follow-up care of high-risk infants · risk factors for preterm (

23
Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants ABSTRACT. A workshop on the follow-up care of high-risk infants sponsored by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Insti- tute of Neurologic Disorders and Stroke, and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention was held June 19-20, 2002. There are currently no standardized guidelines for provision of follow-up services for high-risk infants in tertiary care centers despite the requirement for fol- low-up clinic experience in the 97 approved neonatal fellowship training programs in the United States and the increasing number of centers participating in multi- center networks. As the total number of survivors at risk for neurodevelopmental morbidities increases, many clinical research questions have surfaced that can only be answered by long-term follow-up studies. There is in- creasing awareness of the importance of long-term out- come in randomized, controlled trials, because perinatal interventions may dramatically alter later growth and development. There is also an increased recognition of the potential disconnect between perinatal outcomes and long-term outcomes. The administration of oxygen and postnatal steroids are prime examples of interventions that may have immediate positive effects but negative long-term effects. In addition, multicenter studies have identified significant center differences in the manage- ment and developmental outcome of high-risk infants. These findings led to the recognition of the need to improve standardization and comparability of methodol- ogy and data collection within and among centers and networks as a first step toward research to improve the long-term neurodevelopmental outcome of high-risk in- fants. The workshop participants met to define optimal methods to assess the outcome of high-risk infants, iden- tify gaps in knowledge about the neurodevelopmental outcome of high-risk infants, and prioritize research ef- forts in response to these gaps. Pediatrics 2004;114:1377– 1397. ABBREVIATIONS. NICHD, National Institute of Child Health and Development; NICU, neonatal intensive care unit; RCT, ran- domized, controlled trial; ELBW, extremely low birth weight; VLBW, very low birth weight; CNS, central nervous system; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; PVL, periventricular leukomalacia; NEC, necrotizing enterocolitis; SES, socioeconomic status; MDI, Mental Development Index; PDI, Psychomotor Developmental Index; BSID-II, Bayley Scales of Infants Development-II; GMFCS, Gross Motor Function Classification System; ADHD, attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder; NEPSY, Neuropsychological Test; BINS, Bayley Infant Neurodevelopmental Screener; LRO, Lan- guage Receptive Organization; LEO, Language Expressive Orga- nization; CPT, Continuous Performance Test; CAT/CLAMS, Cog- nitive Adaptive Test/Clinical Linguistic and Auditory Milestone Scale; TOVO, Test of Variables of Attention; CBCL, Child Behavior Check List; QOL, quality of life; CHQ, Child Health Question- naire; HRQL, health-related quality of life. BACKGROUND A dvances in perinatal intensive care have been associated with improved survival of high- risk neonates but have not resulted in de- creased morbidity. Small sample sizes, heterogeneity of cohorts and methodology, diversity of perinatal clinical practice, and the high cost of randomized, controlled trials (RCTs) and follow-up care have all contributed to the lack of rigorous data on the se- quelae of preterm delivery and the therapies used to improve the long-term outcome of high-risk infants. In response to this need, a workshop on follow-up care of high-risk infants sponsored by the National Institute of Child Health and Development (NICHD) and the National Institute of Neurologic Diseases and Stroke was held in Bethesda, Maryland, on June 19 and 20, 2002. The goal of the workshop was to define optimal methods to assess the outcome of high-risk infants, identify gaps in knowledge about the neurodevelopmental outcome of high-risk in- fants, and prioritize research efforts in response to these gaps. There are currently no standardized guidelines for provision of follow-up services for high-risk infants in tertiary care centers despite the requirement for follow-up clinic experience in the 97 approved neo- natal fellowship training programs in the United States and the increasing number of centers partici- pating in multicenter networks. As the total number of survivors at risk for neurodevelopmental morbid- ities increases, many clinical research questions have surfaced that can only be answered by long-term follow-up studies. There is increasing awareness of the importance of long-term outcome in RCTs, be- cause perinatal interventions may dramatically alter later growth and development. There is also an in- creased recognition of the potential disconnect be- tween perinatal outcomes and long-term outcomes. The administration of oxygen and postnatal steroids are prime examples of interventions that may have immediate positive effects but negative long-term effects. 1–4 In addition, multicenter studies have iden- tified significant center differences in the manage- ment and developmental outcome of high-risk in- fants. 5–8 These findings led to the recognition of the need to improve standardization and comparability of methodology and data collection within and among centers and networks as a first step toward research to improve the long-term neurodevelop- mental outcome of high-risk infants. OBJECTIVES OF THE WORKSHOP 1. To discuss the benefits of neonatal follow-up pro- grams for nurseries providing neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) care. Accepted for publication Aug 2, 2004. doi:10.1542/peds.2004-0866 PEDIATRICS (ISSN 0031 4005). Published in the public domain by the American Academy of Pediatrics. PEDIATRICS Vol. 114 No. 5 November 2004 1377 by guest on February 12, 2020 www.aappublications.org/news Downloaded from

Upload: others

Post on 28-Jan-2020

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants

ABSTRACT. A workshop on the follow-up care ofhigh-risk infants sponsored by the National Institute ofChild Health and Human Development, National Insti-tute of Neurologic Disorders and Stroke, and the Centersfor Disease Control and Prevention was held June 19-20,2002. There are currently no standardized guidelines forprovision of follow-up services for high-risk infants intertiary care centers despite the requirement for fol-low-up clinic experience in the 97 approved neonatalfellowship training programs in the United States andthe increasing number of centers participating in multi-center networks. As the total number of survivors at riskfor neurodevelopmental morbidities increases, manyclinical research questions have surfaced that can only beanswered by long-term follow-up studies. There is in-creasing awareness of the importance of long-term out-come in randomized, controlled trials, because perinatalinterventions may dramatically alter later growth anddevelopment. There is also an increased recognition ofthe potential disconnect between perinatal outcomes andlong-term outcomes. The administration of oxygen andpostnatal steroids are prime examples of interventionsthat may have immediate positive effects but negativelong-term effects. In addition, multicenter studies haveidentified significant center differences in the manage-ment and developmental outcome of high-risk infants.These findings led to the recognition of the need toimprove standardization and comparability of methodol-ogy and data collection within and among centers andnetworks as a first step toward research to improve thelong-term neurodevelopmental outcome of high-risk in-fants. The workshop participants met to define optimalmethods to assess the outcome of high-risk infants, iden-tify gaps in knowledge about the neurodevelopmentaloutcome of high-risk infants, and prioritize research ef-forts in response to these gaps. Pediatrics 2004;114:1377–1397.

ABBREVIATIONS. NICHD, National Institute of Child Healthand Development; NICU, neonatal intensive care unit; RCT, ran-domized, controlled trial; ELBW, extremely low birth weight;VLBW, very low birth weight; CNS, central nervous system; MRI,magnetic resonance imaging; PVL, periventricular leukomalacia;NEC, necrotizing enterocolitis; SES, socioeconomic status; MDI,Mental Development Index; PDI, Psychomotor DevelopmentalIndex; BSID-II, Bayley Scales of Infants Development-II; GMFCS,Gross Motor Function Classification System; ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; NEPSY, Neuropsychological Test;BINS, Bayley Infant Neurodevelopmental Screener; LRO, Lan-guage Receptive Organization; LEO, Language Expressive Orga-nization; CPT, Continuous Performance Test; CAT/CLAMS, Cog-nitive Adaptive Test/Clinical Linguistic and Auditory MilestoneScale; TOVO, Test of Variables of Attention; CBCL, Child BehaviorCheck List; QOL, quality of life; CHQ, Child Health Question-naire; HRQL, health-related quality of life.

BACKGROUND

Advances in perinatal intensive care have beenassociated with improved survival of high-risk neonates but have not resulted in de-

creased morbidity. Small sample sizes, heterogeneityof cohorts and methodology, diversity of perinatalclinical practice, and the high cost of randomized,controlled trials (RCTs) and follow-up care have allcontributed to the lack of rigorous data on the se-quelae of preterm delivery and the therapies used toimprove the long-term outcome of high-risk infants.In response to this need, a workshop on follow-upcare of high-risk infants sponsored by the NationalInstitute of Child Health and Development (NICHD)and the National Institute of Neurologic Diseasesand Stroke was held in Bethesda, Maryland, on June19 and 20, 2002. The goal of the workshop was todefine optimal methods to assess the outcome ofhigh-risk infants, identify gaps in knowledge aboutthe neurodevelopmental outcome of high-risk in-fants, and prioritize research efforts in response tothese gaps.

There are currently no standardized guidelines forprovision of follow-up services for high-risk infantsin tertiary care centers despite the requirement forfollow-up clinic experience in the 97 approved neo-natal fellowship training programs in the UnitedStates and the increasing number of centers partici-pating in multicenter networks. As the total numberof survivors at risk for neurodevelopmental morbid-ities increases, many clinical research questions havesurfaced that can only be answered by long-termfollow-up studies. There is increasing awareness ofthe importance of long-term outcome in RCTs, be-cause perinatal interventions may dramatically alterlater growth and development. There is also an in-creased recognition of the potential disconnect be-tween perinatal outcomes and long-term outcomes.The administration of oxygen and postnatal steroidsare prime examples of interventions that may haveimmediate positive effects but negative long-termeffects.1–4 In addition, multicenter studies have iden-tified significant center differences in the manage-ment and developmental outcome of high-risk in-fants.5–8 These findings led to the recognition of theneed to improve standardization and comparabilityof methodology and data collection within andamong centers and networks as a first step towardresearch to improve the long-term neurodevelop-mental outcome of high-risk infants.

OBJECTIVES OF THE WORKSHOP

1. To discuss the benefits of neonatal follow-up pro-grams for nurseries providing neonatal intensivecare unit (NICU) care.

Accepted for publication Aug 2, 2004.doi:10.1542/peds.2004-0866PEDIATRICS (ISSN 0031 4005). Published in the public domain by theAmerican Academy of Pediatrics.

PEDIATRICS Vol. 114 No. 5 November 2004 1377 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 2: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

2. To define who should be followed and what char-acteristics of premature infants and term infantsare associated with an increased risk for neurode-velopmental morbidity.

3. To define the (1) the optimal ages of follow-upassessment and (2) the optimal assessment mea-sures and to minimize barriers to assessmentcaused by motor, vision, or hearing impairments.

4. To discuss approaches to selection of controls andmaintaining follow-up.

5. To describe alternative, less costly methods andapproaches to assessment for the community phy-sician.

6. Describe the challenges encountered in multi-center networks.

7A. To identify gaps in knowledge about assessmentof neurodevelopmental outcome among high-risk infants and the mechanisms contributing tothese outcomes.

7B. To further advance areas of investigation.8. To propose future research questions.

OBJECTIVE 1: DETERMINE THE BENEFITS OFNEONATAL FOLLOW-UP PROGRAMS AT

PERINATAL CENTERSThe 2 primary areas of responsibility for neonatal

follow-up programs are surveillance and research.

SurveillanceThe first step in surveillance is to establish a mech-

anism to systematically monitor the care of high-riskinfants during their initial hospitalization as well astheir general health and neurodevelopmental out-comes after discharge from the NICU. Surveillance isnecessary for NICUs to (1) audit NICU interventions,(2) monitor important quality indicators for the indi-vidual NICU, (3) summarize information about cen-ter outcomes for a specific condition (eg, chroniclung disease), and (4) summarize annual outcomedata to be used to influence policy and as a basis forpublic health programs designed to improve out-comes. Outcomes of interest may include rates ofgrowth failure, cerebral palsy, mental retardation, ordevelopmental delay; hearing impairments, visionimpairments, or autism; need for technologic sup-port such as home oxygen, ventilators, tracheosto-mies, or gastrostomies; and educational resourcesneeded. Information about center outcomes forspecific conditions and specific gestational ages alsoallows staff caring for high-risk infants to counselparents with regard to prognosis.9 Providing com-prehensive feedback of each evaluation to the familyand the primary provider with appropriate counsel-ing and referrals supports the concept of an effectivemedical home for NICU graduates.

ResearchFollow-up results of populations enrolled in RCTs

and well-designed cohort studies are needed to eval-uate the long-term impact of interventions designedto improve the outcome of high-risk populations andidentify previously unknown safety problems.

