fyp-rafia

140
FINAL YEAR PROJECT “FACTORS OF POST-PURCHASE DISSONANCE IN FEMALES AFTER A HAIRCUT” Submitted By: RAFIA ALVI BBA-SP-06-3197 Supervised By: MR. RAJA RUB NAWAZ DATE: 7 th May, 2010 PAKISTAN AIR FORCE

Upload: sidra-javed

Post on 06-Apr-2015

170 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: FYP-Rafia

FINAL YEAR PROJECT

“FACTORS OF POST-PURCHASE DISSONANCE

IN FEMALES AFTER A HAIRCUT”

Submitted By:

RAFIA ALVI

BBA-SP-06-3197

Supervised By:

MR. RAJA RUB NAWAZ

DATE: 7th May, 2010

PAKISTAN AIR FORCEKARACHI INSTITUTE OF ECONOMICS & TECHNOLOGY

THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF BACHELORS OF BUSINESS ADMINISTRATION

Page 2: FYP-Rafia

FINAL PROJECT

“ FACTORS OF POST-PURCHASE DISSONANCE

IN FEMALES AFTER A HAIRCUT ”

Page 3: FYP-Rafia

THESIS APPROVAL

Thesis Title: “Factors of post-purchase dissonance in females after a haircut”

By: RAFIA ALVI - 3197

Thesis Supervisor: Mr. Raja Rub Nawaz

Academic Year: 2010

The Board of Advanced Studies at PAF-KIET has approved this thesis, submitted in partial

fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of bachelors of Business Administration.

Approval Committee:

______________________ _____________________ Mr. RAJA RUB NAWAZ Mr. TARIQ JALEES

(Supervisor) (Director Academics)

Page 4: FYP-Rafia

LETTER OF AUTHORIZATION

May 7th, 2010

RAFIA ALVI

Reg. # 3197

Dear Student,

I am pleased to inform you that you are assigned to conduct a study and prepare a report on

“Factors of post-purchase dissonance in females after a haircut ". In this report you

have to find the major underlying negative factors leading to post-purchase dissonance in

females after having a haircut.

You will get all the assistance, necessary for the information. If you find any difficulty in

your way, please try to overcome those obstacles. I assure you that all possible cooperation

will be offered to enable you to write the report in a good and cooperative environment.

I will appreciate if you submit your findings within given time.

Mr. Raja Rub Nawaz(Supervisor)

Page 5: FYP-Rafia

LETTER OF TRANSMITTAL

May 7th, 2010

Mr. Raja Rub Nawaz

Faculty Member

College Of Management Sciences

PAF KIET (CITY CAMPUS)

Dear Sir,

I am submitting this report on “Factors of post-purchase dissonance in females after a

haircut”. As per your advice, the report includes objective of the study, a brief literature

review, methodology and the research framework. Altogether, 26 negative factors were

collected and tested from which I was able to draw 6 major latent negative factors leading to

dissatisfaction in females after a haircut.

I am grateful for your guidance and support without which this project could not have been

completed.

Sincerely,

RAFIA ALVI

BBA-SP-06-3197

PAF- Karachi Institute of Economics & Technology

Page 6: FYP-Rafia

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

I would like to thank Allah for being my pillar of strength and for His guidance through the

years of my studies. I would also like to thank Him for making it possible for me to reach

this far, for if it was not for Him this would not have been possible. I would like to

acknowledge the help provided by my supervisor to make this project successful.

I wish to express my deepest gratitude to Mr. Raja Rub Nawaz, lecturer at PAF- KIET

for his valuable guidance and support, without which this project could not have been

completed.

Finally, I thank PAF- KIET for providing me a truly inspiring and conductive environment

for studies.

RAFIA ALVIBBA-SP-06-3197

Page 7: FYP-Rafia

DECLARATION

I, Rafia Alvi, hereby declare that the work represented in this dissertation represents my

own work and findings except where indicated and that all references, to the best of my

knowledge are accurately reported.

Rafia Alvi

Page 8: FYP-Rafia

Introduction..................................................................................................................................01

Objective.......................................................................................................................................03

Literature Review........................................................................................................................04

Methodology.................................................................................................................................45

Research Framework...................................................................................................................48

Data Analysis................................................................................................................................49

Factor Analysis……...……………………………………………………………………………49

Principal Component Analysis…………………………………………………...……49

Total Variance Explained……………………………………………………...………50

Scree Plot…………………………………………………………………………...….51

Factor/Component Matrix…………………………………………………………......52

Rotated Factor Matrix………………………………………………………………….54

Factor Naming…………………………………………………………………………56

Frequency Analysis…………………………………………………………………………….…58

Conclusion....................................................................................................................................84

Annexure I - Questionnaire.........................................................................................................85

Annexure II - References.............................................................................................................88

Table of Contents

Page 9: FYP-Rafia

1.0.0 INTRODUCTION

A good hairstyle is the most sought after thing in the realm of beauty in these modern days.

Almost every woman wants to flaunt her beauty with a good haircut or a hairstyle. The right

hairstyle can turn out to be the crowning glory of one's beauty or otherwise can turn your

looks down; a testing period until those cut ends gathers to give you a normal look. Your

head is not testing ground to just venture into experimenting with any haircut according to

your whims and fancies.

Hairstyle plays a very important role; it adds oomph to your personality and can instantly

change the look. However, some people get it wrong and end up looking like a fashion

disaster.

Not every woman can be physically beautiful. We women all know, and lament, this fact.

But almost all of us have the ability to grow long, feminine, beautiful hair, something that is

not only rewarded and encouraged as a sensual, beautiful thing by men but also something

that doesn't require genetic good luck or unreasonable effort to attain. With one simple swirl

of our long shiny locks, we can feel attractive, confident, playful, young and vibrant, even if

nature didn't endow us with good looks. We know that beautiful hair makes even the

plainest woman instantly more attractive to men and that one dollop of sweet smelling

shampoo and a loose cascade of touchable, shiny hair over the shoulders makes our men go

weak in the knees.

For females 'Hair is an adornment as much as a necklace or a bracelet'. Once

females are motivated to get a stylish haircut to feel happy they establish certain

expectations and based on certain standards they have established in their own minds, they

evaluate the service provided. If the service does not meet their evaluation criteria, they may

experience cognitive dissonance in the form of post purchase doubt and concern or anxiety

about the wisdom of the purchase of haircut service.

Page 10: FYP-Rafia

It is most likely to occur among females with a tendency to experience anxiety, after an

irrevocable purchase, when the purchase was important to the consumer, and when it

involved a difficult choice between two or more alternatives. An important role of the hair

dresser is to help females cope with dissonance by reinforcing the wisdom of their purchase

decision.

Cognitive dissonance has been defined as psychological discomfort (Carlsmith & Aronson,

1963; Elliot & Devine, 1994), a psychologically uncomfortable state (Festinger, 1957;

Menasco & Hawkins, 1978), being linked with anxiety, uncertainty or doubt (Menasco «&

Hawkins, 1978; Montgomery & Barnes, 1993; Mowen, 1995) or as synonymous with the

regret or remorse reported in salespeople's anecdotes (Insko & Schopler, 1972). It is

apparent that cognitive dissonance has both cognitive and psychological components.

Page 11: FYP-Rafia

1.1.0 OBECTIVE

The objective of the research was to find the underlying factors that lead to dissonance in

females after having a haircut to help them recognize the major reasons which result in

dissatisfaction because the emotions a woman feels when things aren't going well are almost

as unpleasant as what she feels when she just can't get her hair to look nice or lie flat or

conform to the style she wants. And conversely, you'll never see confident personality and

buoyant self esteem more magnificently displayed than in a woman who has striking hair

and knows it. A woman's emotions are more closely linked to her hair than to any other part

of her, as any woman who seeks out a wig after chemotherapy will tell you.

The importance of the topic can be deduced from the fact that we spend thousands of rupees

a year on shampoos, conditioners, hair dyes, highlights, trims, perms, hair accessories, and

hair salons for the pleasure it gives us to make ourselves as beautiful as possible, but mostly

to illicit that turning head or that smile of delight on the faces of people who take a glance at

us. But, if dissonance is experienced after a haircut, it has the impact of the biblical figure,

Samson who lost his power when Delilah cut off his hair.

This research study will assist in providing the latent factors and will eventually motivate

females to lookout in future for the factors causing post-purchase dissonance, because hair,

for almost every woman, represents femininity and beauty, and sometimes, therefore, what

she thinks of herself. When you deal with these aspects of a woman, emotions naturally run

high.

Page 12: FYP-Rafia

2.0.0 LITERATURE REVIEW

2.1.0 INTRODUCTION

Once customers or prospects are motivated to satisfy their needs and wants, the purchase

process begins. Based on certain standards they have established in their own minds, they

evaluate various alternative products or services. If none of the alternatives meets their

evaluation criteria, they may experience cognitive dissonance in the form of post purchase

doubt and concern or anxiety about the wisdom of the purchase. This is known as post

purchase dissonance/cognitive dissonance. It is most likely to occur among individuals with

a tendency to experience anxiety, after an irrevocable purchase, when the purchase was

important to the consumer, and when it involved a difficult choice between two or more

alternatives. An important role of marketers is to help people cope with dissonance by

reinforcing the wisdom of their purchase decision.

When customers feel abandoned, cognitive dissonance surfaces and repeat sales decline.

Today this issue is more pertinent than ever because customers are far less loyal to brands

and sellers than in the past, because buyers are more inclined to look for the best deal,

especially in the case of poor after-the-sale follow-up. More and more buyers favor building

a relationship with sellers. The literature review includes the literature on cognitive

dissonance domain, post purchase behavior, post purchase evaluation and the purchase

process for services, cognitive consistency theories and methods of reducing dissonant

cognitions.

How do human beings make decisions? What triggers a person to take action at any given

point? These are all questions that I will attempt to answer with my theoretical research into

Leon Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance, as well as many of the other related

theories.

Page 13: FYP-Rafia

The purpose of this chapter is to consider the theory behind the problem that is being

researched, that is, to investigate the effects of cognitive dissonance on customers. It will

thus serve to identify the theoretical basis of the study.

2.2.0 THE COGNITIVE DISSONANCE DOMAIN

Almost half a century ago social psychologist Leon Festinger developed the cognitive

dissonance theory (Festinger, 1957).

Cognitive dissonance theory is based on three fundamental assumptions (see Figure 1).

1. Humans are sensitive to inconsistencies between actions and beliefs.

According to the theory, we all recognize, at some level, when we are acting in a

way that is inconsistent with our beliefs/attitudes/opinions. In effect, there is a built

in alarm that goes off when we notice such an inconsistency, whether we like it or

not. For example, if you have a belief that it is wrong to cheat, yet you find yourself

cheating on a test, you will notice and be affected by this inconsistency.

2. Recognition of this inconsistency will cause dissonance, and will motivate an

individual to resolve the dissonance.

Once you recognize that you have violated one of your principles, according to this

theory; you won’t just say “oh well”. You will feel some sort of mental anguish

about this. The degree of dissonance, of course, will vary with the importance of

your belief/attitude/principle and with the degree of inconsistency between your

behavior and this belief. In any case, according to the theory, the greater the

dissonance the more you will be motivated to resolve it.

Page 14: FYP-Rafia

3. Dissonance will be resolved in one of three basic ways:

a) Change beliefs

Perhaps the simplest way to resolve dissonance between actions and beliefs is

simply to change your beliefs. You could, of course, just decide that cheating is

right. This would take care of any dissonance. However, if the belief is fundamental

and important to you such a course of action is unlikely. Moreover, our basic beliefs

and attitudes are pretty stable, and people don’t just go around changing basic

beliefs/attitudes/opinions all the time, since we rely a lot on our world view in

predicting events and organizing our thoughts. Therefore, though this is the simplest

option for resolving dissonance it’s probably not the most common.

b) Change actions

A second option would be to make sure that you never do this action again. Lord

knows that guilt and anxiety can be motivators for changing behavior. So, you may

say to yourself that you will never cheat on a test again, and this may aid in resolving

the dissonance. However, aversive conditioning (i.e. guilt/anxiety) can often be a

pretty poor way of learning, especially if you can train yourself not to feel these

things. Plus, you may really benefit in some way from the action that’s inconsistent

with your beliefs. So, the trick would be to get rid of this feeling without changing

your beliefs or your actions, and this leads us to the third, and probably most

common, method of resolution.

c) Change perception of action

A third and more complex method of resolution is to change the way you

view/remember/perceive your action. In more colloquial terms, you would

“rationalize” your actions. For example, you might decide that the test you cheated

on was for a dumb class that you didn’t need anyway. Or you may say to yourself

Page 15: FYP-Rafia

that everyone cheats so why not you? In other words, you think about your action in

a different manner or context so that it no longer appears to be inconsistent with your

actions. If you reflect on this series of mental gymnastics for a moment you will

probably recognize why cognitive dissonance has come to be so popular. If you’re

like me, you notice such post-hoc reconceptualiztions (rationalizations) of behavior

on the part of others all the time, though it’s not so common to see it in one’s self.

Figure 1 Cognitive Dissonance Theory

Festinger's early explanation of dissonance did not clearly identify whether dissonance is

cognitive or emotional. The cognitive view is supported by his suggestion that "the obverse

of one element follows from the other" (Festinger, 1957, p. 261). Festinger described a

person as being in a dissonant state if two elements in his cognition, that is, in his

knowledge of himself, his behavior, his feelings, desires, or in his knowledge of the world,

are inconsistent. Cognitive dissonance may result when an opinion is formed or a decision is

taken when cognition and opinions direct us in different directions. Yet Festinger (1957, p.

266) also seems to have intended an emotional conceptualization, suggesting that, "for some

people, dissonance is an extremely painful and intolerable thing."

Cooper and Fazio (1984) considered that dissonance has less to do with an inconsistency

among cognitions per se, but rather with expectations of undesirable consequences. Oliver

Page 16: FYP-Rafia

(1997) also believes dissonance includes concern about unknown outcomes, in terms of

anticipated regret, and a feeling of apprehension on the consumer's part.

Cognitive dissonance has been defined as psychological discomfort (Carlsmith & Aronson,

1963; Elliot & Devine, 1994), a psychologically uncomfortable state (Festinger, 1957;

Menasco & Hawkins, 1978), being linked with anxiety, uncertainty or doubt (Menasco «&

Hawkins, 1978; Montgomery & Barnes, 1993; Mowen, 1995) or as synonymous with the

regret or remorse reported in salespeople's anecdotes (Insko & Schopler, 1972). It is

apparent that cognitive dissonance has both cognitive and psychological components.

