homicide studies 2011 nivette 103 31

Upload: corina-ica

Post on 02-Jun-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    1/30

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/ Homicide Stud ies

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/content/15/2/103The online version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1177/1088767911406397

    2011 15: 103 originally published online 27 April 2011Homicide Studies Amy E. Nivette

    Cross-National Predictors of Crime: A Meta-Analysis

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    On behalf of:

    Homicide Research Working Group

    can be found at:Homicide Studies Additional services and information for

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/content/15/2/103.refs.htmlCitations:

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/content/15/2/103http://hsx.sagepub.com/content/15/2/103http://hsx.sagepub.com/content/15/2/103http://www.sagepublications.com/http://www.icpsr.umich.edu/HRWGhttp://hsx.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://hsx.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://hsx.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://hsx.sagepub.com/content/15/2/103.refs.htmlhttp://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/content/15/2/103.refs.htmlhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://hsx.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://hsx.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://www.icpsr.umich.edu/HRWGhttp://www.sagepublications.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/content/15/2/103http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    2/30

    Homicide Studies15(2) 103 131

    2011 SAGE PublicationsReprints and permission: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

    DOI: 10.1177/1088767911406397

    http://hsx.sagepub.com

    HSX406397 HSX15210.1177/1088767911406397NivetteHomicideStudies

    1University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

    Corresponding Author:Amy E. Nivette, Institute of Criminology, University of Cambridge,Sidgwick Avenue, Cambridge CB3 9DA, UKEmail: [email protected]

    Cross-National Predictorsof Crime: A Meta-Analysis

    Amy E. Nivette 1

    AbstractCross-national research has increased in the past few decades, resulting in a large bodyof empirical research. In particular, cross-national studies are often limited in datasources, which restrict variable selection to debatable proxy indicators. This studytherefore uses meta-analytic techniques to examine major cross-national predictorsof homicide to determine strengths and weaknesses in theory and design. Thefindings indicate several critical limitations to cross-national research, including biasedsample composition, a lack of theoretical clarity in predictor operationalizations, andan overwhelming reliance on cross-sectional design. The predictors that showedthe strongest mean effects were Latin American regional dummy variables, incomeinequality indicators and the Decommodification Index. Conversely, static populationindicators, democracy indices, and measures of economic development had the weakesteffects on homicide.

    Keywordsmeta-analysis, homicide, cross-national, predictors

    Introduction

    In recent decades, a large body of research has emerged to explain variations in levelsof homicide across nations (Marshall, Marshall, & Ren, 2009; Stamatel, 2009a; VanDijk, 2008). These explanations have been primarily drawn from the Durkheimian-Modernization theoretical tradition, which suggests rapid societal and structuralchange to be the primary source of deviance within a society (Kick & LaFree, 1985;LaFree, 1999). Cross-national literature consequently reflects this long-standing influencethrough studies that focus on examining correlations between homicide and population

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    3/30

    104 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    composition, economic conditions, and levels of deprivation. Despite the pervasiveinfluence of the Durkheimian-Modernization perspective in cross-national research,evidence concerning this and other theories remains unclear.

    Certain soft concepts such as trust and cultural values remain scarcely tested atthe international level. Soft, or sociocultural, predictors are measures of attitudesand perceptions, norms and values, or religious beliefs that are often considered medi-ators of structural variables in neighborhood-level studies (Sampson, 2006a). Althoughthe development of worldwide sociocultural data sets (e.g., the World Values Survey)have led to more direct testing of these concepts, their impact remains unknown. Asmuch of the focus in cross-national research concentrates on structural covariates,assessing the predictive potential of these sociocultural concepts can encourage newavenues of cross-national research.

    As yet, no study has attempted to statistically analyze the status of cross-nationalempirical research, which is somewhat surprising due to the proclaimed importance ofthe comparative perspective for theoretical development (Bennett, 2004; Braithwaite,1989; Evans, LaGrange, & Willis, 1996). There are a few narrative research syntheses(LaFree, 1999; Pridemore, 2002; Pridemore & Trent, 2010; Stamatel, 2006), but onlytwo studies have attempted to statistically assess the aggregate effect of macro-levelindicators and theories (see Hsieh & Pugh, 1993; Pratt & Cullen, 2005). Even so, theserely heavily on U.S.-based studies, with only a handful of effect sizes drawn fromstudies using a non-U.S. sample, and even fewer from the cross-national level. For

    instance, Pratt and Cullen (2005) included 12 studies from the cross-national level outof 214, whereas Hsieh and Pugh (1993) included only 10 out of 34. Furthermore, themost recent meta-analysis (Pratt & Cullen, 2005) only includes studies up to the year1999. However, many new studies have emerged since, reflecting the recent momen-tum in cross-national interest that incorporates improvements and expansions in datasources, analytic techniques, and theoretical perspectives (Marshall & Block, 2004;Van Dijk, 2008).

    Although most studies at this level focus on structural influences drawn from theDurkheimian-Modernization perspective, many different theories have been applied

    to cross-national data (e.g., strain, social disorganization, democratization, and routineactivity theories). The majority of studies focus on homicide since it is considered themost reliable crime indicator in cross-national research (Neapolitan, 1997), although ahandful of studies examine other types of crime. Despite the known reliability ofhomicide data, slight differences in data-collection techniques and definitions betweeninternational data sources may affect the results. Thus, it is necessary to organize theseresearch findings so that clear information regarding the size and direction of commoneffects can guide a more efficient research design. A meta-analysis can assess the rela-tive empirical impact and robustness of variables associated with these theories. The

    current study therefore has three main objectives: (a) assess the mean effect sizes forcross-national predictors of homicide, (b) determine the impact of methodologicalvariations on study outcomes, and (c) evaluate the relative strength of sociocultural ver-sus structural predictors.

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    4/30

  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    5/30

    106 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    The outcome variable was restricted to homicide due to the lack of validity in allother categories of international crime data (Neapolitan, 1997). Cross-national researchis highly conditional on the category of the crime statistics used due to cross-cultural

    and operational differences in definitions and reporting (Marshall & Block, 2004; VanDijk, 2008). Homicide data are generally considered to be the most reliable by com- parative criminologists as a victims body is most likely to come to the attention ofofficials, regardless of reporting trends (Neapolitan, 1997). However, while focusingon homicide may aid cross-cultural comparability, this restricts the interpretation andgeneralization of results.

    Relevant quantitative studies that fell within the time frame 1960-2010 were searchedfor using the keywords cross-national or cross-cultural and crime , violence , murder , orhomicide . The search proceeded in three parts. First, major criminological journals were

    canvassed issue by issue, especially those with a comparative and international focus.1

    Second, scholarly electronic databases were searched using the keywords stated above(Criminal Justice Abstracts, National Criminal Justice Reference Service, SociologicalAbstracts, Worldwide Political Science Abstracts, Web of Knowledge, and GoogleScholar). In addition, international organizations publication databases were searchedto incorporate research outside journal publications (World Health Organization[WHO] Library, United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime [UNODC], and EuropeanInstitute for Crime Prevention and Control [HEUNI]). Third, previous syntheses werescanned for cross-national studies that may have been missed. A total of 54 studies were

    included (see appendix for a complete list), resulting in 316 effect sizes.

    Effect Size EstimateTo precisely gauge how much a certain predictor influences the outcome, the effectsize, or the measure of the strength . . . and direction of a relationship between vari-ables (Littell, Corcoran, & Pillai, 2008, p. 80) must be considered. These effect sizes,when combined, can reveal the overall magnitude of like predictors. The measure ofrelationship strength used here is the standardized correlation coefficient r . The r index

    is used because it is most commonly employed in analyses concerning two continuousvariables and easily interpreted (Pratt & Cullen, 2005; Wilson, 2001). In correla-tional research, results are presented as either zero-order correlations or standardizedregression coefficients (). 2 The latter is favored in the current analyses because-coefficients can be considered a more accurate representation of the relationship,whereas bivariate correlations may be inflated due to failure to account for exogenousinfluences. Thus, with the caveat that effect size must be considered in context withother predictors, mean effect sizes can be safely combined (Pratt & Cullen, 2005).

    Fixed Versus Random EffectsThere are two models available in meta-analysis: the fixed effects and random effectsmodel. The former rests on the assumption that there is one true effect size within the

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    6/30

    Nivette 107

    population, and all differences between effects can be accounted for by sampling erroralone (Cooper, 2010; Littell et al., 2008). In random effects models, variations withinand between studies are thought to differ according to model specification, concurrent

    variables, or exogenous factors (Littell et al., 2008). Due to the nature of the effectsizes preferred in this meta-analysis (-coefficients), the random effects model isideal. That is to say, as is an effect size controlling for all other included factors, therandom effects model can appropriately account for any variations occurring due todifferences in the regression model specification.

