eadie et al. 2002, Laws and Bishop 2003). The acquisition of morphologicalfeatures in SLI and DS will be described further below.

Lack of association with medical or environmental causes

When diagnosing SLI, one excludes children for whom there are medical orenvironmental factors that could account for the language impairment. In contrast,there is usually a host of risk factors that could plausibly affect the languagedevelopment of children with DS, including hearing loss (both conductive andsensorineural), and structural and neurological impairments of the speechapparatus. In addition, some people with DS are raised in relatively unstimulatinginstitutional environments; perhaps, more so in the past. Given these factors, itcould be concluded that it is hardly surprising that language is poor in DS.However, this conclusion does appear misguided, given research that shows thatmedical and environmental risk factors cannot account for all the variance inlanguage skills in DS.

Hearing loss is perhaps the most obvious potential cause of languageimpairment in DS. Between 40 and 85% or more of individuals have impairedhearing (Dahle and McCollister 1986, Davies 1996, Roizen 1997), with severeimpairments in 10–15% (Marcell and Cohen 1992, Marcell 1995, Davies 1996). Inyounger children, the problem is generally attributed to conductive hearing lossassociated with glue ear and frequent middle ear infections (e.g. Cunningham andMcArthur 1981). However, using auditory brainstem responses, Roizen et al. (1993)recorded nearly as many children with sensorineural losses as with conductivelosses, and a similar number presented a combination of these problems. Otherreports suggest that sensorineural losses appear in late adolescence (e.g. Widen et al.1987). These losses may become worse in middle age (Evenhuis et al. 1992).

Down’s syndrome and SLI 427

Page 7: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

Evidence for the effect of hearing loss on language development in DS isinconsistent. Some research finds no significant correlation between these functions(e.g. Miller 1988, Marcell 1995, Jarrold and Baddeley 1997). Other researchreports that only a small percentage of the variability in language performancecan be attributed to hearing loss. For example, Chapman has reported hearingloss to account for between 4 and 7% of the variance in adolescents’ grammarcomprehension scores (Chapman et al. 1991, 1998). However, individuals withmore than mild hearing loss had been excluded from this sample. Laws (2004)found that although there was no significant correlation between average hearingthreshold and MLU in a group of adolescents with DS, hearing did discriminatebetween participants who produced an intelligible narrative from which MLUcould be derived and those excluded from the analysis because narrativeswere unintelligible. In the same sample, hearing threshold was also related tovariation in receptive and expressive vocabulary scores, but not to grammarcomprehension (Laws and Gunn 2004). Hearing loss clearly contributes tolanguage outcomes in DS but, since individuals with good hearing neverthelesshave language impairment, it is fair to say that language deficits in DS cannotbe attributed to poor hearing.

Poor language-learning environment is an exclusionary factor in the diagnosisof SLI, but it is not so easy to rule out in the case of DS. Historically, indirectevidence for environmental effects on language in DS comes from comparison ofearly studies of institutionalized children compared with later studies focusing onchildren raised at home. Not unexpectedly, developmental outcomes for earliercohorts were less favourable than for later cohorts raised in families (e.g. Shotwelland Shipe 1964). More recently, improved health care and education mean thatone’s expectations of the developmental potential of people with DS are nowhigher than in the past (Wishart 1998). Although it is not yet clear what effect thismay have on language development, a selective approach to the literature has beenadopted in this review, concentrating on studies of more recent cohorts (forreviews of language development in DS, see Gibson 1978, Gunn 1985, Miller 1987,1988, 1999, Mervis 1988, Dodd and Leahy 1989, Fowler 1990, 1995, Chapman1995, 1997, Chapman et al. 1991, Rondal 1995, Gunn and Crombie 1996, Stoel-Gammon 1997, Tager-Flusberg 1999).

Expressive versus receptive language

Although it is traditional to make a distinction between expressive and expressive–receptive forms of SLI (World Health Organisation 1992, American PsychiatricAssociation 1994), Bishop (1979) argued that most children with SLI can be shownto have some level of receptive impairment when appropriate tests are used.Nevertheless, it is usually the case that expressive problems are more marked thanreceptive difficulties. This is clearly shown when studies report composite measuresbased on standard scores (e.g. Montgomery 2000, McArthur and Hogben 2001).Early expressive language ability is also an important predictor of later languageoutcomes in children with SLI (Bishop and Edmondson 1987a, Davison andHowlin 1997, Botting et al. 2001).

The picture of disproportionately poor expressive language is mirrored in DS(Gibson 1978, Cunningham et al. 1985, Miller 1988, 1999, Mundy et al. 1988,

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop428

Page 8: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

Beeghley et al. 1990, Chapman et al. 1991, 1998, Dykens et al. 1994, Fowler et al.1994, Casselli et al. 1998, Laws and Bishop 2003). Since DS is recognized frombirth, the divergence of expressive language from comprehension can be chartedfrom an early age. Miller (1999) has studied the longitudinal development oflanguage comprehension and production skills relative to other cognitive abilitiesfor young children with DS using parent reports (MacArthur CommunicativeDevelopment Inventories; Fenson et al. 1993). These studies show that languageproduction fails to keep pace with increases in MA, but language comprehension iscloser to the level expected for MA. After 37 months MA (about half CA), thelanguage production skills of 92–100% of the children lagged general cognitiveability by at least 6 months.

The gap between production and comprehension continues to grow throughoutchildhood and adolescence (Chapman et al. 1991, 1998), and expressive languagecontinues out of line with cognitive impairment. MLU is significantly lower thanmental age-matched controls with other intellectual impairments (Rosin et al. 1988).As for children with SLI, there are also differences between individuals with DSand younger typically developing controls with the same MLU. However, unlikechildren with SLI, these comparisons are not all unfavourable since individuals withDS often have larger vocabularies than younger MLU-matched controls (e.g. Harris1983). Laws and Bishop (2003) directly compared the language profiles of a groupof adolescents with DS with those for MA-matched groups of younger childrenwith SLI and typically developing children. The groups with DS and SLI had verysimilar language profiles. In both language-impaired groups, expressive languagewas more severely affected than language comprehension relative to the typicallydeveloping group.

Grammatical difficulties

In both populations, asymmetries in the growth of different aspects of language aremanifested as specific weakness in grammar combined with relative strength invocabulary (SLI: Aram et al. 1984, Gopnik 1990, Tomblin and Zhang 1999, DS:Fowler 1990, 1995, Chapman et al. 1991, Chapman 1995, Rondal 1995, Gunn andCrombie 1996, Tager-Flusberg 1999, Laws and Bishop 2003). Tomblin and Zhang(1999) screened over 7000 US kindergarten children, classifying those scoring morethan 1.25 SD below children of the same CA on at least two composite languagescores as language impaired. These children had non-verbal IQs above 87 and noadditional developmental problems. They were compared with control childrenwho passed the language screening, and to another group that also had lowlanguage scores but were in addition more generally delayed. Tomblin and Zhang(1999) found a composite grammar score, based on tests of expression andcomprehension, was most likely to be failed, and that very few children hadunaffected grammar alongside low vocabulary and narrative scores. Similar patternsof performance in children with low IQ were obtained.

DS research indicates a similar pattern of specific strengths and weaknessesrelative to general cognitive ability (Fowler 1990, 1995, Chapman 1995, Rondal1995, Gunn and Crombie 1996), and relative to the performance of individualswith other syndromes (e.g. Harris et al. 1997, Tager-Flusberg 1999, Abbeduto et al.2001b). Deficits in morphological development are indicated by significantly less

Down’s syndrome and SLI 429

Page 9: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

use of free grammatical morphemes than MLU matched controls, and less reliableuse of bound morphemes (Chapman et al. 1998). The most severely disadvantagedaspect of language for people with DS seems to be the production of complexsyntactic structures (Chapman et al. 1998), although recent research suggests thatthe content of sentences may be in line with MLU (Thordardottir et al. 2002). Theacquisition and use of grammar is poorer than would be predicted from non-verbalabilities (Fowler 1990, Chapman 1995, Abbeduto et al. 2001b), but receptivevocabulary may be better than cognitive abilities would predict (e.g. Chapman1995), and can exceed that of non-disabled children of comparable MA or MLU(Barrett and Diniz 1989). Although morphosyntactic development may reach aceiling, continued vocabulary growth results in a gradual divergence of vocabularyfrom grammar knowledge with CA (Chapman et al. 1993, Fowler 1995, Laws andGunn 2004). Fowler (1995) reported that her entire sample of 31 adolescents hadhigher levels of vocabulary than grammar understanding. Although few studiescomment on individual profiles, group means usually indicate significant differencesbetween levels of receptive vocabulary and grammar comprehension (Miller 1987,1988, Rosin et al. 1988, Fowler 1990, Chapman et al. 1991, Chapman 1995, Laws1998, Laws and Bishop 2003, Laws and Gunn 2004).

In Laws and Bishop’s (2003) direct comparison of language profiles in DSand SLI, there was marked dissociation between grammar understanding andreceptive vocabulary in both populations, relative to controls. Whereas grammarwas poor, receptive vocabulary scores did not differ significantly from those of thetypically developing comparison group. This was the pattern of results forexpressive language scores as well as language comprehension scores. However,there is some question about whether grammar comprehension is as severelyaffected as its expression in other studies of SLI or DS. For example, although thecomposite grammar score reported in Tomblin and Zhang’s (1999) study of SLIindicated an overall weakness in grammar, separate test scores suggested thatgrammar comprehension as well as vocabulary comprehension was a relativestrength for the language delayed children, and that any weakness in grammar wasassociated with expressed language. However, this may not be the case for allchildren with SLI. Other studies show that children can have difficulties inunderstanding grammatical constructions as well as in making correct use of themin speech (Bishop 1979, 1982, Adams 1990, Van der Lely and Stollwerck 1996).These differences between study findings could be related to different means ofascertainment or to different assessment instruments. The criteria employed byTomblin and Zhang (1999) would have identified some children with simplelanguage delay whose problems would resolve and who would not go on todevelop SLI. Children with longer lasting problems of SLI do have grammarcomprehension difficulties more often.