Cooper and Fazio (1984) distinguished between the two psychological components of

"dissonance arousal" and "dissonance as a psychologically uncomfortable state." They

suggested that arousal is a necessary condition for dissonance to occur and that, if labeled

negatively and an attribution is made internally (free choice, "it was my decision"), then

psychological discomfort will arise. This psychological discomfort then motivates

dissonance reduction, as predicted by dissonance theory. Elliot and Devine (1994) claimed

that the latter element has received far less empirical attention than dissonance arousal and

urged a systematic attempt to validate the psychological discomfort component of

dissonance. However, they concluded that the distinct affect experienced by an individual in

a given situation is closely related to the individual's cognitive appraisal of the situation and,

hence, that the cognitive and affective components are not independent.

There are parallels in the conceptualization of dissonance and satisfaction. Satisfaction has

been described as the "emotional response to the judgmental disparity between product

performance and a corresponding normative standard" (Westbrook & Oliver, 1991, p. 85).

Hence satisfaction, while described as emotional in nature, is based on a response to a

cognitive judgment, and the construct is said to comprise cognitive as well as affective,

components (Dabholkar, 1995; Oliver, 1994). Dissonance similarly comprises cognitive and

emotional components. As Festinger describes, it is a psychologically uncomfortable state,

but generated by inconsistent cognitions. However, there are two important differences

between the concepts. First, dissonance is recognized as immediately post decisional (e.g.,

Page 17: FYP-Rafia

Festinger, 1957; Insko & Schopler, 1972). Satisfaction, in contrast, is assessed post purchase

and post use, when performance is compared to expectations. Second, satisfaction is based

on a comparison of known performance and expectations, whereas dissonance concerns

unknown outcomes, generating apprehension that may continue after use of the product or

service, when satisfaction judgments are made (Oliver, 1997).

Oliver (1997) takes a wider view of cognitive dissonance, examining the concept over the

entire purchase decision process. Originating in a pre-purchase phase, the construct is

labeled apprehension and increases over the decision process. These same cognitions and

feelings mutate into true dissonance after the decision is made, when consideration of

foregone alternatives becomes relevant. With use and experience, dissonance dissipates and

yields to dissatisfaction.

The concept of dissonance addressed in the present research best fits the period that

immediately follows the purchase decision but precedes use or experience with the result of

the purchase decision. At this stage, labeled the "gamma" stage by Oliver (1997),

dissonance is maximized and precedes satisfaction formation.

2.3.0 POSTPURCHASE BEHAVIOUR

According to Strydom et al. (2000: 79), after purchasing the product, the buyer will

experience some level of satisfaction or dissatisfaction. The marketer’s job does not end

when the product is bought but continues into the post purchase period. Marketers must

monitor post purchase satisfaction, post purchase actions and cognitive dissonance. The

buyer’s satisfaction or dissatisfaction will influence future behavior. A satisfied buyer will

purchase the product again and recommend it to others. Dissatisfied buyers will respond

differently. They may stop using the product, return it, or take some form of public action.

In support, Lamb et al. (2004: 77) state that when buying products, consumers expect

certain outcomes or benefits to accrue from the purchase. How well these expectations are

met determines whether the consumer is satisfied or dissatisfied with the purchase.

Page 18: FYP-Rafia

Figure 1-1 illustrates the relationships among the post purchase processes. As the figure

indicates, some purchases are followed by a phenomenon called post purchase dissonance.

This occurs when a consumer doubts the wisdom of a purchase he or she has made. Other

purchases are followed by non-use. The consumer keeps or returns the product without

using it. Most purchases are followed by product use, even if post purchase dissonance is

present (Hill and O Sullivan, 1999: 96). Product use often requires the disposal of the

product package and/or the product itself. During and after use, the consumer evaluates the

purchase process and the product.

Figure 2 A Framework for Post purchase Behavior

Source: Singh (2003: 2)

Unsatisfactory evaluations may produce complaints by those consumers. Appropriate

responses by the firm may reverse the initial dissatisfaction among those who complained.

The result of all these processes is a final level of satisfaction, which in turn can result in a

Page 19: FYP-Rafia

loyal, committed customer: one who is willing to repurchase, or a customer who switches

brands or discontinues using the product category (Singh, 2003: 4).

Hasty and Reardon (1997: 154) believe that when people recognize inconsistency between

their values or opinions and their behavior, they tend to feel an inner tension or anxiety

called cognitive dissonance (post purchase doubt). For example, suppose a consumer spends

half his monthly salary on a new high-tech stereo system. If he stops to think how much he

has spent, he will probably feel dissonance. Weitz et al. (2001: 363) point out that customers

like to believe they have chosen intelligently when they make a decision. After important

decisions, they may feel a little insecure about whether the sacrifice is worth it. Such

feelings are called buyer’s remorse or post purchase dissonance.

Hawkins et al. (2001: 629) highlight that the probability of a consumer experiencing post

purchase dissonance, as well as the magnitude of such dissonance, is a function of 1.) The

degree of commitment or irrevocability of the decision. The easier it is to alter the decision,

the less likely the consumer is to experience dissonance. 2.) The importance of the

decision to the consumer. The more important the decision, the more likely dissonance will

result. 3.) The difficulty of choosing among the alternatives. The more difficult it is to

select from among the alternatives, the more likely the experience and magnitude of

dissonance. Decision difficulty is a function of the number of alternatives considered, the

number of relevant attributes associated with each alternative, and the extent to which each

alternative offers attributes not available with the other alternatives. 4.) The individual’s

tendency to experience anxiety. Some individuals have a higher tendency to experience

anxiety than do others. The higher the tendency to experience anxiety, the more likely the

individual will experience post purchase dissonance.

2.3.1 Causes of Cognitive Dissonance

Strydom et al. (2000: 79) furthermore state that some of the alternatives not chosen may

have attractive features, so that the correctness of the choice is not obvious. Cognitive

dissonance is most likely to occur for major purchases that are difficult to select and undo.

People tend to resolve the discomfort or buyer’s remorse by seeking information to support

Page 20: FYP-Rafia

their decision and by becoming more critical of the alternatives they rejected. Marketers can

help consumers feel good about major purchases by providing reassurance after the sale is

complete. In addition, Singh (2003: 4) points out that because consumers are uncertain of

the wisdom of their decisions, they rethink their decisions in the post purchase phase. This

stage serves several functions; it serves to broaden the consumers set of experiences stored

in memory, it provides a check on how well the consumer is doing in selecting products, the

feedback received from this stage helps the consumer to make adjustments in future

purchasing strategies.

Etzel et al. (2001: 100) state that cognitive dissonance is a state of anxiety brought on by the

difficulty of choosing from among alternatives. Unfortunately for marketers, dissonance is

quite common, and if the anxiety is not relieved, the consumer may be unhappy with the

chosen product even if it performs as expected. Post purchase cognitive dissonance occurs

when each of the alternatives seriously considered by the consumer has both attractive and

unattractive features. Czinkota et al. (2000: 164) point out that after purchase is made, the

unattractive features of the product purchased grow in importance in the consumers mind, as

do the attractive features offered by the rejected alternatives. As a result, we begin to doubt

the wisdom of the choice and experience anxiety over the decision. Dissonance typically

increases the greater the importance of the purchase decision and the greater the similarity

between the items selected and item(s) rejected.

According to Singh (2003: 13), it appears that dissonance is likely to occur under certain

conditions. 1.) A minimum threshold of dissonance tolerance is passed. That is, consumers

may tolerate a certain level of inconsistency in their lives until this point is reached. 2) The

action is irrevocable. For instance, when a consumer purchases a new car, there is little

likelihood of reversing this decision and getting the money back. 3.) There are several

desirable alternatives. Today’s car buyer, for example, has an abundance of choices among

similar attractive models. In fact, research indicates that those consumers who experience

greater difficulty in making purchase decisions, or who consider a wider range of store and

brand options, are more likely to experience greater magnitudes of post purchase

dissonance. 4.) Available alternatives are quite dissimilar in their qualities (there is little

Page 21: FYP-Rafia

„cognitive overlap). For instance, although there are many automobile models, each one

may have some unique characteristics. 5.) The buyer is committed to a decision because it

has psychological significance. A large and important living-room-furniture purchase is

likely to have great psychological significance to the buyer because of its dramatic reflection

of the buyer’s decorating tastes, philosophy and lifestyle. Ego involvement will be quite

high. 6.) There is no pressure applied to the consumer to make the decision. If consumers

are subject to outside pressure, they will do what they are forced to do without letting their

own viewpoints or preferences really be challenged. In other words, when pressure is

applied, consumers will externalize the source of their dissatisfaction rather than allow any

mental unease or discomfort regarding their own cognition.

According to Berman and Evans (1998: 222), cognitive dissonance occurs because making a

relatively permanent commitment to a chosen alternative requires one to give up the

attractive features of the un-chosen alternatives. This is inconsistent with the desire for those

features. Thus, nominal and most limited decision making will not produce post purchase

dissonance, since these decisions do not consider attractive features in an un-chosen brand

that do not also exist in the chosen brand. In addition, Hill and O Sullivan (1999: 96) point

out that because most high-involvement purchase decisions involve one or more of the

factors that lead to post purchase dissonance, these decisions often are accompanied by

dissonance. And, since dissonance is unpleasant, consumers generally attempt to avoid or

reduce it. Avoiding dissonance involves actions taken before the purchase is made by either

avoiding/delaying the decision or using a purchase decision rule that will minimize regret.

In making a final choice the buyer not only had to forgo other attractive options but also had

to part with (perhaps a great deal of) money, which could have been used for other purposes.

It is no wonder, therefore, that the buyer often begins to doubt the wisdom of the decision

(Foxall et al. 2001: 130). This negative feeling of doubt and uncertainty in the post purchase

period is referred to as cognitive dissonance, a negative emotion stemming from a

psychological inconsistency in the cognition (the things that a person knows). Dissonant

buyers will try to correct these psychological inconsistencies by attempting to convince

themselves that the original decision was correct and very judicious. In order to do so, they

Page 22: FYP-Rafia

may rationalize by putting forward logical reasons for decisions taken and may also turn to

others for approval and reassurance (Strydom et al. 2000: 80).

Cognitive dissonance occurs because the person knows the purchased product has some

disadvantages as well as advantages. In the case of the stereo, the disadvantage of cost

battles the advantage of technological superiority. In other words, dissonance is post

purchase uncertainty or anxiety (Etzel et al. 2001: 528). Consumers try to reduce dissonance

by justifying their decision. They might seek new information that reinforces positive ideas

about the purchase (confirming that it was the right decision), avoid information that

contradicts their decision, or revoke the original decision by returning the product (Czinkota

et al. 2000: 163).

People who have just bought new cars often read more advertisements of the car they have

just bought than of other cars in order to reduce dissonance and reinforce the correctness of

the decision. In some instances, people deliberately seek contrary information in order to

refute it and reduce dissonance. Dissatisfied customers sometimes rely on word-of-mouth to

reduce cognitive dissonance by letting friends and family knows they are displeased (Lamb

et al. 2004:78).

People usually experience cognitive dissonance only when buying high involvement

products. Cognitive dissonance is the inner tension that a consumer experiences after

recognizing a purchased product’s disadvantages. When a purchase creates cognitive

dissonance, consumers tend to react by seeking positive reinforcement for the purchase

decision, avoiding negative information about the purchase decision, or revoking the

purchase decision by returning the product (Kinicki and Williams, 2003: 350).

Dissonance theory began by postulating that pairs of cognitions (elements of knowledge)

can be relevant or irrelevant to one another, they are either consonant or dissonant. Two

cognitions are consonant if one follows from the other, and they are dissonant if the opposite

of one cognition follows from the other. The existence of dissonance, being psychologically

uncomfortable, motivates the person to reduce the dissonance and leads to avoidance of

Page 23: FYP-Rafia

information likely to increase the dissonance. The greater the magnitude of the dissonance,

the greater is the pressure to reduce dissonance (Harmon-Jones and Mills, 2003: 1).

Festinger (2003: 1) notes that the magnitude of dissonance between one cognitive element

and the remainder of the person’s cognitions depends on the number and importance of

cognitions that are consonant and dissonant with the one in question. Formally speaking, the

magnitude of dissonance equals the number of dissonant cognitions divided by the number

of consonant cognitions plus the number of dissonant cognitions. This is referred to as the

dissonance ratio. Harmon-Jones and Mills (2003: 7) add that holding the number and

importance of consonant cognitions constant, as the number or importance of dissonant

cognitions increases, increases the magnitude of dissonance. Holding the number and

importance of dissonant cognitions constant, as the number or importance of consonant

cognitions increases, decreases the magnitude of dissonance.

Jones and Ince (2001: 5) argue that the thrust of cognitive dissonance theory is that

dissonance is likely to occur after a choice has been made, and will reflect a natural

occurrence because the choice has been made. In terms of post purchase processes, it is the

total amount of dissonance that we experience that is important. The more dissonant

cognitions we have about a decision, and the more important these are to us, the higher our

dissonance will be. And, since dissonance produces unpleasant feelings, we will be

motivated to act to reduce the amount of dissonance we are experiencing.

2.3.2 Results of Cognitive Dissonance

Wells and Prensky (1996: 320) comment that a person will experience feelings of

discomfort, known as cognitive dissonance, when he or she has knowledge, holds attitudes,

or takes actions that conflict with one another. When dissonance occurs the individual will

seek to reduce it by changing the inconsistent cognitive elements. Dylan (2003: 1) shows

that cognitive dissonance is a theory of human motivation that asserts that it is

psychologically uncomfortable to hold contradictory cognitions. The theory is that

Page 24: FYP-Rafia

dissonance, being unpleasant, motivates a person to change his cognition, attitude, or

behavior.

Dissonance and consonance are relations among cognitions, that is, among options; beliefs,

knowledge of the environment, and knowledge of ones own actions and feelings. Two

opinions, or beliefs, or items of knowledge are dissonant with each other if they do not fit

together; that is, if they are inconsistent, or if, considering only the particular two items, one

does not follow from the other (Jones and Ince, 2001: 10).