    IndependenceIn many cases, research findings present more than one effect size. Multiple models

    are often constructed to test for various moderating factors on one or more dependentvariables. Including more than one effect size drawn from the same sample threatensassumptions of independence in meta-analytic procedure (Littell et al., 2008; Wilson,2001). In cross-national research in particular, this assumption is in danger due to theoverreliance on convenience sampling (see Stamatel, 2006), which leads to similarsample composition across studies. These overlapping samples create the potential fora lack of statistical independence in both within- and between-study effect size esti-mates. However, as almost all studies include Western societies while very few man-age to include Islamic or African nations, most studies will have a sample bias in the

    same direction (i.e., toward developed rather than less developed societies). This biascannot be easily overcome, yet it remains important to conduct a meta-analysis to geta comprehensive picture of cross-national knowledge. Therefore, one must proceedwith the added caveat that the results may be limited to developed countries.

    There are several options to account for potential bias stemming from dependence:to average the effect sizes for each predictor within each study to produce an overallstudy estimate (Wilson, 2001), to choose only one effect size per study or, when mul-tiple predictors are assessed against one dependent variable (e.g., income inequalityand urbanization on homicide), to assess each predictor separately (Littell et al., 2008).

    The first and third options are preferable because, although averaging effect sizes andanalyzing separately somewhat reduce the power of the statistical procedures, theoption to select only one effect size may introduce unnecessary researcher bias whendeciding which effect size within a study to choose. Thus in the present analysis, anaverage effect size was calculated per study for each predictor, then pooled and ana-lyzed against methodological variations separately.

    Heterogeneity of Effect Sizes

    Pooling effect sizes must be done with careful consideration to both underlying con-ceptual and theoretical differences between studies as well as statistical heterogeneity(Cooper, 2010; Littell et al., 2008). To ensure against this issue, only measures usedto operationalize the same concept are grouped together (e.g., gross domestic product

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    7/30

    108 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    [GDP], gross national product [GNP], and average income were grouped underEconomic Development), and a coefficient is calculated to test for extreme differ-ences between grouped indicators using the meta-analytic analogue to the ANOVA

    (Q) Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009; Wilson, 2002). Any predictors thatdemonstrate significant statistical heterogeneity are analyzed separately (e.g., ethnicheterogeneity and ethnic homogeneity).

    Predictor DomainsDue to considerable limitations to the availability and reliability of data, cross-nationalresearch suffers from a rather modest development of theory (LaFree, 1999).However, 11 perspectives were identified for exploration in this meta-analysis, from

    which 30 separate predictors emerged. As a general rule, any variable with two ormore contributing independent effect sizes are included in the analysis (Littell et al.,2008). This unfortunately led to the exclusion of several interesting predictors due tounique operationalizations or a simple lack of contributing studies. What follows is a

    brief overview of each theory, its primary predictor domains, and operationalizations. 3

    Absolute DeprivationA link between area poverty and homicide is a consistent finding in most crimino-

    logical research (Pratt & Cullen, 2005; Pridemore, 2002, 2008) stemming from theidea that resource deprivation causes frustration, which ultimately may lead to aggres-sion (Hsieh & Pugh, 1993). The pursuit of the povertycrime link is common in U.S.literature but relatively lacking in cross-national research. Indeed, Pridemore (2008)argues that the substantial evidence behind this association in the American contextdemands the inclusion of poverty in comparative explanations and models. Povertyindicators are not unknown to cross-national research but are often expressed by argu-ably theoretically ineffectual proxies such as GDP (Pridemore, 2008). Pridemore insteadadvocates the more appropriate and conceptually encompassing proxy of Infant Mortality,

    which is used here.4

    Relative DeprivationThe theory of relative deprivation stems from a Mertonian anomie perspective, in that

    blocked opportunities to achieving prescribed cultural goals cause individual frustra-tion and aggression, which may lead to homicide (Chamlin & Cochran, 2006; LaFree,1999). This societal imbalance is most often conceptualized by disparities in the dis-tribution of wealth. Expressed in ratios or indices, the higher proportion of wealth

    held by the top deciles compared with the bottom theoretically indicates a greater potential for feelings of relative deprivation. Due to significant statistical heterogene-ity, three predictors emerged for analysis: Economic Discrimination of Minorities,Income Inequality Measured by Ratios, and Income Inequality Measured by Indices(including the Gini Index).

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    8/30

    Nivette 109

    Modernization/Development

    The Durkheimian-Modernization perspective is the earliest and consequentially most

    common theoretical avenue explored in cross-national literature (LaFree, 1999). Althoughtechnically encompassing a wider range of theories including anomie and social disor-ganization, the Durkheimian-Modernization perspective aims to explain the increasein crime through changing social patterns, which have as a result disrupted traditionalmethods of social control (Hartnagel & Mizanuddin, 1986; LaFree, 1999; Shichor,1990). The shift from agricultural to industrial labor, advances in technology and com-munication, increases in access to education, and breakdowns in traditional commu-nity structure are considered evidence of modernization (Austin & Kim, 1999; Howard& Smith, 2003). Development is thought to adversely affect crime rates in a similar

    fashion to modernization (Bennett, 1991), yet researchers have noted that the associa-tion between measures of modernization/development and homicide appears to benegative rather than positive (DiCristina, 2004; Kick & LaFree, 1985). Because of this,scholars have turned to more opportunity-based interpretations to explain this relation-ship: modernization works to increase the level of available goods and motivatedoffenders for theft, while simultaneously breaking down interpersonal ties that conse-quently diminish interpersonal violence (Kick & LaFree, 1985).

    In practice, modernization and development predictors are dispersed across manydifferent theories, factored into indices, and variously operationalized. Thus the group-

    ing done here focuses on what underlying concept the predictor is trying to capture.The Modernization predictor included three operationalizations: newspapers per1,000, energy consumption per capita, and an index created by Howard and Smith(2003). Predictors encompassing access to education were Adult Literacy and FemaleHigher Education Enrolment. Development was subdivided into two separate predic-tors: economic and human. Economic Development combined measures of GDP percapita, GNP per capita, and average income per capita. Human Development measuresranged from factored indices created by the author to the use of the Human DevelopmentIndex drawn from the United Nations.

    Social DisorganizationSocial disorganization theory follows much of the same line of thought as Durkheimian-Modernization perspectives and therefore will only be discussed briefly. Social changesthat alter the family and community structure are thought to affect the efficiency ofinformal controls that previously prevented deviant behavior (Shaw & McKay, 1942).Population heterogeneity and household disruption are seen as the primary sources ofdiscord in maintaining networks of control, leading to higher homicide rates (Gartner,

    1990). Two conceptually distinct predictors emerged for social disorganization: house-hold disruption as measured by the Divorce Rate and ethnic composition measured interms of Ethnic Heterogeneity and Ethnic Homogeneity. Jensens (2001) multiculturaland Antonaccio and Tittles (2007) population heterogeneity variables were combinedinto the ethnic heterogeneity predictor.

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    9/30

    110 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    Institutional Anomie Theory

    Drawing on Esping-Andersons concept of decommodification and Mertons strain

    theory, Messner and Rosenfeld (1997) devised institutional anomie theory to explainthe interrelated effects of social and economic structures on crime. Simplisticallyspeaking, it follows the Marxist line of thought that the dominance of economic institu-tions creates a harmful environment for the populace, leading to crime. In combatingthis harm, the welfare state provides services, rights, and safety from the anomic pres-sures of a market-dominant, goal-oriented society (Altheimer, 2008; Bjerregaard &Cochran, 2008; Messner & Rosenfeld, 1997). Thus the higher the levels of decom-modificationor protection from the severity of the market, whether through policy orfamilythe lower the potential for crime. The primary predictor used to measure insti-

    tutional anomie theory is the Decommodification Index, although several studies alsoemploy measures of welfare expenditures. The Decommodification Index is created bycombining measures of social welfare expenditures as a percentage of GDP, averageannual benefits per capita, and the percentage of benefits given to employment injuries(Altheimer, 2008, p. 107). Straightforward measures of welfare expenditures, however,will be grouped under social support theory. In addition, Bjerregaard and Cochran(2008) opted to measure the strength of economic institutions with the EconomicFreedom Index and therefore could not be appropriately included in the analysis.

    Social SupportInterrelated with institutional anomie theory, the social support (or altruism, seeChamlin & Cochran, 1997) perspective aims to explain homicide based on levels oflocal and government welfare provisions. This can be in the form of social security,health care, a community program, or family bonds, as they all commonly promote anenvironment of mutual trust and reciprocity that effectively prevents crime (Colvin,Cullen, & Ven, 2002). At the national level, social support is expressed as governmentwelfare expenditures, although emerging research investigating levels of generalized

    trust and volunteerism (e.g., Halpern, 2001) may also be construed as indicators ofinformal support. Thus the predictors comprising Social Support are percentage GNPspent on welfare, health care, or education. Effect sizes within studies that includedmore than one of these variables were averaged.

