impulsive choices in mice lacking imprinted nesp55livrepository.liverpool.ac.uk/3003440/1/dent cl...

32
1 Impulsive choices in mice lacking imprinted Nesp55 Dent CL a , Humby T b , Lewis K a , Plagge A c , Fischer-Colbrie R d , Wilkins JF e , Wilkinson LS a,b & Isles AR a* a Behavioural Genetics Group, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Neuroscience and Mental Health Research Institute, Cardiff University, Hadyn Ellis Building, Maindy Road, Cardiff, United Kingdom b Behavioural Genetics Group, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Tower Building, Cardiff, United Kingdom c Department of Cellular and Molecular Physiology, Institute of Translational Medicine, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom d Department of Pharmacology, Innsbruck Medical University, Innsbruck, Austria e Ronin Institute, Montclair, NJ 07043, USA *Author for correspondence: Anthony R. Isles, E-mail [email protected]

Upload: others

Post on 21-Oct-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 1

    ImpulsivechoicesinmicelackingimprintedNesp55Dent CLa, Humby Tb, Lewis Ka, Plagge Ac, Fischer-Colbrie Rd, Wilkins JFe,

    WilkinsonLSa,b&IslesARa*

    aBehavioural Genetics Group, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and

    Genomics,NeuroscienceandMentalHealthResearchInstitute,CardiffUniversity,

    HadynEllisBuilding,MaindyRoad,Cardiff,UnitedKingdombBehavioural Genetics Group, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Tower

    Building,Cardiff,UnitedKingdomcDepartment of Cellular and Molecular Physiology, Institute of Translational

    Medicine,UniversityofLiverpool,Liverpool,UnitedKingdomdDepartmentofPharmacology,InnsbruckMedicalUniversity,Innsbruck,AustriaeRoninInstitute,Montclair,NJ07043,USA

    *Authorforcorrespondence:AnthonyR.Isles,[email protected]

  • 2

    ABSTRACT

    Genomic imprinting is theprocesswherebygermlineepigenetic events lead to

    parent-of-origin specificmonallelic expression of a number of keymammalian

    genes.The imprintedgeneNespisexpressed fromthematernalalleleonlyand

    encodes for Nesp55 protein. In the brain Nesp55 is found predominately in

    discrete areas of the hypothalamus andmidbrain. Previously, we have shown

    thatlossofNesp55givesrisetoalterationsinnovelty-relatedbehavior.Herewe

    extendthesefindingsanddemonstrate,usingtheNespm/+mousemodel,thatloss

    ofNesp55 leads to impulsive choices asmeasuredby a delayed-reinforcement

    task,wherebyNespm/+micewerelesswillingtowaitforadelayed,largerreward,

    preferring instead to choosean immediate, smaller reward.Theseeffectswere

    highlyspecificasperformanceinanothercomponentofimpulsivebehavior,the

    abilitytostoparesponseoncestartedasassayedinthestop-signalreactiontime

    task, was equivalent to controls. We also showed changes in the serotonin

    system,akeyneurotransmitterpathwaymediatingimpulsivebehavior.First,we

    demonstrated that Nesp55 is co-localisedwith serotonin and thenwent on to

    showthatinmidbrainregionstherewerereductionsinmRNAexpressionofthe

    serotoninspecificgenesTph2andSlc6a4,butnotthedopaminespecificgeneTh

    inNespm/+mice;suggestinganalteredserotonergicsystemcouldcontribute, in

    part,tothechangesinimpulsivebehavior.Thesedataprovideanovelmodeof

    action for genomic imprinting in the brain and may have implications for

    pathologicalconditionscharacterizedbymaladaptiveresponsecontrol.

  • 3

    INTRODUCTION

    Genomicimprintingistheprocessbywhichsomegenesaremarkedinaparent

    oforiginspecificmannerasaconsequenceofepigeneticeventsthattakeplacein

    the mammalian germ line (Ferguson-Smith, 2011). For canonical imprinted

    genes, in the somatic cell lineages this epigeneticmarking leads tomonoallelic

    expressionfromoneparentalalleleonly.Forsomeimprintedgenes,expression

    is solely from thematernal allele, whilst others are solely expressed from the

    paternalallele.

    There are approximately 150 canonical imprinted genes andnon-codingRNAs

    known in the mouse, with similar numbers in humans, although recent next-

    generationsequencingexperimentshaveexpanded thisnumber (Wilkinsetal.,

    2016). Though relatively small in number, imprinted genes are critical for

    normaldevelopmenttotakeplace(Mcgrath&Solter,1984,Suranietal.,1984).

    Furthermore, functionally, imprinted genes converge on specific biological

    processesthathaveprominentimportanceinmammals,suchasinuterogrowth,

    metabolismandbehaviour(Peters,2014,Wilkinsetal.,2016).

    The imprinted gene Nesp, encoding Nesp55, is part of the complex GNAS

    imprintingclusterandisexpressedexclusivelyfromthematernalallele(Peters

    etal., 1999).Nesp is expressed indiscrete areasof themidbrain, including the

    Edinger-Westphalnucleus,dorsalRaphénucleus(DRN),andthelocuscoeruleus

    (LC), and also in the hypothalamus (Plagge et al., 2005). Our previous work

    demonstrated thatmice null formaternally expressedNesp55 show increased

    activitywhenplacedinnovelenvironment,butareducedpropensitytoexplorea

    novelenvironmentwhengiven thechoice (Plaggeetal.,2005).Alterednovelty

    exploration may involve changes in the balance between self-control and

    impulsive responding (Flagel et al., 2010, Stoffel & Cunningham, 2008).

    Furthermore,Nespisexpressedinkeybrainstructures,namelytheDRNandLC,

    thatareimplicatedinmediatingresponsecontrol(Bari&Robbins,2013)andthe

    ability towait for a reward (Fonseca etal., 2015), respectively.Apriori, these

    datasuggestpossiblelinksbetweenNesp55andimpulsivebehavior.

    The term ‘impulsivity’, defined as actionwith limited forethought, is a natural

    part of behavior. However, in certain individuals, impulsive behavior becomes

  • 4

    pathological and maladaptive and can manifest in psychiatric disease such as

    schizophrenia and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), as well as

    addictivedisorderssuchaspathologicalgamblinganddrugaddiction.Agrowing

    bodyofevidencesuggeststhatimpulsivityisnotunitary,andinsteadisamulti-

    facetedconstructdissociableintermsofbehaviorandunderlyingneurobiology

    (Bari & Robbins, 2013). There have been attempts to segregate impulsive

    behaviorintotwoseparatecategories,impulsivechoiceandimpulsiveaction

    (Broos et al., 2012, Dent & Isles, 2014b). Impulsive choice often manifests as

    impulsive decisions resulting from a distorted evaluation of the delayed

    consequences of behavior and an increased preference for smaller immediate

    rewards over larger delayed rewards; on the other hand, impulsive action

    reflectsthemotoricaspectofimpulsivity,andtypicallymanifestsinpoorability

    to override a pre-potent response (Humby & Wilkinson, 2011). A number of

    behavioral tasks exist to tease apart these discrete aspects of impulsivity

    (Humby&Wilkinson,2011),manyofwhicharereadilyavailableinmice(Dent&

    Isles,2014b).

    Hereweusedadelayed-reinforcementtask(Islesetal.,2003)andastop-signal

    reaction time (SSRT) task (Humby etal., 2013), to assay impulsive choice and

    impulsive action, respectively, in mice null for maternally expressed Nesp55

    (Nespm/+mice).WefoundthatNespm/+miceshowedamarkedincreaseinchoice

    impulsivity,manifestindecisionstochooseanimmediate,smallerrewardovera

    largerbutdelayedreward,butexhibitednosignificantdifferencesfromcontrol

    mice (Nesp+/p) in impulsive action,where subjectshad to suppress anongoing

    pre-potentmotorresponse.WealsodemonstratedthatNesp55co-localizeswith

    5HT inmidbrain regions, and furtherdemonstrated that thebehavioral effects

    observed in the Nespm/+ mice were accompanied by selective effects on the

    expression of key serotonergic genes. The data are consistent with a role for

    Nesp55 in mediating specific aspects of impulsive behavior, possibly via

    interactionswithmidbrainserotonergicpathways.

