four cultures of walking

Upload: butnariu-simona

Post on 03-Jun-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    1/13

    Four cultures

    four ways of perceiving and practicing walking?

    Eirin Olaussen Ryeng, Dr.ing.

    Department of Civil and Transport Engineering

    The Norwegian University of Science and Technology

    No-7491 Trondheim

    Norway

    email: [email protected]

    Abstract

    The grid-group theory origins from anthropology, and has been successfully applied within social and

    political sciences. The theory postulates that by categorising along the two axis of group and grid, four

    viable cultures can be identified. These are hierarchism, egalitarianism, individualism and fatalism, all of

    them being present in any group or society of people. These cultures, or worldviews, can be regarded aslenses through which each individual perceives and interprets the world. Thus, an individual adhering to

    one culture may have diverging preferences and behaviours from an individual adhering to another

    culture. These four cultures have been associated with four myths of nature, expressing four different

    ways of considering the vulnerability of nature. It can be hypothesised that individuals adhering to

    different cultures will have diverging preferences to sustainable modes of transport, which can be

    manifested through their actual choice of mode. If so, knowledge about each culture, especially about

    the mindsets of individuals adhering to any less walker friendly culture, would become essential in order

    to target efficient measures to promote walking.

    This paper presents a study in which grid-group theory is used to explore the travel preferences of a

    group of engineering students, both in Norway and in Argentina. Do students adhering to different

    cultures and believing in different myths of nature have different travel patterns and different views on

    transportation issues? Are there any patterns to be found? Are there any differences between

    Norwegian and Argentine students?

    The paper is based on a questionnaire study among 357 students. The results from the analyses will be

    discussed in order to identify any challenges to which policymakers should be aware when planning

    strategies to achieve a more sustainable transport pattern.

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    2/13

    Eirin Olaussen Ryeng is teaching transportation planning. Her research interests are: the link between

    transport system/infrastructure design and road user behaviour, as well as traffic safety, transport

    modelling, sustainable mobility, and universal design. She was a member of COST C6 1998-2001, and

    participated in the EU-funded project PROMPT 2000-2003.

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    3/13

    Four cultures

    four ways of perceiving and practicing walking?

    Eirin Olaussen Ryeng, Dr.ing.

    Department of Civil and Transport Engineering

    The Norwegian University of Science and Technology

    No-7491 Trondheim

    Norway

    email: [email protected]

    A good little typology

    In 1970, Mary Douglas first published her theory about four cultures observed to be found in every

    society or social group (Douglas, 1970). This theory was first labelled cultural theory and in later years

    it has also been referred to as grid-group theory (Perri 6 and Mars, 2008). The latter name origins in

    the 2x2-table based on the two dimensions grid and group, defining four categories, also named

    cultures. The theory is based on observations of how people act and respond to other people in a socialsetting. The dimension group reflects the degree to which persons adhere to and identify themselves

    to a group of other, while the dimension grid reflects the degree to which persons feel restricted or

    controlled by the group. By combining these two dimensions orthogonally, the four categories of grid-

    group theory are found: hierarchism (high group high grid), egalitarianism (high group low grid),

    individualism (low group low grid), and fatalism (low group high grid). According to the theory, all

    individuals could be placed in one of these quadrants1. These four cultures, or worldviews, can be

    regarded as lenses through which individuals perceive and interpret the world, thus defining four ways

    of life. An important feature of this theory, named the impossibility theorem, is that none of these

    cultures are viable without the other three (Thompson et.al., 1990). Consequently, none of them are

    any better or more favourable than the others. The theory simply describes a natural set of social

    cultures, and Mary Douglas herself described it many years later as a good little typology (Coyle, 1994).

    The grid-group theory origins from anthropology, but has later been successfully applied within various

    fields, such as sociology, economy, environmental sciences, engineering, political sciences,

    safety/security, law, history, geology, etc. (Thompson et.al, 1999). Although a standardised

    1Actually, one person can reside in more than one culture, for example adhere to individualism at work while

    adhering to hierarchism at home.

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    4/13

    questionnaire battery is still lacking, a number of studies have explored the possibilities in using

    questionnaires to operationalise the theory on individual level.

    A closer look at the four cultures

    Hierarchism

    Hierarchism is the culture found when combining a high group level with a high grid level. Hierarchism

    comprises strong groups with complex hierarchical structures. Individuals adhering to this culture

    supports tradition and order, as well as clear role definitions and rules of how to act. These are

    individuals who trust experts and authorities. There are strong interactions between individuals, and

    the welfare of the group as whole is emphasised.