OBJECTIVE 2: DETERMINE WHO NEEDS TO BEFOLLOWED AND WHAT THE IMPORTANT RISK

CATEGORIES AREIdentified risk factors for adverse neurodevelop-

mental outcome in preterm and term infants identi-fied in studies to date are listed in Table 1. Infantsshould receive follow-up assessments based on theseverity of the perinatal problems, the interventionsreceived in the NICU, the demographic risk factorsof the infants’ families, the outcome profile of thecohort in the individual NICU, and the NICU’s re-sources. At a minimum, the follow-up cohort shouldinclude extremely low birth weight (ELBW) infants�1000 g birth weight and/or �28 weeks’ gestationand term infants with hypoxic ischemic encephalop-athy or severe hyperbilirubinemia requiring ex-change transfusion. In addition, it is recommendedthat all children, especially those cared for in aNICU, have the benefit of periodic preventive assess-ments by their primary care physicians within thecontext of a medical home. The guidelines estab-lished for the periodicity schedule developed by theCommittee on Practice and Ambulatory Medicine10

in consultation with national committees and sec-tions of the American Academy of Pediatrics includeregular basic assessments of growth, sensory func-tion, behavior, and neurodevelopment. Infants withsuspect findings and families with parenting chal-lenges should be referred for additional, more com-prehensive evaluation to their follow-up program,child development center, and additional commu-nity resources as needed. Moreover, follow-up carein a follow-up program or by the primary care pro-vider should include counseling and support forfamilies and communication among all participantsin the medical home. Evaluation of the family shouldinclude information on adequacy of parenting skills,use of drugs, alcohol, and/or cigarettes, parent phys-ical and mental health status, and adequacy of fi-nances and health insurance. Referrals need to befacilitated by the medical home, as needed, withinput from the family. Early-intervention parentgroups, NICU parent groups, Family Voices, andcommunity mental health services may be beneficial.

Risk Factors for Preterm (<32 Weeks or Very Low BirthWeight, <1500 g) Infants

The developing brain of the premature infant isextremely vulnerable to injury. The long-term conse-quences of such injury include motor deficits com-monly referred to as cerebral palsy (a nonprogressiveinjury of the central nervous system (CNS) charac-terized by abnormal control of movement and pos-ture) as well as cognitive and behavioral prob-lems.11–19 The risk for neurodevelopmental deficitsincreases with decreasing gestational age, resultingin relatively high risk of cerebral palsy, develop-mental delay, hearing and vision impairment, andsubnormal academic achievement among ELBW in-fants.4,8,11 These performance deficits are thoughtto be related to the increased biological vulnerabilityto injury of the basal ganglia, hippocampus, andperiventricular white matter in premature in-

1378 FOLLOW-UP CARE OF HIGH-RISK INFANTS by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 3: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

fants.20–23 Risk for cerebral palsy and neurodevelop-mental disabilities can be predicted in part fromspecific abnormalities on neonatal cranial ultrasoundor magnetic resonance imaging (MRI); however, cog-nitive and behavioral problems may occur in theabsence of identified neonatal neuroimaging abnor-malities.13

Factors That May Affect Outcome: Biological FactorsPeriventricular-intraventricular hemorrhage is the

most common CNS lesion in the premature infant,which in its mildest form is confined to the germinalmatrix and in more severe cases is complicated bybleeding into the adjacent ventricular system and/orwhite matter. Long-term neurodevelopmental defi-cits are strongly linked to these severe lesions.16,17

Approximately 26% of infants with birth weightsbetween 501 and 750 g and 12% of infants with birthweights between 751 and 1000 g develop the moresevere forms of CNS hemorrhage.18

Periventricular white matter injury (or periven-tricular leukomalacia [PVL]) may be identified oncranial ultrasound or MRI by the appearance of cys-tic lesions.12 Cystic PVL is invariably associated withsignificant motor and cognitive deficits. Nonprogres-sive ventriculomegaly, secondary to loss of cortical

mass, is also associated with increased risk of neu-rodevelopmental deficit.20 Linear hyperechogenicitywithin the thalamo-striatal ganglia of premature in-fants also has been associated with lower mental anddevelopmental behavioral scores.21

Severe preterm delivery, male gender, sepsis, men-ingitis, necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC) requiring sur-gery, chronic lung disease/prolonged ventilation, se-vere growth restriction (�10th percentile for age andgestational age), and posthemorrhagic hydrocepha-lus have been identified as risk factors for adverseneurodevelopmental outcome in neonatal follow-updatabases.18,19 Additional risk factors identified in-clude multiple birth, medical complications in theNICU, and congenital abnormalities requiring med-ical or surgical intervention. Finally, research sug-gests that environmental stress in the NICU may bean important risk factor. For example, experimentalevidence from animal models implicates environ-mental stress in the genesis of hippocampal inju-ry.22–24

Therapeutic InterventionsPerinatal interventions may affect neurodevelop-

mental outcome. For example, a large RCT and manylarge observational studies have documented that a

TABLE 1. Defined Risk Categories in the Term and Preterm Infant

Infants Preterm Term

Biologic risk VLBW infants �1500 g birth weight Encephalopathy persisting at dischargeELBW infants �1000 g birth weight Other neurologic problems/meningitisInfants with cranial ultrasound abnormalities

including PVL-intraventricular hemorrhage,linear hyperechogenicity,

Complex medical problems

Other neurologic problems (seizures,hydrocephalus)

Small for gestation

NEC Twin-twin transfusionChronic lung disease Complex congenital anomaliesComplex medical problems Birth defectsSmall for gestation Metabolic disordersHigher-order multiples Sepsis, meningitis, nosocomial infectionsTwin-twin transfusion Hyperbilirubinemia requiring exchangeComplex congenital anomalies Failure to grow in the NICURecurrent apnea and bradycardia MultiparityHyperbilirubinemia requiring exchange Abnormal neurologic exam at dischargeFailure to grow in the NICUSepsis, meningitis, nosocomial infectionsMultiparityAbnormal neurologic exam at discharge

Interventions Resuscitation ResuscitationPostnatal steroids Postnatal steroidsHigh-frequency ventilation High-frequency ventilationProlonged ventilation �7 days Prolonged ventilation �7 daysTotal parenteral nutrition Total parenteral nutritionProlonged oxygen requirements Prolonged oxygen requirementsNutritional therapies Nutritional therapiesOther medications Other medicationsSurgical interventions for NEC, patent ductus

arteriosus, shuntSurgical intervention for NEC, shunt

Extracorporeal membrane oxygenationSocial/environmental Low maternal education, teen mother Low maternal education, teen mother

Low SES: Hollingshead,32 Hauser33 Low SES: Hollingshead,32 Hauser33

Single marital status Single marital statusMinority status Minority statusNo insurance or Medicaid insurance No insurance or Medicaid insuranceLow income Low incomeDrugs/alcohol, smoking, substance abuse Drugs/alcohol, smoking, substance abuseNo prenatal care No prenatal careEnvironmental stress Environmental stress

SUPPLEMENT 1379 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 4: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

single course of antenatal glucocorticoid therapy ma-tures the fetus and improves survival, skin integrity,and renal function and decreases periventricular/intraventricular hemorrhage.25–27 In contrast, pro-longed postnatal steroid therapy to prevent or ame-liorate chronic lung disease seems to be associatedwith negative CNS outcomes.19,28–30 Prolonged expo-sure to glucocorticoids in the animal model is asso-ciated with reductions in regional cerebral bloodflow, specifically to hippocampus, with concomitanthistologic changes, significant reduction in hip-pocampal volume, down-regulation of myelin basicprotein and proteolipid protein in white matter, de-layed myelination of optic axons, hyperactivity of thehypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and alterationsin dopamine receptor responses.31

Family/Environmental CharacteristicsThe social and/or environmental characteristics of

families32–35 become increasingly important as thechild matures. Assessment of socioeconomic status(SES) in an individual or family unit is an indirectrather than direct measure of the social environmentof the family. Mother-child interaction is a moreproximal factor and is more predictive of earlieroutcome �2 years, whereas SES is a status or distalfactor and is more predictive of later outcome.36 In areview of social class and developmental outcomesin 37 studies conducted in 2000,33 low social class, asdetermined by several different means, was associ-ated with poorer growth, greater academic difficul-ties including reading and spelling problems, lowerIQ, poorer language skills, poorer fine motor skills,more aggression and externalizing behavior, moredepression and other psychiatric disorders, poorersibling relationships, and poorer social development.However, social class did not consistently predictsymbolic play in infants or Bayley Mental Develop-ment Index (MDI)37 and Psychomotor Developmen-tal Index (PDI) scores up to 2 years. Although indi-cators of social class are effective predictors for anumber of developmental outcomes, it must be rec-ognized that effects are mediated by the child char-acteristics, family characteristics, and external sup-ports.

The most common single measures used to assessSES are family income, educational level, and occu-pation. The easiest information to obtain is maternaleducational level, but it may be only weakly relatedto the current status of the family. Although incomemay be more closely related to the family’s currentSES status, many respondents are uncomfortableabout disclosing their income or uncertain about theactual amount. To overcome these problems, re-search protocols typically ask for income category,which is easier for the respondent but poses statisti-cal challenges because categories are not continuousvariables. The most common composite measure ofSES is the Hollingshead32 Index of Social Prestige,which is a weighted average of educational and oc-cupational levels. Years of education completed areclassified into 7 categories, and occupations are clas-sified into 9 categories of increasing status. The Holl-ingshead scale has the advantage of combining edu-

cation and occupation across 1 or 2 adult wageearners in the household as an algorithm for classi-fication into SES levels. An important disadvantageis that the original combination rules are designedfor nuclear families. Other disadvantages are that theHollingshead Scale is insensitive to heterogeneitywithin a social class category, and the categories foroccupation were developed almost 30 years ago. In-clusion of income level or percent of poverty levelprovides better discrimination among families.34

Composite scores that combine several SES indica-tors and a standardized assessment of the homeenvironment (Caldwell’s HOME scale38) have beenused to overcome some of these limitations. TheHOME scale is highly predictive of school perfor-mance but is generally too expensive for standarduse.39 Attempts to define a question or set of ques-tions that capture family-life characteristics, eg,monthly rent or mortgage,34 as recommended by theWorld Health Organization for the developingworld, have been less successful in the United States,because many of the questions are subject to non-SESinfluences. Overall, the Hollingshead Scale is a solidmeasure and remains a powerful predictor of out-come. It is one of the best measures of poverty,single-parent families, and the lifetime consequencesof low education.

Any analysis of differences between study groupsthat does not account for SES either by equivalentgroups or statistical adjustment may be flawed, be-cause SES may be an important moderator of peri-natal risk and developmental outcome. The choice ofhow to manage the variance accounted for by SESdepends on the study hypothesis. SES is frequentlyadjusted even with equivalent groups to reduce errorvariance. With very preterm infants or infants sus-taining biological insult, there can be an interactionbetween perinatal risk status and SES such that high-risk infants raised in high-SES families have morepositive outcomes, whereas those raised in low-SESfamilies have poorer outcomes.39 There are numer-ous additional environmental factors that have beenshown to be associated with outcomes, includingmaternal depression, substance abuse, crowding inthe home, and availability of educational materialsand early-intervention services. Investigators mustidentify which variables are important at their centerand which variables contribute to the specific re-search question being asked. In addition, most pro-grams currently collect data on resource utilization,including early intervention, physical therapy, occu-pational therapy, speech therapy, day care, special-ized educational services, and rehospitalizations.These data are collected to determine if the child isreceiving adequate services so that referrals can bemade or information shared as needed. In addition,resource utilization is an important variable to beevaluated in outcome studies.

OBJECTIVE 3A: DETERMINE THE OPTIMAL AGESOF ASSESSMENT

Although long-term follow-up of complete cohortsis optimal for determining the ultimate function ofhigh-risk infants and the safety of antenatal and pre-

1380 FOLLOW-UP CARE OF HIGH-RISK INFANTS by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 5: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

natal interventions, most follow-up studies are short-term (�2 years). The selection of age of assessment isdriven by several factors: developmental acquisitionsavailable at a given age; availability and applicabilityof appropriate test instruments at specific ages;trends found in extant studies; and the cost andfeasibility of long-term tracking in the population inquestion.