  • 5

    MATERIALSANDMETHODS

    Animals

    AllprocedureswereconductedinaccordancewiththerequirementsoftheUK

    Animals(ScientificProcedures)Act1986,undertheremitofHomeOfficelicence

    number30/2673withadditionalethicalapprovalatCardiffUniversity.

    As described previously (Plagge et al., 2005), expression of Nesp55 was

    eliminatedbyadeletionof26bpatthestartcodonoftheORF(bp2362to2387

    in AJ251761). Mice carrying a maternally derived null allele (Nespm/+) lacked

    Nesp55 expression, whereas those carrying a paternally derived null allele

    Nesp+/p have the same level of expression as wild-type mice (Figure S1). As

    before (Plagge et al., 2005), in the present study Nespm/+ were compared to

    Nesp+/p mice as themost appropriate control (see Supporting Information for

    moredetails)andthesearereferredtoascontrolmicethroughout.

    TheNesp55nulllinewasmaintainedonaC57Bl/6J(CharlesRiverLaboratories,

    U.K.) background with separate cohorts of Nesp55 and control mice being

    utilizedinthetwobehavioraltasks.Subjectsweremalemiceagedbetween4-10

    monthsduringtesting;thelargeagerangebeingduetothetimetakentolearn

    and perform the operant tasks. Standard laboratory chow was available ad

    libitum,but justpriortoandduringtheexperiment,waterwasrestrictedto2h

    access per day. This regime maintained the subjects at ≈90% of free-feeding

    bodyweight.Allbehaviouraltestingwasperformedinthelightphase(lightson

    07:00,for12-hours).

    Behavioraltasks

    Apparatus

    Thedelayed-reinforcementtaskandstop-signalreactiontime(SSRT)taskwere

    performedin9-holeoperantchambers(CambridgeCognitionLtd,UK)modified

    foruse inmice,aspreviouslydescribed(Humbyetal.,2013, Islesetal.,2003).

    For the delayed-reinforcement task holes 3, 5 and 7were open,whereas only

    holes4and6wereopenfortheSSRTtask.Themicewerepresentedwithvisual

    stimuli (lights) recessed into the holes and were trained to respond to this

  • 6

    stimulus with a nose-poke recorded by infra-red beams spanning the hole.

    Rewardwaspresentedinarecessedcompartmentconcealedbyapanel,onthe

    wall opposite to thenose-poke/stimulus array; animalswere required topush

    the panel open in order to consume the reward. The control of stimuli and

    recording of the responses weremanaged by an Acorn Archimedes computer

    withadditionalinterfacingbyARACHNID(CambridgeCognitionLtd,UK).

    Delayedreinforcementtask

    Detailsoftheshapingproceduresandbasicaspectsofthedelayed-reinforcement

    taskitselfcanbefoundelsewhere(Islesetal.,2003).Briefly,thetaskcomprised

    ofthreesequentialblocksof12trials,witheachtrialconsistingofaninitialnose-

    poketothecentrallylocatedstimulus,followedbyasecondnose-poketoeither

    theleftorrightapertures.Trials1-4inanyblockwere‘forced’informationtrials,

    wheretheinitialnose-pokeresultedinpresentationofonlyoneofthetwochoice

    options.Thismeasurewasdesignedtoprovidethesubjectswithpriornoticeof

    the extent of any delay associated with choosing the large reward. In the

    remaining 8 trials of each block, designated as ‘choice’ trials, the initial centre

    nose-pokeledtotheoptionofasecondnose-pokeresponsetoeithertheleftor

    rightapertures. Oneresponseresulted inthedeliveryofa largereward(50μL

    10%solutionofcondensedmilk;NestleLtd,UK),andtheotherinthedeliveryof

    small reward (25μL 10% solution of condensed milk). The response

    contingencies were kept constant for each mouse, but were counterbalanced

    betweensubjects.Inblock1bothresponsesledtothedeliveryofrewardaftera

    1s delay. In blocks 2 and 3 increasing delays were introduced between the

    responseandthedeliveryofthelargereward(8s,16s,respectively)whereasthe

    delaybetweenresponseanddeliveryofthesmallrewardwasfixedat1s. Asa

    probe to test the effect of the delays on behaviour, sessions were conducted

    wherethedelayassociatedwiththelargerewardwasfixedat1s,equivalentto

    thatassociatedwiththesmallreward,throughoutallthreeblocksofthesession.

    Thebiasinchoiceofthelargerrewardateachblock(wherebyalwayschoosing

    thelargereward=1;neverchoosingthelargereward=0)wasthemainmeasure

    usedtodetermineimpulsiveresponding.Additionalmeasurementsthatrelated

    to general motoric competence and motivation within the task were also

  • 7

    monitored,includingthe‘Start’and‘Choice’latencies,thetimetakentoinitiatea

    trialandthetimetakentomakeachoiceonceatrialwasinitiated,respectively.

    Alsomeasuredwerethenumberof ‘Non-started’(no initial,centralnose-poke)

    and ‘Omitted’ (no secondary, choice nose-poke, following central nose-poke

    initiatingtrial)trials.

    For the delayed reinforcement task experimental groups were Nespm/+=11,

    controls=7.Animalsweretested6-daysperweek.

    Stop-signalreactiontime(SSRT)task

    Detailsof the shapingproceduresandbasic aspectsof themainSSRT task can

    alsobefoundelsewhere(Humbyetal.,2013).TheSSRTtask itselfconsistedof

    sessions of 100 trials, which involved both ‘go’ and ‘stop’ trials. Go trials

    consisted of rapid double nose-pokes (a ‘go’ response) between two separate

    stimulilocations,whichwererewardedwithreinforcement(22ul,10%solution

    of condensed milk, Nestle Ltd, UK). 20% of trials were stop trials, pseudo-

    randomlydistributedthroughouteachsession,whereastop-signal(65dbwhite

    noisefor0.3s)waspresentedbetweenthefirstandsecondnose-pokeresponses.

    The aim of the stop-signal was to inhibit (‘Stop’) themouse frommaking the

    second (‘Go’) nose-poke, and thenwait for the reward. Failure to refrain from

    makingthispre-potentresponsewaspunishedbytheabsenceofrewardand5

    secondtimeout(chamberlighton).Atbaseline,thestop-signalswerepresented

    concurrently with the initial nose-poke response. To maintain high levels of

    performanceofbothgoandstopresponding,thegostimulusdurationandwait

    periodtorewarddeliveryinastop-signaltrialweredeterminedindividuallyfor

    each subject. To assess the ability to stop once an action had been initiated,

    sessionswereimplementedinwhichtheonsetofthestop-signalwaspresented

    atdifferentpositionswithintheindividualisedgoresponseofeachmouse.Thus,

    thestop-signalwaspseudo-randomlypresented10,40,50,60and90%fromthe

    onset of the go response of each subject, with the assumption that stopping

    would be more difficult the closer the stop-signal presentation was to the

    terminationofthegoresponse.

    Theamountofcorrectstoppinginstop-signaltrialsandtheSSRTwerethemain

    measures of impulsive responding in this task. The SSRT was calculated by

  • 8

    determiningthe50%stoppingabilityforeachsubjectfromtherangeofsessions

    in which the stop-signal onset was varied from baseline (full details of this

    calculationcanbefoundintheSupportingInformationand(Humbyetal.,2013).