    Egalitarianism

    Egalitarianism combines a high group level with a low grid level. This results in strong groups, but with

    weak structures. Since equality is pursued, any organisational form putting one person above another is

    perceived with high scepticism. Decisions should ideally be made by consensus. Individuals adhering to

    this culture support economic resources and other goods to be equally distributed.

    Individualism

    Individualism is the culture in which a low group level is combined with a low grid. Since their social

    bonds to other are loose, and they feel few constraints, authority is continuously challenged and role-

    based rules are not always followed by individuals adhering to individualism. Competition and survival

    of the strongest is supported by this culture, as well as a free market. Individuals adhering to

    individualism pursue liberty, and therefore libertarianism might have been as well suited as a label for

    this culture.

    Fatalism

    Fatalism is the fourth culture, combining a low group level with a high grid level. The combination of

    weak social bonds with the perception of being strongly tied and controlled by others forms a culture

    experiencing powerlessness. Individuals adhering to fatalism believe that the social system is essentially

    unpredictable and unfair, and that they have minimal control over their lives. Thus, they believe in fate,

    hoping to have luck and win the great lottery. Fatalism is often referred to as the passive culture,

    contrasting the other three active cultures (Douglas, 1992).

    Myths of nature

    Inspired from ecology, the theory has been expanded by introducing four myths of nature associated

    with each culture (Thompson, 1996). According to these myths hierarchism sees the nature as tolerant

    within definable limits. Keeping within these limits, nature can be relied upon to behave predictably.

    Egalitarianism, on the other hand, sees nature as ephemeral, fragile and unforgiving. Even small

    disturbances in nature may have the potential to cause catastrophic results. A clear opposite view is

    found among the individualism culture, in which nature is seen as benign, predictable, robust and stable.

    If large disturbances should occur, the nature will seek to and eventually return to equilibrium. The

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    5/13

    fatalism myth of nature sees nature as capricious and unpredictable, as it is impossible to foresee the

    outcome of any disturbance or action. For illustrating purpose a set of cartoons were developed

    showing these four myths of nature as balls balancing in a landscape, see Figure 1.

    Fatalism myth of nature

    The nature is capricious and unpredictable. We do

    not know what will happen.

    Hierarchism myth of nature

    The nature is perverse and tolerant. Within limits,

    nature can be relied upon to behave predictably.

    Individualism myth of nature

    The nature is benign; it is predictable, robust and

    stable, and will return to equilibrium even after large

    disturbances

    Egalitarianism myth of nature

    The nature is ephemeral; fragile and unforgiving.

    Small disturbances may have catastrophic results.

    Figure 1 The four myths of nature, cartoons copied from Grendstad and Selle (2000)

    Transportation issues of concern

    Also within the field of transportation, the grid-group theory has been applied. Hendriks (1994) and

    Hoppe and Grin (1999) have studied how the three active cultures (hierarchism, egalitarianism and

    individualism) impress transportation planning and policies. The cultures are reflected in thetransportation issues of concern, as well as in the preferred measures to cope with the problems.

    Hierarchism focuses on the provision of sufficient supply of infrastructure to meet the demand in order

    to ensure stable, predictable and controllable systems. High tech solutions to manage and improve

    traffic efficiency are welcomed. The main concern of egalitarianism is equality. Thus, equal access to

    public space by all kinds of road users (pedestrians, cyclists, public transport users and motorists) is the

    core focus, although only sustainable modes are regarded as acceptable. Consequently, the solution to

    congestion problems should be to reduce demand for car driving. Individualism, on the other hand, sees

    individual mobility as a fundamental good. Private cars and high speed is highly valued. Therefore, the

    solution to congestion problems should be to expand road capacity.

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    6/13

    A set of hypotheses

    It is suggested that there is a direct link from social relations, via worldviews (the four cultures of grid-

    group theory), to attitudes and behaviours (Boyle and Coughlin, 1994). It can therefore be hypothesised

    that individuals adhering to different cultures will have diverging attitudes and preferences to

    sustainable modes of transport. These attitudes and preferences are further expected to be manifested

    through their actual choice of mode. The following hypotheses are set out based on the typology

    description of the four cultures, emphasising the expected difference between individualism and

    egalitarianism as the most interesting:

    H1: People adhering to individualism are more likely to travel by car, while people adhering to

    egalitarianism are more likely to choose more sustainable modes of transport (walking, cycling and

    public transport).