In DS, most studies report levels of grammar comprehension below those ofnon-verbal ability (Bridges and Smith 1984, Rosin et al. 1988, Buckley 1993, 1995,Laws 1998, Laws et al. 2000, Vicari et al. 2000, Laws and Gunn 2004). Chapman et al.(1991) reported grammar comprehension in line with non-verbal ability, but thiswas probably because the non-verbal measure incorporated a test of short-termmemory, itself a core deficit associated with the syndrome. Chapman (1995)suggests that it is only in adolescence that syntax comprehension begins to fallbehind that of MA controls. However, characteristics of study samples other than

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop430

Page 10: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

CA may be important. Since there is a higher ceiling on non-verbal ability than ongrammar comprehension, findings may depend on the proportion of a sampleachieving high non-verbal scores. The higher the reported mean non-verbal scorefor a research sample, the less likely it is that the age equivalent score for grammarcomprehension will equal it. It is true that this situation will more often apply toolder study groups, but other factors such as school placement might also influencefindings (e.g. Laws et al. 2000).

Morphological development

Rice and Wexler (1996) have argued that clinical observation of the use ofgrammatical morphemes provides a more sensitive test of SLI than a considerationof language profiles. Late acquisition of grammatical morphemes is characteristicof SLI (Johnston and Schery 1976, Khan and James 1983, Johnston and Khami1984, Leonard et al. 1988, Bliss 1989, Marchman et al. 1999). Children with SLIdo not find all grammatical morphemes difficult to acquire (Rice et al. 1998), butthere is regularity in the morphemes that children do find difficult, and these tendto be the ones that young normally developing children are slow to use consistently.A period of inconsistent usage of correct grammatical forms in normaldevelopment is referred to as an ‘optional infinitive’ stage and, because childrenwith SLI are so slow to move out of this stage, Rice and colleagues refer to an‘extended optional infinitive’ stage. For example, there is little variability in the useof verb tense markers by normal 5-year-olds, but children with SLI struggle toacquire correct use of the past tense and third person singular ‘s’, and this difficultycan be persistent (Rice and Wexler 1996, Rice et al. 1998, Van der Lely and Ullman2001, Conti-Ramsden and Windfuhr 2002). The problem does not appear to be dueto poor perception of speech sounds for these morphemes. Some reports find thatthird person singular ‘s’ is affected, but children have no apparent difficulty inacquiring plural ‘s’, which should be no more easily perceived (Oetting and Rice1993).

The sensitivity and specificity of tense marking as a test to identify childrenwith language impairments has been demonstrated in studies to compare theperformance of 5-year-old children with SLI to CA-matched controls. Whenchildren who used correct tense markers less than 80% of the time were classifiedas disordered, 97% of cases of SLI were correctly identified, and 98% of controls(Rice and Wexler 1996). Poor performance by children with SLI persisted up to 8years of age, and although there was some improvement, there was no evidencethat they caught up controls (Rice et al. 1998). Further, children with SLI are poorat identifying, as well as using, correct tense markers (Rice et al. 1999b). Rice (2000)suggests that these properties of the tests for grammatical markers, together withthe poor relationships found between them and environmental factors such asmother’s education (Rice et al. 1999a), make them strong candidates for identifyingcases of SLI for genetic study. However, note that when past tense and noun pluraltasks have been used with children with SLI under 5-years-old, they have notperformed as satisfactory clinical risk markers (Conti-Ramsden and Hesketh 2003).

The acquisition and use of morphemes has been studied in DS. As in SLI, thereis some evidence that particular morphemes may be more difficult to acquire thanothers (Rondal and Lambert 1983, Sabsay and Kernan 1993, Rutter and Buckley1994, Chapman et al. 1998, Eadie et al. 2002, Laws and Bishop 2003). Rutter and

Down’s syndrome and SLI 431

Page 11: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

Buckley (1994) investigated the acquisition of fourteen morphemes by twelveyoung children with DS, by asking parents to record monthly diaries over two tothree years. After taking into account the delayed production of first words by thechildren with DS, the children’s acquisition of most of these morphemes waswithin range of the three typically developing children studied by Brown (1973),although they were not acquired in the same order. However, three rules werenever grasped by any of the children: the uncontractable copula ‘be’ form (e.g. ‘heis’ in response to ‘who’s there?’), the uncontractable auxiliary ‘be’ form (‘he is’ inresponse to ‘who’s coming to the party?’), and third person singular ‘s’ for presenttense verbs. Irregular third person present tense (e.g. ‘Mummy has’ or ‘Mummydoes’) was acquired by just one child. These rules are also reported as difficult forchildren with SLI.

Chapman et al. (1998) reported that regular past tense ‘ed’ as well as plural ‘s’,noun possessive ‘s’, third person singular ‘s’, contractible auxiliaries and copulas,and present progressive ‘ing’ were frequently omitted from the language producedby their sample of 5–20-year-olds with DS. However, many of these individualshad MLUs below 4.5, the stage at which these bound morphemes are expectedto appear in normal development. Some adolescents with DS with MLU above4.5 may make more normal use of third person singular and past tense verbs(Laws and Bishop 2003). Other research involving adults with DS shows persistentuse of telegraphic speech, with poor use of verb tense inflections, and few articles,prepositions, pronouns, copulas, or conjunctions (Rondal and Lambert 1983).Sabsay and Kernan (1993) found that, unlike adults with intellectual disability ofunknown origin, adults with DS fail to master the use of auxiliary verbs, suggestingthat the difficulties are a feature of DS rather than of intellectual impairmentmore generally. Eadie et al. (2002) directly compared the acquisition of morphemesby MLU-matched groups of children with SLI, DS and typically developingchildren. Although both language-impaired groups were limited in their acquisi-tion of specific morphemes relative to the comparison group, they did notresemble each other in the patterns of morpheme use. Laws and Bishop (2003)noted that differences in the morphological difficulties of each groupwere possibly related to the stronger vocabulary skills of the DS group in thestudy.

Table 1 described some general features of SLI. The above sections expandedon these descriptions for SLI and investigated the extent to which they apply alsoto DS. It is argued that these comparisons show that the language characteristics ofthese conditions are very similar in terms of surface descriptions. Of course, similarsurface descriptions do not necessarily mean that the impairments arise fromthe same underlying causes. It is possible that common profiles of languagedevelopment occur because the affected components are simply those mostvulnerable to any sort of risk factor, and that DS is just an additional, if powerful,risk factor. Such an account would not necessarily mean that people with DSshould also resemble those with SLI in terms of deficits to underlying capacities orprocesses. An alternative account could be developed around the idea that peoplewith DS have language profiles similar to SLI because they are prone to the sameunderlying difficulties hypothesized to cause SLI, whether these are biological orenvironmental in origin. The next step in this enquiry is to consider whether thesimilarities observed are superficial or whether they could be explained by similardeficiencies in underlying capacities or processes.

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop432

Page 12: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

Implications for understanding of processes involved inlanguage acquisition

Consideration of underlying causes bears on the more fundamental question ofwhich theoretical account can best explain language impairment. An emphasis onpoor grammatical development in SLI, and the background to the tests ofmorphology described above, arises from a linguistic deficit theory. Given theimportance of difficulties in acquiring specific grammatical morphemes in thisaccount of SLI, it could be important to establish that the same morphemes posedifficulties for people with DS if the pathology is to be shown to mirror SLI.A more general difficulty with the acquisition of morphology could indicate adifferent type of disorder, or perhaps one that arises from one or more differentunderlying problems, such as hearing impairment. Despite the evidence formorphological deficits described above, more direct comparisons of the linguisticfeatures of DS and SLI may be necessary to be confident about a correspondencebetween them at this level.

The linguistic-deficit account contrasts with more psychologically basedaccounts which view language impairment as secondary to underlying processingdeficits. Table 2 lists hypotheses that have been advanced to explain SLI in terms ofunderlying processes, including auditory processing deficits, poor phonologicalmemory skills, motor development delays and atypical cerebral specialization. Givenheterogeneity in the manifestation of SLI it seems unlikely that a single explanationwill suffice, and it is also likely that deficits in underlying processes differentiallyaffect the various components of language. The differential effects of multiplecauses on multiple components of a language system could explain the heterogeneityevident in the language profiles of people with SLI (Bishop 2001a).

Modular approaches to understanding language treat subdomains of languageas separate components, depending on separate neurobiological substrates. Thisapproach distinguishes between computational and conceptual aspects of language(e.g. Chomsky 1980, Tager-Flusberg 1997). The computational component includesthe hierarchical processes associated with phonological and syntactic development,and is distinguished from a conceptual component on which depend lexical andsemantic aspects of language. These components may be differentially affected bydeficits in underlying capacities, and be more or less vulnerable to genetic andenvironmental risks. Chomsky (1986) suggested that biological constraints are moreimportant in limiting syntactic development, but that environment may be moreinfluential in determining other aspects of language such as the lexicon. Forexample, increases in vocabulary depend on exposure to experiences provided bythe environment. Given intact biological functions to support vocabulary learning,variation in the numbers of words acquired is likely to depend on experience. Incontrast, if biological deficits impose the major limitation on syntactic development

Table 2. Underlying processes associated with SLI

Deficits in auditory processing abilityPoor phonological short-term memoryDeficits in motor developmentAtypical patterns of cerebral specialization

Down’s syndrome and SLI 433

Page 13: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

then poor syntactic development could be the outcome whatever the environmentprovides.

In normal development, language acquisition proceeds in an integrated fashion,and in line with non-verbal cognitive abilities, resulting in flat developmentalprofiles. When something goes wrong, the dislocation of functions can result in thegrowth of uneven profiles. Uneven language profiles are a feature of many geneticsyndromes and disorders, and contrasting profiles based on dissociations offunctions have been established between conditions (e.g. Tager-Flusberg et al.1990). As argued herein, the language profiles in DS and in typical SLI showmarked impairment of computational processes alongside relatively intactconceptual aspects of language. If there should be similar patterns of strengthand weakness in terms of underlying capacities or processes, and these can beassociated with the same aspects of language development in both conditions, thiswould provide further evidence for a common language impairment.

