impact of seat belt use on driving behavior

10
IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR HARINDER SlNGH and MARK THAYER* Previous research has indicated that individual compensating behavior, specifically, more risky driving may reduce the efectiveness of seat belt laws. W e test the corn- pensating-behavior hypothesis using individual-specific survey data. The analysis also incorporates individual risk tastes. Our results indicate that the compensating-behavior hypothesis applies only to those that are not strongly risk averse. Other risk-diferen- tiated groups do not exhibit compensating behavior. Finally, it seems that individuals learn to reduce compensating behavior Over time. 1. INTRODUCTION The National Safety Council estimates that the expected reduction in occupant death rates due to usage of lap seat belts would be 7-8.5 percent (see Peltzman [1975]). In addition, Campbell and Camp- bell [1988] estimate that fatalities in the twenty-five states with seat belt laws were 6.6 percent lower than forecast for these states. This improvement amounts to ap- proximately 1,300 lives saved. Swan [1984] reports that seat belts are negatively cor- related with the number of traffic fatali- ties. This conclusion is based on a pooled cross-section, time-series model for Aus- tralia and New Zealand. Finally, McEwin [1986] estimates on the basis of his empir- ical model that a 100 percent usage of seat belts would reduce fatalities by 40 percent. However, a number of studies have pointed out that, contrary to conventional wisdom, seat belt laws may not reduce the total number of fatalities. Their argument is that use of the safety belt provides the driver with an additional sense of security which translates into relatively more reck- less driving. There is a potential for offset- ting or compensating behavior on the part of the driver of the automobile. This no- tion is referred to as the “compensating- behavior hypothesis.” Compensating behavior is justified on both theoretical and empirical grounds. Blomquist [1986] finds that compensating behavior is utility maximizing if an individual’s safety effort and exogenous safety measures (e.g., government regula- tions) are substitutes in determining the probability of and loss from an automobile accident. Peltzman [1975; 19761 tested the impact of automobile safety regulation on both occupant and non-occupant deaths. He found evidence from aggregate time-series and cross-section data that occupants’ lives are saved at the expense of pedes- trian deaths and a larger number of non- fatal accidents. Recently, Garbacz [1991a] tested this hypothesis using aggregate time-series data for New Zealand. He found that a mandatory seat belt law, * Associate Professor, San Diego State University, and Professor, University of Nevada-Las Vegas. The authors wish to thank Christopher Garbacz and an anonymous reviewer for providing helpful comments on an earlier draft. In addition, research support from the National Science Foundation and the United States Geological Survey is gratefully acknowledged. The usual caveat applies. enacted in 1972, was negatively correlated with the deaths of automobile occupants and positively associated with deaths of cyclists and pedestrians. In fact, the Gar- bacz study indicated complete Offsetting in that total deaths showed no relationship Economic Inquiry Vol. XXX, October 1992,649-658 649 @Western Economic Association International

Upload: harinder-singh

Post on 30-Sep-2016

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR

IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR HARINDER SlNGH and MARK THAYER*

Previous research has indicated that individual compensating behavior, specifically, more risky driving may reduce the efectiveness of seat belt laws. W e test the corn- pensating-behavior hypothesis using individual-specific survey data. The analysis also incorporates individual risk tastes. Our results indicate that the compensating-behavior hypothesis applies only to those that are not strongly risk averse. Other risk-diferen- tiated groups do not exhibit compensating behavior. Finally, it seems that individuals learn to reduce compensating behavior Over time.

1. INTRODUCTION

The National Safety Council estimates that the expected reduction in occupant death rates due to usage of lap seat belts would be 7-8.5 percent (see Peltzman [1975]). In addition, Campbell and Camp- bell [1988] estimate that fatalities in the twenty-five states with seat belt laws were 6.6 percent lower than forecast for these states. This improvement amounts to ap- proximately 1,300 lives saved. Swan [1984] reports that seat belts are negatively cor- related with the number of traffic fatali- ties. This conclusion is based on a pooled cross-section, time-series model for Aus- tralia and New Zealand. Finally, McEwin [1986] estimates on the basis of his empir- ical model that a 100 percent usage of seat belts would reduce fatalities by 40 percent.