    H2:People believing in the individualism myth of nature are more likely to travel by car, while people

    believing in the egalitarianism myth of nature are more likely to choose more sustainable modes of

    transport (walking, cycling and public transport).

    H3:People adhering to individualism walk shorter daily distances than other, while people adhering to

    egalitarianism walk longer daily distances than other.

    H4:People believing in the individualism myth of nature walk shorter daily distances than other, while

    people believing in the egalitarianism myth of nature walk longer daily distances than other.

    H5:People adhering to individualism are more likely to have no walking trips at all compared to other,

    while people adhering to egalitarianism are more likely to have at least one walking trip per daycompared to other.

    H6:People believing in the individualism myth of nature are more likely to have no walking trips at all

    compared to other, while people believing in the egalitarianism myth of nature are more likely to have at

    least one walking trip per day compared to other.

    A questionnaire survey among young people

    A questionnaire survey was conducted among 375 undergraduate civil engineering students in Norway

    and in Argentina: 254 students at the Norwegian University of Science and Technology in Trondheim,and 103 students at the University of Buenos Aires, Argentina. By choosing a sample of high

    socioeconomic homogeneity in the two countries respectively, comparison between students adhering

    to the four cultures becomes less distorted by other socioeconomic effects than gender.

    The questionnaires were distributed in auditoriums after or during ordinary scheduled lectures, and

    even though participation was voluntary, almost all students present at these lectures accepted the

    invitation to participate in the survey. To help motivate participation, a lottery was arranged among the

    participants.

    The questionnaire consisted of several parts. Initially the respondents were asked to fill in a travel diary

    describing all of yesterdays trips. The data collection did not take place on Mondays, assuring that only

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    7/13

    ordinary weekdays were included in the travel data set. Information about travel purpose, trip length2

    and mode choice was collected for each trip. Thereafter, the respondents were presented to a set of

    statements for which they had to mark along a Likert scale their agreement or disagreement with each

    statement. These statements constituted the items used for calculating the respondents scores for

    each cultural bias, and subsequently to allocate the respondents to the four cultures. Since there is no

    standardised battery of items to include in a grid-group study, inspiration was sought from several

    previous studies (Boyle and Coughlin, 1994; Grendstad, 1999; Grendstad and Rommetvedt, 1996;

    Grendstad and Selle, 2000; Grendstad and Selle, 1995; Grendstad and Sundback, 2003; Lima and Castro,

    2005; Marris et.al., 2008; Meader et.al., 2006; Steg and Sievers, 2000) when designing the

    questionnaire. A set of statements concerning transportation issues was also included. Finally, the

    respondents were asked to range the four cartoons illustrating the myths of nature according to how

    well they corresponded to the respondents own views on the vulnerability of nature.

    The methods of analyses

    Initially, descriptive statistics were used to get acquainted with the data set and the travel patterns of

    the respondents.

    Secondly, factor and reliability analyses were performed in order to identify the cultures and the items

    included for each culture. Having identified these items, new scores were calculated for all respondents

    indicating their degree of compliance with, or rejection of, each culture. Each respondent was

    thereafter allocated to the culture achieving the highest score.

    Finally, analysis of variance (ANOVA) and chi-square tests were used to compare travel patterns to

    cultural biases. A significance level of 5% was chosen for all tests.

    All analyses were performed parallel for the Norwegian and the Argentine sample.

    Similar travel patterns in the north and in the south?

    The respondents in this study were civil engineering students from Norway and Argentina. Although

    being in the same phase of life, studying the same field of engineering, some of the external conditions

    are quite different and were therefore expected to affect the travel pattern. The most striking is the size

    of the city. Trondheim is a city with 175.000 inhabitants, and with the campus located about two

    kilometres from the city centre. Buenos Aires covers a metropolitan area with about 12 million

    inhabitants. The campus is located about three kilometres from the city centre. While the Norwegian

    students meet for lectures in the mornings and early afternoons, the Argentine students, at least those

    participating in this survey, get their lectures in late afternoons and early evenings.

    Table 1 summarises an overview of some characteristics of the respondents, as well as some main

    figures describing the travel pattern among the two respondent groups. The most striking differences

    between these two samples are commented.

    2Since the responds estimated the length of each trip themselves when filling in the questionnaire, it is important

    to be aware of this variable being less reliable than the others.

