human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

199
Purdue University Purdue e-Pubs Open Access Dissertations eses and Dissertations Summer 2014 Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter dogs Sheryl Lynn Walker Purdue University Follow this and additional works at: hps://docs.lib.purdue.edu/open_access_dissertations Part of the Personality and Social Contexts Commons is document has been made available through Purdue e-Pubs, a service of the Purdue University Libraries. Please contact [email protected] for additional information. Recommended Citation Walker, Sheryl Lynn, "Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter dogs" (2014). Open Access Dissertations. 382. hps://docs.lib.purdue.edu/open_access_dissertations/382

Upload: others

Post on 11-Sep-2021

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

Purdue UniversityPurdue e-Pubs

Open Access Dissertations Theses and Dissertations

Summer 2014

Human and canine personality assessmentinstruments to predict successful adoptions withshelter dogsSheryl Lynn WalkerPurdue University

Follow this and additional works at: https://docs.lib.purdue.edu/open_access_dissertations

Part of the Personality and Social Contexts Commons

This document has been made available through Purdue e-Pubs, a service of the Purdue University Libraries. Please contact [email protected] foradditional information.

Recommended CitationWalker, Sheryl Lynn, "Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter dogs"(2014). Open Access Dissertations. 382.https://docs.lib.purdue.edu/open_access_dissertations/382

Page 2: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

30 08 14

PURDUE UNIVERSITY GRADUATE SCHOOL

Thesis/Dissertation Acceptance

Department

To the best of my knowledge and as understood by the student in the Thesis/Dissertation Agreement, Publication Delay, and Certification/Disclaimer (Graduate School Form 32), this thesis/dissertation adheres to the provisions of Purdue University’s “Policy on Integrity in Research” and the use of copyrighted material.

Sheryl Lynn Walker

Human and Canine Personality Assessment Instruments to Predict Successful Adoptions with ShelterDogs

Doctor of Philosophy

Niwako Ogata Sheila D'Arpino

Chris Eckhardt

Alan M. Beck

Hsin-Yi Weng

Niwako Ogata

Laurent Couetil 08/05/2014

Page 3: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter
Page 4: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

HUMAN AND CANINE PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT INSTRUMENTS TO PREDICT SUCCESSFUL

ADOPTIONS WITH SHELTER DOGS

A Dissertation

Submitted to the Faculty

of

Purdue University

by

Sheryl Lynn Walker

In Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Degree

of

Doctor of Philosophy

December 2014

Purdue University

West Lafayette, Indiana

Page 5: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

ii

This dissertation is dedicated to every shelter dog that I have had the pleasure of

spending time with – during behavior assessments, training sessions, or just some good ol’ TLC.

This dissertation is also dedicated to those adopters and shelter personnel who cared enough

about the human-animal bond to incorporate positive reinforcement and force-free training into

their lives. I hope I made a difference.

A huge shout-out goes to my husband, Bobby, my dogs, Luigi and Spud, and my angel,

Gracey, for teaching me more about life than I taught them. Life is a dance, and they took care

of me while I couldn’t. For that, I am truly thankful.

Page 6: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

iii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

My gratitude goes to my major advisor, Dr. Niwako Ogata, and to Dr. Andrew

Luescher, Dr. Annette Litster, and Dr. George Moore, for the opportunity to be a part of

the Maddie’s Shelter Medicine Program here at Purdue University. Through this

program, I was able to pursue a dream of mine to study dog behavior. By working in a

variety of settings, I felt that I had made a difference in several avenues by working with

shelter dogs and adopters. I am truly grateful for these four and a half years worth of

wonderful opportunities.

I would especially like to thank members of my dissertation committee, Dr. Alan

Beck, Dr. Hsin-Yi Weng, Dr. Christopher Eckhardt, and Dr. Sheila D’Arpino, who, along

with the previously mentioned mentors, have generously given their time and expertise

to better my work. Having high expectations of me, they have challenged me to use my

brain cells and scientifically think about how to apply what I have learned.

The research described in this dissertation, was sponsored by the Maddie’s®

Shelter Medicine Program at Purdue University and funded by Maddie’s Fund®, whose

“mission is to revolutionize the status and well-being of companion animals. Maddie’s

Fund® is helping to achieve and sustain a no-kill nation by providing solutions to the

most challenging issues facing the animal welfare community through its grant-giving,

Page 7: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

iv

hands-on animal care, and research and education” (www.maddiesfund.org). Efforts

toward research and education are of the utmost importance to animal shelters that

strive to use the best current practices.

To my behavioral mentors and colleagues, Dr. Sara Bennett, Julie Shaw, Mindy

Cotton, Carolyn Butera, Colleen Benson, and Connie Swaim, thank you for sharing a

passion of mine. To everyone in the Animal Behavior Management Alliance, Karen Pryor

Academy, Association for Behavior Analysis International, American Humane

Association, and Association of Professional Dog Trainers, you have all been such an

inspiration to me – there are so many wonderful people out in this world doing amazing

things with animals. Thank you for the inspiration. I have continued to embrace Dr.

Richard Malott’s famous advice from during my Master’s degree: “Save the World with

Behavior Analysis.”

I am grateful to my friends who have shared the joys and frustrations of my

graduate work, especially my office mate Dr. Jamieson Nichols, who was always there to

share tears of anxiety and frustration, and to share good food and chocolate with me.

Thank-you to Dr. Rick Meilan, for teaching me that it is OK to not know the answer to

everything, and for his time editing and reviewing my papers. Thank-you to Dr. Ruth

Landau, for being my predecessor as a Maddie’s® Fund Ph.D. Student – she taught me a

lot of the ropes, and that it is important to have a good attitude and sense of humor. I

must acknowledge my fellow shelter workers at Almost Home Humane Society; my

fellow shelter workers at Clinton County Humane Society: Amy Redington, Cassie Tate,

Jim Tate, Jessica Stitt, Donna Wells, Tammy Chasse, and Lora Ford; and my fellow

Page 8: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

v

shelter workers at the Humane Society of Indianapolis. Data collection for this project

would not be possible if it weren’t for Katy Anderson, my Research Technician, who

devoted a very large proportion of her time traveling and working at the Humane

Society of Indianapolis six days a week for 14 months.

I am also very grateful for having an exceptional family who has loved and

supported me unconditionally over the years. They have given up so much for me to

pursue my dreams. To my mother Barb Wright, my father Dave Groessl, Grandma Sell,

Grandma and Grandpa Groessl, my aunts, uncles, cousins, and extended family – thank-

you for your support. To my close friends who not friends but extended family: Dr. Amy

Lossie, Kay Berry, Dawn Caram-Schleicher, Jennifer Trump, Heather McAlister, Julie

Bose, Becky Englund, Adrienne West, Derrick Sell, Andrea Crosby, Ashley Barbour, and

Shannon Roulo, I am so lucky to have you all in my life. To hear that you all are proud of

me brings me to tears, and I am so thankful for those words of encouragement.

To the friends that I have made during my career at Purdue University, thank you

for reminding me that there is such a thing called a “social life” and there is a world

outside of work and school.

I especially want to thank my best friend and husband, Bobby, for sacrificing

many things the past several years so that I could pursue my education. I hope I

convinced him that “someday” this will all be worth it.

Page 9: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

vii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

LIST OF TABLES……………………………………………………………………………………………….…………….xii

LIST OF FIGURES…………………………………………………………………………………………………….…….xiv

ABSTRACT ………………………………..………………………………………………………………………………….xv

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION……………………………………….………..………………..……………………..1

CHAPTER 2. LITERATURE REVIEW………………………….……………………………………………………….5

BACKGROUND INFORMATION……………………………………………………….…………………..6

The Reality of Animal Relinquishment………………………………………….…………6

Relinquishment Risk Factors……………………………………………………….…………..7

Dog Behavior vs. Personality………………………………………………….……………….8

Canine Personality Assessments………………………………………………….………..11

Human Personality………………………………………………………………….…………….16

Human Personality Assessments……………………………………………………….….18

Human Relationship Satisfaction……………………….…………………………….……20

Human-Animal Bond……………………………………………………………….…………….21

Matching Families and Pets…………………………………………………………………..23

Study Relevance……………………………………………………………………………………26

References………………………………………..………………………………………………………………28

CHAPTER 3. MATERIALS AND METHODS………………………………………………………………………42

Methods……………………………………………………………………….………………………………….43

Ethical Approval…………………………………………………………………………………….43

Page 10: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

viii

Page

Questionnaires……………………………………………………………………………………..43

Study Eligibility and Recruitment.………………………………………….………………49

Data Collection Sites and Subjects……….……………………………………………..…………….53

Clinton County Humane Society……………………………………………………………53

Humane Society of Indianapolis………………………………………………………….…54

Dog Demographics………………………………………………………………………………..56

Staff, Volunteers, and Fosters……………………………………………………………….57

Contingencies……………………………………………………………………………………….58

Returned Dogs………………………………………………………………………………………59

Data Management and Analysis……………………………………………………………………….59

Data Security…………………………………………………………………………………………59

Participation and Response Rate…………………………………………………………..60

Descriptive Statistics – Enrolled Dogs……………………………………………………61

References………………………………………………………………………………………………………..63

CHAPTER 4. COMPARISON OF DOG PERSONALITY RATINGS BY RELINQUISHERS AND ADOPTERS AT TWO INDIANA SHELTERS……………………………………………………………………….65

Introduction……………………………………………………………………………………………………..66

Materials and Methods…..……………………………….……………………………………………….68

Study Participants…………………………………………………………………………………68

Dog Personalities…………………………………………………………………………………..69

Statistical Analyses………………………………………………………………………………..70

Results……………….……………………………………………………………………………………………..70

Descriptive Statistics……………………………………………………………………………..70

Comparison of Percent Scores for each Personality Trait between Relinquishers and Adopters…………………………………………………………………..71

Page 11: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

ix

Page

Agreement of Primary Dog Personality Categories between Relinquishers and Adopters…………………………………………………………………………………………76

Discussion…………………………………………………………………………………………………………77

References………………………………………………………………………………………………………..84

CHAPTER 5. ASSESSMENT OF ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN DOG AND HUMAN PERSONALITY TRAITS USING THE MCPQ-R AND IPIP QUESTIONNAIRES….……………………………………..……87

Introduction……………………………………………………………………………………………………..88

Materials and Methods…..……………………………….……………………………………………….91

Study Participants…………………………………………………………………………………91

Dog and Human Personalities……………………………………………………………….93

Statistical Analyses………………………………………………………………………………..94

Results – Study A…….………………………………………………………………………………………..95

Descriptive Statistics……………………………………………………………………………..95

Associations between Primary Dog and Human Personality Traits………..95

Results – Study B………………………………………………………………………………………………97

Descriptive Statistics……………………………………………………………………………..97

Associations between Primary Dog and Human Personality Traits………..97

Results – Study C………………………………………………………………………………………………98

Descriptive Statistics……………………………………………………………………………..98

Associations between Primary Dog and Human Personality Traits………..99

Discussion……………………………………………………………………………………………………….101

References………………………………………………………………………………………………………104

CHAPTER 6. ASSESSMENT OF DOG PERSONALITY, HUMAN PERSONALITY, AND THEIR INTERACTION TO PREDICT SUCCESSFUL ADOPTIONS OF SHELTER DOGS………..………….109

Introduction……………………………………………………………………………………………………110

Materials and Methods…..……………………………….……………………………………………..112

Page 12: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

x

Page

Study Participants……………………………………………………………………………….112

Dog Personality, Human Personality, and Human-Animal Bond………….113

Statistical Analyses………………………………………………………………………………115

Results……………….……………………………………………………………………………………………117

Descriptive Statistics……………………………………………………………………………117

Assessment of Dog Personality and Human Personality as Predictors for the Success of Adoptions…………………………………………………………………….118

Assessment of the Interaction between Dog and Human Personality as a Predictor for the Success of Adoptions………………………………………………..124

Discussion……………………………………………………………………………………………………….125

Assessment of Dog Personality and Human Personality as Predictors for the Success of Adoptions…………………………………………………………………….126

Assessment of the Interaction between Dog and Human Personality as a Predictor for the Success of Adoptions………………………………………………..127

References…………………………………………………………..…………………………………………132

CHAPTER 7. CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS……………………………………………….136

Conclusion………………………………………………………………………………………………………137

Future Directions…………………………………………………………………………………………….138

Adoption Counseling and Dog-Adopter Matching Programs……………….138

Post-Adoption Resources…………………………………………………………………….139

APPENDICES

Appendix A. Randomized Controlled Pilot Study…………………………………………….140

Appendix B. Monash Canine Personality Questionnaires-Revised (CCHS and HSI – Relinquishment)………………………………………………………………………..……………………145 Appendix C. International Personality Item Pool (CCHS and HSI)…………………….150

Appendix D. Monash Canine Personality Questionnaires-Revised (CCHS AND HSI – Adoption)…………………….………………………………….…………………………………………..155

Page 13: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

xi

Page

Appendix E. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (CCHS AND HSI)…………..………160

Appendix F. Wellness Handouts………………………………………………………………………165

Appendix G. Dog Personality Categories (Laminated)……………………………………..167

Appendix H. Information Sheet………………….……………………………………………………169

VITA………………………………………………………………………………………..………………………………….172

Page 14: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

xii

LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

Table 1. MCPQ-R score sheet……………………………………………………………………………………….45

Table 2. The 50-item IPIP score sheet…………………………………………………………………………..47

Table 3. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale score sheet…………………………………….……….49

Table 4. Length of Stay in Days for Study Dogs at CCHS and HSI……………………………………62

Table 5. Primary dog personality categories rated by relinquishers and adopters………..77

Table 6. Frequency Counts of Primary Relinquisher-Rated Dog and Human Personality

Traits (n=262)…………………………………………………………………………………………………………….…96

Table 7. Frequency Counts of Primary Adopter-Rated Dog and Human Personality Traits

(n=251)………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….……..98

Table 8. Frequency Counts of Primary Relinquisher-Rated and Human Personality Traits

(n=197)……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….100

Table 9. Frequency Counts of Primary Adopter-Rated Dog and Human Personality Traits

(n=197)……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………...101

Table 10. Dog Personality Categories Rated by Relinquisher vs. Human Personality

Categories at Adoption…………………………………………………………………………………………….…118

Table 11. Relinquisher-Rated Dog Personality Prediction of Satisfaction………………….…120

Table 12. Human Personality Prediction of Satisfaction………………………………………………121

Page 15: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

xiii

Table Page

Table 13. Frequency Counts of Relinquisher-Rated Dog Personality and Returned

Dogs……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….….121

Table 14. Frequency Counts of Human Personality and Returned Dogs……………………..122

Table 15. Adopter-Rated Dog Personality Prediction of Satisfaction (n=183)………….….123

Table 16. Adopter-Rated Dog Personality Prediction of Satisfaction (n=234)……………..123

Page 16: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

xiv

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure Page

Figure 1. Comparison of Percent Scores for Extraversion between Relinquishers and

Adopters………………………………………………………………………………………………………………..…….72

Figure 2. Comparison of Percent Scores for Motivation between Relinquishers and

Adopters………………………………………………………………………………………………………………..…….73

Figure 3. Comparison of Percent Scores for Training Focus between Relinquishers and

Adopters………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….…..74

Figure 4. Comparison of Percent Scores for Amicability between Relinquishers and

Adopters……………………………………………………………………………………………………………..……….75

Figure 5. Comparison of Percent Scores for Neuroticism between Relinquishers and

Adopters……………………………………………………………………………………………………..……………….76

Page 17: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

xv

ABSTRACT

Walker, Sheryl Lynn Ph.D., Purdue University, December 2014. Human and Canine Personality Assessment Instruments to Predict Successful Adoptions with Shelter Dogs. Major Professor: Niwako Ogata.

Animal shelters are often over-crowded with animals, and efforts to match

potential adopters with shelter dogs, to improve the quality of adoptions, are

increasing. However, a lack of evidence-based practices makes matching difficult. This

research was conducted to investigate the role of dog and human personality, using

questionnaire-based measurements, on adoption success in two Indiana shelters,

Clinton County Humane Society and the Humane Society of Indianapolis. Ultimately, the

aim of this project was to assess dog personality, human personality, and satisfaction, to

evaluate adoption success in shelter dogs.

The present thesis contains three studies exploring dog and human personality

traits, and their possible effect on an adopter’s satisfaction. The first study, presented in

Chapter 4, assessed the agreement of rating dog personality between the relinquishers

and adopters. The Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R) was

distributed to owners relinquishing their dogs and also to adopters of those dogs two

months post-adoption. The MCPQ-R is a 26-item questionnaire which categorizes canine

personality traits based on adjective ratings. Possible personality categories were:

Page 18: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

xvi

Extraversion, Motivation, Training Focus, Amicability, and Neuroticism. Relinquisher-

rated dog personality was compared to adopter-rated dog personality of each dog

(n=197), and results show a lack of agreement between the two responders.

The second study, presented in Chapter 5, assessed the relationships between

dog personality and human personality. Because the previous chapter’s results found

that relinquishers and adopters did not agree on rating dog personality, both

relinquisher-rated and adopter-rated dog personality were used to assess relationships

with human personality. The MCPQ-R data were compared to the 50-item International

Personality Item Pool questionnaire (IPIP) completed by the adopter. Human personality

was measured with the IPIP, which categorizes personality traits based on the Five-

Factor Model (FFM): Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Stability, and

Openness. Results suggested that there were no associations between dog and human

personality.

The third study, presented in Chapter 6, analyzed the predictability of adoption

success with dog personality, human personality, and their interactions. The adopter’s

satisfaction with the new dog was measured using the Lexington Attachment to Pets

Scale (LAPS), a 23-item questionnaire. Using linear and logistic regression, the only

statistically significant associations found were between adopter-rated dog personality

and mean LAPS score, and adopter-rated dog personality and satisfaction. Those

adopters who rated their dogs as Motivated, Training Focused, and Amicable were 4.2,

3.1, and 2.2 times more likely, respectively, to be satisfied with the adopted dog than

those who rated their dogs as Extraverted. Additionally, those adopters who rated their

Page 19: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

xvii

dogs as Neurotic were 0.4 times less likely to be satisfied with the adopted dog than

those who rated their dogs as Extraverted.

Collectively, the results presented in this thesis provide a foundation to

encourage animal shelters to shift their programs away from personality matching and

towards other programs which may better promote a healthy human-animal bond.

Page 20: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

1

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

Page 21: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

2

Millions of dogs are surrendered every year to animal shelters due to a variety of

reasons. Yet to this author’s knowledge, very little research has investigated the role of

personality in retention or the satisfaction of the dog post-adoption. If dog and human

personality influence adoption satisfaction, then efforts can be made in animal shelters

to promote “good” matches for successful adoptions based on personality. In the

current studies, a successful adoption was defined as one where the dog was retained in

the home (not returned to the shelter), and where the adopter was satisfied and had

developed a strong bond with the animal. Satisfaction and human-animal bond are used

synonymously throughout this dissertation. The current research takes a cross-species

approach to examine the influence of dog and human personality on the human-animal

bond and adoption success.

Ratings of dog personality between dog owners and their peers have been found

to have moderate to strong correlations ranging from 0.75 to 0.86 for inter-rater

reliability. However; agreement of rating dog personality has yet to be assessed with

shelter dogs. Dog personality may be an important factor when considering matching

shelter dogs to potential adopters; if dog personality is rated differently between

relinquishers and adopters, this may have implications on how a matching program is

facilitated.

Significant associations have been established between human and dog

personality traits, primarily using methods to measure human personality to also

measure dog personality. The interpretations of these results are questionable, as

components of dog personality may not necessarily be reflected using a human

Page 22: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

3

personality measure. Therefore, the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised

(MCPQ-R) was chosen in the current study to measure dog personality. The 50-item

International Personality Item Pool (IPIP) was chosen in the current project to measure

human personality because of its ease of administration in the shelter setting, and its

reliability of measuring the Five Factor Model of human personality.

There is an abundance of research investigating the influence of human

personality traits on interpersonal relationship factors (e.g., marital satisfaction).

Applying that same concept with a cross-species approach to dog ownership, recent

research has found that dog personalities influence relationship satisfaction. The current

study expands this theory using reliable measures of dog personality, human

personality, and satisfaction of adopted shelter dogs. The Lexington Attachment to Pets

Scale (LAPS) was used to measure satisfaction.

Encouraging appropriate matches between dogs and adopters is a main concern

of animal shelters. However, dog personality, human personality, and the relationships

between the two are not necessarily reflective of an appropriate match. If the

interaction between dog and human personality has an effect on success of adoption,

then dog-human matches might be an important factor to be considered at adoption.

Specific Aims

1.) To compare different responders’ ratings of dog personality. The MCPQ-R was

used to assess canine personality with both the owner surrendering the dog and

the adopter of the same dog as the responders.

Page 23: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

4

2.) To investigate relationships between dog personality and human personality.

Relinquisher-rated and adopter-rated dog personalities were assessed in

comparison with human personality, which was measured using the IPIP.

3.) To assess the effects of dog personality, human personality, and their

interactions on mean LAPS scores, returned dogs, and satisfaction.

Page 24: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

5

CHAPTER 2. LITERATURE REVIEW

Page 25: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

6

BACKGROUND INFORMATION

The Reality of Animal Relinquishment

There are approximately five to seven million homeless dogs in the United

States, who are temporarily cared for in animal shelters (Humane Society of the United

States, 2010; National Council on Pet Population Study & Policy, 2009). As a result,

millions of dogs are being euthanized annually across the country (Salman et al., 1998).

Quite often the adoptions of animals do not counterbalance the influx of animals at

intake (Mondelli et al., 2004; Moulton et al., 1991), making it more difficult for shelter

management personnel to make placement and euthanasia decisions in order to

manage the population of animals in their care. Many dogs are transferred in from

rescues or animal-care-and-control facilities as strays, but large numbers are also

surrendered by owners. This dissertation focuses on surrendered dogs and their

adoptions into new homes.

Behavioral problems can often impact the relationship between dogs and their

owners, and are often a precursor to relinquishment decisions (Arkow and Dow, 1984;

Curb et al., 2013; Mondelli et al., 2004; Patronek et al., 1996; Salman et al., 1998, 2000).

Problematic behaviors that warrant relinquishment include: aggression, separation-

related behavior, house soiling, fearful behavior, destructiveness, disobedience, digging,

chewing, and excessive barking (Mondelli et al., 2004; Salman et al., 1998, 2000; Weng

et al., 2006).

Other common reasons for relinquishment include, but are not limited to:

moving, too many animals in the current household, cohabitating animals not getting

Page 26: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

7

along, cost of pet maintenance, owner having personal problems, inadequate facilities,

lack of time or money, veterinary care was too expensive, the owner simply was not

ready for, or aware of, the responsibilities of owning a companion animal, or the owner

had some unrealistic expectations of the dog (Mondelli et al., 2004; Salman et al., 1998).

In a study by Scarlet et al. (1999), health and personal issues ranked third after

human-housing issues and animal behavior problems as reasons for surrender.

Regarding health and personal issues, the main reasons for relinquishment include:

divorce, the need to travel, the lack of time for an animal, or children-related reasons

such as pregnancy, the birth of a child, or incompatibility between pet and child (Scarlett

et al., 1999).

Relinquishment Risk Factors

Surrendered dogs were more likely to be sexually intact and younger than three

years of age (Salman et al., 1998, 2000), but older than five months of age (Salman et

al., 1998). Some studies have shown that surrendered dogs tend to be owned for less

than one year (Kidd et al., 1992; Salman et al., 1998); another study has shown that

unsuccessful owners owned the dog for less than two years, compared to successful

owners who had owned the dog for over five years (Weng et al., 2006). More research is

needed to investigate the effects of length of ownership on relinquishment. It is often

thought that without a monetary value on an animal, the owner will not become

attached (Weiss and Gramann, 2009). Dogs acquired at no cost were also at an

increased risk of relinquishment (New et al., 2000; Patronek et al., 1996; Salman et al.,

Page 27: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

8

2000; Scarlett et al., 1999); however, more recent research has found that cats acquired

for free, through an animal shelter, were no more likely to be surrendered than cats

whose adoptions were fee-based (Weiss and Gramann, 2009).

Owners with a history of losing or relinquishing their dogs were more likely to

repeat the same behaviors in the future, of losing or rehoming the dog, than were those

owners without such a history (Weng et al., 2006). A change in the family structure,

including the addition or removal of animals in the household, also puts animals at risk

(Salman et al., 2000). Owners were more likely to surrender their dog if they did not

have an educational level beyond high school (Salman et al., 2000); lived in an

apartment (Mondelli et al., 2004); and were first-time adopters (Kidd et al., 1992). Men

were more likely than women to surrender their pet to a shelter (Kass et al., 2001; Kidd

et al., 1992); larger families were more likely than smaller families to surrender (Kidd et

al., 1992); and because with children comes allergies, dog bites, and a time/money

commitment, households with children were more likely than households with no

children to surrender their animals (Kidd et al., 1992).

Dog Behavior vs. Personality

It is critical to differentiate between dog behavior and personality. Dog behavior

is defined by an individual dog’s response to environmental stimuli (Mirko et al., 2013).

Personality, on the other hand, describes an animal’s overall way of responding to the

environment, based on its genetic background and past experiences (Draper, 1995;

Krueger and Johnson, 2008).

Page 28: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

9

Individual behavioral differences in dogs can be measured by quantifiable data,

such as observing and documenting dog body language and recording occurrences,

frequencies, and durations of behaviors with behavioral coding (Martin and Bateson,

2007). Behavioral coding involves training people to observe and score behaviors in

accordance with pre-determined criteria (Highfill et al., 2010; Stephen and Ledger,

2007). This type of standardization is more objective than ratings on a questionnaire,

which may be subjective (Gosling, 2003). Often, ethograms are used in behavioral

coding research so that behavioral definitions are clearly stated and the respondents are

trained to observe those behaviors. However, the behavioral coding method is rarely

applied in animal shelters due to lack of time and training, and also lack of consistency

of established behavioral definitions (e.g., ethograms) among people who interact with

the dogs (e.g., shelter personnel and volunteers).

Individual behavioral differences can also be measured using various test

batteries, which involve manipulating the animal’s environment and documenting

behavioral responses (Diederich and Giffroy, 2006). The Swedish Working Dog

Association (SWDA) developed the Dog Mentality Assessment (DMA) as a tool to

measure breeding dogs’ behavioral reactions to novel stimuli (Svartberg, 2002, 2005;

Svartberg and Forkman, 2002; Svartberg et al., 2005). Although often referred to as a

personality test (Svartberg, 2002, 2005; Svartberg and Forkman, 2002; Svartberg et al.,

2005), the DMA is an extensive behavioral test battery, containing a single assessment

of ten subtests measuring social contact, interest in playing and chasing toys, and startle

reactions to stimuli such as a metallic noise, strangers, and gunshots. Although the DMA

Page 29: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

10

has been used in tandem with performance and obedience tests to compare a dog’s

behavioral reactions with its learning ability (Svartberg, 2002), it cannot be considered a

personality assessment because it does not measure an animal’s habits or patterns of

behavioral responses over time, or across different environments.