However, a number of studies have pointed out that, contrary to conventional wisdom, seat belt laws may not reduce the total number of fatalities. Their argument is that use of the safety belt provides the

driver with an additional sense of security which translates into relatively more reck- less driving. There is a potential for offset- ting or compensating behavior on the part of the driver of the automobile. This no- tion is referred to as the “compensating- behavior hypothesis.”

Compensating behavior is justified on both theoretical and empirical grounds. Blomquist [1986] finds that compensating behavior is utility maximizing if an individual’s safety effort and exogenous safety measures (e.g., government regula- tions) are substitutes in determining the probability of and loss from an automobile accident.

Peltzman [1975; 19761 tested the impact of automobile safety regulation on both occupant and non-occupant deaths. He found evidence from aggregate time-series and cross-section data that occupants’ lives are saved at the expense of pedes- trian deaths and a larger number of non- fatal accidents. Recently, Garbacz [1991a] tested this hypothesis using aggregate time-series data for New Zealand. He found that a mandatory seat belt law,

* Associate Professor, San Diego State University, and Professor, University of Nevada-Las Vegas. The authors wish to thank Christopher Garbacz and an anonymous reviewer for providing helpful comments on an earlier draft. In addition, research support from the National Science Foundation and the United States Geological Survey is gratefully acknowledged. The usual caveat applies.

enacted in 1972, was negatively correlated with the deaths of automobile occupants and positively associated with deaths of cyclists and pedestrians. In fact, the Gar- bacz study indicated complete Offsetting in that total deaths showed no relationship

Economic Inquiry Vol. XXX, October 1992,649-658

649

@Western Economic Association International

Page 2: IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR

650 ECONOMIC INQUIRY

to seat belt use (savings in occupant deaths were just offset by the increase in non-occupant deaths). In other work Gar- bacz [1990; 1991b; 19921 provides further evidence of offsetting behavior, demon- strating that both non-occupants and rear- seat passengers are more at risk when seat belt laws are in effect. A final example of offsetting behavior was provided in a re- cent Wall Street Journal article (10 October 1991) that showed that air bags seem to reduce fatalities, but accidents and other associated injuries have increased.

One concern in the Garbacz [1991a] work was the sensitivity of the results to the "speed variable" (average open-road speed for the 85th percentile of vehicles). He concluded that this variable could be endogenous to the estimation process. Thus, the compensating-behavior hypoth- esis would imply more risk taking (includ- ing speeding) after the imposition of the seat belt law. In fact, Lave and Weber [1970] first suggested the possibility that mandated safety devices might lead to faster driving, offsetting some or all of the beneficial effect of the safety device.

In this paper we re-examine the issue of compensating behavior when individuals use seat belts. Our investigation has a number of significant departures from previous studies. First, since compensat- ing behavior is a hypothesis of individual action, we test the hypothesis using indi- vidual-specific observations. The studies discussed above employed aggregate time-series or cross-section data. At the aggregate level, the statistical regularities could conceivably be caused by other con- founding factors.

Second, we test the compensating-be- havior hypothesis after accounting for tastes in risks (as manifested by precau- tionary steps individuals have taken to reduce everyday risks). The notion of an individual who wears a seat belt taking more or less risk is obviously tied to his/her risk preference. For example, Blomquist [1991] has found that individu-

als seem to consider relative risk when making safety equipment decisions; that is, individuals are not risk incompetent. Testing the compensating hypothesis at an aggregate level without controlling for the risk preferences of specific individuals could result in specification error. One would expect a priori that the compensat- ing-behavior hypothesis holds for individ- uals who have a low regard for various risks. On the other hand, risk averse indi- viduals may not compensate (for the ad- ditional safety provided by a seat belt) by driving more recklessly. Clearly, tastes for risk are an important dimension of the compensating behavior hypothesis.

Third, this study employs a more sen- sitive indicator of compensating behavior. As discussed above, past studies have analyzed the impact of safety belts or safety belt laws on the number of fatalities and/or the number of accidents. We ana- lyze the impact of an individual's seat belt usage on the number of moving violations (tickets).' If seat belt wearing drivers are taking additional risks, then such behav- ior will result in a larger number of mov- ing violations, ceteris paribus.