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    8/13

    Table 1 Respondent characteristics and travel pattern, Norwegian and Argentine students

    Norwegian students Argentine students

    Number of respondents 254 103

    Gender distribution 33,2% female

    66,8% male

    26,2% female

    73,8% male

    Age Mean: 20,8 years

    (SD: 1,5)

    (Range: 18-31)

    Mean: 24,4 years

    (SD: 4,7)

    (Range: 20-57)

    % holding a driving

    license

    88,2% 71,8%

    % having car available

    yesterday among those

    holding a driving license

    27,1% 32,4%

    % having bicycle available

    yesterday

    83,0% 20,6%

    Number of trips

    yesterday

    4,2 trips per person 3,9 trips per person

    Average trip length Mean: 2,1 km per trip

    (SD: 2,5)

    Mean: 10,4 km per trip

    (SD: 26,7)

    Travel purpose To university: 25,6%

    To own home: 43,9%

    To leisure activities: 10,3%

    To health and other services: 0,6%

    To do shopping: 10,8%

    To visit someone: 4,2%

    To own work: 0,5%

    Other: 4,1%

    To university: 26,0%

    To own home: 42,0%

    To leisure activities: 5,8%

    To health and other services: 3,9%

    To do shopping: 5,8%

    To visit someone: 6,4%

    To own work: 8,0%

    Other: 2,1%

    Modal split Walk: 50,0%

    Bicycle: 27,2%

    Public transport: 9,8%

    Car driver: 7,4%

    Car passenger: 4,0%

    Other: 1,6%

    Walk: 30,1%

    Bicycle: 2,2%

    Public transport: 53,1%

    Car driver: 9,1%

    Car passenger: 5,0%

    Other: 0,5%

    Firstly, the Argentine students are on average somewhat older than the Norwegian students, and their

    ages are more spread and cover a wider range of ages, giving a higher standard deviation. Secondly,

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    9/13

    while four out of five Norwegian students had a bicycle available the day before the survey took place,

    only one out of five Argentine students had the same. This reflects in the modal split which shows that

    bicycling was used as travel mode for 27,2% of the Norwegian trips while only for 2,2% of the Argentine

    trips. The low share of bicycle trips in Buenos Aires might intuitively seem a bit surprising given the very

    flat topography of the city, ideal for cycling. However, a dedicated bicycle infrastructure is sorely

    missing, and, as can be seen from Table 1, the average trip length exceeds 10 kilometres, favouring

    motorised transport modes. Comparing the daily number of trips and the travel purpose distribution

    reveals no big differences between the two samples. The apparent major differences are the trip

    lengths and consequently the share of non-motorised modes. The students in Trondheim travel 2,1

    kilometres per trip on average. The walking and bicycling trips c onstitute respectively 50,0% and 27,2%

    of all trips. The Argentine students travel 10,4 kilometres per trip on average. Still, as much as 30,1% of

    their trips are carried out on foot, in addition to the small share of 2,2% trips performed on bicycle.

    Obviously, the significant difference in size between these two cities explains the differences found in

    trip lengths and modal split. However, what happens if we narrow down our focus to include only theshort trips and study the modal split of these? Such a comparison is performed in Figure 2.

    Figure 2 Modal split for short trips, 0-5 kilometres, Norwegian and Argentine students

    As can be seen from Figure 2, there are still differences between the two samples when modal split is

    compared for short trips divided in intervals of one kilometre each. The Argentine students are more

    likely to use public transport within all length intervals, while for trips between two and four kilometres,

    the shares of walking trips among the Norwegian students are about three times as high as those of the

    Argentine students. One possible explanation for these observations might be the travel time budget

    constraints, limiting the total daily time spent for travelling. Since the Argentine students cover far

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    10/13

    longer travel distances per day, it can be suggested that due to this constraint, they cannot allow

    themselves to choose slow modes even for relatively short trips. The figures of Figure 2 should however

    be interpreted with care since the number of trips included for each interval is pretty scarce, especially

    for the Argentine data.

    Four cultures among civil engineering students?