    The proportion of correct go-responses, and latency to respond, were also

    assessed.Additionalmeasurementsthatrelatedtogeneralmotoriccompetence

    andmotivationwithin the taskwerealsomonitored, including the “intitiation”

    and“magazine”latencies,thetimetakentoinitiateatrialandthetimetakento

    collect the reward.Alsomeasuredwas thenumberof trials completed for any

    givensession.

    For theSSRTtaskexperimentalgroupswereNespm/+=17,controls=12.Animals

    wererun6-daysperweek.

    Immunohistochemistry

    Mice were terminally anaesthetised using an intraperitoneal injection of

    pentobarbitone and then trans-cardially perfused using 4%PFA in PBS. Brains

    weredissectedwholeandplacedin4%PFAovernight,thentransferredtoa30%

    sucrose solution (in PBS) at 4˚c for 24 hours in order to dehydrate and

    cryoprotectthem.Brainswerethenmountedontoamicrotomeplatformusing

    anembeddingmatrixandallowedto freeze fully.Serial sectionsof40µmwere

    slicedandputinto25-wellcontainerscontainingcryoprotectant(6sectionsper

    well). The free-floating sections in cryoprotectant were stored at -20°C until

    required.

    Dual-labellingimmunofluorescenceofNespwith5HTwascarriedoutinNesp+/+

    (i.e.wild-type) free-floating brain sections in order to localize the endogenous

    protein. The Nesp55 primary antibody is a well-characterised anti-body

    (generatedandobtainedfromtheReinerFischer-ColbrieLab).Specifically,itisa

    rabbit anti-NESP55 polycolonal anti-body, recognizing the free terminal end

    (GAIPIRRH) ofNESP55 (Ischia etal., 1997). Sectionswerewashed three times

    for10mineachin0.1%PBSbeforebeingincubatedfor15minin0.3Mglycine

    in 0.1% PBS at room temperature, to neutralise endogenous aldehyde groups.

    Sectionswerewashedin0.1%PBSandthenincubatedatroomtemperaturefor

    1hourin10%blockingsolution;0.5%BSA(BBInternational,Cardiff,UK),0.5%

  • 9

    TritonX-100(v/v,SigmaAldrich)in0.1%PBS.Sectionswerethentransferredto

    a solution containing 1:1000 anti-NESP55 and 1:500 anti-serotonin (Abcam)

    dilutedina1%blockingsolution;thiswasallowedtoincubatedovernightat4°C

    whilst gently shaking. The next day sections were washed three times for 10

    minutes in 0.1% PBS. The relevant fluorescent secondary antibodies (Alexa

    Fluor;Lifetechnologies)werediluted1/1000in1%blockingsolution,sections

    were incubated in this solution in the dark at room temperature for 2 hours,

    whilstgentlyshaking.Sectionswerethenwashedin0.1%PBSasbefore(inthe

    dark)andtransferredtopolysinecoatedslidesandallowedtodryover-nightin

    a dark dust-free environment. The mounted slides were then dehydrated

    throughaprocessofincubationinarisingconcentrationofalcohol,followedby

    xylene, then cover-slipped and sealed using DPX (Raymond Lamb DPX), and

    allowed todryover-night.Tocontrol fornon-specificbindingof thesecondary

    antibodies, secondary-only negative controls were carried out alongside all

    experiments.

    Immunofluorescenceslideswereviewedand imagescapturedusinganupright

    fluorescencemicroscope (LeicaDM5000B).Dual-labelled immunofluorescence

    images were acquired through separate channels then subsequently merged

    usingImageJ(Image>colour>mergechannels).

    QuantitativePCR

    RNA from macro-dissected brain regions was isolated using standard Trizol

    methods. Equal amounts ofRNAwere reverse transcribedusingRNA to cDNA

    EcoDry (double primed) premix strips (Takara Bio Europe, France). Gene

    expressionwasassessedusingaRotorgene6000withaCAS1200automatedset

    up (Corbett Research, U.K.). PCR reactions were carried out using custom

    designed primers and Quantace SensiMix NoRef (Bioline). The genes assayed

    were Th (For 5ʹ-AGGAGAGGGATGGAAATGCT-3ʹ; Rev

    5ʹ-GCGCACAAAGTACTCCAGGT-3ʹ), Tph2 (For 5ʹ-CTGCTGTGCCAGAAGATCATCA-

    3ʹ; Rev 5ʹ-TGCTGCTCTCTGTGGTGTCG-3ʹ) and Slc6a4 (For 5ʹ-

    TTGTGCTCATCGTGGTCATC-3ʹ; Rev 5ʹ-GTGGCGTACTCCTCCAGCAG-3ʹ).

    Additionally, three housekeeping genes were assessed in order to provide a

  • 10

    robust measurement of general gene activity. These Gapdh (For 5ʹ-

    GAACATCATCCCTGCATCCA-3ʹ; Rev 5ʹ-CCAGTGAGCTTCCCGTTCA-3ʹ), Hprt (For

    5ʹ-TTGCTCGAGATGTCATGAAGGA-3ʹ; Rev 5ʹ-AATGTAATCCAGCAGGTCAGCAA-3ʹ)

    and β-actin (For 5ʹ-TCTGTGTGGATTGGTGGCTCTA-3ʹ; Rev

    5ʹ-CTGCTTGCTGATCCACATCTG-3ʹ).ΔCtvaluesweregeneratedbynormalisingto

    thegeometricmeanofthesethreehousekeepinggenes.Allindividualreactions

    werecarriedoutintriplicate.RealtimeqPCRdatawasvisualisedusingtheΔΔCt

    method(Livak&Schmittgen,2001).

    Dataanalysisandstatistics

    AllbehavioraldatawereanalysedusingSPSS20(SPSS,USA).Datawereassessed

    fornormalityandthenanalysedbyStudent’st-testormixedANOVA,with

    between-subjectsfactorsofGENOTYPE(Nespm/+vs.Control),andwithin-subject

    factorsDELAY(1s,8s,16s,or1s,1s,1s),CHOICE(choiceoflargeorsmallreward

    duringforcedtrialsofthedelayedreinforcementtask),andSTOP-SIGNAL

    POSITION(positionofstop-signalrelativetoindividualizedGo-response).For

    repeated-measuresanalyses,Mauchly’stestofsphericityofthecovariance

    matrixwasapplied;significantviolationsfromtheassumptionofsphericitywere

    subjecttotheHuynh–Feldtcorrectiontoallowmoreconservativecomparisons

    throughadjustdegreesoffreedom.Duetothenon-parametricnatureofthe

    qPCRdata,ΔCtvalueswereanalysedbyMann-WhitneyUtest.Allsignificance

    testswereperformedatalphalevelof0.05.

    RESULTS

    Nespm/+micearemoreimpulsiveinthedelayedreinforcementtask

    In thedelayed-reinforcement task, subjectsmadea choicebetweenreceivinga

    smallfoodrewardafterashort(1s)delay,oralargerewardafteralongerdelay

    (1,8or16s). Inthisway,theextenttowhichsubjectsmadeimpulsivechoices,

    indexedbythepreference forchoosingthe immediatebutsmallrewardovera

    delayedlargereward,wasdetermined.Increasingthedelaytothelargereward

    within a session decreased the likelihood of choosing that reward for both

    Nespm/+andcontrolmice(Figure1a,maineffectofDELAY,F2,32=3.84,P=0.032).

  • 11

    However,mutantandcontrolmiceshowedsignificantdifferences in theextent

    to which they switched their preference to the small, less delayed reward in

    responsetoincreaseddelaytothelargereward.Specifically,Nespm/+micechose

    the small reward more readily than control mice (Figure 1a, main effect of

    GENOTYPE, F1,16=6.74,P=0.019), indicative of increased impulsive responding.