Finally, our empirical model controls for other individual-specific variables (be- sides seat belt use) which may affect driv- ing performance. These confour~ding fac- tors include the number of years individ- uals have worn seat belts, age, miles driven to work, education level, sex, and annual income.

The remainder of the paper is organized as follows. In the next section we present a theoretical model of individual behavior. In section I11 the data and the basic empir- ical model are discussed. Results are pre- sented in section IV. Concluding remarks and some caveats are offered in section V.

1. We do not presume any relationship between the number of moving violations and traffic accidents and/or fatalities. This relationship is currently poorly understood and should be the focus of future research.

Page 3: IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR

SINGH & THAYER: SEAT BELT USE AND DRIVING BEHAVIOR 651

II. THEORETICAL BASIS

Blomquist [1986] demonstrated that compensating behavior is utility maximiz- ing if individual and exogenous safety measures are substitutes in the production of a specific accident risk level. However, Blomquist did not address the possibility that individual and exogenous measures could be combined in a complementary manner (i.e. exogenous safety measures would enhance individual efforts). In this latter case compensating behavior cannot be predicted a priori. In this paper the manner in which individuals combine safety measures, and the consequent effect on compensating behavior, is taken to be an empirical question.

The model presented herein utilizes the Blomquist framework. Let

e = g =

P =

L =

D =

individual safety measures; exogenous safety measures beyond control of the individual; P(e,g) = probability that an individ- ual is involved in an automobile accident where probability is influ- enced by individual actions (P, < 0, P,, > 0) and exogenous factors

L(e,g) = loss associated with an ac- cident where loss is influenced by individual actions (L, < 0, L,, > 0) and exogenous safety measures

D(e,g) = disutility associated with driver safety (D, > 0, D,, > 0) and exogenous safety factors (Dg > 0, D, > 0). Disutility may result from the interaction of individual and exogenous actions (Deg > 0).

(Pg < 0, Pgg > 0);

(Lg < 0, Lgg > 0);

Also note that we have made no as- sumption concerning the relationship be- tween e and g in either the production function P or the loss function L. If Peg and L, are non-negative, then individual and exogenous actions are substitutes in pro-

duction (see Blomquist [1986]). However, if these values are negative, then individ- uals perceive e and g as complementary goods.

The individual is assumed to maximize expected utility, constrained by income (I). Thus

The individual will take safety mea- sures until the benefits of additional action (benefits of reduction in expected loss) are just offset by the additional disutility. Thus, optimal driver safety effort will con- form to

- D, = P,L + PL,.

The relationship between individual and exogenous safety measures can be determined by treating equation (2) as an implicit function and using the implicit function rule to solve for d e / d g .

Following Blomquist [1986], d e / d g < 0 if individual and exogenous actions are as- sumed to be substitutes in the production and loss functions. This is the formula for compensating behavior. However, the sign of d e / d g is indeterminate if e and g are assumed to be complementary in reducing risk and loss. In this case compensating behavior does not always occur and will depend on the extent that complementar- ity produces additional safety that offsets the extra disutility of any exogenous safety measure.

Page 4: IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR

652 ECONOMIC INQUIRY

The manner in which individuals treat individual and exogenous safety measures is an empirical question. A priori one would expect that relatively risk averse individuals would demonstrate the least amount of compensating behavior. That is, exogenous safety measures might not be offset by reductions in individual careful- ness. Likewise, risk preferrers or risk lov- ers would be relatively strong candidates for compensating behavior. In the next section we present the data and empirical model that allows this hypothesis to be tested.

111. DATA SPECIFICS AND EMPIRICAL MODEL

The data was collected through a mail survey sent to residents in six San Fran- cisco Bay area counties: Alameda, Contra Costa, Marin, San Francisco, San Mateo and Santa Clara. Potential respondents were selected at random from a master data tape of homeowners. We employed the Dillman [1978] Total Design Method (TDM) in order to maximize the response rate. This procedure requires complete personalization of the correspondence and multiple attempts to convince respon- dents to participate in the survey.

Approximately 3,000 surveys were dis- tributed and 1,092 surveys were eventu- ally returned. The response rate was 37.13 percent. This is considered a satisfactory response rate for a mail survey that was extensive, provided no compensation for respondents, and did not utilize all of the TDM approach (we did not follow our mail correspondence with a telephone call due to limited funds).2

2. Dillman 119781 routinely gets in excess of 70 per- cent response rates for mail surveys. However, these are obtained for surveys that (1) target specific groups with an interest in the primary issue and (2) utilize the entire Total Design Method, which requires multiple correspondence procedures, including telephone fol- low-up. Our response rate is considered satisfactory in this context.

