individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...review individual differences in...

11
Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between individuals Michael B. Miller , Christa-Lynn Donovan, Craig M. Bennett, Elissa M. Aminoff, Richard E. Mayer University of California, Santa Barbara, CA, USA abstract article info Article history: Received 15 March 2011 Revised 18 May 2011 Accepted 19 May 2011 Available online 30 May 2011 Keywords: fMRI Inter-individual variability Cognitive style Encoding strategies Memory retrieval Neuroimaging is being used increasingly to make inferences about an individual. Yet, those inferences are often confounded by the fact that topographical patterns of task-related brain activity can vary greatly from person to person. This study examined two factors that may contribute to the variability across individuals in a memory retrieval task: individual differences in cognitive style and individual differences in encoding strategy. Cognitive style was probed using a battery of assessments focused on the individual's tendency to visualize or verbalize written material. Encoding strategy was probed using a series of questions designed to assess typical strategies that an individual might utilize when trying to remember a list of words. Similarity in brain activity was assessed by cross-correlating individual t-statistic maps contrasting the BOLD response during retrieval to the BOLD response during xation. Individual differences in cognitive style and encoding strategy accounted for a signicant portion of the variance in similarity. This was true above and beyond individual differences in anatomy and memory performance. These results demonstrate the need for a multidimensional approach in the use of fMRI to make inferences about an individual. © 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Contents Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 Stimuli . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 Behavioral paradigm . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 Assessments of cognitive style and strategy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 Functional MRI data acquisition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 Functional MRI data analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 Individual variability analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 Behavioral analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 Individual variability analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 Between-subject variability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 Hierarchical regression analyseswhole brain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 Hierarchical regression analysesseparate brain regions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90 Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 Appendix A. Supplementary data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 Introduction One of the goals of neuroergonomics is to use neuroscientic tools to understand the individual mind at work in a naturalistic NeuroImage 59 (2012) 8393 Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106-9660, USA. Fax: +1 805 893 4303. E-mail address: [email protected] (M.B. Miller). 1053-8119/$ see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.060 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg

Upload: others

Post on 15-Jul-2020

8 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

NeuroImage

j ourna l homepage: www.e lsev ie r.com/ locate /yn img

Review

Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in thepatterns of brain activity between individuals

Michael B. Miller ⁎, Christa-Lynn Donovan, Craig M. Bennett, Elissa M. Aminoff, Richard E. MayerUniversity of California, Santa Barbara, CA, USA

⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of PsychoSanta Barbara, CA 93106-9660, USA. Fax: +1 805 893 4

E-mail address: [email protected] (M.B. Miller)

1053-8119/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Aldoi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.060

a b s t r a c t

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:Received 15 March 2011Revised 18 May 2011Accepted 19 May 2011Available online 30 May 2011

Keywords:fMRIInter-individual variabilityCognitive styleEncoding strategiesMemory retrieval

Neuroimaging is being used increasingly to make inferences about an individual. Yet, those inferences areoften confounded by the fact that topographical patterns of task-related brain activity can vary greatly fromperson to person. This study examined two factors that may contribute to the variability across individuals ina memory retrieval task: individual differences in cognitive style and individual differences in encodingstrategy. Cognitive style was probed using a battery of assessments focused on the individual's tendency tovisualize or verbalize written material. Encoding strategy was probed using a series of questions designed toassess typical strategies that an individual might utilize when trying to remember a list of words. Similarity inbrain activity was assessed by cross-correlating individual t-statistic maps contrasting the BOLD responseduring retrieval to the BOLD response during fixation. Individual differences in cognitive style and encodingstrategy accounted for a significant portion of the variance in similarity. This was true above and beyondindividual differences in anatomy and memory performance. These results demonstrate the need for amultidimensional approach in the use of fMRI to make inferences about an individual.

logy, University of California,303..

l rights reserved.

© 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Contents

Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83Methods. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85

Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85Stimuli . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85Behavioral paradigm . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86Assessments of cognitive style and strategy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86Functional MRI data acquisition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86Functional MRI data analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86Individual variability analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87

Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88Behavioral analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88Individual variability analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88

Between-subject variability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88Hierarchical regression analyses—whole brain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89Hierarchical regression analyses—separate brain regions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89

Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92Appendix A. Supplementary data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92

Introduction

One of the goals of neuroergonomics is to use neuroscientifictools to understand the individual mind at work in a naturalistic

Page 2: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

84 M.B. Miller et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

environment (Parasuraman and Wilson, 2008). But the use offunctional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to make an inferenceabout an individual is particularly challenging (see Parasuraman andJiang, 2012-this issue). Imagine a situation in which an institution(e.g., a court of law) seeks to use neuroimaging to establish theveracity of an individual's memory. They have a template based onhundreds of individuals for what that individual's pattern of brainactivity should look like for a true memory. Yet, the individual inquestion has a very different pattern of brain activity. Are they toconclude that the individual must not be experiencing a truememory? The veracity of memory is only one dimension that mayexplain why this individual's pattern of brain activity is so differentfrom the other individuals. For example, there may have beenfundamental differences in the individual's cognitive style thatsignificantly affected their pattern of activity as well. If neuroimagingwill be used to make an inference about an individual, then multipledimensions in which individuals may differ must be considered.

We have shown previously that the topographical pattern of brainactivity underlying a memory retrieval task can vary greatly fromindividual to individual, sometimes with little to no overlap insignificant activations between individuals (Miller et al., 2002; Milleret al., 2009). However, despite that extensive variability, theindividual patterns of brain activity are relatively consistent overtime (Miller et al., 2002; Miller et al., 2009; for review of fMRIreliability, see Bennett and Miller, 2010), suggesting that differencesin the patterns of brain activity are due to systematic differences inindividual characteristics and are not due to random measurementerror (see Fig. 1). In order to effectively use fMRI to infer uniqueaspects of the individual mind, it is necessary to untangle the criticalfactors that can vary the individual patterns of brain activity. In thisstudy, we examine whether individual differences in strategy andcognitive style can account for the degree of similarity between anytwo individual patterns of brain activity during a retrieval task.

Fig. 1. Two participants from two scanning sessions held months apart representing the crinter-individual correlation across all subjects=.224) and sessions (average intra-individuabetween individual t-statistic map volumes, i.e., the higher the r, the more similar the patternfor visualization purposes only (adapted from Miller et al., 2009).

Using neuroscience methods to gain insight into the individualmind is a common goal among many institutions, including education(Byrnes, 2001; Posner and Rothbart, 2005), the military (NationalResearch Council, 2008), and courts of law (Brown and Murphy,2010). Implicit in the goals of these institutions is the ability to makejudgments about an individual based on neuroscientific data collectedfrom a group of individuals. This goal is often incompatible withthe general scientific goal to make an inference about a generalphenomenon that applies to a population by averaging data acrossindividuals (Faigman, 2010). For fMRI in particular, this demand toaverage data across individuals is compounded by the fact that thesignal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of the BOLD signal is very low (Fristonet al., 1999) and that false positives due to the low SNR are far toocommon (Bennett et al., 2010). Yet, as acquisition devices andanalytical tools for fMRI become more and more sophisticated,increasing effort is being made to infer unique aspects of theindividual based on the group data.