    To confirm the specificity of the contingency between reward value and the

    delay in reward delivery, performance was also assessed where the cost

    associatedwith the large rewardwas removed, thus thedelay associatedwith

    the largeand small rewardswasequal (1s) for all trials. In these sessions, all

    subjectsdemonstratedanequalhighpreferenceforthelargerewardthroughout

    thesession(Figure1b,nomaineffectofDELAY,F2,32=0.478,P=0.624)andthere

    were no differences between Nespm/+ and control mice (no main effect of

    GENOTYPE,F1,16=0.010,P=0.927).

    WealsoconfirmedthatthecontrastingpatternofchoicebehaviorintheNespm/+

    mice were not due to any differences between the groups in terms of basic

    learning andmotivation to carry out the task, asNespm/+animals acquired the

    taskatthesamerateascontrolmice(numberofsessionstolastdayofbaseline,

    grandmean=40.3±1.3SEM,t16=1.00,P=0.32).Additionally,variabilitybetween

    Nespm/+ and control mice was not related to differences in experiencing the

    information trial contingencies. Thus, in the ‘forced’ trials (inwhich no choice

    wasavailable)bothNespm/+andcontrolmicemadeequalresponsestothelarge

    andsmallreward-relatedstimuliatalldelays(interactionbetweenGENOTYPEx

    DELAY x CHOICE, F2,32=1.85, P=0.43; data not shown). Finally, there was no

    difference between Nespm/+ and control mice on general measures of task

    performance (see Figure S2). As expected, Nespm/+ and control mice

    demonstratedageneralmaineffectofDELAYonmanyofthesemeasures(Figure

    S2a-d)as follows: increasedstart (F1.7,27.5=22.06,P

  • 12

    Nespm/+mice show no differences in impulsive responding in the stop-

    signalreactiontimetask

    TheSSRTtaskmeasurestheabilitytostopanactiononceinitiatedbypresenting

    a stop-signal (in this case an auditory tone)during a rapid transitionbetween

    two stimuli locations (the ‘go’ response). Correctly inhibiting the go response

    earnedrewardina‘stop’trial,andthedifficultyofstoppingwasmanipulatedby

    presenting the stop-signal at different times within the go response. Thus

    stoppingwas easiestwhen the stop-signal onsetwas near the start andmore

    difficultwhenat theendof thego response.Throughout the training stagesof

    the SSRT task, all subjects showed equivalent behavior in learning the task.

    Nespm/+ mice acquired the task at the same rate as control animals, as

    demonstrated by the similar number of sessions taken to complete the task

    (Nespm/+:44.3±6.6,control:46.6±6.8).Theeffectsofalteringthepositionofthe

    auditory stop-signal during stop trials on stopping efficiency are illustrated in

    Figure2a.InlinewithpreviousSSRTtaskexperimentsinhumansandavariety

    of other mammalian species, including mice (Bari et al., 2009, Humby et al.,

    2013),stoppingbecameincreasinglydifficultasthestop-signalpresentationwas

    movedprogressivelyclosertotheexecutionoftheresponse(10,40,50,60and

    90%intotheindividualisedgoresponse)leadingtosystematicreductionsinthe

    abilitytostopforbothNespm/+andcontrolmice(Figure2a,maineffectofSTOP-

    SIGNALPOSITION,F4,108=43.28,P

  • 13

    stop-signal position was moved from baseline (main effect of STOP-SIGNAL

    POSITION,F4,108=0.49,P=0.74andF4,108=1.67,P=0.16,fortheamountandspeed

    ofcorrectrespondingingotrials,respectively).

    Again, Nespm/+ animals acquired the task at the same rate as control mice

    (numberofsessions,grandmean=45.3±1.2SEM,t27=0.90,P=0.38).Additionally,

    therewasnodifferencebetweenNespm/+andcontrolmiceongeneralmeasures

    of task performance during individualized SSRT session). Thus, the number of

    trialscompleted(t27=0.10,P=0.92), latencyto initiatea trial(t27=-0.36,P=0.73)

    and magazine latency (t27=-1.12, P=0.27) measures were not significantly

    different between Nespm/+ and control mice, but indicated a high degree of

    stimuluscontrolforbothgroupsinthetask(seeFigureS3).

    Nesp55co-localiseswith5-HT;keyserotoningenesshowreducedexpression

    inmid-brainofNespm/+mice

    Previouswork(Baueretal.,1999,Plaggeetal.,2005),which isreplicatedhere

    (Figure S1 d-f), shows thatNesp55 is strongly expressed in the hypothalamus

    andareasofthemidbrain,includingtheEdinger-Westphalnucleus,DRN,andthe

    LC. Using immunohistochemistry, we examined whether Nesp55 co-localises

    with 5-hydroxytryptamine (5HT) in these regions. Qualitative observation

    suggeststhatNesp55isindeedco-localisedwith5HTinthemidbrain(Figure3

    a-c).Thereappearstobeverylittle,ornoco-localisationofNesp55and5HTin

    the hypothalamus (Figure 3 d-f). Additionally, it is clear that Nesp55 is also

    expressednon-5HTexpressingcell-types.

    To investigatetherelationshipbetweenNesp55and5HT, theexpression levels

    ofsome5HT-relatedgeneswereassessedbyqPCRinsamplesofthemidbrainof

    Nespm/+andcontrolmice(Figure4).Thisbrainregionwaschosenasitwasthe

    areashowingthegreatestco-localisationbetweenNesp55and5HT.Therewere

    10-foldreductionsintheexpressionofgenesencodingtryptophanhydroxylase,

    Tph2(U=0.01,P=0.021)andtheserotonintransporter,Slc6a4(U=0.01,P=0.021)

    inNespm/+micerelativetocontrolsubjects,butnodifferenceintheexpressionof

    Th(U=7.0,P=0.886),thegeneencodingtyrosinehydroxylase.

  • 14

    DISCUSSION

    The imprinted gene Nesp, expressed from the maternal allele only, encodes

    Nesp55,andislocatedindiscreteareasofthemidbrainandhypothalamus.Here

    wedemonstratethatlossofNesp55expressionfromthematernalallele,butnot

    paternalallele, leadstohigher levelsof impulsivechoicebehaviorasmeasured

    byadelayedreinforcementtask.TheNespm/+micewerelesswillingtowaitfora

    larger but delayed reward, choosing instead the smaller but immediately

    availablereward.TheseeffectswerehighlyspecificinthatbehaviorintheSSRT

    task, assaying a different form of behavioral control, the ability to stop the

    execution of a pre-potent action, was equivalent betweenNespm/+ and control

    mice. Inaddition,weshowthatNesp55isco-localisedwith5HTandthatthere

    are concomitant reductions in midbrain Tph2 and Slc6a4, but not ThmRNA

    expression in Nespm/+ mice, suggesting an altered serotonergic system could

    contribute,inpart,tothechangesinimpulsivechoicebehavior.

    We have previously shown that Nespm/+ mice have an increased reactivity to

    novelty,butareducedpropensitytoexploreanovelenvironmentwhengivena

    choice(Plaggeetal.,2005).Alterednoveltyexplorationmayinvolvechangesin

    thebalancebetween self-control and impulsive responding (Flageletal., 2010,

    Stoffel&Cunningham,2008).Wedecidedtoexploretheimpulsivityphenotype

    ofNespm/+mice further by using operant tasks to examine discrete aspects of

    impulse control, namely choice impulsivity, as measured on a delayed

    reinforcementtask,andimpulsiveaction,asmeasuredontheSSRTtask(Dent&

    Isles, 2014b). In the delayed reinforcement task,Nespm/+ mice showed higher

    levels of impulsivity, switching their choice of the larger, but increasingly

    delayed, reward, more quickly than control mice. Critically though, when the

    delayassociatedwith the largerrewardwasequivalent to thatassociatedwith

    the smaller reward throughout the session, there was no difference in

    performance between Nespm/+ and control mice. Taken together with other

    control measures within the delayed-reinforcement task, which indicated that

    therewerenodifferencesbetweenNespm/+ and controlmice in their ability to

    learnthetaskortheirgeneralmotivationtoperformthetask,thismanipulation

    demonstrates that the prime factor controlling the difference in behavior

  • 15

    betweenNespm/+ and controlmice on the delayed-reinforcement task,was the

    contingency between delay and reward. Conversely, behavior of Nespm/+ and

    controlmice on the SSRT task, was equivalent. All mice showed the expected

    reductionstoppingasthestop-signalpresentationmovedprogressivelycloserto

    theexecutionofthegoresponse,buttherewerenogenotype-relateddifferences

    acrossthetaskorinthederivedSSRTmeasure.