The survey obtained information con- cerning seat belt usage, years of seat belt use, number of moving violations over the previous three years, risk preferences, and several control variables including in- come, sex, age, and education. A detailed definition of each variable is provided in Table I.

Four variables require further discus- sion. First, the dependent variable in the empirical analysis is the number of mov- ing violations the respondent has received in the previous three years3 Moving vio- lations include exceeding posted speed limits, failure to stop at stop sign or traffic light, reckless driving, etc. Second, the seat belt use variable is used to test the com- pensating-behavior hypothesis by exam- ining its relationship to the number of moving violations. Third, the independent variable for years of seat belt use allows a determination of the magnitude of learn- ing concerning compensating behavior. Fi- nally, the risk index variable, formed by summing the responses to questions con- cerning six different revealed preferences about risk behavior (presence of smoke alarm, burglar alarm, car alarm, earth- quake home insurance, emergency equip- ment, and emergency food items), is used to measure relative risk a ~ e r s i o n . ~ That is, if the individual's risk index is close to the

3. We utilize the previous three-year time period for two reasons. First, this is the usual time period that insurance companies use when evaluating insurance risk and premiums. Thus, respondents are familiar with this measure and will likely respond with little error. Second, most individuals do not receive moving violations yearly (approximately 65 percent of the sam- ple had none in the previous three years) so using three years generates the necessary variation in the dependent variable. However, using the previous three-year period may cause some inconsistency with seat belt use since the latter may not be stable over the previous three years.

4. Blomquist [1991] and McCarthy [1986] examine the relationship between risk and seat belt usage. Thus, they concentrate on risk as it relates to the driving trips (road conditions, length of trip, etc). However, we are not trying to predict seat belt usage. Rather, we concentrate on the individual's general notion of risk as portrayed in response to a set of everyday risks.

Page 5: IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR

SINGH & THAYER SEAT BELT USE AND DRIVING BEHAVIOR 653

TABLE I Variable Definitions

Number of Tickets Number of traffic citations for moving violations in

Seat Belt Use

Years o f sea t Belt Use

Education

Sex

Age

Distance to Work

Income

Risk Index

Underage Children

the past three years. Moving violations include speed- ing, red light, stop sign, reckless driving or "other".

Discrete variable for seat belt use: (0) no seat belt use; (1) yes, but rarely; (2) yes, some of the time; (3) yes, all the time.

Number of years individual has worn a seat belt.

Discrete variable for education completed: (1) 0-5 grades; (2) 6-8 grades; (3) 8-11 grades; (4) finished high school; (5) trade school; (6) some college; (7) col- lege degree; (8) some graduate work; (9) advanced college degree/professional degree.

Discrete variable: (0) female; (1) male.

Number of years.

Distance to work site in miles.

Discrete variable for annual income: (1) less than $5,000; (2) $5,000-$9,999; (3) $10,000-$19,999; (4) $20,000-$29,999; (5) $30,000-$39,999; (6) $40,000- $49,999; (7) $50,000-$59,999; (8) $60,000-$79,999; (9) $80,000-$99,999; (10) over $100,000.

Sum of responses to six yes/no questions concerning revealed preferences about risk. Categories were smoke alarm, burglar alarm, car alarm, earthquake in- surance, emergency equipment, and emergency sup- plies. In all cases "zero" indicates no whereas "one" indicates yes.

Number of children under the age of eighteen.

maximum value (6) then he/she is consid- ered relatively risk averse. On the other hand, if the individuals' risk index is near zero then this individual is considered a risk lover since relatively few precautions are taken.

In the empirical analysis we define the risk averse group as those individuals

with risk index values of 5 or 6. Those individuals that prefer risk (risk lovers) have index values of 0 , l or 2. The remain- ing individuals are considered risk neu- tral. These division values are arbitrary. However, the significance pattern of the results remains basically the same if other groupings are used. For instance, we used

Page 6: IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR

654 ECONOMIC INQUIRY

six categories, conforming to the values one through six. The basic results were unchanged. However, the problem with six categories is that risk index values of one and six have only thirteen and thirty observations, respectively. Consequently, the three-tier demarcation defined above is preferred because of a relatively more symmetric distribution of the number of observations in each category.