Commonly used behavioral test batteries in animal shelters include SAFERTM

(Weiss, 2007b) and the Modified Assess-a-Pet (Bollen and Horowitz, 2008) to evaluate

dogs for handling sensitivities, sociability, playfulness, and potential aggressive

tendencies. These behavior assessments are conducted on dogs in shelters to predict

suitability for adoption, guide enrichment options, make training and treatment

decisions, and match dogs to potential adopters. These assessments, however, do not

assess personality, but instead assess a dog’s behavioral responses to specific stimuli in

a specific environment at a specific point in time (Bennett et al., 2012; Bollen and

Horowitz, 2008; Jones and Gosling, 2005; Kis et al., 2014; Mirko et al., 2013; Poulsen et

al., 2010; van der Borg et al., 2010).

Personality is the set of characteristics, or traits, that describe behavioral

tendencies that persist across time and situations, and are used to differentiate

between individuals (Dowling-Guyer et al., 2011; Pervin and John, 1997; Weinstein et

al., 2008). Inferences of personality traits can be made by observation of behavior over

a period of time (Watanabe et al., 2012); behaviors observed are the animal’s habits of

responding to environmental stimuli (e.g., researchers exposed hermit crabs to different

novel objects or environments over a six-week time span to measure the animal’s

Page 30: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

11

willingness to explore, thus measuring the personality trait “Exploration-Avoidance”)

(Watanabe et al., 2012).

Because measuring dog personality cannot be completed by the animal itself, we

rely on humans for this information; rating personality is a common and reliable way to

examine individual differences in animals, especially when the respondents are well

acquainted with the subjects (Gosling et al., 1998, 2003; Highfill et al., 2010; Jones and

Gosling, 2005; Ley et al., 2008).

Canine Personality Assessments

There is a lack of consensus among professionals regarding the construction,

components, and meaning of dog personality traits (Jones and Gosling, 2005). Among

the literature, a meta-analysis by Jones and Gosling (2005) showed that there were

seven overall categories that depict canine personality: Reactivity, Fearfulness, Activity,

Sociability, Responsiveness to Training, Submissiveness, and Aggression. Four main dog

personality assessments have been reviewed in the literature: the canine-Big Five

Inventory (canine-BFI) (Gosling et al., 2003), the Dog Personality Questionnaire (DPQ)

(Mirko et al., 2013), a proprietary questionnaire developed by Curb et al. (2013), and the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R) (Ley et al., 2008, 2009a, b).

Canine-Big Five Inventory (canine-BFI)

The canine-BFI, a four-factor model of canine personality (Gosling et al., 2003),

was shown to include the following dimensions: Energy, Affection, Emotional Reactivity,

Page 31: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

12

and Intelligence. These were analogous to the human personality traits Extraversion,

Agreeableness, Neuroticism, and Openness, respectively (Cavanaugh et al., 2008;

Gosling et al., 2003). The canine-BFI uses the human version of the Big Five Inventory

(John and Srivastava, 1999), but changes some wording of human-directed behavior,

thoughts, and feelings so that the questions are representative of observable canine

behavior (Cavanaugh et al., 2008; Gosling et al., 2003). Cronbach’s coefficient alpha

across each scale was computed for both the BFI and canine-BFI when completed by the

dog’s owner: the alphas ranged from 0.80 (Neuroticism) to 0.83 (Extraversion and

Openness) for the BFI, and from 0.77 (Extraversion) to 0.89 (Neuroticism) for the canine-

BFI (Gosling et al., 2003). The inter-observer agreement was measured using consensus

correlations, which ranged from 0.55 (Agreeableness and Openness) to 0.76

(Extraversion).

Dog Personality Questionnaire (DPQ)

The DPQ was created by researchers evaluating three phases of questionnaire

development, including focus groups to generate the items and Principal Components

Analysis (PCA) to assess the factorial structure of the questionnaire. All of the items

were systematically clumped together depending on how related they are to each

other, and each of these clumps is considered a factor, or personality trait (Mirko et al.,

2013). The four factors, Stranger-directed Sociability, Activity, Aggressiveness, and

Trainability resulted from the PCA. The DPQ is a 75-item questionnaire administered to

dog owners, and contains items that describe broad personality statements (e.g., “Dog

Page 32: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

13

is shy”) with behavior statements (e.g., “When walking on leash, dog tends to pull

ahead”) (Jones and Gosling, 2005; Mirko et al., 2013). When external validity was

assessed, significant positive associations were found between the length of time the

owner spent with the dog and the personality trait Stranger-directed Sociability

(p=0.04). Furthermore, the personality trait Activity was significantly scored higher with

younger dogs than older dogs (p=0.01), and dogs enrolled in training classes were

significantly scored higher on Trainability than those dogs without any training

background (p<0.01). Interestingly, those dogs enrolled in training classes were

characterized as being more Aggressive than those dogs without any training

background (p<0.01) (Mirko et al., 2013).

Curb et al. (2013) Personality Questionnaire

Researchers have also utilized their own versions of canine personality

measurements, prepared by focus groups and interviews with dog professionals (e.g.,

veterinarians, humane society staff, and dog trainers) to combine frequently used

adjectives and descriptions into matching dog and human personality dimensions, (Curb

et al., 2013). They identified eight overlapping traits between pet owners and dogs,

which included: aggressive/cowardliness, outgoing/shy, noisy/quiet, anxious/calm,

playful/sluggish, affectionate/not affectionate, creative-curious/not creative-curious,

and independent/dependent (Curb et al., 2013). Unfortunately, no reliability or validity

assessments of this particular personality tool have been reported.

Page 33: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

14

Canine Personality Assessment Limitations

Caution should also be warranted when extracting dog personality dimensions

from human personality traits, because they may not necessarily be representative of

canine analogues of human personality (e.g., Conscientiousness may be considered a

human-only personality trait that is unable to be measured in dogs, because it measures

an individual’s self-discipline, organization, and achievement-motivation) (Ley et al.,

2008). Studies of dog behavior and personality also often have a limited sample size,

complicating generalization of results to the population of interest, or draw their

conclusions from specific populations of dogs (e.g., dogs trained for specific purposes

(Wilsson and Sundgren, 1997), or certain breeds of dogs (Svartberg, 2005), so

personality findings may not be representative of the entire dog population (Ley et al.,

2009a).

Also, the interpretation of the personality traits themselves can fluctuate. For

example, one of the canine personality trait Training Focus is associated with being

attentive and obedient (Ley et al., 2008). However, the term attentive can also be used

to describe a specific situation: a dog may be attentive when learning new behaviors

and treats are given out as reinforcers, but the same dog may not be attentive when on

a walk, where there are a lot of different sights and smells (Curb et al., 2013).

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R)

Reliability studies have been conducted at Monash University, assessing the

suitability of the MCPQ-R to measure dog personality traits (Ley et al., 2008, 2009a, b).

Page 34: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

15

First, veterinarians and dog owners were asked to make a list of all applicable

personality adjectives that describe dogs. Through Principal Components Analysis (PCA),

all the available adjectives were systematically clumped together depending on how

related they are to each other, and each of these clumps is considered a factor, or

personality trait (Ley et al., 2008). Five canine personality traits resulted: Extraversion,

Motivation, Training Focus, Amicability (possibly similar to Svartberg’s Sociability

dimension (Svartberg and Forkman, 2002)), and Neuroticism (Ley et al., 2008).

Extraversion describes the energy level of the dog; Motivation characterizes the dog’s

persistence in achieving a goal, even with distractions. Training Focus describes the

dog’s level of trainability. Amicability refers to how sociable the dog is with people or

other dogs. The last trait, Neuroticism, refers to how cautious the dog is (Ley et al.,

2009a). It was revealed that the canine personality category “Extraversion” was similar

in content to the human personality category “Extraversion.” In addition, the canine

personality category “Neuroticism” contained similar items to the human personality

category “Neuroticism” (referred to as “Stability” in this dissertation) (Costa and

McCrae, 1992a; Marsh et al., 2010).

However, differences were also noted between canine and human personality

factors in the primary studies (Ley et al., 2008). Items grouped under the canine

personality category labeled “Self-Assuredness/Motivation” were present on several

human personality dimensions. Items grouped under the canine personality category

“Amicability” were present on both human personality dimensions labeled

“Agreeableness” and “Extraversion.” Finally, the canine personality category labeled

Page 35: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

16

“Training Focus” possibly could be related to the human personality category

“Conscientiousness.” However, because of selective pressures on dogs living with

humans, the differences seen in this category might be attributed to personality

categories which are distinct from humans and unique to canines. When the five dog

personality traits noted by Ley et al. (2009) were compared to Jones and Gosling’s

(2005) dimension classification, Amicability was similar to Sociability, Extraversion was

similar to Activity, Motivation and Training Focus were similar to Responsiveness to

training, and Neuroticism was similar to Fearfulness (Fratkin et al., 2013).

The internal reliability, or how consistent the results are for different items of

the same personality trait, for each of these traits showed Cronbach’s alpha values

between 0.74 and 0.87, consistent with moderate internal reliability (Ley et al., 2009a).

Further research analyzing reliability of the MCPQ-R (Ley et al., 2009b) showed

moderate to strong intra-class correlations, assessing the difference between

personality trait scores. The values ranged from 0.75 (Neuroticism) to 0.86

(Extraversion) for inter-rater reliability and 0.79 (Neuroticism) to 0.93 (Motivation) for

test-retest reliability; suggesting that these specific dog personality traits were

consistent (Fratkin et al., 2013; Ley et al., 2009b).

Human Personality

As mentioned previously, personality consists of a set of distinct characteristics,

behavioral tendencies, and social traits that distinguish individuals from each other

(Costa and McCrae, 1992a, b; Ley et al., 2008). Traits and tendencies imply stability;

Page 36: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

17

when exposed to similar environments, a person will react in a similar manner across

situations. Personality traits define how people differ from each other in their

behavioral tendencies; an Extraverted person may tend to be more talkative than

someone who is Introverted, where that person’s average tendencies are to talk a lot.

However, that is not to say that an Introverted person could not be talkative in certain

situations.

Theories of human personality traits have existed since 1937, when Allport

produced 4,504 descriptive personality terms from a dictionary (Allport, 1937), which

were later reduced to 171 and characterized into 16 key traits using factor analysis

(Cattell, 1946). Eysenck (1975) then later proposed a three-factor “Psychoticism-

Extraversion-Neuroticism” model of personality, but the most current and widely used

assessment of personality is the Five-Factor Model (FFM) that includes the following

personality dimensions: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional

Stability, and Openness to new experiences (Costa and McCrae, 1992a, b; Goldberg,

1990; Ley et al., 2008). Extraversion is displayed through a higher degree of sociability,

assertiveness, activity, adventurousness, excitement seeking, and talkativeness.

Agreeableness is categorized by being helpful, cooperative, altruistic, compliant,

modest, straightforward, trustworthy, and sympathetic towards others.

Conscientiousness is displayed by being self-disciplined, organized, deliberate, non-

impulsive, and achievement-oriented. Emotional Stability, or Neuroticism, is exemplified

by the degree of emotional stability, impulse control, hostility, vulnerability, and

anxiety. Finally, Openness refers to having a strong intellectual curiosity and a

Page 37: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

18

preference for novelty and variety, along with openness to feelings, actions, ideas, and

values. Specific traits are correlated within each dimension, or factor (Cavanaugh et al.,

2008; Costa and McCrae, 1992a, b; Goldberg, 1990; White et al., 2004). When a person

is scored on these factors, an overview of that individual’s personality is described.

In the literature, these personality traits are also considered the “Big Five,”

which may be a bit misleading, implying that there are only five different human

personality dimensions. The FFM is a more accurate and representative phrase,

depicted as a matrix between clusters of variables and separate factors. The factors are

the personality dimensions and the variables are the descriptors that are related to

separate factors (Cavanaugh et al., 2008; Costa and McCrae, 1992a, b; Goldberg, 1990;

White et al., 2004). With PCA, the clusters of variables would be highly correlated with

each other and therefore grouped under a factor separate from variables that are not

correlated at all (Jolliffe, 2002). It is assumed that variables and factors are independent

(Jolliffe, 2002), and the ultimate goal of PCA would be to reduce the number of items

representing each dimension.

Human Personality Assessments

Self-report data are the most common way to reveal information about

individuals, because they have access to information about themselves that is otherwise

private (Larson and Buss, 2010). Interviews, periodic reporting of events, and

questionnaires are examples of self-report data (Larsen and Buss, 2010). Structured

questionnaires (e.g., the International Personality Item Pool (IPIP) (Goldberg et al., 2006;

Page 38: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

19

Socha et al., 2010)) contain closed-ended questions, and provide response options

“Strongly Agree” to “Strongly Disagree.” The three questionnaires described in the

current dissertation use a Likert rating scale to measure the degree to which each

description or question accurately describes the individual.

Common measurements of human personality are utilized for research and

clinical purposes. The Neuroticism-Extraversion-Openness Personality Inventory Revised

(NEO-PI-R) (Costa and McCrae, 1992a, b; Vassand and Skrondal, 2011) contains 240

items, and was developed for use in assessing individuals with mental illness. Other

measures of personality, such as the 567-item Minnesota Multiphasic Personality

Inventory—2 (MMPI-2) (Forbey and Ben-Porath, 2007; Wise et al., 2010), is considered

the gold standard when assessing those diagnosed with psychopathology. The Big Five

Inventory (BFI) (John et al., 1991; John and Srivastava, 1999), containing 44 short

phrases for respondents to rate on a Likert scale, is a reliable measure of the “Big Five”

model of personality. The IPIP (http://ipip.ori.org) is a publicly available collection of

over 2,000 items, each in the form of a short phrase used to measure various human

personality traits (Goldberg et al., 2006). The 50-item IPIP (http://ipip.ori.org/New_IPIP-

50-item-scale.htm) is a common, reliable, and valid tool to measure the Five Factor

Model (FFM) of human personality traits: Extraversion, Agreeableness,

Conscientiousness, Stability, Openness (Costa and McCrae, 1992a, b; Goldberg, 1990;

Goldberg et al., 2006; Marsh et al., 2010; McCord, 2002; Socha et al., 2010), and can be

applied in non-clinical settings. A study by Zheng et al. (2008) showed clear relationships

between corresponding personality traits between the BFI and the 50-item IPIP,

Page 39: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

20

meaning that the same personality traits were reliably being measured on each

questionnaire.

Human Relationship Satisfaction

Qualities that may lead a person to have marital stability and intimacy may be

similar to qualities that lead a person to stay bonded to his or her pet (Zilcha-Mano et

al., 2011). There is an abundance of research investigating the influence of human

personality traits on interpersonal relationship factors (e.g., marital satisfaction)

(Bouchard and Areseneault, 2005; Brehm et al., 2002; Donnellan et al., 2004; White et

al., 2004). Extraversion and Agreeableness have been found to be positively correlated

with the value of relationships, including satisfaction, marital success, and intimacy

(Barry, 1970; Bentler and Newcomb, 1978; Bouchard et al., 1999; Karney and Bradbury,

1995; Kelly and Conley, 1987; Shadish, 1986; Watson et al., 2000; White et al., 2004).

Extraversion has also been negatively associated with marital stability (Karney and

Bradbury, 1995; White et al., 2004). Although there have been mixed findings,

Conscientiousness was found to be positively associated with certain relationship

components, such as satisfaction, stability, length of relationship, and intimacy (Karney

and Bradbury, 1995; Shaver and Brennan, 1992; White et al., 2004). Strong negative

correlations exist between Neuroticism and relationship satisfaction, intimacy, and

stability (Karney and Bradbury, 1995, 1997; Karney et al., 1994; Robins et al., 2000,

2002). Strong positive correlations also exist with Neuroticism and divorce rate (i.e.,

higher divorce rates were associated with individuals higher in Neuroticism) (Karney and

Page 40: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

21

Bradbury, 1995; Shaver and Brennan, 1992; White et al., 2004), and strong negative

correlations with length of marriage (Donnellan et al., 2004). The fifth personality trait,

Openness, has been negatively associated with marital stability, and length and

satisfaction of the relationship (Karney and Bradbury, 1995; White et al., 2004).

Human-Animal Bond

It is apparent that people and animals can have incredibly close and meaningful

relationships (McGreevy et al., 2012). Why is it, then, that so many animals are

surrendered every year to animal shelters? There could be a break in the human-animal

bond, but in order for animal shelter professionals to have an impact on adoption

success and decreasing surrenders, we need to truly understand why bonds become

broken (Curb et al., 2013; Marston et al., 2005; Mondelli et al., 2004; Neidhart and

Boyd, 2002; ). Because animals are such wonderful companions for people, and provide

psychological, social, and health benefits (Archer, 1997; Custance and Mayer, 2012;

Sable, 2013), this understanding has important implications for animal shelters – how to

make sure that the animals have the best care, and how to ensure that the animals are

adopted out to the best homes for them.

The human-animal bond is evident in the positive effects that humans and

canine companions have on each other (Archer, 1997; Custance and Mayer, 2012; Sable,

2013). Dogs have been shown to decrease blood pressure in people, and facilitate social

interactions among the elderly as well as children (Griffin et al., 2011). Dogs are

affectionate and joyful, and show people how to give and receive love; they are keenly

Page 41: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

22

attuned to their owner’s presence, seek out attention and physical interaction, and

develop an attachment to people (Beck and Katcher, 1996; Marston et al., 2005;

Mondelli et al., 2004; Sable, 2013). Attachment is a construct that encompasses

knowledge about the dog’s health and behavioral needs, emotional closeness to the

dog, as well as time spent interacting and caring for the dog (Kwan and Bain, 2013). The

level of attachment negatively correlates with relinquishment of an animal (i.e., the

higher level of attachment, the less likely for surrender). In one study, attachment was

significantly decreased for those relinquishing their pets to an animal shelter compared

to owners who kept their dogs (Kwan and Bain, 2013). Unfortunately, it is not

uncommon in animal shelters to have owners, who are highly bonded to their pets,

make the decision to relinquish due to being financially unable to provide for the pet.

Attachment theory (Beck and Madresh, 2008; Bowlby, 1973; Poresky, 1989;

Sable, 2013; Zilcha-Mano et al., 2011) states that humans and animals to have a

biological need to be emotionally invested in those around them. When Cavanaugh et

al. (2008) took a cross-species approach to evaluating dog and human personality

effects on dog-owner satisfaction, it was found that dog owners higher in Neuroticism

did not report differences in satisfaction when compared to dog owners higher in other

personality categories; unlike in the human personality psychology literature, where

spousal partners high in Neuroticism tended to report lower marital satisfaction

(Donnellan et al., 2004; Karney and Bradbury, 1997). The same study also found there

was a positive relationship between canine personality traits Openness and

Agreeableness with satisfaction of the dog-human relationship (Cavanaugh et al., 2008).

Page 42: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

23

Some studies, such as the one by Cavanaugh et al. (2008), used adapted

questionnaires to measure relationship satisfaction between responder and his/her dog.

Common measures of human-animal attachment include the Companion Animal

Bonding Scale (Poresky et al., 1989); the Pet Attitude Inventory (Wilson et al., 1987); the

Pet Bonding Scale (Angle et al., 1994); and the Pet Relationship Scale (Kafer et al., 1992).

The Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) (Anderson, 2007; Johnson et al., 1992) is

another common measure of the human-animal bond, and has excellent reliability and

content validity, which means the items consistently represent attachment to a

companion animal (Johnson et al., 1992).

Matching Families and Pets

Expanding on human personality psychology research involving relationship

satisfaction, a main goal of successful animal shelter adoptions is to have the adopter

and dog develop a strong human-animal bond, where the adopter is satisfied with the

dog and their relationship together. The ASPCA® Canine-alityTM Adoption Program

(Weiss, 2007b) and Match-Up II (Dowling-Guyer et al., 2011) program are two well

known programs implemented in animal shelters nationwide that assist in making good

matches between adopter and dog, based on personality.

The first part of the Canine-AlityTM program involves a six-item assessment by

which each dog is scored and then assigned, based on energy level, one of the three

different color categories: green, orange, or purple. Within each color category, there

are three different Canine-alityTM descriptions based on motivation: social, internal, or

Page 43: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

24

external. Each dog’s energy level and motivation level are combined to create nine

personality options. The second part of the program is the Dog Adopter Survey, which

assesses how adopters envision their new dogs fitting into their home and family

environment. Based on the results of the survey, a color category is recommended to

the potential adopter, which they should use to choose a dog. He or she then is directed

to look for dogs that have the same color as was recommended (e.g., an individual who

receives a purple category prefers a dog that is low maintenance and easy going) (Curb

et al., 2013). The Canine-alityTM Adoption Program is designed to increase the likelihood

that shelter dogs will be accepted into new homes as welcome members of the family.

However, no research has been published investigating the efficacy of the Canine-alityTM

program, and whether it is successful in making appropriate matches between dogs and

adopters.

Another program that assesses dog behavior/personality is Match-Up II

(Dowling-Guyer et al., 2011). The Match-Up II Shelter Dog Rehoming Program,

developed by Dr. Amy Marder at the Animal Rescue League’s Center for Shelter Dogs, is

a behavioral assessment tool that groups observable behaviors together into personality

traits. The Match-Up II program consists of several parts, including a behavioral history

taken at intake, a behavioral evaluation, personality scoring, and behavior observed in

the shelter by volunteers and staff members (Canine personality:

centerforshelterdogs.org). Unlike Canine-alityTM, Match-Up II does not include a human

personality component to matching adopters with dogs. Instead, staff use the

information gained in the Match-Up II program to counsel and educate potential

Page 44: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

25

adopters about any possible behavioral problems that the dog may exhibit. Reliability

and validity research for Match-Up II is currently being conducted (Match-Up II

reliability study: centerforshelterdogs.org). However, these matching programs to do

not implement scientifically validated methods, nor have they investigated the validity

of the dog and human personality components. The current dissertation assesses dog

and human personality utilizing reliable measures.

The rate of adopted animals being returned to shelters has been reported to be

as high as 20% in the United States (Patronek et al., 1996). The high incidence of dogs

being surrendered or returned to shelters indicates a critical need to not only improve

the dog-adopter matching process, but also to provide owners with post-adoption

strategies for adjusting to a dog in the new home (Ley et al., 2009b; Marston et al.,

2005). Many shelters focus on matching a dog’s behavior with the adopter’s lifestyle

(Cavanaugh et al., 2008; Seeing Dogs as Individuals is the Key to Matching:

maddiesfund.org). When dog behavior was influential in adoption decisions, an

Australian study (Marston et al., 2005) revealed that adopters considered calm and

friendly behavior, solicitation of physical contact or attention, and the dog’s interactions

with an existing pet or children important. However, behavioral assessments are only a

snapshot of a dog’s behavior, and behaviors seen in the shelter environment where

there are a lot of stressors, may not necessarily indicate future behavior in the home

environment (Bennett et al., 2012).

When Marston et al. (2005) contacted adopters one month post-adoption,

common behavioral problems reported included pulling on leash, hyperactivity,

Page 45: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

26

destructive behavior, and inappropriate elimination. The adopters were more likely to

return the dog to the shelter if it had failed to fulfill expectations, including the

perceived amount of work required for the dog’s care (Marston et al., 2005).

Curb et al. (2013) suggested that owners’ satisfaction with their dogs was

negatively associated with resource guarding (e.g., the owners were more satisfied

when their dogs showed less territorial behaviors) and destruction of household objects.

Satisfaction was also positively correlated with motivation to exercise (Curb et al.,

2013). Owners seemed to be more satisfied with their dogs if their lifestyle preferences

matched each others’ (e.g., preference for being physically active or creative and

mentally stimulated) (Curb et al., 2013). No correlation was found between dog-human

matched traits and satisfaction, which may be due to the short length of the satisfaction

survey which only contained four questions.

Matching the prospective adopter with its future companion, based on activity

level, lifestyle, expectations, and personality is important: if a solid match is made,

higher satisfaction and fewer relinquishments are expected (Curb et al., 2013; Palmer

and Custance, 2008). The studies, described in this dissertation, explore dog and human

personality and their combined influences on satisfaction of the newly adopted

companion.

Study Relevance

No current literature reviews the impact of dog or human personality as possible

factors for relinquishment or the adoption of shelter dogs. This dissertation focuses on

Page 46: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

27

the impact of dog and human personality in order to understand more about the

human-animal bond of adopted dogs. Understanding the elements of a strong human-

animal bond, animal shelter personnel may be able to encourage appropriate matches

at adoption, and therefore decrease the amount of returns and surrenders. Currently,

there is no standardized method of assessing canine personality with shelter dogs, and

there is yet to be a validated method of assessing the matching process between dogs

and adoptive families; animal shelters across the nation are performing various

assessments, to match dogs to adoptive families, that are not evidence-based.

Page 47: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

28

References

Allport, G.W. 1937. Personality; a psychological interpretation. H. Holt and Co. New

York, NY, USA.

Anderson, D.S., 2007. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale, LAPS (Johnson et al. 1992). In:

Beck, A.M. (ed.), Assessing the Human-Animal Bond: A Compendium of Actual

Measures. Purdue University Press. West Lafayette, IN, USA.

Angle, R.L., Blumentritt, T., Swank, P., 1994. The Pet Bonding Scale: Internal reliability

and factorial validity. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of Southwestern

Psychological Association, Tulsa, OK.Archer, J., 1997. Why do people love their

pets? Evolution and Human Behavior 18, 237-259.

Arkow, P., Dow, S., 1984. The ties that do not bind: A study of the human-animal bonds

that fail. In: Anderson, R.K., Hart, B.L., Hart, L.A. (eds.), The pet connection: Its

influence on our health and quality of life (pp. 348-354). Minneapolis: University

of Minnesota.

Barry, W.A., 1970. Marriage research and conflict: an integrative review. Psychological

Bulletin 73, 41-54.Beck, A.M., Katcher, A.H., 1996. Between pets and people: the

importance of animal companionship. Purdue University Press, USA.

Beck, A.M., Katcher, A.H., 1996. Between pets and people: the importance of animal

companionship. Purdue University Press, USA.

Beck, L., Madresh, E.A., 2008. Romantic partners and four-legged friends: An extension

of attachment theory to relationships with pets. Anthrozoos 21, 43-56.

Page 48: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

29

Bennett, S., Litster, A., Weng, H.Y., Walker, S., 2012. Investigating behavior assessment

instruments to predict aggression in dogs. Applied Animal Behavior Science 141,

139-148.

Bentler, P.M., Newcomb, M.D., 1978. Longitudinal study of martial success and failure.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 46, 1053-1070.

Bollen, K.S., Horowitz, J., 2008. Behavioral evaluation and demographic information in

the assessment of aggressiveness in shelter dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour

Science 112, 120-135.

Bouchard, G., Areseneault, J.E., 2005. Length of union as a moderator of the relationship

between personality and dyadic adjustment. Personality and Individual

Differences. 39, 1407-1417.

Bowlby, J., 1973. Attachment and Loss. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Brehm, S.S., Miller, R.S., Perlman, D., Campbell, S.M., 2002. Intimate relationships.

McGraw Hill. New York. USA.

Canine personality. The Center for Shelter Dogs. Retrieved February 12, 2011, from

centerforshelterdogs.org website:

http://centerforshelterdogs.org/Home/ResearchandEducation/CSDResearch/Ca

ninePersonality.aspx.

Cattell, R.B., 1946. Personality structure and measurement II: The determination and

utility of trait modality. The British Journal of Psychology 36, 159-174.

Page 49: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

30

Cavanaugh, L.A., Leonard, H.A., Scammon, D.L., 2008. A trail of two personalities: How

canine companions shape relationships and well-being. Journal of Business

Research. 61, 469-479.

Costa, P.T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., 1992a. Professional Manuel: Revised NEO personality

inventory (NEO-PR-I) and NEO Five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI). Odessa, FL:

Psychological Assessment Resources.

Costa, P.T., Jr., McCrae, R.R., 1992b. Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and

Individual Differences. 13, 653-665.

Curb, L.A., Abramson, C.I., Grice, J.W., Kennison, S.M., 2013. The relationship between

personality match and pet satisfaction among dog owners. Anthrozoos 26, 395-

404.

Custance, D., Mayer, J., 2012. Empathetic-like responding by domestic dogs (Canis

familiaris) to distress in humans: an exploratory study. Animal Cognition 15, 851-

859.

Diederich, C., Giffroy, J.M., 2006. Behavioural testing in dogs: a review of methodology

in search for standardization. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 97, 51-72.

Donnellan, M.B., Conger, R.D., Bryant, C.M., 2004. The big five and enduring marriages.

Journal of Research in Personality. 38, 481-504.

Dowling-Guyer, S., Marder, A., D’Arpino, S., 2011. Behavioral traits detected in shelter

dogs by a behavior evaluation. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 130, 107-114.

Draper, T.W., 1995. Canine analogs of human personality factors. The Journal of General

Psychology 11, 241-252.

Page 50: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

31

Eysenck, H., Eysenck, S., 1975. Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. Hodder

& Stoughton, London.

Forbey, J.D., Ben-Porath, Y.S., 2007. Computerized adaptive personality testing: A

review and illustration with the MMPI-2 computerized adaptive version.

Psychological Assessment. 19, 14-24.

Fratkin, J.L., Sinn, D.L., Patall, E.A., Gosling, S.D., 2013. Personality consistency in dogs: A

meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 8, e54907. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054907.

Garrity, T.F., Stallones, L., Marx, M.B., Johnson, T.P., 1989. Pet ownership and

attachment as protective factors in the health of the elderly. Anthrozoos 3, 35-

44.

Goldberg, L.R., 1990. An alternative “description of personality’: The Big-Five Factor

structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 59, 1216-1229.

Goldberg, L.R., Johnson, J.A., Eber, H.W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M.C., Cloninger, R., Gough,

H.G., 2006. The international personality item pool and the future of public-

domain personality measures. 40, 84-96.

Gosling, S.D., 1998. Personality dimensions in spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). Journal

of Comparative Psychology 112, 107-118.

Gosling, S.D., Kwan, V.S.Y., John, O.P., 2003. A dog’s got personality: A cross-species

comparative approach to personality judgments in dogs and humans. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology 85, 1161-1169.

Page 51: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

32

Griffin, J.A., McCune S., Maholmes V., Hurley, K., 2011. Human-animal interaction

research: An introduction to issues and topics. In McCardle P, McCune S, Griffin

JA & Maholmes V (Eds.), How animals affect us. American Psychological

Association, Washington D.C.

Highfill, L., Hanbury, D., Kristiansen, R., Kuczaj, S., Watson, S., 2010. Rating vs. coding in

animal personality research. Zoo Biology 29, 509-516.

Humane Society of the United States. (n.d.). Why you should spay or neuter your pet.

http://www.humanesociety.org/issues/pet_overpopulation/facts/why_spay_ne

uter.html. Accessed on March 4, 2014.

International Personality Item Pool. 2014. International Personality Item Pool: A

scientific collaboratory for the development of advanced measures of

personality and other individual differences. http://ipip.ori.org. Accessed on

March 15, 2014.

John, O.P., Donahue, E.M., Kentle, R.L., 1991. The Big Five Inventory—Versions 4a and

54, Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and

Social Research.John, O.P., Srivastava, S., 1999. The big five trait taxonomy:

history, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In: Pervin, L.A., John, O.P.,

(eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. Guilford Press, New York,

USA; pp. 102-138.

Johnson, T.P., Garrity, T.F., Stallones, L.,1992. Psychometric Evaluation of the Lexington

Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS). Anthrozoos 5, 160-175.

Jolliffee, I.T., 2002. Principal Components Analysis (2nd edn. Springer, New York, USA.

Page 52: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

33

Jones, A.C., Gosling, S.D., 2005. Temperament and personality in dogs (Canis familiaris):

A review and evaluation of past research. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 95,

1-53.

Kafer, R., Lago, D., Wamboldt, P., Harrington, F., 1992. The Pet Relationship Scale:

Replication of psychometric properties in random samples and association with

attitudes toward wild animals. Anthrozoos 5, 93-105.

Karney, B.R., Bradbury, T.N., 1995. The longitudinal course of marital quality and

stability: a review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin 118, 3-

34.

Karney, B.R., Bradbury, T.N., Fincham, F.D., Sullivan, K.T., 1994. The role of negative

affectivity in the association between attribution and marital satisfaction.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66, 413-424.

Kass, P.H., New, Jr., J.C., Scarlett, J.M, Salman, M.D., 2001. Understanding animal

companion surplus in the United States: Relinquishment of nonadoptables to

animal shelters for euthanasia. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 4, 237-

248.

Kelly, E.L., Conley, J.J., 1987. Personality and compatibility: a prospective analysis of

marital stability and marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology 52, 27-40.

Kidd, A.H., Kidd, R.M., George, C.C., 1992. Successful and unsuccessful pet adoptions.

Psychological Reports 70, 547-561.

Page 53: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

34

Kis, A., Klausz, B., Persa, E., Miklosi, A., Gacsi, M., 2014. Timing and presence of an

attachment person affect sensitivity of aggression tests in shelter dogs.

Veterinary Record. DOI: 10.1136/vr.101955.

Krueger, R.F., Johnson, W., 2008. Behavioral genetics and personality. In: John, O.P.,

Robins, R.W., Pervin, L.A. (eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research

(3rd edn.), The Guilford Press, New York, USA.

Kwan, J., Bain, M., 2013. Owner attachment and problem behaviors related to

relinquishment and training techniques of dogs. Journal of Applied Animal

Welfare Science 16, 168-183.

Larson, R.J. and Buss, D.M., 2010. Personality Psychology: Domains of Knowledge About

Human Nature (p. 24). McGraw Hill. New York, New York, USA.

Ley, J., Bennett, P., Coleman, G., 2008. Personality dimensions that emerge in

companion canines. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 110, 305-317.

Ley, J.M., Bennett, P.C., Coleman, G.J., 2009a. A refinement and validation of the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire (MCPQ). Applied Animal Behaviour

Science 116, 220-227.

Ley, J.M., McGreevy, P., Bennett, P.C., 2009b. Inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R). App. Anim. Behav.

Sci. 119, 85-90.

Marsh, H.W., Ludtke, O., Muthen, B., Asparouhov, T., Morin, A.J.S., Trautwein, U.,

Nagengast, B., 2010. A new look at the big five factor structure through

exploratory structural equation modeling. Psych. Assess. 22, 471-491.

Page 54: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

35

Marston, L.C., Bennett, P.C., Coleman, G.J., 2005. Adopting shelter dogs: Owner

experiences of the first month post-adoption. Anthrozoos 18, 358-378.

Martin, P., Bateson, P., 2007. Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge

University Press, Cambridge.

Match-Up II reliability study. The Center for Shelter Dogs. Retrieved July 17, 2014.

http://www.centerforshelterdogs.com/Home/News/tabid/77/ctl/ReadCSDNews

/mid/655/ArticleId/71/Default.aspx.

McCord, D.M. 2002. M5-5- Questionnaire [Administration and scoring materials].

Retrieved from http://paws.wcu.edu/mccord/m5-50/.

McGreevy, P.D., Starling, M., Branson, N.J., Cobb, M.L., Calnon, D., 2012. An overview of

the dog-human dyad and ethograms within it. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 7,

103-117.

Mirko, E., Doka, A., Miklosi, A., 2013. Association between subjective rating and

behavior coding and the role of experience in making video assessments on the

personality of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour

Science 149, 45-54.

Mondelli, F., Previde, E.P., Verga, M., Levi, D., Magistrelli, S., Valsecchi, P., 2004. The

bond that never developed: Adoption and relinquishment of dogs in a rescue

shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 7, 253-266.

Moulton, C., Wright, P., Rindy, K., 1991. The role of animal shelters in controlling pet

overpopulation. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 198,

1172-1176.

Page 55: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

36

National Council on Pet Population Study and Policy (NCPPSP). 2009. National Council

Research. http://www.petpopulation.org/research.html. Accessed on March 4,

2014.

New, J.C. Jr., Salman, M.D., King, M., Scarlett, J.M., Kass, P.H., Hutchison, J.M., 2000.

Characteristics of shelter-relinquished animals and their owners compared with

animals and their owners in U.S. pet-owning households. Journal of Applied

Animal Welfare Science 3, 179-201.

Neidhart, L. Boyd, R., 2002. Companion animal adoption study. Journal of Applied

Animal Welfare Science 5, 175-192.

Palmer, R., Custance, D., 2008. A counterbalanced version of Ainsworth’s Strange

Situation Procedure reveals secure-base effects in dog-human relationships.

Applied Animal Behavior Science 109, 306-319.

Patronek, G.J., Glickman, L.T., Beck, A.M., McCabe, G.P., Ecker, C., 1996. Risk factors for

relinquishment of dogs to an animal shelter. Journal of American Veterinary

Medical Association 209, 572-581.

Pervin, L.A., John, O.P., 1997. Personality: Theory and Research (7th edn.), John Wiley &

Sons, United Kingdom.

Poresky, R.H., 1989. Analyzing human-animal relationship measures. Anthrozoos. 2,

236-244.

Poulsen, A.H., Lisle, A.T., Phillips, C.J.C., 2010. An evaluation of a behavior assessment to

determine the suitability of shelter dogs for rehoming. Veterinary Medicine

International. DOI: 10.4061/2010/523781.

Page 56: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

37

Robins, R.W., Caspi, A., Moffit, R.E., 2000. Two personalities, one relationship: both

partner’s personality traits shape the quality of their relationship. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology 79, 251-259.

Robins, R.W., Caspi, A., Moffitt, R.E., 2002. It’s not just who you’re with, it’s who you

are: personality and relationship experiences across multiple relationships.

Journal of Personality 70, 925-964.

Sable, P., 2013. The pet connection: an attachment perspective. Clinical Social Work

Journal 41, 93-99.

Salman, M.D., New, J.G., Scarlett, J.M., Kass, P.H., Ruch-Gallie, R., Hetts, S., 1998. Human

and animal factors related to the relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected

animal shelters in the United States. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science

1, 207-226.

Salman, M.D., Hutchison, J., Ruch-Gallie, R., Kogan, L., New, Jr., J.C., Kass, P.H., Scarlett,

J.M., 2000. Behavioral reasons for relinquishment of dogs and cats to 12

shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 3, 93-106.

Sanders, C.R., 1993. Understanding dogs: Caretakers’ attributions of mindedness in

canine-human relationships. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 22, 205-226.

Scarlett, J.M., Salman, M.D., New, Jr., J.G., Kass, P.H., 1999. Reasons for relinquishments

of companion animals in U.S. animal shelters: Selected health and personal

issues. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2, 41-57.

Page 57: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

38

Seeing Dogs as Individuals is the Key to Matching. Maddie’s Fund. Retrieved July 22,

2014.

http://www.maddiesfund.org/Maddies_Institute/Articles/Seeing_Dogs_as_Indivi

duals.html.

Shadish, W.R., Jr., 1986. The validity of a measure of intimate behavior. Small Group

Behavior 17, 113-120.

Shaver, P.R., Brennan, K.A., 1992. Attachment styles and the “Big Five” personality

traits: their connections with each other and with romantic relationship

outcomes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 18, 536-545.

Socha, A., Cooper, C.A., McCord, D.M., 2010. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the M5-50:

An Implementation of the International Personality Item Pool Item Set. Psych.

Assess. 22(1), 43-49.

Stephen, J., Ledger, R., 2007. Relinquishing dog owners’ ability to predict behavioural

problems in shelter dogs post adoption. Applied Animal Behavior Science 107,

88-99.

Svartberg, K., 2002. Shyness-boldness predicts performance in working dogs. Applied

Animal Behaviour Science 79, 157-174.

Svartberg, K., 2005. A comparison of behavior in test and in everyday life: evidence of

three consistent boldness-related personality traits in dogs. Applied Animal

Behaviour Science 91, 103-128.

Svartberg, K., Forkman, B., 2002. Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris).

Applied Animal Behaviour Science 79, 133-155.

Page 58: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

39

Svartberg, K., Tapper, I., Temrin, H., Radesater, T., Thorman, S., 2005. Consistency of

personality traits in dogs. Animal Behaviour 69, 283-291.

Van der Borg, J.A.M., Beerda, B., Ooms, M., Silveira de Souza, A., van Hagen, M., Kemp,

B., 2010. Evaluation of behaviour testing for human directed aggression in dogs.

Applied Animal Behaviour Science 128, 78-90.

Vassand, O., Skrondal, A., 2011. The NEO personality inventory revised (NEO-PI-R):

Exploring the measurement structure and variants of the five-factor model.

Personality and Individual Differences 50, 1300-1304.

Watanabe, N.M., Stahlman, W.D., Blaisdell, A.P., Garlick, D., Fast, C.D., Blumstein, D.T.,

2012. Quantifying personality in the terrestrial hermit crab: Different measures,

different inferences. Behavioural Processes 91, 133-140.

Weinstein, T.A.R., Capitanio, J.P., Gosling, S.D., 2008. Personality in Animals. In: John,

O.P., Robins, R.W., Pervin, L.A. (Eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and

Research (3rd edn.), The Guilford Press, New York, NY, USA.

Weiss, E., 2007a. Canine-alityTM Adoption Program Manual and Training Guide. ASPCA.

Weiss, E., 2007b. Meet Your Match SAFERTM manual and training guide. ASPCA.

[email protected].

Weiss, E., Gramann, S., 2009. A comparison of attachmemtn levels of adopters of cats:

Fee-based adoptions versus free adoptions. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare

Science 12, 360-370.

Page 59: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

40

Wells, D.L., Hepper, P.G., 2000. Prevalence of behavior problems reported by owners of

dogs purchased from an animal rescue shelter. Applied Animal Behaviour

Science 69, 55-65.

Weng, H-Y., Kass, P.H., Hart, L.A., Chomel, B.B., 2006. Risk factors for unsuccessful dog

ownership: An epidemiologic study in Taiwan. Preventive Veterinary Medicine

77, 82-95.

White, J.K., Hendrick, S.S., Hendrick, C., 2004. Big five personality variables and

relationship constructs. Personality and Individual Differences. 37, 1519-1530.

Wilson, C.C., Netting, F.E., New, J.C., The Pet Attitude Inventory. Anthrozoos 1, 76-84.

Wilsson, E., Sundgren, P.E., 1997. The use of a behavior test for the selection of dogs for

service and breeding, I: method of testing and evaluating test results in the adult

dog, demands on different kind of service dogs, sex and breed differences.

Applied Animal Behaviour Science 53, 279-295.

Wise, E.A., Streiner, D.L., Walfish, S., 2010. A review and comparison of the reliabilities

of the MMPI-2, MCMI-III, and PAI presented in their respective test manuals.

Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development. 42, 246-254.

Zheng, L., Goldber, L.R., Zheng, Y., Zhao, Y., Tang, Y., Liu, L., 2008. Reliability and

concurrent validation of the IPIP Big-Five factor markers in China: Consistencies

in factor structure between internet-obtained heterosexual and homosexual

samples. Personality and Individual Differences 45, 649-654.

Page 60: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

41

Zilcha-Mano, S., Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P.R., 2011. An attachment perspective on

human-pet relationships: Conceptualization and assessment of pet attachment

orientations. Journal of Research in Personality 45, 345-357.

Page 61: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

42

CHAPTER 3. MATERIALS AND METHODS

Page 62: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

43

Methods

Ethical Approval

This project was approved by the Purdue University Institutional Review Board

(Protocol # 1210012795). As this was an Exempt Category 2 research project, Informed

Consent was not needed at the time of recruitment. However, information sheets

containing pertinent study information was given to the owner along with the

questionnaires (Appendix H). This project was also reviewed by the Clinton County

Humane Society (CCHS) Executive Director and Board of Directors, as well as the

Humane Society of Indianapolis (HSI) Executive/Operations staff for approval.

Questionnaires

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ- R)

The MCPQ-R is a 26-item questionnaire (Appendices B and D) containing

adjectives that describe a dog’s overall traits. Questionnaires were evaluated with score

sheets (Table 1) which placed relative adjectives under each of five personality

dimensions (Extraversion, Motivation, Training Focus, Amicability, and Neuroticism). A

percentage (sum of the scores for the adjectives for each specific trait divided by the

total possible points for each trait) was then calculated for each personality dimension.

For example, if a dog scored 5 for Assertive, 5 for Determined, 5 for Independent, 6 for

Persevering, and 6 for Tenacious, the score for the personality trait Motivation would be

27/30 = 0.9 (Table 1). The trait with the highest percentage would then represent the

Page 63: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

44

personality category for that specific dog. The same method of scoring was described in

Ley et al. (2009a).

Personality traits which received tied scores were marked for shelter staff

members to spend additional time with dogs receiving them over the ensuing two

weeks, in order to determine which of the tied categories was more representative of

that dog. Staff spent at least an hour and a half each day for two weeks, with those

dogs, before completing the MCPQ-R, in order to get an accurate impression of each

dog’s personality. There was never an instance where a dog with tied personality

categories was adopted before the staff had an opportunity to conduct additional

assessment. For those dogs that had tied personality categories on the MCPQ-R

completed by the adopter, one personality category was randomly selected for data

analysis.

Permission to use the MCPQ-R in this study was given via personal

communication with Dr. Jacqui Ley at Monash University. I followed the procedure

described by Ley et al. (2009a, b): the ratings for each adjective for each trait were

summed, then divided by the maximum possible points for each trait and converted to a

percent score. If there were missing values, the percent score was calculated by

omitting the missing value’s maximum points from the sum of the maximum amount of

points for each adjective which had recorded scores (e.g., if there was no value recorded

for “Biddable,” then the Max Score would be 30 instead of 36, and the percent score

would be calculated using that number as the denominator).

Page 64: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

45

Table 1. MCPQ-R score sheet.

MCPQ-R

Scoring

Dog ID: Dog Name: Color:

Add scores for each of the five dimensions together and divide by the maximum for

each dimension (see below). This gives one score for each dimension.

Extraversion Motivation Training Focus Amicability Neuroticism

Active – Assertive – Attentive – Easy Going – Fearful –

Energetic – Determined – Biddable – Friendly – Nervous –

Excitable – Independent – Intelligent – Non-aggressive – Submissive –

Eager – Persevering – Obedient – Relaxed – Timid –

Lively – Tenacious – Reliable – Sociable –

Restless – Trainable –

Max score: 36 Max score: 30 Max score: 36 Max score: 30 Max score: 24

Score: Score: Score: Score: Score:

Percentage: Percentage: Percentage: Percentage: Percentage:

The International Personality Item Pool (IPIP)

A personality psychologist at Michigan State University, Dr. Brent Donnellen, was

consulted to discuss the most efficient way to measure human personality in a shelter

setting with the goals of the research project in mind. The 50-item IPIP, as opposed to

Page 65: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

46

the BFI, was chosen for use in the following studies because it has free access, the 50

items were in a format that was easy-to-administer in a shelter setting, where patrons

do not have a lot of time nor is it always an environment conducive for quiet thinking.

Standard IPIP instructions were presented to participants, who responded on a

five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (“Very Inaccurate”) to 5 (“Very Accurate”),

with a neutral midpoint (Socha et al., 2010). Each value, or rating, was either given that

score (i.e., if the responder rated item 5 (“I have a rich vocabulary”) as a 3, then a score

of 3 was given for that value), or a reversed score. Approximately half of the items were

reverse scored when the item measures the lower end of the corresponding category

(e.g., item 4, under the Stability category, states “I get stressed out easily”: if the

respondent rated this item “Very Inaccurate”, then it was scored a 5 instead of a 1).

Scores were totaled and the personality dimension with the highest score then was the

representative personality category for that responder. If there were missing values,

then the percentages were used. For those adopters that had tied personality categories

on the IPIP, one personality category was randomly selected for data analysis. The score

sheet for the 50-item IPIP is shown in Table 2.

Page 66: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

47

Table 2. The 50-item IPIP score sheet.

IPIP Scoring Adopter ID: Adopter Name: Color:

Reverse scoring (R) – 1 point if answer is 5, 2 points if answer is 4, 3 points if answer is 3, 4 points if answer is 2, and 5 points if answer is 1

Regular scoring – 5 points if answer is 5, 4 points of answer is 4, 3 points if answer is 3, 2 points if answer is 2, and 1 point if answer is 1

Add scores for each of the five dimensions together and divide by the maximum for each dimension (see below). This gives one score for each dimension.

EXTROVERSION AGREEABLENESS CONSCIENTIOUSNESS STABILITY OPENNESS

Item 1 – Item 2(R) – Item 3 – Item 4(R) – Item 5 –

Item 6(R) – Item 7 – Item 8(R) – Item 9 – Item 10(R) –

Item 11 – Item 12(R) – Item 13 – Item 14(R) – Item 15 –

Item 16 (R) – Item 17 – Item 18(R) – Item 19 – Item 20(R) –

Item 21 – Item 22(R) – Item 23 – Item 24(R) – Item 25 –

Item 26(R) – Item 27 – Item 28(R) – Item 29 – Item 30(R) –

Item 31 – Item 32(R) – Item 33 – Item 34(R) – Item 35 –

Item 36(R) – Item 37 – Item 38(R) – Item 39(R) – Item 40 –

Item 41 – Item 42 – Item 43 – Item 44(R) – Item 45 –

Item 46(R) – Item 47 – Item 48 – Item 49(R) – Item 50 –

Max Score: 50 Max Score: 50 Max Score: 50 Max Score: 50 Max Score: 50

Score: Score: Score: Score: Score:

Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS)

The LAPS (Anderson, 2007; Johnson et al., 1992) is a common measure of the

human-animal bond, and was used in the current study to measure satisfaction of the

newly adopted companion. The LAPS was also used in this study because of its ease of

administration to adopters. Other measures of the human-animal bond contained

Page 67: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

48

irrelevant or too few items (e.g., the Companion Animal Bonding Scale (Poresky et al.,

1989)) for use in this study. The questionnaire has been published in two different

locations, so no permission was needed for its use in this study.

Standard LAPS instructions were presented to participants, who responded to 23

items on a four-point Likert-scale ranging from 1 (“Agree Strongly”) to 4 (“Disagree

Strongly”). No midpoint was used, but the fifth column (“Don’t Know or Refuse”) was

available as an optional answer. A score of 1 represented a high-level of bonding, and a

score of 4 represented a low-level, except with two items which were reversed scored.

A scoring sheet is shown in Table 3. Scores were averaged to produce the adopter’s

mean LAPS score. The mean was calculated if the LAPS questionnaire contained two or

less missing values. Mean scores were negatively correlated with human-animal bond: a

mean of 1.00 signified the strongest level of bonding and higher means signified lower

levels.

Page 68: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

49

Table 3. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale score sheet.

LAPS Scoring Adopter ID: Adopter Name: Total Score:

Reverse scoring (R) – N/A or missing value if last column is marked, 1 point if answer is 4, 2 points if answer is 3, 3 points if answer is 2, and 4 points if answer is 1.

Regular scoring – 1 point if answer is 1, 2 points if answer is 2, 3 points if answer is 3, 4 points if answer is 4, and N/A or missing value if last column is marked.

Add total score.

A: B: C: D: E:

F: G: H(R): I: J:

K: L: M: N: O:

P: Q: R: S: T:

U(R): V: W:

Subtotal: Subtotal: Subtotal: Subtotal: Subtotal:

Study Eligibility and Recruitment

Intake

For a dog to be enrolled in this study, the owner had to be at least 18 years old

and must have owned the dog for at least two weeks (to help ensure that the person

could accurately describe the dog’s personality), and the dog must have been at least a

year old. Previous studies investigating dog personality (Svartberg et al., 2005; Ley et al.,

2009a, b) have used an age criterion of one year or older for the dog. This is presumably

because the first year of a dog’s life includes the socialization period with critical phases

where personality is very malleable. I refer to dogs in this dissertation as “shelter dogs.”

Although no data were collected on the dogs themselves during their stay at the

Page 69: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

50

shelters, owners were recruited at two animal shelters, and data were collected on

owned dogs.

I (SW) interacted with owners surrendering their dogs at Clinton County Humane

Society (CCHS) and asked for their participation in the study. If the owners agreed to

participate, they completed the MCPQ-R for their dog(s); a Research Technician (KA)

interacted with patrons at the Humane Society of Indianapolis (HSI) at intake. An

information sheet was given to patrons at the time of recruitment. Time to complete

the MCPQ-R was roughly five to seven minutes. SW and KA were both available at the

respective animal shelters to answer any questions that the owners may have had about

the study. Participants were also aware that participation was completely voluntary,

options were available to complete the questionnaire in privacy, and that the responses

would have absolutely no impact on their dog’s outcome at the shelter.

Adoption

Once each adoption was approved, SW and KA approached and initiated contact

with the new adopters. Adopters were given an Information Sheet to keep, and if the

adopters agreed to be part of the study, they were given the 50-item IPIP questionnaire

to complete. Time to complete the IPIP was roughly seven to 10 minutes and was

completed while shelter staff gathered an adoption packet for the adopter, which

included items such as the legally binding adoption contract, vaccination records,

informational behavioral pamphlets, and receipts. SW and KA were both available at the

respective animal shelters to answer any questions that the new adopters may have had

Page 70: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

51

about the study. Most adopters were very enthusiastic to help. Some adoptions were

contingent on having the entire household present to meet the dog (HSI); dog-to-dog

introductions (CCHS and HSI although HSI changed this policy three quarters through

the study on August 19th, 2013); and a home visit for adults 21 years of age and older

who wanted to adopt a bully breed (CCHS). At HSI, all family members were required to

come in to meet the dog. With occasional exceptions, all adults were required to hear

behavioral and medical information. In these circumstances, the 50-item IPIP

questionnaire was administered once the adoption was finalized.