Assessment schedules should reflect the questionasked in a specific research protocol. For surveillancepurposes, a center may propose periodic enrollment(eg, ELBW infants may be enrolled for 1 year every 5years and followed for 5 years, after which a newcohort may be enrolled).

In many NICU programs, all very low birth weight(VLBW) infants are referred to early intervention atthe time of discharge from the NICU. Eligibility forearly-intervention services, however, varies by state.If a developmental assessment is needed before 12months to establish eligibility for a referral to aninfant stimulation program, a 6- to 8-month correct-ed-age evaluation is recommended, using the BayleyScales of Infant Development-II (BSID-II) MDI. The util-ity of assessment before this age is questionable.

12 Months’ Corrected AgeEnvironmental factors are less influential on per-

formance at 12 months’ corrected age, and biomedi-cal issues such as oxygen supplementation forchronic lung disease have resolved and may be lessintrusive on testing procedures. By 12 months’ cor-rected age, a more varied behavioral repertoire isavailable, and cognitive processes and emerging lan-guage can be assessed. Evaluation at this age alsoencourages family involvement in the program.However, cognitive and motor functions are stillhighly intertwined at 12 months’ corrected age, andthe period of developmental acquisition is a time ofvariability. Also, some neurologic abnormalities thatare identified in the first year of life are transient orimprove, whereas findings in other children mayworsen over time.40

18 to 24 Months’ Corrected AgeBy 18 to 24 months’ corrected age, environmental

factors begin to exert a stronger influence on testresults, cognitive and motor abilities diverge, lan-guage and reasoning skills are developing, and thereis improved prediction to early school-age perfor-mance.41–43 A potential problem is that many intelli-gence tests have weak floors at this age, therebyrestricting use to developmental tests, and thereforeimpairments may be overestimated or underesti-mated. Application of corrected age (the sum of chro-nologic age in weeks minus the difference betweengestational age at birth and 40 weeks’ gestation) to 2or 2 1/2 years is controversial but generally accept-ed.44,45 Standard follow-up for many multicenter net-works is currently at 18 to 24 months’ corrected age.

3 to 4 YearsAt 3 to 4 years, “intelligence” can first be assessed,

as well as concept development, preacademic skills,early indicators of executive function, and visual-

motor integrative abilities. Verbal and nonverbalskills can be better differentiated. There is also anacceptable level of prediction46 from scores at thisage to later IQ scores. SES and social support, as wellas other environmental factors, influence test resultsmore strongly from age 3 years onward.

6 YearsBy 6 years of age, a variety of tests and procedures

can be used, and attention problems and schoolachievement (approximately first grade) can be as-sessed. The array of possible tests that can be used ismore restricted at age 5 than at age 6.

8 YearsIQ, neuropsychological function, learning disabil-

ities, school performance, and behavioral adjustmentcan be adequately assessed at 8 years (approximatelythird grade). IQ measured at 8 years predicts later IQmore accurately than at earlier ages.

OBJECTIVE 3B: DETERMINE THE APPROPRIATEASSESSMENTS FOR ADMINISTRATION AT THE

RECOMMENDED AGESTen different domains of development were iden-

tified and are related to age of assessment in Table 2.Three areas are basic (growth, neurologic status, anddevelopmental status) and should be included inevery follow-up program. The measures presentedare based on the extensive experience of the partici-pants in the workshop. It is anticipated that the in-formation provided will assist follow-up programsto make choices based on this report and practicalconsiderations.

GrowthAssessment of growth should include birth

weight, birth length, and birth head circumferencerelative to an appropriate growth chart and an accu-rate determination of gestational age at birth. Themost commonly used fetal growth charts are those ofLubchenco et al47,48; however, these charts reflectpreterm deliveries at �1 mile above sea level. Theintrauterine growth standards developed by Usherand McLean49 do not reflect the cohort at greatestrisk (22–24 weeks’ gestation); they were published in1969 for infants between 25 and 44 weeks’ gestation.More recent population-based intrauterine growthstandards have been published for the Canadianpopulation for infants between 22 and 44 weeks’gestation.50 The NICHD Neonatal Research Networkuses fetal growth charts developed by Alexander etal51 that provide reference weights for infants bornfrom 20 to 38 weeks’ gestation for US infants. Thesefetal growth charts are often used to plot postnatalgrowth, although the environmental and metabolicdemands of postnatal growth are very different fromthose during fetal development. The NICHD Neona-tal Research Network has developed postnatalgrowth charts for VLBW infants52; however, thesegrowth trajectories should not be considered opti-mal, because �15% of VLBW infants have a weight�10th percentile at birth but 90% are �10th percen-tile for their corrected fetal age at hospital discharge.

SUPPLEMENT 1381 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 6: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

TA

BL

E2.

Bas

icC

ateg

orie

sof

Ass

essm

ent

byA

ge

Are

aof

Ass

essm

ent*

3–4

and

6–8

mo

CA

12–1

4m

oC

A18

mo

CA

3–5

ySc

hool

Age

Gro

wth

*W

t,L

t,H

CW

t,L

t,H

CW

t,L

t,H

CW

t,L

t,H

CW

t,L

t,H

CN

utri

tion

BM

I,sk

info

lds

BM

I,sk

info

lds

BM

I,sk

info

lds

BM

I,sk

info

lds

BM

I,sk

info

lds

Cal

oric

/pr

otei

nfa

tin

take

;en

ergy

expe

ndit

ure

Cal

oric

/pr

otei

nfa

tin

take

;en

ergy

expe

ndit

ure

Cal

oric

/pr

otei

nfa

tin

take

;en

ergy

expe

ndit

ure

Cal

oric

/pr

otei

nfa

tin

take

;en

ergy

expe

ndit

ure

Cal

oric

/pr

otei

nfa

tin

take

;ene

rgy

expe

ndit

ure

Neu

rolo

gic*

asse

ssm

ent

Stan

dar

dfo

rag

eSt

and

ard

for

age

Stan

dar

dfo

rag

eSt

and

ard

for

age

Stan

dar

dfo

rag

e49–

54

Am

iel-

Tis

on55

Am

iel-

Tis

on55

Am

iel-

Tis

on55

Am

iel-

Tis

on55

Neu

roim

agin

gC

onve

ntio

nal

Con

vent

iona

lC

onve

ntio

nal

Con

vent

iona

lC

onve

ntio

nal

Res

earc

h*58

–66

MR

I,D

TI,

DW

I,C

TM

RI,

DT

I,D

WI,

CT

MR

I,D

TI,

DW

I,C

TFu

ncti

onal

,MR

I,D

TI,

DW

I,C

TFu

ncti

onal

,MR

I,D

TI,

DW

I,C

T

Gro

ssm

otor

*B

ayle

yPD

IB

ayle

yPD

IB

ayle

yPD

IG

MFC

SG

MFC

SD

evel

opm

ent

IQ,v

isua

l-m

otor

skill

s,A

DH

D(a

ge5)

IQ,V

isua

l-m

otor

skill

s,re

ason

ing,

mem

ory,

AD

HD

,exe

cuti

vefu

ncti

onB

ayle

yM

DI

Bay

ley

MD

IB

ayle

yM

DI

Bay

ley

PDI

Bay

ley

PDI

Bay

ley

PDI

Scho

olag

ere

sear

ch*

Scho

olpl

acem

ent;

achi

evem

ent;

exec

utiv

efu

ncti

on;a

cad

emic

perf

orm

ance

;abd

omin

alci

rcum

fere

nce

Lan

guag

eL

imit

edfo

ryo

ung

child

ren;

pare

ntch

eckl

ist

Lim

ited

for

youn

gch

ildre

n;pa

rent

chec

klis

tL

imit

edfo

ryo

ung

child

ren;

pare

ntch

eckl

ist

Lim

ited

for

youn

gch

ildre

n;pa

rent

chec

klis

t;PP

VT

101 ;

Gra

mm

arT

EG

I104

Lim

ited

for

youn

gch

ildre

n;PP

VT

101 ;

Gra

mm

arT

EG

I104

Func

tion

alW

eeFI

M95

;Vin

elan

d96

;PE

DI9

8W

eeFI

M95

;Vin

elan

d96

;PE

DI9

8W

eeFI

M95

;Vin

elan

d96

;PE

DI9

8W

eeFI

M95

;Vin

elan

d96

;PE

DI9

8

Beh

avio

rC

BC

L10

9,11

0C

BC

L10

9,11

0C

BC

L10

9,11

0 ;T

OV

A(4

–5)9

0 ;G

DS

(4–5

)116

CB

CL

109,

110 ;

Con

ners

CPT

73;

Con

ners

RS1

11,1

12

QO

Lre

sear

ch*

QO

L;H

RQ

L;h

ealt

hst

atus

QO

L;H

RQ

L;h

ealt

hst

atus

QO

L;H

RQ

L;h

ealt

hst

atus

QO

L;H

RQ

L;h

ealt

hst

atus

CA

,in

dic

ated

corr

ecte

dag

e;W

t,w

eigh

t;L

t,le

ngth

;H

C,

head

circ

umfe

renc

e;B

MI,

bod

ym

ass

ind

ex;

GD

S,G

ord

onD

iagn

osti

cSy

stem

;D

TI,

dif

fusi

onte

nsio

nim

agin

g;D

WI,

dif

fusi

onw

eigh

ted

imag

ing;

CT

,com

pute

dto

mog

raph

yim

agin

g;W

eeFI

M,F

unct

iona

lInd

epen

den

ceM

easu

refo

rC

hild

ren;

Vin

elan

d,V

inel

and

Soci

alM

atur

ity

Scal

e;PE

DI,

Ped

iatr

icE

valu

atio

nof

Dis

abili

ties

Inve

ntor

y;PP

VT

,Pea

bod

yPi

ctur

eV

ocab

ular

yT

est;

TE

GI,

Tes

tof

Ear

lyG

ram

mat

ical

Impa

irm

ent1

04;C

onne

rsR

S,C

onne

rsT

each

erR

atin

gSc

ale1

11an

dPa

rent

Rat

ing

Scal

e.11

2

*R

esea

rch

prot

ocol

s(a

udit

ory

CPT

)ca

nbe

mea

sure

dat

late

rag

es,8

1an

dve

rbal

IQs

can

beob

tain

ed.

1382 FOLLOW-UP CARE OF HIGH-RISK INFANTS by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 7: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

Weight, length, and head circumferences should becollected routinely and serially in all programs byexaminers trained to reliability using standard tech-niques. For children �2 years, weight should beobtained with the child completely undressed. Re-cumbent length is obtained until age 2 by using apediatric length board, and maximal occipital frontalhead circumference is recorded to the nearest milli-meter by using a nonstretch measuring tape. Anthro-pometric measures in the United States are plottedaccording to gender and age (adjusted until age 2) onNational Center for Health Statistics growth charts.Poor growth is defined as weight, length, head cir-cumference, or weight-to-length ratio �10th percen-tile.53 Other measures of growth used in researchprotocols include skinfold thickness,54 calculatedmeasures of weight-to-length ratios, growth velocity,protein and caloric intake, energy expenditure, andbone density.

Neurologic OutcomeThe neurologic examination of the toddler and

older child40,55–57 is an integral part of the neurode-velopmental follow-up assessment, yet it remainspoorly described and standardized in many researchprotocols. This early assessment can identify infantswith mild, moderate, and severe abnormalities. Thisassessment is of interest because infants with mild ortransient neurologic abnormalities often improveover time, unlike infants with severe early neurologicdysfunction, who are less likely to improve and havethe worst neurodevelopmental outcome.40 The neu-rologic examination of the toddler and older childcan be used to determine resource needs such asearly intervention. Follow-up programs need to de-velop a manual of operations for the entire assess-ment, use a standardized neurologic examination,establish definitions, and train and certify examinersto reliability on an annual basis. Components of theexamination should include assessment of gross mo-tor function, tone, reflexes, cerebellar function, cra-nial nerves, and language. In addition, excellent ob-servation skills, adherence to examination protocol,good interpersonal skills, and flexibility are the keyto an accurate assessment of the young child; muchcan be learned by observation of posture, movement,and quality of movement before the onset of the“formal” examination.