Descriptive statistics for each of the variables used in the empirical analysis are presented in Table 11. The data are presented for each of the three risk prefer- ence groups defined above. As is illus- trated, risk lovers have received more moving violations than the other groups. In addition, risk lovers use seat belts less often, have used seat belts for a shorter length of time, have lower incomes, and are less educated and younger than the other groups.

It should also be noted that our sample consists of resident owners of single-fam- ily homes. Thus, the sample is not neces- sarily representative of the general popu- lation. In particular, our sample is rela- tively older, better educated, and has greater income than the general popula- tion. For example, the sample contains only three respondents less than the age of 25. Since the age group 16-24 generally comprises approximately 20 percent of the drivers, our sample is not representative. Thus, our sample likely receives fewer moving violations and wears seat belts more frequently than the general popula- tion. In other words we expect our sample to be relatively more risk averse.

IV. EMPIRICAL RESULTS

The empirical analysis is based on 690 complete data points. The basic model attempts to explain the number of moving violations (Number of Tickets) as a function of the independent variables Seat Belt Use, Years of Seat Belt Use, Risk Index, Education, Income, Age, Distance to Work, Sex, and

Number of Underage Children. The model is estimated via ordinary least squares.

The results for the model in which all risk groups are pooled are presented in Table 111. A dummy variable is used to define the separate groups. This variable, interacted with seat belt use, allows inter- pretation of the relationship between seat belt use and number of tickets by group. Note also that the constant term is sup- pressed.

A number of aspects of the estimated equation are noteworthy. First, the overall significance of the regression is relatively low. This is not uncommon with individ- ual-level data. Further, this problem is alleviated somewhat in later estimation procedures.

Second, the results are reported after White’s [1980] heteroscedastic correction. The Park-Glejser test statistic for heteroscedasticity (t-value = 17.24) indi- cated that White’s correction was needed.

Third, the control variables (Income, Sex, Education, Distance to Work, and Number of Underage Children) are generally not signif- icantly different from zero. The exception is the Age variable that shows that age reduces ticket frequency; that is, older respondents receive fewer tickets. We also experimented with other functional forms for the control variables. For instance, we added an Age Squared term to the equa- tion. However, performance of the control variables was unchanged; the Age vari- able continued to be negative and signifi- cant whereas the Age Squared variable and the other control variables were not signif- icantly different from zero.

Fourth, the impact of seat belt use var- ies according to the risk index. Seat belt use is positive and significant for risk lovers (Risk Index of two or less). This result is indicative of strong compensating behavior; that is, seat belt use is strongly related to an increase in the number of moving violations for members of this group. Compensating behavior is also demonstrated to a lesser extent in the risk

Page 7: IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR

SINGH & THAYER: SEAT BELT USE AND DRIVING BEHAVIOR 655

TABLE I1 Descriptive Statistics

Means and Standard Deviations* Risk Group

Variable Risk Lovers Risk Neutral Risk Averse

Number of Tickets

Seat Belt Use

Years of Belt Use

Risk Index

Education

Income

Age

Sex

Distance to Work

Underage Children

Number of Observations

.52 (.70)

(.39)

2.88

10.22 (7.38)

1.92 (.27)

7.30 (1.58)

8.01 (1.58)

36.59 (8.13)

.77 (.42)

14.21 (13.18)

.79 C95)

125

.43 (.63)

(.30) 2.93

13.17 (8.00)

3.45 (.50)

7.51 (1.42)

8.30 (1.64)

37.52 (8.13)

.75 (.43)

15.95 (21.91)

.86 (1.05)

432

.43 C71)

(.26)

(9.17)

C44)

2.95

13.79

5.25

7.52 (1.37)

8.88 (1.55)

39.57 (8.76)

.74 (.44)

(12.00)

C95)

15.17

.79

102

*Standard deviations in parentheses

neutral group, although the estimated co- efficient is significantly different from zero only at the 10 percent level. The behavior of the risk averse group is counter to these results. Compensating behavior does not occur among members of this group. In fact, the opposite is true. The seat belt wearing members of the risk averse re- spondents receive fewer moving violations than those that do not wear a seat belt. This is evidence that risk averse individu-

als combine personal and exogenous safety measures in a complementary fash- ion.