    Routine Activity Situational perspectives encompass routine activity theory, claiming that changes

    brought by modernization have altered population patterns, effectively increasing the

    opportunity for crime (Cohen & Felson, 1979; LaFree, 1999). The theoretical con-cepts of routine activity theory are interpersonal contacts, opportunity/access togoods, pool of motivated offenders, and guardianship. Essentially, changing structuralfactors (e.g., population growth, urbanization) bring together both potential offenders

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    10/30

    Nivette 111

    (e.g., youth population) and potential targets (e.g., industrial goods) in unsupervisedsituations (due to female labor force participation), which can lead to acts of crime.Like modernization and development theories, the predictors used for these concepts

    tend to overlap with other perspectives. Consequently, three general predictors weredrawn from the pool of studies: Persons per Household for interpersonal contacts,Female Labor Participation for guardianship, and Unemployment for motivatedoffenders.

    DeterrenceBased on the assumption that offenders make rational decisions to commit a crime byweighing potential consequences and benefits, deterrence theory posits that the more

    the criminal justice system activity, the lower the crime (Pratt, Cullen, Blevins, Daigle,& Madensen, 2006). In other words, the more visible the potential costs to commit-ting crime (i.e., the death penalty, high clearance rates, longer sentencing), the lesslikely an individual would be to act. Although extensively studied on the macro levelwithin the United States, deterrence theory is somewhat rare in cross-nationalresearch, producing only six effect sizes from studies, of which the majority are includedas secondary or control variables. The Deterrence predictor was formed combiningthree measures of criminal justice system activity: the number of police personnel,clearance rate, and a death penalty dummy variable.

    Political StructurePolitical structure may influence homicide in a variety of ways. For instance, one

    perspective places democracy alongside modernization forces that work to diffusesocial control and discipline among the populace (Lin, 2007). Alternatively, oppres-sive characteristics of a nondemocratic political regime may affect levels of homicide

    by exposing the population to officially sanctioned violence (Stamatel, 2009b). Bothof these share the basic hypothesis, however, that democracy or democratic values are

    inversely related to violence. Studies that seek to explain cross-national homicidefrom a political perspective tend to focus on levels of Democracy or Political Rights,as measured by indices. The political rights predictor consists of a combination of theCivil Liberties Index and Political Rights Index produced by Freedom House. In stud-ies where both indices were included as independent variables, these values wereaveraged to create a single effect size.

    Culture

    The concept of culture is captured in many ways in cross-national research. Someauthors indicate unique cultural differences through the inclusion of a regionaldummy variable. For example, Neapolitan (1994) included a dummy variable forLatin American nations in his analysis to determine if simply being a Latin American

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    11/30

    112 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    nation can predict homicide rates. He argues that Latin American nations maintaincertain cultural values (e.g., machismo ) that favor the use of violence. The interpreta-tion of these results consequently hinges on controlling for important structural factors,

    but the dummy ultimately represents certain characteristics unmeasured by traditionalvariables. In this sense, regional dummy variables are very important in attempting toencapsulate often difficult-to-operationalize cultural predictors (Pridemore, 2002).The regional dummies included in this analysis are Latin American and Caribbean

    Nations, East Asian Nations (e.g., Japan), and Western Nations (e.g., the United States,the United Kingdom). Only one study (Austin & Kim, 1999) considered an Africanregion variable and therefore could not be included.

    Several interesting predictors were unfortunately excluded for their singularity:Halperns (2001) measure of moral values, Pampel and Gartners (1995) Collectivism

    Scale, Lederman, Loayza, and Menendezs (2002) measurement of religiosity, Jensens(2001) religious fundamentalism variable, and McAlisters (2006) aggregated mea-surement of attitudes toward violence. 5 Although it is not possible to draw conclusionsabout the significance of these predictors on a meta-analytic level, the more generalquestion concerning structural versus sociocultural indicators can address whetherthese areas are worth further pursuit in cross-national research.

    Demographic Predictors

    Demographic predictors are most commonly included in cross-national research ascontrol variables, implying that each of these factors plays a significant enough rolein influencing homicide rates that they cannot be ignored in the model. Theoretically,demographic variables are generally claimed as predictors for social disorganization(e.g., urbanization, youth population, ethnic heterogeneity), routine activity (e.g., youth

    population, population density), or modernization (e.g., population growth and density,urbanization). However, many studies include demographic controls merely based onthe fact that previous studies have done the same. Often ignored in results sections infavor of the target variables, the question remains as to exactly what effect these demo-

    graphic predictors have on homicide.The predictors gathered considered population characteristics, youth population proportion, sex ratio, and urbanization. The population characteristics were subdi-vided into population growth, population density, and population total. Due to signifi-cant statistical heterogeneity, the proportion of youth population (i.e., age structure)was divided into two predictors: age < 15 and age 15 and above. Sex Ratio is measured

    by the ratio of males to females and measures of Urbanism by the percentage of peopleliving in urban areas.

    Methodological VariationsIt is important to code for variations in design, data source, and measurement toaccount for possible methodological influences in the outcome variable. These coded

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    12/30

    Nivette 113

    variations can then be analyzed as moderator variables when conducting theANOVA analogue and meta-regression (Cooper, 2010; Littell et al., 2008). In somecases, an inadequate number of effect sizes prevented analyses of moderator vari-

    ables, and in these instances the effect sizes are merely ranked according to strength.Drawing from Stamatels (2006) criticisms of cross-national research concerning theconsiderable restrictions imposed by outdated data (time) and limited sample size(space), this study specifically aims to assess the relative impact of these varia-tions on resulting effect sizes. In addition, certain categorical variables such as datasource and a quality indicator are assessed to determine the robustness of the meaneffect sizes.

    TimeA researchers choice of model and design can affect how one is to interpret theirresults. In particular, time specifications, such as cross-sectional versus longitudinaldesigns, reflect the assumptions made about the relationships between predictors andcrime. In cross-national research, an overwhelming majority of studies are specified ascross-sectional or pooled cross-sectional time series due to the assumption that timedoes not matter (Stamatel, 2006, p. 190). Dugan (2010) argues that this assumptionignores the inherent dynamic quality of crime data, effectively confound[ing] thecause with the effect (p. 741). Unfortunately, very few cross-national studies incorpo-

    rated time-series analyses, making any categorical comparisons difficult. In light ofthis, time was instead conceptualized in terms of the year from which homicide datawere drawn. This time artifact may signify an important meaning between certain pre-dictors and a particular period in time. Data drawn from a certain year (or across years)are accompanied by a corresponding array of possible political, social, or culturalevents for each nation, which may or may not be reflected in the target predictor vari-ables. In other words, studies using older data may have time-specific contextual dif-ferences hitherto unknown before placed alongside more recent findings.

    For the meta-regression analysis, the year from which the homicide data were

    drawn was coded as a continuous variable for each study. Cross-national researchroutinely computes homicide averages over periods of time to avoid fluctuations inreported rates. In these cases, the latest year included in the average was recorded.

    Sample Size and SelectionSample size is a critical issue in much cross-national research. The lack of availabledata for both crime and predictor statistics often severely limits the sample composi-tion to primarily industrialized, highly developed nations (Stamatel, 2006; Marshall

    et al., 2009). This limitation may affect the outcome in both a statistical and contex-tual manner. In a statistical sense, small samples may lack the power and precision tocorrectly detect an effect (Britt & Weisburd, 2010), which can partially be solved byconducting a meta-analysis (Hunter & Schmidt, 2004). Moreover, examining the

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    13/30

    114 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    relationship between sample size and effect size through meta-regression can illumi-nate exactly how statistical power affects the outcome.

    Alternatively, implicit in the sample size is the distribution of nations, whereby the

    larger the sample, the more diverse the range of countries and contexts. This meaningof sample size attends to the idea that outcomes may be significantly biased by cross-national criminologys reliance on convenience sampling, consequently producing arather narrow picture of global crime (Stamatel, 2009a). One of the fundamental questsin comparative criminology is the search for universal relationships, trends, or mean-ings among numerous cultural contexts (Karstedt, 2001). Therefore, the samplingselection acts as a key ingredient to the testing of criminological theory: significantresults that hold over a large distribution of contexts add strength to that perspective.Conversely, if a predictors effect is significantly reduced by an increase in sample,

    this might signify contextual differences held by nations that may alter the roles andmeanings of the targeted theoretical concept.

