forest islands and kayapó resource management in amazonia: a reappraisal of the apêtê

Download Forest Islands and Kayapó Resource Management in Amazonia: A Reappraisal of the Apêtê

If you can't read please download the document

Upload: eugene-parker

Post on 06-Aug-2016

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • Research Reports

    Forest Islands and Kayap6 Resource Management in Amazonia: A Reappraisal of the A p M

    EUGENE PARKER Department of Geography Universip of Maryland

    If those who plan the future of tropical areas would follow some of the fundamentals of the Kayap6 system, we would be well on a path to a socially and ecologically viable system for the humid tropics.

    -Darrell Addison Posey, 1988 During the past decade, the work of

    Darrell Posey among the Gorotire Kay- ap6 Indians of the Brazilian Amazon has gained increasing attention for its de- scription and analysis of indigenous per- ceptions, concepts, and management of natural resource systems. As reported by Posey, the G. Kayap6 are extraordinarily astute, and adept, manipulators and managers of their local environments. He has argued that the G. Kayap6 establish and maintain complex ecosystems, in- creme local biological diversity, intention- ally create ecotones, and manage and ma- nipulate forest successional stages (1984a, 1984b, 1987a). They plant thou- sands of useful plants along the margins of hundreds of miles of local trail systems, in defecation zones, and in natural and artificially created forest openings (1984a, 1987a, 1987b). Food plant spe- cies are planted in forest openings to in- crease their productivity (1982, 1985a, 1987b). They practice a form of no- madic agriculture (1982) and engage in the semi-domestication of a very large number of plant species (1985b). Swid- dens are intentionally dispersed over large areas to create game reserves for fu- ture generations (1982, 1984c), and around the periphery of swiddens the G. Kayap6 plant species that repel pests

    (1987~). The reforestation of old swid- dens is intensively managed, including the modification ofsoils and the introduc- tion of many species from mature forest areas (1988). Scores of useful plant spe- cies, taken from an area larger than Western Europe, have been collected by the G. Kayap6 during treks and intro- duced into the local area (1988). They have, over time, radically influenced the range, distribution, and diversity of plant species to such an extent, according to Posey, as to call into question the very no- tion of so-called natural flora of sa- vanna and forest ecosystems (1984b; An- derson and Posey 1985).

    This paper reports results of research conducted in the village of Gorotire in 1984 and 1987 that depart significantly from those published by Posey. Specifi- cally, I wish to draw attention to difficul- ties with a crucial element of Poseys eth- noecological architecture for the G. Kay- ap6: the apt%, or forest islands, composed of useful plant species, created and main- tained by the G. Kayap6 in savanna en- vironments. The apt%?, resting at the foun- dation of the elaborate system of Kayap6 ecological knowledge reported by Posey, provides much of the logic and the cohe- sion to his general scheme of G. Kayap6 natural-resource management practices. Perhaps of equal importance, Posey has touted the practice of creating apit2 as a blueprint for how to build a forest from scratch (1984b:32). Indeed, he has ad- vanced the proposition that the apt% rep- resents a simple, effective reforestation scheme for Amazonia (1985a: 144, 156, 1987a: 18; Anderson and Posey 1987:50), a claim of major import given the rate and extent of deforestation in the region. Un- fortunately, aptti, in the form described by Posey, do not exist.

    Study Area and Population One of the largest remaining G&-speak-

    ing groups, the Kayap6 Indians occupy a

  • RESEARCH REPORT^ 407

    reserve of more than three million hec- tares in the middle Xingu River Basin of Amazonia. There are 13 Kayap6 villages and perhaps as many as 3,000 Kayap6 Indians, although not all villages and Kayap6 Indians are located within the re- serve. The largest village, Gorotire (7 45 S/54 46 W), had approximately 7 15 res- idents in mid-1987.

    As Figure 1 indicates, the general re- gion within which Gorotire is located is a broad transition zone from rain forest to the central planalto, a complex region of dry forest, gallery forest, savanna scrub, and savanna (the central planalto is often generally described as the Brazilian cer- rado).* Dry forest, also called tropical for- est to distinguish it from equatorial trop- ical rain forest, is found along the north- ern reach of the cerrado, and is considered by some researchers to be a transitional vegetative association between the cerrado plains and the Amazonian rain forests (e.g., see Askew et al. 1971). The cerrado sweeps up from the south and east, al- most reaching Gorotire, which is located very near the southern margin of the rain forest.

    The village is sited on a high bank above a major bend in the Fresco River, a tributary of the Xingu River. As Figures 1 and 2 suggest, the area surrounding Go- rotire is characterized by significant land- scape variation: rain forest, dry forest, gallery forest, scrub forest, savanna, sa- vanna forest, mountains, and river val- leys. The transitional nature of the gen- eral region, in which the different types of natural communities are highly inter- mixed, is clearly revealed in Figure 2; a walk out from the village of only a few ki- lometers often traverses several natural communities. Open savannas, it should be noted, are almost exclusively found in the general vicinity of the village.

    Posey (1983:229) has reported that the Gorotire Kayap6 recognize three major divisions of their terrestrial environment: forest (bd), scrub savanna or camp0 cerrado (kapGt), and mountains ( k r ~ i i ) . ~ Within each division, according to Posey, they distinguish a number of discrete ecozones (natural communities); for example, they

    recognize almost 40 types of savanna and forest (1984d:37).

    The A p W Thir Significance

    The Gorotire Kayap6 practice of cre- ating forest islands in savanna environ- ments has been reported by Posey in more than 17 papers since 1982.4 At the practical level, apiti can be viewed as re- source supermarkets (Posey, as quoted in Stevens 1990) of sorts for the G. Kayap6, in that plant resources from different lo- cations and ecological contexts have been concentrated in specific sites near the vil- lage. According to Posey, these islands represent an innovative and aggressive manipulation of the environment that maximizes the diversity and the concen- tration of resource species in selected set- tings. Of the forest islands in the savan- nas near Gorotire, Posey reports that 75% were actually created by the Indians (1984b:8; 1985a:141; 1987133182). Fully 98% of plant species found in the islands he studied had perceived use-values (often multiple values), and as many as 85% of these plants were actually planted by the Indians (1985a:141). (Poseys re- peated reporting of different percentages for these categories is discussed in a later section.)

    At a more theoretical level, these is- lands represent something far more im- portant than resource supermarkets or food depots (1982:23) for Posey. As noted above, the existence of apiti calls into question the very idea of a natural flora in the savanna (Posey 1984b:32; Anderson and Posey 1987:50), and pro- vides evidence that the G. Kayap6, in contrast to modern populations, ac- tually increase ecological diversity by their use of the environment (Posey 1985a: 156; Anderson a n d Posey 1989: 1 72). For Posey, the creation of apiti demonstrates clearly that plant distribu- tions in Amazonia have been significantly affected by indigenous practices; 20% of a p i t i species a re from other regions ( 1988:90; Anderson and Posey 1985:86). Finally, apiti represent for Posey a viable reforestation mechanism for the Amazon

  • 408 AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST [94, 19921

    Figure 1 General study area.

    region (1985a; Anderson and Posey 1987).

    Making an A p X

    As Posey reports it, the first step in the formation of an apitiis the modification of the soil base: techniques offorest establish- mmt include the creation of a new soil in savanna hospitable to forest species

    ( 1984b: 10, emphasis added). Most local savanna soils are characterized by high acidity, high exchangeable aluminum content, low cation exchange capacity, and low available phosphorus. To offset these conditions, the G. Kayap6 create a new soil by making compost piles in the forest and in other apiti from sticks, leaves, and limbs. Allowed to rot, the

  • Lege

    nd:

    n

    u Xe

    rom

    orph

    i ver

    y op

    en lo

    w shr

    ub s

    avan

    na

    Xe

    mrp

    hii

    sem

    idec

    iduo

    us tr

    ee a

    nd s

    crub

    sa

    vann

    a with

    woo

    dy fo

    rest

    isla

    nds;

    ka

    pat k

    am &

    I&;

    cam

    po-m

    rrado

    ibe

    tum

    ; mat

    a se

    cund

    aria

    . cap

    oeiia

    Xero

    mor

    phii s

    emi-d

    ecid

    uous

    fore

    st

    (sav

    anna

    woo

    dlan

    d)

    kape

    t kam

    bi (

    bi k

    am &

    I):

    cerr

    adb

    H Dry

    -sea

    son

    sern

    ided

    duou

    s mes

    ophy

    tic

    koad

    leaf

    (mid

    -tall)

    fore

    st

    M k

    amre

    k; m

    ata seca

    bi r

    arar

    a (b

    h tyk);

    mat

    a

    kapa

    t pun

    u; camp0 S

    UN

    (va

    riam

    of

    cam

    pom

    nado

    )

    seco

    ndar

    y for

    est and p

    rese

    nt g

    arde

    n ar

    eas

    Ever

    gree

    n m

    esop

    hytic

    bro

    adle

    af ta

    ll for

    est

    Figu

    re 0

    N

    atur

    al c

    omm

    unit

    ies of

    the

    Gor

    otir

    e are

    a.