    Overall, thesedatasuggestthat lossofmaternalNesp55expressionproducesa

    specific increase in impulsive choicebehavior,whilst impulsiveaction remains

    unaffected.Asaconcept,thedissociationofchoiceandactionimpulsivityisnow

    wellestablished.Forinstance,inbredstrainsofmicehavebeenshowntoexhibit

    dissociations in terms of impulsive behaviors in a delayed reinforcement task

    (Isles et al., 2004), and a measure of action impulsivity in the 5-choice serial

    reaction time task (Patel et al., 2006). Furthermore, a cross-species study

    examiningbehavior inanumberofdifferent tasks found that, inboth ratsand

    humans, measures of impulsive choice and impulsive action did not correlate

    (Broos etal., 2012). Our data add to the growing body of evidence suggesting

    thatimpulsivityisnotunitary,andinsteadisamulti-facetedconstruct.

    Impulsivechoiceandimpulsiveactionarenotonlydissociablebehaviorally,but

    alsointermsoftheunderlyingneurobiology(Bari&Robbins,2013).Oneneural

    pathwaythathas importantdissociableeffectsondiscreteaspectsof impulsive

    behavioristheserotoninsystem(Barietal.,2009,Humbyetal.,2013,Talposet

    al., 2006, Winstanley et al., 2004). Although excluded from the key areas of

    cognitive control important for mediating impulsive behavior, such as the

    frontostriatalcircuitry,Nesp55isstronglyexpressedinanumberofserotonergic

    areasofthemidbrain(Baueretal.,1999,Plaggeetal.,2004),andhereweshow

    that in some cells Nesp55 and 5HT are co-localised. This is particularly

    interesting given the recently established role of serotonergic dorsal raphé

    neurons in mediating waiting for delayed reward (Miyazaki et al., 2012a,

    Miyazakietal.,2012b), independentfromanyeffectsonreward(Fonsecaetal.,

    2015). One mechanism underlying the specificity of impulsive behavior

    phenotypeseeninNespm/+micecouldbeviaanimbalanceinserotoninsignaling

    intheDRN.

  • 16

    QuantitativePCRanalysisrevealedthatinmicelackingNesp55therewasa10-

    fold reduction in midbrain expression of Tph2, which encodes tryptophan

    hydroxylasetheratelimitingenzymeforbrain5HTsynthesis(Fitzpatrick,1999),

    and the serotonin transporter gene, Slc6a4. Expression of Th (tyrosine

    hydroxylase) was unaltered, consistent with a specific effect on the serotonin

    system. Nesp55 is associated with fast anterograde axonal transport in the

    peripheral nervous system and is considered a marker for the constitutive

    secretory pathway (Fischer-Colbrie et al., 2002, Li et al., 2002). It is unknown

    whetherNesp55influencesthetransportorreleaseofneurotransmittervesicles.

    We have previously found no alteration in monoamine levels in brain tissue

    taken from the prefrontal cortex and midbrain ofNespm/+ mice (Plagge et al.,

    2005). However, thesewhole tissue analysesmaymask the impact of Nesp55

    deficiencyonneurobiological indicesmoreclosely related to synaptic function,

    suchasextracellularlevelsoftransmittersandchangesinpre-andpostsynaptic

    receptor moieties, an idea maintained by the correlative changes in Slc6a4

    expression. Moreover, given the known interplay between the serotonin and

    dopaminesystemsininfluencingimpulsivebehaviour(Dalley&Roiser,2012),it

    is not possible to rule out the action of othermonoamines inNespm/+mice as

    beingcausalof thedelayed-reinforcementphenotype.Therefore,atpresentthe

    exact neural mechanism(s) by which Nesp55 influences behavior remains

    unclear.

    Our data provide a novelmode of action for genomic imprinting in the brain,

    addingtothelistofbehaviorssensitivetoimprintedgenefunction(Daviesetal.,

    2015). Imprinted genes are thought to have evolved as a consequence of

    conflictingphenotypic “interests”betweenmaternal andpaternal genes,which

    causesanescalatingarmsraceinrelationtoallelicexpression,eventuallyleading

    tosilencingofoneorotherparentalallele(Moore&Haig,1991,Wilkins&Haig,

    2003). Inadditiontotheallelic level,parentalconflictmayalsobemanifestat

    thegeneandtissuelevels(Daviesetal.,2005,Wilkinsetal.,2016).Evidenceof

    thisparentalconflictisseenintheoppositeactionofimprintedgenesduringin

    utero growth and early post-natal life (Haig, 2004) but as yet there are no

    behaviouralexamples.Ourfindingsmayaddsomesupporttothetheorythat,as

    aconsequenceofintra-genomicconflict,imprintedgenesexpressedintheadult

  • 17

    brain could lead to a “parliamentof themind”with regard todecision-making

    (Haig, 1997) in that there are opposing parental interests pulling impulsive

    choice in different directions. However, whilst here we show that loss of a

    maternallyexpressedgene,Nesp,causesanincreaseinimpulsivechoices,there

    is, currently at least, no paternally expressed imprinted gene that has been

    shown to influence impulsive responding. A potential candidate here isGrb10,

    which shows overlapping expression with Nesp (Dent & Isles, 2014a), and

    influencessocialdominance(Garfieldetal.,2011),abehavioraldomainthathas

    beenlinkedtoimpulsivity(Davisetal.,2009).Finally,anextensionofthisidea

    andourexperimental findings is that, an imbalance inexpressionof imprinted

    genes,suchasNesp,maynotonlycontributetofacetsof impulsivitywithinthe

    normal range, but also to pathological conditions, such as gambling and drug

    addiction, where response control becomes maladaptive. It is therefore, of

    particularinterestthataNESPdeletionpolymorphismhasbeenassociatedwith

    theoccurrenceofneuropsychiatricillness,includingaddictivebehaviour(Kimet

    al.,2000).

    REFERENCESBari,A.,Eagle,D.M.,Mar,A.C.,Robinson,E.S.&Robbins,T.W.(2009)Dissociable

    effectsofnoradrenaline,dopamine,andserotoninuptakeblockadeonstoptaskperformanceinrats.Psychopharmacology,205,273-283.

    Bari,A.&Robbins,T.W.(2013)Inhibitionandimpulsivity:behavioralandneuralbasisofresponsecontrol.Progressinneurobiology,108,44-79.

    Bauer,R.,Ischia,R.,Marksteiner,J.,Kapeller,I.&Fischer-Colbrie,R.(1999)Localizationofneuroendocrinesecretoryprotein55messengerRNAintheratbrain.Neuroscience,91,685-694.

    Broos,N.,Schmaal,L.,Wiskerke,J.,Kostelijk,L.,Lam,T.,Stoop,N.,Weierink,L.,Ham,J.,deGeus,E.J.,Schoffelmeer,A.N.,vandenBrink,W.,Veltman,D.J.,deVries,T.J.,Pattij,T.&Goudriaan,A.E.(2012)Therelationshipbetweenimpulsivechoiceandimpulsiveaction:across-speciestranslationalstudy.PloSone,7,e36781.

    Dalley,J.W.&Roiser,J.P.(2012)Dopamine,serotoninandimpulsivity.Neuroscience,215,42-58.