Auditory processing deficits

Clearly, any disruption of normal processing of language sounds could be expectedto affect language development. One major difference between SLI and DS is thatwhereas hearing problems are excluded as a cause of SLI, hearing losses arecommon in children with DS (see above). However, defective peripheral hearing isnot the only way in which auditory processing can be disrupted and major effortshave been devoted to identifying defects in higher level auditory processes toaccount for SLI (e.g. Tallal and Piercy 1973a, Tallal 1976, Tallal et al. 1981, Nevilleet al. 1993, Wright et al. 1997, McArthur and Hogben 2001). Whether difficulties liein the perception of sounds by the ear or in the processing of speech sounds by thebrain, the effects on language outcomes could be similar. Despite different historiesin terms of hearing loss, it is reasonable to compare the two populations in termsof possible relationships between auditory processing and language development.

An auditory temporal processing deficit in SLI is said to result in impaireddiscrimination of rapid or briefly presented non-speech sounds, even when there isno problem differentiating the same stimuli presented more slowly or for longerduration (e.g. Tallal and Piercy 1973a, b). It is argued that this deficit makes itdifficult for children to discriminate between consonants that differ by just a briefpart of the acoustic signal, and leads to problems in acquiring the phonologyessential to the development of spoken language (Tallal 2000). Despite extensiveexperimental evidence to support this hypothesis, there are many contrary reportsand conflicting evidence arising from methodological problems associated with theexperiments and from individual differences within samples of people with SLI(McArthur and Bishop 2001).

While it is possible to speculate that similar problems could account for slowacquisition of phonology in DS, attempts to study higher level auditory perceptionin this population are problematic due to difficulties in explaining the tasks toparticipants, and the fact that the tasks can impose a memory load beyond thecapacity of many people with DS. Further, since hearing losses are common in DS,it could be difficult to differentiate high level processing effects from peripheraldeficits. However, Marcell and Cohen (1992) and Marcell (1995) used a procedureinvolving backward masking to assess the identification of words by people withDS. Presentation of stimulus words was followed by a burst of white noise

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop434

Page 14: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

presented after intervals of varying duration. The task is analogous to one used todetect deficits in auditory temporal processing in SLI where backward masking isused to manipulate the time available for the detection of a non-verbal stimulustone (e.g. Wright et al. 1997). Children with SLI have higher thresholds fordetecting a tone than control children, i.e. to make a correct response, they requireto hear more of the tone before the masking noise appears. Marcell (1995) foundthat adolescents with DS identified fewer words than adolescents from a matchedgroup with other intellectual impairments when the words were followed by amasking noise after 40 ms. They did not differ from the comparison group whenthe masking noise appeared after 320 ms, giving them longer to identify the word.However, much of the difficulty with the 40 ms condition could be attributed tohearing losses. Individuals with bilateral hearing losses identified significantlyfewer words than a subgroup of adolescents with DS with normal hearing in one orboth ears. Poor hearing was not a factor in the identification of words in the320 ms condition. Marcell and Cohen (1992) suggest that the poor performance ofthe hearing impaired group in the 40 ms condition might be attributed to thecombined effects of low auditory acuity and slow processing speed. Clearly,further experiments would be required to establish whether people with DS havethe same kind of auditory temporal processing deficit that is thought to be a featureof SLI.

Phonological memory deficits

A more established similarity between DS and SLI is the occurrence of poorphonological memory, or memory for speech sounds. Phonological memory isthought to be functionally important for the normal development of language(Gathercole and Baddeley 1993, Baddeley et al. 1998), and language development isslower in children with poor phonological memory (e.g. Gathercole et al. 1992).This relationship has been studied extensively by Gathercole and her colleaguesusing non-word repetition, a task which assesses the accuracy with which unfamiliarbut word-like phoneme sequences such as ballop or blonterstaping are repeated. Thetask may reflect the role of phonological memory in mediating the development oflong-term representations as the lexicon grows. Phonological memory may also beimportant for the acquisition of grammar by allowing multiword utterances to bemaintained while the long term representations of abstract syntactic rules areconstructed (Speidel 1989, 1993, Baddeley et al. 1998). This process allows childrento acquire templates of syntactic constructions used in adult speech that can bedrawn on to support their own spoken output.

Children with SLI find the non-word repetition task difficult, especially whenasked to repeat long non-words (Kamhi and Catts 1986, Gathercole and Baddeley1990, Bishop et al. 1995, 1996a, Montgomery 1995, 2000, Dollaghan and Campbell1998, Edwards and Lahey 1998, Ellis Weismer et al. 2000, Conti-Ramsden andHesketh 2003). Bishop et al. (1996a) administered the task to twins, at least one ofwhom had experienced speech or language disorder in the past. Non-wordrepetition was impaired even for individuals whose language problems hadresolved. This could indicate that these individuals had been able to compensate insome way for poor phonological memory, and so achieve normal language skills,but that the underlying phonological memory problem remained. In thisinterpretation of results, poor phonological memory is the cause of poor language,

Down’s syndrome and SLI 435

Page 15: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

rather than a consequence of language problems. Conti-Ramsden and Hesketh(2003) found that poor non-word repetition was an accurate risk marker for SLI inyoung children, but cautioned that more research was necessary before any causalrelationship between poor phonological memory and language impairment could beestablished.

Phonological memory has also been described as a selective deficit in DS(Varnhagen et al. 1987, Hulme and Mackenzie 1992, Kay-Raining Bird andChapman 1994, Wang and Bellugi 1994, Fowler et al. 1995, Vicari et al. 1995, Jarroldand Baddeley 1997), and there is some evidence of a relationship between thisdeficit and language. Vallar and Papagno (1993) described a case of a young Italianwoman with DS with good vocabulary who, unusually for a person with thiscondition, also recorded a digit span within the normal range. Although this mightindicate that her good language was a consequence of spared short term memory,other studies report only a weak correlation between phonological memory andreceptive vocabulary, compared with that found in normal development (Hulmeand Mackenzie 1992), and some studies report no relationship (Marcell 1995,Jarrold and Baddeley 1997, Chapman et al. 2002). However, these researchersassessed phonological memory using digit span. When non-word repetition hasbeen used, significant correlations have been established between phonologicalmemory and language development (Laws 1998, 2004, Laws and Gunn 2004). In a5-year follow up study of 10–24-year-olds with DS, earlier phonological memoryscores predicted later receptive vocabulary and grammar comprehension (Laws andGunn 2004). The expressive language abilities of this sample were also assessed atthe end of this study. Fifty per cent of the variation in MLU and sentence recallwas attributed to non-word repetition scores, even after allowance was made forthe effects of CA, MA and word repetition (Laws 2004). Some researchers haveargued that this relationship is due to the common demands placed on outputprocesses by non-word repetition and expressive language tasks (e.g. Snowling et al.1991, Van der Lely and Howard 1993). If this was the case, the correlation betweenverbal memory and expressive language should be abolished if a memory task isused which has no spoken language requirement (Adams and Gathercole 2000).However, just as Adams and Gathercole reported for typically developing children,Laws found that a verbal memory span task which required a pointing responsewas as strongly related to MLU and sentence recall as non-word repetition.

Interestingly, phonological memory has been linked not only to the languagephenotypes of SLI and DS but also to a genetic background in SLI. Bishop et al.’s(1995) analysis of twin data described earlier showed non-word repetition to be auseful phenotypic marker for inherited language impairment; MZ (identical) co-twins resembled each other more closely than DZ (non-identical) co-twins. A morerecent twin study replicated this finding of high heritability for non-word repetition(Bishop et al. 1999). In this study, Tallal’s repetition test was also administered(Tallal and Piercy 1973a), so that memory for speech sounds could be comparedwith memory for non-verbal tones. The results showed that although phonologicalmemory was under strong genetic influence there was no genetic influence on non-verbal auditory deficits. Instead, deficits in memory for tones were more subject toenvironmental influence. Since the children most affected by SLI in this study werethose who showed evidence of both deficits, Bishop et al. (1999) concluded that thedeficits act as independent risk factors and are most likely to cause SLI when theyoccur together. This could explain the high levels of language impairment in the

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop436

Page 16: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

DS population where probably a large majority of individuals have co-occurringauditory processing deficits and phonological memory deficits.

Association with deficits in motor development

Another factor worth exploring as a potential link between language impairment inSLI and DS is motor delay. Poor motor skills are often a problem for children withSLI (Hill 2001), and several studies have identified relationships between motordelay and language (e.g. Johnston et al. 1981, Bishop and Edmundson 1987b,Moore and Law 1990, Schwartz and Regan 1996, Owen and McKinlay 1997,Bishop 2002). Although some relationship between fine motor skill and speechmight be expected, on the basis that both depend on systems for motorprogramming and control, the interesting thing about some of these studies is thatmotor skills have been related to language comprehension. For example, Schwartzand Regan (1996) found that fine motor tasks requiring rapid timing and repetitivesequencing were strongly related to language comprehension measures in 4–8-year-olds with SLI. Some evidence that this association can be linked to heritable factorswas provided by Bishop (2002) who studied motor skill in the sample of twinsdescribed earlier (Bishop et al. 1995, 1996a). Motor skill, assessed using a simpletapping task, was significantly related to speech impairment but children with purelanguage impairments and unaffected speech also had motor deficits. There wasevidence for strong genetic effects on speech and language impairments and onmotor skill, and evidence that motor skill and language impairment (but not speechproblems) shared a common heritable factor.

Unfortunately, although delayed motor development is an invariable feature ofDS, there is little research to investigate relationships between motor skill andlanguage development. Studies of children with DS show that motor developmentfollows a normal sequence but remains below the level expected for MA (e.g. Frithand Frith 1974, Connolly et al. 1993, Reid and Block 1996, Jobling 1998, Torres andBuceta 1998). There is some evidence to link motor skill with vocal features. As intypical development, there is synchrony in the onset of canonical babbling, that isinfants’ repetition of well-formed syllables such as ba, da and ma, and rhythmic handbanging. A similar association in seen in infants with DS (Cobo-Lewis et al. 1996).Cobo-Lewis et al. suggest that these rhythmic behaviours may be underpinned bycommon neuromuscular systems. In adulthood, there is poor motor performancein those who stutter compared with those with fluent speech (Devenny et al. 1990),and greater speech dysfluency in left- or mixed-handed adults (Devenny andSilverman 1990). However, there is limited information about whether individualvariation in the degree of motor difficulties accounts for variation in languagecompetence throughout childhood. Given evidence for a common genetic basisto motor immaturity and language impairment in SLI (Bishop 2002), furtherexploration of the part played by motor deficits in the language development ofindividuals with DS would be worthwhile.