    Categorical Variables: Data Source and Quality There are several sources of international crime statistics: the Comparative CrimeData File [CCDF], the United Nations, WHO, the International Criminal and PoliceOrganization [Interpol], HEUNI, and the European Sourcebook. The strengths andlimitations of these data sources are reported extensively elsewhere (see Howard &

    Smith, 2003; Marshall & Block, 2004; Marshall et al., 2009; Van Dijk, 2008) andtherefore are only briefly discussed here, but it is important to note that WHO homi-cide statistics are considered to be the most reliable (Neapolitan, 1997). Internationaldata sources have been criticized for several reasons, namely, the variation in interna-tional definitions of intentional and unintentional homicide, the ambiguity pertainingto the inclusion or exclusion or attempted homicides in overall data, and the potentialfor official misclassification of violent deaths (Marshall & Block, 2004). The use ofdifferent and potentially unreliable data sources may significantly compromise theresults, producing misleading conclusions. Therefore, each study will be coded accord-

    ing to the source of data used: CCDF, United Nations, WHO, Interpol, Other (includ-ing HEUNI and European Sourcebook). In cases where data are drawn from multiplesources, these will be coded as mixed.

    To account for potential differences in quality, each report was coded by whether itwas peer-reviewed. Analyzing the differences between these categories can helpassess the file-drawer problem, which is the tendency of journals to favor publishingsignificant over nonsignificant results (Rosenthal, 1979).

    Statistical AnalysisFollowing standard meta-analytical procedure, effect sizes drawn from studies aretransformed into z (r ) values using Fishers r -to- z transformation and pooled into meaneffect size estimates (Littell et al., 2008; Pratt & Cullen, 2005; Wilson, 2001). This

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    14/30

    Nivette 115

    process normalizes the sampling distribution of r , which is skewed for all nonzerovalues. Using the random effects procedures, the estimates are then weighted by theirinverse variance (Borenstein et al., 2009). Diagnostic tests are also conducted to test

    for heterogeneity by using the ANOVA analogue ( Q) to detect significant differences between predictor subcategories. 6 Any extreme effect size values are divided intoseparate predictors to minimize bias. In addition, a coefficient of heterogeneity ( Q) iscomputed to examine the distribution of effects around the mean (Borenstein et al.,2009). A heterogeneity coefficient can indicate how widely individual effect sizesvary for a given predictor. The resulting mean effect size estimates are then ranked byoverall strength. Using Wilsons (2002) macros for SPSS, the meta-analytic analogueto the ANOVA is performed on selected categorical variables to detect significantdifferences in effect sizes between the data sources used as well as whether the study

    was peer reviewed. Generalized least squares meta-regression is used for analyzinginfluences of time and sample size on effect sizes. Analyses are conducted separatelyto maintain independence among effect size estimates.

    ResultsThe results are presented in the following order: Information about the sample of stud-ies is described, mean effect sizes and confidence intervals organized by theory are

    presented (Table 1) and then rank-ordered for clarity (Table 2), categorical influences

    in design are investigated (Table 3), and finally meta-regression results are consideredin relation to time and sample size (Table 4).

    Sample CharacteristicsThe number of countries included in the sampled studies ranged from 11 to 100, withan average of 44 and a mode of 44 nations. The most frequently tested indicators weremeasures of economic development ( n = 37), followed by income inequality indices(n = 31) and urbanism ( n = 27). Predictors with the fewest contributing effect sizes

    are the cultural region variables ( n = 2 each), literacy rates ( n = 2), female educationindicators ( n = 3), household size ( n = 3), unemployment ( n = 4), and theDecommodification Index ( n = 4). Overall, demographic, relative deprivation, anddevelopment perspectives are most often the subject of cross-national inquiry,whereas cultural region indicators, institutional anomie theory, deterrence, and politi-cal perspectives are the most infrequent.

    Strength of Effects

    Table 1 presents the mean effect sizes, contributing study sample, and 95% confi-dence intervals for each predictor. Table 1 shows that there remains a need for researchin many theoretical areas, as the majority of predictors have less than 10 contributingeffect sizes. Consequently, this adds tentativeness to interpretation.

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    15/30

    116 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    Table 1. Mean Effect Sizes () by Predictor Domain

    Predictordomain Predictor Study N Mr CIL CIU Q

    Absolutedeprivation

    Infant mortality 8 .196* .019 .362 27.406***

    Institutionalanomietheory

    Decommodification 4 .279** .415 .131 2.453

    Culture West 2 .108 .302 .094 0.573East 2 .326*** .484 .149 1.053Latin America 2 .445*** .255 .602 1.654

    Demographic Urbanism 27 .103 .049 .028 24.023***Population density 12 .012 .097 .074 6.104

    Population growth 9 .251** .080 .407 24.321Population total 16 .024 .101 .053 22.569Sex ratio 15 .095 .243 .058 60.413***Age > 15 17 .116*** .054 .177 23.509Age < 15 8 .100 .227 .030 2.360

    Deterrence 6 .072 .018 .160 5.393Relative

    deprivationIncome inequality

    (ratio)13 .416*** .339 .488 4.605

    Income inequality(index)

    31 .224*** .149 .297 80.626***

    Economicdiscrimination

    6 .136 .066 .328 15.282***

    Development Index 14 .163** .274 .047 28.124***Economic

    development37 .055 .137 .028 182.662***

    Modernization 9 .173 .344 .009 33.955***Female education 3 .110 .457 .614 20.650***Literacy 2 .219 .218 .583 2.354

    Politicalstructure

    Democracy indices 6 .012 .366 .346 153.777***Political rights 5 .088 .230 .056 4.520

    Routine

    activity

    Household size 3 .166 .317 .581 20.217***

    Female labor 13 .223* .027 .402 85.509***Unemployment 4 .043 .042 .128 5.175

    Socialdisorganization

    Ethnicheterogeneity

    12 .163** .046 .275 19.600

    Ethnic homogeneity 5 .247* .423 .052 13.750Divorce 10 .277*** .157 .389 2.261

    Social support 15 .072 .164 .020 29.785***

    Note: Mr = mean effect size correlation coefficient after weighting and transformations; CI

    L = 95% confidence interval

    lower limit; CIU = 95% confidence interval upper limit; Q = heterogeneity coefficient indicating distribution of effects

    around the mean (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). All analyses were performed on Fishers z values andsubsequently transformed back to r for presentation.*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    16/30

    Nivette 117

    Table 2. Rank-Ordered Mean Effect Sizes

    Rank Predictor M r Rank Predictor M r

    1 Latin American nation .445* 16 Economic discrimination .136 2 Income inequality (ratio) .416* 17 Age structure (age > 15) .116* 3 Eastern nation .326* 18 Female education (%) .110 4 Decommodification

    Index.279* 19 Western nation .109

    5 Divorce rate .277* 20 Urbanism .103 6 Population growth .251* 21 Age structure (age < 15) .100 7 Ethnic homogeneity .247* 22 Sex ratio .095 8 Income inequality

    (indices).224* 23 Political rights .088

    9 Female laborparticipation

    .223* 24 Social support .072

    10 Literacy (%) .219 25 Deterrence .07211 Infant mortality .197* 26 Economic development .05512 Modernization .173 27 Unemployment .04313 Household size .166 28 Population total .02414 Development index .163* 29 Democracy index .01215 Ethnic heterogeneity .163* 30 Population density .012

    Significant predictors (*p

  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    17/30

    118 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    Table 4. Generalized Least Squares Regression Results for Time and Space on Selected MeanEffect Size Predictors

    Predictor Time Sample size R2

    Income inequality (indices) .190 .321* .165Income inequality (ratio) .366 .620 .507

    Development index .073 .348 .095Economic development .228 .072 .054Social support .157 .162 .038Urbanism .008 .051 .003Population density .149 .169 .068Population total .298 .107 .065Sex ratio .321 .352 .166Age structure (age > 15) .495** .186 .230Female labor participation .143 .387 .183Ethnic heterogeneity .539 .877** .430Divorce .182 .793 .439

    Note: Standardized regression coefficients ( ) shown represent the relationship between methodologicalmoderators and mean effect sizes (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009).*p < .05. **p < .01.

    Table 3. Significant Methodological Variations Using the ANOVA Analogue for SelectedPredictors

    Moderator variable

    Predictor Peer-review a Data source ( df )

    Income inequality (indices) 0.023 4.190 (3)Income inequality (ratio) 0.085 2.612 (4)Development index 0.004 3.795 (3)Economic development 0.399 2.631 (3)Social support 7.413** 0.997 (2)Urbanism 0.196 3.788 (4)Population density n/a b 2.249 (3)

    Population total 0.170 2.217 (4)Sex ratio 0.357 8.267** (3)Age structure (age > 15) 0.037 3.728 (1)Female labor participation 1.311 12.044** (4)Ethnic heterogeneity 0.100 4.599 (2)Divorce 0.143 0.685 (3)

    Note: df = degrees of freedom. Numbers in the columns are the meta-analytic analogue to the ANOVA(Q) and are interpreted as the F statistic found in traditional ANOVA analyses.a. For all peer-reviewed results, df = 1.

    b. All studies including population density were peer reviewed.**p < .01.