A critical question remains within functional neuroimaging: do theresults of a group analysis accurately represent the individuals thatmake up that group? Many studies have concluded that it does not(Heun et al., 2000; Machielsen et al., 2000; McGonigle et al., 2000;Miller et al., 2002; Feredoes and Postle, 2007; Seghier et al., 2008;Miller et al., 2009; Seghier and Price, 2009; Parasuraman and Jiang,2012-this issue). For example, we found that the observed variationsin functional brain activity across the whole brain during a simplerecognition task were extensive, with some individuals activatingmostly prefrontal regions while others activated mostly parietalregions (Miller et al., 2002; Miller et al., 2009). This was in contrast tothe group analysis, which prominently showed both regions to beequally active. Are there quantifiable factors that might help toexplain this effect? We found indirect evidence in a recent study thatsome variations in the degree of brain activity similarity betweenindividuals during a memory retrieval task may be due to individual

oss-correlation of unthresholded t-maps (retrievalNbaseline) across subjects (averagel correlation across all subjects=.482). Each r-value represents the degree of similarityof brain activity. The depiction of t-statistic maps is thresholded (pb .001 uncorrected)

Page 3: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

85M.B. Miller et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

differences in strategy (Miller et al., 2009). That is, the larger thedifference in decision criterion themore dissimilar the two patterns ofbrain activity. However, a criterion measure is not a direct measure ofmemory strategy. Therefore, in this study we directly measureindividual differences in cognitive style and strategy and test whetheror not these differences can account for a significant portion of thevariance in the similarity of brain activity between individuals.

Brain activity during a recognition memory task is a usefulplatform to study individual variability for two reasons: (1) thetremendous amount of previous research conducted on the relation-ship between recognition memory and fronto-parietal regionsand (2) the widespread and distributed nature of the activity un-derlying the task (Miller and Van Horn, 2007). Many of the fronto-parietal regions implicated in recognition memory are known tounderlie cognitive processes peripheral to the actual retrieval process(Shimamura, 1995; Fletcher et al., 1998; Moscovitch and Winocur,2002). One potential implication of this distributed architecture isthat one and the same behavioral outcome—such as an “old” responseon a recognition test—could be the result of information processingand neural circuitry that are distinct to the individual but vary acrossindividuals.

There is considerable evidence that individuals engage in a varietyof strategies during the encoding phase of a standard recognitionmemory task (Stoff and Eagle, 1971; Battig, 1975; Weinstein et al.,1979; Paivio, 1983; Reder, 1987; Graf and Birt, 1996) and that in-dividual differences in strategy can modulate the BOLD activity incertain brain regions (Savage et al., 2001; Casasanto et al., 2002; Speeret al., 2003; Kondo et al., 2005; Tsukiura et al., 2005). One notablestudy by Kirchhoff and Buckner (2006) identified the variousstrategies people adopt during the unconstrained encoding of anunrelated pairs of pictures. They found that verbal elaborationcorrelated with activity in prefrontal regions associated withcontrolled verbal processing, while visual inspection correlated withactivity in the extrastriate cortex known to be involved in higher-order visual processing. These findings suggest that different encod-ing strategies may recruit different brain regions, even though thememory performance is similar between the two strategies.While theKirchhoff and Buckner (2006) study showed how different encodingstrategies can recruit different brain regions during the encodingphase, we investigated how different encoding strategies may accountfor the similarity in brain activity during the retrieval phase. One ofthe classic principles of episodic memory, the encoding specificityprinciple, states that encoding operations are directly related toretrieval operations (Tulving and Thomson, 1973). This relationship isillustrated by the fact that differences in encoding strategies can havea direct effect on the brain activity that occurs during retrieval(Raposo et al., 2009; Kirchhoff, 2009). From this evidence, wehypothesize that individual differences in encoding strategy willaccount for a significant portion of the variance that is observed in thesimilarity of brain activity between individuals during a retrieval task.

Aside from the particular strategy that an individual may chooseto engage in during a memory task, individuals may also have aparticular style of thinking or a preferred set of cognitive operationsthat could affect their pattern of brain activity. For example, thevisualizer–verbalizer dimension of cognitive style is based on the ideathat some people (who could be called visualizers) are better atprocessing visual material, whereas other people (who could be calledverbalizers) are better at processing verbal material (Mayer andMassa, 2003; Massa and Mayer, 2006). Although individual differ-ences in the tendency to engage in visual or verbal processing haslittle relationship to memory recall (Richardson, 1978, 1998) and thesearch for research-based attributes that may interact with instruc-tional methods has had a somewhat disappointing history (Cronbachand Snow, 1977; Pashler et al., 2009; Sternberg and Zhang, 2001;Zhang and Sternberg, 2009), differences in cognitive style may still bepertinent to the issue of individual differences in brain activity during

a retrieval task. The likelihood of involvement of any particularprocess and its underlying brain region may depend a great deal onthe processing style of the individual. In other words, a visualizer andverbalizer may not necessarily differ in performance on learningoutcome tests but may have highly distinct patterns of regional brainactivity during learning (i.e., encoding) and retrieval that achieve thesame level of performance. There is evidence suggesting that thismay well be the case. Two recent studies by Kozhevnikov et al.(2002)) and Kozhevnikov et al. (2005) attempted to clarify and revisethe visualizer–verbalizer dimension and subsequently introduced twosubtypes of visualizers: object visualizers who tended to focus onobject properties such as shape and color and spatial visualizers whotended to focus on spatial properties such as location and spatialrelations. A later fMRI study revealed that object visualizers activatedmore ventral regions of the visual processing stream while spatialvisualizers activated more dorsal regions of the visual processingstream (Motes and Kozhevnikov, 2006). We hypothesize that dif-ferences in cognitive style may also be a significant contributor to thevariability of the patterns of brain activity during a retrieval task.

In this study we test the hypothesis that individual differences instrategy and cognitive style will account for a significant portion of thevariance in the similarity in the patterns of whole-brain activitybetween individuals during a memory retrieval task. To measure thevariability in the patterns of brain activity across individuals we cross-correlate the unthresholded t-statistic maps contributed by eachindividual from the retrieval task (Miller et al., 2002; Miller et al.,2009). The more similar two individual patterns of brain activity, thehigher the correlation value (see Fig. 1). Individuals were assessed onstrategies and cognitive style after the fMRI scanning session using abattery of tests. In addition, strategy was implicitly manipulatedwithin participants by varying the imageability of the word stimuli. Ifstrategies and style depend to some degree on visualizing the wordstimuli, thenmanipulating the imageability of the words should affectthe predictability of those factors. We predict that individualdifferences in strategy and cognitive style will significantly accountfor inter-individual differences in brain activity above and beyond anyfactors attributable to individual differences in anatomy and memoryperformance.

Methods

Participants

A group of 50 participants (age 18–55, M=25.8) were recruitedfrom the undergraduate and graduate student population at Univer-sity of California, Santa Barbara (UCSB) and were paid for theirparticipation. Data from three participants was excluded due toexcessive motion (1), scanner malfunction (1), or withdrawal fromthe study (1). Two more participants were excluded because theywere left-handed. The remaining 45 participants were comprised of22 men and 23 women. All participants gave informed consent asapproved by the UCSB Institutional Review Board.

Stimuli

Participants completed two study–test sessions, the order of whichwas counterbalanced across participants. In one session, participantswere presented lists of highly imageable, concrete nouns, while theother session presented lowly imageable, abstract nouns. Words werechosen to be 4–12 letters in length. High-imageability words hadimageability scores greater than one standard deviation above themean on the MRC Psycholinguistic Database Imageability rating(Coltheart, 1981) while low-imageability words were chosen to begreater than 1 SD below the mean. Within each study-test session theword order was randomized. Whether the word was old or new wasalso counterbalanced across participants.