Czinkota et al. (2000: 163) believe that the consumer’s decision process does not end with

the purchase. Rather, the experience of buying and using the product provides information

that the consumer will use in future decision making. In some cases, the consumer will be

pleased with the experience and will buy the same product from the same supplier again. In

other cases, the consumer will be disappointed and may even return or exchange the

product. In general, the post purchase process includes four steps: decision confirmation,

experience evaluation, satisfaction or dissatisfaction, and future response (exit, voice, or

loyalty).

After a consumer makes an important choice decision, he or she experiences an intense need

to confirm the wisdom of that decision. The flip side is that he or she wants to avoid the

disconfirmation. One of the processes that occur at this stage is cognitive dissonance: a post

purchase doubt the buyer experiences about the wisdom of the choice. Methods of reducing

dissonance and confirming the soundness of ones decision are seeking further positive

information about the chosen alternative and avoiding negative information about the

chosen alternative (Phipps and Simmons, 2000: 152).

Hawkins et al. (2001: 312) state that the occurrence of post decision dissonance is related to

the concept of cognitive dissonance. This theory states that there is often a lack of

consistency or harmony among an individual’s various cognitions, or attitudes and beliefs,

after a decision has been made – that is, the individual has doubts and second thoughts about

the choice made. Further, it is more likely that the intensity of the anxiety will be greater

Page 25: FYP-Rafia

when 1.) The decision is an important one psychologically or financially, or both. 2.)

There are a number of forgone alternatives. 3.) The forgone alternatives have many

favorable features.

The decision process does not end with the purchase - not for the buyer at least! A product,

once purchased, yields certain levels of satisfaction and dissatisfaction. Purchase satisfaction

comes from receiving benefits expected, or greater than expected, from a product. If buyers

experiences from the use of a product exceed expectations, they are satisfied, but if

experiences are below expectations, customers are dissatisfied (Futrell, 2004: 126).

Gilbert (2003: 60) shows that the buyer can experience purchase dissonance after the

product’s purchase. Dissonance causes tension over whether the right decision was made in

buying the product. Some people refer to this as buyer’s remorse. Dissonance increases with

the importance of the decision and the difficulty of choosing between products. If

dissonance occurs, buyers may get rid of a product by returning it or by selling it to

someone else. Alternatively, they may seek assurance from the salesperson or friends that

the product is a good one and that they made the correct purchase decision (positively

reinforcing themselves).

2.3.3 Motivational Nature of Cognitive Dissonance

Dylan (2003: 1) notes that post purchase behavior/cognitive dissonance is a theory of human

motivation that asserts that it is psychologically uncomfortable to hold contradictory

cognitions. The theory is that post purchase doubt, being unpleasant, motivates a person to

change his belief/cognition, attitude, or behavior. In support, Schiffman and Kanuk (2000:

219) state that post purchase behavior is a psychological phenomenon which refers to the

fact that people seek out information which supports their currently held views, and seek to

avoid information which challenges them. If they cannot avoid doubtful viewpoints, they

tend to hear selectively only that part of the information that supports them and/or

reinterpret what they are hearing, so that it does match their current opinions.

Page 26: FYP-Rafia

According to Fuller (1999: 329), for a customer to want to repeat a purchase or recommend

one to someone else, he or she has to have a positive experience the first time around. In the

decision process model, the positive/negative influence of customer satisfaction is shown as

a feedback loop that fuels or aborts repeat purchase behaviors. When a purchase is less than

satisfactory for whatever reason(s), it creates a state of tension called post purchase

dissonance. The author describes this as a form of behavior that occurs because of a

discrepancy between benefits expected and benefits actually delivered by a product.

Boyd et al. (2002: 119) argue that whether a particular consumer feels adequately rewarded

following a purchase depends on two things: the person’s aspiration or expectation level –

how well the product was expected to perform (delivery of a quality pizza while it is hot) –

and the consumers evaluation of how well the product actually did perform (the pizza

arrived cold). Consumer’s expectations about a product’s performance are influenced by

several factors. These include the strength and importance of each person’s need and the

information collected during the decision-making process. Even with services there is a

danger for marketers in using exaggerated claims in product advertising. Such claims can

produce inflated expectations the product cannot live up to – resulting in dissatisfied

customers.

Because purchase decisions often require some amount of compromise, post purchase

dissonance is quite normal. Nevertheless, it is likely to leave consumers with an uneasy

feeling about their prior beliefs or actions – a feeling that they tend to resolve by changing

their attitudes to conform to their behavior. Thus in the case of post purchase dissonance,

attitude change is frequently an outcome of an action or behavior. The conflicting thoughts

or dissonant information that follows a purchase are prime factors that induce consumers to

change their attitudes so that they will be consonant with their actual purchase behavior

(Schiffman and Kanuk, 2000: 220).

People try to make sense of the world they encounter. In effect, they do this by looking for

some consistency amongst their own experiences and memories and by turning to other

people for comparison and confirmation. If all factors check out, then all is well and good,

Page 27: FYP-Rafia

but what if there is some inconsistency and supposing the inconsistency is amongst the

person’s own experiences, beliefs or actions (Rudolph, 2003: 1). Many social psychologists

believe that this will trigger some general trend to restore cognitive consistency: to

reinterpret the situation so as to minimize whatever inconsistency there may be. This is

because any perceived inconsistency amongst various aspects of knowledge, feelings and

behaviors brings up an unpleasant internal state (post purchase dissonance) which people try

to reduce whenever possible (Harmon-Jones and Mills, 2003:10).

Zikmund and d’Amico (2002: 148) state that consumption naturally follows the purchase. If

the decision maker is also the user, the matter of purchase satisfaction (or dissatisfaction)

remains. In some cases, satisfaction is immediate, as when the buyer chews the just-bought

gum or feels pleased that the decision-making process is over. We are telling ourselves that

we are pleased with the purchase because our expectations have been confirmed. In this

case, marketing has achieved its goal of consumer satisfaction.

Czinkota and Kotabe (2000: 31) point out that the opposite can occur – a consumer can feel

uneasy about a purchase. Second thoughts can create an uneasy feeling, a sensation that the

decision-making process may have yielded the wrong decision. These feelings of

uncertainty can be analyzed in terms of the theory of cognitive dissonance. In the context of

consumer behavior, cognitive dissonance is a psychologically uncomfortable post purchase

feeling. More specifically, it refers to the negative feelings, or buyer’s remorse, that can

follow a commitment to purchase. Hill and O Sullivan (1999: 97) point out that cognitive

dissonance results from the fact that people do not like to hold two or more conflicting

beliefs or ideas at the same time. Dissonance theory describes such feelings as a sense of

psychic tension, which the individual will seek to relieve. Each alternative has some

advantages and some disadvantages.

According to Gilbert et al. (1997: 147), buyers reduce cognitive dissonance by focusing on

the advantages of the purchase – by carrying out post purchase evaluation in a way that

supports the choice made. Buyers may seek reinforcement from friends or from the seller.

They may mentally downgrade the unselected alternatives and play up the advantages of the

Page 28: FYP-Rafia

selected brand to convince themselves that they made the right choice. In addition, Sheth et

al. (1999: 405) state that effective marketers do not want dissatisfied customers. Zikmund

and d’Amico, (2002: 148) report that when marketers understand that any choice can create

cognitive dissonance, they can seek to support their customer’s choices. Fulfilling customer

expectations, which leads to satisfaction, is the purpose of many marketing activities

2.4.0 POST-PURCHASE EVALUATION

After buying a product, consumers formally or informally evaluate the outcome of the

purchase. In particular, they consider whether they are satisfied with the experience of

making the purchase and with the good or service they bought. A consumer who repeatedly

has favorable experiences may develop loyalty to the brand purchased. Also, consumers

may tell their family, friends, and acquaintances about their experiences with buying and

using products. Cognitive dissonance/post purchase dissonance may result because of the

difficulty or even impossibility of fully considering every possible alternative course of

action (Gilbert et al. 1997: 147).

Moreover, Schiffman and Kanuk (2004: 570) show that as consumers use a product,

particularly during a trial purchase, they evaluate its performance in light of their own

expectations. There are three possible outcomes of these evaluations as shown in Figure 1.2:

actual performance matches expectations, leading to a neutral feeling; performance exceeds

expectations, causing what is known as positive disconfirmation of expectations (which

leads to satisfaction); and performance is below expectations, causing negative

disconfirmation of expectations and dissatisfaction. For each of these three outcomes,

consumer’s expectations and satisfaction are closely linked; that is, consumers tend to judge

their experience against their expectations when performing a post purchase evaluation.

Cant et al. (2002: 182) report that post-buying assessment involves a customer’s evaluation

of the performance of the product or service, in relation to the criteria, once it has been

bought, i.e. it is the customer’s perception of the outcome of the consumption process. The

post-buying phase involves different forms of psychological processes that customers can

Page 29: FYP-Rafia

experience after buying something. After buying something, the customer discovers

something about a product or service, stores this new knowledge in long-term memory,

modifies relevant attitudes, and is ready for the next decision process with an improved base

of knowledge.

Kurtz and Clow (1998: 418) point out that an important component of post purchase

evaluation is the reduction of any uncertainty or doubt that the consumer might have had

about the selection. As part of their post purchase analysis, consumers try to reassure

themselves that their choice was a wise one; that is they attempt to reduce post purchase

cognitive dissonance.

Peter and Donnelly (2004: 52) believe that the degree of post purchase analysis that

consumers undertake depends on the importance of the product decision and the experience

acquired in using the product. When the product lives up to expectations, they probably will

buy it again. When the product’s performance is disappointing or does not meet

expectations, however, they will search for more suitable alternatives. Thus, the consumers

post purchase evaluation “feeds back” as experience to the consumer’s psychological field

and serves to influence future related decisions.

According to Arens (2004: 164), the customer’s decision process does not end with the

purchase. Rather, the experience of buying and using the product provides information that

the customer will use in future decision making. In some cases, the customer will be pleased

with the experience and will buy the same product from the same supplier again. In other

cases, the customer will be disappointed and may even return or exchange the product. In

general, as shown in Figure 1.2, the post purchase process includes four steps: decision

confirmation, experience evaluation, satisfaction or dissatisfaction, and future response

(exit, voice, or loyalty).

Page 30: FYP-Rafia

Figure 1.2 The Purchase Evaluation Process

Source: Singh (1988:102)

2.4.1 Decision Confirmation

Schiffman and Kanuk, (2000: 220) point out that after a customer makes an important

choice decision, he or she experiences an intense need to confirm the wisdom of that

decision. The flip side is that he or she wants to avoid the disconfirmation. One of the

processes that occur at this stage is cognitive dissonance.

Page 31: FYP-Rafia

Figure 1.3 A Model of Consumer Post acquisition Process

Source: Singh (1988:104)

2.4.2 Experience Evaluation

Sheth et al. (2000: 548) narrates that following purchase, the product or service is actually

consumed. Marketers need to know whether customers consume it routinely or while

consciously evaluating it. This depends on the level of enduring involvement in the product

or service and the finality of the preference that caused this purchase.

2.4.3 Consumer Satisfaction/Dissatisfaction

According to Cant et al. (2002: 183), a positive assessment of the purchase decision results

in post-buying satisfaction. Satisfaction occurs when the outcome, which may be a product,

brand or store, and the conditions surrounding its purchase, are matched with the customer’s

Page 32: FYP-Rafia

expectations. Lee et al. (2000: 217) found that perceived quality determines satisfaction,

rather than vice versa. During shopping, customers reach their final choices with quite

different decision goals in mind. Shiv and Huber (2000: 203) believe that while some

customers goals could be choice oriented (deciding on which alternative to buy from a set of

choices), the goal of others may be value oriented (evaluating each alternative with the aim

of obtaining good value for money), and the goal of a third category may be anticipated

satisfaction (where customers assess the likely satisfaction with each alternative before

making the final choice).

From Figure 1.3, it is clear that providing a quality product or service is all about meeting or

exceeding customer expectations. Where expectations are met then the customer is satisfied.

If expectations are not met then the customer experiences dissatisfaction. This logic is even

more applicable to service provision where there are fewer tangibles. The customer has

certain expectations of the level of service that they will receive based, perhaps, on

advertising, past experiences or hearsay. When the customers actually consume the service,

they will consciously or unconsciously, evaluate the performance of the company against

their expectations. If their expectations and evaluations do not match then there will be a

gap leading to satisfaction or dissatisfaction depending on the direction and extent of the

gap. Dissatisfaction, so the theory goes, will lead to a perception of poor quality and vice

versa.

Whether or not they actively evaluate a product during product use or consumption, users do

experience the usage outcome. This outcome is characterized as satisfaction or

dissatisfaction. What is more challenging is to understand why consumers feel the way they

do (Griffin, 1997: 25). Research indicates that consumers do not evaluate the performance

of a product on an absolute basis, but compare it to the expected performance. Thus, if the

product fulfils pre purchase expectations, then satisfaction results. On the other hand, if the

pre purchase expectations are not met, dissatisfaction results. This makes intuitive sense in

our everyday experience (Sheth et al. 1999: 549).

Page 33: FYP-Rafia

Kerin et al. (2006: 122) indicate that following the experience of satisfaction or

dissatisfaction, consumers have three possible responses: exit, voice, or loyalty. If

consumers are dissatisfied with their experience with a brand, they may decide never again

to buy the brand. This place them back to the start of the decision process the next time the

problem recognition arises.

Figure 1.4 The Formation of Consumer Satisfaction/Dissatisfaction Model

Source: Singh (2003: 4)

Some dissatisfied consumers may complain, and then decide either to give the brand or

marketer another chance, or simply to exit (Wilkie, 2000: 621). According to Kotler and

Keller (2006: 199), dissatisfied consumers may abandon or return the product. They may

take public action by complaining to the company, going to a lawyer, or complain to other

groups. Private actions include making decisions to stop buying (exit option) the product or

warning (voice option) friends.

2.4.4 Customer expectations and future expectations

Page 34: FYP-Rafia

According to Cant et al. (2002: 239), customer expectations serve as a benchmark against

which present and future service encounters are compared. Customer expectations are what

customers think they will receive in the service encounter, and they can be divided into at

least three levels:

The predicted service level is the customer’s anticipated level of performance

The desired service level reflects the ideal level of service that the customer wants or

hopes to receive compared to the predicted service level

The adequate level represents the minimum level of service that the customer will still

tolerate and accept without being dissatisfied.