    Davies,J.R.,Dent,C.L.,McNamara,G.I.&Isles,A.R.(2015)Behaviouraleffectsofimprintedgenes.CurrentOpinioninBehavioralSciences,2,28-33.

    Davies,W.,Isles,A.R.&Wilkinson,L.S.(2005)Imprintedgeneexpressioninthebrain.Neuroscienceandbiobehavioralreviews,29,421-430.

  • 18

    Davis,J.F.,Krause,E.G.,Melhorn,S.J.,Sakai,R.R.&Benoit,S.C.(2009)Dominantratsarenaturalrisktakersanddisplayincreasedmotivationforfoodreward.Neuroscience,162,23-30.

    Dent,C.L.&Isles,A.R.(2014a)Brain-expressedimprintedgenesandadultbehaviour:theexampleofNespandGrb10.Mammaliangenome:officialjournaloftheInternationalMammalianGenomeSociety,25,87-93.

    Dent,C.L.&Isles,A.R.(2014b)AnOverviewofMeasuringImpulsiveBehaviourinMice.CurrentProtocolsinMouseBiology,4,35-45.

    Ferguson-Smith,A.C.(2011)Genomicimprinting:theemergenceofanepigeneticparadigm.Naturereviews.Genetics,12,565-575.

    Fischer-Colbrie,R.,Eder,S.,Lovisetti-Scamihorn,P.,Becker,A.&Laslop,A.(2002)Neuroendocrinesecretoryprotein55:anovelmarkerfortheconstitutivesecretorypathway.AnnNYAcadSci,971,317-322.

    Fitzpatrick,P.F.(1999)Tetrahydropterin-dependentaminoacidhydroxylases.AnnuRevBiochem,68,355-381.

    Flagel,S.B.,Robinson,T.E.,Clark,J.J.,Clinton,S.M.,Watson,S.J.,Seeman,P.,Phillips,P.E.&Akil,H.(2010)Ananimalmodelofgeneticvulnerabilitytobehavioraldisinhibitionandresponsivenesstoreward-relatedcues:implicationsforaddiction.Neuropsychopharmacology:officialpublicationoftheAmericanCollegeofNeuropsychopharmacology,35,388-400.

    Fonseca,M.S.,Murakami,M.&Mainen,Z.F.(2015)Activationofdorsalrapheserotonergicneuronspromoteswaitingbutisnotreinforcing.Currentbiology:CB,25,306-315.

    Garfield,A.S.,Cowley,M.,Smith,F.M.,Moorwood,K.,Stewart-Cox,J.E.,Gilroy,K.,Baker,S.,Xia,J.,Dalley,J.W.,Hurst,L.D.,Wilkinson,L.S.,Isles,A.R.&Ward,A.(2011)DistinctphysiologicalandbehaviouralfunctionsforparentalallelesofimprintedGrb10.Nature,469,534-538.

    Haig,D.(1997)Parentalantagonism,relatednessasymmetries,andgenomicimprinting.ProcRSocLondBBiolSci,264,1657-1662.

    Haig,D.(2004)Genomicimprintingandkinship:howgoodistheevidence?AnnuRevGenet,38,553-585.

    Humby,T.,Eddy,J.B.,Good,M.A.,Reichelt,A.C.&Wilkinson,L.S.(2013)Anoveltranslationalassayofresponseinhibitionandimpulsivity:effectsofprefrontalcortexlesions,drugsusedinADHD,andserotonin2Creceptorantagonism.Neuropsychopharmacology:officialpublicationoftheAmericanCollegeofNeuropsychopharmacology,38,2150-2159.

    Humby,T.&Wilkinson,L.S.(2011)Assayingdissociableelementsofbehaviouralinhibitionandimpulsivity:translationalutilityofanimalmodels.Currentopinioninpharmacology,11,534-539.

    Ischia,R.,Lovisetti-Scamihorn,P.,Hogue-Angeletti,R.,Wolkersdorfer,M.,Winkler,H.&Fischer-Colbrie,R.(1997)MolecularcloningandcharacterizationofNESP55,anovelchromogranin-likeprecursorofapeptidewith5-HT1Breceptorantagonistactivity.TheJournalofbiologicalchemistry,272,11657-11662.

    Isles,A.R.,Humby,T.,Walters,E.&Wilkinson,L.S.(2004)Commongeneticeffectsonvariationinimpulsivityandactivityinmice.TheJournalofneuroscience:theofficialjournaloftheSocietyforNeuroscience,24,6733-6740.

  • 19

    Isles,A.R.,Humby,T.&Wilkinson,L.S.(2003)Measuringimpulsivityinmiceusinganoveloperantdelayedreinforcementtask:effectsofbehaviouralmanipulationsandd-amphetamine.Psychopharmacology,170,376-382.

    Kim,S.J.,Gonen,D.,Hanna,G.L.,Leventhal,B.L.&Cook,E.H.,Jr.(2000)DeletionpolymorphisminthecodingregionofthehumanNESP55alternativetranscriptofGNAS1.MolCellProbes,14,191-194.

    Li,J.Y.,Lovisetti-Scamihorn,P.,Fischer-Colbrie,R.,Winkler,H.,Dahlstrom,A.,Departmentof,A.&CellBiology,U.o.G.S.j.l.a.g.s.(2002)Distributionandintraneuronaltraffickingofanovelmemberofthechromograninfamily,NESP55,intheratperipheralnervoussystem.Neuroscience,110,731-745.

    Livak,K.J.&Schmittgen,T.D.(2001)Analysisofrelativegeneexpressiondatausingreal-timequantitativePCRandthe2(-DeltaDeltaC(T))Method.Methods,25,402-408.

    McGrath,J.&Solter,D.(1984)Completionofmouseembryogenesisrequiresboththematernalandpaternalgenomes.Cell,37,179-183.

    Miyazaki,K.,Miyazaki,K.W.&Doya,K.(2012a)Theroleofserotoninintheregulationofpatienceandimpulsivity.MolNeurobiol,45,213-224.

    Miyazaki,K.W.,Miyazaki,K.&Doya,K.(2012b)Activationofdorsalrapheserotoninneuronsisnecessaryforwaitingfordelayedrewards.TheJournalofneuroscience:theofficialjournaloftheSocietyforNeuroscience,32,10451-10457.

    Moore,T.&Haig,D.(1991)Genomicimprintinginmammaliandevelopment-aparentaltug-of-war.TrendsinGenetics,7,45-49.

    Patel,S.,Stolerman,I.P.,Asherson,P.&Sluyter,F.(2006)AttentionalperformanceofC57BL/6andDBA/2miceinthe5-choiceserialreactiontimetask.Behaviouralbrainresearch,170,197-203.

    Peters,J.(2014)Theroleofgenomicimprintinginbiologyanddisease:anexpandingview.Naturereviews.Genetics,15,517-530.

    Peters,J.,Wroe,S.F.,Wells,C.A.,Miller,H.J.,Bodle,D.,Beechey,C.V.,Williamson,C.M.&Kelsey,G.(1999)AclusterofoppositelyimprintedtranscriptsattheGnaslocusinthedistalimprintingregionofmousechromosome2.ProceedingsoftheNationalAcademyofSciencesoftheUnitedStatesofAmerica,96,3830-3835.

    Plagge,A.,Gordon,E.,Dean,W.,Boiani,R.,Cinti,S.,Peters,J.&Kelsey,G.(2004)TheimprintedsignalingproteinXLalphasisrequiredforpostnataladaptationtofeeding.Naturegenetics,36,818-826.

    Plagge,A.,Isles,A.R.,Gordon,E.,Humby,T.,Dean,W.,Gritsch,S.,Fischer-Colbrie,R.,Wilkinson,L.S.&Kelsey,G.(2005)Imprintednesp55influencesbehavioralreactivitytonovelenvironments.MolCellBiol,25,3019-3026.