Page 4: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

86 M.B. Miller et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

Behavioral paradigm

This experiment employed an event-related fMRI design andincluded two study–test sessions—one for the low-imageability wordsand the other for the high-imageability words. Each study sessionconsisted of 106 words that were presented for 2 s each. These studyword trials were randomly intermixed with eighty 2-s fixation trials(‘+’). During the study session participants were simply instructed totry and learn the words for a later memory test. There was a 10-mininterval between study and test phases. During this time, structuralscans (first high-resolution T1 MPRAGE and then diffusion tensorimaging scans) were collected. The test sessions consisted of 318events: 212 test words (50% studied and 50% new) and 106 fixationtrials, again randomly intermixed. Participants were instructed torespond with their index finger if an item was studied (i.e., “old”) andwith middle finger if the item was new. Responses were collectedusing a MR-compatible button box held in the right hand. Once thescanning portion of the experiment was complete, participantscompleted an assessment of their cognitive style and strategies.

Assessments of cognitive style and strategy

Cognitive style was assessed using a visualizer–verbalizer testbattery. This consisted of (1) a participant questionnaire on Verbal–Spatial Ability Rating and Verbal–Visual Style Rating (Mayer andMassa, 2003); (2) the Card Rotation Test (Ekstrom et al., 1976);(3) the Paper Folding Test (Ekstrom et al., 1976); (4) a VocabularyTest (18 items adapted from the Vocabulary scale of the ArmedServices Vocational Aptitude Battery (ASVAB) as selected from atest preparation book (Hogan and Cannon, 2007); (5) the Verbalizer–Visualizer Questionnaire (VVQ; Richardson, 1977); (6) the SantaBarbara Learning Style Questionnaire (Mayer and Massa, 2003); and(7) the Object–Spatial Imagery Questionnaire (Blajenkova et al.,2006). The results from the Visualizer–Verbalizer test battery datawere submitted to a factor analysis using Principle ComponentsAnalysis and VARIMAX rotation to yield four orthogonal factors thatare related to visualizing and verbalizing tendencies. Factors wereinterpreted and named based on the weightings of each of themeasures. Prior research has been conducted showing the relation-ship of these scales to visualizing and verbalizing cognitive styles(Mayer andMassa, 2003; Massa and Mayer, 2006). Factor scores werethen computed for each participant from the linear combinations ofscale scores.

Cognitive encoding strategies were measured using a strategyquestionnaire adapted from Kirchhoff and Buckner (2006). Althoughthis questionnaire is meant to probe the strategies used during thestudy phase of the experiment, those encoding strategies would berelated to the strategies and processes utilized during retrieval as well(Tulving and Thomson, 1973). This questionnaire consisted of 10strategy descriptions (e.g., “Repeated the words to yourself” or“Formed a picture of the word in your mind”) and instructed theparticipant to rate on a 5-point scale how often they used eachstrategy during the study phase (see Appendix for a sample of theQuestionnaire). A separate questionnaire was used for the set of high-imageability words and the set of low-imageability words. As in theanalysis above, the ratings were submitted to a Principle ComponentsAnalysis and VARIMAX rotation to yield four components witheigenvalues greater than 1.0.

After the scanning was complete, in addition to the questionnaire,cognitive strategy was probed by simply asking the participants theirmemory strategy. Questions were asked regarding the first set ofwords, the second set of words, whether there was a difference whenstudying the two sets of words, and how they made a decision as towhether the word was studied or not. The participants answeredthese questions through free response. These responses were thencategorized as either verbal or visual by three blind raters (50%

agreement across all three raters and 87% agreement across tworaters). One of the authors then mediated any disagreements usingthe general rule that any indication of visual imagery would be codedas visual.

Functional MRI data acquisition

Functional images were acquired with gradient-recalled echo-planar imaging (TR=2000 ms, TE=30 ms, RF flip angle=90, gradi-ent-echo pulse sequence, 33 contiguous axial slices at 3.0 mm thickwith a 0.5 mm slice gap, and an in-plane resolution of 64×64 pixels ina FOV of 192 cm, producing voxels of 3 mm×3 mm×3 mm) on a 3 TSiemens TrioMRI scanner equippedwith high-performance gradients.Each BOLD run was preceded by four scans to allow for steady-statemagnetization to be approached. In addition to the functional scans,a high-resolution T1-weighted structural image was acquired using a3-D SPGR pulse sequence (TR=25 ms, TE=6 ms, RF flip angle=25°,bandwidth=15.6 kHz, voxel size=.9375 mm×1.25 mm×1.2 mm).Diffusion-weightedMRI datawere acquiredusing adiffusionweighted,single-shot spin-echo, echo-planar sequence (TR=9022 msec;TE=91msec; flip angle=90°; slice thickness=2.0 mm; number ofslices=70 (axial); FOV=240mm; matrix size=128×128; acquisitiontime=5:24 min). Diffusion weighting (b-value=1000 s/mm2) wasapplied along 32 directions with one additional reference imageacquired having no diffusion weighting (b-value=0 s/mm2). Foampadding was used to minimize head motion.

Functional MRI data analysis

Initial data preprocessing utilized SPM5 (Wellcome Department ofCognitive Neurology, London, UK) for slice acquisition correction,motion correction, coregistration, and spatial normalization. Thespatially normalized scans were then smoothed with an 8 mm FWHMisotropic Gaussian kernel to accommodate anatomical differencesacross participants. Statistical analysis was conducted using customizedMatlab scripts implementing a standard least-squares voxel-wisegeneral linear model, as described in Guerin and Miller (2009). Therewere 10 separate parameters to model each 2-s post-stimulus timepoint up to 20 s (Ollinger et al., 2001). This unique set of 10 parameterswas utilized for each trial type: old-high imageable, new-high image-able, old-low imageable, new-low imageable. To contrast responsesbetween trial types, we calculated the mean of the 2nd, 3rd, and 4thtime points of the estimated event-related response. In addition to theparameters already discussed, parameters were included to modellinear drift within each session and the session-specific means. Thecritical contrast used for the individual variability analysis was retrievalgreater than fixation for both the high-imageability condition and thelow-imageability condition. The t-statistic maps from these contraststhat were input into the cross-correlation analysis were unthresholdedso that a particular result would not be determined by the setting of anarbitrary threshold (seeMiller et al., 2009). The thresholds used for Fig. 2are for visualization purposes only.

Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) analysis

All analyses were carried out using the SPM Tools diffusiontoolbox as implemented in SPM5 (http://sourceforge.net/apps/trac/spmtools/). For preprocessing, diffusion-weighted imagesweremotion-corrected and coregistered to the high-resolution T1-weighted image,which we spatially normalized to the MNI template brain. The resultingnormalization parameters were subsequently applied to the diffusion-weighted images, reorienting the gradient directions accordingly.Following preprocessing, second-order diffusion tensors and fractionalanisotropy (FA) values were established using the standard multipleregression approach. Individual FA images reflect the coherence of waterdiffusion on a voxel-by-voxel basis.

Page 5: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

Fig. 2. Display of widespread inter-individual variability across the retrieval condition with high-imageability words. The large rendering in the top left represents the results of asecond-level group analysis of the data. The remaining 45 renderings each represent the results from one subject for the retrieval(high)Nbaseline contrast. The threshold for allimages was identical and set to pb0.005 (uncorrected) for purposes of illustration. The mean inter-individual correlation value across the entire dataset was 0.35.

87M.B. Miller et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

Individual variability analysis

To quantify the variability in individual patterns of brain activityacross the whole brain or across particular regions the unthresholdedt-statistic map image volumes were cross-correlated across partici-pants. This was done separately for each imageability condition. If onetakes a three-dimensional volume of continuous values in each voxeland correlates that volumewith another three-dimensional volume ofcontinuous values in the same voxel matrix and atlas space then theresult will be a single correlation value that represents the similaritybetween two volumes (see Miller et al., 2009, for a discussion of themethod). A mask generated during the group analysis of all 45participants was used so that only voxels with a signal intensity valuefor every participant were included in the analysis. The cross-correlation of 45 individual t-statistic maps produced 990 observa-tions of unique pairings between different individuals. The resultingcorrelation values were then submitted to a multivariate hierarchicalregression analysis with 60 predictor variables. Each factor wasentered into the regression equation as part of a set of factors based ontheir theoretical relevance (see Table 1). Each set of factors isdescribed below in the order that they were entered into theregression equation. It was critical to enter cognitive style andstrategy after the other variables so that any relationship betweenthem and the correlation values could not be attributed to differencesin another characteristic.