Factors that could influence customer’s expectation levels, as well as their zone of tolerance,

include personal needs, self-perceived service roles, implicit service promises, word-of-

mouth communication and past experiences. For this reason, it is extremely important to

ensure that the organization’s promises to its customers reflect what the organization is

actually able to deliver (Van Birgelen et al. 2002: 44).

Snipes et al. (2005: 1336) believe that customer satisfaction/dissatisfaction is the end result

of the service experience. In cases where customer’s expectations exceed the perceived

service delivery they will be dissatisfied, and where the perceived service exceeds their

expectations they will be satisfied. Total satisfaction can therefore be seen as consistently

exceeding customer expectations. Achieving this demands some basic prerequisites, such as

1.) Allowing customers to define service value 2.) Exceeding customer’s expectations in key

areas, not all areas 3.) Differentiating the customer base and investing in serving profitable

customers 4.) Investing in training, education and systems, because quality service may be

defined by customers, but it is delivered by employees.

Customer satisfaction is created through a combination of responsiveness to customer’s

expectations and views, continuous improvement of the organization’s offering, as well as

the continuous improvement of the overall customer relationship. The cornerstone is that

Page 35: FYP-Rafia

quality is a fundamental customer requirement, and if customers are not satisfied, the

opportunities for creating loyalty will cease to exist (Keh and Lee, 2006: 129).

2.4.5 Exit, voice, or loyalty

What happens when consumers experience dissatisfaction? Consumers may exhibit

unfavorable word-of-mouth communication, that is, they tell others about their problem.

Consumers may also not repurchase the brand. Another course of action is to complain.

Following the complaint, negative word-of-mouth is less likely, and repatriate more likely,

if the complaint is successfully redressed (Levine, 2003: 202). If the complaint is not

successfully redressed, the negative word-of-mouth might in fact be further intensified

beyond what it would have been had the consumer not made the complaint in the first place.

Research has found that consumer complaints may actually be good for marketers-

complainers care enough to complain. Non-complainers simply walk out, taking their

patronage to a competitor (Baker, 2000: 55).

The third response is, of course, loyalty. Consumer loyalty means the consumer buys the

same brand repeatedly. It is reasonable to assume that loyal consumers are more likely to be

satisfied. However the converse is not necessarily true, as some researchers have found that

not all satisfied consumers are loyal. Some consumers will still exhibit a switching behavior

despite being satisfied with the current brand (Czinkota et al. 2000: 164).

Belch and Belch (2001: 152) state that this theory of satisfaction has important implications

for shaping expectations. If marketing communications and other elements of the marketing

mix (e.g., advertising, salespersons, price, appearance of the store, and so on) promise too

much, they may create expectations that the product or service would almost surely fail to

fulfill, thus risking customer dissatisfaction. Of course, if the expectations are too low, the

sale may not result. The right strategy therefore ought to be to create realistic expectations

and not over-promise, and to design the product or service so that the realistic expectations

imply a performance level that the target market finds attractive enough to select the brand.

Page 36: FYP-Rafia

Post purchase evaluation focuses on whether customers have received good value.

Customers may weigh the benefits received by purchasing against the costs of making the

purchase. When the benefits significantly outweigh the costs, customers perceive high value

and are satisfied (Swartz and Iacobucci, 2000: 230). The more satisfied customers are, the

more likely they will become loyal to the brand and the seller, and the more likely the seller

is to establish a long-term relationship with the customer. In other words, customer value

and satisfaction influence future buying decisions (Arens, 2002: 235).

According to Arens (2004: 164), a key feature of the post purchase evaluation is cognitive

dissonance. During the post purchase period, the consumer may enjoy the satisfaction of the

purchase and thereby receive reinforcement for the decision. Or the purchase may turn out

to be unsatisfactory for some reason. In either case, feedback from the post purchase

evaluation updates the consumer’s mental files, affecting perceptions of the brand and

similar purchase decisions in the future.

An important component of post purchase evaluation is the reduction of any uncertainty or

doubt that the consumer might have had about the selection. As part of their post purchase

analyses, consumers try to reassure themselves that their choice was a wise one; that is, they

attempt to reduce post purchase cognitive dissonance (Schiffman and Kanuk, 2000: 457).

Dissonance also poses problems for those trying to assess customer satisfaction post-

purchase – as occurs during the measurement of service quality. Dissonance theory would

suggest that post-purchase dissonance might affect the size of the customer satisfaction gap

because of the positive attitude that arises from the desire to reduce dissonance (Phipps and

Simmons, 2000: 237).

Lamb et al. (2000: 353) highlight that almost all major purchases result in cognitive

dissonance, or discomfort caused by post purchase conflict. After the purchase, consumers

are satisfied with the benefits of the chosen brand and are glad to avoid the drawbacks of the

brands not bought. However, every purchase involves compromise. Consumers feel uneasy

about acquiring the drawbacks of the chosen brand and about losing the benefits of the

Page 37: FYP-Rafia

brands not purchased. Thus, consumers feel at least some post purchase dissonance for

every purchase.

Although post-buying assessment is the final stage in the customer decision-making process,

it is not necessarily the end of the process. The information gained as a result of buying and

post-buying evaluation is stored in individual’s memories as part of their experience.

Customers remember this information when starting another buying decision-making

process. In other words, regardless of the outcome, post purchase evaluation is a learning

process that provides feedback to the customer and is stored as information for future

reference (Cant et al. 2000: 185).

2.5.0 THE PURCHASE PROCESS FOR SERVICES

When customers decide to buy a service to meet an unfilled need, they go through what is

often a complex purchase process. This process has three separate stages: the pre purchase

stage, the service encounter stage, and the post purchase stage, each containing two or more

steps (Lovelock and Wright, 2002:88).

Bateson and Hoffman (1999: 170) point out that buyers form their expectations on the basis

of messages received from sellers, friends, and other information sources. If the seller

exaggerates the benefits, the buyer’s expectations will be disconfirmed; this leads to

dissatisfaction. The larger the gap between expectations and performance, the greater the

buyer’s dissatisfaction. Here the buyer’s coping style comes into play. Some buyers magnify

the gap when the product is not perfect, and they are highly dissatisfied. Other buyers

minimize the gap and are less dissatisfied.

2.5.1 Pre-purchase Stage

According to Bateson and Hoffman (1999: 34), the decision to buy and use a service is

made in the pre purchase stage. Individual needs and expectations are very important here

because they influence what alternatives customers will consider. If the purchase is routine

Page 38: FYP-Rafia

and relatively low risk, customers may move quickly to selecting and using a specific

service provider. In addition, Futrell (2004: 126) states that when more is at stake or a

service is about to be used for the first time, they may conduct an intensive information

search (contrast how one approaches the process of applying to college versus buying a

pizza or a hamburger!). The next step is to identify potential suppliers and then weigh the

benefits and risks of each option before making a final decision.

Gilbert (2003: 60) indicates that this element of perceived risk is especially relevant for

services that are high in experience or credence attributes and thus difficult to evaluate prior

to purchase and consumption. Perceived risk is defined as the uncertainty that consumers

face when they cannot foresee the consequences of their purchase decisions. First-time users

are especially likely to face greater uncertainty. Fitzsimmons and Fitzsimmons (2004: 135)

point out that risk perceptions reflect customer’s judgments of the probability of a negative

outcome. The worse the possible outcome and the more likely it is to occur, the higher the

perception of risk. One strategy to help reduce the risk perceived by customers is to educate

them about the features of the service, describe the types of users who can most benefit from

it, and offer advice on how to obtain the best results.

The degree of risk that consumers perceive and their own tolerance of risk taking are factors

that influence their purchase strategies. It should be stressed that consumers are influenced

by risks that they perceived, no matter how real or how dangerous would not influence

consumer behavior (Schiffman and Kanuk, 2004: 197).

According to Fitzsimmons and Fitzsimmons (2004: 136), the types of perceived risk are 1.)

Functional risk is the risk that the service or product will not perform as expected. 2.)

Physical risk is the risk to self and others that the product or service may pose. 3.)

Financial risk is the risk that the product will not be worth its cost. 4.) Social risk is the

risk that a poor product or service choice may result in social embarrassment. 5.)

Psychological risk is the risk that a poor product or service choice will bruise the

consumer’s ego. 6.) Time risk is the risk that the time spent in product search may be

wasted if the product does not perform as expected.

Page 39: FYP-Rafia

2.5.2 Service Encounter Stage

Lovelock and Wright (2002: 89) report that after deciding to purchase a specific service,

customers experience one or more contacts with their chosen service provider. The service

encounter stage often begins with submitting an application, requesting a reservation, or

placing an order. Contacts may take the form of personal exchanges between customers and

service employees, or impersonal interactions with machines or computers. Berman and

Evans (1998: 221) point out that in high contact services, such as restaurants, health care,

hotels, educational institutions, and public transportation, customers may become actively

involved in one or more service processes. Often, they experience a variety of elements

during service delivery, each of which may provide clues to service quality.

The intangibility of services together with other service characteristics, inseparability,

ownership, heterogeneity and perishability, create challenges for retailers, especially to

deliver over time, clear customer expectations and experience of service quality. These can

be used for purposes of organization or store brand differentiation in an intensely

competitive market facing slow growth of the customer base.

Service environments include all of the tangible characteristics to which customers are

exposed. The appearance of building exteriors and interiors, the nature of furnishings and

equipment; the presence or absence of dirt, odor, or noise; and the appearance and behavior

of other customers can all serve to shape expectations and perceptions of service quality

(Hasty and Reardon, 1997: 155).

Service Personnel are the most important factor in most high-contact service encounters,

where they have direct, face-to-face interactions with customers. But they can also affect

service delivery in low-contact situations like telephone-based service delivery.

Knowledgeable customers often expect employees to follow specific scripts during the

Page 40: FYP-Rafia

service encounter; excessive deviations from these scripts can lead to dissatisfaction.

Handling service encounters effectively on the part of the employee usually combines

learned skills with the right type of personality (Weitz et al. 2001: 362).

Support services are made up of the materials and equipment plus all of the backstage

processes that allow front stage employees to do their work properly. This element is

critical, because many customer-contact employees cannot perform their jobs well without

receiving internal services from support personnel (Kurtz and Clow, 1998: 419).

Bateson and Hoffman (1999: 25) report that consumers evaluate services by comparing the

service they perceive they have received with their expectations. If the perceived service is

equal to or better than the expected service, then the consumer is satisfied. It is crucial to

point out that this entire process takes place in the mind of the consumer. It is perceived

service that matters, not the actual service. Once this simple idea is established, two

subsidiary questions emerge: What is it that drives expectations and what is it that drives

perceptions?

2.5.3 Post-purchase stage

Wilkie (2000: 620) argues that during the post purchase stage, customers continue a process

they began in the service encounter stage – evaluating service quality and their

satisfaction/dissatisfaction with the service experience. The outcome of this process will

affect their future intentions, such as whether or not to remain loyal to the provider that

delivered service and whether to pass on positive or negative recommendations to family

members and other associates.

Lovelock and Wright (2002: 88) indicate that customers evaluate service quality by

comparing what they expected with what they perceive they received. If their expectations

are met or exceeded, they believe they have received high-quality service. If the

price/quality relationship is acceptable and other situational and personal factors are

positive, then these customers are likely to be satisfied. In addition, Futrell (2004: 126)

states that as a result, they are more likely to make repeat purchases and become loyal

Page 41: FYP-Rafia

customers. However, if the service experience does not meet customer’s expectations, they

may complain about poor service quality, suffer in silence, or switch providers in the future.

The consumer’s evaluation of a purchase feeds back into memory where the information can

be recalled for a similar purchase decision. Stored information about one or more negative

past experiences with a brand or supplier will reduce the odds that the consumer will make

the same purchase again (Cassel et al. 2001: 4). Consistent positive experiences can

ultimately lead to brand loyalty – the routine repurchase of the same brand with little

consideration of any alternatives (Peter and Donnelly, 2004: 53). Some experts argue that

consumers more often develop loyalty to service providers than to physical products

because of the difficulty of evaluating alternatives before actually experiencing the service.

Also, repeated patronage can bring additional benefits, such as discounts, or more

customized service as the provider gains more insights into the customer’s preferences

(Burnett, 2002: 81).

Direct experience of the product is an important part of the decision process. Feedback from

use helps learning and attitude development and is the main contributor to long-run

behavior. Communication activity must continue to provide satisfaction and prevent the

onset of cognitive dissonance. Marketing communications, at this stage, should be aimed at

reinforcing past decisions by stressing the positive features of the product or by providing

more information to assist its use and application (Fill, 2002: 92).

2.5.4 Service Quality and Customer Satisfaction

Fernie et al. (2004: 119) point out that service quality has been defined as the ability of the

organization to meet and exceed customer expectations. If the main difference between

service quality and customer satisfaction is that the former relates to managing the quality of

the service and the latter to customer’s expectation and experience of the quality of service

delivery, then improving customer service means delivering service quality improvements

which are customer defined.

Page 42: FYP-Rafia

Customer’s perception of the quality of service provided by firms depends upon the level of

satisfaction they experience in the process of shopping. Their satisfaction is affected by both

their expectations of the shopping experience and the actuality of the experience (Fernie et

al. 2004: 226). In addition Cant et al. (2002:239) state that if customers experience higher

levels of service than expected over time, then they will perceive the company as offering a

high quality service. If the level of service is lower than expected over time, the company

will be perceived as offering a low level service quality.

Dwyer and Tanner (2006: 450) highlight that the main difference between service quality

and customer satisfaction is that the former relates to managing the quality of the service

and the latter to customer’s expectation and experience of the quality of service delivery.

Therefore, improving customer service means delivering service quality improvements

which are customer defined.

2.6.0 COGNITIVE CONSISTENCY THEORIES

According to Sheth and Parvatiyar (2000: 180) the general concept of cognitive consistency

theories is that the various cognitions people hold have to be consistent with one another.

Inconsistency among ideas causes tension or drive, which people are moved to reduce by

bringing the inconsistent cognitions into consistency. Sheth et al. (1999: 405) highlight the

view that people accomplish this by changing one of the cognitions in order to make it

consistent with another. One specific theory based on the cognitive consistency principle is

Festinger’s cognitive dissonance theory.