Association with atypical patterns of cerebral specialization

In most people, it is usual for the left cerebral hemisphere to be specialized forprocessing language. This arrangement is reflected in a right ear advantage for theperception of speech sounds in dichotic listening tasks. In this paradigm, different

Down’s syndrome and SLI 437

Page 17: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

sounds or words are presented to each ear simultaneously, but the listener generallyreports only the stimulus received by the advantaged ear (e.g. Kimura 1967).Lateralization of motor function is also indicated indirectly, by the preference forusing the right hand demonstrated by about 90% of the population. It has beenargued that if the process of lateralization is incomplete, or if functions areorganized differently, the cost could be developmental disabilities such as SLI ordyslexia (e.g. Geschwind and Galaburda 1987, Gauger et al. 1997).

Dichotic listening performance by children with SLI varies across studies, butsome research finds qualitative differences in responses. Children with SLI maymore often show a left ear advantage or weak right ear preference compared withtypical children (e.g. Cohen et al. 1999). Although most children with SLI are righthanded (e.g. Bishop 1990), quantitative assessment suggests this is only a weakpreference (Bishop et al. 1996b, Hill and Bishop 1998, Bishop 2001c). This featurecould reflect atypical morphological asymmetries (e.g. Jernigan et al. 1991, Planteet al. 1991) or could reflect immature neuromotor development rather than a radicaldifference in the brains of these children (Hill and Bishop 1998).

In DS, extensive research has been directed to understanding whether atypicalorganization of motor and language functions limits language development (Chuaet al. 1996). These researchers have developed a model of cerebral organization thatfeatures the separation of speech and motor functions into different cerebralhemispheres. In this model, the centre for motor control is in the left hemisphere,as evidenced by a normal pattern of finger tapping performance favouring the righthand (Elliott 1985, Elliott et al. 1986). However, unlike the general population,children and adults with DS tend to display a left ear advantage in dichotic listeningstudies, indicating right hemisphere processing of language (Elliott et al. 1994).Separation of motor and language functions could reduce the effectiveness ofcommunication between these systems. There is some evidence for this in thatperformance on tasks which require either oral or limb movements is poorer inresponse to verbal instruction than to visual demonstration (Elliott and Weeks1993). Elliott and Weeks found that performance was poorest for participants whohad shown a left ear advantage in dichotic listening tasks.

Summary and implications

The review has addressed the question of whether the language impairment in DScould be the same impairment as that found in otherwise typically developingchildren diagnosed with SLI. The question has been approached in terms of (1) acomparison of language profiles in the two groups and (2) a discussion ofunderlying deficits that could be implicated in the language impairments.Conclusions are summarized in table 3.

In terms of the phenotypes of language disorder, there is considerable evidencefor similarity between the language profiles of individuals with DS and those ofthe majority group of children with SLI. Despite very different backgrounds tolanguage development in terms of general cognitive ability, the main features ofimpairment in both populations are more severe expressive language deficitsrelative to levels of language comprehension, dissociation between grammatical andlexical components of the language system, and difficulties in the acquisition ofmorphology. Research studies which have investigated the effects that variousunderlying deficits might have on language impairment paint similar pictures in

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop438

Page 18: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

Table 3. Summary of conclusions

SLI Down’s syndrome

Language in relation togeneral cognitive ability

Poor language relative to cognitive ability is a diagnostic criterion Although vocabulary can be equal to or in advance ofcognitive ability, phonology and grammar are usuallypoorer. Also, language is poor relative to that of peoplewith other learning disabilities

Association with otherproblems

Hearing problems, neurological impairment and psychiatric disorderare ruled out as a cause of language problems

Multiple potential causes, but these do not explain thesubstantial variation in language skill. Further researchis needed to understand the effects of mild-to-moderatehearing loss

Expressive versusreceptive language

Expressive problems w receptive problems. Early expressive abilitiespredict later language outcomes

Expressive problems w receptive problems

Co-morbid speechimpairment

Proportion of children have speech difficulties Speech impairment is often a feature. It is not clear howthis affects language development

Grammatical difficulties Grammar is the most frequently affected component. Difficulties inunderstanding and in producing complex sentences

Possible ceiling on grammatical development. Difficultiesin understanding and in producing complex sentences

Morphology Late acquisition of grammatical morphemes. Research on whetherverb tense marking is a specific deficit gives inconsistent results

Poor use of grammatical morphemes, but may be morenormal for individuals with longer mean lengths ofutterances. Patterns of morpheme use differ fromspecific language impairment, but only two comparativestudies have been reported so far

Vocabulary Proportion of children have word-learning and word-finding difficulties.For others, vocabulary is in line with cognitive abilities

Often, but not always, a strength. Evidence for continuedvocabulary growth into young adulthood

Higher level auditoryprocessing

Some evidence for deficits in processing brief or rapid sounds, andalso conflicting evidence and contrary reports. Further research isnecessary

Little available research

Phonological memorydeficits

Evidence for difficulties in non-word repetition. Poor scores predictlanguage impairment. Less evidence for direct correlation withlanguage measures

Dissociation between phonological memory and visuospatialmemory. Non-word repetition is variable, and variation isrelated to language abilities

Motor development Motor delays related to language impairment Evidence for motor delay. Little research to investigate anassociation with language development

Dow

n’ssyndrom

eand

SLI

439

Page 19: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

both populations. This is particularly so with respect to phonological memorydeficits. In terms of the effects of higher level auditory processing deficits, theimpact of motor delay, and the part that atypical cerebral specialization might playin language development, there is less parallel research available from which todraw conclusions about common causes and more direct comparisons would beworthwhile in future research.

The review has been restricted to behavioural data but the implications ofsimilar language phenotypes are that they could be the expression of similargenotypes, and perhaps reflect similar atypical brain development. Althoughresearchers are beginning to uncover some links between genetics and languageimpairment, the chain of events between genes (and the chemical and regulatoryprocesses governed by them) and a phenotype of language impairment remainsmysterious. However, somewhere between genes and expressed phenotypes therelie more proximal explanations for language impairment in the form of atypicalpatterns of brain development or defective underlying capacities or processes whichrestrict language development. If language impairments in DS and SLI are of thesame nature then one might find the same cascades of causes and effects linkinggenes to language phenotypes.

In DS, there is the possibility that the severity of disorders associated with thecondition, including language impairment, could be influenced by variation inspecific genetic factors on chromosome 21. For example, if there is an allele of agene on chromosome 21 that increases the risk of a particular disease, people withDS who have an extra copy of this allele may show unusually high rates of thedisease, whereas those with a different allele do not show such vulnerability. Thiskind of pattern has been observed for heart disease, where genetic variability in agene on chromosome 21 is associated with the congenital heart defects observed inmany individuals with DS (e.g. Davies et al. 1995). A second possibility is thattrisomy 21 produces a genetic background against which risk factors from genes onother chromosomes are more strongly expressed. This pattern has been observed inthe development of Alzheimer’s disease in DS, where the expression of a geneassociated with the disease on chromosome 21 is modified by allelic variation ingenes encoded on chromosome 19 (Corder et al. 1993, Roses and Saunders 1994,Del-Bo et al. 1997, Schupf et al. 1998).

A similar mechanism might be implicated in SLI. Suppose, for instance, thatthere are several alleles that act as quantitative trait loci for language impairment,i.e. they raise the probability that language will be impaired but do not have asimple one-to-one relation with SLI. The likelihood of a child having SLI will be afunction of the number of risk quantitative trait loci present. It is very likely thatenvironmental factors will also play a part, so that SLI is only seen when a high-riskgenotype is exposed to a non-optimal environment (Bishop 2001a). However, thesame alleles may have a much lower threshold for expression when they occur inthe context of trisomy 21 because of the general disruption to developmentalprocesses that the chromosome abnormality entails.

At present, the suggestion of shared genetic factors as background to languageimpairment in the two populations is speculative but is worth considering in futureresearch. Similarly, although consideration of the research on the complexneurobiology of these conditions is beyond the scope of this review, it is worthnoting two features that they have in common. First, there is some evidence of

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop440

Page 20: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

minor abnormalities of neuronal migration in DS (Coyle et al. 1986, Wisniewski1990, Golden and Hyman 1994) and in SLI (Galaburda et al. 1985, Clark and Plante1998). Second, there appears to be disproportionate reduction in the size of thecerebellum and frontal lobes in DS (Jernigan et al. 1993, Raz et al. 1995, Murphyet al. 2000, Capone 2001, Pinter et al. 2001). Eckert et al. (2003) have noted similarabnormalities in the brains of dyslexic individuals, and there is suggestive evidencethat the same circuits may be implicated in SLI (Jernigan et al. 1991). Althoughthere are also many points of difference between DS and SLI at this level, futureresearch should identify whether shared atypical features of brain developmentcontribute to language difficulties.

Clinical implications

The main implication for practitioners of a similarity between language profiles inDS and SLI is that it weakens any notion that language impairment in DS shouldbe considered as an inevitable consequence of the learning disability. If languageimpairment is relatively independent of non-verbal ability then, logically, therapeuticgoals should not be determined by the child’s non-verbal intelligence level. In bothDS and SLI, understanding the problems posed by deficits in the processesunderlying language development could provide targets for intervention. If theseconditions do share the same language pathology, it might make sense to transfersuccessful interventions from one condition to the other.

To date, interventions at the level of underlying deficits have focused ondifferent targets in each population. In SLI, intervention has targeted auditoryprocessing deficits, based on the theory (described above) that language deficits canbe attributed to difficulties with processing rapid, successive sounds. The aim ofintervention is to provide intensive training exercises to improve the discriminationand representation of sounds. The most widely offered training programme is FastForWord (Scientific Learning Corporation 1996), and researchers claim remarkablelanguage gains for children with SLI following the training (e.g. Merzenich et al.1996, Tallal et al. 1996). However, there remains the need for considerable researcheffort to determine which children can benefit from auditory training, whichaspects of language can be improved and what the precise mechanism is for anyimprovement (Gillam et al. 2001). If it can be established that higher level auditoryprocessing deficits contribute to language impairment in DS, there is the hope thatauditory training might also benefit these children. However, until one understandsmore about the processes involved, it would be premature to offer such hope.