1. Procedural was a single factor representing the presentation order(high- or low-imageability blocks first), coded as 0 for same and 1

for different. This was considered a nuisance variable and thereforeit was entered first.

2. Demographics included variables for the similarity in age and sex.Differences in the similarity of brain activity could be attributed todifferences in age and/or sex of the individuals, so this factor wasentered next.

3. Anatomy included a cross-correlation of each individual's normal-ized high-resolution anatomical (mprage) image and of eachindividual's normalized diffusion tensor image (FA). Similar to thedemographics factors, any differences in the similarity of brainactivity could be attributed to structural differences in anatomy asmeasured by a high-resolution T1 scan or by DTI. Therefore, thisfactor was entered next.

4. Performance was represented by the difference between theindividuals' measures in d´ and in criterion. Any of the predicteddifferences in similarity of brain activity that could be accountedfor by differences in cognitive style and strategy could be attributedinstead to differences in memory performance. Therefore, it wasnecessary to account for these factors prior to entering in the setshaving to do with cognitive style and strategy.

5. Encoding Strategy was represented by five variables. The firstvariable represented the categorization of the individual's responseto the open-ended strategy question, coded as 0 for same and 1 fordifferent across the two study sessions. The prediction was thatdifferent strategies will have lower correlations. Strategy wasfurther represented by the four factor scores from the strategyquestionnaire. The four factors represented four different strategiesfor high-imageability words (verbal, visual, categorization, and

Page 6: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

1 Although the focus of this study was on the variability of brain activity during theretrieval phase of the task, we did have data on the brain activity during the encodingphase as well. However, the fMRI data for the encoding phase turned out to possess amuch lower SNR relative to the retrieval phase (the average correlation betweenparticipants for encoding was .185 compared to .358 for retrieval). This was mostlikely due to the fact that only half as many trials were collected during the studyphase. Any inferences that could be drawn about the differences in variability betweenencoding and retrieval (e.g., differences in encoding strategy were a significant factorfor the variability in brain activity during retrieval but not during encoding) would beconfounded by the differences in the number of trials.

Table 1The unique variance (standardized betas) contributed by each factor in the prediction of the degree of similarity in the patterns of brain activity between individuals during amemory retrieval task.

High imageability Low imageability

Whole Frontal Parietal Tempo. Occip. Whole Frontal Parietal Tempo. Occip.

Procedural Order −.015 −.002 −.022 .003 −.005 −.028 −.022 −.029 −.031 −.005Demographics Age −.042 .080 −.016 −.120* −.004 −.100* −.116* −.078 −.012 −.118*

Sex .019 −.009 .037 .030 .013 .005 .001 .057 .014 −.016Anatomy MPRAGE .100 .010 .017 .032 .100 .123* .017 .058 .080 .118*

DTI (FA) .281* .205* .152* .284* .256* .407* .351* .247* .412* .170*Performance Memory (d´) −.006 −.010 −.013 −.015 .020 .005 .039 .020 −.052 −.032

Criterion −.060 −.004 .082 −.115* −.023 −.037 −.052 .067 −.063 −.011Encodingstrategya Open-ended −.009 .005 −.043 −.018 −.002 .130* .168* .112* .029 .071

Verbal −.100 −.073 −.066 −.147* −.061 −.068 −.106* −.091 −.013 −.005Visual −.044 .024 .011 .027 −.161* −.171* −.180* −.199* −.059 −.160*Categorical −.065 −.141* −.036 .055 −.115* −.176* −.205* −.181* −.102* −.072Rote/Elaborate .106* .129* .070 .064 .048 .107* .096 .105 .092 .033

Cognitivestyle Verbal .016 −.029 .033 .047 .100 .052 .005 .046 .065 .159*Visual −.139* −.129* −.131* −.174* −.024 −.020 −.004 .000 −.057 .027Imagery −.081 −.016 −.158* −.114* −.019 −.112* .023 −.105* −.114* −.130*Spatial −.025 −.032 −.055 −.020 .040 −.040 −.010 −.067 −.076 .002

Beta values in bold type are significant at the pb .01 level, and those with an * are significant at the pb .001 level. aThe five factors were determined separately for the high and low-imageability conditions, but those factors with a similar intuitive label were included in the same row (see Supplementary Materials).

88 M.B. Miller et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

rote memorization) and four different strategies for low-image-ability words (verbal, visual, categorization, and elaboration) (seeSupplemental Material). Only the factor scores relevant to thehigh-imageability words were entered into the equationwith brainactivity associated with the high-imageability condition, and viceversa. The prediction was the bigger the difference in factor scores,the lower the correlation.

6. Cognitive Stylewas represented by the four factors derived from thebattery of visualize–verbalize tests. The four factors representedfour different cognitive styles: verbal, visual, imagery, and spatial(see Supplemental Material). The prediction was the bigger thedifference in factor scores, the lower the correlation. The set ofcognitive style factors was entered after the set of strategy factorsfor arbitrary reasons, and the order of these two sets had littleeffect on the results (indicating little shared variance between thetwo sets).

7. Individual Residuals represented 44 dummy variables, each onerepresenting whether or not a particular individual was a part ofthe correlation pair. As reported in Miller et al. (2009), someindividuals are more deviant from the group than others in theirpatterns of brain activity. It was necessary to enter these variableslast because some of the differences between individuals wereexpected to be captured by the variables previously entered, butthere could be differences in certain characteristics that we havenot captured. These variables were meant to illustrate uniqueaspects of individuals that have yet to be represented.

Results

Behavioral analysis

A signal detection analysis was separately conducted on theresults of the recognition test for the high-imageability words andthe low-imageability words. Using a repeated-measures ANOVA wefound that memory accuracy, or d´, was significantly higher for thehigh-imageability words (1.51) than the low-imageability words(0.84) (F(1,44)=81.25, MSE=.123, pb .001), while criterion, C, wasnot significantly different between the high-imageability words(0.18) than the low-imageability words (0.13) (F(1,44)=1.80,MSE=.029, n.s.).

Details of the results of the factor analysis for both cognitive styleand strategy are contained in the Supplemental Material. In brief, theresults from the battery of visualizing and verbalizing tests loadedonto

four style factors that we labeled based on an intuitive evaluation ofthe component scores as verbal (Factor 1), visual (Factor 2), imagery(Factor 3), and spatial (Factor 4). The results from the strategyquestionnaire similarly loaded onto four factors for the both the high-imageability and the low-imageability words labeled as verbal(Factor 1), visual (Factor 3 for high imageability and Factor 2 for thelow imageability), categorical (Factor 2 for high imageability andFactor 3 for the low imageability), rote memory for high-imageabilitywords (Factor 4) and elaboration for low-imageabilitywords (Factor 4).

Correlation tests (with alpha at .05) showed that individualdifferences in the visualizing and verbalizing style factor scores had nosignificant relationship with memory (d´) in either the high imagerycognition or the low imagery condition. Some of the strategy factorsdid have a significant relationship to d´, but only in the high imagerycondition. The visual strategy factor (Factor 3) in the high imagerycondition strongly correlated with memory performance (d′):r=.446, pb .002. In contrast, the rote strategy factor (Factor 4) hada negative effect on memory performance: r=−.407, pb .006. Sincethe words used in the high imagery condition were those that werehighly imageable, it makes sense that a visual strategy may benefitlong-term memory performance.