Follow-up

Researchers told the adopter that they would be contacted in two months, and

that a dog-personality questionnaire (MCPQ-R) and a satisfaction/human-animal bond

survey (LAPS) would be sent to them in the mail. Prior to sending the MCPQ-R and the

LAPS questionnaires, each adopter was contacted by phone or e-mail to remind them

that a packet would be arriving in the mail with the questionnaire along with a pre-

stamped/addressed envelope for returning them to SW. If adoptions of study dogs

occurred while SW or KA were not on site, SW would telephone the adopter to get

verbal agreement to participate in the study and send out the Information Sheet and

IPIP questionnaire in the mail. Once the IPIP was received, the dog personality

questionnaire and LAPS questionnaires were sent out two months after the adoption

date.

Page 71: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

52

The first year of pet ownership is the most critical (Salman et al., 1998). Some

studies looked at return rates at one month (Marston et al., 2005) and even three

months (Salman et al., 2000) post-adoption. For various reasons, returns tend to occur

within the first two months of adoption (Mondelli et al., 2004; Wells and Hepper, 2000).

Two months was chosen as the follow-up time point in the current study as a

compromise between the previously mentioned studies; two months was assumed to

be a sufficient duration of time for the new adopter to develop a relationship with the

new companion and, thus, be able to identify and rate descriptive adjectives according

to personality.

Throughout the study, if an adopter scored a particularly high LAPS

questionnaire (e.g., an arbitrary sum score of 55 or higher), SW would follow-up with

the adopter (from CCHS), via phone call or e-mail, to make sure that everything was

going well with the new pet. For dogs adopted from HSI, SW would alert adoption-

counseling staff, and they would follow-up with that adopter via a phone call. This was

done as a courtesy and not a part of data collection, as a higher score on the LAPS may

indicate a greater risk of relinquishment; this did not affect retention, as after the LAPS

was returned to SW, the dog was considered a completed data point and no further

information was collected.

Page 72: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

53

Data Collection Sites and Subjects

Clinton County Humane Society

Located 26 miles southeast of Purdue University in rural Frankfort, Indiana, CCHS

was an open-admission animal shelter that took in stray dogs, transferred dogs to and

from other facilities, and took in owner surrendered dogs. Dogs that were surrendered

were available for adoption the same day, given they were medically and behaviorally

sound. Dogs brought into the shelter as strays were held for five days in a quarantine

area that was isolated from the adoption population. The five-day hold was to allow

adequate time for owners to reclaim their animals, if desired. Kennels were either 1.22

m x 3.05 m or 1.52 m x 2.13 m in size and were cleaned each morning. Dogs were fed

twice daily with water bowls filled as necessary, and were taken outside as time and

volunteers were available (i.e., at least twice a day). CCHS maximally housed 47 dogs (26

in the large-dog adoption area, seven in the smaller dog adoption area, and 14 in the

isolation area). During the data-collection period, capacity was surpassed on several

occasions and small dogs were housed in crates lining the hallways.

There was no veterinarian or veterinary technician on staff, so animals that

needed immediate medical attention were usually transported to one of four local

veterinarians for care. Behavior evaluations were conducted occasionally. Shelter staff

tended to retain dogs until adoption and did not euthanize for time or space. With only

three full-time and two part-time employees, shelter tasks were often delegated to

volunteers and community-service students, who dedicated their time to clean kennels

and feed and water the animals in the morning before the shelter opened to the public

Page 73: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

54

in the afternoon. Three shelter staff members shared animal-control responsibilities for

Clinton County.

Humane Society of Indianapolis

Located 56 miles southeast of Purdue University in Indianapolis, Indiana, HSI was

a limited admission animal shelter and handled owner surrenders by appointment.

Historically, small- and medium-sized dogs spent on average less time in the HSI shelter,

before finding a home, than did large dogs. HSI scheduled appointments for incoming

dogs several weeks to a month in advance, so intake appointments were limited to

seven small and five medium/large dogs weekly. The appointment slots for

medium/large dogs were almost all booked each week. The seven appointment times

for small dogs were often not booked as not as many small dogs are surrendered,

according to the intake/scheduling staff at HSI. Often people never showed up for

scheduled appointments, and sometimes arrived three hours earlier than their

appointment time, so intake at HSI is often as unpredictable as at CCHS. Along with

owner surrenders, HSI also pulls highly adoptable dogs from the Indianapolis Animal

Care and Control (IACC). The IACC is responsible for obtaining dogs running at large and

for home seizures of abuse, neglect, and bite/court cases.

HSI did not have a specific quarantine period for surrendered dogs. However, the

Behavior Team preferred to give dogs an acclimation period of at least four days to

settle in before the behavior assessments. They usually had a backlog of dogs on their

list to assess; therefore, the acclimation period was often more than four days. Once the

Page 74: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

55

medical and behavioral teams approved each dog as healthy, then the dog was moved

to the adoption floor. There were 141 dog kennels/cages at HSI. Litters of puppies

stayed together as a group in each kennel, and HSI also had a foster program so some

dogs had the luxury of staying in a home until they were adopted. The kennels were

sanitized each morning, and spot cleaned as necessary throughout the day. Dogs were

fed twice daily with water bowls filled as necessary. Dogs were taken outside as time

and volunteers were available (i.e., at least three times a day).

In the first dog adoption area, there were 45 kennels (2.4 m x 1.1 m) used for

dogs of all sizes, and in the second dog adoption area, there were two living rooms (2.8

m x 2.9 m) for dogs of any size plus 24 kennels, usually for puppies and small dogs. In

the large dog evaluation area, there were 38 kennels (1.8 m x 1.1 m) for dogs of all sizes;

in the small dog evaluation area, there were 12 cages (0.7 m x 0.7 m) for adult dogs less

than 15 pounds; and in the puppy evaluation area, there were ten kennels (1.5 m x 0.7

m) that can fit dogs of all sizes, but an attempt was made to only house puppies in that

specific area. The enrichment ward held eight kennels (2.4 m x 1.9 m) for dogs of all

sizes, and the outside ringworm isolation area contained two kennels. During the data-

collection period, the dog population was managed so that capacity was never reached;

HSI operates between 70 and 80% of maximum capacity.

HSI, combined with their Holmes Spay and Neuter/Low Cost Vaccine Clinic,

employed 50 full-time and 8 part-time staff members, including five full-time

veterinarians and eight full-time veterinary technicians/veterinary technician assistants.

Page 75: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

56

Proportionately, HSI tended to have a much higher volume of animals and adopters

than CCHS.

Dog Demographics

Dog demographics (breed, coat color, age, sex, and neuter status) were also

recorded upon intake. Breed was either specified by the owner at intake or determined

by shelter staff if unknown. Age was either provided by the owner at intake or

estimated by shelter staff via a tooth inspection and basic physical exam. Neuter status

of females was verified by observing a spay scar or tattoo. Males without visible testicles

were assumed to be neutered if no neuter tattoo was observed.

110 dogs from CCHS were initially enrolled; 80 dogs were surrendered and 30

dogs were enrolled by staff, volunteers, or foster parents who completed the MCPQ-R

questionnaire. Two dogs were excluded due to age ineligibility, four died or were

euthanized due to severe injury or behavioral reasons, 15 were transferred to other

rescues, 13 had not been adopted by the end of the data-collection period, and 12 were

excluded because the adopter declined to participate, leaving 64 to be included in the

present study. Fifteen additional dogs were adopted by people who had been

randomized (mentioned in Appendix A), but chose a dog not initially enrolled in the

study.

Initial enrollment from HSI was 268 dogs: 178 were surrendered and 90 were

enrolled by staff or foster parents who completed the MCPQ-R questionnaire. Five were

excluded due to age ineligibility, 29 died or were euthanized due to medical or

Page 76: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

57

behavioral reasons, 10 were transferred to other rescues, five had not been adopted by

the end of the data-collection period, 18 were excluded because the adopter declined to

participate, and 201 were included in the study. Additionally, 63 additional dogs were

adopted by people who had been randomized (mentioned in Appendix A), but chose a

dog not initially enrolled in the study.

Staff, Volunteers, and Fosters

To increase the number of dogs in our study, shelter staff and volunteers were

asked to complete MCPQ-R questionnaires for some shelter dogs (n=120), which

included strays brought in by patrons or animal control officers, or dogs that were

transferred in from other humane organizations. In order to be familiar enough with

each dog to complete the MCPQ-R accurately, each individual had spent at least 1.5

hours each day for two weeks with the specific dog during husbandry, enrichment, and

social time. Several foster families also completed the MCPQ-R to enroll extra dogs into

the study (n=9). Some intake dates were unknown for these dogs, making the Length of

Stay difficult to calculate. However, if the month of intake was known, then the middle

date of the month (i.e., the 16th) was used as the intake date. The dogs that were

enrolled either by owner surrender or by staff, volunteers, and foster families are

referred to in this dissertation as “enrolled dogs.”

Page 77: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

58

Contingencies

If the dog was returned to the shelter, was lost, or died within two weeks of

adoption, the MCPQ-R was not administered, as this is insufficient time to become

familiar with the dog’s personality. If returned within two weeks, the dog was re-

enrolled in the study, and the first IPIP was disregarded. If the dog was returned to the

shelter between two weeks and the two-month adoption follow-up date, the MCPQ-R

was completed on site by the surrendering owner, and the LAPS questionnaire was sent

to them in the mail. In this circumstance, on the rare occasion that the LAPS was not

returned by mail to SW, the dog was re-enrolled in the study in an attempt to obtain all

four questionnaires for the dog (relinquisher-rated MCPQ-R, IPIP, adopter-rated MCPQ-

R, and the LAPS). If the dog was lost or died between two weeks and the two-month

follow-up date of adoption, I sent the MCPQ-R and LAPS surveys in the mail with our

condolences.

Stray dogs were not excluded from the study, as there was an equal opportunity

for stray and relinquished dogs to become adopted. If a stray dog was adopted and

included in this study, I followed up with adopters as usual.

SW sometimes contacted adopters on multiple occasions to increase the

response rate of the adopters. These repeated attempts to contact the adopters were

documented. If the adopter did not respond after four phone calls, four e-mail

messages, or a combination of those, then the associated dog was considered a “loss to

follow-up.” For these dogs, the adopter-rated MCPQ-R and the LAPS were not received.

Page 78: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

59

Returned Dogs

Return of study dogs to the shelter was tracked throughout the study; there

were 12 returns out of 79 adoptions (15%) at CCHS, and 19 returns out of 264 adoptions

(7%) at HSI. An adopted study dog was considered to be a return if the adoptive owner

returned the dog to the shelter. Therefore, it was assumed that all dogs were retained in

the adopter’s home, unless the owner notified the researchers that the dog was

rehomed to another family.

Data Management and Analysis

Data Security

Questionnaires were printed on Purdue University Veterinary Clinical Sciences

letterheads, stapled, hole-punched, and individually labeled with a unique number. In

order to reach a broader audience of relinquishers and adopters, the final versions of

the Information Sheet and all questionnaires (IPIP, MCPQ-R, and LAPS) were translated

by a member of Purdue University’s Cultural Center who was certified to translate

written English into written Spanish. A veterinary student, fluent in English and Spanish,

reviewed the documents before the final versions were printed and distributed. All

questionnaires were completed by eight native Spanish speakers.

For questionnaires completed at surrender, each patron would return the

completed questionnaire to SW or KA and it would be secured in a manila envelope until

transferred to SW’s office for data entry. All data were kept in three-ring binders in a

locked office.

Page 79: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

60

SW and KA had access to HSI’s PetPoint account (i.e., information database) with

a unique login and password. They were able to document study-relevant notes for each

dog as well as post-adoption notes entered in PetPoint. This database was not available

at CCHS and all study-relevant notes were hand-written and secured in SW’s locked

office.

Participation and Response Rate

In general, patrons were enthusiastic about participating in the study, and many

adopters have contacted SW regarding study results. Ninety-eight percent of the

relinquishers approached at CCHS and 99% of the relinquishers approached at HSI

agreed to participate in the study. If SW and KA were not on site for adoptions of study-

enrolled dogs, then the adopter was contacted by phone or e-mail and the IPIP

questionnaire was sent through the mail. If the adopter could not be contacted and the

IPIP questionnaires were not sent back, this was calculated as part of the participation

rate instead of the response rate. Adopters’ initial participation was 87% at CCHS, and

96% at HSI.

There was a moderate response rate of 78% for adopters from CCHS, and 76%

for adopters from HSI, sending the follow-up questionnaires back to SW in the mail or

completing the forms online. Overall response rates were 72% for CCHS, and 76% for

HSI. These numbers are lower than the participation rates when participants were

approached upon intake of the animal, possibly because face-to-face interactions with

people encouraged interest and participation in the study. At the follow-up stage of the

Page 80: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

61

study, “losses to follow-up” may have been due to the incorrect phone number or

address being given by the adopter, or general lack of interest in participation after

adoption.

Descriptive Statistics – Enrolled Dogs

Average surrenders per month were 6.2 at CCHS and 11.9 at HSI, with April 2013

(n=15) and March 2013 (n=24) having the highest number of surrenders during the

study at each site, respectively. Adoptions of enrolled study dogs averaged 5.1 per

month at CCHS and 13.4 at HSI, with October 2013 (n=11) and March 2014 (n=24)

having the highest amount of adoptions at each site, respectively. The numbers of

adopted dogs depended on how many staff, volunteer, and foster families completed

additional questionnaires, so the descriptive statistics for adoptions are biased.

Seasonally, both shelters saw an increase in the number of surrenders and adoptions in

the spring. The dogs in our sample were found to score, on average, higher on

Extraversion and Amicability, moderately on Motivation and Training Focus, and lower

on Neuroticism.

Length of Stay, calculated as the time between the date of intake and the date of

adoption, was quite different between the two shelters. Not surprisingly, the Length of

Stay was shorter for animals at HSI; HSI pulls highly adoptable dogs from Indianapolis

Animal Care and Control and implements intake appointments for owner surrenders, to

manage its in-house animal population. Overall, the Length of Stay varied between

different personality traits (Table 4).

Page 81: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

62

Table 4. Length of Stay in Days for Study Dogs at CCHS and HSI.

Length of Stay CCHS HSI

Average 62.7 50.8

Range 0 - 513 (overall median = 15.5) 5 - 223 (overall median = 37.5)

Extraversion* 20 33

Motivation* 56 41

Training Focus* 6 49

Amicability* 16 36.5

Neuroticism* 1 43

*Median Length of Stay reported for those dogs initially enrolled in the study, categorized by primary personality trait. All numbers are reported in days. * Note: For example, the median Length of Stay for those dogs categorized as Extraversion was shorter than those dogs categorized as Motivation.

Page 82: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

63

References

Anderson, D.S., 2007. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale, LAPS (Johnson et al. 1992). In:

Beck, A.M. (ed.), Assessing the Human-Animal Bond: A Compendium of Actual

Measures. Purdue University Press. West Lafayette, IN, USA.

Johnson, T.P., Garrity, T.F., Stallones, L.,1992. Psychometric Evaluation of the Lexington

Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS). Anthrozoos 5, 160-175.

Ley, J.M., Bennett, P.C., Coleman, G.J., 2009a. A refinement and validation of the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire (MCPQ). Applied Animal Behaviour

Science 116, 220-227.

Ley, J.M., McGreevy, P., Bennett, P.C., 2009b. Inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R). App. Anim. Behav.

Sci. 119, 85-90.

Marston, L.C., Bennett, P.C., Coleman, G.J., 2005. Adopting shelter dogs: Owner

experiences of the first month post-adoption. Anthrozoos 18, 358-378.

Mondelli, F., Previde, E.P., Verga, M., Levi, D., Magistrelli, S., Valsecchi, P., 2004. The

bond that never developed: Adoption and relinquishment of dogs in a rescue

shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 7, 253-266.

Poresky, R.H., 1989. Analyzing human-animal relationship measures. Anthrozoos. 2,

236-244.

Page 83: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

64

Salman, M.D., New, J.G., Scarlett, J.M., Kass, P.H., Ruch-Gallie, R., Hetts, S., 1998. Human

and animal factors related to the relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected

animal shelters in the United States. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science

1, 207-226.

Salman, M.D., Hutchison, J., Ruch-Gallie, R., Kogan, L., New, Jr., J.C., Kass, P.H., Scarlett,

J.M., 2000. Behavioral reasons for relinquishment of dogs and cats to 12

shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 3, 93-106.

Socha, A., Cooper, C.A., McCord, D.M., 2010. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the M5-50:

An Implementation of the International Personality Item Pool Item Set. Psych.

Assess. 22(1), 43-49.

Svartberg, K., Tapper, I., Temrin, H., Radesater, T., Thorman, S., 2005. Consistency of

personality traits in dogs. Animal Behaviour 69, 283-291.

Wells, D.L., Hepper, P.G., 2000. Prevalence of behavior problems reported by owners of

dogs purchased from an animal rescue shelter. Applied Animal Behaviour

Science 69, 55-65.

Page 84: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

65

CHAPTER 4. COMPARISON OF DOG PERSONALITY RATINGS BY RELINQUISHERS AND ADOPTERS AT TWO INDIANA SHELTERS

Page 85: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

66

Introduction

Personality influences how animals (including humans) act, how they interact

with conspecifics as well as other species, and how they react to different environments

and circumstances (Larsen and Buss, 2010). Environmental interactions include

perceptions and how we interpret different situations: two people may live with the

same dog, yet how they interact with the dog and how they rate the dog’s personality

may be very different (Larsen and Buss, 2010). People also have different perceptions

and views about dog ownership, which may or may not lead to successful adoptions or

how personality is rated (Kidd et al., 1992; Marston et al., 2005; Weng et al., 2006).

Perception is particularly of interest, especially in animal sheltering, where many

environmental and personal factors influence a person’s decision to surrender their own

dog or adopt a new companion.

Because animals cannot complete self-reports, we rely on people to examine

individual differences in animals. This can be done by behavioral coding, where humans

score an animal’s behavior in accordance with pre-determined criteria, recording

duration of behavior, frequency of behavior, or occurrence of behavior in a specific

context (Highfill et al., 2010). Another way to measure animal personality is to rate

behavioral or personality tendencies using adjectives which summarize a range of traits

or dimensions (Cavanaugh et al., 2008; Curb et al., 2013; Gosling et al., 2003; Highfill et

al., 2010; Ley et al., 2008, 2009a,b; Mirko et al., 2013). Administering questionnaires is a

common way to survey those people who are familiar with the focal animal. This is

practically important in an animal shelter setting because shelter staff often rely on the

Page 86: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

67

information given (i.e., obtaining medical and behavioral histories) by the owner

surrendering the dog to make disposition decisions. Because these personality ratings

are subjective and based on the responder’s experience with the animal (Highfill et al.,

2010), perception of an animal’s personality may differ between responders. No

previous research has been conducted with shelter dogs regarding how different people

rate the same dog’s personality.

The Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R) (Ley et al.,

2008, 2009a,b) was chosen as a measure of dog personality in this study, because it is a

reliable measure of dog personality with strong inter-rater reliability. Researchers who

developed the MCPQ-R analyzed inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the MCPQ-R,

showing that agreement between raters for each dog was strong on all dimensions (Ley

et al., 2009b). These correlations range from 0.75 (Neuroticism) to 0.86 (Extraversion)

for inter-rater reliability and 0.79 (Neuroticism) to 0.93 (Motivation) for test-retest

reliability. The MCPQ-R was also chosen as a measure of dog personality in this study,

because the short 26-item questionnaire is easy to administer in a shelter setting and

takes the responder roughly five minutes to complete. Shelters can use dog personality

information, completed by the previous owner, to assist in making decisions (i.e.,

implementing an enrichment program for a dog that scored high in Neuroticism), or

possibly to implement a personality matching program. The aim for this study is to

investigate the level of agreement on the ratings of dog personality by the relinquishers

in comparison to the new adopters using the MCPQ-R.

Page 87: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

68

Materials and Methods

Study Participants

Between January 24, 2013 and March 28, 2014, a convenience sample of 197

dogs from two Indiana shelters – Clinton County Humane Society (CCHS) and the

Humane Society of Indianapolis (HSI) – was included in this study. Each dog had two

questionnaires completed: one by the owner surrendering upon intake, and one by the

adopter upon a two-month follow-up.

To increase the number of dogs in our study, shelter staff and volunteers were

asked to complete MCPQ-R questionnaires for some dogs (n=11 at CCHS and n=41 at

HSI) that were found as strays, or were transferred from other shelters. In order to be

qualified, the staff had to spend at least one and a half hours each day with the certain

dog during husbandry, enrichment, and social time for two weeks. Several foster

families also completed the MCPQ-R to enroll extra dogs into the study (n=1 at CCHS

and n=8 at HSI), with the only inclusion criteria of having the dog living with the family

for at least two weeks.

This was a subset of dogs enrolled in this research project as outlined in Chapter

3 of this dissertation. The dog must have been at least a year old upon surrender in

order to be eligible for enrollment in the study. The relinquishers must have been at

least 18 years old and have owned the dog for at least two weeks in order to be eligible

for the study at intake. There was no other eligibility criterion for the adopters other

than being at least 18 years old to participate in the study. For those responders who

adopted more than one dog, one dog was randomly selected for data analysis.

Page 88: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

69

Dog Personalities

The MCPQ-R was used to assess dog personalities, and is a 26-item

questionnaire (Appendices B and D) containing adjectives which describe five overall

personality traits of a dog: Extraversion, Motivation, Training Focus, Amicability, and

Neuroticism. The MCPQ-R was administered to the owners surrendering their dogs at

participating shelters by the researchers, and also to the new adopters of those dogs

after two months of adoption via mail. Responders rated 26 adjectives about their dog

on a Likert scale with 1 = “Really does not describe my dog” to 6 = “Really describes my

dog.” A percent score (i.e., sum divided by the maximum and multiplied by 100) was

computed for each of the five traits. The trait with the largest percent score were

selected to represent the primary personality category for that specific dog. For those

dogs that had tied primary personality traits on the MCPQ-R completed by the

relinquisher, shelter staff members were asked to spend additional time with those

dogs over the next two weeks, in order to determine which of the tied categories were

more representative of that dog. For those dogs that had tied primary personality

categories on the MCPQ-R completed by the adopters, one personality category was

randomly selected for data analysis. It took, on average, about five minutes for the

study participants to complete the MCPQ-R. Please refer to Chapter 2 of this

dissertation for further questionnaire administration and scoring details.

Page 89: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

70

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics were presented to describe the demographics of the study

dogs, as well as their lengths of stay. Dog personality was measured as the percent

scores for each personality trait and also as primary traits. The differences in the

average percent scores between the two raters were statistically compared using the

paired t-tests. Concordance correlation coefficients were derived to assess the

agreement in percent scores between the two raters (Lin 1989). Kappa statistics were

used to assess the agreement on the primary personality category between the

relinquishers and the adopters. Microsoft Excel (2007), IBM® SPSS® Statistics for

Windows (Version 21.0.0.0.), and MedCalc for Windows (Version 13.2.2.0.) were used

for data analyses. Statistical significance was set at 0.05.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Out of 197 dogs, 175 (89%) were mixed breed. The median age was 26 months,

and ranged from 12 to 180 months (average = 36.5 months). There were similar

numbers of males (n=103) and females (n=94) in this sample. The median length of stay

at CCHS was 22 days, ranging from zero to 513 days (average = 69.6 days). The median

length of stay at HSI was 37 days, ranging from five to 223 days (average = 49.9 days).

Further details are explained in Chapter 3 of this dissertation.

Page 90: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

71

Comparison of Percent Scores for each Personality Trait between Relinquishers and

Adopters

The paired t-test was used to compare percent scores between responders on

each personality trait. Comparatively, the average differences in paired means for each

of the five personality traits were all relatively close to zero, signifying that there are no

differences between the percent scores from each responder’s rating of each category.

On average, relinquishers’ percent scores for Training Focus and Amicability were 2%

and 3% lower, respectively, than adopters’ ratings for those traits. However, just

because the average differences in means were close to zero does not mean that the

responders agree on rating the primary dog personality traits. Further statistical analysis

using concordance correlation coefficients confirmed low agreement of ratings. These

data are visually presented in scatter plots (Figures 1 through 5), with the 45 degree line

representing perfect agreement (i.e., if most of the data points fell along the 45 degree

line, the concordance correlation coefficient would be closer to 1). The data points

located furthest from the line represent low agreement (i.e., if most of the data points

were far away from the 45 degree line, the concordance correlation coefficient would

be closer to, or even below, 0). All personality traits had low concordance correlation

coefficients: Extraversion (0.15), Motivation (-0.02), Training Focus (0.08), Amicability

(0.15), and Neuroticism (0. 21).

Page 91: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

72

Figure 1. Comparison of Percent Scores for Extraversion between Relinquishers and

Adopters.

Note: The 45 degree line represents perfect agreement.

0%

10%

20%

30%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

90%

100%

0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100%

Relin

quis

her

Rating

Adopter Rating

Extraversion

Page 92: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

73

Figure 2. Comparison of Percent Scores for Motivation between Relinquishers and

Adopters.

Note: The 45 degree line represents perfect agreement.

0%

10%

20%

30%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

90%

100%

0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100%

Relin

quis

her

Rating

Adopter Rating

Motivation

Page 93: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

74

Figure 3. Comparison of Percent Scores for Training Focus between Relinquishers and

Adopters.

Note: The 45 degree line represents perfect agreement.

0%

10%

20%

30%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

90%

100%

0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100%

Relin

quis

her

Rating

Adopter Rating

Training Focus

Page 94: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

75

Figure 4. Comparison of Percent Scores for Amicability between Relinquishers and

Adopters.

Note: The 45 degree line represents perfect agreement.

0%

10%

20%

30%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

90%

100%

0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100%

Relin

quis

her

Rating

Adopter Rating

Amicability

Page 95: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

76

Figure 5. Comparison of Percent Scores for Neuroticism between Relinquishers and

Adopters.

Note: The 45 degree line represents perfect agreement.

Agreement of Primary Dog Personality Categories between Relinquishers and Adopters

Table 5 shows the comparison of primary personality traits between the

responders, with an overall agreement of 36% (i.e., 36% of all dogs included in the

sample were rated the same primary personality trait between both responders).

Included in the table are frequency counts and conditional percentages of dogs with

those relinquisher-rated primary personality traits that were also rated by the adopters.

The bolded diagonal values represent frequency counts and conditional percentages of

0%

10%

20%

30%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

90%

100%

0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100%

Relin

quis

her

Rating

Adopter Rating

Neuroticism

Page 96: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

77

dogs rated to be the same primary trait by the relinquisher and the adopter. With a

kappa value of 0.10 (p=0.02), there is low agreement on primary dog personality

categories reported by the relinquishers and the adopters.

Table 5. Primary dog personality categories rated by relinquishers and adopters.a

Adopters

Relinquishers Extraversion Motivation Training Focus Amicability Neuroticism

Extraversion 30 (37%) 10 (12%) 13 (16%) 27 (33%) 1 (1%)

Motivation 4 (36%) 0 (0%) 2 (18%) 5 (46%) 0 (0%)

Training Focus 6 (19%) 3 (10%) 7 (23%) 12 (39%) 3 (10%)

Amicability 15 (22%) 8 (12%) 7 (10%) 33 (49%) 5 (7%)

Neuroticism 1 (17%) 0 (0%) 2 (33%) 2 (33%) 1 (17%)

Note: Frequency counts (% of dogs rated by the relinquisher that were rated by the adopter; e.g., 22% of the dogs, rated by the relinquisher as Amicability, were rated by the adopter as Extraversion). a: The bolded diagonal values represent frequency counts and conditional percentages of dogs rated to be the same primary trait by the relinquisher and the adopter.