  • 410 AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOCIST [94, 19921

    piles are then beaten with sticks to pro- duce a mulch. The mulch is carried to a spot in the savanna, usually a slightly de- pressed area so as to enhance moisture re- tention, and piled to form a mound. Other materials are mixed in as well: hearth debris and ashes, organic mate- rials, portions of termite hills (Nasutitemes sp.), ant nests (Aztecn sp.), slashed vege- tation, and mulch from backyard gar- dens. The result is a low-slung mound, a few meters in diameter, into which are planted useful, sun-tolerant species that are known to be hardy enough for the rig- ors of the savanna (Posey 1985a:142). Through time, additional species are in- troduced into the incipient forest island- in the understory and around the periph- ery-extending both its general area and its density.

    Apit t are created and maintained by family units (each extended family has its own forest island [Posey 1984b:9]), generally extended matrilineal families, and are not regarded as communal prop- erty (Posey 1984b:g; Anderson and Posey 1985:82).

    Classijcation, Internal Zones, and Transplants

    Apt t t are classified according to their size and internal complexity. Posey has reported that the G. Kayap6 recognize four distinct stages in the development and growth of apitt: ap2ttbnu, apttx, apt%, and aptti. Figure 3 represents Poseys con- ception of the internal organization and evolution of aptti; each internal zone rep- resents a discrete planting zone. For Po- sey, it is the elaboration of additional in- ternal zones, in part a function of Kayap6 management (e.g., the creation and man- agement of the ir6, the cleared central area), that determines to a very large ex- tent the classification of a particular forest island.

    For the Kayap6, internal zones repre- sent different planting areas into which they introduce plant species from forest ecozones (and, to a much lesser extent, from savanna ecozones) that are concep- tually linked to specific internal zones (see Table 1): Planting zones in aptt2are

    matched with ecological types recognized by the Indians in the forest ( 1985a: 155).

    Planting zones within aptti are based upon perceived variations in shade, light, temperature , and moisture (Posey 1985a: 154-1 55, 1990:20; Anderson and Posey 1987:49). Apparently, these diag- nostic features are employed by the Indi- ans in matching up the internal zones of upit2 with natural communities:

    For example, plants found in the dark, damp forest (ba-tyk) are likely to do well in the a-ti% or the nhi-pik of an ap&ti. Plants that thrive in the light-penetrating forest (ba-rarcira) would be planted [in the] no-ka or a-ki-ki t . Species found at the margins of the forest or the edges of other apt%? would be transferred to the j a -ka or a-ka-k&. [Posey 1985a: 1541

    Ecological Diversio and Adaptive Solutions

    As Posey describes it, then, the Goro- tire Kayap6 engage in the systematic col- lection of useful species from recognized forest communities, some at great dis- tances from the village, and transplant them into cognitively linked internal zones of aptti located near the village. The range of perceived use-values for the plant species found in ap2tt; and reported by Posey is nothing short of spectacular: food (roots, tubers, nuts, fruits), bever- age, medicinal (e.g., for fevers, dizziness, head- and toothaches, diarrhea, abortion, contraception), and ceremonial plants, plants used for artistic purposes, craft plants (e.g., bows, arrows, warclubs), plants for pipes, needles, sandpaper, lip plugs, and body paints, plants possessing spiritual value and others with aesthetic importance, hunt-magic plants, war- magic plants, game-poison plants, fire- wood plants, numerous plants to attract game animals and birds, insect repel- lents, plants for fibers and wrapping ma- terial, and so on (Posey 1985a:142; An- derson and Posey 1989: 160).

    Ap2t2, it would seem, represent a re- markably adaptive solution to the peren- nial problem of resource plant species with low densities and numbers, and large spatial dispersion. Equally impres- sive, the up2tt solution entails the crea- tion and maintenance of forest patches in

  • 41 1

    Figure 3 Stages of development and internal organization.

    savanna environs that constitute, in ef- fect, microlandscapes of maximum eco- logical diversity. In this sense, then, Po- sey advances the proposition that what informs G. Kayap6 resource manage- ment strategies and methodologies is the recognition of the value of a patchy en- vironment in which many different natu-

    ral communities are found. The upit i , with its collection of internal planting zones linked to different natural commu- nities, would represent a particularly patchy environment. Apitti are, in Po- seys own words, forest patches in which maximum biological diversity oc- curs. To put such a statement in more

  • 41 2 AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST [94, 19921

    Table 1 Internal apiti zones and their ecological counterparts (from Posey 1985a: 155).

    Api t i planting zones nhi-pik ba-rarara, ba-kamrek, ba-kri &-ki b i -k i t , ba-rariri, ba-kri-ti j a - k i kapit, ba-kit irZ-&-ki ba-rarara, ba-kamrek, ba-kri a-ta-ri ba-tyk, ba-kamrek a-ka-kit ba-rarark, ba-kumrenx

    Corresponding ecological units

    ecological terms, the Indians not only rec- ognize the richness of ecotones, they cre- ate them (1985a:156, 1990:19).

    Posey has described the G. Kayap6 as ecological engineers (1987a: 16) whose re- source management strategies result in an actual increase in biodiversity and in- clude reforestation methodologies (how to build forests from scratch, 1984b:32) that have far-reaching implications for the study offorestation in savanna and re-

    forestation in areas denuded by deforestation (1985a: 156, emphasis added). This de- scription, and the apt22 work in general, have captured the imagination, and spurred the hopes, of a broad range of in- dividuals and researchers concerned with the future of Amazonia.

    Methodology and Results of ApiW Research

    From Poseys extensive literature on apt%, it would appear that the crucial evi- dence was collected during a three-week field period in November 1983. (Al- though a ten-day follow-up collection was undertaken in April 1984, the results have never been reported or incorporated into the 1983 data base.) Ten forest islands were surveyed within the area labeled Posey Study Area on Figure 2 and all plants above one meter were collected. Informants were interviewed about the use-value(s) of each collected plant and whether it had been planted in the apt%.

    Posey has been inconsistent in report- ing the results of the plant survey and in- formant interviews, leaving readers to fend for themselves in determining the ac- tual results. In various publications he

    has reported that 120 (Anderson and Po- sey 1985:77, 1987:48) , 1 2 8 (Posey 1984e:6), and 140 (Posey 1984b:8, 1985a: 141, 1987b:182) species had been collected and that 60% (Posey 1984b:8, 1987b:182), 75% (Anderson and Posey 1985:77, 1989:159; Posey 1987a:58), and 85% (Posey 1984e:6, 1985a:141) of the plants collected in a p d i had been planted there (Posey has reported six different combinations of plants collected and per- centages planted). However, he has been consistent in stating that 75% of apt%? is- lands were created by the Gorotire Kay- ap6 and that 98% of species collected had at least one perceived use-value. Using a simple frequency count of the differing re- sults reported by Posey, the following seems a reasonable interpretation: 120 plant species were collected, 75% of which were reported by informants to have been planted and 98% of which had at least one perceived use-value, and 75% of the forest islands were created by the G. Kayap6.

    Posey has described and discussed sev- eral reasons why the G. Kayap6 engage in the practice of creating and maintain- ing up&!. The most important is the con- centration and increase of useful plant species near the village. Other reasons, clearly secondary to the concentration of useful species but still significant, accord- ing to Posey, are the use of ap2t2 for refuge and defensive purposes, and as sites for rest, relaxation, and sexual assignations.