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    18/30

    Nivette 119

    a wider range of societal change, displays a much stronger negative impact on homi-cide ( M

    r = .163, p < .01).

    Interestingly, two of the top three predictors when ranked are cultural region dum-

    mies (see Table 2). Latin American and Caribbean nations show a strong positiveeffect on homicide ( M r = .445, p < .001), whereas East Asian dummies have the con-

    verse effect ( M r = .326, p < .001).

    Methodological VariationsThe investigation of categorical moderating variables produced few significant results(see Table 3). This may be due to insufficient or unbalanced between-group samplesizes for most predictors. Assumptions of independence prevented predictors from

    being pooled for moderator analyses, which would have granted a sufficient sample. Nevertheless, predictors with more than 10 contributing studies were analyzed usingthe meta-analytic analogue to the ANOVA (Wilson, 2002).

    Only three predictors showed significant fluctuations between methodologicalvariations, and even so there appears to be no discernable pattern. The effects offemale labor and sex ratio were significantly influenced by the data source used ( Q = 12.043, p < .05 and Q = 8.266, p < .05, respectively); however, the specific groupeffects vary. For female labor, only Interpol and HEUNI/European Sourcebook datashow significant positive effects ( M

    Interpol = .305, p < .05, n = 4; M

    Other = .546, p < .05,

    n = 2). In comparison, the sex ratio predictor is only significant and negative whenusing WHO data ( M WHO

    = .204, p < .05, n = 10). Interestingly, the UN and Interpoldata report a positive effect between sex ratio and homicide, although these are notsignificant.

    Finally, effect sizes for social support were significantly different when divided bywhether they were peer reviewed. Here, peer-reviewed studies show more significantresults as expected due to the file-drawer problem ( M

    peer = .110, p < .05, n = 15).

    Importantly, the non-peer-reviewed effect sizes were positive but nearly significant( M

    nonpeer = .213, p = .053, n = 2), effectively bringing the overall mean effect size to

    nonsignificance.The findings reported here can only be considered tentative, and any significantdifferences must be viewed with caution. That measurement dependence is a signifi-cant factor for only two variables may be indicative of a wider problem in data reli-ability, or alternatively, as a reassuring sign that most effects are not significantlyaffected by how homicide is measured.

    Time and Sample Size

    Similar limitations apply to the meta-regression, although the varying results cansignify the dynamic properties of cross-national predictors. Table 4 presents the stan-dardized coefficients for data year (time) and sample size when regressed on selected

    predictors (i.e., those with 10 or more contributing effect sizes). Income inequality as

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    19/30

    120 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    measured by indices and ethnic heterogeneity were the only predictors significantlyrelated to sample size ( = .321, p < .05 and = .877, p < .01, respectively). Incomeinequality measured by ratios and divorce rate show a similar relationship ( = .620,

    p = .183 and = .793, p = .062, respectively) but do not reach significance.However, the effect of youth population (15 + ) appears to decrease through time ( = .495, p < .01).

    DiscussionCross-national research is a comparatively recent venture in the application of crimi-nological theory. The transference of theory on this larger unit of analysis has natu-rally occurred with the advancement of international data; however, the state of

    knowledge in this realm remains relatively disorganized. In particular, a focus onstructural predictors of crimeperhaps the easiest data to obtainhas obscured theinfluence of more complex sociocultural concepts such as Latin American machismo (Neapolitan, 1994), generalized trust (Halpern, 2001; Lederman et al., 2002), or reli-gious beliefs (Antonaccio & Tittle, 2007). On the large-n scale, these are difficultto reliably operationalize, and whether these concepts can or should be pursued on thislevel remains an open question. Many of the sociocultural predictors had too few con-tributing effect sizes to make accurate judgments about their worth in cross-nationalresearch. However, in contrast to the predictors that were most often included in

    designs (e.g., urbanism, economic development), sociocultural predictors showed thestrongest relationship with homicide. This signifies the need to reconsider whichsocial mechanisms are important at the national level.

    Individually, certain predictors such as income inequality and age structure demon-strate interesting results. Poverty, inequality, ethnic composition, youth populationabove age 15, and divorce are all well-known predictors that emerged strong and posi-tive as expected based on previous findings (Hsieh & Pugh, 1993; LaFree, 1999; Land,McCall, & Cohen, 1990). Income inequality, as measured by the Gini Index, rankedsomewhat lower than when measured by ratios, signifying a possible conceptual

    departure between the two predictors. Index-measured income inequality was also theonly predictor to be significantly affected by sample size, supporting doubts about therobustness and validity of the Gini Index made by cross-national scholars (Messner,Raffalovich, & Shrock, 2002; Neapolitan, 1997; Pratt & Godsey, 2003; Pridemore &Trent, 2010). However, ratio-based income inequality, although nonsignificant, appearedto be subject to differences in methodology, whereby half of the variation betweeneffect sizes is attributable to changes in time and sample size. The observed vulnera-

    bility of both indicators of income inequality to differences in contexts places thisusually robust predictor under scrutiny. As there are a decent amount of studies report-

    ing these strong effect sizes for income inequality and it is unlikely that every one ofthem has made a Type II error, it is reasonable to conjecture that income inequalityoperates differently outside the developed, industrialized nations that typically com-

    prise the core sample in comparative research. This does not dismiss the importanceof income inequality in explaining homicide, and indeed these findings indicate the

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    20/30

    Nivette 121

    need to control for such a strong predictor. Nevertheless, sample distribution, andsubsequently context, must be carefully considered in future models.

    Other predictors show considerable promise based on the size of their effects, includ-

    ing decommodification, regional indicators, human development indices, and adult lit-eracy. As a measure of institutional anomie theory, the Decommodification Index proves to be a significant influence on national homicide rates. However, when placedalongside the relatively weak effect of social support, the idea that protection frommarket forces via governmental aid will prevent homicide becomes dubious. The indexitself incorporates a measure of welfare expenditures, so it is unclear as to which mech-anisms are effectively operating in relation to homicide. Governmental support appearsto have very little effect on national homicide rates; however, some scholars suggestcertain types of aid may be more important in preventing frustration and aggression than

    others (Altheimer, 2008). Furthermore, these predictors only capture support provided by public means, whereas nongovernmental sources, such as family, community pro-grams, and religious organizations, may provide more effective means of social support(Chamlin & Cochran, 1997; Colvin et al., 2002). Therefore, future research should con-sider developing more precise indicators of private and public social support (see alsoPratt & Cullen, 2005), and disaggregate the Decommodification Index used to test insti-tutional anomie theory to identify the specific mechanisms at work.

    Regional predictors require some interpretation, as each could potentially representa number of characteristics (Pridemore, 2002). Neapolitan (1994) uses the Latin

    American and Caribbean dummy variable to capture values that permeate the regionand promote violent behavior, namely machismo . By way of providing historical con-text, however, Neapolitan gives way to alternative explanations that align more closelywith a political perspective. Characterized by a long history of revolution, politicalterrorism, and authoritarian leadership, many Latin American nations have been con-tinually exposed to legitimized official violence and political oppression (Ayres,1998). Furthermore, the strong presence of the drug trade throughout many of thesecountries presents additional outlets and opportunities for violence (United NationsOffice on Drugs and Crime, 2007). However, an East Asian region dummy is expected

    to have an inverse effect on homicide due to accompanying religious and culturalcharacteristics. Nations such as Japan gather significant scholarly attention in crimi-nology due to their seemingly unique maintenance of low crime rates in the face ofrapid social transformations (Leonardsen, 2004). Whether it is the communitarian val-ues conveyed through Islam and Confucianism (Antonaccio & Tittle, 2007), stronginformal social controls (Adler, 1983; Komiya, 1999), or a strong paternalistic govern-ment (Austin, 1987), the operation of social mechanisms in East Asian nations areconsidered anomalous to the West. While the significance of these cultural findings isdampened by the small amount of contributing studies, at the very least they further

    attest to the consequence of spatial context in cross-national research. Future researchshould aim to qualitatively and quantitatively develop each of these cultural mecha-nisms to precisely test them on the cross-national scale.