Finally, the Years of Seat Belt Use variable is negative and significantly different from zero. Thus, the number of moving viola- tions declines in all groups the longer one wears a seat belt. This is an indication of learning, which results in less compensat- ing behavior over time.

Page 8: IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR

656 ECONOMIC INQUIRY

TABLE 111 Impact of Seat Belt Use on Number of Tickets

Pooled Estimates Explanatory Variables Coefficients t-statistics

Risk Lover Group

Risk Lover*Seat Belt Use

Risk Neutral Group

Risk NeutraPSeat Belt Use

Risk Averse Group

Risk Averse'Seat Belt Use

Years of Seat Belt Use

Education

Income

Sex

Age

Distance to Work

Underage Children

R-Square

Number of Observations

-.005

.327

.487

.136

3.68

-.935

-.011

.0003

.007

.048

-.011

-.0005

.012

.08

690

-.017

4.28

1.64

1.62

2.23

-1.69

-3.65

.02

.40

.83

-3.78

-.57

.47

In Table IV an alternative empirical specification is presented. We performed an F-test to determine the efficacy of in- cluding the three groups, distinguished by the risk index, in a single equation. The F-statistic for the unrestricted specifica- tion (three separate equations) compared to the restricted specification (single equa- tion) is 2.20 versus the critical value of 2.10 (at 1 percent level). This implies that the three equations may be analytically dis- tinct. Thus, in Table IV we present three separate estimated equations.

As in the previous pooled model all results are reported with White's heteroscedastic correction. The test statis-

tics for the three equations (risk lovers, risk neutral, risk averse) are 2.16, 2.49, and 2.47, respectively. All exceed the 5 percent significance level, indicating that White's correction is needed.

As is illustrated, the overall significance of the individual regressions for the risk lover and risk averse groups are markedly improved. However, the conclusions drawn above for the single-equation model generally remain in effect. The con- trol variables continue to perform poorly. Moreover, the pattern for Seat Belt Use and Years W e a r i n g Seat Belt remains the same. The risk lover group demonstrates strong compensating behavior whereas the risk

Page 9: IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR

SINGH & THAYER SEAT BELT USE AND DRIVING BEHAVIOR 657

TABLE IV Impact of Seat Belt Use on Number of Tickets Group-Specific Estimates

Model I Model I1 Model I11 Explanatory Risk Lover Risk Neutral Risk Averse Variables Respondents Respondents Respondents

Cons tan t

Seat Belt Use

Years of Seat Be1 t Use

Education

Income

Sex

Age

Distance to Work

Underage Children

R-Square

Number of Observations

-.343 (-.62)

.319 (3.36)

-.006 (-.72)

-.OM (-1.97)

.067 (1.47)

.om (.55)

-.005 (-.71)

,004 (.82)

(1.88) .144

.14

125

.521 (1.62)

.lo9 (1.28)

-.009 (-2.35)

.028 (1.36)

-.0003 (-.017)

.021 (.30)

-.013 (-3.87)

-.001 (-1.32)

-.009 (-.31)

.05

432

3.73 (2.24)

-.883 (-1.55)

-.021 (-3.27)

-.008 (-.158)

-.019 (-.52)

.042 (.293)

-.006 (-.81)

(.77)

(-.54)

.004

-.039

.23

102

averse group does not compensate. Learn- ing (illustrated by the Years o f s e a t Belt Use variable) continues to be significantly dif- ferent from zero for the risk neutral and risk averse groups. However, this variable is not significant among the risk lover group, implying that this group shows minimal learning. Thus, compensating be- havior is not offset over time in this group.

Since the control variables do not per- form as expected, we eliminated them as a final test of the stability of the estimated coefficients. Our conclusions regarding

Seat Belt Use and Years Wear ing Seat Belt are unchanged. The estimated coefficients ( t - statistics in parentheses) on Seat Belt Use were .29 (3.71), .10 (1.22), and -.89 (-1.61) for the three groups (risk lovers, risk neu- tral, risk averse), respectively. In addition, the pattern of learning established above remains the same.

V. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND CAVEATS

Models based on individual-specific survey data are estimated to investigate the relationship between seat belt usage

Page 10: IMPACT OF SEAT BELT USE ON DRIVING BEHAVIOR

658 ECONOMIC INQUIRY

and the number of citations for moving violations. The analysis incorporates risk tastes of individuals as revealed by the degree of precaution exhibited against ev- eryday risks. The results indicate that the compensating-behavior hypothesis ap- plies only to those individuals who are not strongly risk averse. Conversely, seat belt use is associated with relatively fewer moving violations for the individuals who exhibit risk aversion. Taken together, the results imply that individual risk prefer- ences are an important dimension which should be considered when testing the compensating-behavior hypothesis. Ag- gregate models which do not make this distinction may be suspect.

Three caveats about our analysis are in order. First, the investigation is based on survey data which is generally less pre- ferred than revealed preference data. However, note that respondents had no incentive to misrepresent preferences since they were assured of complete ano- nymity. Second, our sample is not repre- sentative of the general population since it consists entirely of resident owners of single-family homes. Further analysis with other segments of the population is needed to verify our results. Third, proxy variables for evaluating risk preferences may not have accurately discerned the underlying risk state of each individual. The risk preference demarcations are de- veloped on the basis of revealed choices regarding everyday exposure to different types of risks such as theft, fire and earth- quakes. All these risky events entail poten- tial for bodily harm to self and/or family. Since the risk of an automobile accident has a similar dimension, one would expect consistent risk behavior across similar risks. However, further tests of the com- pensating-behavior hypothesis based on individual-specific analysis and risk pref- erences developed from other data sources need to be attempted.

REFERENCES

Blomquist, G. “A Utility Maximization Model of Driver Traffic Safety Behavior.” Accident Analysis and Prevention 18 (5), 1986, 371-75.

-. “Motorist Use of Safety Equipment: Expected Benefits or Risk Incompetence?” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 4(2), 1991, 135-52.

Campbell, B., and F. Campbell. “Injury Reduction and Belt Use Associated with Occupant Restraint Laws,” in Preventing Automobile Inju y: New Find- ingsfrom Evaluation Research, edited by J. Graham. Dover, Massachusetts: Auburn House Publishing Company, 1988, 24-50.

Dillman, D. Mail and Telephone Surveys: The Total Design Method. New York John Wiley and Sons, 1978.

Garbacz, Christopher. ”Estimating Seat Belt Effective- ness with Seat Belt Usage Data from the Centers for Disease Control.” Economic Letters 34(1), 1990, 83-88.

-. “Impact of the New Zealand Seat Belt Law.” Economic Inquiry, April, 1991a, 310-16.

-. “Are Rear Seat Passengers and Non-Occupants More at Risk Due to Seat Belt Laws?” Unpub- lished manuscript, University of Missouri-Rolla, 1991b.

-. ”More Evidence on the Effectiveness of Seat Belt Laws.” Applied Economics, forthcoming, 1992.

Lave, L., and W. Weber. “A Benefit-Cost Analysis of Auto Safety Features.” Applied Economics 4(1),

McCarthy, P. ”Seat Belt Usage Rates: A Test of Peltzman’s Hypothesis.” Accident Analysis and Prevention 18(5), 1986, 425-38.

McEwin, R. No Fault and Motor Vehicle Accidents, Work- ing Paper D105, Center for Policy Studies, Monash University, September, 1986, 1-30.

Peltzman, Sam. ”The Effect of Automobile Safety Reg- ulations.” Journal OJ Political Economy, August

-. “The Regulation of Automobile Safety,” in Auto Safety Regulation: 77ie Cure or the Problem?, edited by H. G. Manne and R. L. Miller. Glen Ridge, N.J.: Thomas Horton, 1976, 1-52.

Swan, P. “The Economics of Law: Economic Imperial- ism in Negligence Law, No Fault Insurance, Oc- cupational Licensing and Criminology.” Australian Economic Review, 3rd Quarter 1984,92- 108.

White, Halbert. ”A Heteroscedasticity-Consistent Co- variance Matrix and A Direct Test for Heteroscedasticity.” Econometrica 48, May 1980, 82146.

1970, 265-75.

1975, 677-726.