    The most important evidence that Po- sey has to offer is the plant collection it- self. If these islands are anthropogenic in origin, if they were planted with useful species taken not only from surrounding

  • RESEARCH REPORT^ 413

    forest types but also from very distant re- gions, then it would be reasonable to ex- pect that the species composition of these islands would differ considerably from those islands not planted. Posey offers no evidence for such differences because, quite unaccountably, it apparently never occurred to him, or Anderson, the bota- nist who collaborated with Posey in the actual plant collection, that having a con- trol island was crucial methodologically. The only islands they surveyed were the ten within the Posey study area (Figure 2). In addition, they never reported any data regarding the incidence of particular plant species found in the islands sur- veyed. Thus, they offer no data demon- strating that a select species frequency of occurrence is higher than would be nor- mally expected. And, finally, it appears that Posey never investigated the prove- nance of the species collected. That is to say, no effort was made to work through a herbarium collection or herbarium lists to ascertain where the species they col- lected are commonly found.

    From the extensive published record by Posey concerning apitt?, now number- ing at least 13 separate publications in- cluding those coauthored with Anderson, we know only that a species occurred in at least one of the ten islands surveyed, its perceived use-value, and whether it had been planted in the island.

    Concentration and Increase of Usejid Plant Species

    As noted earlier in this discussion, the principal logic for apt%? is quite straight- forward; up&? address one of the persis- tent and ubiquitous problems of indige- nous resource economics: low incidence and high spatial dispersion of useful plants. The attractiveness of the apt%? so- lution is in its elegance; the G. Kayap6 simply relocate, concentrate, and in- crease the numbers of useful plant species in artificial forest islands near the village, thus resolving problems of incidence and dispersion. Useful plant species are trans- planted into apt22 internal zones cogni- tively linked with the type of forest zone from which the plant was taken.

    In addition to those plant species trans- planted from forest zones around Goro- tire, Posey contends that 20% of upit2 plant species come from other regions. The extent of these other regions is suggested by the following: Equally amazing is that these forest islands are composed of botanical resources taken from an area the size of Western Europe (Posey 1988:90, emphasis added).

    Plant Collection Analysis

    In April 1987, after some months of growing concern about Poseys work among the Gorotire Kayap6, I was able to undertake a series of plant collections not only in apt%? but also in the principal forest and savanna forestlwoodland com- munities found in the general area. The general purpose of these collections was to obtain a preliminary data base regard- ing the distribution and frequency of flo- ral species in the several communities. In addition, these collections would allow for comparative analysis, albeit tentative, of species found in these communities and those found in ~ p t t t i . ~

    I selected one of the larger upit2 origi- nally surveyed by Posey and Anderson as well as a control island of comparable size. The control island was situated in another savanna environment, away from the Posey study area, its loca- tion such that none of the reasons Posey had advanced for the creation of up&? by the G. Kayap6 obtained (defense, illness, concentration of resources). In selecting the two islands, I controlled for general size and shape (long axis of the Posey is- land was 56 meters, the control island 49 meters), proximity to forest margin, di- rection of long axis, and nearby islands.

    The aptt i survey consisted of demarcat- ing a ten-meter-wide transect through the middle and along the long axis of each up&;. All plants above one meter were col- lected (as was the case in the Posey-An- derson collection).

    One additional control of sorts is wor- thy of mention. The actual collection and preparation of the plants in the field, and the subsequent identification of speci- mens, were conducted by the same indi-

  • 414 AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST [94, 19921

    vidual for both my survey and Poseys: Carlos Rodrio da Silva, a respected for- est specialist on the botanical staff of the Museu Paraense Emilio Goeldi and one of the more experienced botanical field- workers in the Brazilian Amazon.

    Three data sets are referred to in the following discussion. AP1 stands for the large island I resampled, which had been part of the original 1983 Posey-An- derson island study, CT2 represents the control island I sampled in 1987, and PA1 0 refers to the original ten-island study undertaken by Posey and Anderson in 1983.

    Total Species and Co- Occurrences

    Posey has repeatedly asserted that the G. Kayap6, in marked contrast to other populations, actually increase ecological diversity through their resource manage- ment practices. This claim has consider- able theoretical import given that the so- cioeconomic activities of virtually all so- cieties have resulted in the simplification of their natural environment: in the re- duction of ecological diversity and com- plexity. In support of his claim, Posey has offered as evidence the G. Kayap6 prac- tice of creating and maintaining apdi.

    Table 2 shows the total number of spe- cies found in each of the three ape t^e^collec- tions. There is no significant difference in total species found in APl and CT2: 79 in APl and 75 in CT2. The additional 4 spe- cies found in APl might well be a function of its slight size advantage: 12% larger than CT2. In any event, a 4-species dif- ference is inconsequential. Thus, in com- paring a purported anthropogenic island (AP1)-in which upwards of 85% of spe- cies are claimed to have been trans- planted from local and distant settings-

    with a naturally occurring island (CT2), we find that the natural island possesses roughly the same degree of ecological di- versity as does the proposed anthropo- genic island. Further casting doubt upon Poseys claim that the G. Kayap6 ac- tually increase ecological diversity is the fact that his survey of ten islands, in which every plant over one meter was col- lected, yielded a total of 120 species while the control island transect, in which the plant collection was limited to a 10 x 49 meter area, yielded 75 species.

    Inasmuch as APl is one of the original PA10 islands and is claimed to be anthro- pogenic in origin, it would seem reasona- ble for us to expect the species composi- tion of APl to be significantly different from that found in CT2. It would be ex- ceedingly odd for the G. Kayap6 to go to the trouble of creating and maintaining islands of transplanted species when these plant species are readily available in natural islands andlor surrounding sa- vanna and forest ecozones.

    Sixty-one percent of APl species (48 of 79) were also found in CT2. Given that Posey has reported that as many as 85% of PA10 species were transplants, the 6 1 YO co-occurrence seems quite remark- able. More to the point, I was able to sample only one control a&W; with the very large numbers of upi t ; found in nearby savanna, it would seem reasona- ble to expect that an additional number of APl species not found in CT2 would oc- cur in some of these other natural islands. Of the 23 APl species not found in CT2, 33% (10 of 30) were also found in at least one of the 10 meter x 250 meter transect collections I conducted in dry forest, sa- vanna woodland, and savanna forest. Thus, 58 of 79 APl species, or 73%, are

    Table 2 Total plant species found in up&? plant collections.

    A@ Species Genus Family

    API 79 66 49 CT2 PA10 API + CT2

    75 120 107

    58 88 83

    45 50 53

  • RESEARCH REPORTS 415

    available in natural communities, all of which are found in ample abundance within a couple ofkilometers of the village (Figure 2).

    With regard to the 21 remaining spe- cies not found in either the CT2 apiti or the savannalforest transects, 16 are in- cluded on lists of common savanna or sa- vanna-forest species compiled by leading authorities of the Brazilian cerrado (Eiten 1972; Goodland and Ferri 1979; Ratter et al. 1973). If taken together, then, we can say that 75 of the 79 plant species found in APl were found either in the control a p i t i or the surrounding environment, and/or were listed as common savanna or savanna-forest species.

    Further evidence that apiti species in general are naturally occurring, as op- posed to having been planted, can be ob- tained by examining where apiti species are also found in the local area. Because apt22 occur most frequently in the bound- ary area between dry, open forestlsa- vanna forest and savanna (Figure 2), it should be expected that apit i species, if naturally occurring, could also be found most often in those communities. Table 3 demonstrates this relationship. Of those species found both in upit; (APl, CT2, and/or PA10 collections) and in the com- munity transect collections I conducted, the great majority (87%) were found to occur in savanna forest (37%), dry, open forest (29%), or savanna woodland

    Here again, we find evidence of the fact that upiti species are typical of those spe- cies found in transitional environments, exactly the sorts ofenvironments in which aptti are most often found. Moreover, the remarkable correspondence among the three apiti collections with regard to the

    (21%).

    occurrence of apiti species in local natural communities suggests that little differ- ence exists between the purported an- thropogenic apiti (APl and PA10) and the natural, control upit2 (CT2). Such correspondence would be unexpected if 75%-85% of APlIPAlO species were in fact transplants, particularly given that roughly 57% of APIIPAIO species oc- curred in the savanna forest andlor sa- vanna woodland transects. There are ex- tensive areas of savanna forest and sa- vanna woodland throughout the local area and within easy reach of the village; it makes little sense to bother transplant- ing species from these areas when they are so readily available.

    Thirteen percent (9 species) were found to occur in the b i rararalba pk forest transect, the relatively dark, dense ever- green mesophytic broadleaf tall forest (Figure 2). Interestingly, 5 of these 9 spe- cies also occurred in the control island, and 6 of the 9 are very common in at least one of the other transects I collected: Amaioua guianensis, commonly found in dry, open forest; Himanthus sucuuba, com- monly found in savanna forest; Protium unifoliolatum, very common in dry, open forest; Sacoglottis guianensis, very common in dry, open forest and found in savanna forest and savanna woodland; Siparuna guianensis, common in savanna forest; Vi- rola sebifera, common in dry, open forest and savanna forest.

    Herbarium Collection

    In the summer of 1988, I had the op- portunity to work at the herbarium of the Museu Paraense Emilio Goeldi; both Po- sey and Anderson were staff researchers there, and the Museu Goeldi herbarium houses both our collections. I worked

    Table 3 Occurrence of up&? species in local natural communities (YO).

    Aptti Savanna woodland Savanna forest Dry, open forest Tropical rain forest AP 1 20 39 30 1 1 CT2 20 38 27 15 PA10 24 33 30 13 Mean 21 37 29 13

  • 416 AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOCIST [94, 19921

    through the herbarium collection folders for each of the 120 species collected in the original Posey-Anderson ten-island sur- vey, recording the collection locale de- scriptions made by other researchers who had also deposited specimens of these species with the herbarium. I n most cases, there were several specimens for each species. As Table 4 clearly demon- strates, the vast majority of those species collected by Posey and Anderson were commonly found (by other researchers) in savanna-forest transitions, savanna woodlands (camp0 cerrado), savanna forest (cerradio), disturbed areas (including abandoned pastures), and secondary growth areas. In sum, then, the Museu Goeldis own herbar ium collection clearly and definitively demonstrates that the species found by Posey and Anderson in their study area were precisely those commonlyfound in such areas: the disturbed margin ofa savanna and forest transition.

    Informant Plant Identijcation Interviews

    In separate sessions, each plant speci- men collected in the API/CT2 transects was shown to Beptopoop, a key Posey in- formant for the original PA10 study, and to Bakoti, a very well respected village elder. Information elicited included com- mon name(s), use-value(s), where com- monly found, and which, if any, animals, birds, or fish used the plant for food. Dr. Anne Gely, a French ethnobotanist then on the staff of the botany department at the Museu Goeldi, sat in on the informant interviews.

    Posey reported that his informants in- dicated no less than 98% of the 120 PA 10 plant species had at least one use-value, and that 75% (and perhaps even 85%) of these plants had actually been trans- planted into the up&?. Neither Beptopoop nor Bakoti provided support for Poseys claims.

    Table 5 summarizes the interview re- sults. What is most striking is the agree- ment between Beptopoop and Bakoti re- garding the number of plants considered useless (kuigo). Beptopoop described al- most half(47%) of the APl plants (the anthropogenic apt%) as having no value and Bakoti listed 49% as useless. Inter-

    estingly enough, the CT2 plant collection was considered by both informants to have a greater percentage of useful spe- cies. The percentage of kaigo species re- ported by both informants is particularly puzzling in the context of Poseys claim that of the 120 species collected only Heteroptelys sp. 2 (Malpighiaceae) and Eu- genia cf. protacta (Myrtaceae) were said to be useless (kaigo) (Anderson and Posey 1989: 160).

    A considerable percentage (25% when informant responses for APl/CT2 were averaged) of plant specimens were con- sidered to have value only in the sense that they served as a food source for ani- mals, birds, or fish. Moreover, fully 15% of both APl and CT2 species were con- sidered useful only because one bird spe- cies or another ate some portion of the plant. It is not entirely clear to what ex- tent the G. Kayap6 consider a nonagri- cultural plant useful when its only recog- nized value is that some faunal species uses it as a food. This point was made abundantly obvious on several occasions when Beptopoop described a plant as use- less and then added as an afterthought that birds eat it.

    I t is clear tha t knowledge of what plants are consumed by which faunal spe- cies is valuable as a general piece of infor- mation about the habits of particular an- imals. Such knowledge is valuable simply because the natural world is better under- stood. Knowledge of feeding habits, of course, has a more functional value in that it can increase hunting success. However, i t seems extremely unlikely that the G. Kayap6 would transplant spe- cies simply because they serve as a source of food for birds when these plants occur naturally in savanna, savanna forest, and dry forest: all commonly found in the lo- cal area. More to the point, neither infor- mant reported any APl /CT2 species listed as a food source for animals, birds, or fish as having been planted.

    If we combine the species without rec- ognized value together with those only valuable as bird food, we find that 6 1 O/O of APl and 51% of CT2 species have little or no value. If we take the further step of adding the additional plant species listed

  • RBEARCH REPORT^ 41 7

    Table 4 Description of collection locations for species listed in the Posey-Anderson study area (PAlO) and housed at the herbarium of the Museu Coeldi, Belkm.

    Plant species Where found

    Alibntia edulis Rich.

    Aliberfia myciijolia Schum. Alibntia U C M C O S ~ Moore Amaioua guiancnsis Aubl. Andira cuiabensis Benth. Amona dmicoma Mart. Antonia ouata Pohl Ardisia sp. Arrabidaea inaequalis (DC. ex. Spletz) Schum. Bombax quaticum (Aubl.) K. Sch. Buchmauia sp.

    Byrsonima coriacca (Sw.) Kunth. Byrsonima crassifolia H.B.K. Carprofochc sp. Caryocar braziliense St. Hil. Carearia arborca (Rich.) Urb. Casearia sylvcstris Sw. Cecropia palmafa Willd. Chaetocarpus sp. Clusia insignis Coccoloba excclsa Benth. Coccoloba paniculata Meissn. Combrefum rotundijolium Rich. Crcpidospermum gondotianum (Tul.) Tr. & PI. Curatella amcricana L. Cybianthus myrianthos Miq. vel. sp. aK. Cybianthus SQ. Dioclca macrocarpa Huber Diospyros artanthaefolia Mart. ex Miq. Diospyros practennissa Sandw. Doliocarpus cf. dentafus (Aubl.) Standl. Duquctia spixiana Mart. Emmotum fagijolium Dew. Emmotum nitens (Benth.) Miers Enferolobium ellipticum Benth. Enferolobium schomburgkii Benth. Erythroxylurn macroplyllum Cav. Erythroxylum suberosum St. Hil.

    Erythroxylum subracernosum Turcz. Eugenia cf. pafrisii Vahl. Eugenia cf. cupulata Amsh. Eugenia cf. profacta Berg. Eugenia eurychcila Berg. Faramea cf. longijolia Ficus amazonica (Miq.) Miq. Ficusgomcllcira Kunth. & Bouche Forstcronia aK. guianensis M. Arg. Guatteria gracilipes R. E. Fries Guatteria sp. Heteroptnys sp. 1 and 2 Himatanthus articulata (Vahl.) W k d . Himatanthus sucuuba (Spruce) Wood. Hirtclla cf. racernosa Lam. Humiriasfrum cf. curpidatum (Benth.) Cuatr. Hymenaea courbaril L.

    High campo; disturbed vegetation; margin degraded

    Capoeiro; savanna-forest margin; campina Dry forest on laterite ridge Campina; campo; low forest; secondary forest margin Cmado Capoeiro Common in campo; campina; low forest; camp0 rupcstrc Campina forrst Campo; natural campo; disturbed areas Common, frcquently along margin of river Common in natural campo; campo rupestre; flooded

    Low forest; campo; capoeiro Savanna; common in campo; common in pasture Capoeiro Camp0 cerrado; cenado; campina on white sand (w/s) Margin secondary forest; capoeiro; natural campo Margin secondary forest; campo; w/s campo Campina; capoeiro No collection folder Disturbed w/s area; campo; campian on w/s Small granite islands Capoeiro 1 specimen, 2-year-old capoeiro Capoeiro; pasture margin Cenado tipo campina; savanna; ubiquitous in campo No collection folder No collection rolder Capoeiro; campina; capoeirojna (fine or recent) Capoeiro Campo; capoeiro alto (high) Ccrrado tip0 mmpina; open forest; capoeiro; campo Tenajrme forrst; uarzea (flooded areas) Pasture; campo; low scrub forest Campina; secondary forest; common in campo No collection folder 2-year capoeiro; disturbed forest areas Capoeiro; open forest; secondary vegetation Open cenado w/scattered trees; campo sujo; c a m p

    Margin of tree islands in cerrado; savanna; cerrado Capoeiro; campina forest Tenajrme; gallery forest Cerrado; campina; campo 2 samples: f enojrme; igapo (flooded forest) Campo Campina; low capociro forest; capoeiro Open campina 1 sample, forested hill No collection folder Many exemplars from all environments No collection folders Campina; high campo; low forest Disturbed w/s campina forest; secondary forest; cerrado Capoeiro; cerrado Campinarara; campina No collection folder

    forest

    areas

    edge

    continued on next page

  • 418 AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST [94, 19921

    Table 4 (continued)

    Plant spccirs, Whrrc Ibund

    l lex am. aJini.r Gardn. Jacaranda rufa Manso Kielintyera cf. rugnsn Choisy Imistema agre,qntum (Brrg.) Rushy I,asiacis am. ligulntn Hitrhc. & Chase. Lirania latlfolia Bcnth. rx Hook. f. Mabea,/.rtulijera Mart. Machaerium acutijolium Vog. Marhnerium pilo.rum Brnth. Maprnunea guianen.ris Auhl.

    Ma.\cagnia sp. 1 and 2

    Matqyba ,pianenri.r Aubl.

    Mcvtenu.r sp . Miconin alata 1)C

    Mironia cf. mrlinonis Naud. Micnnia riliata (Rich.) DC.

    Micnnia macrnthvrtn Brnth. Mirrophnlis cf. ralophyl1nide.r Iircs Mnuriri sp. Myrcia atramentijern Myrriafallax (Rich.) I X . Mvrria nbtusa Schau. Neen sp.

    Oenocarpus di.rtirliu.\ Mart. Ourntea nitida EngI. falirnuren crorea (Sw.) Rocni. & Schult.

    Iera distichnpl~vlla (Mart.) Bsill. fhilodendrnn neutntum Schott . Pi/>torarpha sp. I ln t /~ymei t inf~l io~n.ra Bcnth. Prntium unlfoliolatum EngI. I.yhotria sp. Qualea paruijlnm Mart. Qualea sp. Roupnla montann Auhl. var. dentatn Rnurea r f . cu.rpidatn Brnth. rs Bakcr Sacogloftis piunensis Brnth. Sacn,$ottis cf . ceratnnrar/)n h c k c Sapium sp. Srheflera sp. Serjania sp.

    Simarouba amarn Auhl. Sifiaruna guianeitris Auhl. Smilax cf. .rrlinmbur,okiana Kunth. Solnnum cr. juripebn Rich. Snlanum grandipnrum Snrncea pilleinininnn Gaud.

    Stvrax Ruyanen.ri.r A. 1)C. Syap.r cnrnide.r Mart. Sva,qrus comosn (Mart.) Mart. Svmp1oo.r ,pianensis (Auhl.) Gurkc Svmplocns sp.

    Capoeiro; frrqurnt in rerrada Common in rerrado; rampo cerrado; rampino on w/s Cerrado: campo cerrado Opcn forest; rapoeirn Oprn forrst; srcondary vrgrtation Capoeirn; transitional forest; low forest; roadside Cerrado: vrry common roadside; airport margin Capneiro; camjin rerrado; trrc islands in savanna Capoeiro Open disturhcd cerrado; disturbrd camfiina forest;

    Campo; forest w/hrokrn canopy; disturbrd forest

    Capoeiro; savanna intrrsprrscd w/trrr and scrub

    No collrction foldrr Campo; cerrado mixrd w/forrst patchcs; srcondary

    Campo; low campo Cerrado forrst; cerrado w/trrr islands; margin

    savanna/forrst Cerrado; natural campo; savanna margin No collection folder No collection bldcr Capoeiro: srcondary vcgrtation nrar airport Campina: low forest; common capoeiro Low cnpoeirnr rainpn cerradn: primary low Forest Margin savanna/forrst; scmi-dcciduous forest;

    Common in campo, rerrado Cerrado Common srcondary vcgrtation roadsidr; campo

    Campo; rerrado Capoeiro: oprn rocky outcrops Sccondary vrgrtation; horder of gallrry forest Campo sujn; margin ofpasturr and kmst No collrction foldrr No collrction foldrr Cerrndo; campo cerradn: oprn rerrado w/scattcrcd trrrs Canipina; campo rupe.rtre Campo cerrado; ce~rado Campina: low hr rs t Capoeirn; cerradn; eampina Capoeiro: campojrhado (closrd) Low forest; campina: hurned arras; rupe.rtre vegetation Forrst; savanna forcst; cnatinga Secondary forest; w/s campinn; margin or primary

    Edgr of forest; natural campo w/groups trces; c a m p Capoeiro; high capoeirn; margin of forest and garden No collection foldrr Capoeiro; roadsidc Natural campo w/groups or trees Savanna; cerradn; disturhrd tropical forest; disturbed

    Disturbed uarzea forcst; riverbank Conimon in campo and cerradn Common in campo and cerrado Campina: w/s campo; srcondary forest; capoeiro Cerrado; savanidforrst margin

    low forrst

    rdar

    islands

    forrst

    cerrado

    margin

    forest

    forest

  • RESEARCH REPORTS 419

    Table 4 (continued)

    Table 5 Informant descriptions of a@%l plant use-values.

    API ("/o) CT2 ("/o) Informants Informants

    Plant use-values Beptopoop Bakoti Beptopoop Bakoti Kaigo 47 49 38 30 Medicinal 37 12 39 25

    (including birds)

    Human food 0 15

    Animals only 17 25 20 39

    (15) (20) (Birds only) (13) (14) 4 6

    as having value only as a food source for animals, the percentage of plants with lit- tle or no value increases to 69% for API and to 64% for CT2.6

    The number of plant species reported to have some sort of medicinal value was significant. Beptopoop, a principal Kay- ap6 shaman, described 37% of APl spe- cies and 39% of CT2 species as having medicinal value. Again, however, we find that little difference exists between the

    anthropogcnic island and the control is- land with respect to thc pcrcentagc of usc- ful spccics. Incidcntally, Bakoti, whilc rc- porting fcwcr spccics to have mcdicinal valuc than was the case with Beptopoop, still dcscribcd twicc as many CT2 spccics as having medicinal valuc comparcd to API spccics (25 vs. 12).

    Informant statcmcnts regarding whcrc API and CT2 plants are commonly found arc particularly instructive. Each of thc

  • 420 AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST [94, 19921

    two informants was asked to list the types of environments (forest and savanna types) in which a plant species could be expected to be encountered. It was often the case that multiple environments were mentioned. If upit2 are natural-and not constructed-and composed of species characteristic of the dynamic zone be- tween savannalsavanna forest and dry forest areas, then we should expect infor- mant descriptions of species occurrence to reflect this: Table 6 demonstrates that they did.

    In the case of APl, both informants in- cluded savanna or savanna forest for 80% of species, and dry forest for 7 1 YO of plant specimens. A remarkable 98% of plant species had either savanna/savanna for- est or dry forest listed as areas within which they could be found. For 51% of API plants, both savannalsavanna forest and dry forest were listed.

    The CT2 plant distribution descrip- tions were much the same as those re- ported for APl . Savannalsavanna forest was listed for 84%, dry forest for 72%, sa- vannalsavanna forest or dry forest for 96%, and savannalsavanna forest and dry forest for 7 1 O/O.

    General Informant Interviews

    My interviews in Gorotire were con- ducted in Portuguese, not GE, the lan- guage of the Kayap6. With the exception of one informant, Beptopoop, Posey con- ducted all ofhis interviews with key infor- mants in Portuguese as well (Kwyrb KB, Ir i , Ute). For the first half of my 1987 field stay, I worked with Ira, a remarka-

    ble individual and the son of Kwyrl KB, the animal spirits shaman in the village and one of the most knowledgeable of the G. Kayap6. I r i was not only fluent in Portuguese, but was one of the very few KayapB who could both read and write Gt . In addition to his repeated assertions to me that aptt i as described by Posey were nonexistent, he informed me that he had told Posey on several occasions in 1983 and 1984 that his ideas about apt%? were wrong.

    It was I r i who first brought to my at- tention a linguistic objection to Posey's description of apit i as anthropogenic in origin. Recall that the first stage in Po- sey's aptti development hierarchy was la- beled up&-nu (Figure 3). When I asked if a small island in the savanna was an aptti- nu, I ra said that my question did not make sense. He went on to explain that the suffix -nu (or -turn) is only attached to objects or things that are ofhuman origin. Nu indicates something is young or new, while turn indicates that something is old. So, a new garden ispuru-nu and an old gar- den is puru-turn, and a relatively new old field is an ibe-nu while an older old field is an ibe-turn (the term ibe encompasses the concept of secondary forest succession). New garden and old fields, of course, are the product of human activity. Ira's ob- jection to the description of a small wooded island being labeled apt%-nu was consistent with his statement that upitti are natural in origin.

    During the 1987 field period, I spoke with nearly every Kayap6 who was con- versant in Portuguese. Not one of these individuals supported the idea that apt%?

    Table 6 Local communities in which apiti? species are also found, according to informants.

    API ("/o) CT2 (Yo) Informants Informants

    Type of natural community Beptopoop x Bakoti Beptopoop A? Bakoti 1. Savanna, savanna forest 78 80 82 96 84 72 2. Dry forest 63 71 79 68 72 76 1 o r 2 100 98 96 99 96 93 A@; 22 13 4 4 6 8

  • RESEARCH REPORT^ 42 1

    were created or that they planted useful plant species in apiti. I should observe that I did not invoke Posey as the source for my questions in these interviews, thus leaving the informants to believe that I was the source of the concept; this did not redound to my credit.

    I interviewed several of the village el- ders regarding the apitiproblem, with Irii providing translation. In particular, I worked with Bakoti, the respected village elder mentioned earlier, and taped the in- terviews. Bakoti rejected every aspect of Poseys description of upit? creation and management, including the notion that plants are transplanted into apit2. This pattern of rejection and denial was re- peated again and again as I spoke with various Kayap6, including Kanhon and Totoi, the principal chiefs of the village.

    During my stay, I had occasion to dis- cuss the apiti concept with Earl Trapp, a missionary who has lived among the Kay- ap6, including the G. Kayap6, for more than 25 years. He said he was unaware of any practice involving the creation of for- est islands or of any planting taking place in apiti. He also said he was unaware of any use of these islands during periods of illness (the concept of apiti as refuge sites is discussed below).

    Oddly enough, one of the more com- pelling informant refutations of the apiti concept took place entirely serendipi- tously. Having been ill for awhile in March 1987, I was trying to leave the vil- lage to obtain medical attention. One day, having heard that a plane might pass by, I had gone out to the landing strip in hopes of hitching a ride. Durval, a Brazil- ian ex-missionary fluent in G t , also awaited the plane. He was then employed by the Kayap6 as the village accountant and as their translator in all negotiations with the outside. I took the opportunity to ask him about apiti-I explained Poseys theory to him in detail-and he replied that he had never heard anything about the Kayap6 either creating or utilizing aptti in any particular way and that he thought the idea very unlikely.

    The spot where we waited was on a main path from distant gardens and pass- ing Kayap6 often stop and chat with

    whomever is waiting by the dirt landing strip. For more than three hours (the plane never arrived), Durval queried passing Kayap6 about aptti: more than 20 men and women were questioned. Without exception, each individual and/ or group of individuals replied that they neither created ap2ti nor planted the plants found in apiti.

    Ape t^e ^Frequency In support of his claim that aptti were

    anthropogenic in origin, Posey stated that the frequency of forest island occurrence in savanna notably increases the closer the savanna is to the village (1985a: 141). Apitido not notably increase in frequency in savanna that is closer to the village. Figure 4, derived from Thematic Map- per-processed Landsat imagery at a 1: 100,000 scale, shows Poseys assertion to be incorrect. Indeed, it would appear that precisely the opposite is the case: apiti frequency is highest at greater dis- tances from the village.

    Apt% Fire Protection Posey also observed that upit2 are often

    protected from the regular burns of the savanna conducted during the summer months (1984b:12). I have not been in Gorotire during the drier summer months (July and August), when major campo burns are undertaken. However, in 1984 and 1987 I was in the village at the begin- ning of the dry season and witnessed a number of small burns (three in the gen- eral vicinity of Poseys study area) and one major burn elsewhere. I did not ob- serve any attempt to protect apit2, but it must be said that the small size of the fires might account for this.

    Posey has spoken of firebreaks con- structed around valued apit2, but I could find no physical evidence for these (either above ground or in cursory examinations of soils for variations in ash content). Where burns had been conducted, it ap- peared that the fires would burn up to and just into the margin of islands but rarely penetrated (unless quite small) deep into the interior. I could distinguish little difference between a p i t i in burn

  • 422 A AILRl~:.A N ANTHROPOLOGIST (94, 19921

    Figure 4 Frequency and distribution of major forest islands in savanna.

    areas near the village and other forest is- lands in more distant savanna where burns had been conducted at some point. One would expect protected a p t t i to somehow be difrerent from those not pro- tected; such differences were not de- tected. Finally, I could not find a single informant who supported Poseys claim that aptt i were protected during campo burns.

    Additional Rationales for Constructing Apeti?

    Poscy has repeatedly asserted that apt% serve as important refuge sites for Indian families during epidemics and warfare (for womcn and children). The idea is that families wait out periods-even

    prolonged periods (1984b: 1 1)-of ill- ness and/or hostilities within the protec- tion of the aptti. He has also reported per- sonally witnessing the refuge use of apt% during a measles outbreak in 1983 (An- derson and Posey 1987:49, 1989:172).

    Apdi as refuge make little sense. The apitt investigated by Posey are concen- trated along the margin of the forest, less than 700 meters from the village. If one sought refuge from disease in any of these aptti, both the proximity of the village and other nearby apit t would keep risk high, particularly for children playing in the general area. Presumably, more than one family would be seeking refuge in aptti; indeed, Posey reported that five families with young children moved to a p i t i , where they stayed isolated until the threat h a d passed ( A n d e r s o n a n d Posey

  • RESEARCH REPORTS 423

    1987:49). Perhaps more problematic is the simple fact that the obvious place to go during such periods would be to ones swidden garden (Figure 5). Shelter, food, water (always nearby), and reasonable isolation (especially in comparison to the upttt) are all available, plus much better opportunities for hunting. True, some food sources could be found in aptt2, but they are neither of the value nor the quan- tity that common sense dictates would be found in garden environs. All aptti lack water, and their general location within the much hotter savanna environment leads to average midday temperatures several degrees higher than found in swidden sites within the forest (not to mention the immediate proximity of streams for bathing).

    The problem for the refuge rationale is not resolved by arguing that Indians travel to more distant updiduring periods of illness and warfare. If one is to travel that far, it would make more sense to travel to ones garden.

    With regard to Poseys claim of having witnessed use of aptti as refuge by Goro- tire Kayap6 during a measles outbreak in 1983, I was unable to obtain a single con- firmation from informants, missionaries,

    or FUNAI (Brazilian National Indian Agency) personnel that any Indians had ever stayed in apitt near the village for any reason. For that matter, no one could re- call any particular threat of measles dur- ing this period.

    Dgeme Posey has described the use of aptti for

    defensive purposes in terms of their value as hiding and observation posts (para- pets) and as locations from which the G. Kayap6 can prepare for an approaching enemy (lines of defense) (1985a: 142). He has further argued that two stages of apit t development, apitipoire and a p i t i rhynh, represent intentionally linked (joined) apiti with interconnecting trails along which warriors can move, noting that the long types are specifically for purpose of attack (1984b:ll).

    The notion that upit; are valued defen- sive installations seems farfetched in- deed. To begin with, Figures 2 and 4 show the general location of apiti not only in Poseys study locale but also in other savanna environs of the general area. It should be noted that the incidence and concentration of apttt increase with prox- imity to the forest margin; that is, we find

    Figure 5 AWi? and principal agricultural areas near Gorotire.

  • 424 AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST [94, 19921

    the highest number of forest patches (aptti?) along the savanna-forest bound- ary. Why build apttt along forest margins when the forest is already there? I t would appear obvious that the forest itselfwould provide far greater tactical advantage and strategic flexibility in the event of an impending attack. Moreover, the very fact that apttt stand apart from the forest poses problems for G. Kayap6 who would use them; movement to and from the apttt would increase the risk of being seen, and the communication of information from observers to the main war party (includ- ing chiefs) would necessitate departure from the uptttwith its attendant risk ofex- posure.

    A far more curious aspect of this de- fense argument is the fact that the defen- sive value of apttt would appear limited to those occasions when the attacking force moved through the open savanna. This would be a singularly dubious bit of strat- egy. The general environment provides ample opportunity for surreptitious ap- proach. A range of low mountains rises up immediately behind the village with forest cover along the lower slopes, offer- ing several advantageous routes for the opposing force. The extensive reach of forests (and forest cover) in the general area (Figures 2, 4) also suggests that an approach through open campo would be unlikely.

    Rest and Rendezvous The same general criticism of the de-

    fense rationale applies here as well. Why would one rest in a small forest patch in the hotter savanna environment with no water when the cooler forest and a nearby stream are available? Sexual rendezvous might take place in apttt, but I should think they would be opportunistic in na- ture. For planned assignations, it would seem far safer to find an isolated spot within the forest where the possibility of being seen reaching and leaving the site would be considerably less than would be the case with an aptti rendezvous.

    Location of the PA10 Study Area Finally, and particularly troublesome

    for Poseys upttt claims, are the actual lo-

    cation and recent history ofhis aptttstudy area (PA1 0). Figure 6 is a copy of a pho- tograph taken from a recent article on apttt (Anderson and Posey 1989: 16 1 ) that shows the general area within which Po- seys ten islands are found.8 In the pho- tograph can be seen the landing strip, a road leading to the landing strip (top left), a logging road winding away from the strip (bottom left to right), and a small building.

    The building was erected, and the gen- eral area around and behind the building site cleared, by the Brazilian Air Force in the 1970s. The original idea was for a small detachment of air force personnel and aircraft to be stationed there to sup- port government efforts to quell a land war that had arisen in the Concei@o de Araguaia region just to the southeast of Gorotire. Deployment of personnel did not take place.

    The building itself has been long aban- doned. The G. Kayap6 have neither used the building nor allowed it to be affected by the frequent burns of the nearby land- ing-strip area (evidence of a recent burn can be seen in the darkened areas near the lower end of the landing strip).

    With this brief history of the Posey study area shown in the photograph, an altogether different reality than the one Posey has constructed suggests itself. Far from being an intensively managed area of intentionally created islands composed ofuseful transplants, the Posey study area is in fact an indentation in the forest mar- gin caused by a nonindigenous popula- tion: the Brazilian Air Force. Because the area was not subsequently used or devel- oped by the air force, and because the G. Kayap6 have exercised some selective protection of the area from burns, the area is undergoing secondary forest succession. Poseys anthropogenic islands are in actuality the leading elements of a forest recapturing territory lost. They may contain useful species, and the G. Kayap6 may make occasional use of these species, but they certainly were not cre- ated and maintained by Kayap6. Apttt are the result of natural processes along the dynamic frontier between forest (both

  • RESEARCH REPORTS 425

    Figure 6 Aerial imagery of Posey study area. (Reprinted with permission from Anderson and Posey 1989:161. Circled number on photograph is from the original article.)

    savanna and dry forest types) and the sa- vanna.

    Summary and Conclusion

    Poseys work has received extraordi- nary attention in recent years. His pub- lished findings, particularly with regard to upttt, have considerable theoretical im- port for tropical forest research, both hu- man and nonhuman. He has argued that the Gorotire Kayap6 actually increase ecological diversity, intentionally create and manage ecotones, transplant species from an area larger than Western Europe, and have altered the ecology of the sa- vanna and forest so as to call into ques- tion the very notion of a natural flora.

    Unfortunately, there is no basis for ac- cepting Poseys statements regarding uFtt2. Despite the inconsistencies in his published findings, and notwithstanding significant problems in the methodologi- cal approach adopted by Posey (e.g., the lack of controls), upik? have become firmly established in both Amazonian ethnog- raphy and in the public imagination. In

    this discussion, I have provided several reasons for questioning Poseys claims.

    There exists no evidence that upit; found in Poseys study area (e.g., AP1) possess greater ecological diversity than natural forest islands (e.g., CT2)-a key Posey claim. It was shown that a signifi- cant percentage (61%) of AP1 plants were also found in CT2. Furthermore, it was noted that the vast majority of API species not found in CT2 were either common to surrounding forest or sa- vanna-forest communities or were in- cluded on lists of common savanna plants. No evidence exists to support Po- seys claim that 20% of upit2 species were transplanted from other regions having a total area the size of Western Europe.

    Poseys claim that upit2 increase in fre- quency as one approaches the village was shown to be incorrect. I t was also noted that aptti occur most often in the bound- ary area between savanna and dry forest, precisely the sort of dynamic environ- ment in which small islands of forest (re- sulting either from forest advance or re- treat) are likely to be encountered.

  • 426 AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST 194, 19921

    The review of the Museu Goeldi her- barium collection of the 120 species that Anderson a n d Posey collected in their ten-island study indicated that virtually all these species are common to savanna, savanna forest, dry forest, and disturbed environments: a n accurate description of the PA10 study area.

    Informant plant identifications yielded no evidence that API species were more useful than CT2 species; ironically, the opposite proved to be the case. Almost half of the species collected in both aptti were considered to have no value by both informants (compare this with Poseys re- port that 98% of the 120 plants collected in the PA10 study were considered use- ful). The great majority of both @ti col- lections (87%) were reported by infor- mants to be common to savanna wood- land, savanna forest, and dry forest areas found throughout the local area.

    The very notion that apt% are created and maintained by the G. Kayap6, that these i s lands a r e composed of t r ans - planted species, was repudiated by the villagers themselves. Repeated efforts to have them confirm Poseys scheme of things proved fruitless. The secondary ra- tionales offered by Posey for constructing aptti were shown to be less than compel- ling. Finally, the general features of Po- seys study area were shown to be the re- sult of environmental modification by the Brazilian Air Force, not by the Gorotire Kayap6.

    Notes Acknowledgments. The research reported in

    this paper was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation (INT 8405279 and SES 8608123), and by a Summer Faculty Fellowship from the University of Maryland, Baltimore County (UMBC). Partial support for the expenses of the plant collection team was provided by a Conselho Nacional de De- senvolvimento Cientifico e Tecnol6gico (CNPq) grant administered by Dr. Anthony Anderson of thr Museu Paraense Emilio Goeldi. Figures were prepared by the Carto- graphic Services Staff at UMBC: special thanks to Joseph School (director) and to Me- lanie Orwig. The plant surveys could not have been accomplished without Carlos Rosirio da Silva, whose insights and good humor under

    very trying circumstances were invaluable. I would like to thank Dr. Anne Gely for her par- ticipation in informant interviews on plant uses. Dr. Elaine Elisabetsky provided crucial field support during both the 1984 and 1987 field seasons, and her comments and sugges- tions regarding both the research and this pa- per were of great value. Thanks to Andrew Miller and an anonymous reviewer for sug- gesting ways to improve the organization of the discussion. I would like to thank the Go- rotire community, Chiefs Kanhon, Totoi, and Ute, and key informants Ir5, Bakoti, and Bep- topoop for their patience and assistance. Fi- nally, I would like to acknowledge and re- member the late Kwyri K i , shaman, for his counsel, stories, and humanity.

    The significance of Poseys work extends well beyond publications read largely by the anthropological community. He has spoken to and reported his findings for an extraordinary number of conferences, meetings, seminars, workshops, universities, and reporters. Kay- ap6 resource management practices as de- scribed by Posey have received much atten- tion and publicity: essayed in Nature (May 1984), discussed in the National Academy of Sciences landmark Biodiversity edited by E. 0. Wilson (1988), reported in many newspapers, highlighted in the widely read New Yvrk Times Science Times section (Stevens 1990), fea- tured in documentaries by the National Geo- graphic Society and the British Broadcasting Company, and reported upon by CNN (sev- eral times), ABC, NBC, CBS, and NPR news organizations.

    2The southern boundary of rain forest in- dicated in Figure 1 is based on the work (and published maps) of Colinvaux (1989), Eiten (1972, 1983), Goodland and Ferri (1979), Huber (1987), and Soares (1953).

    Terms in parentheses are Kayap6; the Por- tuguese term campo cerradv is often used in place of the English equivalent ofsavanna and scrub savanna.

    4Three @2tt? papers were coauthored with Anthony Anderson, then on the staff of the De- partment of Botany at the Museu Paraense Emilio Goeldi, in Belim, Para State, Brazil. Posey was responsible for all ethnographic and cultural data relating to ap2ti?, while An- dersons role was limited largely to directing the plant collections taken within the study area demarcated by Posey.

    The forest and savanna-forest community transects were 10 X 250 meters with all plants >7 cm DBH (diameter at breast height) col- lected. Each plant was subsequently shown to at least two informants at different times and

  • RESEARCH REPOREY 427

    the name(s), use-value(s), and locale where commonly found were elicited for each speci- men.

    %alculated by averaging and adding re- sponses of both informants for each ap&. For example, API mean useless response was 48%; bird food value only 13%; animal value (including birds) 2 1 %: 48 + 13 = 6 1, and 48 + 21 = 69.

    In my interviews with Bakoti, I was careful not to invoke Posey as the source for the con- cept of apiti. In a key interview session, it is clear from the tape that as we work through the concept, and as I repeatedly try new ways of getting Bakoti to confirm that the Kayap6 actually do create and manage apiti, Bakoti is becoming more and more amused with me. My pride increasingly wounded by his pitiful glances in my direction, I am heard to say in frustration and humiliation to Ira (in Portu- guese, and not translated for Bakoti): Well, its not ~y idea!

    RA second aerial view of the study area, from a different perspective, can be found on page 48 of a 1987 article entitled Refloresta- mento Indigena (Anderson and Posey 1987); in English the title is Indigenous Reforesta- tion.

    References Cited

    Anderson, Anthony, and Darrell Posey 1985 Manejo de Cerrados pelos Indios

    Kayap6. Boletim de Museu Paraense Emilio Goeldi (Bothica) 2( 1):77-98.

    1987 Reflorestamento Indigena. Citncia Hoje 6(Maio):44-51.

    1989 Management of a Tropical Scrub Savanna by the Gorotire Kayap6 of Bra- zil. Advances in Economic Botany 7:159- 173.

    Askew, G. P., D. J. Moffatt, R. Montgomery, and P. L. Sear1

    Soils and Soil Moisture as Factors Influencing the Distribution of the Veg- etation Formations of the Sierra do Ron- cador, Mato Grosso. In 111 Simp6sio sobre o Cerrado. Mario Ferri, coordena- dor. Pp. 150-160. SHo Paulo: Universi- dade de SHo Paulo.

    1971

    Colinvaux, Paul 1989 The Past and Future Amazon. Sci-

    entific American (May):102-108. Eiten, George

    1972 The Cerrado Vegetation of Brazil. Botanical Review 38:210-341.

    1983 Classificaqio da VegetaqHo do Bra- sil. Brasilia: Conselho Nacional de De- senvolvimento Cientifico e Tecnolbgico.

    Goodland, Robert, and Mario Ferri 1979 Ecologia do Cerrado. Sio Paulo:

    Universidade de Sio Paulo. Huber, Otto

    1987 Neotropical Savannas: Their Flora and Vegetation. Trends in Evolution and Ecology 2167-7 1.

    May, Robert 1984 Prehistory of Amazonian Indians.

    Nature 312:19-20. Posey, Darrell

    1982 Keepers ofthe Forest. Garden 6:1& 24.

    1983 Indigenous Ecological Knowledge and Development of the Amazon. I n The Dilemma of Amazon Development. Em- ilio Moran, ed. Pp. 225-257. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

    A Preliminary Report on Diversi- fied Management of Tropical Forest by the Kayap6 Indians of the Brazilian Am- azon. Advances in Economic Botany

    1984b Keepers of the Campo. Garden

    1984c Ethnoecology as Applied Anthro- pology in Amazonian Development. Hu- man Organization 43:95-107.

    1984d 0 s Kayap6 e a Natureza. CiCncia Hoje 4(Maio):35-41.

    1984e An Ethnological Investigation of Resource Management by the Kayap6 Indians of Gorotire, Para (Brazil). Grant Proposal, National Geographic Society (Dec. 1984).

    1985a Indigenous Management of Tropi- cal Forest Ecosystems: The Case of the Kayap6 Indians of the Brazilian Ama- zon. Agroforestry Systems 3:139-158.

    1985b Native and Indigenous Guidelines for New Amazonian Development Strat- egies: Understanding Biodiversity through Ethnoecology. In Change in the Amazon, Vol. 1. John Hemming, ed. Pp. 156-181. Manchester: Manchester Uni- versity Press.

    1987a Alternatives to Destruction: Sci- ence of the Mebengbkre [Kayapb]. Be- Em: Museu Paraense Emilio Goeldi.

    1987b Manejo de Floresta Secundaria, Capoeiras, Campos, e Cerrados (Kay- ap6). In SUMA Etnol6gica Brasileira, Vol. 1: Etnobiologia. Berta Ribeiro, ed. Pp. 173-185. Petropolis: FINEP.

    Temas e Inquiriqoes em Etnoento- mologia. Boletim de Museu Paraense Emilio Goeldi (Antropologia) 3:99-133.

    1984a

    1:112-126.

    8~8-12, 32.

    1987c

  • 428 AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST [94, 19921

    1988 Kayap6 Indian Natural Resource Management. In People of the Tropical Rainforest. J. S. Denslow and C. Pad- doch, eds. Pp. 89-90. Berkeley: Univer- sity of California Press.

    The Science of the Mebeng6kre. Or- ion (Summer): 16-23.

    Ratter, J. A., P. W. Richards, G. Argent, and D. R. Gifford

    Observations on the Vegetation of Northeastern Mato Grosso, 1: T h e Woody Vegetation Types of the Xavan- tina-Cachimbo Expedition Area. Philo- sophical Transactions of the Royal Soci- ety (Series B) 266:449-492.

    1990

    1973

    Soares, L. C. 1953 Limites Meridionais e Orientais da

    Area de Ocorrtncia da Floresta Amaz6n- ica em Territ6rio Brasileiro. Revista Brasileira de Geografia 15:3- 122.

    Stevens, William 1990 Research in Virgin Amazon Un-

    covers Complex Farming. New York Times, Science Times, April 3, pp. C 1, 12.

    Wilson, E. O., ed. 1988 Biodiversity. Washington, DC: Na-

    tional Academy Press.

    Fishing the Waters of Amazonia: Native Subsistence Economies in a Tropical Rain Forest

    TED L. GRAGSON Department of Anthropology Tulane University

    This report provides a comparative look at the ecology of native fishing in Amazonia with respect to the geographic distribution of water types. I t has been argued that viable populations of human foragers cannot exist in tropical rain for- ests such as those of Amazonia because naturally occurring dietary resources are poor in quality, variable in availability, and spatially dispersed (Bailey et al. 1989; Bailey, Jenike, and Rechtman 1991; Headland 1987). However, the basis for this argument rests on the exploitability of terrestrial food resources by humans, in particular the hunting of game animals, and reflects the common land and hunt- ing bias inherent to most discussions on

    foragers. When this argument is applied to South America, it overlooks the impor- tance of fish to the diet of native groups (Colinvaux and Bush 199 1 ; Beckerman 1989; Hames 1989). The available eth- nographic information on native fishing for lowland South America is very selec- tive. This report draws attention to those issues about fishing for which information is still inadequate for a full understanding of this subsistence strategy. Particular at- tention is given to issues pertaining to the relation of fishing to hunting.

    Fishing and hunting in Amazonia have been proposed as interchangeable food- procurement strategies because, at a proximate level, both appear to be di- rected at the capture of protein (Hames 1989). Fishing and hunting, however, dif- fer dramatically in their structure and or- ganization as subsistence strategies be- cause fishing and hunting trips and the techniques of procurement within each strategy differ in their associated risks and rewards (Beckerman 1983; Hill and Hawkes 1983; Yost and Kelly 1983; Hames 1979; Gragson 1991, 1992). The structural and organizational differences between fishing and hunting are a reflec- tion of life-history characteristics of fish and game species used by humans as well as the inherent scaling of such ecosystem processes as landscape development, rates of production, and changes in habi- tat (Redford and Robinson 1987; Vickers 1984; Schindler 1988; ONeill 1989; Hoekstra, Allen, and Flather 1991; Moran 1990; Rasanen, Salo, and Kalliola 1987; Frost and Miller 1987; Cummins 1988). If the structure, organization, and, by extension, function of fishing are to be understood and not simply described, then greater attention needs to be given to the life-history characteristics of the spe- cies used as food and the scalar attributes of the environments they occupy. Atten- tion to these two issues is even more crit- ical if human choices between alternative food-procurement strategies and their in- corporation into subsistence economies are the objective of study (Smith 1991; Gragson 1992; Stephens and Krebs 1986).