    Measures of development and modernization are some of the most frequently tested predictors in cross-national research but surprisingly demonstrate rather weak overall

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    21/30

    122 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    effects. Excepting education indicators, the general negative relationships support theopportunity-based theoretical explanations (Agha, 2009; Kick & LaFree, 1985;Shelley, 1981). Interestingly, only development measured by indices showed a signifi-

    cant effect, and even so a relatively small one. As the majority of studies used factoredindices as an indicator of development, the exact variables and hence precise relation-ships are difficult to decipher. Development indices generally tend to represent a com-

    bination of societal and economic levels of development through indicators such aseducation enrollment, human rights scales, GNP/GDP per capita, population growth,or infant mortality. Individually, these predictors portray a very different picture thanwhen they are combined: infant mortality, population growth, and education indica-tors show medium positive relationships whereas economic and political rights indica-tors show small negative relationships. Which predictor is the greater influence?

    Which is more theoretically and empirically important? Some criminologists arguethat lumping together social processes and covariates merely obscures causality andhinders interpretation (Cook, Shagle, & Degirmencioglu, 1997; Land et al., 1990;Sampson, 2006b). Development is a highly complex and longitudinal concept thatdefies one-dimensional operationalizations (Inglehart & Welzel, 2005; LaFree &Tseloni, 2006). Thus the problem here is essentially a theoretical one. Alternatively, asSampson (2006b) explains, [n]o statistical method can solve what is fundamentally atheoretical issue about causal mechanisms (p. 52).

    It is important to note that economic developmentincluded in almost all of the

    sample studiesproved to be consistently immaterial to the explanation of homicide.This result, combined with the confusion in the theoretical literature, perhaps indicatesthat it is time to move past the use of this variable in cross-national designs. Not sur-

    prisingly, it appears that the distribution of wealth in a society is more important in predicting homicide rates than total or per capita measures of wealth (see Land et al.,1990). Thus if economic development is to remain in cross-national discourse, research-ers must investigate how political and cultural factors may influence the uneven spreadof wealth in societies.

    Other commonly included demographic variables show similar ineffectual pat-

    terns: urbanism, population density, population total, youth population less than age15, and sex ratio. Although these are often used as control variables, the general weak-ness of these predictors carries implications for the Durkheimian-Modernization,social disorganization, and routine activity theories as well. In each of these per-spectives, there is the hypothesis that the close proximity of youth (primarily males)will increase crime. Although nonsignificant and generally negligible, the meaneffects are all negatively related to homicide. However, this must be consideredwith caution, as it is equally likely these predictors are simply irrelevant to homicideat the national level. For population density, population total and youth population

    below 15, the weak findings were robust across methodological variations, indicat-ing that these can be safely excluded from future cross-national designs withoutconsequence.

    Interestingly, the somewhat small effect of youth population aged above 15 is aug-mented by its susceptibility to time. The significant decrease in the effect of the size of

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    22/30

    Nivette 123

    youth population as data become more recent bares consequence to the proclaimedinvariance of the agecrime curve (see Hirschi & Gottfredson, 1983). The direct effectof the agecrime relationship is not disputed here, as a generous amount of studies con-

    firm a positive effect. However, the strength of the association appears to have declinedwith time. Speculative reasoning may suggest that the impact of age composition mayrely on time-specific, often exogenous, factors. Alternatively, with the advance of newsources of indicator data, influences that were previously unaccounted for are now beingincluded in cross-national research, portraying a more accurate model of homicide.

    Democracy indices proved rather weak influences on homicide overall, though thismay be attributable to the lack of empirical and theoretical development of the specificmechanisms involved (Stamatel, 2008). The large body of emerging research on crimein post-Communist nations suggests that processes of democratization are dependent

    on political culture and economic conditions (e.g., Karstedt, 2003; Stamatel, 2008).Thus even within a region undergoing similar democratic transitions, the impact onhomicide is not always the same. Furthermore, LaFree and Tseloni (2006) stress theuse of the word processes , arguing that democracy, like modernization, development,and globalization, is a longitudinal concept that must be examined over time. Thereforemore sociohistorical comparative research that focuses on a particular region, as in the

    post-Communist example above, is needed to better understand how the change todemocracy interacts with cultural institutions and values (Karstedt, 2003).

    ConclusionsThis meta-analysis confirmed the importance of cross-national predictors that repre-sent levels of social integration and stability in explaining homicide. Certain predic-tors, such as population density, economic development, and deterrence, show verylittle impact on national-level homicide and must be carefully reevaluated if includedin future research. However, it also confirmed concerns about theoretical and meth-odological limitations to cross-national research (Marshall & Block, 2004; Marshallet al., 2009; Stamatel, 2006; Van Dijk, 2008), namely, the following:

    Samples are small and biased. On average, only about 20% of the global population of countries are covered, of which the overwhelming majority are primarily developed and industrialized.

    Indicators are theoretically vague, with considerable empirical overlap andno agreement on operationalization.

    Time is nearly nonexistent in cross-national research, as designs predomi-nantly favor cross-sectional over longitudinal.

    These limitations require future research to incorporate predictors into models withcareful theoretical consideration toward disentangling overlapping covariates and samplecomposition. Theories that rely on factored indices, proxy indicators, and highly correlatedvariables in particular must clarify the mechanisms involved to sort out empiricaldiscrepancies. Overall, the overwhelming use of cross-sectional design to test the

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    23/30

  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    24/30

    Nivette 125

    Acknowledgements

    The author would like to thank the PhD writing group at the Institute of Criminology, her

    supervisor Prof. Manuel Eisner, and three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments onan earlier draft of this article.

    Declaration of Conflicting Interests

    The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship,and/or publication of this article.

    Funding

    The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of

    this article.

    Notes

    1. Individual journals searched were Criminology , Homicide Studies , European Journal ofCriminology , Asian Journal of Criminology , British Journal of Criminology , Interna-tional Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice , and International Journal ofOffender Therapy and Comparative Criminology .

    2. Effect sizes reported in unstandardized b-coefficient format were standardized using thefollowing equation: = b x

    y

    SD

    SD

    .Where SD x and SD y are the standard deviations of the indepen-

    dent and dependent variable, respectively, and b is the reported unstandardized regressioncoefficient.

    3. It is important to note that the grouping of certain predictors with theories is rather tenta-tive. For instance, infant mortality has only recently been used as an indicator for absolutedeprivation on the cross-national level (Pridemore, 2008), having previously representedconcepts of social inequality (Lee & Bankston, 1999), health care (Shichor, 1990), andsocial disorganization (Jacobs & Richardson, 2008). The following associations betweentheories and predictors follow major arguments in the literature, with conceptual overlap to

    be examined in the discussion.

    4. One study included a Poverty Index (Par, 2006), but due to significant heterogeneity and alack of similar operationalizations, this effect size was excluded.

    5. Gartners (1990, 1991) cultural predictor Battle Deaths had to be excluded due to heruse of nearly identical samples. Unfortunately, measures of American culture wereexcluded because these studies did not employ standardized regression coefficients(Chamlin & Cochran, 2007; Jensen, 2002) or did not have a large-enough sample size(Cao, 2004). In addition, Chamlin and Cochrans (2006) study on perceptions of politi -cal legitimacy was excluded because they used negative binomial regression with met-ric coefficients.

    6. This Q statistic is mathematically equivalent to the Q statistic used to examine the over-all dispersion of effect sizes around the mean effect. However, the former is modifiedto assess between-subgroup differences (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein,2009).

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    25/30

    126 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    References

    Adler, F. (1983). Nations not obsessed with crime . Littleton, CO: Fred B. Rothman.

    Agha, S. (2009). Structural correlates of female homicide: A cross-national analysis. Journal ofCriminal Justice, 37 , 576-585.

    Altheimer, I. (2007). Assessing the relevance of ethnic heterogeneity as a predictor of homicideat the cross-national level. International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice,31, 1-20.

    Altheimer, I. (2008). Social support, ethnic heterogeneity, and homicide: A cross-nationalapproach . Journal of Criminal Justice, 36 , 103-114.

    Antonaccio, O., & Tittle, C. R. (2007). A cross-national test of Bongers theory of criminalityand economic conditions. Criminology, 45 , 925-958.

    Austin, W. T. (1987). Crime and custom in an orderly society: The Singapore prototype. Crimi-nology, 25 , 279-294.

    Austin, R. L., & Kim, Y. S. (1999). Educational development and homicide in Sub-SaharanAfrica. Homicide Studies, 3 , 78-98.

    Avison, W. R., & Loring, P. L. (1986). Population diversity and cross-national homicide: Theeffects of inequality and heterogeneity. Criminology, 24 , 733-749.

    Ayres, R. L. (1998). Crime and violence as development issues in Latin America and the Carib-bean . Washington, DC: The World Bank.

    Babones, S. J. (2008). Income inequality and population health: Correlation and causality.

    Social Science and Medicine, 66 , 1614-1626.Barber, N. (2004). Single parenthood as a predictor of cross-national variation in violent crime.