Zikmund and d’Amico (2002: 148) point out that cognitive irrelevance probably describes

the bulk of the relationships among a person’s cognitions. Irrelevance simply means that the

two cognitions have nothing to do with each other. Two cognitions are consonant if one

cognition follows from, or fits with, the other. Berman and Evans (1998: 222) believe that

people like consonance among their cognitions. We do not know whether this stems from

the nature of the human organism or whether it is learned during the process of

Page 43: FYP-Rafia

socialization, but people appear to prefer cognitions that fit together to those that do not. It is

this simple observation that gives the theory of cognitive dissonance its interesting form.

Sometimes consumers experience post purchase conflict or discomfort about the wisdom of

a particular purchase decision. They might then hold illogical views about the brand or

service they have bought. In such cases, consumers might wish to reduce such dissonance

through cognitive and attitudinal changes. They will find a balance in their psychological

field by seeking supportive information or distorting contradictory information regarding the

product or service (du Plessis and Rousseau, 2003: 121).

2.7.0 REDUCING COGNITIVE DISSONANCE

Boyd et al. (2002: 120) argue that what makes post purchase dissonance relevant to

marketing strategists is the premise that dissonance propels consumers to reduce the

unpleasant feelings created by the rival thoughts. A variety of tactics are open to consumers

to reduce post purchase dissonance. The consumer can rationalize the decision as being

wise, seek out advertisements that support the choice, try to “sell” friends on the positive

features of the brand, or look to known satisfied owners for reassurance.

In addition to such consumer-initiated tactics to reduce post purchase uncertainty, a

marketer can relieve consumer dissonance by including messages in its advertising

specifically aimed at reinforcing consumer’s decisions by “complimenting their wisdom,”

offering stronger guarantees or warranties, increasing the number and effectiveness of its

services, or providing detailed brochures on how to use its products correctly (Elliot and

Devine, 1998: 383). Beyond these dissonance-reducing tactics, marketers increasingly are

developing affinity or relationship programmers designed to reward good customers and to

build customer loyalty and satisfaction (Schiffman and Kanuk, 2004: 281).

Consumers try to reduce their post purchase anxieties. They avoid information that is likely

to increase the dissonance. And they seek out information that supports their decision, such

as reassurance from friends. Post sale service reduces the customer’s post purchase

Page 44: FYP-Rafia

cognitive dissonance – the anxiety that usually occurs after a person makes a buying

decision (Wilkie, 2000: 619). In this final stage of the selling process, a sales person can

minimize the customer’s dissonance by summarizing the product’s benefits after the

purchase, repeating why the product is better than alternatives not chosen, describing how

satisfied other buyers have been with the product, and emphasizing how satisfied the

customer will be with the product (Etzel et al. 2001: 528).

According to du Plessis and Rousseau (2003: 325), the consumer might purchase the

product on the basis of a recommendation by some non-media source and then attempt to

support the decision by developing a positive attitude toward the brand and perhaps negative

feelings toward the rejected alternative(s). This reduces post purchase dissonance and

involves selective learning, whereby the consumer seeks information that supports the

choice made and avoids information that would raise doubts about the decision. In these

situations the main effect of communication is not the promotion of original choice behavior

and attitude change, but rather the reduction of dissonance by reinforcing the wisdom of the

purchase or providing supportive information.

2.6.1 Changing Product Evaluations

One of the ways consumers seek to reduce dissonance is to re-evaluate product alternatives.

This is accomplished by the consumer’s enhancing the attributes of the products selected

while decreasing the importance of the unselected product’s attributes (Aronson et al. 2001:

302). Another approach is for the consumer to re-evaluate product alternatives to view them

as being more alike than was thought at the purchase stage; this is, to establish or imagine

that cognitive overlap exists (Bahk, 2001: 4).

2.6.2 Seeking New Information

Consumers may reduce dissonance by seeking additional information in order to confirm the

wisdom of their product choice (Beauvois and Joul, 1999: 202).

Page 45: FYP-Rafia

2.6.3 Changing Attitudes

As a result of dissonance, consumers may change their attitudes to make them consonant

with their behavior (Sappenfiled, 2002: 6). By re-evaluating a product and adopting a

positive attitude toward it, attitudes and behavior become consistent and consonance is

achieved (Aronson et al. 2001: 304). Changing Cognitions - if two cognitions are discrepant,

we can simply change one to make it consistent with the other. Or we can change each

cognition in the direction of the other (Kaplan and Krueger, 1999: 196). Adding Cognitions

- if two cognitions cause a certain magnitude of dissonance, adding one or more consonant

cognitions can reduce that magnitude (Baumeister et al. 1999: 127). Altering Importance -

since the discrepant and consonant cognitions must be weighed by importance, it may be

advantageous to alter the importance of the various cognitions (Levine, 2003: 204).

While post purchase dissonance may be reduced by internal re-evaluations, searching for

additional external information that serves to confirm the wisdom of a particular choice is

also a common strategy. Naturally, information that supports the consumer’s choice acts to

bolster confidence in the correctness of the purchase decision (Hawkins et al. 2001: 629).

Peter and Donnelly (2004: 52) define cognitive dissonance as a condition reflecting a

tendency toward mental unease, which occurs when an individual holds two attitudes, ideas,

beliefs (or other cognitions) that are not in harmony with each other. In this situation, the

person tries to reduce dissonance – perhaps by dropping a cognition, perhaps by

strengthening one – in an effort to make beliefs and attitudes consistent. Dissonance may

thus be a factor in motivation because it leads the individual to change an opinion, attitudes

or behavior in order to reach a state of consonance or harmony.

The more effort a person exerts to attain a goal, the more dissonance is aroused if the goal is

less valuable than expected. Dissonance is reduced as the individual increases his liking for

Page 46: FYP-Rafia

a goal, and therefore it is thought that the higher the price paid by the consumer, the greater

is the tendency to like the brand and become loyal to it (Foxall et al. 2001: 123). There are

several major ways in which consumers strive to reduce dissonance.

According to Harmon-Jones and Mills (2003:2), dissonance can be reduced by removing

dissonant cognitions, adding new consonant cognitions, reducing the importance of

dissonant cognitions, or increasing the importance of consonant cognitions. The likelihood

that a particular cognition will change to reduce dissonance is determined by the resistance

to change of the cognition. Cognitions that are less resistant to change will change more

readily than cognitions that are more resistant to change. Wilkie (2000: 621) indicates that

resistance to change is based on the responsiveness of the cognition to reality and on the

extent to which the cognition is consonant with many other cognitions. Resistance to change

of a behavioral cognitive element depends on the extent of pain or loss that must be endured

and the satisfaction obtained from the behavior.

Festinger (2003: 3) reports that the concept of cognitive dissonance is an important feature

of attitude theory for marketers because of the emphasis it places on consumer’s need to

maintain cognitive consistency. That is, this theory tells marketers that consumers seek to

reduce mental discomfort or dissonance that could arise from the presence of conflicting or

inconsistent attitudes. Consumers do this by changing their behaviors and attitudes, or by

distorting the messages they receive in order to maintain a balance or consistency across the

whole system of beliefs, attitudes, intentions and behaviors. Marketers have found that

consumers frequently use advertising and the information it contains to justify or reinforce

prior behaviors such as product purchase and that targeted efforts to support consumer

decisions may prove to influence future buying behavior (Foxall et al. 2001:130).

Chow and Thompson (2003: 5) believe that the special contribution of cognitive dissonance

theory rests in its explicit stress on the consumer’s motivation to reduce tension following

an important purchase decision. 1) Attitude Spread. One likely outcome is that consumers

will strive to see their chosen brand as significantly better than the rejected ones. 2)

Selective Information Seeking. Promotional materials and ads provide very favorable

information about a brand, as do satisfied owners of the product. For this reason we would

Page 47: FYP-Rafia

expect consumers to seek out such information as a means of reducing their dissonance.

Some marketers believe that consumers read more ads (for the brand they’ve chosen) after

they purchase than they had before they bought (Rudolph, 2003: 1). 3) Motivated Opinion

Giving. More acceptances by others can also serve to reduce dissonance. Thus - especially

for innovations, which most consumers haven’t yet accepted – we would expect to see early

adopters wanting to bring about further acceptance. One way to do this is to engage in

favorable influence attempts on their friends (Harmon-Jones and Mills, 2003: 12).

Much of the research in dissonance theory has been conducted on the paradigm of induced

compliance. A person is persuaded to behave in ways contrary to his or her private beliefs.

Research has shown that attitudes are likely to change in order to restore consistency

between attitudes and behavior. Generally, the magnitude of attitude change is inversely

proportional to the amount of justification which is provided to engage in the attitude-

discrepant behavior. Induced compliance leads to dissonance arousal when the behavior is

engaged in freely, the actor feels committed to his attitude-discrepant stance, the behavior

results in unwanted consequences, and the actor feels personally responsible for bringing

about the unwanted consequences.

2.7.0 CHALLENGES TO THE ORIGINAL THEORY

Beginning in the late 1960s, researchers began to propose motivational explanations for

dissonance effects that differed from Festinger’s originally proposed theory. Three revisions

of dissonance theory have been proposed, and their originators have provided evidence to

support these conceptions. These include Aronson’s (1968, 1999) self consistency theory,

Steele’s (1988) self-affirmation theory, and Cooper and Fazio’s (1984) new look at

dissonance.

2.7.1 Self-Consistency

In his self-consistency theory, Aronson proposed that dissonance is not due merely to an

inconsistency between cognitions. Instead, he posited that dissonance occurs when a person

Page 48: FYP-Rafia

acts in a way that violates his or her self-concept, that is, when a person performs a behavior

inconsistent with his or her sense of self. Since most persons have a positive self-concept,

dissonance is most often experienced when a person behaves negatively, behaving in an

incompetent, irrational, or immoral manner. One of the primary predictions derived from

this revision is that low and high self-esteem individuals should respond with less and more

dissonance reduction (e.g., attitude change), respectively, because in dissonance

experiments high self-esteem individuals are induced to act in ways that are more discrepant

from their positive self-views. Experiments testing this prediction have produced mixed

results.

2.7.2 Self-Affirmation

Steele (1988) proposed a different alternative to Festinger’s dissonance theory. He proposed

that persons possess a motive to maintain an overall self-image of moral and adaptive

adequacy. He stated that dissonance-induced attitude change occurs because dissonance

threatens this positive self-image. While Festinger’s dissonance theory posited that

individuals are motivated to reconcile inconsistent cognitions, Steele proposed that, instead,

individuals are merely motivated to affirm the integrity of the self. In support of this idea,

Steele presented experiments, where, following a dissonance induction, participants either

were, or were not, presented with an opportunity to affirm an important value. When

participants were allowed to affirm an important value, dissonance-related attitude change

did not occur.

However, Simon, Greenberg and Brehm (1995) presented evidence supporting an

alternative explanation for Steele’s findings that was in line with the original theory of

dissonance. Festinger’s original theory proposed that the degree of dissonance experienced

depended upon the importance of the dissonant and consonant cognitions. Simon et al.

hypothesized that making an important value salient could reduce dissonance by reducing

the individual’s perception of the importance of the dissonant act. They conducted an

experiment in which participants who opposed a tuition increase were given high choice to

write essays in support of a tuition increase (a counter-attitudinal statement). After writing

Page 49: FYP-Rafia

the essay, participants either were given an opportunity to affirm an important value (self-

affirmation condition), were asked to write about a value that was not important to them

personally but was of general importance (value salient condition, e.g., world hunger), or

neither (control condition). Participants were then asked to rate the degree to which they

supported a tuition increase. Participants in the control condition changed their attitudes to

be more favorable toward a tuition increase, as expected. Participants in both the self-

affirmation and value salient conditions did not change their attitudes. They had trivialized,

or reduced the importance of, the tuition increase issue by thinking about other important

values, even when these values were not personally important and thus not self-affirming.

Other evidence has been presented that is difficult to interpret in self-affirmation theory

terms (for review, Aronson, Cohen, & Nail, 1999).

2.7.3 New Look

Cooper and Fazio (1984) proposed the idea that the discomfort experienced in dissonance

experiments was not due to an inconsistency between the individual’s cognitions, but rather

to feeling personally responsible for producing an aversive consequence. They stated,

“Dissonance has precious little to do with the inconsistency among cognitions per se, but

rather with the production of a consequence that is unwanted” (Cooper & Fazio, 1984). In

support of this idea, Cooper and Worchel (1970) replicated and extended Festinger and

Carlsmith’s (1959) classic experiment. In addition to the conditions of the original

experiment, Cooper and Worchel added conditions in which, when the participant tells the

confederate that the boring task is interesting, the confederate is not convinced by the lie.

They found that attitude change occurred only in the low-justification condition where the

confederate believed the lie. This result and others have been interpreted as indicating that

dissonance-related attitude change only occurs when individuals feel personally responsible

for producing an aversive consequence. The new look, or aversive consequences, revision of

cognitive dissonance theory was widely accepted (but see Berkowitz & Devine, 1989; Eagly

& Chaiken, 1993).

Page 50: FYP-Rafia

However, the results obtained in paradigms other than the counter-attitudinal action

paradigm are not consistent with the aversive consequences model. Dissonance research

using a selective-exposure paradigm has demonstrated that persons are more willing to

examine materials that confirm their beliefs than materials that dispute their beliefs (Brock

& Balloun, 1967; Frey, 1986). Research using a belief disconfirmation paradigm has shown

that, when persons are exposed to information that challenges their beliefs, they often

strengthen their original belief (Batson, 1975; Burris, Harmon-Jones, & Tarpley, 1997).

Research using a hypocrisy paradigm has shown that persons change their behavior to be

more in line with their beliefs when they are reminded of times when they did not live up to

their beliefs (Aronson, Fried, & Stone, 1991; Stone et al., 1994). It is difficult to reconcile

any of these lines of dissonance research with a conception of dissonance theory in which

the production of an aversive consequence is the only motivator of dissonance-related

attitude change.

According to the original theory of cognitive dissonance, the production of aversive

consequences would be expected to increase the amount of dissonance produced because an

aversive consequence may be an important dissonant cognition (see Harmon-Jones, 1999).

However, the original theory would deny that an aversive consequence is necessary to

produce dissonance. In the induced-compliance experiments testing the necessity of aversive

consequences, attitude change may have occurred only when participants caused aversive

consequences for a number of reasons. First, the lack of attitude change in the no-aversive-

consequences conditions is a null effect. Null effects are difficult to explain and subject to

multiple alternatives. Second, attitude change may have been produced, but may have been

too slight to be detected with the small sample size of these experiments. Third, not enough

dissonance may have been aroused in these experiments to produce attitude change without

the additional help of an aversive consequence. For example, too much justification for the

counter-attitudinal behavior may have been provided. Fourth, in these experiments,

dissonance may have been produced in the no-aversive consequences conditions, but may

have been reduced by a route other than attitude change.