ImagingThe application of MRI to predicting outcome in

high-risk children or for evaluating them later in lifeis an evolving field.58–66 Conventional MRI is effec-tive for detecting developmental abnormalities of theCNS and PVL virtually any time after birth. Diffu-sion magnetic resonance methods measure changesin water apparent diffusion coefficient and are usedas an early indicator of brain injury becoming sensi-tive 2 to 5 days after injury in infants. T1- and T2-weighted images also detect CNS injury, althoughtheir sensitivity varies with the time after the injury.Conventional T1- and T2-weighted MRI is routinelyused to assess myelin development and maturationstatus in children and preterm infants. Functional

MRI may be used to assess the cortical reorganiza-tion associated with injury and recovery. These tech-niques and the development of age-based norms areunder continuous development. Imaging technolo-gies are currently being used in the NICU to evaluateCNS injury to determine risk category and potentialpostdischarge needs and to predict outcome. Currentstudies are evaluating the use of MRI in older chil-dren to evaluate the association between neurocog-nitive skills and brain injury.

Gross Motor SkillsSpecific tests of gross motor function are currently

frequently combined with the standard neurologicexamination. The Gross Motor Function Classifica-tion System (GMFCS)67 is the first reliable and vali-dated system available to describe the severity ofmotor dysfunction in children with cerebral palsyand has been shown to be reasonably stable betweenthe ages of 2 and 12 years.68 The GMFCS for cerebralpalsy provides classification of age-adjusted varia-tions in 5 levels of gross motor function, with thehighest level of mobility expected between 6 and 12years of age. Descriptions are available for children�2 years, 2 to 4 years, 4 to 6 years, and 6 to 12 years.The GMFCS is quick and easy to use; classification ismade by determining which level best represents thechild’s present abilities and limitations in gross mo-tor function. The description for each level is broadand not intended to describe all aspects of the motorfunction of individual children. Distinctions betweenlevels of gross motor function are based on func-tional limitations, the need for assistive mobility de-vices (eg, walkers, crutches, canes), wheeled mobil-ity, and, to a lesser extent, quality of movement. Thescale is ordinal, with no intent that the distancesbetween levels be considered equal or that childrenwith cerebral palsy are equally distributed amongthe 5 levels. The 5 levels of function (level 1 is thehighest) are:

1. Walks without restrictions; limitations in moreadvanced gross motor skills

2. Walks without assistive devices; limitations walk-ing outdoors and in the community

3. Walks with assistive mobility devices; limitationswalking outdoors and in the community

4. Self-mobility with limitations; children are trans-ported or use power mobility outdoors and in thecommunity

5. Self-mobility is severely limited even with the useof assistive technology

The algorithm shown, based on the GMFCS, wasdeveloped to classify infants at 24 to 28 months’corrected age in the NICHD- and National Instituteof Neurologic Diseases and Stroke-sponsored ran-domized clinical trial of the Beneficial Effects of An-tenatal Magnesium Sulfate in the Maternal FetalMedicine Units Network (Fig 1). In addition, thecharacteristics of upper extremity function includingbimanual function and bilateral pincer grasp areneeded to differentiate among right hemiplegia, lefthemiplegia, diplegia, and other minor motor impair-ments.

SUPPLEMENT 1383 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 8: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

Cognitive and Developmental SkillsThe emergence of high-prevalence/low-severity

dysfunction (borderline IQ, learning disabilities, at-tention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder [ADHD], neu-ropsychologic deficits, and behavior problems) inchildren at biological risk has underscored the needfor detailed assessment of cognitive development.Although major deficits can be detected in infancy,more subtle high-prevalence/low-severity dysfunc-tions become increasingly obvious as the child growsolder, which is assumed to be because of continuedcortical development and increased demands forperformance in emerging areas of cognitive function.Moreover, there is some evidence that the cognitivefunction of infants with severe CNS injury tends todeteriorate over time, whereas the cognitive scores ofneurologically intact but immature infants tend toimprove as they mature.69 These data support theimportance of long-term follow-up to obtain school-age outcomes on high-risk infants.

IQsEarlier meta-analyses of cognitive deficits in low

birth weight (�2500 g) infants indicated a 5- to7-point deficit in comparison to term infants. Morerecent comparisons suggest a 0.3- to 0.6-SD decreasein IQ in children born prematurely, producing areported decrement that ranges from 4.5 to 9.0points. Therefore, in VLBW infants free of majordisabilities, mean group IQs fall in the borderline toaverage range, with low average scores being themode. However, use of IQ scores masks more subtledeficits; a verbal, performance, or area index score isthe average of various subtests (ie, verbal compre-hension, processing speed, etc), whereas a full-scaleIQ is an average of averages. IQ scores are influenced

by socioeconomic variables, particularly with respectto verbal function.

Visual-Motor SkillsThe majority of children born at ELBW and VLBW,

even those with average IQs, manifest visual-motorproblems that include deficits on neuropsychologicaltasks such as copying, perceptual matching, spatialprocessing, finger-tapping, pegboard performance,visual memory, and visual-sequential memory. Esti-mates of visual-perceptual and visual-motor integra-tion problems are in the 11% to 20% range; finemotor problems are as high as 71%. These deficitsseem mostly related to biological risks rather thanenvironmental risks.

Neuropsychological FunctionsPreliminary data suggest an increased incidence of

executive-function problems in planning, organiza-tion, problem-solving, working memory, and inhibi-tion as well as attention. Related to this, a sizeablenumber of children born VLBW display nonverballearning disabilities (in which verbal cognitive skillsare better developed than nonverbal abilities, pro-ducing verbal IQ/performance IQ discrepancies)and associated problems in visual-motor integration,visual perception, mathematics, spatial skills, andfine motor speed. Verbal abstracting, reading com-prehension, written output, and social skills are alsoareas of deficit with nonverbal learning disabilities.Neuropsychologic deficits are related to both biolog-ical and environmental risks.

The array of potential problems found in the fol-low-up of preterm infants mandates that assessmentextend beyond traditional IQ and achievement test-ing. The pattern of developmental change or im-

Fig 1. Gross motor function classification system at 24 months corrected age.

1384 FOLLOW-UP CARE OF HIGH-RISK INFANTS by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 9: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

provement may be more informative than the mid-point score in a given area of function. These processissues, which have often been overlooked, may beinformative and useful for intervention and predic-tive purposes. Assessment of intelligence in fol-low-up studies to school age should include 5 keyelements: reasoning, knowledge, quantitative rea-soning, visual/spatial processing, and workingmemory.

Areas of AssessmentSpecific tests and rating scales that assess the fol-

lowing areas should be considered in follow-up pro-tocols (those falling under a broad categorization of“cognitive” are in italic type):

1. Intelligence (including verbal and nonverbal as-pects) and executive function

2. Achievement3. Functional status (self-care, mobility, communica-

tion)4. Language (phonological awareness, syntax, ver-

bal fluency, comprehension of instructions, speednaming, higher-order abstracting functions)

5. Sensorimotor functions (visual-motor precision, finemotor speed)

6. Visual spatial processes (design-copying, visual clo-sure)

7. Memory and learning (list-learning, delayed recall,narrative memory, and use of semantic/strategicand rote/episodic verbal and visual tasks)

8. Behavioral adjustment (ADHD, internalizing andexternalizing behaviors, and social functioning).

Table 3 lists specific tests recommended to evalu-ate developmental/cognitive function.70–85 Tests inthe first row are those considered most desirable,and tests in subsequent rows are acceptable alterna-tives; listed tests are not necessarily all administeredat any given time. Moreover, some tests have notbeen included because of lack of valid test norms orbecause they do not sample cognitive constructs ofinterest. Finally, certain functions (eg, visual percep-tion) may be measured on several different tests (eg,Neuropsychological Test [NEPSY]78 and Motor FreeVisual Perception Test86).

Most IQ tests require so-called level C qualification

for administration, meaning that they should be ad-ministered by qualified masters-level or above psy-chologists: pediatric, clinical, child, and school psy-chologists and child neuropsychologists. Centersthat use limited assessment batteries because of lim-itations of staff or funding require level B qualifica-tions for administration. Examples of these tests in-clude the Bayley Infant NeurodevelopmentalScreener (BINS),87 Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test,88

Developmental Test of Vision Motor Integration,70

rating scales, and similar instruments. Adequatelytrained support staff can administer level B tests, andappropriately trained developmental pediatriciansand psychologists can administer the BSID-II.37

Minimizing Barriers to AssessmentAssessment of children with sensorimotor impair-

ments remains challenging for new examiners. Chil-dren with visual impairment are extremely difficultto evaluate before the preschool age, because mosttest items are sensorimotor in nature. Evaluation isrestricted to verbal behaviors and gross motor mile-stones. The Mullen Scales of Early Learning,89 Lan-guage Receptive Organization (LRO), and LanguageExpressive Organization (LEO) scales may be usefulfor children with vision impairment, although itemsof an “intersensory” (ie, auditory and visual) natureare eliminated or scored in a prorated fashion. Withincreasing age, verbal subtests can be used, althoughmany still require viewing pictures and then produc-ing a response. Auditory memory as well as auditoryattentiveness can be measured with the auditoryContinuous Performance Test (CPT)73 at later ages,and verbal IQs can be obtained. The standard use ofthe BSID-II is not appropriate for children who arelegally blind.

Hearing impairment precludes adequate assess-ment of verbal abilities; sensorimotor subtests can beadministered, but an overall BSID-II MDI cannot becomputed. The Mullen Scales of Early Learning89

may be considered, using the LRO and LEO scalesfor children with hearing impairment, although in-tersensory items (requiring hearing and vision orhearing and motor skills) must be addressed. Withincreasing age, nonverbal tests can be used (allowingcomputation of nonverbal indexes), but the hearing-

TABLE 3. Comprehensive Cognitive Assessment Protocol

12 mo CA 24 mo CA 3–4 y 6 y 8 y

BSID-II37 BSID-II37 DAS75 WISC-IV85 WISC-IV85

MDI and PDI MDI and PDI BSID-II (36 mo)Bayley MDI Bayley MDI McCarthy79 WASI84 WASI84

Stanford Binet-V81 Stanford Binet-V81 Stanford Binet-V81

WPPSI-III83 NEPSY78 NEPSY78

Bracken Basic Concepts-R71 VMI70 BRIEF76

Grooved Pegboard80 Parent and teacher versionsK-ABC77 (sequencing simultaneous

processing subscales)Conners CPT checklists73 Conners CPT checklists73

WRAML82/CMS72

CVLT-C74

DAS indicates Differential Ability Scale; MSCA, McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities; SB-V, Stanford-Binet V; WPPSI-IV, WechslerPreschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-IV; K-ABC, Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children; WISC-IV, Wechsler Intelligence Scalefor Children-IV; WASI, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; VMI, Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration; BRIEF,Behavior Rating Index of Executive Function (questionnaire); WRAML, Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning; CMS,Children’s Memory Scale; CVLT-C-California Verbal Learning Test–Children’s Version.

SUPPLEMENT 1385 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 10: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

impaired child still may find it difficult to under-stand directions and provide the required response.In these settings, performance IQs can be derived,and visual CPTs,73 visual memory tasks,90 visual-motor integration,70 rating scales, and more specificneuropsychological tests may be used. The LeiterInternational Performance Scales-R91 is a useful testfor use in the hearing-disabled population (2–20years), because auditory skills are not required.

Finally, it is important to realize that the referencenorms of most tests are children without sensoryimpairments; hence, administration to children withvisual or hearing deficits may affect the tests’ psy-chometric qualities. The NICHD protocol codes spe-cifically for children who have a sensory loss (code 6indicates that the child cannot be tested but appearsnormal; code 5, the child cannot be tested but ap-pears to be mild to moderately delayed; code 4, thechild is severely developmentally delayed). For thechild classified as severely developmentally delayed,a Bayley score of 49 is assigned.

Assessment of cognitive abilities in children withmotor impairments is particularly difficult beforepreschool because of the interweaving of cognitiveand motor function. Substantial alterations of testprocedures are necessary that, in turn, may have animpact on comparability to norm-referenced data.Subsequently, verbal tasks as well as nonverbal tasksthat do not require a motor response (eg, matrices)can be used. Assistive devices/technology also maybe used in such situations. Tests recommended forprograms with comprehensive follow-up protocolsinclude the Differential Ability Scales,75 McCarthyScales of Children’s Abilities,79 Stanford Binet,81

Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children,85 KaufmanAssessment Battery for Children,77 Wechsler Pre-school and Primary Scale of Intelligence-III,83

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence,84 theNEPSY,78 Developmental Test of Visual Motor In-tegration,70 CPTs,73 Behavior Rating Inventory ofExecutive Function,76 Wide Range Assessment ofMemory and Learning,82 Children’s Memory Scale,72

and the California Verbal Learning Test-Children74

(Table 3).Cognitive tests recommended for programs

with limited assessment protocols include theBINS,87 the BSID-II MDI,37 Ages and Stages,92 andthe Cognitive Adaptive Test/Clinical Linguistic andAuditory Milestone Scale93 (CAT/CLAMS) (Table 4).

Additional assessments are brief versions of some ofthe tests listed in Table 3.70,73,77–79,84,85,88,94

Functional SkillsInstruments for measuring functional status in es-

sential activities of self-care, mobility, communica-tion, and learning are assessed with the FunctionalIndependence Measure for Children,95 the VinelandAdaptive Behavior Scale,96 the Battelle,97 and thePediatric Evaluation of Disability Inventory98 psy-chometric properties of these assessments are shownin Table 5. Examination of the functional strengthsand challenges of children with neurodevelopmentalimpairments provides additional information re-garding disablement (attitudes, barriers, and policiesthat restrict opportunities) and enablement (supportsthat break down barriers). A similar assessment offunctional skills obtained by parent interview wasdeveloped by Stein et al.99 Functional assessmentshave important strengths including the need for aminimal database, use of criterion referencing, ob-taining information on typical performance, andability to measure severe disability. In addition,functional assessments can be administered to a re-liable observer/informant by staff trained in a vari-ety of disciplines and by telephone interview andmay be linked to population surveys.

LanguageEffective receptive and expressive language is fun-

damental for communication, adaptive behavioraland academic success, and literacy. Assessing recep-tive and expressive language is particularly challeng-ing, because different language skills emerge and aremastered at different ages, the rate of developmentof different language skills is not linear, and differentmeans of assessment are suitable for different ages.Comprehensive language testing including expres-sive and receptive language, organization, and gram-mar is required to obtain a complete assessment oflanguage function. Parental checklists of expressivelanguage are available including the McArthur100 forages �1 year. Receptive language can be tested fromages 2 to adulthood with the Peabody Picture Vocab-ulary Test-III.101 The combination of receptive andexpressive language can be assessed with the Expres-sive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test102 and theReceptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test.103

Language organization can be assessed with the

TABLE 4. Limited Cognitive-Assessment Protocol

12 mo 24 mo 3–4 y 6 y 8 y

BINS87 BINS87 K-BIT88 WASI84 WASI84

Ages and Stage92 Ages and Stage92

CAT/CLAMS93 CAT/CLAMS93

BSID-II MDI37 BSID-II MDI37 WRIT94 4 subtests from WISC-IV85 4 subtests from WISC-IV85

BSID-II MDI (3)37

McCarthy Screener79/K-ABC77

short formConners CPT73 Selected NEPSY78 subtests

Conners CPT73

DTVMI70 DTVMI70 DTVMI70

K-BIT indicates Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; WRIT, Wide Range Intelligence Test; WASI, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence;MSCA, McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities; WISC-IV, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Subtests; DVTMI, Developmental Testof Vision Motor Integration.

1386 FOLLOW-UP CARE OF HIGH-RISK INFANTS by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 11: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

Mullen Scales of Early Learning,89 which has bothLRO and LEO scales. Assessment of grammar usingthe Rice/Wexler Test for Grammatical Impair-ment104 targets elements of morphosyntax known tobe weak in children with language impairments. Ithas strong predictive validity for children 4 to 6 yearsof age and has been standardized for ages 3 to 8years. The Clinical Evaluation of Language Funda-mentals III105 is another language test that assesseshigher-level language function for children �6 years.

Behavior and AttentionThe choice of which behavioral domain to measure

is a difficult decision for individuals designing afollow-up study. Target outcomes should be selectedon the basis of theoretical and clinical concerns forthe specific at-risk group. These concerns are com-bined with known stages of development to deter-mine when to assess. Fig 2 describes the timeline forbehavioral domains relevant to high-risk children.Cognition and motor control may be a higher prior-ity among ELBW children, whereas psychopathol-ogy and antisocial behavior may be of greater con-cern in drug-exposed children. Specific methodologyissues such as what measures to use, where to con-duct the assessment, and how to train testers areguided by good research practices and practical con-sideration of time and resources.

In general, the easiest, least demanding method isparent report. The next easiest are tests and childinterviews. The most difficult are procedures thatrequire specific settings and extensive scoring. Com-

puter implementations have helped to bring somedifficult procedures within reach (eg, CambridgeNeuropsychological Test Automated Battery,106 Di-agnostic Interview Schedule for Children,107 andTest of Variables of Attention [TOVA]90), but thesetests are demanding in terms of equipment and soft-ware.

Parent questionnaires are useful in cases in whichthe child exhibits a behavior but is too young toself-report or when brief observation is insufficient tocapture the outcome of interest. A behavior assess-ment administered by questionnaire is the Brief In-fant-Toddler Social Emotional Assessment Version1.0.108 It is designed as a first step in the early iden-tification of 1- to 3-year-olds with social-emotionalproblems and/or delays in social competence. It con-sists of 60 interview questions. A trained medical ornonmedical professional may administer this scale tothe primary caretaker of the child. Estimated time toadminister the Brief Infant-Toddler Social EmotionalAssessment is 15 minutes. Another good example ofa behavior assessment for children is the AchenbachChild Behavior Check List (CBCL).109,110 The CBCLis a comprehensive measure of parental perceptionof children’s behaviors designed to assess the socialcompetencies and behavioral problems of childrenaged 2 to 3 (CBCL 2-3) and 4 to 18 (CBCL 4-18) in astandardized format. The CBCL provides norms forinterpretation, can be administered by interview forparents with low reading skills, and has a simpleyes/no response format. There are versions forteachers and adolescents, which can be useful. Re-

Fig 2. Timeline: child outcomes.

TABLE 5. Levels of Follow-up Program Intensity

Level 1 Level 2 Level 3 Level 4

Telephone interview to screen:developmental screeners;Ages and Stages92 andCAT/CLAMS93

In-clinic single visit: growth;neurologic exam; screen;developmental screeners;Ages and Stages92 andCAT/CLAMS93

developmental screeners:BINS87

Single visit: comprehensiveassessment; growth;neurologic exam;developmentalassessment (Tables 2 and3) (behavior, otherreduced comprehension

Serial comprehensive assessments:growth; neurologic exam;developmental assessment,behavior (may includevideotapes, MRIs, actigraphy,parent IQ, telemedicine,biochemical parameters,

or comprehension) geneticsRefer for diagnostic or

intervention services asneeded

Refer for diagnostic orintervention services asneeded

Refer for diagnostic orintervention services asneeded

Refer for diagnostic orintervention services as needed

Collect data Collect data Collect data Collect dataClinical Clinical Clinical/Research Clinical/Research

SUPPLEMENT 1387 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 12: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

sults from all parent-report questionnaires must beinterpreted with caution, because they reflect theparent’s own bias and relationship with the child aswell as the child’s behavior. Therefore, it is probablybest among school-aged children to have both parentand teacher versions. The Conners111,112 parent andteacher rating forms are frequently used when as-sessing school-aged children for attention deficit andhyperkinesis.

Parent and child report as well as tests are subjectto sources of error. As noted, parent reports aresubject to parental bias. Child reports are also subjectto the child’s desire to please or hide bad things andto the child’s understanding of the questions and theresponse scale. Tests with good psychometric prop-erties typically have highly constrained acceptableanswers and strict instructions for administration.Thus, children’s spontaneous responses and strate-gies, including how they reach their conclusion, arenot typically measured.

For older children (7–17 years), the Children’s De-pression Inventory113 and the Behavior AssessmentSystems for Children114 may be useful. The BehaviorAssessment Systems for Children is a broad-bandtool that provides a good representation of thechild’s behavioral functioning. Diagnostic and Statis-tical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition115 cri-teria are used to establish diagnostic categories.

TOVA90 is a computerized test that provides acontinuous performance task involving sustained at-tention and response inhibition. It is less fun for thechildren by design but provides data that can becompared with norms (separately for girls and boys).The Gordon Diagnostic System,116 which is similar,is a microprocessor-based system with tasks for di-agnosing attention deficits, especially ADHD. Bothtests give insight into attention and impulsivity dis-orders. The Conners111,112 parent and teacher ratingforms, which are frequently used when assessingschool-aged children for attention deficit and hy-perkinesis, differ somewhat but also give compara-ble information. Finally, the Diagnostic InterviewSchedule for Children107 has the advantage of gen-erating psychiatric diagnoses without professional-level staff, which makes it accessible for a researchstudy, but it is demanding for children, particularlythose who are lower functioning.

School-Age OutcomesStudies of school-age outcomes of high-risk chil-

dren to date have been predominantly descriptiverather than hypothesis driven. Assessments have in-cluded measurement of growth; parent and teacherquestionnaires concerning health, school perfor-mance, behavior and psychiatric disorders, quality oflife (QOL), self-esteem, impact on the family, andexamination and testing of children for neurosensorydisorders, cognition, specific neuropsychologic andmotor functioning, and academic achievement. Theimpact on society of the increased survival of high-risk children and the costs to the health and educa-tional system at school age have not been fully ex-amined.

At school age and older, it becomes important to

know how the children and their families function inthe various outcome categories; however, a noncat-egorical combined or functional approach may pro-vide better information concerning the overall im-pact of modern intensive care methods.117

The traditional assessment of high-risk outcomesbased on specific diagnoses or problems has limita-tions. Health problems in normal child populationsare reported to occur in clusters, with many childrenhaving �1 type of problem.118 Additionally, highhealth utilization tends to be associated with thepresence of a variety of health problems rather thanwith a particular type of illness.119 Extremely pre-term or high-risk children epitomize the pattern ofmultiple health and educational problems. Categori-zation of the medical, neurologic, developmental,and behavioral problems individually thus will notreflect the impact of the combined effects on theiroverall health status and health care and educationalservice utilization. The use of diagnostic labels hasadditional disadvantages in that there may be incon-sistency in diagnoses. A diagnosis, furthermore, doesnot convey the extent of morbidity, especially func-tional consequences, for individual children, caregiv-ers, and health and educational providers.

The noncategorical approach provides for compre-hensive assessment of the functional impact of neo-natal intensive care survival on health and psycho-logical outcomes. It can be applied to meet variousservice- and policy-related objectives such as identi-fication of needs for service, functional impact, andprogram eligibility for Title V programs.120,121 Thisapproach does not preclude assessment of the obvi-ous biological differences that distinguish one dis-ease from another but complements the more tradi-tional categorical approach. It has been advocated forperinatal outcomes research but has rarely beenused.122–124

The 2 health instruments based on the generic ornoncategorical approach for late childhood andyoung-adult studies124–126 are the Child Health Ill-ness Profile (CHIP-CE)127 and the Questionnaire forIdentifying Children With Chronic Conditions(QuICC).128 The 2 questionnaires complement eachother in that the CHIP-CE examines the profile of thechild’s general health and developmental status,whereas the QuICC examines functioning and spe-cial health care and educational needs attributed tochronic illness. The CHIP-CE includes 6 domains: (1)satisfaction with health, (2) discomfort, (3) achieve-ment, (4) risk, (5) resilience, and (6) medical andpsychosocial disorders. It is applicable from 6 to 11years of age. The adolescent version (the CHIP-E129)can be used from 11 years old until late adolescence.The QuICC incorporates the consequences of chronicphysical, cognitive, psychological, and behavioralhealth conditions into 3 domains: (1) functional lim-itations, (2) dependence on compensatory mecha-nisms, and (3) need for services beyond routinecare.128,130 It was developed for children with specialhealth care needs. The Child Health Questionnaire(CHQ)131 is an instrument that has been designedand normed for children 5 to 18 years of age. TheCHQ can be used independently or in conjunction

1388 FOLLOW-UP CARE OF HIGH-RISK INFANTS by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 13: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

with Health Act condition-specific surveys in re-search or clinical settings.

QOLThere has been increasing interest in the parent

and child perception of “well-being” regarding as-pects of health that are relevant to the child andfamily, which has resulted in the concept of measure-ment of QOL.132 Although health status, functionalabilities, and QOL are 3 different concepts, they areused interchangeably. Broadly speaking, QOL is anall-inclusive concept that incorporates many factorsthat impact on the life of an individual, of whichhealth is only 1 domain. Health-related QOL(HRQL),132–135 on the other hand, is a multidimen-sional measure that takes into account all aspects ofhealth and implicitly reflects the personal values ofan individual. These values may be unrelated todisease state or severity. It is the characteristic ofpersonal values that distinguishes HRQL from othermeasures of health. It is important to take into ac-count the fact that young children are constantlydeveloping and have varying capabilities thatchange over time.136 In addition, their personal val-ues also evolve over time and generally differ fromadults in their views about QOL. Investigators re-cently attempted to involve older children in mea-suring their own HRQL. Others have also obtainedparallel proxy ratings from parents.137

Which Groups Should Have Measurement of HRQLIt is useful to learn from the personal perspectives

of the premature children and their parents.135,137 Todate, self-reported HRQL has been measured for in-fants of �1000-g birth weight at adolescence137; re-cent studies of preschool-aged children have shownthat, by parent report, the HRQL of infants �32weeks’ gestational age was significantly lower thanthat for infants �32 weeks’ gestation and the refer-ence group.138 HRQL measures also may have thepotential of providing complementary informationon subjects in pediatric clinical trials, although littlework has been done on this.

Practical ConsiderationsIt is important that the HRQL measures include

those aspects of functioning that are important to thechild and family: they should reflect the multipledomains of age-appropriate activities, and theyshould be reliable, valid, and sensitive to changes inQOL of the child.136,139,140 In addition, because of theshort attention span of the children, the question-naires should be brief and impose as little burden aspossible to the respondents and staff. It is also essen-tial that the self-completed measures are phrased inways that children can understand and are at theappropriate reading level of the child.

Strictly speaking, because HRQL measures are de-signed to reflect the personal perspective of the pa-tient, it is not possible to accurately obtain the datafrom children younger than 7 to 8 years. However,investigators recently showed that young childrenare capable of recognizing their emotions, althoughthey may have limited memory and ability to carry

out logical thinking of tasks. Given the opportunityand correct context, children as young as 7 years areable to answer questions regarding their own healthstatus. However, at least grade 2 skills are necessaryfor rating the feeling thermometer, and grade 6 read-ing skills are required to complete the StandardGamble technique.141 In older children, the opinionof the child should be sought whenever possible.Thus, unless parental proxy responses are obtained,it is not possible to obtain HRQL measures accu-rately for children �8 years old.

There are several modalities of administration ofthe HRQL measures: self-administered question-naires at the study site, mailed questionnaires (self-administered at home), telephone interview, face-to-face interview questionnaires at the study site orhome visit, and face-to-face interviews for youngerchildren (�8 years) using interactive pictures, vid-eos, and computer programs. A self-completed ques-tionnaire at the study site is obviously the mostcost-effective and suitable for older children and ad-olescents. A self-completed mailed questionnaire isalso cost-effective but less reliable, and the responserates are lower than with other modes of adminis-tration.142 Telephone interviews achieve a highercompliance than mailed questionnaires, but the va-lidity is questionable in young children. Face-to-faceinterviews are the most costly, but compliance isgreater and the data are more reliable.143

Finally, despite the growing literature, there con-tinues to be skepticism by the medical community inaccepting self-reported HRQL as relevant and validinformation.136 Eiser and Morse136 suggests thatQOL measures need to be used more routinely inevaluation of alternative treatments to understandthe burden experienced by the family.

The following is a brief description of the availablemeasures for young children:

1. The PedsQL144 is a generic 23-item developmen-tally appropriate questionnaire for children aged2 to 4, 5 to 7, 8 to 12, and 13 to 18 years. Pediatricself-report measures are available for children andadolescents aged 5 to 18 years, and parent proxy-report measures of child HRQL are available forchildren aged 2 to 18 years. A complementaryPedsQL disease-specific measure for chronichealth conditions is also available. The PedsQL isthe only generic self- and proxy-reported pediat-ric HRQL instrument that spans a wide range of 2to 18 years while maintaining items and scale.144

2. The TACQOL145 is a 108-item parent form thattaps 7 domains and also measures the child’s emo-tional reaction to his or her health problems.

3. The Quality of Life Preschool Children (TAPQOL)questionnaire133 has 43 items, taps 12 domains,and is also weighted by the emotional response toproblems in health states. In a study of preschoolpreterm infants between ages 1 and 4 years, whowere infants of �32 weeks’ gestation, differenceswere found between neonatologists’ and parents’perception of the HRQL of preterm children.138

4. The CHQ131 is a generic QOL instrument that has14 unique physical and psychosocial concepts. A

SUPPLEMENT 1389 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 14: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

parent-completed form is available for children 5to 11 years of age (full-length, 50 items; shortform, 28 items). In addition, a parent-completedInfant/Toddler Health Questionnaire with 8 in-fant concepts and 5 parent concepts is also avail-able. The CHQ and Infant/Toddler Health Ques-tionnaire essentially provide health profiles anddo not have a valuative component.

5. Health Status Classification System-PS146,147 isadapted from the widely used HU12/3 sys-tems.148 It is comprised of 12 dimensions encom-passing a broad range of physical and develop-mental parameters, each with 3 to 5 levels perdimension.

OBJECTIVE 4: DETERMINE THE RELEVANT ISSUESRELATED TO SELECTION OF CONTROLS AND

TRACKING OF SUBJECTSWell-selected control populations with back-

ground variables similar to study subjects are impor-tant for the assessment of outcomes of high-risk chil-dren. Variables to consider in selecting controlsinclude those related to sociodemographic status,ethnicity, gender, and age of the child. Measures ofsociodemographic status most commonly used in-clude parental education, age, marital status, type ofhealth insurance, and area of residence. Maternalrather than paternal sociodemographic status is usu-ally considered in the United States, because manymothers of high-risk children are unmarried, and thefathers may have little contact with their children.Siblings have been suggested as ideal controls, be-cause they share the same sociodemographic andgenetic background as the study child. However,siblings are rarely used, because they need to be ofthe same gender and of an age fairly close to that ofthe study child. Additional problems in using sib-lings are that they, themselves, are often preterm,and even if born at term gestation, their behavior andschool performance may be influenced by having a“special-born” sibling.

Normal birth weight control populations are usu-ally not needed when RCTs of perinatal therapies areundertaken. Normal birth weight controls are oftennot included when comparing the outcome of low-risk versus high-risk birth weight groups, eg, in stud-ies assessing the effects of chronic lung disease, brainhemorrhage, jaundice, etc.149

In contrast to school-age studies, descriptive stud-ies of early-childhood outcomes of high-risk children(�3 years) have often not included control subjects.However, it is currently recommended that if thetrue impact of prematurity is to be identified, “nor-mal” control populations should be included at allages. In 1992, Gross et al150 stressed the importanceof concurrent controls, because the cognitive testsused in earlier time periods may be outdated. Expe-rience with the BSID-II presents problems in thatrates of cognitive impairment are overestimatedwhen compared with those found at school age. Inaddition, average IQ scores increase by 0.5 points peryear or 5 points per decade. Ideally, controls shouldbe selected at birth by matching the study infant withthe next born infant of the same race, gender, se-

lected maternal characteristics (age, parity, etc), andhospital of birth, excluding infants with intrauterineinfections and major congenital malformations.151–155

Race is often used as a proxy for SES in inner-citypopulations. Hospital of birth may serve as a proxyfor type and quality of perinatal care and area ofresidence, although high-risk mothers may be trans-ported to tertiary perinatal centers from other areasof residence.

Because many early-childhood studies have notincluded birth controls, it may be necessary to findcontrols at school age.156–160 The most commonlyused method is to randomly select a classmate of thesame gender, race, and age (within 3–6 months) asthe study child. The school serves as a proxy for areaof residence and type and quality of education. Ide-ally, all the children of the same age in the child’sschool, rather than class, should be considered aspotential controls, because high-risk children repeatgrades more often than low-risk children. When chil-dren are home-schooled, attend special schools forthe disabled, or have moved out of the area, matchesare usually selected from the school the child wouldhave attended. Other study-design methods includerandom selection of controls from regional school-system records.158,161 Studies of large regional ornational populations have obtained outcomes throughlinkage of birth records to school records162,163 orlinkage of birth records to military-recruitmentrecords164,165 with comparison of the subjects to therest of the population.

Follow-up rates of at least 90% should be achievedat early childhood, at least 80% at early school age,and at least 70% at middle-school age and duringadolescence.124,159 Follow-up rates of control subjectsare usually 10% to 20% lower than those of studysubjects. In addition, despite the best attempts, con-trol subjects usually have subtly better home envi-ronments than study subjects. Intervention and con-trol subjects who are lost to follow-up are usually oflower SES than those followed. Because there arehigher rates of loss to follow-up among control low-er-SES subjects, the controls are usually of higher SESthan study subjects.166,167 Also, families of childrenconsidered healthy may be less likely to return.Therefore, it is imperative that strategies be devel-oped to maintain compliance, because it is difficult todetermine the “true” outcome of lost infants.

Elements needed to maintain good follow-up ratesare:

1. Enrollment of subjects before discharge from hos-pital and identification of a specific contact per-son.

2. Multiple back-up addresses of family and place ofwork.

3. Postdischarge and interim visits during the firstyear of life, for clinical purposes of developmentaland health surveillance, and maintenance of fam-ily involvement even when the first formal studyvisit may only be at 2 years of age; ideally, com-prehensive health care should be provided but isusually not feasible.168

1390 FOLLOW-UP CARE OF HIGH-RISK INFANTS by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 15: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

4. Close communication with, and respect for, healthcare providers of the subjects.

5. A dedicated study coordinator who is prepared totrack and call families at night and over weekendsand a research staff prepared to accommodateparent work schedules and to work weekends andperform home visits if necessary.

6. Reimbursement for transportation and parkingand specific financial incentives for time spent forthe collection of study data.

7. Adequate funding for all of the above.

OBJECTIVE 5: DETERMINE ALTERNATE, LESSCOSTLY METHODS AND APPROACHES FOR

FOLLOW-UP AND THE ROLE OF THECOMMUNITY PHYSICIAN

Because of limited funding sources, it is currentlynot feasible for all NICUs to have a comprehensivefollow-up program. NICUs that do not have the re-sources to establish a program need to share infor-mation with the family and primary provider aboutincreased risk of neurodevelopmental, behavioral,growth, and medical sequelae and facilitate referralsto early intervention and/or a child developmentcenter. Clinical programs with limited funding avail-able, however, may use screening methods such as aphone or in-house parent interview as a first-stagescreen to determine if more in depth assessments arewarranted or if referrals to early intervention or ed-ucational resources are indicated. In addition, a clin-ical program with limited resources may by necessityoffer a single follow-up visit. An optimal assessmentage combines a short interval between discharge andassessment age (to minimize tracking) with a devel-opmental age at which the accurate diagnosis ofmotor and developmental handicaps is feasible. Theapproach should facilitate a timely diagnosis andreferrals as needed for families and center surveil-lance objectives. The feasibility of collecting someannual well-defined short-term follow-up data for allNICUs continues to improve. The ability to providefeedback on individual child outcome and supportfor families is available to all NICUs.

The NICHD Network identified 18 to 22 months’corrected age as the optimal time for a single visit.Interim visits or phone contacts are recommendedfor tracking purposes.

Community physicians can take an active role par-ticipating in follow-up by performing periodic devel-opmental screening and responding to requests forhealth information on a child who does not return tothe follow-up program. This requires a partnershipbetween the community physicians and the tertiarycare center and the use of standardized tools. Inaddition, follow-up programs can provide valuableinformation about the child’s growth, neurodevelop-ment, and behavior to the community physician.

Table 6 contrasts 4 levels of management for theindividual provider, for programs with clinical sur-veillance or referral objectives, and for programswith combined research and clinical objectives.Whereas the clinical program may maintain a data-base with a limited number of key variables, screenchildren (an important service for families), and re-

fer, the research program has the opportunity todevelop more sophisticated protocols that are hy-pothesis driven and explore novel assessments.Screening tools such as the Ages and Stages92 andCAT/CLAMS93 may be administered during an of-fice visit or over the phone. Primary care providerswho perform a screen may share information withthe follow-up or diagnostic program and refer fam-ilies as needed. Diagnostic and/or follow-up pro-grams reciprocate by sharing results of comprehen-sive evaluations and recommendations. Primary carephysicians can collaborate by completing studyforms requesting data on growth, medical status,vision, and hearing.

OBJECTIVE 6: DETERMINE THE CHALLENGESENCOUNTERED IN MULTICENTER NETWORKSMulticenter networks have the advantage of being

able to assess low-incidence neonatal illness such asperinatal asphyxia and achieve adequate samplesizes for hypothesis-driven studies within a shorterperiod of time than a single-center study.169,170 How-ever, a number of challenges must be addressed if anetwork is going to succeed:

1. Achieving standardization of study protocols,manuals, definitions, and methodology amongcenters in diverse geographic areas is a majorchallenge for networks. Multicenter networksvary in their approaches to planning well-coordi-nated follow-up studies.

2. Achieving adequate follow-up rates is a majorchallenge, particularly with populations havingdiverse SESs, cultures, and primary spoken lan-guages. The NICHD network endorses a fol-low-up rate of at least 80% for centers at 18 and 30months’ corrected age.

3. A third challenge for multicenter networks is todefine the critical study population and the inter-val for enrollment. Network centers may enrollinfants with a birth weight of �1000 g annuallyand follow them for 18 months as a framework formulticenter trials. This approach facilitates on-go-ing standardization of follow-up protocols andreliability among examiners. An alternate ap-proach is to enroll children born �1000 g every 3years and follow each cohort for 3 years. Thisapproach has somewhat higher costs, follows chil-dren to preschool age, and has changing protocolsconsistent with the age assessed. Networksshould identify their unique study populationswith high illness severity and poor outcomes foradditional study (eg, extracorporeal membraneoxygenation patients).

4. A fourth challenge is to identify and enroll anappropriate cost-effective control population.

The following 4 multicenter networks provide in-sights into the complexities of conducting multi-center follow-up studies.

Vermont Oxford NetworkThe Vermont Oxford Network is a nonprofit vol-

untary collaboration of NICUs dedicated to improv-

SUPPLEMENT 1391 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 16: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

TA

BL

E6.

Psyc

hom

etri

cPr

oper

ties

ofW

eeFI

M,V

inel

and

,Bat

telle

,and

Ped

iatr

icE

valu

atio

nof

Dis

abili

tyIn

vent

ory

Wee

FIM

95B

atte

lle97

VA

BS9

6PE

DI9

8

Purp

ose

Dis

cipl

ine-

free

mea

sure

ofco

nsis

tent

perf

orm

ance

offu

ncti

onal

skill

sin

child

ren

aged

6m

oto

8y

and

child

ren

thro

ugh

adol

esce

nce

wit

hne

urod

evel

opm

enta

ld

isab

iliti

es

Dis

crim

inat

ive

mea

sure

ofd

evel

opm

enta

lsk

ills

inch

ildre

nag

edbi

rth

to8

yD

escr

ipti

vem

easu

reof

com

mun

icat

ion,

dai

lyliv

ing,

soci

aliz

atio

n,an

dm

otor

skill

sfo

rch

ildre

nag

edbi

rth

to18

y,d

isab

led

and

nond

isab

led

Dis

crim

inat

ive

mea

sure

offu

ncti

onal

limit

atio

nsin

child

ren

aged

6m

oto

7.5

y

Dom

ains

Self

-car

e,m

obili

ty,c

ogni

tion

Dai

lyliv

ing

(ad

apti

ve),

mot

or,

com

mun

icat

ion,

cogn

itiv

e,pe

rson

al-

soci

al

Dai

lyliv

ing

skill

s,m

otor

,co

mm

unic

atio

n,so

cial

izat

ion

Mot

or,s

elf-

care

,soc

ial

Rel

iabi

lity

Exc

elle

ntte

st-r

etes

tan

din

terr

ater

relia

bilit

y;eq

uiva

lenc

ere

liabi

lity

ofte

leph

one

inte

rvie

w

Tes

t-re

test

and

inte

rrat

erre

liabi

lity

good

toex

celle

ntSp

litha

lffo

rd

omai

nsan

dco

mpo

site

,ex

celle

nt;t

est-

rete

stre

liabi

lity,

exce

llent

;int

erra

ter

relia

bilit

y,go

od

Exc

elle

ntte

st-r

etes

tan

din

terr

ater

relia

bilit

y

Val

idit

yC

onte

ntva

lidat

ion

byex

pert

grou

p;co

ncur

rent

valid

ity

wit

hV

AB

S,B

atte

lle,A

AQ

,and

PED

I;to

tal

Wee

FIM

atki

nder

gart

enen

try

corr

elat

esw

ith

spec

ial-

educ

atio

nre

sour

ces

Exc

elle

ntco

rrel

atio

nw

ith

VA

BS

and

Wee

FIM

;con

curr

ent

valid

ity

and

WIS

C-R

Exc

elle

ntco

rrel

atio

nw

ith

scho

ol-a

geIQ

Con

curr

ent

wit

hB

atte

llean

dW

eeFI

M

App

licat

ion

Mea

sure

offu

ncti

onal

stat

usat

age

5.5

yin

VL

BW

Cry

oRO

Pco

hort

;4-

you

tcom

ein

mul

tice

nter

surf

acta

ntco

mpa

riso

ntr

ials

;fu

ncti

onal

goal

-set

ting

inch

ildre

nw

ith

cere

bral

pals

y,sp

ina

bifi

da,

and

gene

tic

and

acqu

ired

dis

abili

ties

Nor

mal

and

dis

able

dpr

esch

oole

rin

earl

yin

terv

enti

on;c

hild

ren

wit

hd

evel

opm

enta

ld

isab

iliti

esin

earl

yel

emen

tary

scho

ol

Pres

choo

lan

dlo

ng-t

erm

outc

ome

inV

LB

Wch

ildre

n;us

ein

dev

elop

men

tal

dis

abili

ties

Pare

ntin

terv

iew

form

atfo

rch

ildre

nw

ith

cere

bral

pals

y;cl

inic

alev

alua

tion

afte

rrh

izot

omy

and

trau

mat

icbr

ain

inju

ry

Ad

min

istr

atio

nti

me,

min

2045

3045

Wee

FIM

ind

icat

esFu

ncti

onal

Ind

epen

den

ceM

easu

refo

rC

hild

ren;

PED

I,Pe

dia

tric

Eva

luat

ion

ofD

isab

ility

Inve

ntor

y;V

AB

S,V

inel

and

Ad

apti

veB

ehav

ior

Scal

e;W

ISC

-R,W

echs

ler

Inte

llige

nce

Scal

efo

rC

hild

ren-

Rev

ised

;AA

Q,A

mou

ntof

Ass

ista

nce

Que

stio

nnai

re.

1392 FOLLOW-UP CARE OF HIGH-RISK INFANTS by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 17: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

ing the quality and safety of medical care for new-born infants and their families through a coordinatedprogram of clinical trials, research, education, andquality-improvement projects. Established in 1988,the network has grown to �400 member centersrepresenting NICUs throughout the United Statesand 17 other countries. The network maintains adatabase of information about the care and short-term outcomes of VLBW infants. In 1998, the Ver-mont Oxford Network began to collect follow-updata for ELBW infants at 2 years of age. To partici-pate, centers had to have a follow-up clinic, evaluatechildren until 2 years of age, and administer theBSID-II.

Canadian Neonatal NetworkThe Canadian Neonatal Network was developed

in 1996. Data were prospectively collected by trainedabstractors at 17 tertiary-level NICUs across Canada.This network has assessed variations in practice rel-ative to outcome. As a result, more regionalized ap-proaches to follow-up are in place in Canada. Afollow-up network was developed in Quebec in 2001with funds from the Fonds de la Recherche en Santedu Quebec and included all infants already enrolledin 6 follow-up programs.

NICHD Research Follow-up StudyThe NICHD Research Follow-up Study was initi-

ated in 1993 and consists of 16 academic tertiary carecenters. The primary study objective for the NICHDFollow-up Network is to track and successfully eval-uate �80% of infants entered in to the generic data-base with birth weights of 401 to 1000 g born afterJanuary 1, 1993, and infants enrolled in clinical trialswith neurodevelopment as an outcome. Currently,the network is conducting a pilot 30-month neuro-developmental outcome study on infants enrolled inthe glutamine trial. The network has an infrastruc-ture that includes a steering committee, protocol re-view committee, study manuals, standardized defi-nitions, training protocols, and a data-coordinatingcenter. Annual workshops and regular conferencecalls and meetings facilitate communication, coordi-nation, and standardization. The network has estab-lished standardized methods to assess growth, mo-tor skills, cognitive skills, language and behavior,and strict criteria for annual certification of Bayleyexaminers and neurologic examiners. All networkstudy interviews and assessments are available inEnglish and Spanish. Studies and assessments areconducted by network investigators at the individualcenter and have resulted in numerous publications.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Networkfor Developmental Disabilities and Birth Defects:Surveillance System and Centers of Excellence

This national surveillance system under the Na-tional Center on Birth Defects and DevelopmentalDisabilities was mandated by the Children’s HealthAct of 2000 and implemented in April, 2001. Themission is to prevent birth defects and developmen-tal disabilities, pursue the causes and risk factorsassociated with defects, and promote wellness of

individuals living with a disability. It supports apublic health approach to the prevention of birthdefects with surveillance systems, epidemiologicstudies, and prevention programs. An example of asuccessful population-based surveillance system op-erating continuously since 1968 is the MetropolitanAtlanta Congenital Defects Program. Success ismaintained with intensive case ascertainment, a caseregistry, a surveillance laboratory, and professionaltraining.

OBJECTIVE 7A. IDENTIFY GAPS IN KNOWLEDGEABOUT HIGH-RISK SURVIVORS IN YOUNG

ADULTHOOD

1. Do they attain the social/educational status oftheir parents, or do they demonstrate downwardmobility (measured by comparing educationaland occupational attainment, associated rating ofprestige, and earning capacity)?

2. Can they live independently and support them-selves?

3. Do they develop normal social and family rela-tionships (measured by specific questionnairesand from their history of permanent personal re-lationships of marriage)?

4. Does catch-up growth occur in late adolescence?5. What are the long-term metabolic and cardiovas-

cular implications of growth failure in utero, neo-natally, and during infancy and of catch-upgrowth during various time periods of childhoodand adolescence?

6. What are the long-term health effects of chronicmedical sequelae of prematurity (especially pul-monary, visual, and neurologic)?

7. Do they have higher rates of psychiatric disorders,substance abuse, contact with police, and incarcer-ation?

8. Does the ADHD associated with prematurity per-sist into adulthood?

9. Are there intergenerational effects of high-riskbirth, prematurity, and/or low birth weight, and,if so, is it possible to break the cycle?

OBJECTIVE 7B. FURTHER AREAS OFINVESTIGATION

The following areas of investigation with a poten-tial to improve and optimize the outcomes of high-risk infants were identified. The recommendationswere:

1. to propose hypothesis-driven studies and RCTs toidentify the mechanisms that contribute to bothinjury and plasticity and recovery of the CNS,including animal and tissue studies

2. to identify the assessment strategies and sched-ules that are the most predictive of long-termoutcomes and identify earlier surrogate markersfor long-term outcomes

3. to determine the predictive value of neonatal im-aging modalities in the prediction of neurodevel-opmental outcome

4. to identify perinatal interventions that contributeto positive neurodevelopmental outcomes

SUPPLEMENT 1393 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 18: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

5. to conduct population-based studies to determinethe true societal burden of illness within a geo-graphic region or a national sample (currently, thevast majority of studies report on subgroups ofinfants such as VLBW, ELBW, chronic lung dis-ease, asphyxia, small for gestational age, etc); ageographic sample would include all infants bornwithin a geographic region (such a study couldask questions regarding the total population ofinfants born within a specific time interval, in aspecific region, and followed for a predeterminedlength of time)

OBJECTIVE 8: PROPOSE RESEARCH QUESTIONS

1. What are the mechanisms by which intrauter-ine/neonatal inflammation/cytokines and hyp-oxia ischemia produce CNS injury, and what arethe effects on long-term outcome?