    Based on the respondents degree of agreement or disagreement with a selection of statements, a score

    for each cultural bias was calculated. Thereafter each respondent was allocated to the culture for which

    the highest score was achieved. If a respondent achieved identical high scores for two cultures, the

    respondent was not allocated to any culture and therefore not included in further analyses. How the

    respondents distribute on the four cultural biases are shown in Table 2. The allocation reveals clear

    differences between the two samples, with individualism as the dominating worldview among the

    Norwegian students contrasting hierarchism as the culture of which the majority of Argentine students

    adhere to. Fatalism is almost negligible in both samples. Since fatalism is regarded as a passive culture,

    this finding is not surprising. Civil engineer students are young people in pursuit of a higher education

    by investing a lot of effort into a high workload. These students represent therefore typically active

    cultures.

    Table 2. Allocating respondents to cultural biases, Norwegian and Argentine students

    Cultural bias Norwegian students

    N=249

    Argentine students

    N=100

    Hierarchism 22,1% 55,0%

    Egalitarianism 32,5% 23,0%

    Individualism 44,6% 18,0%

    Fatalism 0,8% 4,0%

    Four myths of nature among civil engineering students?

    The four myths of nature represent four ways of perceiving the balance of nature. The respondents

    were asked to range them according to how well they corresponded to their own views and beliefs. As

    shown in Figure 3, the majority of students in both samples believe strongest in the hierarchism myth,

    that nature is tolerant within limits.

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    11/13

    Figure 3 The distribution of myths of nature given first rang, Norwegian and Argentine students

    Four ways of perceiving walking?

    The questionnaire covered a selection of statements regarding transportation issues, of which one was

    directly related to walking, emphasising the importance of prioritising pedestrians and cyclists when

    planning the road user infrastructure: Pedestrians and cyclists should be given higher priority when

    road spaces are divided between vulnerable and motorised road users. The analysis found that the

    Norwegian students adhering to egalitarianism stated a significantly higher support to this statement

    than students adhering to hierarchism, individualism and fatalism. For the Argentine students the

    analysis found a similar significant higher support among the students believing in the egalitarianism

    myth of nature compared to those believing in the individualism myth of nature. Two other statementsemphasised the need for improving the infrastructure for car traffic, and the analysis found that

    students adhering to individualism, as well as students believing in the individualism myth of nature,

    gave significantly higher support to these statements compared to the others. Given that attitudes

    reflect in action, these findings suggest that students adhering to egalitarianism and believing in the

    egalitarianism myth of nature are more likely to walk, while on the other hand, students adhering to

    individualism and believing in the individualism myth of nature are more likely to drive a car, as

    hypothesised previously in this paper.

    Four ways of practicing walking?Six hypotheses were initially set out, linking the cultural biases and myths of nature to mode choice, to

    the daily travel distance by foot, and to the likelihood of walking at all.

    The analysis reveals almost none significant differences, and thus hardly any support for these

    hypotheses. The only significant difference found relates to H3: People adhering to individualism walk

    shorter daily distances than other, while people adhering to egalitarianism walk longer daily distances

    than other and were found among the Norwegian students, showing that students adhering to

    individualism stated to walk shorter daily distances compared to students adhering to hierarchism.

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    12/13

    Given such scarce support for the hypotheses, this study concludes that neither cultural biases nor

    myths of nature seem to affect the walking practices. This was found both for students in Trondheim

    who travel short daily distances mainly by foot or cycling, and for students in Buenos Aires whose travel

    distances are about five times as long as those of their northern colleagues, and mainly done by public

    transport. Is this a surprising result? On one hand, yes, it is rather surprising. The issue of global

    warming and the importance of choosing sustainable transport modes have been targeted by

    governments and media for some time. According to the grid-group theory, people adhering to the

    different cultural biases should perceive and react to these issues in quite varying ways. On the other

    hand, no, this is not a surprising result. Steg and Sievers (2000) found a similar result when studying the

    use of car versus the myths of nature. They found no significant relationship between the two, although

    the analysis indicated that respondents committed to the egalitarianism myth of nature were more

    likely to use other modes of transport, while respondents committed to the hierarchism and fatalism

    myths of nature seemed to user their cars more often. The lack of significant relationships may be due

    to societal constraints. ORiordan and Jordan (1999) emphasise energy use and transport as two areas

    in which individuals are forced by the organisation of the society to behave in unsustainable ways, evenif it conflicts with their basic worldview. The latter may be a plausible explanation for the lack of

    support for the hypotheses from the Argentine dataset, as suggested due to long travel distances and

    travel time budget constraints. It is h owever less obvious when studying the Norwegian data. These

    students have already a pretty sustainable pattern of daily travel. Still, differences between the cultures

    were expected to be seen since the combination of short travel distances, a high share of students

    holding a driving license, as well as having bicycle available, opens for a considerable freedom of choice

    when decisions about travel modes are made. A restrictive parking policy at the campus may be one

    explanation why no differences between the cultures are revealed. But also economic constraints when

    being a student may reduce the possibility for them to choose unsustainable transport modes, even if

    they otherwise would be preferred.