Page 51: FYP-Rafia

To examine whether attitude change could occur in an induced compliance setting in which

aversive consequences were not produced, Harmon-Jones and colleagues (Harmon-Jones,

2000d; Harmon-Jones et al., 1996) conducted several experiments. Under the guise of an

experiment on memory, participants were exposed to an attitudinal object. Participants were

assured of privacy and anonymity, and then given high or low choice to write a counter-

attitudinal statement about the object (to manipulate justification). They were asked to

discard the statement in the trash after writing it, so that there was no chance of the

statement causing an aversive consequence. This manipulation was based on Cooper and

Fazio’s (1984) statement, “making a statement contrary to one’s attitude while in solitude

does not have the potential for bringing about an aversive event” (p. 232).

In one experiment (Harmon-Jones et al., 1996), participants were asked to read a boring

passage. They were then given high or low choice to write that they found the boring

passage interesting. Non-specific skin conductance responses (NS-SCRs) were assessed

during the 3 minutes between the writing of the statement and the assessment of the

participants’ attitudes toward the passage. Although no aversive consequences were

produced, persons in the high choice condition changed their attitudes to be more favorable

toward the passage. In addition, NS-SCRs indicated that participants in this condition

experienced more arousal.

In another experiment, participants who liked chocolate were asked to eat a piece of

chocolate and then given high or low choice to write a statement that they disliked the

chocolate (Harmon-Jones, 2000d). Participants in the high choice condition changed their

attitudes to dislike the chocolate. In addition, self-reported negative affect was increased

following dissonance-producing behavior and was reduced following the attitude change.

The results obtained in these and other experiments demonstrate that dissonance affect and

dissonance-related attitude change can occur in situations in which a cognitive inconsistency

is present but the production of aversive consequences is not present. They also demonstrate

that the experience of cognitive dissonance evokes an unpleasant motivational state that

motivates dissonance reduction. These experiments have supported the original conception

of dissonance theory over the revisions. But why does dissonance evoke this negative

Page 52: FYP-Rafia

motivational state? Why is inconsistency aversive? Festinger proposed no answer to the

question of what underlies dissonance processes.

2.8.0 CONCLUSION ACCORDING TO MY REVIEW

In summary, the cognitive-dissonance theory is a part of our everyday lives, whether we

realize it or not. When we are presented with viewpoints or opinions that differ from our

own often times we feel dissonance. We, as human beings, are always striving to keep our

lives in balance. Often a balance in our psyche requires that we not heed the warnings of

things to come. As I have shown, cognitive-dissonance is utilized to avoid taking actions.

The chapter discussed the effects of cognitive dissonance on consumers. The literature

review shows that when consumers are faced with cognitive dissonance they seek ways in

which to reduce this phenomenon. It also shows that marketers can develop ways in which

they can help reduce the effects of cognitive dissonance as these can also affect their

products. It also shows that consumers will seek advice from friends or family when faced

with cognitive dissonance.

As many theorists have stated cognitive dissonance does create an internal conflict that

causes someone to take action. Apparently no matter how much dissonance is felt and to

what degree it is felt does not matter. Therefore, it may not be possible to get rid of

dissonance or even to reduce it materially by changing one's behavior or feeling.

As any other customers, haircut clients also go through the same experience for many

different reasons. For most clients, selection of a hair salon for a good haircut is a decision

of considerable involvement, which can cause major changes in ones attitude and outlook.

Given the level of involvement of the decision about the hair salon, some clients may feel

this discomfort at some point in their lives. Many hair salons teach their customer-contact

people how to resolve problems and diffuse customer anger. Some hair dressers go to

Page 53: FYP-Rafia

extremes to see things the customer’s way and to reward complaining, seemingly without

regard for profit impact.

3.0.0 METHODOLOGY

In this section I will discuss the procedure used to gather data and the statistical techniques

used to analyze the data. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (version 15.0).

3.1.0 Research Design

For the purpose of this study the research design used is the descriptive research method to

collect data and to find latent negative factors leading to post-purchase dissonance in

females after a haircut. The technique proposed to conduct this descriptive research was

through a survey with the assistance of a questionnaire which gave me clear and direct

access to female’s views as to what makes them feel dissatisfied after they get a haircut.

3.2.0 Survey Instrument

Two questionnaires were designed to find the major underlying negative factors leading to

the post-purchase dissonance in females after a haircut.

The first questionnaire was open ended in which 50 female respondents (25 Young females :

15-21 years of age) and (25 Old females : ≤ 27 years of age) were asked to state ten negative

factors/reasons they think, consider or usually discuss with others while talking about their

views after they had gone for a haircut. The results helped compile 26 negative factors in all,

which females think lead to post-purchase dissonance after a haircut.

Using the results of the first questionnaire, second closed-ended questionnaire was

constructed, in which random 130 female respondents were asked to state accuracy or

Page 54: FYP-Rafia

inaccuracy of the negative factors from their own experience and observation on a five-point

Likert Scale. The respondents indicated their degree of agreement with the factor by giving

a numerical value corresponding with their level of agreement.

Both the questionnaires were designed to provide simplicity, understandability,

comprehensiveness and ease of recording.

3.3.0 Sample Size & Statistical Methods

The sample study used tells the variability and reliability of the data of the respondents.

The selected sample size for the first questionnaire were 50 female respondents (25 Young

females: 15-21 years of age) and (25 Old females: ≤ 27 years of age) and for the second

questionnaire random 130 female respondents were selected.

The statistical methods involved those of inferential (exploratory factor analysis [EFA] and

principal components analysis [PCA]) and descriptive (mean and standard deviation)

statistics. Statistical analysis was performed on the results using SPSS (version 15.0) to

discover the major latent factors resulting in the post-purchase dissonance in females after a

haircut.

3.4.0 Inferential Statistical Tools

Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and principal components analysis (PCA) both are

methods that are used to help investigators represent a large number of relationships among

interval-level variables in a simpler (more parsimonious) way. Both of these approaches

allow the computer to determine which, of a fairly large set of items, "hang together" as a

group, or are answered most similarly by the participants.

The primary difference, conceptually, between exploratory factor analysis and principal

components analysis is that in EFA. one postulates that there is a smaller set of unobserved

(latent) variables or constructs that underlie the variables that actually were observed or

Page 55: FYP-Rafia

measured; whereas, in PCA. one is simply trying to mathematically derive a relatively small

number of variables to use to convey as much of the information in the observed/measured

variables as possible.

In other words, EFA is directed at understanding the relations among variables by

understanding the constructs that underlie them, whereas PCA is simply directed toward

enabling one to use fewer variables to provide the same information that one would obtain

from a larger set of variables. There are actually a number of different ways of computing

factors for factor analysis; but in this research project, I have only used one of these

methods, principal axis factor analysis (PA).

I selected this approach because it is highly similar mathematically to PCA. The primary

difference, computationally, between PCA and PA is that in the former, the analysis

typically is performed on an ordinary correlation matrix, complete with the correlations of

each item or variable with itself, whereas in PA factor analysis, the correlation matrix is

modified such that the correlations of each item with itself are replaced with a

"communality"—a measure of that item's relation to all other items (usually a squared

multiple correlation). Thus, PCA is trying to reproduce all information (variance and

covariance) associated with the set of variables, whereas PA factor analysis is directed at

understanding only the co-variation among variables.

Page 56: FYP-Rafia

4.0.0 RESARCH FRAMEWORK

The research framework below describes the connection between post purchase dissonance

experienced by females after a haircut and the latent negative factors leading to it. It also

shows the negative impact it has on females to make them dissatisfied.

Six major underlying negative factors that result in post-purchase dissonance in young and

old females are, Inconsistency between actions and beliefs, Irrevocability of Action,

Consciousness, Importance of Decision, Service Dissonance and Covert Actions. My

research focus is on young females (15-21 years of age) and old females (≤ 27 years of age).

Following is the chart given in support of the above described text.

Personal Internal Factors

External Factors

Inconsistency between Actions & Beliefs

Consciousness

Importance of Decision

Irrevocability of Action

Service Dissonance

Covert Actions

PostPurchase

Dissonance

Page 57: FYP-Rafia

5.0.0 DATA ANALYSIS

5.1.0 FACTOR ANALYSIS

5.1.1 PRINCIPAL COMPONENT ANALYSIS

Communalities

1.000 .661

1.000 .814

1.000 .636

1.000 .728

1.000 .610

1.000 .634

1.000 .745

1.000 .683

1.000 .744

1.000 .667

1.000 .636

1.000 .605

1.000 .709

1.000 .734

1.000 .605

1.000 .737

1.000 .553

1.000 .667

1.000 .533

1.000 .493

1.000 .562

1.000 .623

1.000 .595

1.000 .533

1.000 .728

1.000 .698

Uneven haircutIn-expertise of the hairdresserHaircut incompatiblewith personalityPrice paid is not worththe resultHaircut is too shortHairstyling tools are nothygienicHair dresser rushing thehaircut due to lessnumber of staffWrong decision ofhairstyleNobody accompanyingto adviseNegative commentsfrom peopleNew hairstyle is wellsupported only withblow dryPoor cleaning serviceafter the haircutLong hours of waitingNo refund policy after abad haircutVery ExpensiveUnable to experimentwith new hair stylesHaircut does not give anew lookUnable to communicateone's desired hair styleNot getting hair cut fromthe regular stylistHair texture and faceshape does not supportthe new hair styleThe hair cut goesunnoticed by everyoneComments regardinghair's health by the hairdresserHair dresser's constanttalking and gossipprolonging the haircutMisguiding the clientwith calling every style avolume cutComplicated blow dryhides the flaws of thehaircutHair dresser does notlisten to the client'sconcerns

Initial Extraction

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

5.1.2 TOTAL VARIANCE EXPLAINED

INTERPRETATION:

In principal component analysis i.e.

a correlation matrix which is

modified such that the correlations

of each item with itself are replaced

with a "communality"—a measure

of that item's relation to all other

items (usually a squared multiple

correlation), when applied to the

negative factors collected from the

questionnaires showed that all

communalities are closely related

and the data collected is very

strong.

Page 58: FYP-Rafia

Total Variance Explained

6.905 26.556 26.556 6.905 26.556 26.556 4.1222.623 10.089 36.645 2.623 10.089 36.645 3.2492.315 8.904 45.549 2.315 8.904 45.549 2.9892.182 8.392 53.941 2.182 8.392 53.941 2.8451.540 5.923 59.864 1.540 5.923 59.864 1.9851.367 5.257 65.121 1.367 5.257 65.121 1.741.981 3.772 68.893.917 3.528 72.420.802 3.084 75.504.746 2.867 78.371.683 2.627 80.998.627 2.411 83.409.556 2.137 85.546.530 2.038 87.583.492 1.894 89.478.486 1.869 91.346.397 1.528 92.874.348 1.337 94.211.298 1.146 95.356.291 1.120 96.477.231 .890 97.367.215 .827 98.193.145 .558 98.751.130 .500 99.251.111 .428 99.679.083 .321 100.000

Component1234567891011121314151617181920212223242526

Total % of Variance Cumulative % Total % of Variance Cumulative % Total

Initial Eigenvalues Extraction Sums of Squared Loadings Rotation Sums of Squared Loadings

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

INTERPRETATION:

The Total Variance Explained table shows how the variance is divided among the 26

possible factors. Note that 26 factors have eigenvalues (a measure of explained variance)

greater than 1.0, which is a common criterion for a factor to be useful. When the eigenvalue

is less than 1.0, this means that the factor explains less information than a single item would

have explained.

Page 59: FYP-Rafia

5.1.3 SCREE PLOT

Component Number2625242322212019181716151413121110987654321

Eig

enva

lue

6

4

2

0

Scree Plot

INTERPRETATION:

It is the graphical representation of the table Total Variance Explained and visually shows

the number of extracted factors i.e. 6 factors.

Page 60: FYP-Rafia

5.1.4 FACTOR/COMPONENT MATRIX

Component Matrixa

.730

.714

.678 -.410

.649

.632

.629

.628 .412

.605

.566 .474

.561

.558 -.435

.541 .453

.524 -.470

.522

.461 -.450

.420

.658

.629

.441 -.492 .400

.410 .422

-.607

.436 .547

.488 .585

.533

.416 .589

.444 .487 .501

Price paid is not worththe resultUnable to communicateone's desired hair styleNegative commentsfrom peopleUneven haircutHair texture and faceshape does not supportthe new hair styleHaircut incompatiblewith personalityWrong decision ofhairstyleHaircut is too shortIn-expertise of the hairdresserHaircut does not give anew lookThe hair cut goesunnoticed by everyoneNobody accompanyingto adviseHair dresser rushing thehaircut due to lessnumber of staffMisguiding the clientwith calling every style avolume cutHair dresser's constanttalking and gossipprolonging the haircutHairstyling tools are nothygienicComments regardinghair's health by the hairdresserNew hairstyle is wellsupported only withblow dryUnable to experimentwith new hair stylesNot getting hair cut fromthe regular stylistPoor cleaning serviceafter the haircutComplicated blow dryhides the flaws of thehaircutLong hours of waitingHair dresser does notlisten to the client'sconcernsVery ExpensiveNo refund policy after abad haircut

1 2 3 4 5 6

Component

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

6 components extracted.a.

Page 61: FYP-Rafia

INTERPRETATION:

For this analysis in this research project, I have used an orthogonal rotation (varimax). This

means that the final factors should be as uncorrelated as possible with each other. As a

result, we can assume that the information explained by one factor is independent of the

information in the other factors. I rotated the factors so that they would be easier to interpret.

Rotation made it possible that different items are now explained or predicted by different

underlying factors, and each factor explained more than one item. This is a condition called

simple structure. One thing to look for in the Rotated Matrix of factor loadings is the

extent to which simple structure is achieved.

Page 62: FYP-Rafia

5.1.5 ROTATED FACTOR MATRIX

Rotated Component Matrixa

.812

.725

.634

.624 .464

.619

.565 .480

.553

.550 .479

.838

.835

.736

.435

.764

.733

.659

.577

.751

.672

.658

.655

.818

.494 .562 .485 .527

.520

.707

-.609

Unable to experimentwith new hair stylesHaircut is too shortNegative commentsfrom peopleNobody accompanyingto adviseHaircut does not give anew lookUnable to communicateone's desired hair styleHair texture and faceshape does not supportthe new hair styleThe hair cut goesunnoticed by everyoneHair dresser rushing thehaircut due to lessnumber of staffIn-expertise of the hairdresserUneven haircutHaircut incompatiblewith personalityNew hairstyle is wellsupported only withblow dryComments regardinghair's health by the hairdresserHairstyling tools are nothygienicPoor cleaning serviceafter the haircutHair dresser does notlisten to the client'sconcernsPrice paid is not worththe resultMisguiding the clientwith calling every style avolume cutWrong decision ofhairstyleNo refund policy after abad haircutLong hours of waitingVery ExpensiveNot getting hair cut fromthe regular stylistComplicated blow dryhides the flaws of thehaircutHair dresser's constanttalking and gossipprolonging the haircut

1 2 3 4 5 6

Component

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.

Rotation converged in 12 iterations.a.

Page 63: FYP-Rafia

INTERPRETATION:

The Rotated Factor Matrix table, which contains loadings, is a key for understanding the

results of the analysis. Note that the computer has sorted the 26 questions (item 01 to item

26) into 6 overlapping groups of items, each which has a loading of |.40| or higher (|.40|

means the absolute value, or value without considering the sign, is greater than .40).

Actually, every item has some loading from every factor, but there are blanks in the matrix

where weights were less than |.40|. Within each factor, the items are sorted from the one

with the highest factor weight or loading for that factor. Loadings resulting from an

orthogonal rotation are correlation coefficients of each item with the factor, so they range

from -1.0 through 0 to + 1.0. A negative loading just means that the question needs to be

interpreted in the opposite direction from the way it is written for that factor. Usually, factor

loadings lower than |.40| are considered low, which is why I suppressed loadings less than

|.40|.

Page 64: FYP-Rafia

5.1.6 FACTOR NAMING

Factor 1:

Factor # Factor (Attribute) Description Value Extracted Attribute

16 Unable to experiment with new hair styles .812

Inconsistency between

Actions & Beliefs

5 Haircut is too short .725

10 Negative comments from people .634

9 Nobody accompanying to advise .624

17 Haircut does not give a new look .619

18 Unable to communicate one's desired hair style .565

20 Hair texture and face shape does not support the new hair style .553

21 The hair cut goes unnoticed by everyone .550

Factor 2:

Factor # Factor (Attribute) Description Value Extracted Attribute

7 Hair dresser rushing the haircut due to less number of staff .838

Irrevocability of Action

2 In-expertise of the hair dresser .835

1 Uneven haircut .736

3 Haircut incompatible with personality .435

Factor 3:

Factor # Factor (Attribute) Description Value Extracted Attribute

11 New hairstyle is well supported only with blow dry .764

Consciousness22 Comments regarding hair's health by the hair dresser .733

6 Hairstyling tools are not hygienic .659

12 Poor cleaning service after the haircut .577

Page 65: FYP-Rafia

Factor 4:

Factor # Factor (Attribute) Description Value Extracted Attribute

26 Hair dresser does not listen to the client's concerns .751

Importance of Decision

4 Price paid is not worth the result .672

24 Misguiding the client with calling every style a volume cut .658

8 Wrong decision of hairstyle .655

Factor 5:

Factor # Factor (Attribute) Description Value Extracted Attribute

14 No refund policy after a bad haircut .818

Service Dissonance

13 Long hours of waiting .562

15 Very Expensive .527

19 Not getting hair cut from the regular stylist .520

Factor 6:

Factor # Factor (Attribute) Description Value Extracted Attribute

25 Complicated blow dry hides the flaws of the haircut .707

Covert Actions23 Hair dresser's constant talking and gossip

prolonging the haircut -.609

5.2.0 FREQUENCY ANALYSIS

Page 66: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 1) - Uneven haircut

Statistics

Uneven haircut130

04

1.154

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Uneven haircut

4 3.1 3.1 3.122 16.9 16.9 20.022 16.9 16.9 36.944 33.8 33.8 70.838 29.2 29.2 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Uneven haircutVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Uneven haircut

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Uneven haircut had the response as Quite Accurate by 33.8% of the respondents.(FACTOR 2) - In-expertise of the hair dresser

Page 67: FYP-Rafia

Statistics

In-expertise of the hair dresser130

03

1.087

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

In-expertise of the hair dresser

3 2.3 2.3 2.333 25.4 25.4 27.739 30.0 30.0 57.734 26.2 26.2 83.821 16.2 16.2 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

In-expertise of the hair dresserVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

30

20

10

0

In-expertise of the hair dresser

INTERPRETATION:

The factor In-expertise of the hair dresser had the response as I am Neutral by 30% of

the respondents.

(FACTOR 3) - Haircut incompatible with personality

Page 68: FYP-Rafia

Statistics

Haircut incompatible with personality130

04

1.295

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Haircut incompatible with personality

11 8.5 8.5 8.523 17.7 17.7 26.221 16.2 16.2 42.339 30.0 30.0 72.336 27.7 27.7 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Haircut incompatible with personalityVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

30

20

10

0

Haircut incompatible with personality

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Haircut incompatible with personality had the response as Quite Accurate by

30% of the respondents.

(FACTOR 4) - Price paid is not worth the result

Page 69: FYP-Rafia

Statistics

Price paid is not worth the result130

04

1.188

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Price paid is not worth the result

9 6.9 6.9 6.925 19.2 19.2 26.230 23.1 23.1 49.242 32.3 32.3 81.524 18.5 18.5 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Price paid is not worth the resultVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Price paid is not worth the result

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Price paid is not worth the result had the response as Quite Accurate by

32.3% of the respondents.

Page 70: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 5) - Haircut is too short

Statistics

Haircut is too short130

04

1.103

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Haircut is too short

4 3.1 3.1 3.116 12.3 12.3 15.418 13.8 13.8 29.250 38.5 38.5 67.742 32.3 32.3 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Haircut is too shortVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Haircut is too short

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Haircut is too short had the response as Quite Accurate by 38.5% of the

respondents.

Page 71: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 6) - Hairstyling tools are not hygienic

Statistics

Hairstyling tools are not hygienic130

04

1.233

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Hairstyling tools are not hygienic

15 11.5 11.5 11.518 13.8 13.8 25.435 26.9 26.9 52.340 30.8 30.8 83.122 16.9 16.9 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Hairstyling tools are not hygienicVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Hairstyling tools are not hygienic

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Hairstyling tools are not hygienic had the response as Quite Accurate by

30.8% of the respondents.

Page 72: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 7) - Hair dresser rushing the haircut due to less number of staff

Statistics

Hair dresser rushing the haircut due to less number of staff130

05

1.375

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Hair dresser rushing the haircut due to less number of staff

17 13.1 13.1 13.121 16.2 16.2 29.225 19.2 19.2 48.532 24.6 24.6 73.135 26.9 26.9 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Hair dresser rushing the haircut due to less number of staffVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

30

20

10

0

Hair dresser rushing the haircut due to less number of staff

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Hair dresser rushing the haircut due to less number of staff had the response

as Very Accurate by 26.9% of the respondents.

Page 73: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 8) - Wrong decision of hairstyle

Statistics

Wrong decision of hairstyle130

05

1.228

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Wrong decision of hairstyle

6 4.6 4.6 4.621 16.2 16.2 20.832 24.6 24.6 45.428 21.5 21.5 66.943 33.1 33.1 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Wrong decision of hairstyleVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Wrong decision of hairstyle

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Wrong decision of hairstyle had the response as Very Accurate by 33.1% of

the respondents.

Page 74: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR9) - Nobody accompanying to advise

Statistics

Nobody accompanying to advise130

04

1.156

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Nobody accompanying to advise

8 6.2 6.2 6.215 11.5 11.5 17.717 13.1 13.1 30.856 43.1 43.1 73.834 26.2 26.2 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Nobody accompanying to adviseVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

50

40

30

20

10

0

Nobody accompanying to advise

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Nobody accompanying to advise had the response as Quite Accurate by

43.1% of the respondents.

Page 75: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 10) - Negative comments from people

Statistics

Negative comments from people130

02

1.405

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Negative comments from people

15 11.5 11.5 11.538 29.2 29.2 40.88 6.2 6.2 46.9

37 28.5 28.5 75.432 24.6 24.6 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Negative comments from peopleVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

30

20

10

0

Negative comments from people

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Negative comments from people had the response as Quite In-Accurate by

29.2% of the respondents.

Page 76: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 11) - New hairstyle is well supported only with blow dry

Statistics

New hairstyle is well supported only with blow dry130

05

1.417

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

New hairstyle is well supported only with blow dry

19 14.6 14.6 14.610 7.7 7.7 22.34 3.1 3.1 25.4

46 35.4 35.4 60.851 39.2 39.2 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

New hairstyle is well supported only with blow dryVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

New hairstyle is well supported only with blow dry

INTERPRETATION:

The factor New hairstyle is well supported only with blow dry had the response as Very

Accurate by 39.2% of the respondents.

Page 77: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 12) - Poor cleaning service after the haircut

Statistics

Poor cleaning service after the haircut130

05

1.259

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Poor cleaning service after the haircut

7 5.4 5.4 5.424 18.5 18.5 23.834 26.2 26.2 50.024 18.5 18.5 68.541 31.5 31.5 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Poor cleaning service after the haircutVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Poor cleaning service after the haircut

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Poor cleaning service after the haircut had the response as Very Accurate by

31.5% of the respondents.

Page 78: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 13) - Long hours of waiting

Statistics

Long hours of waiting130

04

1.386

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Long hours of waiting

26 20.0 20.0 20.025 19.2 19.2 39.217 13.1 13.1 52.343 33.1 33.1 85.419 14.6 14.6 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Long hours of waitingVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Long hours of waiting

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Long hours of waiting had the response as Quite Accurate by 33.1% of the

respondents.

Page 79: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 14) - No refund policy after a bad haircut

Statistics

No refund policy after a bad haircut130

05

1.501

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

No refund policy after a bad haircut

27 20.8 20.8 20.86 4.6 4.6 25.4

31 23.8 23.8 49.223 17.7 17.7 66.943 33.1 33.1 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

No refund policy after a bad haircutVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

No refund policy after a bad haircut

INTERPRETATION:

The factor No refund policy after a bad haircut had the response as Very Accurate by

33.1% of the respondents.

Page 80: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 15) - Very Expensive

Statistics

Very Expensive130

05

1.295

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Very Expensive

14 10.8 10.8 10.815 11.5 11.5 22.332 24.6 24.6 46.934 26.2 26.2 73.135 26.9 26.9 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Very ExpensiveVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

30

20

10

0

Very Expensive

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Very Expensive had the response as Very Accurate by 26.9% of the

respondents.

Page 81: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 16) - Unable to experiment with new hair styles

Statistics

Unable to experiment with new hair styles130

04

1.274

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Unable to experiment with new hair styles

14 10.8 10.8 10.812 9.2 9.2 20.022 16.9 16.9 36.947 36.2 36.2 73.135 26.9 26.9 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Unable to experiment with new hair stylesVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Unable to experiment with new hair styles

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Unable to experiment with new hair styles had the response as Quite Accurate

by 36.2% of the respondents.

Page 82: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 17) - Haircut does not give a new look

Statistics

Haircut does not give a new look130

04

1.471

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Haircut does not give a new look

26 20.0 20.0 20.026 20.0 20.0 40.014 10.8 10.8 50.835 26.9 26.9 77.729 22.3 22.3 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Haircut does not give a new lookVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Perc

en

t

30

20

10

0

Haircut does not give a new look

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Haircut does not give a new look had the response as Quite Accurate by

26.9% of the respondents.

Page 83: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 18) - Unable to communicate one's desired hair style

Statistics

Unable to communicate one's desired hair style130

04

1.278

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Unable to communicate one's desired hair style

13 10.0 10.0 10.024 18.5 18.5 28.528 21.5 21.5 50.037 28.5 28.5 78.528 21.5 21.5 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Unable to communicate one's desired hair styleVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

30

20

10

0

Unable to communicate one's desired hair style

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Unable to communicate one's desired hair style had the response as Quite

Accurate by 28.5% of the respondents.

Page 84: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 19) - Not getting hair cut from the regular stylist

Statistics

Not getting hair cut from the regular stylist130

04

1.202

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Not getting hair cut from the regular stylist

9 6.9 6.9 6.913 10.0 10.0 16.928 21.5 21.5 38.541 31.5 31.5 70.039 30.0 30.0 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Not getting hair cut from the regular stylistVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Not getting hair cut from the regular stylist

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Not getting hair cut from the regular stylist had the response as Quite

Accurate by 31.5% of the respondents.

Page 85: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 20) - Hair texture and face shape does not support the new hair style

Statistics

Hair texture and face shape does not support the new hair style130

04

1.149

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Hair texture and face shape does not support the new hair style

8 6.2 6.2 6.216 12.3 12.3 18.532 24.6 24.6 43.145 34.6 34.6 77.729 22.3 22.3 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Hair texture and face shape does not support the new hair styleVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Hair texture and face shape does not support the new hair style

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Hair texture and face shape does not support the new hair style had the

response as Quite Accurate by 34.6% of the respondents.

Page 86: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 21) - The hair cut goes unnoticed by everyone

Statistics

The hair cut goes unnoticed by everyone130

04

1.258

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

The hair cut goes unnoticed by everyone

13 10.0 10.0 10.020 15.4 15.4 25.421 16.2 16.2 41.549 37.7 37.7 79.227 20.8 20.8 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

The hair cut goes unnoticed by everyoneVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

The hair cut goes unnoticed by everyone

INTERPRETATION:

The factor The hair cut goes unnoticed by everyone had the response as Quite Accurate

by 37.7% of the respondents.

Page 87: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 22) - Comments regarding hair's health by the hair dresser

Statistics

Comments regarding hair's health by the hair dresser130

04

1.240

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Comments regarding hair's health by the hair dresser

11 8.5 8.5 8.520 15.4 15.4 23.816 12.3 12.3 36.253 40.8 40.8 76.930 23.1 23.1 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Comments regarding hair's health by the hair dresserVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

50

40

30

20

10

0

Comments regarding hair's health by the hair dresser

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Comments regarding hair's health by the hair dresser had the response as

Quite Accurate by 40.8% of the respondents.

Page 88: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 23) - Hair dresser's constant talking and gossip prolonging the haircut

Statistics

Hair dresser's constant talking and gossipprolonging the haircut

13004

1.244

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Hair dresser's constant talking and gossip prolonging the haircut

11 8.5 8.5 8.512 9.2 9.2 17.720 15.4 15.4 33.145 34.6 34.6 67.742 32.3 32.3 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Hair dresser's constant talking and gossip prolonging the haircutVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Hair dresser's constant talking and gossip prolonging the haircut

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Hair dresser's constant talking and gossip prolonging the haircut had the

response as Quite Accurate by 34.6% of the respondents.

Page 89: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 24) - Misguiding the client with calling every style a volume cut

Statistics

Misguiding the client with calling every style a volume cut130

04

1.384

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Misguiding the client with calling every style a volume cut

19 14.6 14.6 14.615 11.5 11.5 26.226 20.0 20.0 46.236 27.7 27.7 73.833 25.4 25.4 99.21 .8 .8 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery Accurate6Total

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Misguiding the client with calling every style a volume cut6Very AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

30

20

10

0

Misguiding the client with calling every style a volume cut

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Misguiding the client with calling every style a volume cut had the response as

Quite Accurate by 27.7% of the respondents.

Page 90: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 25) - Complicated blow dry hides the flaws of the haircut

Statistics

Complicated blow dry hides the flaws of the haircut130

04

1.282

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Complicated blow dry hides the flaws of the haircut

11 8.5 8.5 8.521 16.2 16.2 24.618 13.8 13.8 38.544 33.8 33.8 72.336 27.7 27.7 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Complicated blow dry hides the flaws of the haircutVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Complicated blow dry hides the flaws of the haircut

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Complicated blow dry hides the flaws of the haircut had the response as Quite

Accurate by 33.8% of the respondents.

Page 91: FYP-Rafia

(FACTOR 26) - Hair dresser does not listen to the client's concerns

Statistics

Hair dresser does not listen to the client's concerns130

05

1.380

ValidMissing

N

ModeStd. Deviation

Hair dresser does not listen to the client's concerns

18 13.8 13.8 13.813 10.0 10.0 23.835 26.9 26.9 50.824 18.5 18.5 69.240 30.8 30.8 100.0

130 100.0 100.0

Very InaccurateQuiet InaccurateI am NeutralQuiet AccurateVery AccurateTotal

ValidFrequency Percent Valid Percent

CumulativePercent

Hair dresser does not listen to the client's concernsVery AccurateQuiet AccurateI am NeutralQuiet InaccurateVery Inaccurate

Pe

rce

nt

40

30

20

10

0

Hair dresser does not listen to the client's concerns

INTERPRETATION:

The factor Hair dresser does not listen to the client's concerns had the response as Very

Accurate by 30.8% of the respondents.

Page 92: FYP-Rafia

6.0.0 CONCLUSION

The primary objective of the study was to find the key driving factors which lead to post-

purchase dissonance in females after a haircut.

Twenty six different negative factors were tested using (Exploratory factor analysis and

principal components analysis) to elaborate the study. The research concluded that there

are six major latent negative factors 1) Inconsistency between actions and beliefs, 2)

Irrevocability of Action, 3) Consciousness, 4) Importance of Decision, 5) Service

Dissonance, 6) Covert Actions. All of them are closely co-related and tend to create post-

purchase dissonance in females after a haircut.

People are very reluctant to talk openly, but I don’t see any harm in asking for what you

want. And if you are not sure what you want then ask them for a photo album of various

haircuts. Shortlist few of them and find out which one will best suit your facial structure,

your hair texture, length and body of the hair. Getting a good haircut is not that difficult.

You just need to have some presence of mind and preparation and of course a good

hairdresser.

These results support the cognitive dissonance theory of (Leon Festinger, 1957) and of other

theorists mentioned in the literature review because the females surveyed for this research

are sensitive to inconsistencies between their actions and beliefs, irrevocability of their

decision/action, the importance of their decision, and recognition of these inconsistencies

cause dissonance, and will eventually motivate them to resolve the issue.

Page 93: FYP-Rafia

ANNEXURE I:

Page 94: FYP-Rafia

Dear Respondent,

Kindly write negative factors / reasons that you think, consider or usually discuss with others while talking about your views AFTER you had gone for a haircut. It simply means post – haircut negative factors that you consider and point to yourself.

QUESTIONNAIRE 1

1. __________________________

2. __________________________

3. __________________________

4. __________________________

5. __________________________

6. __________________________

7. __________________________

8. __________________________

9. __________________________

10. __________________________

Page 95: FYP-Rafia

Dear Respondent,

Following are the negative factors / attributes that usually are discussed by females AFTER they had gone for a haircut. Please rate each factor that how accurate these factors are to your own experience and observation. Use following scale to rate your accuracy opinion.

Sr. #

Negative Factors Your Rating

1 2 3 4 5

1 Uneven haircut

2 In-expertise of the hair dresser

3 Haircut incompatible with personality

4 Price paid is not worth the result

5 Haircut is too short

6 Hairstyling tools are not hygienic

7 Hair dresser rushing the haircut due to less number of staff

8 Wrong decision of hairstyle

9 Nobody accompanying to advise

10 Negative comments from people

11 New hairstyle is well supported only with blow dry

12 Poor cleaning service after the haircut

13 Long hours of waiting

14 No refund policy after a bad haircut

15 Very Expensive

16 Unable to experiment with new hair styles

17 Haircut does not give a new look

18 Unable to communicate one's desired hair style

19 Not getting hair cut from the regular stylist

20 Hair texture and face shape does not support the new hair style

21 The hair cut goes unnoticed by everyone

22 Comments regarding hair's health by the hair dresser

23 Hair dresser's constant talking and gossip prolonging the haircut

QUESTIONNAIRE 2

Very Inaccurate

Quite Inaccurate

I am not SureQuite

AccurateVery Accurate

1 2 3 4 5

Page 96: FYP-Rafia

24 Misguiding the client with calling every style a volume cut

25 Complicated blow dry hides the flaws of the haircut

26 Hair dresser does not listen to the client's concerns

ANNEXURE II:

REFERENCES

Jones, E. (1985). “Major Developments in Social Psychology During the Past Five Decades,” Handbook of Social Psychology: Vol I, Theory and Methods, Eds. G. Lindzey and E. Aronson, 3rd Edition, 47 – 107.

Page 97: FYP-Rafia

Sweeney, J., Hausknecht, D., & Soutar, G. (2000). “Cognitive Dissonance after Purchase: AMultidimensional Scale,” Psychology & Marketing, Vol 17, No. 5, p. 369-385.

Landman, J. (1993). “Regret: The Persistence of the Possible,” New York, Oxford University Press.

Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 38 (1), 2007, 7–16

Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Festinger, L. and Carlsmith, J.M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 58, 203-210.

Haber, H., Leach, A., Schudy, S. and Sideleau, B. (1982). Comprehensive Psychiatric Nursing. (2nd ed.) New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc.

Aaker, D.A., Kumar, V., and Day, G.S. 2003. Marketing Research. Eighth Edition. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Arens, W.F. 2004. Contemporary Advertising. Ninth Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Arens, W.F. 2002. Marketing and Consumer Behaviour: The Foundations of Advertising. Eighth Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Armstrong, G., and Kotler, P. 1999. Marketing. Fifth Edition. London: Prentice Hall.

Arnould, E., Price, L., and Zinkhan, G. 2004. Consumers. International Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Aronson, E., Wilson, A., and Akert, B.K. 2001. Dissonance Theory. Psychological Inquiry. 3: 303 – 311.

Bahk, M.E. 2001. Emotional Dissonance. Online. Available On: http//books.nap.edu/books/0309049784/html/99.html/#pagetop. [Accessed On: 05 April 2005].

Baker, K.M. 2000. Expectations of Social Behavior and Cognitive Dissonance. Journal of Psychology. 6(52): 55-57 Bateson, J.E.G., and Hoffman, K.D. 1999. Managing Services Marketing. Fourth Edition. Fort Worth: The Dryden Press.

Baumeister, R.F., Smart, L., and Boden, J.M. 1999. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 9: 121 – 127.

Page 98: FYP-Rafia

Bearden, S., Ingram, L.E., and LaForge, P.J. 2004. Marketing Principles and Perspective.Online.AvailableOn:http://highered.mcgrawhill.com/sites/d1/free/0074709763/117142/13.2002. [Accessed On: 11 May 2005].

Beauvois, J.L., and Joul, R.V. 1999. A Radical Dissonance Theory. London: Taylor & Francis.

Belch, G.E. and Belch, M.A. 2001. Advertising and Promotion: An Integrated Marketing Communications Perspective. Fifth Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Berman, B. and Evans, J.R. 1998. Retail Management: A Strategic Approach. Seventh Edition. London: Prentice Hall.

Blumberg, B., Cooper, D.R., and Schindler, P.S. 2005. Business Research Methods. London: McGraw-Hill.

Boyd, JR. H.W., Walker, JR. O.C., Mullins, J.W., and Larrenche, J. 2002. Marketing Management: A Strategic Decision-Making Approach. Fourth Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Bozzi, E. 2002. Expectations of Social Behaviour and Cognitive Dissonance among College Freshman. Doctoral Dissertation. New York University. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B.p3876.

Burnes, B. 2004. Managing Change. Fourth Edition. London: Prentice Hall.

Burnes, B. 2000. Managing Change: A Strategic Approach to Organisational Dynamics. Third Edition. London: Prentice Hall.

Burnett, J. 2002. Core Concepts of Marketing. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Burris, C.T., Harmon-Jones, E., and Tarpley, W.R. 1997. By Faith Alone Religious

Agitation and Cognitive Dissonance. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 19: 17 - 31.

Cant, M. 2003. Marketing Research. Claremont: New African Books.

Cant, M.C., Brink, A., and Brijball, S. 2002. Customer Behaviour: A Southern African Perspective. Cape Town: Juta.

Cassel, R.N., Chow, P., DeMoulin, D.F., and Reiger, R.C. 2001. The Cognitive DissonanceTest.Online.AvailableOn:http://www.findarticles.com/p/articles/mi_qa3673/is_200110/ai_n8976559. [Accessed On: 26 August 2005].

Chow, P. and Thompson, I.S. 2003. Personal Development and Cognitive Dissonance.Online.AvailableOn:http://gateway.proquest.com/openurl? Url_ver=Z39.88. [Accessed on: 29 October 2004].

Page 99: FYP-Rafia

Chow, P., Thompson, I.S., Wood, W., Beauchamp, M., and Lebrun, R. 2002.

Comparing the Personality Development of Post-Secondary Students, High School Students, and the Incarcerated Individuals in a Democratic Society. Education. 123(1): 167-179.

Cooper, D.R. and Schindler, P.S. 2003. Business Research Methods. Eighth Edition. Boston: McGraw Hill.

Czinkota, M.R., Dickson, P.R., Dunne, P., Griffin, A., and Urbany, J.E. 2000. Marketing: Best Practices. Fort Worth: The Dryden Press.

Czinkota, M.R. and Kotabe, M. 2000. Marketing Management. Online. Available On: http://www.westga.edu/-mktreal/ch4mktgmgmt.ppt. [Accessed On: 05 April 2005].

Davies, B. and Ward, P. 2002. Managing Retail Consumption. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Dehne, G.C. 2004. The New Student Generation: Are we Ready? Do We Care? Online.AvailableOn:http://books.nap.edu/books/0309049784/html/99.html#pagetop. [Accessed On: 25 August 2005].

Denzin, N.K., and Lincoln, Y.S. 2003. The Landscape of Qualitative Research: Theories and Issues. Second Edition. London: Sage Publications.

Du Plessis, P.J., and Rousseau, G.G. 2003. Buyer Behaviour. Third Edition. Cape Town: Oxford University Press.

Dwyer, F.R. and Tanner, J.F. Business Marketing: Connecting Strategy, Relationship, and Learning. 2006. Third Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Dylan, B. 2003. Cognitive Dissonance: There’s No Success Like Failure. Online. Available on: http://skepdic.con/cognitivedissonance.html. [Accessed on: 18 June 2004].

Elliot, A.J., and Devine, P.G. 1998. On the Motivational Nature of Cognitive Dissonance: Dissonance as Psychological Discomfort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67: 382 – 394.

Etzel, M.J., Walker, B.J., and Stanton, W.J. 2001. Marketing. Twelfth Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill. Festinger, L. 2003. Cognitive Dissonance. Online. Available on: http://www.wsuprof.com/cognitivedissonance.htm. [Accessed on: 18 June 2004].

Fill, C. 2002. Marketing Communications: Contexts, Strategies and Applications. Third Edition. London: Prentice Hall.

Fitzsimmons, J.A. and Fitzsimmons, M.J. 2004. Services Management: Operations, Strategy, and Information Technology. Fourth Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Page 100: FYP-Rafia

Foxall, G.R., Goldsmith, R.E., and Brown, S. 2001. Consumer Psychology for Marketing. Second Edition. London: Thomson Learning.

Fuller, D.A. 1999. Sustainable Marketing: Managerial-Ecological Issues. London: Sage Publications.

Futrell, C.M. 2004. Fundamentals of Selling: Customers for Life through Service. Eighth Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Gilbert, A., Churchill, JR., and Peter, J.P. 1997. Marketing: Creating Value for Customers. Second Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Gilbert, D. 2003. Retail Marketing Management. Second Edition. London: Prentice Hall. Griffin, E. 1997. A First Look at Communication Theory. Third Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Hair, J.F., Babin, B., Money, A.H., and Samouel, P. 2003. Essentials of Business Research Methods. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Hair, J.F., Bush, R.P., and Ortinau, D.J. 2003. Marketing Research. Second Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Harmon-Jones, E. and Judson, M. 2003. Cognitive Dissonance: Progress on a Pivotal Theory in Social Psychology. Online. Available On: http://www.apa.org/books/4318830s.html. [Accessed On: 8 June 2004].

Hasty, R. and Reardon, J. 1997. Retail Management. New York: McGraw-Hill. Hawkins, D.I., Best, R.J., and Coney, K. 2001. Consumer Behavior: Building Marketing Strategy. Eighth Edition. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Hill, E. and O‟Sullivan, T. 1999. Marketing. Second Edition. New York: Longman. Jeroen, J. and Timmers, M. 2002. Emotional Dissonance: When the Experience of Emotions Jeopardizes an Individual‟s Identity. Journal of Theory and Psychology. 79 - 95.