    Cross-Cultural Research, 38 , 343-358.Barber, N. (2009). Countries with fewer males have more violent crime: Marriage markets and

    mating aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 35 , 49-56.Bennett, R. R. (1991). Development and crime: A cross-national, time-series analysis of com-

    peting models. Sociological Quarterly, 32 , 343-363.Bennett, R. R. (2004). Comparative criminology and criminal justice research: The state of our

    knowledge. Justice Quarterly, 21 , 1-21.

    Bennett, R. R., & Bennett, S. B. (1983). Police personnel levels and the incidence of crime:A cross-national investigation. Criminal Justice Review, 8 , 32-40.

    Bjerregaard, B., & Cochran, J. K. (2008). A cross-national test of institutional anomie theory:Do the strength of other social institutions mediate or moderate the effects of the economyon the rate of crime? Western Criminology Review, 9 , 31-48.

    Braithwaite, J. (1989). The state of criminology: Theoretical decay or renaissance. Australiaand New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 22 , 129-135.

    Braithwaite, J., & Braithwaite, V. (1980). The effect of income inequality and social democracyon homicide. British Journal of Criminology, 20 , 45-53.

    Britt, C. L., & Weisburd, D. (2010). Statistical power. In A. R. Piquero & D. Weisburd (Eds.), Handbook of quantitative criminology (pp. 313-332). London, UK: Springer.

    Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction tometa-analysis . Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    26/30

    Nivette 127

    Butchart, A., & Engstrm, K. (2002). Sex- and age-specific relations between economic devel-opment, economic inequality and homicide rates in people aged 0-24 years: A cross-sectionalanalysis. Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 80 , 797-805.

    Cao, L. (2004). Is America more anomic? A test of Mertons theory with cross-national data. International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice, 28 , 17-31.

    Chamlin, M. B., & Cochran, J. K. (1997). Social altruism and crime. Criminology, 35 , 203-228.Chamlin, M. B., & Cochran, J. K. (2006). Economic inequality, legitimacy, and cross-national

    homicide rates. Homicide Studies, 10 , 231-252.Chamlin, M. B., & Cochran, J. K. (2007). An evaluation of the assumption that underlie institu-

    tional anomie theory. Theoretical Criminology, 11 , 39-61.Clark, R. (1989). Cross-national perspectives on female crime: An empirical investigation.

    International Journal of Comparative Sociology, 30 , 195-215.

    Cohen, L. E., & Felson, M. (1979). Social change and crime rate trends: A routine activityapproach. American Sociological Review, 44 , 588-608.

    Colvin, M., Cullen, F. T., & Ven, T. V. (2002). Coercion, social support, and crime: An emerg-ing theoretical consensus. Criminology, 40 , 19-42.

    Cook, T. D., Shagle, S. C., & Degirmencioglu, S. M. (1997). Capturing social processes fortesting mediational models of neighborhood effects. In J. Brooks-Gunn, G. Duncan,& L. Aber (Eds.), Neighborhood poverty Vol. 2: Policy implications in studying neighbor-hoods (pp. 94-119). New York, NY: Russell Sage.

    Cooper, H. (2010). Research synthesis and meta-analysis: A step-by-step approach (4th ed.).

    Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.DiCristina, B. (2004). Durkheims theory of homicide and the confusion of the empirical litera-

    ture . Theoretical Criminology, 8 , 57-91.Dugan, L. (2010). Estimating effects over time for single and multiple units. In A. R. Piquero

    & D. Weisburd (Eds.), Handbook of quantitative criminology (pp. 741-763). London, UK:Springer.

    Evans, T. D., LaGrange, R. L., & Willis, C. L. (1996). Theoretical development of compara-tive criminology: Rekindling an interest. International Journal of Comparative and AppliedCriminal Justice, 20 , 15-29.

    Gartner, R. (1990). The victims of homicide: A temporal and cross-national perspective. AmericanSociological Review, 55 , 92-106.

    Gartner, R. (1991). Family structure, welfare spending, and child homicide in developed democ-racies. Journal of Marriage and Family, 53 , 231-240.

    Groves, W. B., McCleary, R., & Newman, G. R. (1985). Religion, modernization, and worldcrime. In R. F. Tomasson (Ed.), Comparative social research (Vol. 8; pp. 59-80). London,UK: JAI Press.

    Halpern, D. (2001). Moral values, social trust and inequality: Can values explain crime? British Journal of Criminology, 41 , 236-251.

    Hansmann, H. B., & Quigley, J. M. (1982). Population heterogeneity and the sociogenesis ofhomicide. Social Forces, 61 , 206-224.

    Hartnagel, T. F., & Mizanuddin, M. (1986). Modernization, gender role convergence and femalecrime: A further test. International Journal of Comparative Sociology, 27 , 1-14.

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    27/30

    128 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    Hartnagel, T. F. (1982). Modernization, female social roles, and female crime: A cross-nationalinvestigation. Sociological Quarterly, 23 , 477-490.

    He, N., Cao, L., Wells, W., & Maguire, E. R. (2003). Forces of production and direction: A test

    of an expanded model of suicide and homicide. Homicide Studies, 7 , 36-57.Hirschi, T., & Gottfredson, M. (1983). Age and the explanation of crime . American Journal of

    Sociology, 89 , 552-584.Howard, G. J., & Smith, T. R. (2003). Understanding cross-national variations of crime rates in

    Europe and North America. In K. Aromaa, S. Leppa, S. Nevala, & N. Ollus (Eds.), Crimeand criminal justice in Europe and North America 1995-1997: Report on the sixth United

    Nations survey on crime trends and criminal justice systems (pp. 23-70). Helsinki, Finland:European Institute for Crime Prevention and Control.

    Hsieh, C., & Pugh, M. D. (1993). Poverty, income inequality, and violent crime: A meta-analysis

    of recent aggregate data studies. Criminal Justice Review, 18 , 182-202.Huang, W. S. W. (1995). A cross-national analysis on the effect of moral individualism on murder

    rates. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 39 , 63-75.Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in

    research findings (2nd ed.). London, UK: SAGE.Inglehart, R., & Welzel, C. (2005). Modernization, cultural change and democracy: The human

    development sequence . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Jacobs, D., & Richardson, A. M. (2008). Economic inequality and homicide in the developed

    nations from 1975 to 1995. Homicide Studies, 12 , 28-45.

    Jensen, G. F. (2001). Religion and lethal violence: Unraveling Durkheims mystery. InM. D. Smith, P. H. Blackman, & J. P. Jarvis (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2001 annual meetingof the Homicide Research Working Group (pp. 159-176). Washington, DC: Federal Bureauof Investigation.

    Jensen, G. F. (2002). Institutional anomie and societal variations in crime: A critical appraisal. International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy, 22 , 45-74.

    Karstedt, S. (2001). Comparing cultures, comparing crime: Challenges, prospects and problemsfor global criminology. Crime, Law & Social Change, 36 , 285-308.

    Karstedt, S. (2003). Legacies of a culture of inequality: The Janus face of crime in post-communist

    countries. Crime, Law & Social Change, 40 , 295-320.Kick, E. L., & LaFree, G. (1985). Development and the social context of murder and theft. In

    R. F. Tomasson (Ed.), Comparative social research (Vol. 8; pp. 37-58). London, UK: JAI Press.Krahn, H., Hartnagel, T. F., & Gartrell, J. W. (1986). Income inequality and homicide rates:

    Cross-national data and criminological theories. Criminology, 24 , 269-295.Komiya, N. (1999). A cultural study of the low crime rate in Japan. British Journal of Criminology,

    39, 369-390.Krohn, M. D. (1978). A Durkheimian analysis of international crime rates. Social Forces, 57 ,

    654-670.

    Krohn, M. D., & Wellford, C. F. (1977). Static and dynamic analysis of crime and the primarydimensions of nations. International Journal of Criminology and Penology, 5 , 1-16.

    LaFree, G. & Kick, E.L. (1986). Cross-national effects of developmental, distributional, anddemographic variables on crime: A review and analysis. International Annals of Criminology,24, 213-235.

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    28/30

    Nivette 129

    LaFree, G. (1999). A summary and review of cross-national comparative homicide. InM. D. Smith & M. A. Zahn (Eds.), Homicide: A sourcebook of social research (pp. 125-145) .London, UK: SAGE.

    LaFree, G., & Tseloni, A. (2006). Democracy and crime: A multilevel analysis of homicidetrends in forty-four countries, 1950-2000. The Annals of the American Academy of Politicaland Social Science, 605 , 25-49.

    Land, K. C., McCall, P. L., & Cohen, L. E. (1990). Structural covariates of homicide rates: Arethere any invariances across time and social space? American Journal of Sociology, 95 ,922-963.