    While the link between the worldviews of grid-group theory and attitudes has been explored in several

    studies, few studies have explored the link between worldviews and behaviours. The study presented in

    this paper is an attempt to contribute to this lacking field of research. The lack of support for the

    hypotheses set out in this paper may on one hand be interpreted as an unsuccessful application of the

    theory. On the other hand, however, it can be interpreted as a successful application of the theory

    identifying an area of behaviour for which societal constraints restrict the individuals freedom to make

    choices in accordance to ones worldview.

    References

    Boyle, R.P., Coughlin, R.M. (1994) Conceptualizing and Operationalizing Cultural Theory. In Coyle and

    Ellis (eds),Politics, Policy, and Culture. Westview Press, 191-218

    Coyle, D.J. (1994) The Theory That Would Be King. In Coyle and Ellis (eds), Politics, Policy, and Culture.

    Westview Press, 219-239

    Douglas, M. (1970) Natural Symbols.Explorations in Cosmology. Barrie & Rockliff, The Cresset Press,

    London

    Douglas, M. (1992) Risk and blame. Essays in cultural theory.Routledge, London

  • 8/12/2019 Four Cultures of Walking

    13/13

    Grendstad, G., Selle, P. (1995)Cultural theory, postmaterialism and environmental attitudes. LOS-senter,

    Notat 9511, ISSN 0802-3646, Bergen

    Grendstad, G., Rommetvedt, H. (1996) Fem tyver p samme marked? In Grendstad and Selle (eds),Kultur som levemte, Det Norske Samlaget, Oslo, 314-329

    Grendstad, G. (1999) A political cultural map of Europe. A survey approach. GeoJournal, 47, 463-475

    Grendstad, G., Selle, P. (2000) Cultural Myths of Human and Physical Nature: Integrated or Separated?

    Risk Analysis, Vol. 20, No 1, 27-39

    Grendstad, G., Sundback, S. (2003) Socio-Demographic Effects on Cultural Biases. A Nordic Study of Grid-

    Group Theory. Acta Sociologica, 46 (4), 289-306

    Hendriks, F. (1994) Cars and Culture in Munich and Birmingham: The Case for Cultural Pluralism. In Coyle

    and Ellis (eds), Politics, Policy, and Culture. Westview Press, 51-69

    Hoppe, R., Grin, J. (1999) Pollution through traffic and transport. The praxis of cultural pluralism in

    parliamentarian technology assessment. In Thompson, Grendstad and Selle (eds)Cultural theory as

    political science, Routledge, London and New York, 154-169

    Lima, M.L., Castro, P. (2005) Cultural theory meets the community: Worldviews and local issues.Journal

    of Environmental Psychology, 25, 23-35

    Marris, C., Langford, I.H., ORiordan, T. (2008) A Quantitative Test of the Cultural Theory of Risk

    Perceptions: Comparison with the Psychometric Paradigm. In 6 and Mars (eds),The Institutional

    Dynamics of Culture, Vol 1, 221-233

    Meader, N., Uzzell, D., Gatersleben, B. (2006) Cultural theory and quality of life. Revue europenne depsychologic appliqu, 56, 61-69

    Perri 6, Mars, G. (2008) Introduction. In Perri 6 and Mars (eds), The Institutional Dynamics of Culture,

    Vol 1, 221-233

    ORiordan, T., Jordan, A. (1999) Institutions, climate change and cultural theory: towards a common

    analytical framework. Global Environmental Change, 9, 81-93

    Steg, L., Sievers, I. (2000) Cultural theory and individual perceptions of environmental risks.Environment

    and behavior, Vol. 32, No 2, 250-269

    Thompson, M., Ellis, R., Wildavsky, A (1990) Cultural Theory. Westview Press, Boulder

    Thompson, M (1996) Inherent Relationality. An Anti-Dualist Approach to Institutions. LOS report 9608,

    The Norwegian Research Centre in Organisation and Management, Bergen

    Thompson, M., Grendstad, G., Selle, P. (1999) Cultural theory as political science. In Thompson,

    Grendstad and Selle (eds) Cultural theory as political science, Routledge, London and New York, 1-23