    Stoffel,E.C.&Cunningham,K.A.(2008)Therelationshipbetweenthelocomotorresponsetoanovelenvironmentandbehavioraldisinhibitioninrats.Drugandalcoholdependence,92,69-78.

    Surani,M.A.,Barton,S.C.&Norris,M.L.(1984)Developmentofreconstitutedmouseeggssuggestsimprintingofthegenomeduringgametogenesis.Nature,308,548-550.

    Talpos,J.C.,Wilkinson,L.S.&Robbins,T.W.(2006)Acomparisonofmultiple5-HTreceptorsintwotasksmeasuringimpulsivity.JPsychopharmacol,20,47-58.

  • 20

    Wilkins,J.F.&Haig,D.(2003)Whatgoodisgenomicimprinting:thefunctionofparent-specificgeneexpression.Naturereviews.Genetics,4,359-368.

    Wilkins,J.F.,Ubeda,F.&VanCleve,J.(2016)Theevolvinglandscapeofimprintedgenesinhumansandmice:Conflictamongalleles,genes,tissues,andkin.Bioessays,38,482-489.

    Winstanley,C.A.,Dalley,J.W.,Theobald,D.E.&Robbins,T.W.(2004)Fractionatingimpulsivity:contrastingeffectsofcentral5-HTdepletionondifferentmeasuresofimpulsivebehavior.Neuropsychopharmacology:officialpublicationoftheAmericanCollegeofNeuropsychopharmacology,29,1331-1343.

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    CLD,KLandARIperformedtheexperiments;TH,JFW,LSWandARIdesignedthe

    study;APandRF-Csuppliedreagents;CLD,TH,LSWandARIwrote thepaper.

    Thisworkwas funded by a Leverhulme Trust project grant (F/00 407/BF) to

    ARI and JFW, which supported CLD. The work was also funded by the MRC

    Centre forNeuropsychiatricGenetics andGenomics (G0801418),ofwhichARI,

    THandLSWaremembers,andwhichalsosupportedKLviaaPhDstudentship.

    Theauthorshavenoconflictsofinteresttodeclare.

  • 21

    Figure1.Nespm-/p+miceshowincreasedimpulsivechoicebehaviorinthe

    delayed-reinforcementtask.BehaviorofbothNespm/+andcontrolmice

    changedacrosssessionblocks(blk)withincreasingdelay,suchthatchoicebias

    movedawayfromtheresponseleadingtothelargerewardtowardsthesmall

    rewardwithincreasingdelay(a).However,thereweresystematicdifferences

    betweenthegroupsintheirbehavior,suchthatNespm/+animals(N=11)

    switchedtheirchoicetothesmall,lessdelayedrewardmorequicklythancontrol

    mice(N=7).Whenthedelayassociatedwiththelargeandsmallrewardswas

    equal(1s)throughoutthesession(b),choicebiaswasconsistentlyhigh(large

    rewardchosenapproximately90%ofthetime).Moreover,underthese

    conditionstherewerenodifferencesinchoicebiasbetweenNespm/+andcontrol

    mice.Datashowsmean±SEMofthreeconsecutivestablesessions;*represents

    P

  • 22

    Figure2.NodifferencebetweenNespm/+andcontrolmiceperformancein

    thestop-signal(SSRT)task.BothcontrolandNespm/+miceshowedan

    equivalentabilitytoperformtheSSRTtaskshowingtheexpectedchangein

    percentagecorrectrespondingduringa‘stop’trial(a)asthepositionofthestop-

    signalwasaltered,buttherewerenodifferencesbetweenNespm/+(N=17)and

    controlmice(N=12).Nespm/+andcontrolmicealsoshowedequivalentSSRTsat

    50%correctstopping(b).Therewerenogenotypedifferencesforthe‘Go’

    responseforbothcohortsofmice,intermsofpercentagecorrectresponding(c)

    orresponsespeed(d).Datashowsmean±SEM;**representsP

  • 23

    Figure3.Co-localisationofNesp55with5HTinthebrain.Sectionswere

    dual-labelledwithanti-bodiesagainstNesp55and5HT,imageswerethen

    mergedtogaugecellularco-localisation.Whitearrowsdepictareasofco-

    localisation.Therewasevidenceofco-localisationintheregionsofthemidbrain

    (a-c),forexamplethelocuscoeruleus.However,therewasverylittleco-

    localisationobservedinthehypothalamus(d-e).Allimagesareatx40

    magnification

  • 24

    Figure4.Altered5HT-relatedgeneexpressioninNespm/+mice.Gene

    expressionanalysisofmidbrainmacro-dissectionsfromNespm/+andcontrol

    mice(N=4forbothgroups)showedsignificant10-foldreductionsinTph2and

    Slc6a4expression,butnodifferenceinThexpression.Datashowsmean±SEM;*

    representsP

  • 25

    ImpulsivechoicesinmicelackingimprintedNesp55-

    supplementarymaterial

    Dent CLa, Humby Tb, Lewis Ka, Plagge Ac, Fischer-Colbrie Rd, Wilkinson LSa,b,

    WilkinsJFf&IslesARa

    aBehavioural Genetics Group, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and

    Genomics,NeuroscienceandMentalHealthResearchInstitute,CardiffUniversity,

    HadynEllisBuilding,MaindyRoad,Cardiff,UnitedKingdom

    bBehavioural Genetics Group, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Tower

    Building,Cardiff,UnitedKingdom

    cDepartment of Cellular and Molecular Physiology, Institute of Translational

    Medicine,UniversityofLiverpool,Liverpool,UnitedKingdom

    dDepartmentofPharmacology,InnsbruckMedicalUniversity,Innsbruck,Austria

    eRoninInstitute,Montclair,NJ07043,USA

    Authorforcorrespondence:AnthonyR.Isles,[email protected]

    SUPPLEMENTARYMATERIALSANDMETHODSAnimals

    Loci closely linked to a targeted mutation may cause concern with regard to

    phenotypicdifferencesbetweenknock-outsandcontrols,sincetheymostlikely

    retaintheoriginal129inbredstrainEScellalleles,evenafterseveralgenerations

    ofbackcrossingtoadifferentstrain.Foranalysisofmicecarryingahomozygous

    mutation of a biallelically expressed gene, this may imply comparing an

    associated homozygous 129/Sv genetic background with wild-type C57BL/6J

    counterparts.However,theknockoutanalysisofmonoallelicallyexpressedgenes

    providestheopportunityforamoredirectcomparisonofheterozygousgroups.

    In the case of maternally expressed Nesp, heterozygous deficient mice

    (Nespm/+)can be compared with heterozygous Nesp-expressing mice (Nesp+/p)

    obtained fromreciprocalcrosses toC57BL/6J. Inbothof thesegroups, the loci

  • 26

    surrounding the targeted mutation will comprise compound C57BL/6J and

    129/Svalleles,andthus,anypotentialstrainvariationsshouldmanifestequally

    in both groups. In contrast, thewild-type littermates of the two heterozygous

    groupsarehomozygousC57BL/6Jforthesegenomicregions,couldconfoundthe

    interpretation of behavioral assays (Gerlai, 1996), particularly given known

    differences in impulsive behavior between inbred strains (Isles et al., 2004).

    Thus,weconsidered itmostappropriate tocompareNespm/+withNesp+/pmice

    ascontrols.

    Immunohistochemistry

    For the ImmunohistochemistryanalysisaVectastain®EliteABCKit (PK-6100)

    was used on brain sections fromNespm/+ (lacking maternal Nesp55), Nesp+/P

    (control, carrying paternal knockout), and wild-type (Nesp+/+) mice. Sections

    werefirstwashedinTBS(4x10mins).Theywerethenincubatedinaperoxidase

    block on a stirrer for 30minutes (0.6%hydrogenPeroxidase inTBS) to block

    endogenousperoxidase.SectionswerewashedinTBSagain(3x10mininTBS),

    andthenincubatedinTBSTwith3%normalgoatserum(NGS)(S-1000,vector

    labs)onastirrerfor30minutesatroomtemperature.FollowingNGSblocking,

    sectionswereincubatedinprimaryantibody(anti-Nesp55)dilutedat1:1000in

    TBSTwith3%NGS.Thiswasstirredfor10minutesandthencoveredandstored

    overnightat4˚c.The followingdaythesectionswerewashed3x10minutes in

    TBSTwith3%NGS. Sectionswere then incubated for1hour in the secondary

    antibody diluted 1:200 in TBSTwith 3%NGS at room temperature. Following

    thesecondaryantibodyincubationsectionswerewashed3x10minutesinTBST,

    andthenallowedtoincubateintheABCcomplex(madeasperkitspecifications)

    at room temperature, on a stirrer for 1 hour. Sections were then washed as

    before(3x10minutesinTBST),thenwashed2x10minutesin0.05MTrisbuffer.

    Sections were then incubated in DAB solution for 35 seconds at room

    temperature; theywere then immediatelywashed in coldPBS inorder to stop

    thereaction.FinallysectionswerethenwashedinTBSTfor2minutesandleftin

    a new change of TBST solution overnight. The following day sections were

    mountedon topolysinecoatedslides,andallowedtodryover-night.Themounted

    slides were then dehydrated through a process of incubation in a rising

  • 27

    concentration of alcohol followed by xylene, then cover-slipped and sealed using

    DPX (Raymond Lamb DPX), andallowed todryover-night.All experimentswere

    carriedoutalongsidenegativecontrols.

    CalculatingtheStopSignalReactionTime

    Correct go reaction timeswere determined directly, however the stop-signal reaction

    time(SSRT)hadtobederivedfromthedistributionofcorrectgoreactiontimesandthe

    proportionofcorrectlystoppedtrials.SSRTsinthetaskwereestimatedemployingthe

    standard procedure described in Logan et al. (1984), using data from where the

    proportion of correct stop responses was ~50%. For each subject, data from the

    sessionsinwhichthestop-signalpositionswerevariedrelativetotheindividualisedgo

    reactiontime,wererankedbytheproportionofcorrectstopresponses,anddatafrom

    sessionsinwhichthisvaluewasbetween40%and60%(i.e.50%±10%)wereaveraged.

    The latency of stopping as defined by the SSRTwas derived from the distribution of

    correct go reaction times and the proportion of correctly stopped trials as previously

    described (Eagle & Robbins, 2003, Logan, 1994). Hence, for each of the sessions

    determined above, the correct go reaction timeswere rank ordered from smallest to

    largest and the nth value found, where n is the rank order position based on the

    proportionoffailingtostopcorrectlyinstoptrialswascorrectedfortheoccurrenceof

    omittedgotrials.Althoughomittedgotrialswerearareoccurrence,thiscorrectionwas

    implemented based on the rationale that they could alter the observed inhibition

    functionandaffectdeterminationofthenthcorrectgoreactiontimevalueandhencethe

    final SSRT (Eagle & Robbins, 2003, Solanto et al., 2001, Tannock et al., 1989). To

    determine the SSRT, the time the stop-signal was presented (i.e. ‘mean correct go

    reaction time’ x ‘%meanstop-signalposition’)was subtracted from thenth correct go

    reactiontimevalue.

    SUPPLEMENTARYRESULTS

    Initial validation of the Nesp55 anti-body was carried out by completing

    immunohistochemistrystainingonbrainsectionsfromWT,Nespm/+andNesp+/p

    adultmice(FigureS1).ThestainingrevealedthattheNespanti-bodysuccessfully

    targetstheNesp55protein,showingcellspecificstaininginbothWTandNesp+/p

    (wherebymaternalNespispresentinthebrain);inthehypothalamus,ponsand

    mid-brain regions, consistent with the discrete regions previously described

  • 28

    (Plaggeetal.,2005);andshowingnostaining intheNespm/+ sections(whereby

    maternalNesp55hasbeendeleted).

    Additionalmeasuresinthedelayedreinforcementtask

    Inadditiontochoice,anumberofothermeasuresofbasicbehaviorwithin the

    delayed reinforcement task were obtained. These relate to general motoric

    competence andmotivation within the task. As expected,Nespm/+ and control

    micedemonstratedageneralmaineffectofDELAYonmanyof thesemeasures

    (Figure S2) as follows: start (F1.7,27.5=22.06, P

  • 29

    Gerlai,R.(1996)Gene-targetingstudiesofmammalianbehavior:isitthemutationorthebackgroundgenotype?Trendsinneurosciences,19,177-181.

    Isles,A.R.,Humby,T.,Walters,E.&Wilkinson,L.S.(2004)Commongeneticeffectsonvariationinimpulsivityandactivityinmice.TheJournalofneuroscience:theofficialjournaloftheSocietyforNeuroscience,24,6733-6740.

    Logan,G.(1994)Ontheabilitytoinhibitthoughtandaction:Ausers'guidetothestopsignalparadigm.InDagenbach,D.&Carr,T.(eds),Inhibitoryprocessesinattention,memory,andlanguage..AcademicPress,SanDiego,pp.189-239.

    Plagge,A.,Isles,A.R.,Gordon,E.,Humby,T.,Dean,W.,Gritsch,S.,Fischer-Colbrie,R.,Wilkinson,L.S.&Kelsey,G.(2005)ImprintedNesp55influencesbehavioralreactivitytonovelenvironments.Molecularandcellularbiology,25,3019-3026.

    Solanto,M.V.,Abikoff,H.,Sonuga-Barke,E.,Schachar,R.,Logan,G.D.,Wigal,T.,Hechtman,L.,Hinshaw,S.&Turkel,E.(2001)TheecologicalvalidityofdelayaversionandresponseinhibitionasmeasuresofimpulsivityinAD/HD:asupplementtotheNIMHmultimodaltreatmentstudyofAD/HD.JAbnormChildPsychol,29,215-228.

    Tannock,R.,Schachar,R.J.,Carr,R.P.,Chajczyk,D.&Logan,G.D.(1989)Effectsofmethylphenidateoninhibitorycontrolinhyperactivechildren.JAbnormChildPsychol,17,473-491.

  • 30

    FigureS1

    FigureS1ImmunohistochemistrystainingforNespincoronalsectionsoftheadultmouse

    brain.(a-c)ShowsstaininginWT,Nesp+/p(usedascontrols)andNespm/+adultmice;showingthe

    presenceofdiscreteexpressionofNesp55 in thehypothalamus inbothWT(a)andNesp+/p(b)

    mice,butnotNespm/+mice(c).(d-f)WTmousebrainprobedusinganti-Nesp55antibodyshows

    distinct staining in the hypothalamus, specifically the DM, dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus,

    Arc, arcuatehypothalamicnucleus, and theLH, lateral hypothalamic area (d); in themidbrain,

    specifically the EWn, Edinger-Westphal nucleus (e); and in the pons, specifically the LC, Locus

    Coeruleus(f).

  • 31

    FigureS2

    FigureS2AdditionalbehavioralmeasuresofperformanceofNespm/+andcontrol

    mice in the delayed reinforcement task. a Start latency; b Choice latency; c Non-

    startedtrials;dPanelpushes.Alldatarepresentmeans±s.e.m.

  • 32

    FigureS3

    FigureS3AdditionalbehavioralmeasuresofperformanceofNespm/+andcontrol

    miceintheSSRTtask.aNumberoftrialsinasessionwithindividualisedSSRT(when

    thestopcueispresented50%intotheindividual’sGo-reactiontime); b Initiationand

    magazinelatencyinasessionwithindividualisedSSRT.Alldatarepresentmeans±s.e.m.