The main focus of intervention at the level of underlying deficits in DS hasbeen on training short-term memory (e.g. Hulme and Mackenzie 1992, Broadleyand MacDonald 1993, Comblain 1994, Laws et al. 1996, Conners et al. 2001). Therationale for memory training is based on Baddeley’s working memory model(Baddeley 1986, Baddeley and Hitch 1974), which includes a phonological loopcomponent specialized for the short term memory of speech based material. Arange of clinical and experimental evidence points to the dissociation of processeswithin the phonological loop so that deficits can be associated either with thestorage of phonological material or with the maintenance of inputs by articulatoryrehearsal. Individuals with DS may not develop spontaneous rehearsal strategies(Hulme and Mackenzie 1992) and so have difficulty in maintaining inputs (e.g. new

Down’s syndrome and SLI 441

Page 21: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

vocabulary items), with adverse consequences for language development. The aimof memory training studies has been to teach a cumulative rehearsal strategy.Despite the short term success of programmes in increasing memory span (e.g.Broadley and MacDonald 1993), skills have not been maintained over the longerterm (Laws et al. 1995) and the impact of increasing memory span on languagedevelopment is unknown. Despite evidence for phonological memory problems inSLI, there appear to be no comparable studies. However, Montgomery (2002), in arecent review of the evidence for the role of phonological memory in the languagedifficulties of children with SLI, has suggested teaching cumulative rehearsalalongside a range of games aimed at increasing metaphonological awareness.Comparing the effects of training interventions based on auditory processing ormemory training in both populations could further understanding of the complexrelationships between these deficits and general language skills, as well as potentiallybenefiting affected children.

Acknowledgement

Research was funded by a grant from The Wellcome Trust to D. V. M. B.

References

ABBEDUTO, L., EVANS, J. and DOLAN, T., 2001a, Theoretical perspectives on language and communicationproblems in mental retardation and developmental disabilities. Mental Retardation andDevelopmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 1, 45–55.

ABBEDUTO, L., PAVETTO, M., KESIN, E., WEISSMAN, M. D., KARADOTTIR, S. and O’BRIEN, A., et al. 2001b, Thelinguistic and cognitive profile of Down syndrome: evidence from a comparison with fragile Xsyndrome. Down Syndrome: Research and Practice, 7, 9–15.

ADAMS, A.-M. and GATHERCOLE, S. E., 2000, Limitations in working memory: implications for languagedevelopment. International Journal of Language and Communication, 35, 95–116.

ADAMS, C., 1990, Syntactic comprehension in children with expressive language impairment. BritishJournal of Disorders of Communication Impairment, 25, 149–171.

AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION, 1994, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [DSM-IV], 4thedn (Washington, DC: APA).

ARAM, D., EKELMAN, B. and NATION, J., 1984, Preschoolers with language disorders: 10 years later. Journalof Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 27, 232–244.

BADDELEY, A. D., 1986, Working Memory (Oxford: Oxford University Press).BADDELEY, A. D. and HITCH, G., 1974, Working memory. In G. Bower (ed.), The Psychology of Learning

and Motivation, vol. 8 (New York: Academic Press), pp. 47–90.BADDELEY, A. D., GATHERCOLE, S. E. and PAPAGNO, C., 1998, The phonological loop as a language

learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.BARRETT, M. D. and DINIZ, F. A., 1989, Lexical development in mentally handicapped children. In

M. Beveridge, G. Conti-Ramsden and I. Leudar (eds), Language and Communication in MentallyHandicapped People (London: Chapman & Hall), pp. 3–32.

BEEGHLEY, M., WEISS-PERRY, B. and CICCHETTI, D., 1990, Beyond sensorimotor functioning: earlycommunicative and play development of children with Down syndrome. In D. Cicchetti andM. Beeghly (eds), Children with Down Syndrome: A Developmental Perspective (Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press), pp. 329–368.

BELLUGI, U., MARKS, S., BIHRLE, A. and SABO, H., 1988, Dissociation between language and cognitivefunctions in Williams syndrome. In D. V. M. Bishop and K. Mogford (eds), LanguageDevelopment in Exceptional Circumstances (London: Churchill Livingstone), pp. 117–189.

BERGLUND, E., ERIKSSON, M. and JOHANSSON, I., 2001, Parental reports of spoken languageskills in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 44,179–191.

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop442

Page 22: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

BISHOP, D. V. M., 1979, Comprehension in developmental language disorders. Developmental Medicine and

Child Neurology, 21, 225–238.BISHOP, D. V. M., 1982, Comprehension of spoken, written and signed sentences in childhood

language disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 23, 1–20.BISHOP, D. V. M., 1990, Handedness, clumsiness and developmental language disorders.

Neuropsychologia, 28, 681–690.BISHOP, D. V. M., 1997, Uncommon Understanding (Hove: Psychology Press).BISHOP, D. V. M., 2000, Pragmatic language impairment: a correlate of SLI, a distinct subgroup, or part

of the autistic continuum? In D. V. M. Bishop and L. B. Leonard (eds), Speech and Language

Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention and Outcome (Hove: Psychology Press),pp. 99–113.

BISHOP, D. V. M., 2001a, Genetic and environmental risks for specific language impairment in children.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 356, 369–380.

BISHOP, D. V. M., 2001b, Genetic influences on language impairment and literacy problems in children:same or different? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 189–198.

BISHOP, D. V. M., 2001c, Individual differences in handedness and specific speech and languageimpairment: evidence against a genetic link. Behavioural Genetics, 31, 339–351.

BISHOP, D. V. M., 2002, Motor immaturity and specific language impairment: evidence for a commongenetic basis. American Journal of Medical Genetics: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 114, 56–63.

BISHOP, D. V. M. and ADAMS, C., 1990, A prospective study of the relationship between specificlanguage impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology

and Psychiatry, 31, 1027–1050.BISHOP, D. V. M. and EDMUNDSON, A., 1987a, Language impaired 4-year-olds: distinguishing transient

from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 156–173.BISHOP, D. V. M. and EDMUNDSON, A., 1987b, Specific language impairment as a maturational lag:

evidence from longitudinal data on language and motor development. Developmental Medicine and

Child Neurology, 29, 442–459.BISHOP, D. V. M., BISHOP, S. J., BRIGHT, P., JAMES, C., DELANEY, T. and TALLAL, P., 1999, Different origin of

auditory and phonological processing problems in children with language impairment: evidencefrom a twin study. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 155–168.

BISHOP, D. V. M., NORTH, T. and DONLAN, C., 1995, Genetic basis of specific language impairment:evidence from a twin study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 37, 56–71.

BISHOP, D. V. M., NORTH, T. and DONLAN, C., 1996a, Nonword repetition as a phenotypic marker forinherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychiatry and

Psychology, 37, 391–403.BISHOP, D. V. M., ROSS, V. A., DANIELS, M. S. and BRIGHT, P., 1996b, The measurement of hand

preference: a validation study comparing three groups. British Journal of Psychology, 87,269–285.

BLISS, L., 1989, Selected syntactic usage by language-impaired children. Journal of Communication

Disorders, 22, 277–289.BOTTING, N., FARAGHAR, B., SIMKIN, Z., KNOX, E. and CONTI-RAMSDEN, G., 2001, Predicting pathways of

specific language impairment: what differentiates good and poor outcome? Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 1012–1020.BRIDGES, A. and SMITH, J. V., 1984, Syntactic comprehension in Down’s syndrome children. British

Journal of Psychology, 75, 197–196.BROADLEY, I. and MACDONALD, J., 1993, Teaching short-term memory skills to children with Down’s

syndrome. Down’s Syndrome: Research and Practice, 1, 56–62.BROWN, R., 1973, First Language (London: Allen & Unwin).BUCKLEY, S., 1993, Developing the speech and language skills of teenagers with Down’s syndrome.

Down’s Syndrome: Research and Practice, 1, 63–71.BUCKLEY, S., 1995, Improving the expressive language skills of teenagers with Down’s syndrome.

Down’s Syndrome: Research and Practice, 3, 110–115.CAPONE, G. T., 2001, Down syndrome: advances in molecular biology and the neurosciences. Journal of

Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 22, 40–59.CASSELLI, M. C., VICARI, S., LONGOBARDI, E., LAMI, L., PIZZOLI, C. and STELLA, G., 1998, Gestures and

words in early development of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language and

Hearing Research, 41, 1125–1135.CHAPMAN, R. S., 1995, Language development in children and adolescents with Down syndrome.

Down’s syndrome and SLI 443

Page 23: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

In P. Fletcher and B. Macwhinney (eds), Handbook of Child Language (Oxford: Blackwell),pp. 641–663.

CHAPMAN, R. S., 1997, Language development in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Mental

Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 3, 307–312.CHAPMAN, R. S. and HESKETH, L. J., 2000, Behavioural phenotype of individuals with Down syndrome.

Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 6, 84–95.CHAPMAN, R. S., HESKETH, L. J. and KISTLER, D. J., 2002, Predicting longitudinal change in language

production and comprehension in individuals with Down syndrome: hierarchical linearmodelling. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45, 902–915.

CHAPMAN, R. S., SCHWARTZ, S. E. and KAY-RAINING BIRD, E., 1991, Language skills of children andadolescents with Down syndrome: I. Comprehension. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34,1106–1120.

CHAPMAN, R. S., SEUNG, H. K., SCHWARTZ, S. E. and KAY-RAINING, BIRD, E., 1998, Language skills ofchildren and adults with Down syndrome: production deficits. Journal of Speech, Language and

Hearing Research, 41, 861–873.CHOMSKY, N., 1980, Rules and Representations (New York: Columbia University Press).CHOMSKY, N., 1986, Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin and Use (New York: Praeger).CHUA, R., WEEKS, D. J. and ELLIOTT, D., 1996, A functional systems approach to understanding verbal–motor

integration in individuals with Down syndrome. Down’s Syndrome: Research and Practice, 4, 25–36.CICCHETTI, D., 1984, The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 1–7.CLARK, M. M. and PLANTE, E., 1998, Morphology of the inferior frontal gyrus in developmentally

language-disordered adults. Brain and Language, 61, 288–303.COBO-LEWIS, A. B., OLLER, D. K., LYNCH, M. P. and LEVINE, S. L., 1996, Relations of motor and vocal

milestones in typically developing infants and infants with Down syndrome. American Journal on

Mental Retardation, 100, 456–467.COHEN, M. J., RICCIO, C. A. and HYND, G. W., 1999, Children with specific language impairment:

quantitative and qualitative analysis of dichotic listening performance. Developmental

Neuropsychology, 16, 243–252.COMBLAIN, A., 1994, Working memory in Down’s syndrome: training the rehearsal strategy. Down’s

syndrome: Research and Practice, 2, 123–126.CONNERS, F. A., ROSENQUIST, C. J. and TAYLOR, L. A., 2001, Memory training for children with Down

syndrome. Down’s Syndrome: Research and Practice, 7, 25–33.CONNOLLY, B. H., MORGAN, S. B., RUSSELL, F. F. and FULLITON, W. L., 1993, A longitudinal study of

children with Down syndrome who experienced early intervention programming. Physical

Therapy, 73, 170–181.CONTI-RAMSDEN, G. and HESKETH, A., 2003, Risk markers for SLI: a study of young language-learning

children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 251–263.CONTI-RAMSDEN, G. and BOTTING, N., 1999, Characteristics of children attending language units in

England: a national study of 7-year-olds. International Journal of Language and Communication

Disorders, 34, 359–366.CONTI-RAMSDEN, G. and WINDFUHR, K., 2004, Productivity with work order and morphology: a

comparative look at children with SLI and children with normal language abilities. International

Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 17–30.CORDER E. H., SAUNDERS, A. M., STRITTMATTER, W. J., SHMECHEL, D. E., GASKELL, P. C. and SMALL, G. W., et al.

1993, Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onsetfamilies. Science, 261, 921–923.

COYLE, J. T., OSTER-GRANITE, M. L. and GEARHART, J. D., 1986, The neurobiologic consequences ofDown syndrome. Brain Research Bulletin, 16, 773–787.

CUNNINGHAM, C. and MCARTHUR, 1981, Hearing loss and treatment in young Down’s syndrome children.Child: Care, Health and Development, 7, 357–374.

CUNNINGHAM, C., GLENN, S., WILKINSON, P. and SLOPER, P., 1985, Mental ability, symbolic play, andexpressive language of young children with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 26, 255–265.DAHLE, A. J. and MCCOLLISTER, F. P., 1986, Hearing and otologic disorders in children with Down

syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 636–642.DAVIES, B., 1996, Auditory disorders. In B. Stratford and P. Gunn (eds), New Approaches to Down

Syndrome (London: Cassell), pp. 100–121.DAVIES, G. E., HOWARD, C. M., FARRER, M. J., COLEMAN, M. M., CULLEN, L. M. and WILLIAMSON, R., et al.

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop444

Page 24: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

1995, Genetic variation in the COL6A1 region is associated with congenital heart defects intrisomy 21, Down’s syndrome. Annals of Human Genetics, 59, 253–269.

DAVISON, F. M. and HOWLIN, P., 1997, A follow-up study of children attending a primary-age languageunit. European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 32, 19–36.

DEL-BO, R., COMI, G. P., BRESOLIN, N., CASTELLI, E., CONTI, E. and DEGIULI, A., et al. 1997, Theapolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele causes a faster decline of cognitive performances in Downsyndrome. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 145, 87–91.

DEVENNY, D. A. and SILVERMAN, W. P., 1990, Speech dysfluency and manual specialisation in Down’ssyndrome. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 34, 253–260.

DEVENNY, D. A., SILVERMAN, W., BALGLEY, H., WALL, M. J. and SIDTIS, J. J., 1990, Specific motor abilitiesassociated with speech fluency in Down’s syndrome. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 34,437–443.

DODD, B. and LEAHY, J., 1989, Phonological disorders and mental handicap. In M. Beveridge, G. Conti-Ramsden and Y. Leudar (eds), Language and Communication in Mentally Handicapped People

(London: Chapman & Hall), pp. 33–56.DOLLAGHAN, C. and CAMPBELL, T., 1998, Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of

Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41, 1136–1146.DYKENS, E. M. and HODAPP, R. M., 2001, Research in mental retardation: toward an etiologic approach.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 49–71.DYKENS, E. M., HODAPP, R. M. and EVANS, D. W., 1994, Profiles and development of adaptive

behavior in children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 98,580–587.

EADIE, P. A., FEY, M. E., DOUGLAS, J. M. and PARSONS, C. L., 2002, Profiles of grammatical morphologyand sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome.Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45, 720–732.

ECKERT, M. A., LEONARD, C. M., RICHARDS, T. L., AYLWARD, E. H., THOMSON, J. and BERNINGER, V., 2003,Anatomical correlates of dyslexia: frontal and cerebellar findings. Brain, 126, 482–494.

EDWARDS, J. and LAHEY, M., 1998, Nonword repetitions of children with specific language impairment.Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 279–309.

ELLIOTT, D., 1985, Manual asymmetries in the performance of sequential movement by adolescentsand adults with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 90–97.

ELLIOTT, D. and WEEKS, D. J., 1993, Cerebral specialization for speech perception and movementorganisation in adults with Down’s syndrome. Cortex, 29, 103–113.

ELLIOTT, D., WEEKS, D. J. and CHUA, R., 1994, Anomalous cerebral lateralisation and Down syndrome.Brain and Cognition, 26, 191–195.

ELLIOTT, D., WEEKS, D. J. and JONES, R., 1986, Lateral asymmetries in finger-tapping by adolescents andyoung adults with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 472–475.

ELLIS WEISMER, S. R., TOMBLIN, J. B., ZHANG, X., BUCKWALTER, P., CHYNOWETH, J. G. and JONES, M., 2000,Nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without languageimpairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 865–878.

EVENHUIS, H., VAN ZANTEN, G., BROCAAR, M. and ROERDINKHOLDER, W., 1992, Hearing loss in middle-agepersons with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 47–56.

FENSON, L., DALE, P. S., REZNICK, J. S., THAL, D., BATES, E. and HARTUNG, J. P., et al. 1993, The

MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual (San Diego:Singular).

FOWLER, A., GELMAN, R. and GLEITMAN, L. R., 1994, The course of language learning in children withDown syndrome. In H. Tager-Flusberg (ed.), Constraints on Language Acquisition (Hillsdale:Lawrence Erlbaum), pp. 91–140.

FOWLER, A. E., 1988, Determinants of rate of language growth in children with Down syndrome.In L. Nadel (ed.), The Psychobiology of Down Syndrome (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press),pp. 217–245.

FOWLER, A. E., 1990, Language abilities in children with Down syndrome: evidence for a specificsyntactic delay. In D. Cicchetti and M. Beeghly (eds), Children with Down Syndrome: A

Developmental Perspective (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), pp. 302–328.FOWLER, A. E., 1995, Linguistic variability in persons with Down syndrome. In L. Nadel and

D. Rosenthal (eds), Down Syndrome: Living and Learning in the Community (New York: Wiley-Liss),pp. 121–131.

FOWLER, A. E., DOHERTY, B. J. and BOYNTON, L., 1995, The basis of reading skill in young adults with

Down’s syndrome and SLI 445

Page 25: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

Down syndrome. In L. Nadel and D. Rosenthal (eds), Down Syndrome: Living and Learning in the

Community (New York: Wiley-Liss), pp. 182–196.FRITH, U. and FRITH, C., 1974, Specific motor disabilities in Down syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology

and Psychiatry, 15, 293–301.GALABURDA, A. M., SHERMAN, G. F., ROSEN, G. D., ABOITIZ, F. and GESCHWIND, N., 1985, Developmental

dyslexia: four consecutive cases with cortical anomalies. Annals of Neurology, 18, 222–233.GATHERCOLE, S. E. and BADDELEY, A. D., 1990, Phonological memory deficits in language disordered

children: is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360.GATHERCOLE, S. E. and BADDELEY, A. D., 1993, Working Memory and Language (Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum).GATHERCOLE, S. E., WILLIS, C., EMSLIE, H. and BADDELEY, A., 1992, Phonological memory and vocabulary

development during the early school years: a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28,887–898.

GAUGER, L. M., LOMBARDINO, L. J. and LEONARD, C. M., 1997, Brain morphology in children with specificlanguage impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1272–1284.

GESCHWIND, N. and GALABURDA, A., 1987, Cerebral Lateralization: Biological Mechanisms, Associations and

Pathology (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).GIBSON, D., 1978, Down’s Syndrome: The Psychology of Mongolism (London: Cambridge University Press).GILLAM, R. B., FROME LOEB, D. and FRIEL-PATTI, S., 2001, Looking back: a summary of five exploratory

studies of Fast ForWord. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 10, 269–273.GOLDEN, J. A. and HYMAN, B. T., 1994, Development of the neocortex is anomalous in trisomy 21.

Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 53, 513–520.GOPNIK, M., 1990, Feature blindness: a case study. Language Acquisition, 1, 139–164.GOULANDRIS, N. K., SNOWLING, M. J. and WALKER, I., 2000, Is dyslexia a form of specific language

impairment? A comparison of dyslexic and language impaired children as adolescents. Annals of

Dyslexia, 50, 103–120.GRIFFITHS, C. P. S., 1969, A follow up of children with disorders of speech. British Journal of Disorders of

Communication, 4, 46–56.GUNN, P., 1985, Speech and language. In D. Lane and B. Stratford (eds), Current Approaches to Down’s

Syndrome (London: Cassell), pp. 260–281.GUNN, P. and CROMBIE, M., 1996, Language and speech. In B. Stratford and P. Gunn (eds), New

Approaches to Down Syndrome (London: Cassell), pp. 249–267.HALL, K. H. and TOMBLIN, J. B., 1978, A follow up study of children with articulation and language

disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 43, 227–241.HARRIS, J., 1983, What does mean length of utterance mean? Evidence from a comparative study of

normal and Down’s syndrome children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 18, 153–169.HARRIS, N. G. S., BELLUGI, U., BATES, E., JONES, W. and ROSSEN, M., 1997, Contrasting profiles of

language development in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Developmental

Neuropsychology, 13, 345–370.HESKETH, L. J. and CHAPMAN, R. S., 2000, Verb use by individuals with Down syndrome. American

Journal on Mental Retardation, 103, 288–304.HILL, E. L., 2001, Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: a review of the literature with

regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication

Disorders, 36, 149–171.HILL, E. L. and BISHOP, D. V. M., 1998, A reaching test reveals weak hand preference in specific

language impairment and developmental co-ordination disorder. Laterality, 3, 295–310.HULME, C. and MACKENZIE, S., 1992, Working Memory and Severe Learning Difficulties (Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum).JARROLD, C. and BADDELEY, A. D., 1997, Short-term memory for verbal and visuo-spatial information in

Down’s syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 101–122.JERNIGAN, T. L., BELLUGI, U., SOWELL, E., DOHERTY, S. and HESSELINK, J. R., 1993, Cerebral morphologic

distinctions between Williams and Down syndromes. Archives of Neurology, 50, 186–191.JERNIGAN, T., HESSELINK, J. R., SOWELL, E. and TALLAL, P., 1991, Cerebral structure on magnetic resonance

imaging in language- and learning-impaired children. Archives of Neurology, 48, 539–545.JOBLING, A., 1998, Motor development in school-aged children with Down syndrome: a longitudinal

perspective. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 45, 283–293.JOHNSON, C. J., BEITCHMAN, J. H., YOUNG, A., ESCOBAR, M., ATKINSON, L. and WILSON, B., et al. 1999,

Fourteen-year follow-up of children with and without speech/language impairments:speech/language stability and outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42,744–760.

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop446

Page 26: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

JOHNSTON, J. and KHAMI, A., 1984, Syntactic and semantic aspects of the utterances of language-impaired children: the same can be less. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 66–85.

JOHNSTON, J. and SCHERY, T. K., 1976, The use of grammatical morphemes by children withcommunication disorders. In D. M. Morehead and A. E. Morehead (eds), Normal and Deficient

Child Language (Baltimore: University Park Press), pp. 239–258.JOHNSTON, R. B., STARK, R. E., MELLITS, E. D. and TALLAL, P., 1981, Neurological status of language-

impaired and normal children. Annals of Neurology, 10, 159–163.KAMHI, A. G. and CATTS, H. W., 1986, Toward an understanding of developmental language and

reading disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 51, 337–347.KAY-RAINING BIRD, E. and CHAPMAN, R. S., 1994, Sequential recall in individuals with Down syndrome.

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 1369–1380.KHAN, L. and JAMES, S., 1983, Grammatical morpheme development in three language disordered

children. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 6, 85–100.KIMURA, D., 1967, Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Cortex, 3, 163–178.KLEIN, B. P. and MERVIS, C. B., 1999, Contrasting patterns of cognitive abilities of 9- and 10-year-olds

with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 177–196.LAWS, G., 1998, The use of nonword repetition as a test of phonological memory in children with

Down syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 1119–1130.LAWS, G., 2004, Contributions of phonological memory, hearing and language comprehension to the

expressive language of adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry (in press).LAWS, G. and BISHOP, D. V. M., 2003, A comparison of language in adolescents with Down syndrome

and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research,46, 1324–1339.

LAWS, G. and GUNN, D., 2004, Phonological memory as a predictor of language comprehension in Downsyndrome: a five year follow up study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 326–337.

LAWS, G., BYRNE, A. and BUCKLEY, S., 2000, Language and memory development in children with Downsyndrome at mainstream schools and special schools: a comparison. Educational Psychology, 20,447–457.

LAWS, G., MACDONALD, J. and BUCKLEY, S., 1996, The effects of a short training in the use of a rehearsalstrategy on memory for words and pictures in children with Down syndrome. Down’s Syndrome:

Research and Practice, 4, 70–78.LAWS, G., MACDONALD, J., BUCKLEY, S. and BROADLEY, I., 1995, Long term maintenance of trained memory

skills in children with Down’s syndrome. Down’s Syndrome: Research and Practice, 3, 103–109.LENNEBERG, E. H., 1967, Biological Foundations of Language (New York: Wiley).LEONARD, L. B., 1998, Children with Specific Language Impairment (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).LEONARD, L. B., SABBADINI, L., VOLTERRA, V. and LEONARD, J. S., 1988, Some influences on the grammar of

English- and Italian-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycho-

linguistics, 9, 39–57.MARCELL, M. M., 1995, Relationships between hearing and auditory cognition in Down’s syndrome

youth. Down’s Syndrome: Research and Practice, 3, 75–91.MARCELL, M. M. and COHEN, S., 1992, Hearing abilities of Down syndrome and other intellectually

impaired adolescents. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 13, 533–551.MARCHMAN, V., WULFECK, B. and ELLIS WEISMER, S., 1999, Morphological productivity in children with

normal language and SLI: a study of the English past tense. Journal of Speech, Language and

Hearing Research, 42, 206–219.MCARTHUR, G. M. and BISHOP, D. V. M., 2001, Auditory perceptual processing in people with reading

and oral language impairments: current issues and recommendations. Dyslexia, 7, 150–170.MCARTHUR, G. M. and HOGBEN, J. H., 2001, Auditory backward recognition masking in children with a

specific language impairment and children with specific reading disability. Journal of the Acoustical

Society of America, 109, 1092–1100.MERVIS, C., 1988, Early lexical development: theory and application. In L. Nadel (ed.), The Psychobiology

of Down Syndrome: Issues in the Biology of Language and Cognition (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press),pp. 101–143.

MERVIS, C. B. and BERTRAND, J., 1997, Developmental relations between cognition andlanguage: evidence from Williams syndrome. In L. B. Adamson and M. A. Romski (eds),Communication and Language Acquisition: Discoveries from Atypical Development (Baltimore: PaulBrookes), pp. 75–106.

Down’s syndrome and SLI 447

Page 27: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

MERVIS, C. B. and ROBINSON, B. F., 2000, Expressive vocabulary ability of toddlers with Williamssyndrome or Down syndrome: a comparison. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17, 111–126.

MERZENICH, M. M., JENKINS, W. M., JOHNSTON, P., SCHREINER, C., MILLER, S. and TALLAL, P., 1996, Temporalprocessing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training. Science, 271,77–81.

MILLER, J. F., 1987, Language and communication characteristics of children with Down syndrome. InS. Pueschel, C. Tingey, J. Rynders, A. Crocker and D. Crutcher (eds), New Perspectives on Down

Syndrome (Baltimore: Paul Brookes), pp. 233–262.MILLER, J. F., 1988, The developmental asynchrony of language development in children with Down

syndrome. In L. Nadel (ed.), The Psychobiology of Down Syndrome (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press),pp. 167–198.

MILLER, J. F., 1999, Profiles of language development in children with Down syndrome. In J. F. Miller,M. Leahy and L. A. Leavitt (eds), Improving the Communication of People with Down Syndrome

(Baltimore: Paul Brookes), pp. 11–39.MONTGOMERY, J. W., 1995, Examination of phonological working memory in specifically language

impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 355–378.MONTGOMERY, J. W., 2000, Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension in children with

specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 293–308.MONTGOMERY, J. W., 2002, Understanding the language difficulties of children with specific language

impairments: does verbal working memory matter? American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology,11, 77–91.

MOORE, V. and LAW, J., 1990, Copying ability of preschool children with delayed language development.Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 32, 249–257.

MUNDY, P., SIGMAN, M., KASARI, C. and YIRMIYA, N., 1988, Nonverbal communication skills in Downsyndrome children. Child Development, 59, 235–249.

MURPHY, E. J., SCHAPIRO, M. B., RAPOPORT, S. I. and SHETTY, H. U., 2000, Phospholipid composition andlevels are altered in Down syndrome brain. Brain Research, 867, 9–18.

NADEL, L., 1999, Down syndrome in cognitive neuroscience perspective. In H. Tager-Flusberg (ed.),Neurodevelopmental Disorders (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), pp. 197–221.

NEVILLE, H. J., COFFEY, S. A., HOLCOMB, P. J. and TALLAL, P., 1993, The neurobiology of sensoryand language processing in language-impaired children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5,235–253.

NEWCOMER, P. and HAMMILL, D., 1988, Test of Language Development: Primary—2 (Austin: ProEd).OETTING, J. and RICE, M., 1993, Plural acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal

of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 62–74.OWEN, S. E. and MCKINLAY, I. A., 1997, Motor difficulties in children with developmental disorders of

speech and language. Child: Care, Health and Development, 23, 315–325.PAUL, R., 1993, Outcomes of early expressive language delay. Journal of Childhood Communication

Disorders, 15, 7–14.PINTER, J. D., ELIEZ, S., SCHMITT, J. E., CAPONE, G. T. and REISS, A. L., 2001, Neuroanatomy of Down’s

syndrome: a high-resolution MRI study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1659–1665.PLANTE, E., SWISHER, L., VANCE, R. and RAPCSAK, S., 1991, MRI findings in boys with specific language

impairment. Brain and Language, 41, 52–66.RAZ, N., TORRES, I. J., BRIGGS, S. D., SPENCER, W. D., THORNTON, A. E. and LOKEN, W. J., et al. 1995,

Selective neuroanatomic abnormalities in Down’s syndrome and their cognitive correlates:evidence from MRI morphometry. Neurology, 45, 356–366.

REID, G. and BLOCK, M. E., 1996, Motor development and physical education. In B. Stratford andP. Gunn (eds), New Approaches to Down Syndrome (London: Cassell), pp. 309–340.

RICE, M., 2000, Grammatical symptoms of specific language impairment. In D. V. M. Bishop and L. B.Leonard (eds), Speech and Language Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention and

Outcome (Hove: Psychology Press), pp. 17–34.RICE, M. and WEXLER, L., 1996, Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment.

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1239–1257.RICE, M., WEXLER, K. and HERSHBERGER, S., 1998, Tense over time: the longitudinal course of tense

acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing

Research, 41, 1421–1431.RICE, M. L., SPITZ, R. V. and O’BRIEN, M., 1999a, Semantic and morpho-syntactic language outcomes in

biologically at-risk children. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 12, 213–234.

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop448

Page 28: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

RICE, M. L., WEXLER, K. and REDMOND, S. M., 1999b, Grammaticality judgements of an extendedoptional infinitive grammar: evidence from English-speaking children with specific languageimpairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 943–961.

ROBINSON, R. J., 1991, Causes and associations of severe and persistent specific speech and languagedisorders in children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, 943–962.

ROIZEN, N., 1997, Hearing loss in children with Down syndrome: a review. Down Syndrome Quarterly, 2,1–4.

ROIZEN, N. J., WOLTERS, C., NICOL, T. and BLONDIS, T. A., 1993, Hearing loss in children with Downsyndrome. Journal of Pediatrics, 123, 9–12.

RONDAL, J. A., 1995, Exceptional Language Development in Down Syndrome: Implications for the Cognition–

Language Relationship (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).RONDAL, J. A. and EDWARDS, S., 1997, Language in Mental Retardation (London: Whurr).RONDAL, J. A. and LAMBERT, J. L., 1983, The speech of mentally retarded adults in a dyadic

communication situation: some formal and informative aspects. Psychologica Belgica, 23, 49–56.ROSES, A. D. and SAUNDERS, A. M., 1994, APOE is a major susceptibility gene for Alzheimer’s disease.

Current Opinions in Biotechnology, 5, 663–667.ROSIN, M., SWIFT, E., BLESS, D. and VETTER, D., 1988, Communication profiles in adolescents with

Down syndrome. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 12, 49–64.RUTTER, T. and BUCKLEY, S., 1994, The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in children with Down

syndrome. Down Syndrome: Research and Practice, 2, 76–82.SABSAY, S. and KERNAN, K. T., 1993, On the nature of the language impairment in Down syndrome.

Topics in Language Disorders, 13, 20–35.SCHUPF, N., KAPELL, D., NIGHTINGALE, B., RODRIGUEZ, A., TYCKO, B. and MAYEUX, R., 1998, Earlier onset of

Alzheimer’s disease in men with Down syndrome. Neurology, 50, 991–995.SCHWARTZ, M. and REGAN, V., 1996, Sequencing, timing, and rate relationships between language

and motor skill in children with receptive language delay. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12,255–270.

SCIENTIFIC LEARNING CORPORATION, 1996, Fast ForwordTM [computer software] (Berkeley: SCL).SHOTWELL, A.-M. and SHIPE, D., 1964, Effect of out-of-home care on the intellectual and social

development of Mongoloid children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 68, 693–699.SHRIBERG, L. D., TOMBLIN, J. B. and MCSWEENY, J. L., 1999, Prevalence of speech delay in 6-year-old

children and comorbidity with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing

Research, 42, 1461–1481.SNOWLING, M. J., CHIAT, S. and HULME, C., 1991, Words, nonwords and phonological processes: some

comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, & Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369–377.SPEIDEL, G. E., 1989, A biological basis for individual differences in learning to speak. In G. E. Speidel

and K. E. Nelson (eds), The Many Faces of Imitation in Language Learning (New York: Springer),pp. 199–229.

SPEIDEL, G. E., 1993, Phonological short-term memory and individual differences in learning to speak:a bilingual case study. First Language, 13, 69–91.

STEELE, J., 1996, Epidemiology: incidence, prevalence and size of the Down’s syndrome population.In B. Stratford and P. Gunn (eds), New Approaches to Down Syndrome (London: Cassell),pp. 45–72.

STOEL-GAMMON, C., 1997, Phonological development in Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and

Developmental Disabilities: Research Reviews, 3, 300–306.STOTHARD, S. E., SNOWLING, M. J., BISHOP, D. V. M., CHIPCHASE, B. B. and KAPLAN, C. A., 1998, Language

impaired pre-schoolers: a follow up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing

Research, 41, 407–418.TAGER-FLUSBERG, H., 1997, Language acquisition and theory of mind: contributions from the study of

autism. In L. B. Adamson and M. A. Romski (eds), Research on Communication and Language

Disorders: Contributions to Theories of Language Development (Baltimore: Paul Brookes).TAGER-FLUSBERG, H., 1999, Language development in atypical children. In M. Barrett (ed.), The

Development of Language (Hove: Psychology Press), pp. 311–348.TAGER-FLUSBERG, H., CALKINS, S., NOLIN, T., BAUMBERGER, T., ANDERSON, M. and CHADWICK-DIAS, A., 1990,

A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down syndrome children. Journal of

Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 1–21.TALLAL, P., 1976, Rapid auditory processing in normal and disordered language development. Journal of

Speech and Hearing, 3, 561–571.

Down’s syndrome and SLI 449

Page 29: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

TALLAL, P., 2000, Experimental studies of language learning impairments: From research toremediation. In D. V. M. Bishop and L. B. Leonard (eds), Speech and Language Impairments in

Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention and Outcome (Hove: Psychology Press), pp. 131–155.TALLAL, P. and PIERCY, M., 1973a, Defects of non-verbal auditory perception in children with

developmental aphasia. Nature, 241, 468–469.TALLAL, P. and PIERCY, M., 1973b, Developmental aphasia: Impaired rate of non-verbal processing as a

function of sensory modality. Neuropsychologia, 11, 389–398.TALLAL, P., MILLER, S., BEDI, G., BYMA, G., WANG, X., NAGARAJAN, S., SCHREINER, C., JENKINS, W. M. and

MERZENICH, M. M., 1996, Language comprehension in language-learning impaired childrenimproved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 81–84.

TALLAL, P., STARK, R. E., KALLMAN, C. and MELLITS, D., 1981, A re-examination of some nonverbalperceptual abilities of language-impaired and normal children as a function of age and sensorymodality. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 351–357.

TEW, B., 1979, The cocktail party syndrome in children with hydrocephalus and spina bifida. British

Journal of Disorders of Communication, 14, 89–101.THAL, D., TOBIAS, S. and MORRISON, D., 1991, Language and gesture in late talkers: a 1-year follow-up.

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 604–612.THORDARDOTTIR, E. T., CHAPMAN, R. S. and WAGNER, L., 2002, Complex sentence production by

adolescents with Down syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 163–183.TOMBLIN, J. B., FREESE, P. and RECORDS, N., 1992, Diagnosing specific language impairment in adults for

the purpose of pedigree analysis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 832–843.TOMBLIN, J. B. and ZHANG, X., 1999, Language patterns and etiology in children with specific language

impairment. In H. Tager-Flusberg (ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (Cambridge, MA: MITPress), pp. 361–382.

TORRES, C. and BUCETA, J., 1998, Effect of parental intervention on motor development of Down syndromeinfants between birth and age two years. British Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 44, 94–101.

VALLAR, G. and PAPAGNO, C., 1993, Preserved vocabulary acquisition in Down’s syndrome: the role ofphonological short term memory. Cortex, 29, 467–483.

VAN DER LELY, H. K. J. and HOWARD, D., 1993, Children with specific language impairment: Linguisticimpairment or short-term memory deficit? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1193–1207.

VAN DER LELY, H. K. J. and STOLLWERCK, L., 1996, A grammatical specific language impairment inchildren: an autosomal dominance inheritance? Brain and Language, 52, 484–504.

VAN DER LELY, H. K. J. and ULLMAN, M. T., 2001, Past tense morphology in specifically languageimpaired and normally developing children. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 177–217.

VARNHAGEN, C. K., DAS, J. P. and VARNHAGEN, S., 1987, Auditory and visual memory span: cognitiveprocessing by TMR individuals with Down syndrome and other etiologies. American Journal on

Mental Deficiency, 91, 398–405.VICARI, S., CARLESIMO, A. and CALTAGIRONE, C., 1995, Short term memory in persons with intellectual

disabilities and Down’s syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 39, 532–537.VICARI, S., CASELLI, M. C. and TONUCCI, F., 2000, Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic

development in children with Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 38, 634–644.WANG, P. P. and BELLUGI, U., 1994, Evidence from two genetic syndromes for a dissociation between

verbal and visual-spatial short-term memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,16, 317–322.

WATKINS, R., RICE, M. and MOLZ, C., 1993, Verb use by language impaired and normally developingchildren. First Language, 37, 133–143.

WHITEHURST, G. J., ARNOLD, D. S., FISCHEL, J. E., LONIGAN, C. J. and VALDEZ-MANCHACA, M. C., 1991a,Family history of developmental expressive language delay. Journal of Speech and Language

Disorders, 34, 1150–1157.WHITEHURST, G. J., FISCHEL, J. E., LONIGAN, C. J., VALDEZ-MANCHACA, M. C., ARNOLD, D. S. and SMITH, M.,

1991b, Treatment of early expressive language delay: if, when, and how. Topics in Language

Disorders, 11, 55–68.WIDEN, J. E., FOLSON, R. C., THOMPSON, G. and WILSON, W. R., 1987, Auditory brainstem responses in

young adults with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Deficiency, 91, 472–479.WISHART, J., 1998, Development in children with Down syndrome: facts, findings, the future.

International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 45, 343–363.WISNIEWSKI, K. E., 1990, Down syndrome children often have brains with maturation delay, retardation

of growth and cortical dysgenesis. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Supplement, 7, 274–281.

G. Laws and D. V. M. Bishop450

Page 30: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders · SLI could reflect neurological and/or genotypic commonalities between these disorders. Over the past decade there has

Dow

nloa

ded

By: [

Uni

vers

ity C

olle

ge D

ublin

] At:

16:5

4 22

Mar

ch 2

007

WORLD HEALTH ORGANISATION, 1992, The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: ClinicalDescriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines (Geneva: WHO).

WRIGHT, B. A., LOMBARDINO, L. D., KING, W. M., PUANIK, C. S., LEONARD, C. M. and MERZENICH, M. M.,1997, Deficits in auditory temporal and spectral resolution in language-impaired children.Nature, 387, 176–178.

WULPERT, M., INGLIS, S., KRIEGSMAN, E. and MILLS, B., 1975, Language delay and associated mother–childinteractions. Developmental Psychology, 11, 61–70.

Down’s syndrome and SLI 451