Discussion

Dog personality questionnaires were administered upon intake of surrendered

dogs at two Indiana shelters. Dog personality questionnaires were then administered

two months after adoption to the new owners. This study compared personality ratings

between the two responders. Dog personality was analyzed as a continuous variable,

comparing mean differences in percent scores, and as a categorical variable, comparing

agreement on primary personality categories between two raters. The results from

Cavanaugh et al. (2008) showed that two different raters of the same dog’s personality

Page 97: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

78

are moderately consistent. In this study, the results from the within-paired t-test

showed that on average, relinquishers’ percent scores for Training Focus and Amicability

were 2% and 3% lower, respectively, than adopters’ ratings for those traits. Because the

average difference in paired mean percent scores for Amicability was near statistical

significance, these results may not be due to chance. Adopters may have rated this trait

higher, on average, than did relinquishers, due to an effect of the emotion behind

adoption: adopters are happy with their new companions; and therefore, rate their dogs

as more Amicable.

Concordance correlation coefficients (Lin, 1989) were calculated to assess

agreement of percent scores. All dog personality traits had low concordance correlation

coefficients, ranging from -0.02 (Motivation) to 0.21 (Neuroticism), indicating weak

agreement of rating dog personality traits between responders. With a kappa value of

0.10 (p=0.02), there was low agreement on primary dog personality categories reported

by the relinquishers and the adopters. The kappa value was statistically significant,

signifying that the results were not due to chance; this demonstrates a significant yet

small effect (i.e., a small but important indication that relinquishers and adopters show

weak agreement on rating dog personality). An association exists, as indicated by the

statistical test, but it is just a small effect. However, this association is not meaningless;

the relinquishers and adopters agree more than chance levels, but less than expected.

Because of the significant finding, increasing the number of dogs enrolled in this study

would not change the effect size resulting in low agreement.

Page 98: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

79

These results could be due to human factors such as differences in exposures

(i.e., individual experiences or duration of ownership) to the animal between

responders or differences in perception of personality traits (Highfill et al., 2010). The

biases that people have about themselves when relinquishing or adopting may cloud

their judgment and rating of their dogs’ personality, thus lending to the results of low

agreement. Currently, there are no standard methods of assessing dog personality

within the animal shelter context (i.e., at relinquishment, during the dog’s stay in the

shelter, or post-adoption). The low agreement found in this study suggests that more

research needs to be done in order to accurately integrate assessments of dog

personality into current shelter policies.

These results could also be circumstantial due to environmental factors, where

relinquishers might not completely answer the personality questionnaire honestly if

they think the results will impact the adoptability of that dog (New et al., 1999; Posage

et al., 1998; Segurson et al., 2005). To try and combat this Social Desirability Bias,

research participants were told that participation was completely voluntary, their

information would be completely confidential, and their answers would have no impact

on their dog’s disposition in the shelter. However, it is unknown how much influence

these factors had on how truthfully relinquishers rated dog personality.

Possibly choosing responders who have had different exposure histories to the

same dog, may not be the most reliable way to rate dog personality (Highfill et al.,

2010). Behavioral coding may prove to help obtain higher agreement between

responders (e.g., behavior assessments). However, for practicality and application in a

Page 99: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

80

shelter setting, personality rating instead of behavioral coding was used in the current

study, and the raters included both relinquishers and new adopters of each dog. Each

responder cohabitated and provided care to enrolled study dogs, which provided the

foundation to rate that specific dog’s personality on several dimensions. Staff-

completed questionnaires of strays, (unowned dogs that were brought in by either

patrons or animal control officers) and dogs that were transferred in from other

humane organizations, were integrated into the data set to increase sample size. It was

considered that the staff had spent quality time with the dogs in order to thoroughly

rate their personalities. Further eligibility criteria are explained in Chapter 3 of this

dissertation. Due to lack of sample size when separating relinquisher-completed

questionnaires and staff-completed questionnaires, this study did not include a

comparison of ratings between relinquishers and staff. However, it would be interesting

to assess any differences between these two groups of responders in future studies;

results might lead to a standardized dog personality assessment used during the dog’s

stay at the shelter.

Another possible explanation for the low agreement between responders could

be the exposure time, which could also be quite variable. Length of ownership is an

important factor (Dotson and Hyatt, 2008), because the owner surrendering may have

owned the dog for several years and had much more exposure to the dog than the new

adoptive family, who had only had approximately two months with the dog in this study

protocol. I did not record length of ownership upon surrender in this study. However, it

would be interesting to assess relationships between length of ownership at surrender

Page 100: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

81

and length of ownership at adoption, and its effects on dog personality ratings in future

studies.

Ratings could have also differed between responders due to the opposite

circumstances of each individual (New et al., 2000; Segurson et al., 2005). Surrendering

a family pet is not an easy decision to make, so relinquishers may have responded to the

survey according to their emotions, or according to what they think the shelter staff

wanted to see. On the other hand, adopting a family pet is usually quite a happy time,

so adopters may have responded to the survey in an overly positive light. Further

research is needed to assess the impact of emotions at the time of questionnaire

completion on the dog personality assessment results.

The 26 items on the MCPQ-R are only adjectives with no operational definitions,

which leave room for interpretation. It is very possible that this study showed no

agreement between responders because the responders had different interpretations of

what the adjectives meant when describing their dogs. Anecdotally, the adjectives

“Persevering” and “Tenacious” (factors of the trait “Motivation”) and “Biddable” (factor

of the trait “Training Focus”) were most often left blank with no rating; patrons

completing the MCPQ-R at surrender were also most likely to have questions about the

meaning of these three words, as well. One limitation of the current study was using an

Australian-developed questionnaire, where these three terms may not be as widely

known in the general population in Indiana where these studies were conducted. A

Principal Components Analysis (PCA) of common American English terms used to

describe dog personality would be useful in developing a questionnaire that may be

Page 101: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

82

more relevant in the United States. Animal shelters could also standardize the

questionnaire by including a reference page to respondents, explaining each adjective’s

operational definition.

Another major limitation in this study was the scoring system to choose a

primary personality category for each dog, because personality is multi- dimensional

(i.e., a dog’s personality is comprised of more than one trait, or category, so choosing a

primary category to represent the dog’s personality as a whole is not representative.

Differences in the means of each personality trait between responder were compared;

this method attempted to overcome the previously mentioned limitation, and

accounted for all five traits for each dog. Another limitation encountered during the

study was low enrollment of study dogs at relinquishment. Although the staff were the

second best choice for stray or transferred dogs, it was ideal to be able to recruit as

many dogs as possible.

Despite these limitations, this study is important because it discusses the

characteristics of personality ratings of dogs at two Indiana shelters and possible human

factors that may impact the ratings. Currently, there is no standardized method of

intake or behavioral/personality assessment of surrendered dogs. This poses a threat to

the reliability of information given by the owner upon a dog’s surrender to an animal

shelter. Shelters should use caution when implementing intake procedures and

gathering information from the relinquisher; the results from this study show that even

when a reliable dog personality assessment is administered to relinquishers and

adopters, there is very low agreement on rating dog personality.

Page 102: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

83

The current research project compared the differences between responders on

rating dog personality. Further research is needed, especially in animal shelters, to

invest in a longitudinal study with adopters to see if dog personality is consistently rated

by the same responder over multiple administrations of the same questionnaire.

However, because relinquishers and adopters rate dog personality differently, shelter

personnel should be cautious about using the relinquisher-rated dog personality to

market certain types of dogs, or how dogs are matched to potential adopters. Chapter 5

explores the associations between both relinquisher- and adopter-rated dog personality

with human personality traits. Chapter 6 explores the predictability of a successful

adoption using dog and human personality.

Page 103: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

84

References

Cavanaugh, L.A., Leonard, H.A., Scammon, D.L., 2008. A trail of two personalities: How

canine companions shape relationships and well-being. Journal of Business

Research 61, 469-479.

Curb, L.A., Abramson, C.I., Grice, J.W., Kennison, S.M., 2013. The relationship between

personality match and pet satisfaction among dog owners. Anthrozoos 26, 395-

404.

Dotson, M.J., Hyatt, E.M., 2008. Understanding dog-human companionship. Journal of

Business Research 61, 457-466.

Gosling, S.D., Kwan, V.S.Y., John, O.P., 2003. A dog’s got personality: A cross-species

comparative approach to personality judgments in dogs and humans. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology 85, 1161-1169.

Highfill, L., Hanbury, D., Kristiansen, R., Kuczaj, S., Watson, S., 2010. Rating vs. coding in

animal personality research. Zoo Biology 29, 509-516.

Kidd, A.H., Kidd, R.M., George, C.C., 1992. Successful and unsuccessful pet adoptions.

Psychological Reports 70, 547-561.

Larsen, R.J. and Buss, D.M., 2010. Personality Psychology: Domains of Knowledge About

Human Nature, pp. 8. McGraw Hill. New York, New York, USA.

Ley, J., Bennett, P., Coleman, G., 2008. Personality dimensions that emerge in

companion canines. Applied Animal Behavior Science 110, 305-317.

Page 104: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

85

Ley, J.M., Bennett, P.C., Coleman, G.J., 2009a. A refinement and validation of the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire (MCPQ). Applied Animal Behaviour

Science 116, 220-227.

Ley, J.M., McGreevy, P., Bennett, P.C., 2009b. Inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R). Applied Animal

Behaviour Science 119, 85-90.

Lin, L.I-K., 1989. A concordance correlation coefficient to evaluate reproducibility.

Biometrics 45, 255-268.

Marston, L.C., Bennett, P.C., Coleman, G.J., 2005. Adopting shelter dogs: Owner

experiences of the first month post-adoption. Anthrozoos 18, 358-378.

Mirko, E., Doka, A., Miklosi, A., 2013. Association between subjective rating and

behavior coding and the role of experience in making video assessments on the

personality of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour

Science 149, 45-54.

New, J.C., Salman, M.D., Scarlett, J.M., Kass, P.H., Vaughn, J.A., Scherr, S., et al., 1999.

Moving: Characteristics of dogs and cats and those relinquishing them to 12 U.S.

animal shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2, 83-96.

Posage, J.M., Bartlett, P.C., Thomas, D.K., 1998. Determining factors for successful

adoption of dogs from an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary

Medical Association 213, 478-482.

Page 105: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

86

Segurson, S.A., Serpell, J.A., Hart, B.L., 2005. Evaluation of a behavioral assessment

questionnaire for use in the characterization of behavioral problems of dogs

relinquished to animal shelters. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical

Association 227, 1755-1761.

Weng, H-Y., Kass, P.H., Hart, L.A., Chomel, B.B., 2006. Risk factors for unsuccessful dog

ownership: An epidemiologic study in Taiwan. Preventive Veterinary Medicine

77, 82-95.

Page 106: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

87

CHAPTER 5. ASSESSMENT OF ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN DOG AND HUMAN PERSONALITY TRAITS USING THE MCPQ-R AND IPIP QUESTIONNAIRES

Page 107: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

88

Introduction

Thousands of dogs are surrendered every year to animal shelters due to a variety

of dog-related reasons, such as health or behavioral problems, or owner-related

reasons, such as relocation and being unable to take the dog with them, unemployment,

or they do not have time for the dog (Arkow & Dow, 1984; Mondelli et al., 2004;

Patronek et al., 1996; Salman et al., 1998). Some relinquishments may also be due to a

disconnect in the human-animal bond, where a lack of attachment to the pet influences

the owner’s decision to relinquish (Kass et al., 2001; Kidd et al., 1992; Salman et al.,

2000). One of the goals for animal shelter personnel is to ensure a good match is made

at adoption, so that a strong human-animal bond can form between adopter and new

companion (Kullar, 2013). Matching the prospective adopter with its future companion,

based on activity level, lifestyle, expectations, and personality is important: if a solid

match is made, higher satisfaction and fewer relinquishments are expected (Curb et al.,

2013; Palmer and Custance, 2008). If reasons for some surrenders are due to a

mismatch between dog and owner personality, then this may have implications on how

shelters match dogs with potential adopters

There is an abundance of research investigating relationships between

personality traits in different people and relationship values such as success and

satisfaction (Bentler and Newcomb, 1978; Bouchard and Areseneault, 2005; Brehm et

al., 2002; Donnellan et al., 2004; Karney and Bradbury, 1995; Kelly and Conley, 1987;

White et al., 2004). Some people develop very strong relationships with canine

companions (Beck and Katcher, 1996; Marston et al., 2005; Mondelli et al., 2004; Sable,

Page 108: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

89

2013), so personality was investigated using a cross-species approach to assess

associations between dog and human personality traits.

In one study, dog and human personality traits were found to be significantly

related, when the Big Five Inventory (BFI) (John and Srivastava, 1999) was used to

measure human personality in dog owners, and the canine-Big Five Inventory (canine-

BFI) (Gosling et al., 2003) was used to measure dog personality (Cavanaugh et al., 2008).

Because Cavanaugh et al. (2008) felt that Conscientiousness was a human-only

personality trait that was unable to be measured in dogs (i.e., Conscientiousness

measures an individual’s self-discipline, organization, and achievement-motivation), the

resulting matched human-dog pairs included Openness/Intelligence,

Extraversion/Energy, Agreeableness/Affection, and Neuroticism/Emotional Reactivity.

Authors reported that owners are more likely to score their dogs higher on Extraversion

and Neuroticism if the owners also scored higher on Extraversion (Cavanaugh et al.,

2008).

However, the canine-BFI was developed by changing the wording on the BFI,

where necessary, to make it applicable to dogs. This method is flawed, as it assumes

that the human personality traits measured on the BFI are projected onto dogs, and that

dogs exhibit the same personality traits as humans (Cavanaugh et al., 2008; Gosling et

al., 2003). Because dogs and humans are different species, dog personality dimensions

may not necessarily be representative of human analogues of personality. Therefore,

the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R) (Ley et al., 2008; Ley et

al., 2009a,b) was used to measure dog personality. In primary studies using the MCPQ-

Page 109: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

90

R, (Ley et al., 2008) used principal components analysis and revealed five factors for

grouping adjectives. These factors were labeled Extraversion, Motivation, Training

Focus, Amicability, and Neuroticism and were used in this study to represent canine

personality traits. Additional information on how the MCPQ-R was developed can be

found in Chapter 2 of this dissertation.

For assessing human personality, the 50-item International Personality Item Pool

(IPIP) (Goldberg et al., 2006; Socha et al., 2010) was chosen because it measures

personality on the five-factor model (FFM), the most commonly used model for human

personality (Costa and McCrae, 1992a, b; Goldberg, 1990). Possible human personality

categories that were extracted from the 50-item IPIP were: Extraversion, Agreeableness,

Conscientiousness, Stability, and Openness. The 50-item questionnaire also was easily

obtainable and feasible to implement in an animal shelter setting, where adopters

already are required to fill out paperwork (i.e., an adoption application per shelter

procedures), and there was a limited amount of time available to complete the extra

questionnaire from this study. The IPIP questionnaire was estimated to only take seven

minutes to complete.

The aim of this study was to evaluate relationships between shelter dog and

human personality traits using the MCPQ-R and IPIP questionnaires. The previous study

(Chapter 4 of this dissertation) showed that there was no agreement between

responders in how dog personality is rated using the MCPQ-R. In order to thoroughly

assess the relationships between dog and human personality traits, the data were

analyzed using both relinquisher- and adopter-rated dog personalities.

Page 110: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

91

Materials and Methods

Study Participants

Between January 24, 2013, and March 28, 2014, a convenience sample of 262

dogs from two Indiana shelters – Clinton County Humane Society (CCHS) and the

Humane Society of Indianapolis (HSI) – was included in this study using relinquisher-

rated dog personality data (Study A). Also, a convenience sample of 251 dogs was

included in the second study using adopter-rated dog personality data (Study B). A third

study (Study C), examined relationships between relinquisher-rated dog personality and

adopter-rated dog personality with human personality using the dogs that were rated

by both relinquishers and adopters, which included a sample size 197 dogs.

Dogs were included in Study A if the relinquisher completed the MCPQ-R and the

adopter completed the IPIP. For these dogs, no follow-up questionnaires needed to be

completed. For Study B, dogs were included if the adopter completed the IPIP and also

completed the MCPQ-R two months after adoption. 54 of these dogs were recruited by

the randomization process and had no relinquisher-rated dog personality recorded.

Study C includes dogs which have two MCPQ-R questionnaires (one completed by the

relinquisher and the other completed by the adopter) and the IPIP completed. The 197

dogs in Study C are also included in both Studies A and B. To increase the number of

dogs in our study, shelter staff and volunteers were asked to complete MCPQ-R

questionnaires for some dogs (Study A: n=18 at CCHS and n=61 at HSI; Studies B and C:

n=11 at CCHS and n=41 at HSI) that were found as strays or were transferred from other

shelters. In order to be qualified, the staff had to spend at least an hour and a half each

Page 111: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

92

day with the specific dog during husbandry, enrichment, and social time for two weeks.

Several foster families also completed the MCPQ-R to enroll extra dogs into the study

(Study A: n=1 at CCHS and n=9 at HSI; Studies B and C: n=1 at CCHS and 8 at HSI), with

the only inclusion criteria of having the dog living with the family for at least two weeks.

Each sample was a subset of dogs enrolled in this research project as outlined in

Chapter 3 of this dissertation. The dog must have been at least a year old upon

surrender in order to be eligible for enrollment in the study. For those adopted more

than one dog, one dog per responder was randomly selected for data analysis.

The relinquishers must have been at least 18 years old and have owned the dog

for at least two weeks in order to be eligible for the study at intake. There was no other

eligibility criterion for new adopters other than being at least 18 years old to participate

in the study. Some adopters included in the follow-up portion (Studies B and C) were

recruited using a randomization protocol that is explained in Appendix A of this

dissertation. If participants were recruited in the lobby of each shelter per the

randomization protocol, then the research staff would approach them in the same

manner when other adoptions were finalized. Follow-up procedures were the same.

Details about each shelter’s intake procedures and study eligibility criteria were

explained previously in Chapter 3 of this dissertation. The same research staff (SW at

CCHS and KA at HSI) collected data for this project. The same adoption and follow-up

procedures were followed per Chapters 3 and 4 of this dissertation. Two months after

the adoption, SW called or e-mailed adopters to let them know that a packet would be

sent to them in the mail which included the MCPQ-R and a pre-addressed/stamped

Page 112: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

93

envelope. For those adopters who SW was unable to contact or did not return the

follow-up questionnaires to SW via standard mail, these dogs were considered lost to

follow-up.

Dog and Human Personalities

The details of the MCPQ-R and how dog personalities are categorized are

explained previously in Chapters 3 and 4 of this dissertation. Briefly, the MCPQ-R is a 26-

item questionnaire (Appendices B and D) containing adjectives which describe five

overall personality traits of a dog: Extraversion, Motivation, Training Focus, Amicability,

and Neuroticism. The MCPQ-R was administered to the owners surrendering their dogs

at participating shelters by the researchers and also to the new adopters of those dogs

after two months of adoption via mail or online survey. Responders rated 26 adjectives

about their dog on a Likert scale with 1 = “Really does not describe my dog” to 6 =

“Really describes my dog.” A percent score (i.e., sum divided by the maximum and

multiplied by 100) was computed for each of the five traits. The trait with the largest

percent score would be selected to represent the primary personality category for that

specific dog. For those dogs that had tied primary personality traits on the MCPQ-R

completed by the relinquisher, shelter staff members were asked to spend some quality

time with those specific dogs over the next two weeks in order to determine which of

the tied categories were more representative of that dog. For those dogs that had tied

primary personality categories on the MCPQ-R completed by the adopters, one

Page 113: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

94

personality category was randomly selected for data analysis (Studies B and C). It took,

on average, about five minutes for the study participants to complete the MCPQ-R.

The 50-item IPIP (Appendix C) is a reliable measure of the five-factor model of

human personality. The five personality traits include Extraversion, Agreeableness,

Conscientiousness, Stability, and Openness. Standard IPIP instructions were presented

to participants, who responded on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1

(inaccurate) to 5 (accurate) with a neutral midpoint (Socha et al., 2010). A percent score

(i.e., sum divided by the maximum and multiplied by 100) was computed for each of the

five traits. The trait with the largest percent score would be selected to represent the

primary personality category for that specific adopter. For those individuals who had

tied primary personality traits on the IPIP, one personality category was randomly

selected for data analysis. It took, on average, about 10 minutes for the adopters to

complete the IPIP. Further details about scoring are explained in Chapter 3 of this

dissertation.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics were presented to describe the demographics of the study

dogs, as well as their lengths of stay. Dog and human personality were measured as

primary personality categories. A Pearson’s chi-squared test was initially used to look at

the frequency distribution between the five dog personality traits and the five human

personality traits. However, because the expected value was less than five for multiple

combinations, the exact test was used for further analysis to determine the significance

Page 114: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

95

of any associations. Microsoft Excel (2007), IBM® SPSS® Statistics for Windows (Version

21.0.0.0.), and SAS 9.4 for Windows (Version 6.1.7601) were used for data analyses.

Statistical significance was set at 0.05.

Results – Study A

Descriptive Statistics

Out of 262 dogs, 232 (89%) were mixed breed. The median age was 24 months,

and ranged from 12 to 180 months. There were similar numbers of males (n=138) and

females (n=124) in this sample. The median length of stay at CCHS was 16 days, ranging

from zero to 513 days (average = 70 days). The median length of stay at HSI was 36.5

days, ranging from five to 223 days (average = 50 days). Further details are explained in

Chapter 3 of this dissertation.

Associations between Primary Dog and Human Personality Traits

Of the dogs in this sample, 107 (41%) were categorized as Extraverted, 18 (7%)

were categorized as Motivated, 45 (17%) were categorized as Training Focused, 84

(32%) were categorized as Amicable, and eight (3%) were categorized as Neurotic, as

rated by the relinquisher. Table 6 shows the frequency counts of primary dog and

human personality traits. An exact test was run assessing relinquisher-rated dog

personality with human personality. The exact test shows no significant associations

between dog and human personality (p=0.68). The expected values were computed

under the null hypothesis that there was no association between relinquisher-rated dog

Page 115: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

96

personality and human personality traits, under the assumption that these two factors

were completely independent of each other (Table 6). The biggest differences between

expected and observed dog-human personality trait combinations were

Amicability(Dog)-Agreeableness(Human) (difference of 5), and Amicability(Dog)-

Conscientiousness(Human) (difference of 4.4); these samples have larger sample sizes

and thus, these differences, related to the sample size, are not that big.

Table 6. Frequency Counts of Primary Relinquisher-Rated Dog and Human Personality

Traits (n=262).

Humana Dog

Extra. Agree. Conscientious. Stab. Open. Total

Extraversion 6 (6.1) 52 (52.3) 31 (28.6) 4 (3.7) 14 (16.3) 107

Motivation 1 (1.0) 7 (8.8) 5 (4.8) 0 (0.6) 5 (2.7) 18

Training Focus 1 (2.6) 21 (22.0) 13 (12.0) 1 (1.5) 9 (6.9) 45

Amicability 7 (4.8) 46 (41.0) 18 (22.4) 3 (2.9) 10 (12.8) 84

Neuroticism 0 (0.5) 2 (3.9) 3 (2.1) 1 (0.3) 2 (1.2) 8

Total 15 128 70 9 40 262

Note: Expected values are in parentheses. a: Extra. = Extraversion, Agree. = Agreeableness, Conscientious. = Conscientiousness, Stab. = Stability, Open. = Openness.

Page 116: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

97

Results – Study B

Descriptive Statistics

Out of 251 dogs, 228 (91%) were mixed breed. The median age was 25 months,

and ranged from 12 to 180 months. There were similar numbers of males (n=132) and

females (n=119) in this sample. The median length of stay at CCHS was 16 days, ranging

from zero to 513 days (average = 63.5 days). The median length of stay at HSI was 33

days, ranging from five to 223 days (average = 45.3 days). Further details are explained

in Chapter 2 of this dissertation.

Associations between Primary Dog and Human Personality Traits

Of the dogs in this sample, 79 (32%) were categorized as Extraverted, 24 (10%)

were categorized as Motivated, 40 (16%) were categorized as Training Focused, 98

(39%) were categorized as Amicable, and 10 (4%) were categorized as Neurotic, as rated

by the adopter. Table 2 shows the frequency counts of primary dog and human

personality traits. An exact test was run assessing adopter-rated dog personality with

human personality, and showed no significant associations between dog and human

personality (p=0.94). Expected values were calculated based on the current dataset

(Table 7). The biggest differences between expected and observed dog-human

personality trait combinations were Amicability(Dog)-Agreeableness(Human) (difference

of 4.6), Extraversion(Dog)-Agreeableness(Human) (difference of 4), and

Amicability(Dog)-Conscientiousness(Human) (difference of 4).

Page 117: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

98

Table 7. Frequency Counts of Primary Adopter-Rated Dog and Human Personality

Traits (n=251).

Humana Dog

Extra. Agree. Conscientious. Stab. Open. Total

Extraversion 3 (4.1) 35 (39.0) 22 (20.1) 4 (2.5) 15 (13.2) 79

Motivation 1 (1.2) 14 (11.9) 5 (6.1) 0 (0.8) 4 (4.0) 24

Training Focus 3 (2.1) 17 (19.8) 14 (10.2) 1 (1.3) 5 (6.7) 40

Amicability 5 (5.1) 53 (48.4) 21 (25.0) 3 (3.1) 16 (16.4) 98

Neuroticism 1 (0.5) 5 (4.9) 2 (2.5) 0 (0.3) 2 (1.7) 10

Total 13 124 64 8 42 251

Note: Expected values are in parentheses. a: Extra. = Extraversion, Agree. = Agreeableness, Conscientious. = Conscientiousness, Stab. = Stability, Open. = Openness.

Results – Study C

Descriptive Statistics

Out of 197 dogs, 175 (89%) were mixed breed. The median age was 26 months,

and ranged from 12 to 180 months (average = 36.5 months). There were similar

numbers of males (n=103) and females (n=94) in this sample. The median length of stay

at CCHS was 22 days, ranging from zero to 513 days (average = 69.6 days). The median

length of stay at HSI was 37 days, ranging from five to 223 days (average = 49.9 days).

Further details are explained in Chapter 2 of this dissertation.

Page 118: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

99

Associations between Primary Dog and Human Personality Traits

Of the dogs that were relinquished, 81 (41%) were categorized as Extraverted, 11

(6%) were categorized as Motivated, 31 (16%) were categorized as Training Focused, 68

(35%) were categorized as Amicable, and six (3%) were categorized as Neurotic. Of

these same dogs, 56 (28%) were categorized as Extraverted, 21 (11%) were categorized

as Motivated, 31 (16%) were categorized as Training Focused, 79 (40%) were

categorized as Amicable, and 10 (5%) were categorized as Neurotic at adoption follow-

up. Tables 8 and 9 show the frequency counts of the primary dog and human

personalities using both relinquisher-rated and adopter-rated dog personality for the

same dogs, respectively. The exact test shows a near-significant association between

relinquisher-rated dog and human personality (p=0.06), but no significant association

between adopter-rated dog personality and human personality (p=0.93), with this

sample of dogs. Expected values were calculated based on the current dataset. With

Relinquisher-rated dog personality (Table 8), the biggest differences between expected

and observed dog-human personality trait combinations were Amicability(Dog)-

Conscientiousness(Human) (difference of 5.2), and Amicability(Dog)-

Agreeableness(Human) (difference of 5.1). With Adopter-rated dog personality (Table

9), the biggest difference between expected and observed dog-human personality trait

combinations was Motivation(Dog)-Agreeableness(Human) (difference of 3.2).

In Chapter 4, it was shown that relinquishers and adopters do not agree on

rating dog personality of the same dog, so it was assumed that there was no agreement

Page 119: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

100

between relinquisher-rated dog-human personality combinations and adopter-rated

dog-human personality combinations.

Table 8. Frequency Counts of Primary Relinquisher-Rated and Human Personality

Traits (n=197).

Humana Dog

Extra. Agree. Conscientious. Stab. Open. Total

Extraversion 3 (4.1) 40 (41.5) 24 (19.3) 3 (2.9) 11

(13.2)

81

Motivation 0 (0.6) 3 (5.6) 4 (2.6) 0 (0.4) 4 (1.8) 11

Training Focus 0 (1.6) 17 (15.9) 5 (7.4) 1 (1.1) 8 (5.0) 31

Amicability 7 (3.5) 40 (34.9) 11 (16.2) 2 (2.4) 8 (11.0) 68

Neuroticism 0 (0.3) 1 (3.1) 3 (1.4) 1 (0.2) 1 (1.0) 6

Total 10 101 47 7 32 197

Note: Expected values are in parentheses. a: Extra. = Extraversion, Agree. = Agreeableness, Conscientious. = Conscientiousness, Stab. = Stability, Open. = Openness.

Page 120: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

101

Table 9. Frequency Counts of Primary Adopter-Rated Dog and Human Personality

Traits (n=197).

Humana Dog

Extra. Agree. Conscientious. Stab. Open. Total

Extraversion 1 (2.8) 26 (28.7) 15 (13.4) 4 (2.0) 10 (9.1) 56

Motivation 1 (1.1) 14 (10.8) 3 (5.0) 0 (0.7) 3 (3.4) 21

Training Focus 2 (1.6) 15 (15.9) 9 (7.4) 0 (1.1) 5 (5.0) 31

Amicability 5 (4.0) 41 (40.5) 18 (18.8) 3 (2.8) 12 (12.8) 79

Neuroticism 1 (0.5) 5 (5.1) 2 (2.4) 0 (0.4) 2 (1.6) 10

Total 10 101 47 7 32 197

Note: Expected values are in parentheses. a: Extra. = Extraversion, Agree. = Agreeableness, Conscientious. = Conscientiousness, Stab. = Stability, Open. = Openness.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to evaluate relationships between shelter dog and

human personality traits using the MCPQ-R and IPIP questionnaires. This study

compared dog personality with human personality, using both relinquisher-rated as well

as adopter-rated dog personality. It was shown in Chapter 3 of this dissertation that

different responders (i.e., relinquishers and adopters) did not agree on rating the same

dog’s personality.

Studying the relationships between personality traits in different people has

been used in several areas of psychology, including domestic relationships to predict

relationship satisfaction and outcome (Cavanaugh et al., 2008; Kidd and Kidd, 1992;

Page 121: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

102

White et al., 2004). It was proposed that the same reasoning could be applied to

investigate the relationships between dog and human personality traits. The current

study results show that there are no statistically significant dog-human personality

relationships, using both relinquisher-rated and adopter-rated dog personality. The

exact test was insignificant when relinquisher-rated dog personality was compared to

human personality for 262 dogs (Study A; p=0.68), when adopter-rated dog personality

was compared to human personality for 251 dogs (Study B; p=0.94), and also when the

data were analyzed from 197 dogs looking at both relinquisher-rated (p=0.06) and

adopter-rated (p=0.93) dog personality compared to human personality (Study C).

However, in Study C, the near-statistically significant association between relinquisher-

rated dog and human personality (p=0.06) should not be dismissed. An association may

be likely, but other factors may decrease the confidence in the observed effect (e.g., an

underpowered study).

One limitation encountered during the study was low enrollment of study dogs

at relinquishment, and an overall response rate of 77% at follow-up. Although the staff

were the second best choice to enroll stray or transferred dogs, it was ideal to be able to

recruit as many dogs as possible. Increasing sample size to obtain a more representative

view of the dog owning population is always a concern, but not always practical in a

shelter setting; the number of participants was often unpredictable.

One major limitation in this study was the scoring system to choose a primary

personality category for each dog and owner, because personality is multi- dimensional

(i.e., a dog’s or human’s personality is comprised of more than one trait, or category, so

Page 122: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

103

choosing a primary category to represent the individual’s personality as a whole is not

representative). It is not really accurate to measure personality according to this

project’s system; dogs and humans cannot be distilled to just one primary personality

trait because personality is all about the combinations, and how the traits interact with

each other. Future research could possibly look at rank ordering personality traits, based

on percent scores, and assessing associations between dog and human personality. This

method would help retain information that would otherwise be lost by choosing a

primary personality category.

Despite these limitations, this study is important because it discusses that

without any intervention, an adopter’s selection of dog for adoption was independent

from the dog’s personality. It is unknown how an intervention would alter the adopter’s

decision (Appendix A). Results of this study provide the foundations for further

investigations of the roles of dog personality, human personality, and dog-human

personality interactions with regard to relationship satisfaction, and animal retention,

and welfare, which will be discussed in Chapter 5 of this dissertation.

Page 123: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

104

References

Arkow, P., Dow, S. 1984. The ties that do not bind: A study of the human-animal bonds

that fail. In: Anderson, R.K., Hart, B.L., Hart, L.A. (Eds.), The pet connection: Its

influence on our health and quality of life (pp. 348-354). Minneapolis: University

of Minnesota.

Beck, A.M., Katcher, A.H., 1996. Between pets and people: the importance of animal

companionship. Purdue University Press, USA.

Bentler, P.M., Newcomb, M.D., 1978. Longitudinal study of martial success and failure.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 46, 1053-1070.

Bouchard, G., Areseneault, J.E., 2005. Length of union as a moderator of the relationship

between personality and dyadic adjustment. Personality and Individual

Differences. 39, 1407-1417.

Brehm, S.S., Miller, R.S., Perlman, D., Campbell, S.M., 2002. Intimate relationships.

McGraw Hill. New York. USA.

Cavanaugh, L.A., Leonard, H.A., Scammon, D.L., 2008. A trail of two personalities: How

canine companions shape relationships and well-being. Journal of Business

Research 61, 469-479.

Costa, P.T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., 1992a. Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PIR) and

NEO Five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional manual. Odessa, FL:

Psychological Assessment Resources.

Costa, P.T., Jr., McCrae, R.R., 1992b. Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and

Individual Differences 13, 653-665.

Page 124: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

105

Curb, L.A., Abramson, C.I., Grice, J.W., Kennison, S.M. 2013. The relationship between

personality match and pet satisfaction among dog owners. Anthrozoos 26, 395-

404.

Donnellan, M.B., Conger, R.D., Bryant, C.M., 2004. The big five and enduring marriages.

Journal of Research in Personality. 38, 481-504.

Goldberg, L.R., 1990. An alternative “description of personality”: The Big-Five Factor

structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 59, 1216-1229.

Goldberg, L.R., Johnson, J.A., Eber, H.W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M.C., Cloninger, R., Gough,

H.G., 2006. The international personality item pool and the future of public-

domain personality measures 40, 84-96.

Gosling, S.D., Kwan, V.S.Y., John, O.P. 2003. A dog’s got personality: a cross-species

comparative approach to personality judgments in dogs and humans. J Pers Soc

Psychol 85, 1161-1169.

John, O.P., Srivastava, S. 1999. The big five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and

theoretical perspectives. In: Pervin, L.A., John, O.P., (eds). Handbook of

Personality: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, p. 102-138.

Karney, B.R., Bradbury, T.N., 1995. The longitudinal course of marital quality and

stability: a review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin 118, 3-

34.

Page 125: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

106

Kass, P.H., New, Jr., J.C., Scarlett, J.M, Salman, M.D., 2001. Understanding animal

companion surplus in the United States: Relinquishment of nonadoptables to

animal shelters for euthanasia. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 4, 237-

248.

Kelly, E.L., Conley, J.J., 1987. Personality and compatibility: a prospective analysis of

marital stability and marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology 52, 27-40.

Kidd, A.H., Kidd, R.M., George, C.C., 1992. Successful and unsuccessful pet adoptions.

Psychological Reports 70, 547-561.

Kullar, L., 2013. Adoptions, adopters, and adoptees: The roles each plays in the dog-

human relationship in Britich Columbia, Canada. Master’s Thesis.

Ley, J., Bennett, P., Coleman, G., 2008. Personality dimensions that emerge in

companion canines. Applied Animal Behavior Science 110, 305-317.

Ley, J.M., Bennett, P.C., Coleman, G.J., 2009a. A refinement and validation of the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire (MCPQ). Applied Animal Behavior

Science 116, 220-227.

Ley, J.M., McGreevy, P., Bennett, P.C., 2009b. Inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R). Applied Animal

Behaviour Science 119, 85-90.

Marston, L.C., Bennett, P.C., Coleman, G.J., 2005. Adopting shelter dogs: Owner

experiences of the first month post-adoption. Anthrozoos 18, 358-378.

Page 126: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

107

Mondelli, F., Previde, E.P., Verga, M., Levi, D., Magistrelli, S., Valsecchi, P. 2004. The

bond that never developed: Adoption and relinquishment of dogs in a rescue

shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 7, 253-266.

Palmer, R., Custance, D., 2008. A counterbalanced version of Ainsworth’s Strange

Situation Procedure reveals secure-base effects in dog-human relationships.

Applied Animal Behavior Science 109, 306-319.

Patronek, G.J., Glickman, L.T., Beck, A.M., McCabe, G.P., Ecker, C., 1996. Risk factors for

relinquishment of dogs to an animal shelter. Journal of American Veterinary

Medical Association 209, 572-581.

Sable, P., 2013. The pet connection: an attachment perspective. Clinical Social Work

Journal 41, 93-99.

Salman, M.D., New, J.G., Scarlett, J.M., Kass, P.H., Ruch-Gallie, R., Hetts, S., 1998. Human

and animal factors related to the relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected

animal shelters in the United States. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science

1, 207-226.

Salman, M.D., Hutchison, J., Ruch-Gallie, R., Kogan, L., New, Jr., J.C., Kass, P.H., Scarlett,

J.M., 2000. Behavioral reasons for relinquishment of dogs and cats to 12

shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 3, 93-106.

Socha, A., Cooper, C.A., McCord, D.M., 2010. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the M5-50:

An Implementation of the International Personality Item Pool Item Set.

Psychological Assessment 22, 43-49.

Page 127: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

108

White, J.K., Hendrick, S.S., Hendrick, C., 2004. Big five personality variables and

relationship constructs. Personality and Individual Differences 37, 1519-1530.

Page 128: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

109

CHAPTER 6. ASSESSMENT OF DOG PERSONALITY, HUMAN PERSONALITY, AND THEIR INTERACTION TO PREDICT SUCCESSFUL ADOPTIONS OF SHELTER DOGS

Page 129: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

110

Introduction

Thousands of dogs are surrendered every year to animal shelters due to a variety

of reasons, yet, no research has investigated the influence of dog and human

personality on the human-animal bond with shelter dogs, or retention of the dog post-

adoption. The reason for surrenders and returns could be a mismatch between dog and

owner personality, with a lower satisfaction of the dog resulting in poor human-animal

bond (Curb et al., 2013; Neidhart and Boyd, 2002). In animal shelters, many

relinquishments are the result of broken attachments; yet, many healed and new

relationships form when these animals are adopted (Mondelli et al., 2004). It is

important to understand human-animal relationships in order to better grasp at how

animal shelters can encourage successful adoptions of shelter dogs.

Successful adoptions are likely based on several things: getting an animal out of

the shelter environment and into a home environment (Live Release Rate) (Weiss et al.,

2013), retention of the animal in the home environment, high satisfaction with the new

companion, and a strong human-animal bond post-adoption (Arkow and Dow, 1984;

Curb et al., 2013; Kidd et al., 1992; Mondelli et al., 2004). In this study, a successful

adoption is defined as the dog-owner pair having a strong human-animal bond, and the

dog was not returned to the shelter. The level of attachment to one’s pet has been

shown to be negatively correlated with relinquishment (e.g, the higher level of

attachment, the less likely for surrender) (Arkow and Dow, 1984; Marston and Bennett,

2003; Serpell, 1996). In the current study, human-animal bond, or satisfaction with the

dog, was measured with the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) (Anderson,

Page 130: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

111

2007; Johnson et al., 1992). The LAPS is a common and a reliable measure of

companion-animal attachment (Johnson et al., 1992), and was used in this study

because of its ease of administration to adopters.

Higher levels of attachment are characterized by caring for and wanting to spend

time with the pet (Poresky, 1989). The psychological principle, attachment theory,

proposes that people are biologically predisposed to form physical and emotional

attachments with those individuals around them (Bowlby, 1973). Pets generate positive

feelings of comfort, connection, and security, and also reduce stress (Sable, 2013). Just

as humans seek out animals to help make us feel better, companion animals seek out

human attention, and develop an attachment to people, as well (Beck and Madresh,

2008; Zilcha-Mano et al., 2011).

Attachment theory (Beck and Madresh, 2008; Bowlby, 1973; Poresky, 1989;

Sable, 2013; Zilcha-Mano et al., 2011) is applicable to relationships between both

people and people and dogs, and when Cavanaugh et al. (2008) took a cross-species

approach to evaluating dog and human personality effects on dog-owner satisfaction, it

was found that dog owners higher in Neuroticism did not report differences in

satisfaction when compared to dog owners higher in other personality categories; unlike

in the human personality psychology literature, where spousal partners high in

Neuroticism tended to report lower marital satisfaction (Donnellan et al., 2004; Karney

and Bradbury, 1997). The same study also found there was a positive relationship

between canine personality traits Openness and Agreeableness with satisfaction of the

dog-human relationship (Cavanaugh et al., 2008). Since Cavanaugh et al. (2008) used the

Page 131: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

112

canine-BFI (Gosling et al., 2003) to measure dog personality dimensions which may not

be reflective of true dog personality, the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-

Revised (MCPQ-R) (Ley et al., 2008, 2009a, b) was utilized in the current study. The

MCPQ-R was chosen as a measure of dog personality in this study, because it is a

reliable measure of dog personality with strong inter-rater reliability (Ley et al., 2009b).

The MCPQ-R was also chosen as a measure of dog personality in this study, because the

short 26-item questionnaire is easy to administer in a shelter setting, and takes the

responder roughly five minutes to complete. The aim of this study was to evaluate

adoption success based on dog personality, human personality, and dog-human

personality trait combinations.

Materials and Methods

Study Participants

Between January 24, 2013, and March 28, 2014, a convenience sample of 185

dogs from two Indiana shelters – Clinton County Humane Society (CCHS) and the

Humane Society of Indianapolis (HSI) was included in this study. This sample included

dogs that were enrolled either by surrender (n=31 at CCHS, n=94 at HSI), or with the

assistance of staff/volunteers (n=11 at CCHS, n=39 at HSI) and foster families (n=1 at

CCHS, n=8 at HSI) who completed the MCPQ-R to enroll additional dogs in the study.

This sample was a subset of dogs enrolled in this research project as outlined in

Chapter 3 of this dissertation. The dogs must have been at least a year old upon

surrender in order to be eligible for enrollment in the study. The relinquishers must

Page 132: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

113

have been at least 18 years old and have owned the dog for at least two weeks in order

to be eligible for the study at intake. There was no other eligibility criterion for new

adopters other than being at least 18 years old to participate in the study. Some

adopters were recruited using a randomization protocol that is explained in Appendix A

of this dissertation. For those responders that adopted more than one dog, one dog per

responder was randomly selected for data analysis.

Details about each shelter’s intake procedures and study eligibility criteria were

explained previously in Chapters 3, 4, and 5 of this dissertation. The same research staff

(SW at CCHS and KA at HSI) collected data for this project, and the same adoption and

follow-up procedures were followed per Chapters 3, 4, and 5 of this dissertation. Two

months after the adoption, SW called or e-mailed adopters to notify them of a packet

being sent to them in the mail, which included the MCPQ-R, LAPS, and a pre-

addressed/stamped envelope. For those adopters who we were unable to contact or did

not return the follow-up questionnaires to SW via standard mail or online survey, these

dogs were considered “lost to follow-up” and not included in data analysis.

Dog Personality, Human Personality, and Human-Animal Bond

The details regarding the MCPQ-R and the IPIP questionnaires, as well as their

implementation, are explained in Chapters 3, 4, and 5 of this dissertation. Briefly, the

MCPQ-R is a 26-item questionnaire (Appendices B and D) that measures five dog

personality dimensions (Extraversion, Motivation, Training Focus, Amicability, and

Neuroticism) using adjective ratings, and the IPIP is a 50-item questionnaire (Appendix

Page 133: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

114

C) that measures human personality on the Five-Factor Model (Extraversion,

Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Stability, and Openness) using phrase ratings. A

percent score (i.e., sum divided by the maximum and multiplied by 100) was computed

for each of the five traits for both dogs and humans. The trait with the largest percent

score would be selected to represent the primary personality category for that specific

dog or adopter. For tied primary personality categories for the adopter-rated MCPQ-R

and IPIP, one personality category was randomly selected for each questionnaire for

data analysis.

The LAPS questionnaire was used to measure adoption satisfaction, and was sent

in questionnaire format to adopters two months after adoption. Adopters also had the

opportunity to complete the survey online. The LAPS was scored as a Likert scale with 1

= Agree Strongly, 2 = Agree Somewhat, 3 = Disagree Somewhat, and 4 = Disagree

Strongly. The fifth answer option was “Don’t Know or Refuse” and was scored as a

missing value. Scores were then averaged, given two or less values missing, to produce

the adopter’s mean LAPS score. The same sample of dogs from Chapter 4 (Study C) was

used in this chapter. However, if three or more values on the LAPS were missing, then

the data were excluded from analysis (n=13). Mean scores were negatively correlated

with human-animal bond: a mean of 1.00 signified the strongest level of bond and

higher means signified lower levels of bond. In this study, a successful adoption was

defined as one where the dog was retained in the home (not returned to the shelter),

and where the primary caregiver had developed a strong bond with the animal. The

Page 134: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

115

terms attachment, satisfaction, and human-animal bond (Palmer and Custance, 2008)

are used interchangeably in the current study.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics were presented to describe the demographics of the study

dogs, as well as their lengths of stay. Dog personality, human personality, and their

interaction were measured using primary personality categories (please refer to

Chapters 4 and 5 of this dissertation). The mean LAPS scores were measured initially as

a continuous variable, and subsequently as a dichotomous categorical variable: satisfied

(a mean LAPS score of less than 2 = strong human-animal bond) or not satisfied (a mean

LAPS score of 2 or more = weak human-animal bond).

Linear regression was used to measure the relationship between dog

personality, human personality, and also their interaction with mean LAPS scores.

Logistic regression was used to measure the relationship between dog personality,

human personality, and also their interaction with satisfaction (satisfied (n=155) vs. not

satisfied (n=30)) and returns (returned (n=13) vs. not returned (n=172)), as well as the

relationship between satisfaction and returns; odd ratios (OR) were calculated and

confidence intervals (CI) were set at 95%. A Pearson’s chi-squared test was initially used

to look at the frequency distribution between the incidence of returns and the five

relinquisher-rated dog personality traits, as well as the five human personality traits.

However, because the expected value was less than five for multiple cells, the Fisher’s

Page 135: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

116

exact test was calculated between returns and dog and human personality for further

analysis to determine the significance of any associations.

Confounding variables included age, breed, and dog personality information

given when potential adopters walked through the door of each participating shelter

(please refer to Appendix A for more information). Although personality is relatively

stable, personality may fluctuate over an individual’s lifetime (young vs. geriatric), and

therefore, the age of the dog could influence the adoption choice, as well as satisfaction

with adopted dog. Breed could also influence a person’s adoption choice as well as

satisfaction. Dog breeds were categorized as purebred (n=18) vs. mixed (n=167); No

other breed categories were specified, in order to save as much power as possible. If the

adopters were given dog personality information before looking at adoptable dogs

(n=15), this certainly could influence adoption choice and satisfaction of the new dog.

The exposure variables of interest (dog personality, human personality, and their

interaction), and the confounding variables were initially put into the linear and logistic

regression models. Confounding variables were included in the final model if they were

statistically significant (p<0.05).

The sample of 185 dogs included only those that had a relinquisher-rated dog

personality category and a completed LAPS questionnaire. In Chapters 4 and 5 of this

dissertation, a comparison was made regarding how different responders rated dog

personality, and how that influences relationships between human personality traits. In

the current study, relinquisher-rated dog personality data, completed by the owners

surrendering their dogs, was used in data analysis. If the results of the current study will

Page 136: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

117

be used to possibly implement a matching program, then it makes practical sense to use

information collected at relinquishment. However, for research purposes, adopter-rated

dog personality was also analyzed in the regression models, to see if those results were

more predictive of satisfaction using the same sample of dogs. Microsoft Excel (2007)

and IBM® SPSS® Statistics for Windows (Version 21.0.0.0.) were used for data analyses.

Statistical significance was set at 0.05.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Out of 185 dogs, 167 (90%) were mixed breed. The median age was 26 months,

and ranged from 12 to 144 months. There were similar numbers of males (n=98) and

females (n=87) in this sample. The median length of stay at CCHS was 16 days, ranging

from 2 to 513 days (average = 75.6 days). The median length of stay at HSI was 39.5

days, ranging from 5 to 223 days (average = 50.4 days). Further details are explained in

Chapter 3 of this dissertation.

When looking at relinquisher-rated dog personality and human personality

combinations, Amicability(Dog)-Agreeableness(Human) (n=39), Extraversion(Dog)-

Agreeableness(Human) (n=33), Extraversion(Dog)-Conscientiousness(Human) (n=23),

and Training Focus(Dog)-Agreeableness(Human) (n=17) were the most frequent pairings

(Table 10).

Page 137: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

118

Table 10. Dog Personality Categories Rated by Relinquisher vs. Human Personality

Categories at Adoption.

Human Dog

Extraversion Agreeableness Conscientiousness Stability Openness

Extraversion 3 (4%) 33 (45%) 23 (31%) 3 (4% 12 (16%)

Motivation 0 (0%) 3 (30%) 3 (30%) 0 (0%) 4 (40%)

Training

Focus

0 (0%) 17 (55%) 5 (16%) 1 (3%) 8 (26%)

Amicability 7 (11%) 39 (60%) 10 (15%) 2 (3%) 7 (11%)

Neuroticism 0 (0%) 1 (20%) 2 (40%) 1 (20% 1 (20%)

Note: Frequency counts (% of dogs rated by the relinquisher that were adopted by individuals in each human personality category). For example, 11% of dogs, rated by the relinquisher to have the primary personality category of Amicability, were adopted by those individuals in the Extraversion category.

Assessment of Dog Personality and Human Personality as Predictors for the Success of

Adoptions

Relinquisher-Rated Dog Personality, Adopter-Rated Dog Personality, and Human

Personality: Associations with Mean LAPS Scores

Linear regression was used to statistically analyze relinquisher-rated dog

personality, adopter-rated dog personality, and human personality as separate

predictors of mean LAPS scores; the final model only included the main variables, as no

confounding variables were statistically significant. Neither relinquisher-rated dog

personality nor human personality was a statistically significant predictor for mean LAPS

Page 138: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

119

scores (p=0.27 and p=0.12, respectively). Adopter-rated dog personality was found to be

statistically significant predictor for mean LAPS scores (p=0.01), with the sample of 183

dogs (two dogs out of the original sample of 185 had missing data for adopter-rated dog

personality). Post-hoc tests were adjusted for multiple comparisons with Bonferroni,

and the difference in mean LAPS scores between Extraverted and Training Focused dogs

was 0.31 (p<0.01; 95% CI: 0.07, 0.55). Also, the difference in mean LAPS scores between

Extraverted and Amicable dogs was 0.21 (p=0.02; 95% CI: 0.03, 0.39). The difference in

mean LAPS scores between Training Focused and Neurotic dogs was -0.44 (p=0.03; 95%

CI: -0.85, -0.02).

Relinquisher-Rated Dog Personality, Adopter-Rated Dog Personality, and Human

Personality: Associations with Adoption Satisfaction and Returned Dogs

Logistic regression was used to analyze the predictability of adoption satisfaction

and returned dogs using relinquisher-rated dog personality, adopter-rated dog

personality, and human personality; the final model only included the main variables, as

no confounding variables were statistically significant. The results showed that

relinquisher-rated dog personality (Table 11) and human personality (Table 12) were not

statistically significant predictors for satisfaction (p=0.36 and p=0.14, respectively). The

Fisher’s exact test showed that no statistically significant relationships exist between

relinquisher-rated dog personality and the number of returned dogs (p=0.75) (Table 13).

6.8% of Extraverted dogs were returned, 10% of Motivated dogs were returned, 3.2% of

Page 139: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

120

Training Focused dogs were returned, 9.2% of Amicable dogs were returned, and 0% of

Neurotic dogs were returned. A larger sample size is needed to confirm these results.

The Fisher’s exact test showed that there was a statistically significant

relationship between human personality and the number of returned dogs (p<0.01)

(Table 14). 20% of Extraverted people returned their dogs, 6.5% of Agreeable people

returned their dogs, 4.7% of Conscientious people returned their dogs, 42.9% of Stable

people returned their dogs, and 0% of Open people returned their dogs.

Table 11. Relinquisher-Rated Dog Personality Prediction of Satisfaction.

Dog Personalitya P-Value Odds Ratio 95% CI Lower

Limit

95% CI Upper

Limit

Motivation 0.37 2.7 0.32 22.72

Training Focus 0.25 2.0 0.62 6.56

Amicability 0.06 2.5 0.95 6.41

Neuroticism 0.88 1.2 0.13 11.40

aNote: Extraversion was used as the reference group.

Page 140: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

121

Table 12. Human Personality Prediction of Satisfaction.

Human

Personalitya

P-Value Odds Ratio 95% CI Lower

Limit

95% CI Upper

Limit

Extraversion 0.52 2.1 0.22 19.68

Agreeableness 0.25 1.9 0.64 5.78

Conscientiousness 0.64 0.8 0.25 2.37

Stability 0.18 0.3 0.05 1.75

aNote: Openness was used as the reference group.

Table 13. Frequency Counts of Relinquisher-Rated Dog Personality and Returned Dogs.

Was the dog Returned?

Dog Personality

No Yes Total

Extraversion 69 5 74

Motivation 9 1 10

Training Focus 30 1 31

Amicability 59 6 65

Neuroticism 5 0 5

Total 172 13 185

Page 141: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

122

Table 14. Frequency Counts of Human Personality and Returned Dogs.

Was the dog Returned?

Human Personality

No Yes Total

Extraversion 8 2 10

Agreeableness 87 6 93

Conscientiousness 41 2 43

Stability 4 3 7

Openness 32 0 32

Total 172 13 185

Adopter-rated dog personality was not a statistically significant predictor of

satisfaction (p=0.12) within the sample of 183 dogs (Table 15). Interestingly, when the

sample size was expanded to those dogs that did not have Relinquisher-completed data

(Chapter 3 and Appendix A), adopter-rated dog personality was a statistically significant

predictor for satisfaction (n=234; p=0.04); the step-wise comparisons are shown in Table

16.

Page 142: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

123

Table 15. Adopter-Rated Dog Personality Prediction of Satisfaction (n=183).

Dog Personalitya P-Value Odds Ratio 95% CI Lower

Limit

95% CI Upper

Limit

Motivation 0.10 5.8 0.70 48.11

Training Focus 0.21 2.4 0.61 9.52

Amicability 0.27 1.7 0.67 4.26

Neuroticism 0.25 0.4 0.10 1.82

aNote: Extraversion was used as the reference group.

Table 16. Adopter-Rated Dog Personality Prediction of Satisfaction (n=234).

Dog

Personalitya

P-Value Odds Ratio 95% CI Lower

Limit

95% CI Upper

Limit

Motivation 0.07 4.2 0.90 19.52

Training Focus 0.05 3.1 0.98 10.04

Amicability 0.05 2.2 1.0 4.72

Neuroticism 0.42 0.43 0.15 2.3

aNote: Extraversion was used as the reference group.

Page 143: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

124

Assessment of the Interaction between Dog and Human Personality as a Predictor for

the Success of Adoptions

Relinquisher-Rated/Adopter-Rated Dog Personality and Human Personality Interactions:

Associations with Mean LAPS Scores

Linear regression was used to measure the relationship between the interaction

of relinquisher-rated dog personality and human personality, and mean LAPS scores; the

final model only included the main variables, as no confounding variables were

statistically significant. The interaction was not a statistically significant predictor of the

mean LAPS scores (p=0.35). The interaction between adopter-rated dog personality and

human personality was a moderately statistically significant predictor of the mean LAPS

scores (p=0.10).

Relinquisher-Rated/Adopter-Rated Dog Personality and Human Personality Interactions:

Associations with Satisfaction and Returned Dogs

Logistic regression was used to statistically analyze the interaction between

relinquisher-rated dog personality and human personality as a predictor for satisfaction

and returns; the final model only included the main variables, as no confounding

variables were statistically significant. A mean LAPS score of 2 or less represented an

adopter who was satisfied with the adoption (i.e., had a strong human-animal bond),

and a mean LAPS score of more than 2 represented an adopter who was not satisfied

with the adoption (i.e., had a weak human-animal bond). Despite there only being 13

Page 144: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

125

dogs returned in the sample of 185 dogs, returns was still an outcome variable of

interest.

The results showed that the interaction between relinquisher-rated dog

personality and human personality was not a statistically significant predictor for

satisfaction (p=0.61). The results also showed that the interaction was not a statistically

significant predictor for returned dogs (p=0.89). The interaction between adopter-rated

dog personality and human personality was not a statistically significant predictor for

satisfaction (n=183; p=0.76) nor returned dogs (p=0.66).

Assessment of Satisfaction as a Predictor for Returned Dogs

Logistic regression was used to analyze satisfaction as a predictor for returned

dogs; no statistical significance was found in the sample of 185 dogs (p=0.15).

Discussion

Because humans and dogs have attachment relationships (Beck and Madresh,

2008; Bowlby, 1973; Poresky, 1989; Sable, 2013; Zilcha-Mano et al., 2011) and dog

personality has been shown to be a significant predictor of dog-owner relationship

satisfaction (Cavanaugh et al., 2008), the current study assessed dog personality, human

personality, and also the interaction between dog and human personality for prediction

of success of adoption. Adoption success was measured as mean LAPS score,

satisfaction, and returned dogs.

Page 145: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

126

Assessment of Dog Personality and Human Personality as Predictors for the Success of

Adoptions

Relinquisher-rated dog personality and human personality were not statistically

significant predictors for mean LAPS score, satisfaction, or returned dogs; a larger

sample size is needed to confirm the results. However, adopter-rated dog personality

was found to be statistically significant predictor for mean LAPS scores (p=0.01). The

difference in mean LAPS scores between Extraverted and Training Focused dogs was

0.31 (p<0.01; 95% CI: 0.07, 0.55). Also, the difference in mean LAPS scores between

Extraverted and Amicable dogs was 0.21 (p=0.02; 95% CI: 0.03, 0.39). The difference in

mean LAPS scores between Training Focused and Neurotic dogs was -0.44 (p=0.03; 95%

CI: -0.85, -0.02).Although the confidence intervals are close to zero, the very small

confidence intervals reflect precise values.

It was also shown that there was a statistically significant relationship between

human personality and the number of returned dogs. 20% of Extraverted people

returned their dogs, and 0% of Open people returned their dogs. If shelters are going to

use number of returned dogs as a statistic for measuring adoption success, then these

percentages are valuable; animal shelter personnel may want to consider implementing

a human personality questionnaire into their adoption process to assess who is more at-

risk for returning their dogs.

Adopters who rated their dogs as Extraverted were slightly less satisfied with

their dogs than those who rated their dogs as Training Focused or Amicable, and

adopters who rated their dogs as Training Focused were slightly more satisfied with

Page 146: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

127

their dogs than those who rated their dogs as Neurotic. Adopter-rated dog personality

was also a statistically significant predictor for satisfaction (p=0.04), when the sample

included those dogs that did not necessarily have relinquisher-rated dog personality

data (Chapter 2 and Appendix A explain in further detail). Those adopters who rated

their dogs as Motivated, Training Focused, or Amicable were 4.2 times, 3.1 times, and

2.2 times, respectively, more likely to be satisfied with their new dog than those

adopters who rated their dogs as Extraverted. Those adopters who rated their dogs as

Neurotic were 0.43 times less likely to be satisfied with their new dog than those

adopters who rated their dogs as Extraverted.

This may be because those adopters who adopted dogs that they categorized as

Motivated, Training Focused, or Amicable may have found those traits more appealing,

and thus rated the related adjectives as more favorable, increasing satisfaction. On the

other hand, those adopters who adopted dogs that they categorized as Neurotic may

have found that trait less appealing, and thus despite rating the related adjectives high

enough to where Neuroticism was chosen as the primary category for that specific dog,

were less satisfied. The wide confidence intervals for Motivation and Training Focus

suggest that the sample size may be too small to detect a true effect.

Assessment of the Interaction between Dog and Human Personality as a Predictor for

the Success of Adoptions

The interactions between relinquisher-rated dog personality and human

personality, and adopter-rated dog personality and human personality, were not

Page 147: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

128

significant predictors for satisfaction or returned dogs. Due to a lack of statistically

power, it would not be useful to assess the 25 dog-human personality combinations as

separate predictors for adoption success.

This project was intended to assess the interaction between relinquisher-rated

dog personality and human personality with successful adoptions (i.e., if a matching

program will be implemented, only relinquisher-rated dog personality would be

available at the time of adoption). Therefore, adopter-rated dog personality is not

practically relevant in this context. However, because there were statistically significant

findings when adopter-rated dog personality was included in the analyses, future

studies may be able to use the current study design for assisting individuals post-

adoption. It is important to note that the interaction between relinquisher-rated dog

and human personality were not statistically significant predictors for the mean LAPS

scores, satisfaction, or for returned dogs.

One reason for these results could be that the MCPQ-R, IPIP, and LAPS

questionnaires are not assessing the factors important for matching dogs and adopters

together. One factor that influences one person’s attachment to their dog is how much

the dog is attached to that individual, but this and other factors are not necessarily

represented well in personality tests. Another reason for these results could be the

methodology of using the MCPQ-R and IPIP questionnaires to measure personality in

animal shelters. Although these questionnaires were chosen for this study carefully,

Page 148: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

129

there may be other measures available to assess influences of dog and human

personality on adoption success (e.g., ASPCA’s Meet Your Match Program).

There was a lack of homogeneity (i.e., equal sample size) between the five dog

personality categories. Small sample sizes were seen for dogs that were categorized as

Motivated or Neurotic. This may be due, in part, to more determined or nervous dogs,

respectively, not entering the shelter system as often as dogs that were categorized as

Extraverted or Amicable. However, it is unknown how the personality traits are

distributed in the general pet dog population, and there may not have been a

representative sample collected for this study. More likely the lack of homogeneity

between the five dog personality categories, may be due to Social Desirability Bias, as

the adjectives used to describe the personality category Neuroticism (Fearful, Nervous,

Submissive, and Timid) may not portray the dog in the best light, so responders may

rate their dogs lower on these scales. The personality categories with the lower percent

scores were not chosen for the primary personality category (i.e., the primary

personality categories were chosen in this study using the highest percentage scores).

A limitation of using the LAPS as a measure of adoption satisfaction was the bias

towards measuring a strong human-animal bond, and not a weak attachment. In other

words, adopters tended to rate each item reflective of higher attachment (i.e., rating

each item as a 1 or a 2 instead of a 3 or a 4) (Johnson et al., 1992). The range of the LAPS

mean score was between 1.0 and 2.96 (median = 1.65; mean = 1.68). Therefore, using

the LAPS as a measurement tool for weak human-animal bond may not be realistic.

Page 149: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

130

Personality was not found to be a positive predictor of adoption success,

because it is very possible that other dog-related variables need to be considered when

assessing the human-animal bond (e.g., physical appearance, size, presence or absence

of medical or behavioral problems, and integration into the household (Weiss et al.,

2012)). Also, other human-related variables need to be considered when assessing

human-animal bond, such as knowledge about general physical and behavioral health

(Salman et al., 1998, 2000), positive reinforcement and force-free training methods

(Duxbury et al., 2003), and realistic expectations (Marder and Duxbury, 2008). However,

the importance and impact of dog and human personality should not be dismissed.

Future research regarding the efficacy of the ASPCA’s Meet Your Match Program may be

an alternative to the current study design to assess adoption success with dog and

human personality factors.

It is suggested, that based on the outcome of the current study and with the

current knowledge, animal shelters can shift to other programs such as development of

a post-adoption program for support. The ultimate goal of adoptions is to make a good

match between dog and adopter, where the dog and adopter have a strong human-

animal bond, and the dog is retained in the home environment. This study provided the

foundational contribution to objective evidence to build a successful adoption program

in the animal shelter field.

Based on the outcome of this study, and with the current knowledge, it is

suggested that animal shelters should focus their limited time and resources on

education, adoption counseling, and matching on factors other than personality (Kidd et

Page 150: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

131

al., 1992). With more thorough screenings and interviews at adoption counseling,

families may be able to adopt a dog that would be matched to fit their expectations and

lifestyle, and therefore help prevent surrendering their dogs for reasons such as

unrealistic expectations or mismatches (Arkow and Dow, 1984; Curb et al., 2013; Kidd et

al., 1992; Marston et al., 2005; Mondelli et al., 2004; Salman et al., 1998).

Page 151: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

132

References

Anderson, D.S. 2007. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale, LAPS (Johnson et al. 1992). In:

Assessing the Human-Animal Bond: A Compendium of Actual Measures. Purdue

University Press. USA.

Arkow, P., Dow, S., 1984. The ties that do not bind: A study of the human-animal bonds

that fail. In: Anderson, R.K., Hart, B.L., Hart, L.A. (eds.), The pet connection: Its

influence on our health and quality of life (pp. 348-354). Minneapolis: University

of Minnesota.

Beck, L., Madresh, E.A., 2008. Romantic partners and four-legged friends: An extension

of attachment theory to relationships with pets. Anthrozoos 21, 43-56.

Bowlby, J., 1973. Attachment and Loss. New York, NY: Basic Books.Cavanaugh, L.A.,

Leonard, H.A., Scammon, D.L., 2008. A trail of two personalities: How canine

companions shape relationships and well-being. Journal of Business Research.

61, 469-479.

Cavanaugh, L.A., Leonard, H.A., Scammon, D.L., 2008. A trail of two personalities: How

canine companions shape relationships and well-being. Journal of Business

Research. 61, 469-479.

Curb, L.A., Abramson, C.I., Grice, J.W., Kennison, S.M., 2013. The relationship between

personality match and pet satisfaction among dog owners. Anthrozoos 26, 395-

404.

Page 152: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

133

Donnellan, M.B., Conger, R.D., Bryant, C.M., 2004. The big five and enduring marriages.

Journal of Research in Personality. 38, 481-504.

Duxbury, M.M., Jackson, J.A., Line, S.W., Anderson, R.K. 2008. Evaluation of association

between retention in the home and attendance at puppy socialization classes.

Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 38, 1145-1162.

Gosling, S.D., Kwan, V.S.Y., John, O.P., 2003. A dog’s got personality: A cross-species

comparative approach to personality judgments in dogs and humans. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology 85, 1161-1169.

Johnson, T.P., Garrity, T.F., Stallones, L. 1992. Psychometric Evaluation of the Lexington

Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS). Anthrozoos 5, 160-175.

Karney, B.R., Bradbury, T.N., 1997. Neuroticism, marital interaction, and the trajectory

of marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72, 1075-

1092.

Kidd, A.H., Kidd, R.M., George, C.C. 1992. Successful and unsuccessful pet adoptions.

Psychological Reports 70, 547-561.

Ley, J., Bennett, P., Coleman, G., 2008. Personality dimensions that emerge in

companion canines. Applied Animal Behavior Science 110, 305-317.

Ley, J.M., Bennett, P.C., Coleman, G.J., 2009a. A refinement and validation of the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire (MCPQ). Applied Animal Behaviour

Science 116, 220-227.

Page 153: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

134

Ley, J.M., McGreevy, P., Bennett, P.C., 2009b. Inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the

Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R). Applied Animal

Behaviour Science 119, 85-90.

Live Release Rate. ASPCAPro. http://www.aspcapro.org/live-release-rate-and-animals-

risk. Accessed June 30, 2014.

Marder, A., Duxbury, M.M., 2008. Obtaining a pet: Realistic expectations. Veterinary

Clinics Small Animal Practice 38, 1145-1162.

Marston, L.C., Bennett, P.C., 2003. Reforging the bond—towards successful canine

adoption. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 83, 227-245.

Mondelli, F., Previde, E.P., Verga, M., Levi, D., Magistrelli, S., Valsecchi, P., 2004. The

bond that never developed: Adoption and relinquishment of dogs in a rescue

shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 7, 253-266.

Neidhart, L. Boyd, R., 2002. Companion animal adoption study. Journal of Applied

Animal Welfare Science 5, 175-192.

Palmer, R., Custance, D. 2008. A counterbalanced version of Ainsworth’s Strange

Situation Procedure reveals secure-base effects in dog-human relationships.

Applied Animal Behavior Science 109, 306-319.

Poresky, R.H., 1989. Analyzing human-animal relationship measures. Anthrozoos 2, 236-

244.

Sable, P. 2013. The pet connection: an attachment perspective. Clinical Social Work

Journal 41, 93-99.

Page 154: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

135

Salman, M.D., New, J.G., Scarlett, J.M., Kass, P.H., Ruch-Gallie, R., Hetts, S., 1998. Human

and animal factors related to the relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected

animal shelters in the United States. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science

1, 207-226.

Salman, M.D., Hutchison, J., Ruch-Gallie, R., Kogan, L., New, Jr., J.C., Kass, P.H., Scarlett,

J.M. 2000. Behavioral reasons for relinquishment of dogs and cats to 12 shelters.

Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 3, 93-106.

Serpell, J.A., 1996. Evidence for an association between pet behavior and owner

attachment levels. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 47, 49-60.

Weiss, E., Miller, K., Mohan-Gibbons, H., Vela, C., 2012. Why did you choose this pet?:

Adopters and pet selection preferences in five animal shelters in the United

States. Animals 2, 144-159.

Weiss, E., Patronek, G. Slater, M. Garrison, L., Medicus, K., 2013. Community partnering

as a tool for improving live release rate in animal shelters in the United States.

Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 16, 221-238.

Zilcha-Mano, S., Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P.R., 2011. An attachment perspective on

human-pet relationships: Conceptualization and assessment of pet attachment

orientations. Journal of Research in Personality 45, 345-357.

Page 155: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

136

CHAPTER 7. CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

Page 156: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

137

Conclusion

Some animal shelters make a rigorous effort to ensure compatibility between

adopter and shelter dog with adoption counseling, education, and administering an

application that reviews different aspects of the adopter’s lifestyle. Research on

compatibility between dog and human personalities is lacking, and matching dogs to

prospective adopters, based on both dog and human personality, is one method to

potentially increase the likelihood of a successful lifelong relationship. This is the first

study which attempted to assess the effects of dog personality, human personality, and

their interaction on the adoption success of shelter dogs. The goal of this research

project was to provide data to determine how influential dog and human personality

were on adoption success. Agreement on rating dog personality was assessed (Chapter

4), and relationships between dog and human personality was assessed (Chapter 5).

Because relationships between dog and human personality may or may not reflect the

human-animal bond, dog personality, human personality, and their interaction were

assessed for influences on successful adoptions (Chapter 6). It was assumed in this

study, that a “good” match between dog and adopter is made, when individuals adopt a

dog that integrates well into their household, and therefore the human-animal bond

strengthens.

An interesting finding was the lack of agreement of dog personality ratings

between responders (Chapter 4) and no relationships found between dog and human

personality (Chapter 5). However, it was shown that there was a statistically significant

relationship between human personality and the number of returned dogs (Chapter 6);

Page 157: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

138

animal shelter personnel may want to consider implementing a human personality

questionnaire into their adoption process to assess who is more at-risk for returning

their dogs.

Although the MCPQ-R and IPIP questionnaires were chosen for this study

carefully, there may be other measures available to assess influences of dog and human

personality on adoption success (e.g., ASPCA’s Meet Your Match Program). Therefore,

with limited resources and time, it would be more beneficial and effective for animal

shelters to shift their focus and efforts to adoption counseling, client education, and

matching based on other factors (e.g., lifestyle, activity level, and age of the dog), which

are more likely to have a long-lasting impact. The findings from this study support a new

direction for the shelter community that desperately needs a cost, time, and resource

effective successful adoption program.

Future Directions

Adoption Counseling and Dog-Adopter Matching Programs

There is a dire need for enhanced pet-owner education to combat unrealistic

expectations and misconceptions concerning dog ownership. Proper counseling is

necessary for clients to understand how to be a responsible pet owner, and discussion

points should include basic animal health, supervision, normal dog behavior, and the

importance of a lifetime commitment. Adoption counselors also have an opportunity to

educate adopters about realistic expectations, the roles of maintaining a healthy and

happy dog, and developing a strong relationship with their new companion.

Page 158: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

139

Decisions about placing dogs into adoptive homes should be made based on

dog-related factors such as: size and needs for basic care, activity level, and behavior

evaluation results. Placement should also emphasize human-related factors such as: age

and activity levels of household members, previous dog experience, as well as the

potential adopter’s lifestyle and expectations of the dog. If expectations are unrealistic,

then the adoption has potential to fail and the dog returned to the shelter. However,

the efficacy of these counseling and interview methods warrants future research.

Post-Adoption Resources

Dog behavior problems are highly associated with relinquishment and decreased

human-animal bond. Because dogs are often returned within the first two months of

adoption, there is a very narrow window of opportunity to provide new adopters with

the necessary help and guidance they need to be satisfied with their new pet, and retain

the animal in the home. With such a short timeframe, it is important to address issues

with adopters as soon as possible, to provide practical assistance with dog training and

behavior problems. Education about evidence-based methods of positive

reinforcement, force-free training, and behavior modification techniques can help

strengthen the human-animal bond; the bond between the dog and its new adoptive

family is one that should ultimately last a lifetime.

Page 159: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

APPENDICES

Page 160: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

140

Appendix A. Randomized Controlled Pilot Study

Page 161: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

141

Controlled, Randomized Pilot Study

A randomization protocol was put into place between August 19th, 2013, and

January 30th, 2014, at CCHS and between June 19th, 2013, and March 28th, 2014, at HSI.

As randomization was not used to answer the three main research questions in this

dissertation, further details of this study are explained in this Appendix. Because of very

high sample size calculations that were unrealistic and unobtainable given the shelters

that were data collection sites (n=1,308 dogs at relinquishment and n=1,334 dogs at

adoption), I only gathered a sample size of 100 (n=18 at CCHS and n=82 at HSI). Briefly,

here is a synopsis of the study.

Our aim was to evaluate the application of the MCPQ-R with shelter dogs and

whether it altered the success of adoption. The specific uses of the MCPQ-R, the IPIP,

and the LAPS questionnaires have been previously described in Chapters 3 through 6 of

this dissertation. I applied these three questionnaires during this study to investigate

whether providing information about a dog’s personality to potential adopters before

adoption changed the success of adoptions.

Upon the completion of the MCPQ-R by the owner surrendering the dog, dogs

were categorized based on personality results and a colored sticker was designated for

each dog. Each colored sticker signified a different personality category based on the

MCPQ-R survey. The sticker was placed on each dog’s kennel at CCHS and HSI.

Participants were randomly assigned to one of two study groups. To do this, a

stack of identical envelopes were prepared for each site. The contents of the envelopes

were based on the treatment assignment, which was generated from a random number

Page 162: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

142

generator. A sticker with an identification (ID) number was placed on the envelope.

Research personnel opened the envelope and gave each study participant the contents

of the randomized envelope. Ninety-seven percent of patrons approached at CCHS and

86% of patrons approached at HSI agreed to participate. There was a lower response

rate of 47% for adopters from CCHS, and 75% for adopters from HSI, sending the follow-

up questionnaires back to SW in the mail or completing the forms online.

Wellness information (Appendix F) was provided to both groups to ensure that

both groups received an envelope, and that information provided to the control group

would not influence satisfaction of the adopted dog. The treatment group envelopes

also contained a laminated half-sheet (Appendix G) explaining the different personality

categories (Extraversion, Motivation, Training Focus, Amicability, and Neuroticism).

To encourage participation of potential adopters, SW was on-site at CCHS to

recruit participants before they looked at dogs available for adoption. If patrons were

interested, SW handed them an Information Sheet to keep, and also the contents of the

envelopes. KA was on-site at HSI to recruit participants in the same fashion.

The control group adopted dogs per the already established procedures of each

humane society. Research personnel explained to the potential adopters in the

treatment group that the laminated sheet described the five different personality

categories. They further explained that each dog will have components of each

category, but the category that is most prominent will be represented by a colored

sticker on the cage cards on each kennel. Potential adopters in the treatment group

carried the laminated sheet with them as they walked around the adoption floor. The

Page 163: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

143

laminated sheet was then returned to the research personnel before patrons left the

building.

Adopters in both groups were free to adopt the animal of their choice and that

dog’s colored sticker, if applicable, was documented. As previously explained, once

adoptions were finalized, research personnel approached the adopters to complete the

IPIP questionnaire, and followed-up two months later with an MCPQ-R and LAPS

questionnaires.

This was a randomized controlled study, with the estimated sample sizes of 1,308

for relinquishers and 1,334 for adopters. These sample sizes were calculated based on

previous publications, personal experience and education from local shelters. The

following parameters were used to calculate sample size needed for this pilot study:

- 60% of the relinquished dogs pass the behavior assessment and medical examination,

- 85% of these get adopted, - 50% of the potential adopters successfully adopt a dog at their visits, - 20% return rate in the control group, - 10% return rate in the treatment group, - 60% follow-up rate (2-month post-adoption), - 0.8 power.

Based on our estimates, 667 dogs of the initial 1,308 relinquished dogs would

need to be successfully adopted. This number of dogs accounted for potential Loss-to-

Follow-up during the two-month follow-up period. I also would have needed to recruit

1,334 potential adopters (n=667 for each study group) for the study, assuming that 50%

of them would adopt a dog at their visits. This was not the case and only 12% of

adopters (18/151) visiting CCHS, who received an envelope, and 11% of patrons

Page 164: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

144

(82/743) visiting HSI, who received an envelope, finalized an adoption. I assumed that

both treatment groups were equally likely to adopt a dog during their visits.

I planned to analyze satisfaction by comparing the mean scores of the LAPS

between control group and treatment group using the two sample t-test. However,

because of a very low sample size of n=100, I were unable to statistically analyze the

data. Completed data points included having a completed MCPQ-R questionnaire by the

owner surrendering the dog at intake, a completed IPIP questionnaire by the new

adopter at adoption finalization, and completed MCPQ-R and LAPS questionnaires by

the new adopter at the two-month follow-up period. Of these 100 dogs, only three dogs

at CCHS and 19 dogs at HSI had all four questionnaires completed. A majority of dogs

were considered strays or transfers from other organizations and I did not have

relinquisher-rated dog personality data for them.

Future questions I could have answered with a complete data set include:

- Is the treatment group more likely to adopt an enrolled study dog than the

control group?

- Are adopters more satisfied with enrolled dogs than stray dogs?

Page 165: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

145

Appendix B. Monash Canine Personality Questionnaires-Revised (CCHS and HSI – Relinquishment)

Page 166: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

146

Canine Personality Study

Please rate your dog’s personality using the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised. Please answer this survey as honestly as possible. Answers will be kept strictly confidential. Please return completed questionnaire to the “Personality Research Drop Box.”

Please rate how well each word describes your dog’s personality by marking the appropriate box.

1 = really does not describe my dog, 6 = really describes my dog

Really does not describe my dog

Really describes my dog

Friendly 1 2 3 4 5 6

Persevering 1 2 3 4 5 6

Nervous 1 2 3 4 5 6

Energetic 1 2 3 4 5 6

Attentive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Easy Going 1 2 3 4 5 6

Independent 1 2 3 4 5 6

Trainable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Non-Aggressive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Eager 1 2 3 4 5 6

Submissive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Determined 1 2 3 4 5 6

Relaxed 1 2 3 4 5 6

Tenacious 1 2 3 4 5 6

Timid 1 2 3 4 5 6

Biddable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Active 1 2 3 4 5 6

Page 167: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

147

Really does not describe my dog

Really describes my dog

Intelligent 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sociable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Restless 1 2 3 4 5 6

Fearful 1 2 3 4 5 6

Obedient 1 2 3 4 5 6

Lively 1 2 3 4 5 6

Reliable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Assertive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Excitable 1 2 3 4 5 6

On behalf of Clinton County Humane Society and Purdue University, thank-you for your time.

Page 168: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

148

Canine Personality Study

Please rate your dog’s personality using the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised. Please answer this survey as honestly as possible. Answers will be kept strictly confidential. Please return completed questionnaire to the “Personality Research Drop Box.”

Please rate how well each word describes your dog’s personality by marking the appropriate box.

1 = really does not describe my dog, 6 = really describes my dog

Really does not describe my dog

Really describes my dog

Friendly 1 2 3 4 5 6

Persevering 1 2 3 4 5 6

Nervous 1 2 3 4 5 6

Energetic 1 2 3 4 5 6

Attentive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Easy Going 1 2 3 4 5 6

Independent 1 2 3 4 5 6

Trainable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Non-Aggressive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Eager 1 2 3 4 5 6

Submissive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Determined 1 2 3 4 5 6

Relaxed 1 2 3 4 5 6

Tenacious 1 2 3 4 5 6

Timid 1 2 3 4 5 6

Biddable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Active 1 2 3 4 5 6

Page 169: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

149

Really does not describe my dog

Really describes my dog

Intelligent 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sociable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Restless 1 2 3 4 5 6

Fearful 1 2 3 4 5 6

Obedient 1 2 3 4 5 6

Lively 1 2 3 4 5 6

Reliable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Assertive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Excitable 1 2 3 4 5 6

On behalf of Humane Society of Indianapolis and Purdue University, thank-you for your time.

Page 170: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

150

Appendix C. International Personality Item Pool (CCHS and HSI)

Page 171: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

151

International Personality Item Pool Developed by Goldberg (1999)

Please answer this survey as honestly as possible. Any questions you may object to can be left blank. Answers will be kept strictly confidential. Please return completed questionnaire to the “Personality Research Drop Box.” Please describe yourself as you generally are now, not as you wish to be in the future. Describe yourself as you honestly see yourself, in relation to other people you know of the same sex as you are, and roughly your same age. Below are phrases describing people’s behaviors. Please read each statement carefully, and then indicate how accurately each statement describes you by marking the appropriate number on the scale beside each question. Use the following format: 1 2 3 4 5 Very Moderately Neither inaccurate Moderately Very Inaccurate Inaccurate nor Accurate Accurate Accurate

1. … I am the life of the party. 1 2 3 4 5

2. … I feel little concern for others. 1 2 3 4 5

3. … I am always prepared. 1 2 3 4 5

4. … I get stressed out easily. 1 2 3 4 5

5. … I have a rich vocabulary. 1 2 3 4 5

6. … I don’t talk a lot. 1 2 3 4 5

7. … I am interested in people. 1 2 3 4 5

8. … I leave my belongings around. 1 2 3 4 5

9. … I am relaxed most of the time. 1 2 3 4 5

10. … I have difficulty understanding abstract ideas. 1 2 3 4 5

11. … I feel comfortable around people. 1 2 3 4 5

12. … I insult people. 1 2 3 4 5

13. … I pay attention to details. 1 2 3 4 5

14. … I worry about things. 1 2 3 4 5

15. … I have a vivid imagination. 1 2 3 4 5

16. … I keep in the background. 1 2 3 4 5

17. … I sympathize with others’ feelings. 1 2 3 4 5

18. … I make a mess of things. 1 2 3 4 5

19. … I seldom feel blue. 1 2 3 4 5

20. … I am not interested in abstract ideas. 1 2 3 4 5

Page 172: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

152

21. … I start conversations. 1 2 3 4 5

22. … I am not interested in other people’s problems. 1 2 3 4 5

23. … I get chores done right away. 1 2 3 4 5

24. … I am easily disturbed. 1 2 3 4 5

25. … I have excellent ideas. 1 2 3 4 5

26. … I have little to say. 1 2 3 4 5

27. … I have a soft heart. 1 2 3 4 5

28. … I often forget to put things back in their proper place. 1 2 3 4 5

29. … I get upset easily. 1 2 3 4 5

30. … I do not have a good imagination. 1 2 3 4 5

31. … I talk to a lot of different people at parties. 1 2 3 4 5

32. … I am not really interested in others. 1 2 3 4 5

33. … I like order. 1 2 3 4 5

34. … I change my mood a lot. 1 2 3 4 5

35. … I am quick to understand things. 1 2 3 4 5

36. … I don’t like to draw attention to myself. 1 2 3 4 5

37. … I take time out for others. 1 2 3 4 5

38. … I shirk my duties. 1 2 3 4 5

39. … I have frequent mood swings. 1 2 3 4 5

40. … I use difficult words. 1 2 3 4 5

41. … I don’t mind being the center of attention. 1 2 3 4 5

42. … I feel others’ emotions. 1 2 3 4 5

43. … I follow a schedule. 1 2 3 4 5

44. … I get irritated easily. 1 2 3 4 5

45. … I spend time reflecting on things. 1 2 3 4 5

46. … I am quiet around strangers. 1 2 3 4 5

47. … I make people feel at ease. 1 2 3 4 5

48. … I am exacting in my work. 1 2 3 4 5

49. … I often feel blue. 1 2 3 4 5

50. … I am full of ideas. 1 2 3 4 5

On behalf of Clinton County Humane Society and Purdue University, thank-you for your time.

Page 173: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

153

International Personality Item Pool Developed by Goldberg (1999)

Please answer this survey as honestly as possible. Any questions you may object to can be left blank. Answers will be kept strictly confidential. Please return completed questionnaire to the “Personality Research Drop Box.” Please describe yourself as you generally are now, not as you wish to be in the future. Describe yourself as you honestly see yourself, in relation to other people you know of the same sex as you are, and roughly your same age. Below are phrases describing people’s behaviors. Please read each statement carefully, and then indicate how accurately each statement describes you by marking the appropriate number on the scale beside each question. Use the following format: 1 2 3 4 5 Very Moderately Neither inaccurate Moderately Very Inaccurate Inaccurate nor Accurate Accurate Accurate

1. … I am the life of the party. 1 2 3 4 5

2. … I feel little concern for others. 1 2 3 4 5

3. … I am always prepared. 1 2 3 4 5

4. … I get stressed out easily. 1 2 3 4 5

5. … I have a rich vocabulary. 1 2 3 4 5

6. … I don’t talk a lot. 1 2 3 4 5

7. … I am interested in people. 1 2 3 4 5

8. … I leave my belongings around. 1 2 3 4 5

9. … I am relaxed most of the time. 1 2 3 4 5

10. … I have difficulty understanding abstract ideas. 1 2 3 4 5

11. … I feel comfortable around people. 1 2 3 4 5

12. … I insult people. 1 2 3 4 5

13. … I pay attention to details. 1 2 3 4 5

14. … I worry about things. 1 2 3 4 5

15. … I have a vivid imagination. 1 2 3 4 5

16. … I keep in the background. 1 2 3 4 5

17. … I sympathize with others’ feelings. 1 2 3 4 5

18. … I make a mess of things. 1 2 3 4 5

19. … I seldom feel blue. 1 2 3 4 5

20. … I am not interested in abstract ideas. 1 2 3 4 5

Page 174: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

154

21. … I start conversations. 1 2 3 4 5

22. … I am not interested in other people’s problems. 1 2 3 4 5

23. … I get chores done right away. 1 2 3 4 5

24. … I am easily disturbed. 1 2 3 4 5

25. … I have excellent ideas. 1 2 3 4 5

26. … I have little to say. 1 2 3 4 5

27. … I have a soft heart. 1 2 3 4 5

28. … I often forget to put things back in their proper place. 1 2 3 4 5

29. … I get upset easily. 1 2 3 4 5

30. … I do not have a good imagination. 1 2 3 4 5

31. … I talk to a lot of different people at parties. 1 2 3 4 5

32. … I am not really interested in others. 1 2 3 4 5

33. … I like order. 1 2 3 4 5

34. … I change my mood a lot. 1 2 3 4 5

35. … I am quick to understand things. 1 2 3 4 5

36. … I don’t like to draw attention to myself. 1 2 3 4 5

37. … I take time out for others. 1 2 3 4 5

38. … I shirk my duties. 1 2 3 4 5

39. … I have frequent mood swings. 1 2 3 4 5

40. … I use difficult words. 1 2 3 4 5

41. … I don’t mind being the center of attention. 1 2 3 4 5

42. … I feel others’ emotions. 1 2 3 4 5

43. … I follow a schedule. 1 2 3 4 5

44. … I get irritated easily. 1 2 3 4 5

45. … I spend time reflecting on things. 1 2 3 4 5

46. … I am quiet around strangers. 1 2 3 4 5

47. … I make people feel at ease. 1 2 3 4 5

48. … I am exacting in my work. 1 2 3 4 5

49. … I often feel blue. 1 2 3 4 5

50. … I am full of ideas. 1 2 3 4 5

On behalf of Humane Society of Indianapolis and Purdue University, thank-you for your time.

Page 175: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

155

Appendix D. Monash Canine Personality Questionnaires-Revised (CCHS and HSI – Adoption)

Page 176: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

156

Canine Personality Study

Please rate your dog’s personality using the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised. Please answer this survey as honestly as possible. Answers will be kept strictly confidential. Please return completed questionnaire to Sheryl Walker within one week (directions given on Greeting Letter).

Please rate how well each word describes your dog’s personality by marking the appropriate box.

1 = really does not describe my dog, 6 = really describes my dog

Really does not describe my dog

Really describes my dog

Friendly 1 2 3 4 5 6

Persevering 1 2 3 4 5 6

Nervous 1 2 3 4 5 6

Energetic 1 2 3 4 5 6

Attentive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Easy Going 1 2 3 4 5 6

Independent 1 2 3 4 5 6

Trainable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Non-Aggressive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Eager 1 2 3 4 5 6

Submissive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Determined 1 2 3 4 5 6

Relaxed 1 2 3 4 5 6

Tenacious 1 2 3 4 5 6

Timid 1 2 3 4 5 6

Biddable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Active 1 2 3 4 5 6

Page 177: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

157

Really does not describe my dog

Really describes my dog

Intelligent 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sociable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Restless 1 2 3 4 5 6

Fearful 1 2 3 4 5 6

Obedient 1 2 3 4 5 6

Lively 1 2 3 4 5 6

Reliable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Assertive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Excitable 1 2 3 4 5 6

On behalf of Clinton County Humane Society and Purdue University, thank-you for your time.

Page 178: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

158

Canine Personality Study

Please rate your dog’s personality using the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised. Please answer this survey as honestly as possible. Answers will be kept strictly confidential. Please return completed questionnaire to Sheryl Walker within one week (directions given on Greeting Letter).

Please rate how well each word describes your dog’s personality by marking the appropriate box.

1 = really does not describe my dog, 6 = really describes my dog

Really does not describe my dog

Really describes my dog

Friendly 1 2 3 4 5 6

Persevering 1 2 3 4 5 6

Nervous 1 2 3 4 5 6

Energetic 1 2 3 4 5 6

Attentive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Easy Going 1 2 3 4 5 6

Independent 1 2 3 4 5 6

Trainable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Non-Aggressive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Eager 1 2 3 4 5 6

Submissive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Determined 1 2 3 4 5 6

Relaxed 1 2 3 4 5 6

Tenacious 1 2 3 4 5 6

Timid 1 2 3 4 5 6

Biddable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Active 1 2 3 4 5 6

Page 179: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

159

Really does not describe my dog

Really describes my dog

Intelligent 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sociable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Restless 1 2 3 4 5 6

Fearful 1 2 3 4 5 6

Obedient 1 2 3 4 5 6

Lively 1 2 3 4 5 6

Reliable 1 2 3 4 5 6

Assertive 1 2 3 4 5 6

Excitable 1 2 3 4 5 6

On behalf of Humane Society of Indianapolis and Purdue University, thank-you for your time.

Page 180: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

160

Appendix E. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (CCHS and HSI)

Page 181: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

161

Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale

Please answer this survey as honestly as possible. Answers will be kept strictly confidential. Please return completed questionnaire to Sheryl Walker within one week (directions given on Greeting Letter).

Please tell us whether you agree or disagree with some very brief statements about your newly adopted pet. For each statement, check whether you strongly agree, somewhat agree, somewhat disagree, or strongly disagree. You may refuse to answer.

Agree Strongly

Agree Somewhat

Disagree Somewhat

Disagree Strongly

Don’t Know Or Refuse

A. My pet means more to me than any of my friends.

B. Quite often I confide in my pet.

C. I believe that pets should have the same rights and privileges as family members.

D. I believe my pet is my best friend.

E. Quite often, my feelings toward people are affected by the way they react to my pet.

F. I love my pet because he/she is more loyal to me than most of the people in my life.

G. I enjoy showing other people pictures of my pet.

H. I think my pet is just a pet.

I. I love my pet because it never judges me.

J. My pet knows when I’m feeling bad.

K. I often talk to other people about my pet.

L. My pet understands me.

M. I believe that loving my pet helps me stay healthy.

N. Pets deserve as much respect as humans do.

Page 182: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

162

Agree Strongly

Agree Somewhat

Disagree Somewhat

Disagree Strongly

Don’t Know Or Refuse

O. My pet and I have a very close relationship.

P. I would do almost anything to take care of my pet.

Q. I play with my pet quite often.

R. I consider my pet to be a great companion.

S. My pet makes me feel happy.

T. I feel that my pet is a part of my family.

U. I am not very attached to my pet.

V. Owning a pet adds to my happiness.

W. I consider my pet to be a friend.

On behalf of Clinton County Humane Society and Purdue University, thank-you for your time.

Page 183: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

163

Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale

Please answer this survey as honestly as possible. Answers will be kept strictly confidential. Please return completed questionnaire to Sheryl Walker within one week (directions given on Greeting Letter).

Please tell us whether you agree or disagree with some very brief statements about your newly adopted pet. For each statement, check whether you strongly agree, somewhat agree, somewhat disagree, or strongly disagree. You may refuse to answer.

Agree Strongly

Agree Somewhat

Disagree Somewhat

Disagree Strongly

Don’t Know Or Refuse

A. My pet means more to me than any of my friends.

B. Quite often I confide in my pet.

C. I believe that pets should have the same rights and privileges as family members.

D. I believe my pet is my best friend.

E. Quite often, my feelings toward people are affected by the way they react to my pet.

F. I love my pet because he/she is more loyal to me than most of the people in my life.

G. I enjoy showing other people pictures of my pet.

H. I think my pet is just a pet.

I. I love my pet because it never judges me.

J. My pet knows when I’m feeling bad.

K. I often talk to other people about my pet.

L. My pet understands me.

M. I believe that loving my pet helps me stay healthy.

N. Pets deserve as much respect as humans do.

Page 184: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

164

Agree Strongly

Agree Somewhat

Disagree Somewhat

Disagree Strongly

Don’t Know Or Refuse

O. My pet and I have a very close relationship.

P. I would do almost anything to take care of my pet.

Q. I play with my pet quite often.

R. I consider my pet to be a great companion.

S. My pet makes me feel happy.

T. I feel that my pet is a part of my family.

U. I am not very attached to my pet.

V. Owning a pet adds to my happiness.

W. I consider my pet to be a friend.

On behalf of Humane Society of Indianapolis and Purdue University, thank-you for your time.

Page 185: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

165

Appendix F. Wellness Handouts

Page 186: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

166

Personality Study Wellness Information

Regular wellness exams allow your veterinarian to evaluate your pet’s general health and become aware of any health problems before they become serious illnesses.

Healthy dogs should visit the veterinarian once a year for a complete exam. Healthy senior dogs should receive a wellness exam every six months. Depending on your pet’s age and health, your veterinarian will suggest an appropriate physical examination schedule to help keep your pet in tip-top shape.

A wellness exam includes an inspection of your pet’s lungs, heart, ears, eyes, mouth, skin condition, body condition, weight, and may include blood or urine tests to evaluate your pet’s health.

Do not underestimate the importance of taking your pet to the veterinarian for regular wellness examinations. These regular examinations will help your pet live a longer and healthier life, so do your part to care for your furry friend!

Source: http://www.healthypet.com/PetCare/DogCareArticle.aspx?title=Wellness_Exams

Page 187: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

167

Appendix G. Dog Personality Categories (Laminated)

Page 188: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

168

Personality Study Dog Personality Categories

Amicable – This dog is easy going, sociable, and friendly.

Extraverted – This dog is active, energetic, and lively.

Motivated – This dog is determined, independent, and persistent.

Sensitive – This dog is shy, nervous, and timid.

Training Focused – This dog is attentive, intelligent, and reliable.

Page 189: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

169

Appendix H. Information Sheet

Page 190: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

170

Personality Research Project – Information Sheet

Purpose of Study: This study is a randomized controlled study utilizing the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R) and the International Personality Item Pool (IPIP) to measure canine and human personality, respectively. Our first objective is to investigate whether there is an association between human personality and dog personality. Our second objective is to assess which matches between human and dog personalities result in better successful adoptions.

Eligibility: Part One (Relinquishment): In order to be eligible to participate, the person (the “Participant”) bringing the dog into the shelter must have owned the dog for longer than two weeks, must be at least 18 years of age, and the dog must be at least 1 year old.

Part Two (Adoption): The dog must pass the animal shelter’s physical and behavioral evaluations before being placed on the adoption floor. In order to be eligible to participate, the person adopting the dog (the “Participant”) must be at least 18 years of age.

Procedures: Relinquishment: Once the person bringing their dog to the shelter receives this information sheet, s/he will be asked to complete the MCPQ-R for the dog that s/he is relinquishing to the shelter. Once the MCPQ-R is completed, it will be placed in a sealed envelope and placed into the “Personality Research Drop Box”, to ensure confidentiality. The estimated time to complete the MCPQ-R questionnaire is less than 10 minutes.

Adoption: Before study groups are assigned, potential adopters will receive this information sheet. All study participants will be given a paper version of the IPIP questionnaire to complete. Once the IPIP is completed, it will be placed in a sealed envelope and placed into the “Personality Research Drop Box”, to ensure confidentiality. The estimated time to complete the IPIP questionnaire is less than 15 minutes. Participants will be randomly assigned to one of two groups and given an envelope. The contents of the envelope will contain information on what participants need to do in the next part of the study. Adopters in both groups will be free to adopt the animal of their choice.

Follow-up: Two months after adoption, adopters in both groups will be contacted via telephone to remind them that the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) questionnaire and the MCPQ-R questionnaire will be sent to their home addresses for completion. These follow-up questionnaires will also be made available to complete online.

Confidentiality: All original data will be kept at Purdue University. Contact information and answers to each questionnaire will be kept strictly confidential.

Associated Risks: There are no associated risks with this study.

Compensation: Unfortunately, we are unable to provide compensation for completion of these questionnaires. However, please note that your generosity in providing your time is much appreciated.

Page 191: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

171 Incentives: The procedures during this study will have a positive impact on the dogs’ health and well-being, as one of our main outcome variables is measurement of a “successful” adoption. Understanding dog personality, human personality, and how they interact with one another will help us to assist animal shelters around the nation to improve adoption methods. By having scientifically evaluated personality assessments to match dogs with adopters, we hope, with your help, to give animal shelters a simple and effective tool to help increase adoptions and decrease the number of animals returned to shelters.

Questions about this project may be directed to Dr. Niwako Ogata at (765) 494-8775.

Your participation in this research study is entirely voluntary. You can skip any question that you feel uncomfortable answering. You are free to withdraw from this study at any time.

Page 192: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

VITA

Page 193: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

172

VITA Sheryl L. Walker

Current Address

2125A Fincastle Way Lafayette, Indiana 47909 (847) 668-8788 [email protected]

Academic Record 2014 Ph.D., Purdue University 2009 M.A., Western Michigan University 2006 B.S., Michigan State University Employment July 2010 – August 2014 Graduate Research Assistant, Maddie’s® Shelter Medicine Program, Department of Veterinary Clinical Sciences, College of Veterinary Medicine, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN May 2008 – June 2010 Research Auditor I, Quality Assurance Department, MPI Research, Mattawan, MI June 2007 – May 2008 Research Associate II, Reproductive Toxicology Department, MPI Research, Mattawan, MI June 2006 – June 2007 Research Associate I, Reproductive Toxicology Department, MPI Research, Mattawan, MI

Page 194: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

173

Awards and Honors 2012 SAFER Re-certification, ASPCA 2011 SAFER Certification, ASPCA 2010 Graduate Student Research Assistantship, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 2010 Registered Quality Assurance Professional-Good Laboratory Practices (RQAP-GLP) Certification, SQA 2009 Graduate Student Research Grant, Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI 2008 National AALAS Meeting Scholarship, Xigma Engineering Services/MI-AALAS 2008 Laboratory Animal Technologist (LATG), AALAS 2008 Laboratory Animal Technician (LAT), AALAS 2007 Assistant Laboratory Animal Technician (ALAT), AALAS Memberships in Academic, Professional, and Scholarly Societies 2014 – Present The Pet Professional Guild 2013 – Present Association of Professional Dog Trainers 2012 – Present Association for Behavior Analysis International 2011 – Present International Society for Applied Ethology 2011 – Present International Society for Anthrozoology 2009 – Present Animal Behavior Management Alliance 2008 – 2010 Michigan-branch, American Association for

Laboratory Animal Science

Page 195: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

174

2007 – 2010 National, American Association for Laboratory Animal Science

Lectures, Workshops, Abstract and Poster Presentations 1. Dog Training Tricks to Increase Adoptability. A New Leash on Life, Clinton County Sheriff, Frankfort, IN, July 2014. 2. Dog Training Tricks to Increase Adoptability. Purdue Veterinary Medicine Fall Conference, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, September 2013. 3. Introduction to Implementing Match-Up II in Your Shelter. Purdue Veterinary Medicine Fall Conference, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, September 2012. 4. Comparison of SAFERTM Aggression Assessment Results in Shelter Dogs at Intake and After a 3-Day Acclimation. Purdue University Summer Research Fellows Program, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, August 2012. 5. How to Clicker Train Your Dog. Indiana State Fair, Indianapolis, IN, August 2011. Publications Peer Reviewed Publications 1. Bennett, S.L., Weng, H-Y., Walker, S.L., Placer, M., *Litster, A. Comparison of SAFERTM behavior assessment results in shelter dogs at intake and after a 3-day acclimation period. J App Anim Welf Sci. In press, 2014. 2. Bennett, S.L., Litster, A., Walker, S.L., *Luescher, A.U. Investigating behavior assessment instruments to predict aggression in dogs. App Anim Behav Sci. 2012;141(3-4):139-148. 3. *Baker, L.E., Panos, J., Killinger, B., Peet, M., Bell, L., Haliw, L. Walker, S. Comparison of the discriminative stimulus effects of salvinorin A and its derivatives to U69,593 and U50,488 in rats. Psychopharm. 2009;203(2):203-211. Ph.D. Thesis Title Human and Canine Personality Assessment Instruments to Predict Successful Adoptions with Shelter Dogs. Purdue University, 2014.

Page 196: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

175

M.A. Thesis Title Salvinorin A Establishes Conditioned Place Aversion in Male Sprague-Dawley Rats. Western Michigan University, 2009. Professional Meetings and Courses Attended December 2013 Visit, Share, Learn Day. Michigan Humane Society, Rochester, MI September 2013 Purdue Veterinary Medicine Fall Conference. Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN May 2013 Good Clinical Practices and Good Documentation. Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN September 2012 Purdue Veterinary Medicine Fall Conference. Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN May 2012 Animal Behavior Management Alliance National Conference. San Francisco, CA September 2011 Purdue Veterinary Medicine Fall Conference. Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN July/August 2011 International Society for Applied Ethology 45th Congress. Indianapolis, IN July 2010 SAFER Certification Preparatory Course. American Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals, Holland, OH April 2010 Animal Behavior Management Alliance National Conference. Pittsburgh, PA April 2009 Animal Behavior Management Alliance National Conference. Providence, RI November 2008 American Association for Laboratory Animal Science National Conference. Indianapolis, IN

Page 197: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

176

April/May 2008 American Association for Laboratory Animal Science District 5 Conference. Newark, OH Current Research Interests Keywords: Dog behavior, behavior modification, operant conditioning, positive reinforcement, force-free training, clicker training, animal shelters Ph.D. research project: Human and Canine Personality Assessment Instruments to Predict Successful Adoptions with Shelter Dogs. Purdue University, 2014. Graduate Student Advising Major Advisor

Ph.D. 2013 – 2014 Dr. Niwako Ogata: Department of Veterinary Clinical Sciences, Purdue University. Proposed thesis title: Human and Canine Personality Assessment Instruments to Predict Successful Adoptions with Shelter Dogs. Dr. Ogata is currently an Assistant Professor of Animal Behavior at Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN. .

2012 – 2013 Dr. Annette Litster: Department of Veterinary Clinical Sciences, Purdue University. Dr. Litster left Purdue University December 2013, and is currently the Senior Veterinary Specialist, CAD Veterinary Operations for the US Companion Animal Division of Zoetis, West Lafayette, IN.

2011 – 2012 Dr. Annette Litster: Department of Veterinary Clinical

Sciences, Purdue University; Dr. George Moore: Department of Comparative Pathobiology, Purdue University. Dr. Moore left his position as co-advisor in 2012, and is currently a Professor of Clinical Epidemiology, Department of Comparative Pathobiology, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN.

Page 198: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

177

2010 – 2011 Dr. Andrew Luescher: Department of Veterinary Clinical Sciences, Purdue University. Dr. Luescher retired from Purdue University June 2011. M.A. (Thesis) 2007 – 2009 Dr. Lisa Baker: Department of Psychology, Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI. Thesis title: Salvinorin A Establishes Conditioned Place Aversion in Male Sprague-Dawley Rats. Dr. Baker is currently a Professor at Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI. Committee Member

Ph.D. 2010 – 2014 Dr. Alan Beck: Department of Comparative Pathobiology, Purdue University. Dr. Beck is currently the Director of Center of the Human-Animal Bond at Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN. 2010 – 2014 Dr. Christopher Eckhardt: Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University. Dr. Eckhardt is currently an Associate Clinical Professor at Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN. 2012 – 2014 Dr. Hsin-Yi Weng: Department of Comparative Pathobiology, Purdue University. Dr. Weng is currently an Assistant Professor of Clinical/Analytical Epidemiology at Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN. 2012 – 2014 Dr. Sheila D’Arpino: Maddie’s® Fund. Dr. D’Arpino is currently the Director of Maddie’s Animal Care Center at Maddie’s Fund®, Pleasanton, CA. 2010 – 2012 Dr. George Moore: Department of Comparative Pathobiology, Purdue University. Dr. Moore left the Advisory committee in 2012 to be replaced by Dr. Weng. Dr. Moore is currently a Professor of Clinical Epidemiology at Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN.

Page 199: Human and canine personality assessment instruments to predict successful adoptions with shelter

178

M.A. (Thesis) 2008 – 2009 Dr. Bradley Huitema: Department of Psychology, Western Michigan University. Dr. Huitema is currently a Professor at Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI. 2008 – 2009 Dr. Richard Spates: Department of Psychology, Western Michigan University. Dr. Spates is currently a Professor at Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI. Engagement 2013 – Present Board of Directors, Clinton County Humane

Society, Frankfort, IN 2012 – Present Behavior Specialist Volunteer, Clinton County

Humane Society, Frankfort, IN