    Lederman, D., Loayza, N., & Menendez, A. M. (2002). Violent crime: Does social capital mat-ter? Economic Development and Cultural Change, 50 , 509-539.

    Lee, M. R., & Bankston, W. B. (1999). Political structure, economic inequality, and homicide:

    A cross-national analysis. Deviant Behavior, 20 , 27-55.Lee, M. R. (2001). Population growth, economic inequality, and homicide. Deviant Behavior,

    22, 491-516.Leonardsen, D. (2004). Japan as a low-crime nation . New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.Li, D. (1995). Economic development, social control, and murder rates: A cross-national

    approach. Cross-Cultural Research, 29 , 361-382.Lin, M. (2007). Does democracy increase crime? The evidence from international data. Journal

    of Comparative Economics, 35 , 467-483.Littell, J. H., Corcoran, J., & Pillai, V. (2008). Systematic reviews and meta-analysis . Oxford,

    UK: Oxford University Press.Marshall, I. H., & Block, C. R. (2004). Maximizing the availability of cross-national data on

    homicide. Homicide Studies, 8 , 267-310.Marshall, I. H., & Marshall, C. E. (1983). Toward a refinement of purpose in comparative crimi-

    nological research: Research site selection in focus. International Journal of Comparativeand Applied Criminal Justice, 7 , 89-97.

    Marshall, I. H., Marshall, C. E., & Ren, L. (2009). Mixed method measurement of homicideevents in comparative research: An illustration of the potential of qualitative comparativeanalysis. International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice, 33 , 273-307.

    McAlister, A. L. (2006). Acceptance of killing and homicide rates in nineteen nations. European Journal of Public Health, 16 , 259-265.

    Messner, S.F. (1982). Societal development, social equality, and homicide: A cross-national testof a Durkheimian model. Social Forces, 61 , 225-240.

    Messner, S. F. (1985). Sex differences in arrest rates for homicide: An application of the generaltheory of structural strain. In R. F. Tomasson (Ed.), Comparative social research (Vol. 8;

    pp. 187-202). London, UK: JAI Press.Messner, S. F. (1989). Economic discrimination and societal homicide rates: Further evidence

    on the cost of inequality. American Sociological Review, 54 , 597-611.

    Messner, S. F., Raffalovich, L. E., & Shrock, P. (2002). Reassessing the cross-national relation-ship between income inequality and homicide rates: Implications of data quality control inthe measurement of income distribution. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 18 , 377-395.

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    29/30

    130 Homicide Studies 15(2)

    Messner, S. F., & Rosenfeld, R. (1997). Political restraint of the market and levels of criminal homi-cide: A cross-national application of institutional anomie theory. Social Forces, 75 , 1393-1416.

    Neapolitan, J. L. (1994). Cross-national variation in homicides: The case of Latin America.

    International Criminal Justice Review, 4 , 4-22. Neapolitan, J. L. (1997). Cross-national crime: A research review and sourcebook . Westport,

    CT: Greenwood. Neapolitan, J. L. (1998). Cross-national variation in homicides: Is race a factor? Criminology,

    36 , 139-156.Pampel, F. C., & Gartner, R. (1995). Age structure, socio-political institutions, and national

    homicide rates. European Sociological Review, 11 , 243-260.Par, P. (2006). Income inequality and crime across nations reexamined (Doctoral dissertation).

    Pennsylvania State University, University Park.

    Pratt, T. C., & Cullen, F. T. (2005). Assessing macro-level predictors and theories of crime: Ameta-analysis. Crime and Justice, 32 , 373-450.

    Pratt, T. C., Cullen, F. T., Blevins, K. R., Daigle, L. E., & Madensen, T. D. (2006). The empiricalstatus of deterrence theory: A meta-analysis. In F. T. Cullen, J. P. Wright, & K. R. Blevins(Eds.), Taking stock: The status of criminological theory (pp. 367-396). New Brunswick,

    NJ: Transaction.Pratt, T. C., & Godsey, T. W. (2002). Social support and homicide: A cross-national test of an

    emerging criminological theory. Journal of Criminal Justice, 30 , 589-601.Pratt, T. C., & Godsey, T. W. (2003). Social support, inequality, and homicide: A cross-national

    test of an integrated theoretical model. Criminology, 41 , 611-643.Pridemore, W. A. (2002). What we know about social structure and homicide: A review of the

    theoretical and empirical literature. Violence and Victims, 17 , 127-156.Pridemore, W. A. (2008). A methodological addition to the cross-national empirical literature

    on social structure and homicide: A first test of the poverty-homicide thesis. Criminology,46 , 133-154.

    Pridemore, W.A. & Trent, C.L.S. (2010). Do the invariant findings of Land, McCall, and Cohengeneralize to cross-national studies of social structure and homicide? Homicide Studies, 14 ,296-335.

    Rahav, G. (1983). Homicide and death penalty: A cross-sectional time series analysis. Interna-tional Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice, 7 , 61-71.

    Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file-drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86 , 638-641.

    Sampson, R. J. (2006a). Collective efficacy theory: Lessons learned and directions for futureinquiry. In F. T. Cullen, J. P. Wright, & K. R. Blevins (Eds.), Taking stock: The status ofcriminological theory (Vol. 15; pp. 149-167). London, UK: Transaction.

    Sampson, R. J. (2006b). How does the community context matter? Social mechanisms andexplanations of crime rates. In P. H. Wikstrm & R. J. Sampson (Eds.), The explanation

    of crime: Context, mechanisms and development (pp. 31-60). Cambridge, UK: CambridgeUniversity Press.

    Savage, J., Bennett, R. R., & Danner, M. (2008). Economic assistance and crime: A cross-national investigation. European Journal of Criminology, 5 , 217-238.

    at University of Bucharest on August 5, 2014hsx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/http://hsx.sagepub.com/
  • 8/10/2019 Homicide Studies 2011 Nivette 103 31

    30/30

    Nivette 131

    Savolainen, J. (2000). Inequality, welfare state, and homicide: Further support for the institu-tional anomie theory. Criminology, 38 , 1021-1042.

    Shaw, C. R., & McKay, H. D. (1942). Juvenile delinquency in urban areas . Chicago, IL:

    University of Chicago Press.Shelley, L. I. (1981). Crime and modernization: The impact of industrialization and urbaniza-

    tion on crime . Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.Shichor, D. (1990). Crime patterns and socioeconomic development: A cross-national analysis.

    Criminal Justice Review, 15 , 64-78.Stamatel, J. P. (2006). Incorporating socio-historical context into quantitative cross-national crimi-

    nology. International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice, 30 , 177-207.Stamatel, J. P. (2008). Using mortality data to refine our understanding of homicide patterns in

    select postcommunist countries. Homicide Studies, 12 , 117-135.

    Stamatel, J. P. (2009a). Contributions of cross-national research to criminology at the beginningof the 21st century. In M. D. Krohn, A. J. Lizotte, & G. P. Hall (Eds.), Handbook on crimeand deviance . London, UK: Springer. 3-22.

    Stamatel, J. P. (2009b). Correlates of national-level homicide variation in post-CommunistEast-Central Europe. Social Forces, 87 , 1423-1448.

    Sun, I. Y., Sung, H., & Chu, D. C. (2007). Collateral gains from the military? A cross-nationalanalysis of the armed forces crime relationship. International Journal of Offender Therapyand Comparative Criminology, 51 , 599-614.

    Sung, H. (2006). Democracy and criminal justice in cross-national perspective: From crime

    control to due process. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science,605 , 311-337.

    Unnithan, N. P., & Whitt, H. P. (1992). Inequality, economic development and lethal violence:A cross-national analysis of suicide and homicide. International Journal of ComparativeSociology, 33 , 182-196.

    United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime. (2007). Crime, violence, and development: Trends,costs, and policy options in the Caribbean (Report No. 38720). Vienna, Austria: Author.

    Van Dijk, J. (2008). The world of crime: Breaking the silence on problems of security, justice,and development across the world . London, UK: SAGE.

    Van Wilsem, J. (2004). Criminal victimization in cross-national perspective: An analysis ofrates of theft, violence and vandalism across 27 countries. European Journal of Criminol-ogy, 1 , 89-109.

    Wells, E. (2009). Uses of meta-analysis in criminal justice research: A quantitative review. Justice Quarterly, 26 , 268-294.

    Wilson, D. B. (2001). Meta-analytic methods for criminology. Annals of the American Academyof Political and Social Science, 578 , 71-89.

    Wilson, D. B. (2002). Meta-analysis macros for SAS, SPSS, and Stata . Retrieved from http://mason.gmu.edu/~dwilsonb/ma.html

    Bio

    Amy E. Nivette is a PhD candidate at the Institute of Criminology at the University ofCambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom.