how simple strategies can explain - philipp aerni · gigerenzer, hertwig, & pachur, 2011). the...

34
How simple strategies can explain ‘good’ and ‘bad’ behavior Ralph Hertwig Center for Adaptive Rationality (ARC)

Upload: others

Post on 04-Aug-2020

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

How simple strategies can explain ‘good’ and ‘bad’ behavior

Ralph Hertwig

Center for Adaptive Rationality (ARC)

Page 2: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

A hero of an unprecedented kind

(Chateaubriand)

Page 3: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

110 rules of civility and decent behavior

Rule 6.

Sleep not when others speak, sit not when others stand, speak not when you should hold your peace, walk not on when others stop.

Rule 100.

Cleanse not your teeth with the table cloth napkin, fork, or knife; but if others do it, let it be done without a peep to them.

Rule 69.

If two contend together, take not the part of either unconstrained, and be not obstinate in your own opinion. In things indifferent be of the major side.

Page 4: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

I will argue …

Homo heuristicus Much of human decision making can be understood in terms of simple heuristics.

(NB: They often result in surprisingly accurate performance; see Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011).

The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties of the world can produce complex behavior, including moral inconsistencies.

Judging character (or lack thereof) Moral dispositions (e.g., virtuousness, fairness) or lack thereof can (often) not be inferred from behavior alone.

Rather, normative evaluations require the analysis of heuristic‒environment interactions (see Doris, 2002).

Page 5: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

What heuristics are

• A heuristic is a strategy that neither searches, weighs, and adds up all available information, nor makes extensive use of computation to reach optimized solutions. – “Approximate methods” (Simon, 1990)

• Rule 69. …. In things indifferent be of the major side.

Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur (2011). Heuristics: The foundations of adaptive behavior. OUP.

Page 6: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Why heuristics are indispensable tools

• Computational intractability

No brain or existing machine can compute the optimal strategy (if it existed)

E.g., in chess Deep Blue would need 55 billion years to think 10 moves by each party ahead

• Competing, incommensurable reasons in social

environments

Sacred values preclude tradeoffs (Tetlock et al., 2000)

Hertwig, Hoffrage, & the ABC Research Group (2009). Simple heuristics in a social world. Oxford.

Page 7: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

© Die Zeit

“I appointed Guttenberg as a defense minister. I did not

appoint him as a research assistant or doctoral student or

a holder of a degree. I am concerned with his work as a

defense minister, and he is accomplishing that

excellently. That is what counts for me.”

Page 8: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Why heuristics are indispensable tools

• Computational intractability

No brain or existing machine can compute the optimal strategy (if it existed)

E.g., in chess Deep Blue would need 55 billion years to think 10 moves by each party ahead

• Competing, incommensurable reasons in social

environments

Sacred values preclude tradeoffs (Tetlock et al., 2000)

• Time pressure

Hertwig, Hoffrage, & the ABC Research Group (2009). Simple heuristics in a social world. Oxford.

Page 9: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

• Heuristics based on

– one-reason decision

making (e.g., fully

zipped, bulky coat)

– few cues: older,

female and single

Page 10: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

The emergence of behavioral

complexity

The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties of the world can produce complex behavior, including moral inconsistencies.

Page 11: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Allocation patterns of parental time

Page 12: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Hertwig, Davis, & Sulloway (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 728–745.

Allocation patterns of parental time

Page 13: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Sociological analysis. “As members of an egalitarian society, Americans typically espouse equal treatment of children by parents.” (Zervas & Sherman, 1994, p. 31)

Page 14: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Equity heuristic (1/N heuristic)

Divide resources (e.g., time, attention, money,

medicine, food) equally among your N children at any

given point in time.

Hertwig, Davis, & Sulloway (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 728–745.

Page 15: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Do parents use 1/N?

Price (2008). Journal of Human Resources, 43, 240–265.

Division of estate (last will)

More than two-thirds in U.S. divide their

estates equally among their children (Bernheim & Severinov, 2003, Journal of Political Economy)

Page 16: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Allocation patterns of parental time

Hertwig, Davis, & Sulloway (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 728–745.

Page 17: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Some assumptions

Parental investment period. Divided into a number of intervals of equal length.

1/N heuristic. Applied within each of these intervals.

Donor of resources. Only parents but not children donate resources.

Resource budget. Limited and stable.

• Interpersonal resources (e.g., time)

• Cognitive resources (e.g., intellectual stimulation)

• Material resources (e.g., health care, higher education fees)

Page 18: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

1970‒74

1975‒79

1980‒84

1985‒89

1990‒94

1995‒99

Page 19: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Cumulative resources: U-shaped trend

Middle-born resource handicap

Page 20: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Hertwig, Davis, & Sulloway (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 728–745.

Page 21: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Judging character (or lack thereof)

Moral dispositions (e.g., virtuousness, fairness) or

lack thereof can (often) not be inferred from

behavior alone. Rather, normative evaluations

require the analysis of heuristic‒environment

interactions.

Page 22: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Hertwig, Davis, & Sulloway (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 728–745.

Page 23: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties
Page 24: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Micromotives and macrobehavior

Schelling (1971). Journal of Mathematical Sociology, 1, 143–186.

Page 25: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Conclusions

• People use simple heuristics to navigate complex, conflicting, uncertain and fast-changing environments.

• The heuristics’ behavioral outcomes—often used to infer “character”—depend often on their interaction with properties of the environment. But inferences about character underestimate the impact of the environment.

• Teach and practice simple rules (e.g., Washington).

• Change the environment (e.g., default heuristic and organ donation opt-out system; Johnson & Goldstein, 2003).

Page 26: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

How simple strategies can explain ‘good’ and ‘bad’ behavior

Ralph Hertwig

Center for Adaptive Rationality (ARC)

Page 27: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Interbirth interval and inequality

Hertwig, Davis, & Sulloway (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 728–745.

Page 28: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

• Evolutionary analysis. A unit of investment is divided equally unless its marginal benefit is larger for one child than for another. (Hamilton, 1964; Trivers, 1974)

• Economic analysis. If the parental payoff curve, which depends on child “quality” (e.g., abilities, health), is the same for each child, parents distribute equally among children. (Becker, 1991)

• Sociological analysis. “As members of an egalitarian society, Americans typically espouse equal treatment of children by parents.” (Zervas & Sherman, 1994, p. 31)

Possible solutions

Page 29: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Family size and inequality

Hertwig, Davis, & Sulloway (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 728–745.

Page 30: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

The complex world of parental

investment

• Children are “gas guzzlers,” and parental resources are limited.

• In hunter–gatherer societies, a child consumes between 10 and 13 million calories more than s/he produces (age 0 to 18; Kaplan, 1994).

• How should parents allocate the myriad resources required (e.g., food, medicine, attention, love, and education)?

Coall & Hertwig (2010). Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 1–19.

Page 31: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Allocation patterns of parental time

Hertwig, Davis, & Sulloway (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 728–745.

Page 32: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties
Page 33: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties
Page 34: How simple strategies can explain - Philipp Aerni · Gigerenzer, Hertwig, & Pachur, 2011). The emergence of behavioral complexity The interaction of a simple heuristic with properties

Tit-for-tat strategy

• Cooperate on the first move, and then do whatever the opponent has done on the previous move.

Tit-for-tat Always defect

Cooperate Defect

Defect Defect

Defect Defect

Defect Defect

Defect Defect

Tit-for-tat Always coop.

Cooperate Cooperate

Cooperate Cooperate

Cooperate Cooperate

Cooperate Cooperate

Cooperate Cooperate

Round

1

2

3

4

5