Individual variability analysis1

Between-subject variabilityThe extreme difference in the patterns of brain activity between

individuals is evident in Figs. 2 and 3. The findings replicate ourprevious studies (Miller et al., 2002; Miller et al., 2009) in that theaverage correlation between high-imageability and low-imageabilitywords within the same participant (.626) was significantly higherthan the average correlation between high-imageability and low-imageability words between different participants (.341) (F(1,2023)=312.15, MSE=.011, pb .001). There was also a difference in variability

Page 7: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

Fig. 3. Display of widespread inter-individual variability across the retrieval condition with low-imageability words. The large rendering in the top left represents the results of asecond-level group analysis of the data. The remaining 45 renderings each represent the results from one subject for the retrieval(low)Nbaseline contrast. The threshold for allimages was identical and set to pb0.005 (uncorrected) for purposes of illustration. The mean inter- individual correlation value across the entire dataset was 0.37.

89M.B. Miller et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

between the imageability conditions, with the average correlation oflow-imageability words (.371) being significantly higher than theaverage correlation of high-imageability words (.346) (F(1,990)=56.96, MSE=.005, pb .001).

Hierarchical regression analyses—whole brainThe results are summarized in Fig. 4 (which collapses the results

across imageability conditions) and Table 1 (which lists thestandardized betas for each factor). For high-imageability words,the full model accounted for 79.3% of the variance in correlationvalues across pairs of whole-brain patterns of activity. The modelfirst takes into account the differences in the order of the condi-tions (procedural), which was not significant (R2 change=.000,F(1,988)=0.05, n.s.); then differences in demographics (R2 change=.006, F(2,986)=3.09, p=.046); differences in anatomy (R2 change=.108, F(2,984)=59.83, pb .001); and differences in performance (R2

change=.006, F(2,982)=2.46, n.s.). Above and beyond those differ-ences, differences in strategywere significantly related to the variability(R2 change=.023, F(5,977)=5.16, pb .001). Above and beyonddifferences in strategy and the other characteristics, differences incognitive style were also significantly related to the variability (R2

change=.025, F(4,973)=7.25, pb .001). The shared variance betweenthe set of cognitive style factors and the set of strategy factors was lessthan2%. Finally, another 62.7%of thevariance in correlationvalues couldbe accounted for by the dummy variables coding for each individual (R2

change=.627, F(44,929)=63.81, pb .001). For the low-imageabilitycondition, the full model accounted for 82.3% of the variance incorrelation values. The results were similar to the high-imageability

condition except that anatomy and strategy differences accounted for amuch larger portion of the variance in the correlation values. Again,the model first takes into account the differences in the order ofthe conditions (procedural), which was not significant (R2 change=.002, F(1,988)=1.63, n.s.); then differences in demographics (R2

change=.012, F(2,986)=6.13, p=.002); differences in anatomy (R2

change=.252, F(2,984)=168.72, pb .001); and differences in perfor-mance (R2 change=.002, F(2,982)=1.22, n.s.). Above and beyondthose differences, differences in strategy were significantly related tothe variability (R2 change=.094, F(5,977)=28.75, pb .001). Above andbeyond differences in strategy and other characteristics, differences incognitive style were also significantly related to the variability (R2

change=.017, F(4,973)=6.63, pb .001). Finally, another 44.5% of thevariance in correlation values could be accounted for by the dummyvariables coding for each individual (R2 change=.445, F(44,929)=53.23, pb .001).

Hierarchical regression analyses—separate brain regionsAn identical analysis as the one used for the whole brain was

conducted separately for the cortices of each of the lobes. Masks foreach lobe were generated using the Wake Forest Pickatlas Matlabtoolbox (Maldjian et al., 2003). The results for each lobe were verysimilar to the whole brain, with the differences summarized in Fig. 4and Table 1 with the standardized betas for each factor listed. One ofthe notable differences between the lobeswas in the low-imageabilitycondition, in which strategy differences (i.e., differences in theparticular strategies that individuals chose to utilize for that particularset of words) had a much stronger relationship to variability in the

Page 8: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

Fig. 4. Results of the hierarchical regression analysis predicting the similarity in the patterns of brain activity between individuals collapsed across the high and low-imageabilityconditions. Each set of factors was entered in the order depicted in the legend. The far left chart represents the results for the whole brain, while the other four represent the resultsfor the cortex of each of the four lobes. The number in parentheses is the average correlation between individuals for that brain region.

90 M.B. Miller et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

frontal and parietal lobes than in the temporal and occipital lobes.Another notable difference between the lobes was in the high-imageability condition, in which cognitive style differences (i.e.,differences in the general tendency of cognitive style regardless of theparticular set of words) had a stronger relationship to variability inthe parietal and temporal lobes than in the frontal and occipital lobes.

Most of the direct effects reported in Table 1 were in the expecteddirection. The direct effects represent the unique variance of each ofthe factors derived from the regression equation prior to entering theindividual residuals. Some of the notable relationships include thefollowing: the bigger the difference in age, the lower the correlationvalues (i.e., the more variable), particularly in the low-imageabilitycondition, while the difference in sex had no relationship tocorrelation values in either condition. As for anatomy, differences inwhite matter integrity as measured by the FA maps were a muchstronger factor than overall differences in anatomy as measured bythe high-resolution T1 MPRAGE. The more similar the white mattermaps, the more similar the functional maps for every lobe in bothconditions. This was clearly the strongest factor in every analysis.Differences in performance had little relationship to variability, exceptfor criterion differences in the temporal lobe in the high-imageabilitycondition.

The critical factors were in the set labeled cognitive style or the setlabeled strategy.Whilemost of the direct effects for these factors werein the predicted direction, a few factors were surprising. For example,three factors had significant effects in the direction opposite from theexpected one (positive betas): differences in verbal tendency in theoccipital lobe, differences in the open-ended query about strategy inthe low-imageability condition in the frontal and parietal lobe, anddifferences in the fourth strategy factor across several regions. In thesecases, the standardized beta was positive indicating that the biggerthe difference between the two individuals the more similar theirpatterns of brain activity. However, this may be due to the imbalancein the number of individuals within each of these categories. Forexample, in the low-imageability condition, most of the participantsutilized a verbal strategy. Only five individuals maintained a visual

strategy. Yet, for all five of these participants, the average correlationbetween them and the rest of the participants was higher than theoverall average, i.e., these five individuals happened to be more likethe group thanmost of the other individuals. Therefore, due to the lownumber of individuals and the characteristics of this group, thedifference in strategy factor is biased towards being more similar tothe group than maybe would be represented if we had a largersample.

Most of the significant factors in these sets were in the predicteddirection (negative betas). That is, the more two individuals weredifferent in their strategy and cognitive style, the more the individualswere different in their patterns of brain activity. One critical factorwasthe tendency to visualize, which was the main target in manipulatingthe imageability of the words. As shown in Table 1, the bigger thedifference in the tendency to visualize the lower the correlation value,but only in the high-imageability condition and not in the low-imageability condition as predicted. While, in general, cognitive stylefactors seemed to play a slightly larger role in the high-imageabilitycondition, strategy factors clearly played a larger role in the low-imageability condition and most notably in the frontal and parietalregions. Differences in the two strongest strategy factors (verbal andvisual strategies) were strongly related to variability in the low-imageability condition in the frontal and parietal lobes but not in thehigh-imageability condition. Interestingly, differences in the visualstrategy factor scores were significantly related to memory perfor-mance (d´) in the high imagery condition but not the low imagerycondition, while differences in the same factor scores were signifi-cantly related to similarity in brain activity in the low imagerycondition but not the high imagery condition.

Discussion

What makes the pattern of brain activity so similar for twoindividuals and, yet, so different for another two individuals? Wefound, as predicted, that individual differences in cognitive style andencoding strategy during a memory retrieval task were significant

Page 9: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

91M.B. Miller et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

factors in explaining this variability. For example, the more differenttwo individuals were in their tendency to visualize highly imageableword stimuli, the more different their two patterns of brain activitywere across the whole brain. Further, we found that the relationshipbetween differences in cognitive style and strategy and differences inthe similarity of brain activity was significantly affected by implicitmanipulations of strategy, in this case, manipulating the imageabilityof the word stimuli.

We defined strategy as a specific set of cognitive operations that anindividual utilized to better encode certain word stimuli, such asforming an image of the word in their mind or using the word in asentence. We found participants used a combination of operations orstrategies during a particular block of trials. Thus, certain strategiestended to group together, depending on the imageability condition andthat those groupings (factors) could be intuitively identified as visual,verbal, or otherwise (see Supplemental Material). Likewise, we definedcognitive style as a specific set of cognitive processes thatmaybe similarto the cognitive operations engaged by specific strategies, but wedefined them separately because they represent general processingtendencies by the individual that are not necessarily a direct or explicitstrategy, like the tendency to visualize written material. A number ofvisualizing–verbalizing test batteries were completed by the partici-pants and the results of those tests also tended to group together intointuitively identifiable factors, like visual tendency or verbal tendency.While the two sets of factors, cognitive style and strategy, may haveengaged similar processes, they accounted for uniqueproportions of thevariance in the similarity of brain activity. In the analysis of the wholebrain, both sets of factors together accounted for 8.0% of the variance inthe similarity of brain activity, while less than 0.3% of the variance wasshared between the two sets. Each set of factors was a significant andunique predictor of similarity in brain activity.

It should be noted that not just any difference between individualscould be related to the similarity in their patterns of brain activity. Forexample, whether or not the two individuals were the same sex hadno effect on their brain similarity. Also, the difference in age betweenthe two individuals had no relationship to similarity in brain activityin the high-imageability condition, but it did have a significantrelationship in the low-imageability condition. We also found thatdifferences in memory performance had little relationship todifferences in the similarity of brain activity. Even though differencesin memorability (d´) are known to modulate the activity in key brainregions (Nyberg et al., 1996; Tulving et al., 1999; Kirchhoff, 2009), itappears to have little relationship to differences in the topography ofbrain activity across large regions.

In our previous study we found that correlations of spatiallynormalized high-resolution anatomical images were not related tocorrelations of functional brain activity (Miller et al., 2009). Wesurmised, though, that finermeasures of anatomical differencesmighteventually prove to be related. In the current study, we found that thecorrelations of the high-resolution MPRAGE images were significantlyrelated to the correlations of functional brain activity across the wholebrain, but the effect was inconsistent across different cortical regions.It was significant in the occipital lobe, but not in the frontal, parietal ortemporal lobes.

Still, not all differences in anatomy were so weakly related todifferences in functional brain activity. Clearly one of the strongestfactors overall was the difference in white matter integrity asmeasured by fractional anisotropy. The effect was significant acrossevery region of the brain, but it was strongest in the temporal cortex(with standardized betas as high as .412). The study of individualdifferences in white matter connectivity has been of great interestacross neuroscience (Wolbers et al., 2006; Boorman et al., 2007; Niogiet al., 2010; Tomassini et al., 2011) and this finding is consistent withthat literature demonstrating the greater the difference in whitematter integrity between two individuals the bigger the difference intask-related functional brain activity. Although the degree of

similarity between individual DTI images seemed to have littlerelationship to differences in actual memory performance, it clearlyhad a significant impact on the similarity of brain activity underlyingmemory performance. This would suggest that similar memoryperformance on a recognition test could be associated with verydifferent patterns of brain activity, and that individual differences inwhite matter connectivity plays a major role in that variance.

The most striking factor, though, as depicted in Fig. 4, was theleftover amount of variability that could be attributed to specificindividuals (some individuals were more deviant in their pattern ofbrain activity than others). For example, in the whole-brain analysis,the full model accounted for 81% of the variance in the similarity ofbrain activity, but only 27% of the variance could be attributed tounique characteristics of the individuals that we were able to identify,with the differences in cognitive style and strategy being two of thosefactors. That means that there was another 54% of the variance thatwas attributable to specific individual traits but that we have not beenable to identify yet. This variability could conceivably be due todifferences in cognition but also due to differences in personality,physiology, state of mind, and/or other factors, such as caffeine intake.

Though a large amount of variance remains unexplained, it is clearthat differences in cognitive style and strategy are significant predictivefactors. In previous studies and reports on brain activity associatedwithmemory retrieval (Miller and Van Horn, 2007; Miller et al., 2009) wehave proposed that differences in strategy could account for some ofthat variability. Episodic memory retrieval engages widespread regionsthroughout the cortex, and includes cognitive processes that might beperipheral to the actual retrieval of episodic information (Shimamura,1995; Fletcher et al., 1998; Moscovitch and Winocur, 2002). A single“old” response on a recognition test could be accomplished alongmultiple processing routes through the brain, many of which maydepend on the strategy of the individual. The Encoding SpecificityPrinciple states that memory performance relies on the relationshipbetween processes engaged during retrieval and the processes engagedduring encoding (Tulving and Thomson, 1973). Previously, it has beenshown that differences in encoding processes can have a direct effect onthe brain activity that occurs during retrieval (Raposo et al., 2009;Kirchhoff, 2009). Indeed, we found that differences in encodingstrategies had a significant relationship to the similarity in brainactivity. This was particularly pronounced in the low-imageabilitycondition relative to the high-imageability condition,whichmay be dueto the increased demands on strategy forwords that do not easily evokeavisual image.Wealso found that strategydifferenceshad a larger effectin frontal and parietal regions than in temporal and occipital regions,which would be consistent with literature on memory strategies andthose brain regions (Gershberg and Shimamura, 1995; Davachi et al.,2001; Savage et al., 2001; Crescentini et al., 2010). For example, previousstudies with aging populations have suggested that as memoryperformance declines prefrontal and parietal cortex activations becomemore variable, suggesting thatolder individuals compensate for reducedmemory by engaging in more strategic processing (Cabeza et al., 2002;Spreng et al., 2010).

Individual differences in cognitive style go back as far as thework ofPaivio (1971, 1986), including a distinction between visualizers andverbalizers. Since that time, a great deal of effort has been put intoassessing and categorizing those differences (Mayer andMassa, 2003;Sternberg and Zhang, 2001; Zhang and Sternberg, 2009). Yet, thepractical effect of those distinctions on memory (Richardson, 1978,1998) and on other research-based interventions has been largelyabsent (Massa and Mayer, 2006; Pashler et al., 2009). However, ourresults suggest that those distinctions in cognitive style are notwithout consequences. Individual differences in cognitive style canhave a significant effect on the similarity of brain activity. In particular,we found that differences in visual style had a significant effect onsimilarity throughout the frontal, parietal and temporal lobes. Aspredicted, we found that this effect was evident for highly imageable

Page 10: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

92 M.B. Miller et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

words but not for lowly imageable words. This suggests thatknowledge about an individual's cognitive style may be necessarywhen evaluating the individual's pattern of brain activity, dependingon the type of stimuli.

There are many methods to probe individual differences in brainactivity, like correlating brain activity for a particular region withindividual differences in behavior or separating individuals intodistinct groups and comparing the activity between the two groups(for a brief review, see Miller and Van Horn, 2007). While thesemethods are useful and easy to visualize, they are examining correla-tions only along a single dimension. Our method explores multipledimensions by utilizing a multivariate regression analysis to predictthe correlation of functional t-statisticmaps (i.e., the similarity in brainactivity). We found that multiple factors predict similarity in brainactivity during a memory retrieval task, including differences in age,normalized white matter integrity, and cognitive style and strategy.

The multidimensional quality of individual differences in brainactivity could be a critical factor in achieving the goal of neuroergo-nomics to use neuroimaging to understand unique aspects theindividual mind in the work place. In the Introduction, we presentedan example in which the veracity of an individual's memory wasquestioned by the fact that the individual's pattern of brain activitywas different than a typical pattern of activity for a true memory.However, the difference may have been due to other factors, like theindividual's cognitive style or the strategy the individual used toretrieve the episode. In some extreme cases, the failure to consider themultiple factors that can drive differences in brain activity betweenindividuals can have life or death consequences. For example, a recentcourt case grappledwith the use of functional brain images as evidenceof a defendant's past mental state (Brown and Murphy, 2010). In thatcase, a neuroscientist testified on the brain images of convictedchild rapist and murderer Brian Dugan during the sentencing phase,arguing that the scans of Dugan were different enough from non-psychopath individuals and that this dissimilarity indicated thatDugan lacked the capacity to make normal moral decisions. Thisargument was intended to persuade jurors that Dugan's mental defectprecluded the use of the death penalty. However, the difference inactivity that was evident in Dugan's brain image may not be the resultof diminished moral reasoning, but could be attributed to otherpotential differences, such as the difference in white matter integrityor cognitive style or personality. Our study indicates that any inferencethat can be made about an individual based on their pattern of brainactivity must take into consideration the multiple factors that cancontribute to the variations in that activity.

Variability is often considered a nuisance, leaving the study ofindividual variability in brain activity to the dustbin of statisticalaveraging. But the systematic nature of variations in brain activitybetween individuals offers a unique opportunity to fulfill one of thegoals of neuroergonomics; to use neuroscience to understand uniqueaspects of the individual mind.

Acknowledgments

This work was funded by an Institute for Collaborative Bio-technologies grant to MBM through contract number W911NF-09-D-0001 from the U.S. Army Research Office.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.060.

References

Battig, W., 1975. Within-individual differences in ‘cognitive’ processes. In: Solso, R.L.(Ed.), Information processing and cognition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 195–228.

Bennett, C.M., Miller, M.B., 2010. How reliable are the results from functional magneticresonance imaging? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1191, 133–155.

Bennett, C.M., Baird, A.A., Miller, M.B., Wolford, G.L., 2010. Neural correlates ofinterspecies perspective taking in the post-mortem Atlantic Salmon: an argumentfor multiple comparisons correction. Journal of Serendipitous and UnexpectedResults 1 (1), 1–5.

Blajenkova, O., Kozhevnikov, M., Motes, M., 2006. Object–Spatial imagery: a new self-report imagery questionnaire. Applied Cognitive Psychology 20, 239–265.

Boorman, E.D., O'Shea, J., Sebastian, C., Rushworth, M.F., Johansen-Berg, H., 2007.Individual differences in white-matter microstructure reflect variation in func-tional connectivity during choice. Current Biology 17 (16), 1426–1431.

Brown, T., Murphy, E., 2010. Through a scanner darkly: functional neuroimaging asevidence of criminal defendant's past mental states. Stanford Law Review 62, 1119.

Byrnes, J.P., 2001. Minds, Brains, And Learning. Guilford, New York.Cabeza, R., Anderson, N.D., Locantore, J.K., McIntosh, A.R., 2002. Aging gracefully:

compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. NeuroImage 17,1394–1402.

Casasanto, D.J., Killgore, W.D.S., Maldjian, J.A., Glosser, G., Alsop, D.C., Cooke, A.M.,Grossman, M., Detre, J.A., 2002. Neural correlates of successful and unsuccessfulverbal memory encoding. Brain and Language 80, 287–295.

Coltheart, M., 1981. The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal ofExperimental Psychology Section A-Human Experimental Psychology 33, 497–505.

Crescentini, C., Shallice, T., Del Missier, F., Macaluso, E., 2010. Neural correlates ofepisodic retrieval: An fMRI study of the part-list cueing effect. NeuroImage 50,678–692.

Cronbach, L., Snow, R., 1977. Aptitudes and Instructional Methods: A Handbook forResearch on Interactions. Irvington Publishers, New York.

Davachi, L., Maril, A., Wagner, A., 2001. When keeping in mind supports later bringingto mind: neural markers of phonological rehearsal predict subsequent remember-ing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13, 1059–1070.

Ekstrom, R.B., French, J.W., Harman, H.H., 1976. Manual for kit of factor-referencedcognitive tests. Educational Testing Service, Princeton.

Faigman, D.L., 2010. Evidentiary incommensurability: a preliminary exploration of theproblem of reasoning from general scientific data to individualized legal decision-making. Brooklyn Law Review 75, 1137.

Feredoes, E., Postle, B.R., 2007. Localization of load sensitivity of working memorystorage: quantitatively and qualitatively discrepant results yielded by single-subject and group-averaged approaches to fMRI group analysis. NeuroImage 35,881–903.

Fletcher, P.C., Shallice, T., Frith, C.D., Frackowiak, R.S.J., Dolan, R.J., 1998. The functionalroles of prefrontal cortex in episodic memory: II. Retrieval. Brain 121, 1249–1256.

Friston, K.J., Holmes, A.P., Worsley, K.J., 1999. How many participants constitute astudy? NeuroImage 10, 1–5.

Gershberg, F., Shimamura, A., 1995. Impaired use of organizational strategies in freerecall following frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia 13, 1305–1333.

Graf, P., Birt, A.R., 1996. Explicit and implicit memory retrieval: Intentions andstrategies. In: Reder, L.M. (Ed.), Implicit Memory and Metacognition. Erlbaum,Mahwah, NJ, pp. 25–44.

Guerin, S.A., Miller, M.B., 2009. Lateralization of the parietal old/new effect: an event-related fMRI study comparing recognition memory for words and faces. Neuro-Image 44 (1), 232–242.

Heun, R., Jessen, F., Klose, U., Erb, M., Granath, D.O., Freymann, N., Grodd, W., 2000.Interindividual variation of cerebral activation during encoding and retrieval ofwords. European Psychiatry 15, 470–479.

Hogan, T.P., Cannon, B., 2007. Psychological Testing: A Practical Introduction. JohnWiley & Sons, Hoboken (NJ).

Kirchhoff, B.A., 2009. Individual differences in episodic memory: the role of self-initiated encoding strategies. The Neuroscientist 15 (2), 166–180.

Kirchhoff, B.A., Buckner, R.L., 2006. Functional-anatomic correlates of individualdifferences in memory. Neuron 51, 263–274.

Kondo, Y., Suzuki, M., Mugikura, S., Abe, N., Takahashi, S., Iijima, T., Fujii, T., 2005.Changes in brain activation associated with use of a memory strategy: a functionalMRI study. NeuroImage 24, 1154–1163.

Kozhevnikov, M., Hegarty, M., Mayer, R.E., 2002. Revising the visualizer–verbalizerdimension: evidence for two types of visualizers. Cognition and Instruction 20,47–77.

Kozhevnikov, M., Kosslyn, S.M., Shephard, J., 2005. Spatial versus object visualizers: anew characterization of visual cognitive style. Memory & Cognition 33, 710–726.

Machielsen, W.C.M., Rombouts, S.A.R.B., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Witter, M.P., 2000.FMRI of visual encoding: reproducibility of activation. Human Brain Mapping 9,156–164.

Maldjian, J.A., Laurienti, P.J., Burdette, J.B., Kraft, R.A., 2003. An automated method forneuroanatomic and cytoarchitectomic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets.NeuroImage 19, 1233–1239.

Massa, L.J., Mayer, R.E., 2006. Testing the ATI hypothesis: shouldmultimedia instructionaccommodate verbalizer–visualizer cognitive style. Learning and IndividualDifferences 16, 321–335.

Mayer, R.E., Massa, L.J., 2003. Three facets of visual and verbal learners: cognitive ability,cognitive style, and learning preference. Journal of Educational Psychology 95,833–846.

McGonigle, D.J., Howseman, A.M., Athwal, B.S., Friston, K.J., Frackowiak, R.S.J., Holmes,A.P., 2000. Variability in fMRI: an examination of intersession differences.NeuroImage 11, 708–734.

Miller, M.B., Van Horn, J.D., 2007. Individual variability in brain activations associatedwith episodic retrieval: a role for large-scale databases. International Journal ofPsychophysiology 63 (2), 205–213.

Page 11: Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy …...Review Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between

93M.B. Miller et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 83–93

Miller, M.B., Van Horn, J., Wolford, G.L., Handy, T.C., Valsangkar-Smyth, M., Inati, S.,Grafton, S., Gazzaniga, M.S., 2002. Extensive individual differences in brainactivations during episodic retrieval are reliable over time. Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 14, 1200–1214.

Miller, M.B., Donovan, C.L., Sokol-Hessner, P., German, E., Van Horn, J.D., Wolford, G.L.,2009. Unique and persistent individual patterns of brain activity across differentmemory retrieval tasks. NeuroImage 48, 625–635.

Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., 2002. The frontal cortex and working with memory. In:Stuss, D.T., Knight's, R.T. (Eds.), The Frontal Lobes. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Motes, M., Kozhevnikov, M., 2006. Cognitive style and individual differences inimagery: perspectives from neuroscience [Technical report]. Office of NavalResearch, Arlington, VA.

National Research Council, 2008. Report of the NRC Committee on Opportunities inNeuroscience for Future Army Applications. The National Academies Press,Washington, DC.

Niogi, S.N., Mukherjee, P., Ghajar, J., McCandliss, B.D., 2010. Individual differences indistinct components of attention are linked to anatomical variations in distinctwhite matter tracts. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy 4 (1), 1–12.

Nyberg, L., McIntosh, A.R., Houle, S., Nilsson, L.G., Tulving, E., 1996. Activation of medialtemporal structures during episodic memory retrieval. Nature 380, 715–717.

Ollinger, J.M., Shulman, G.L., Corbetta, M., 2001. Separating processes within a trial inevent-related functional MRI. NeuroImage 13, 210–217.

Paivio, A., 1971. Imagery and Verbal Processes. Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, New York.Paivio, A., 1983. The empirical case for dual coding. In: Yuille, J.C. (Ed.), Imagery,

Memory, and Cognition: Essays in Honor of Allan Paivio. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ,pp. 39–63.

Paivio, A., 1986. Mental Representations: A Dual Coding Approach. Oxford UniversityPress, New York.

Parasuraman, R., Jiang, Y., 2012. Individual differences in cognition, affect, andperformance: behaivioral, neuroimaging, and molecular genetic approaches.Neuroimage 59, 70–82 (this issue).

Parasuraman, R., Wilson, G.F., 2008. Putting the brain to work: neuroergonomics past,present, and future. Human Factors 50, 468–474.

Pashler, H., McDaniel, M., Rohrer, D., Bjork, R., 2009. Learning styles: concepts andevidence. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 9, 105–119.

Posner, M.I., Rothbart, M.K., 2005. Influencing brain networks: implications foreducation. Trends in Cognitive Science 9 (3), 99–103.

Raposo, A., Han, S., Dobbins, I.G., 2009. Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and self-initiatedsemantic elaboration during memory retrieval. NeuroImage 47, 2261–2271.

Reder, L.M., 1987. Beyond associations: strategic components in memory retrieval. In:Gorfein, D.S., Hoffman, R.R. (Eds.), Memory and learning: The Ebbinghaus CentennialConference. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Hillsdale, NJ., pp. 203–220.

Richardson, A., 1977. Verbalizer–visualizer: a cognitive style dimension. Journal ofMental Imagery 1, 109–126.

Richardson, J.T.E., 1978. Reported mediators and individual differences in mentalimagery. Memory and Cognition 6, 376–378.

Richardson, J.T.E., 1998. The availability and effectiveness of reported mediators inassociative learning: a historical review and an experimental investigation.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 5, 597–614.

Savage, C.R., Deckersbach, T., Heckers, S., Wagner, A.D., Schacter, D.L., Alpert, N.M.,Fischman, A.J., Rauch, S.L., 2001. Prefrontal regions supporting spontaneous anddirected application of verbal learning strategies: evidence from PET. Brain 124,219–231.

Seghier, M.L., Price, C.J., 2009. Dissociating functional brain networks by decoding thebetween-subject variability. NeuroImage 45, 349–359.

Seghier, M.L., Lazeyras, F., Pegna, A.J., Annoni, J.-M., Khateb, A., 2008. Group analysis andthe subject factor in functional magnetic resonance imaging: analysis of fiftyrighthanded healthy subjects in a semantic language task. Human Brain Mapping29, 461–477.

Shimamura, A.P., 1995. Memory and frontal lobe function. In: Gazzaniga, M.S., et al.(Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 803–813.

Speer, N., Jacoby, L., Braver, T., 2003. Strategy-dependent changes in memory: effectson behavior and brain activity. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience 3,155–167.

Spreng, R.N., Wojtowicz, M., Grady, C.L., 2010. Reliable differences in brain activitybetween young and old adults: a quantitative meta-analysis across multiplecognitive domains. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral ReviewsI 34, 1178–1194.

Sternberg, R.J., Zhang, L. (Eds.), 2001. Perspectives on Thinking, Learning, and CognitiveStyles. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.

Stoff, D.M., Eagle, M.N., 1971. The relationship among reported strategies, presentationrate, and verbal ability and their effects on free recall learning. Journal ofExperimental Psychology 87, 423–428.

Tomassini, V., Jbabdi, S., Kincses, Z.T., Bosnell, R., Douaud, G., Pozzilli, C., Matthews, P.M.,Johansen-Berg, H., 2011. Structural and functional bases for individual differencesin motor learning. Human Brain Mapping 32 (3), 494–508.

Tsukiura, T., Mochizuki-Kawai, H., Fujii, T., 2005. The effect of encoding strategies onmedial temporal lobe activations during the recognition of words: An event-relatedfMRI study. NeuroImage 25, 452–461.

Tulving, E., Thomson, D.M., 1973. Encoding specificity and retrieval processes inepisodic memory. Psychological Review 80, 352–373.

Tulving, E., Habib, R., Nyberg, L., Lepage, M., McIntosh, A.R., 1999. Positron emissiontomography correlations in and beyond medial temporal lobes. Hippocampus 9,71–82.

Weinstein, C.E., Underwood, V.L., Wicker, F.W., Cubberly, W.E., 1979. Cognitivelearning strategies: verbal and imaginal elaboration. In: O'Neil, H.F., Spielberger,C.D. (Eds.), Cognitive and Affective Learning Strategies. Academic Press, New York,pp. 45–75.

Wolbers, T., Schoell, E.D., Büchel, C., 2006. The predictive value of white matterorganization in posterior parietal cortex for spatial visualization ability. Neuro-Image 32 (3), 1450–1455.

Zhang, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds). (2009). Perspectives on the Nature of IntellectualStyles. New York: Springer.