2. What are the mechanisms contributing to and theinterventions that can further reduce the severityof chronic lung disease in ELBW infants?

3. Does stress and/or recurrent pain in the neonateresult in permanent physiologic changes in re-sponse to pain?

4. What threshold of bilirubin and what duration ofhyperbilirubinemia result in neurodevelopmen-tal injury?

5. What modalities of resuscitation contribute tooptimal neurodevelopmental outcome of ELBWinfants?

6. What is the outcome of the macro prematureinfant (1500- to 2500-g birth weight)?

7. What is the impact of major congenital malfor-mations and cardiac anomalies on outcome, andwhat are the effective perinatal interventions?

8. What are the neurodevelopmental and growthmorbidities associated with monochorionic twinpregnancy and higher-order multiples?

9. What is the cost/benefit of follow-up care andassessment?

10. What is the impact of neonatal and postdischargenutrition on early catch-up growth and develop-mental outcome?

11. What is the role of early calorie and proteinintake and human milk on brain growth, andwhat are the long-term effects on cardiovascularand endocrine function disorder?

12. What is the impact of specific antenatal and neo-natal interventions (magnesium sulfate, antena-tal steroids, postnatal steroids, repeat doses ofindomethacin antioxidants) on brain growth andfunction?

13. What is the impact of medical and/or surgicalinterventions for NEC on outcomes? What arethe potential interventions to prevent NEC?

14. What are the neonatal neuroimaging techniquesthat best identify neurologic insult and predictneurologic and developmental outcome?

15. What are the genetic factors contributing to CNSmorbidity and recovery? Is there a genomic vul-nerability to injury?

16. What are the mechanisms by which an enrichedenvironment contributes to plasticity? Are therecritical periods for intervention?

17. What are the best screening tools for identifica-tion of infants at risk of adverse neurodevelop-mental sequelae?

18. What are the best early surrogate markers forlong-term neurodevelopmental disability?

Systematic long-term outcome studies of ex-tremely premature or ELBW infants are indispens-able for perinatologists and neonatologists: (1) out-come data facilitate informed decision-making in theNICU and the development of strategies for identi-fying infants at high risk for medical and neurode-velopmental sequelae; (2) families of infants identi-fied as high risk may be guided to comprehensivefollow-up and early intervention, and (3) morbidityrates may be used to estimate resources that societyshould be prepared to provide to optimize QOL ofsurvivors and their families. Considerable chal-lenges, however, secondary to limitations of staffing,costs, and expertise with assessments confront inves-tigators performing long-term outcome studies andthese challenges contribute to a paucity of high-qual-ity outcome data. Limitations of many studies in-clude small sample sizes, heterogeneity of cohorts,heterogeneity of methodology, limited informationabout both perinatal clinical practice and perinatalcomplications associated with long-term sequelae,and heterogeneity of specific neurodevelopmentalassessments and age of outcome.

CONCLUSIONSAlthough single-center studies may address some

of these issues and answer important questions, mul-ticenter research networks with standardized studymanuals, protocols, and methods are needed for rep-resentative surveillance and clinical trials. It is hopedthat this supplement provides information that willhelp follow-up program directors and primary careproviders make choices about assessment protocolsbased on options provided and practical consider-ations.

Unfortunately, ELBW infants remain at increasedrisk of neurodevelopmental sequelae. Well-con-ducted multicenter trials, which include neurodevel-opmental follow-up, continue to be the catalysts thatallow investigators to identify the mechanisms thatcontribute to injury and recovery and the perinatalinterventions that contribute to positive neurodevel-opmental outcome.

REFERENCES1. Barrington KJ. The adverse neuro-developmental effects of postnatal

steroids in the preterm infant: a systematic review of RCTs. BMCPediatr. 2001;1:1

2. Thebaud B, Lacaze-Masmonteil T, Watterberg K. Postnatal glucocorti-coids in very preterm infants: “the good, the bad, and the ugly”?Pediatrics. 2001;107:413–415

3. Kinsey VE, Arnold HJ, Kalina RE, et al. PaO2 levels and retrolentalfibroplasia: a report of the cooperative study. Pediatrics. 1977;60:655–668

4. Msall ME, Phelps DL, DiGaudio KM, et al. Severity of neonatal reti-nopathy of prematurity is predictive of neurodevelopmental func-tional outcome at age 5.5 years. Pediatrics. 2000;106:998–1005

5. Avery ME, Tooley WH, Keller JB, et al. Is chronic lung disease in lowbirth weight infants preventable? A survey of eight centers. Pediatrics.1987;79:26–30

1394 FOLLOW-UP CARE OF HIGH-RISK INFANTS by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 19: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

6. Horbar JD, Rogowski J, Plsek PE, et al. Collaborative quality improve-ment for neonatal intensive care. Pediatrics. 2001;107:14–22

7. Richardson DK, Shah BL, Frantz ID 3rd, Bednarek F, Rubin LP, Mc-Cormick MC. Perinatal risk and severity of illness in newborns at 6neonatal intensive care units. Am J Public Health. 1999;89:511–516

8. Vohr BR, Wright LL, Dusick AM, et al. Center differences and out-comes of extremely low birth weight infants. Pediatrics. 2004;113:781–789

9. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Fetus and Newborn.The initiation or withdrawal of treatment for high-risk newborns.Pediatrics. 1995;96:362–363

10. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Practice and Ambu-latory Medicine. Recommendations for preventative pediatric healthcare. Pediatrics. 2000;105:645–646

11. Perlman JM. Neurobehavioral deficits in premature graduates of in-tensive care—potential medical and neonatal environmental risk fac-tors. Pediatrics. 2001;108:1339–1348

12. Perlman JM. White matter injury in the preterm infant: an importantdetermination of abnormal neurodevelopment outcome. Early HumDev. 1998;53:99–120

13. Hack M, Fanaroff AA. Outcomes of children of extremely low birth-weight and gestational age in the 1990’s. Early Hum Dev. 1999;53:193–218

14. Botting N, Powls A, Cooke RW, Marlow N. Attention deficit hyperac-tivity disorders and other psychiatric outcomes in very low birth-weight children at 12 years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:931–941

15. Stjernqvist K, Svenningsen NW. Ten-year follow-up of children bornbefore 29 gestational weeks: health, cognitive development, behaviourand school achievement. Acta Paediatr. 1999;88:557–562

16. Cooke RW, Abernethy LJ. Cranial magnetic resonance imaging andschool performance in very low birth weight infants in adolescence.Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1999;81:F116–F121

17. Stewart AL, Rifkin L, Amess PN, et al. Brain structure and neurocog-nitive and behavioural function in adolescents who were born verypreterm. Lancet. 1999;353:1653–1657

18. Stevenson DK, Wright LL, Lemons JA, et al. Very low birth weightoutcomes of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Devel-opment Neonatal Research Network, January 1993 through December1994. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1998;179:1632–1639

19. Vohr BR, Wright LL, Dusick AM, et al. Neurodevelopmental andfunctional outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants in theNational Institute of Child Health and Human Development NeonatalResearch Network, 1993–1994. Pediatrics. 2000;105:1216–1226

20. Ment LR, Vohr B, Allan W, et al. The etiology and outcome of cerebralventriculomegaly at term in very low birth weight preterm infants.Pediatrics. 1999;104:243–248

21. Chamnanvanakij S, Rogers CG, Luppino C, Broyles SR, Hickman J,Perlman JM. Linear hyperechogenicity within the basal ganglia andthalamus of preterm infants. Pediatr Neurol. 2000;23:129–133

22. Meaney MJ, Diorio J, Francis D, et al. Postnatal handling increases theexpression of cAMP-inducible transcription factors in the rathippocampus: the effects of thyroid hormones and serotonin. J Neuro-sci. 2000;20:3926–3935

23. McEwen BS. Stress and hippocampal plasticity. Annu Rev Neurosci.1999;22:105–122

24. Sapolsky RM. Why stress is bad for your brain. Science. 1996;273:749–750

25. Ballard RA, Ballard PL. Antenatal hormone therapy for improving theoutcome of the preterm infant. J Perinatol. 1996;16:390–396

26. Effect of corticosteroids for fetal maturation on perinatal outcomes.NIH Consensus Development Panel on the Effect of Corticosteroids forFetal Maturation on Perinatal Outcomes. JAMA. 1995;273:413–418

27. Gardner MO, Papile LA, Wright LL. Antenatal corticosteroids in preg-nancies complicated by preterm premature rupture of membranes.Obstet Gynecol. 1997;90:851–853

28. Jobe AH. Glucocorticoids in perinatal medicine: misguided rockets?J Pediatr. 2000;137:1–3

29. Yeh TF, Lin YJ, Huang CC, et al. Early dexamethasone therapy inpreterm infants: a follow-up study. Pediatrics. 1998;101(5). Available at:www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/101/5/e7

30. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Fetus and Newborn.Postnatal corticosteroids to treat or prevent chronic lung disease inpreterm infants. Pediatrics. 2002;109:330–338

31. Sapolsky RM, Uno H, Rebert CS, Finch CE. Hippocampal damageassociated with prolonged glucocorticoid exposure in primates. J Neu-rosci. 1990;10:2897–2902

32. Hollingshead A. Four Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven, CT:University Press; 1975

33. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child develop-ment. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:371–399

34. Entwisle DR, Astone NM. Some practical guidelines for measuringyouth’s race/ethnicity and socio economic status. Child Dev. 1994;65:1521–1540

35. Hauser RM. Measuring socioeconomic status in studies of child devel-opment. Child Dev. 1994;65:1541–1545

36. Aylward GP. The relationship between environmental risk and devel-opmental outcome. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1992;13:222–229

37. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infants Development-II. San Antonio, TX: ThePsychological Corporation; 1993

38. Bradley RH, Caldwell BM, Brisby J, Magee M, Whiteside L, Rock SL.The HOME inventory: a new scale for families of pre- and earlyadolescent children with disabilities. Res Dev Disabil. 1992;13:313–333

39. Bradley RH, Caldwell BM. The relation of infants’ home environmentsto achievement test performance in first grade: a follow-up study. ChildDev. 1984;55:803–809

40. Drillien C. Abnormal neurologic signs in the first year of life inlow-birth weight infants: possible prognostic significance. Dev MedChild Neurol. 1997;14:575–584

41. Weisglas-Kuperus N, Baerts W, Smrkovsky M, Sauer PJ. Effects ofbiological and social factors on the cognitive development of very lowbirth weight children. Pediatrics. 1993;92:658–665

42. Dezoete JA, MacArthur BA, Tuck B. Prediction of Bayley and Stanford-Binet scores with a group of very low birthweight children. Child CareHealth Dev. 2003;29:367–372

43. Lee H, Barratt MS. Cognitive development of preterm low birth weightchildren at 5 to 8 years old. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1993;14:242–249

44. Blasco PA. Preterm birth: to correct or not to correct. Dev Med ChildNeurol. 1989;31:816–821

45. Lems W, Hopkins B, Samson JF. Mental and motor development inpreterm infants: the issue of corrected age. Early Hum Dev. 1993;34:113–123

46. Dewey DG, Crawford SG, Creighton DE, Sauve RS. Long-term neuro-psychological outcomes in very low birth weight children free ofsensorineural impairments. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1999;21:851–865

47. Battaglia FC, Lubchenco LO. A practical classification of newborninfants by weight and gestational age. J Pediatr. 1967;71:159–163

48. Lubchenco LO, Hansman C, Boyd E. Intrauterine growth in length andhead circumference as estimated from live births at gestational agesfrom 26 to 42 weeks. Pediatrics. 1966;37:403–408

49. Usher R, McLean F. Intrauterine growth of live-born Caucasian infantsat sea level: standards obtained from measurements in 7 dimensions ofinfants born between 25 and 44 weeks of gestation. J Pediatr. 1969;74:901–910

50. Arbuckle TE, Sherman GJ. An analysis of birth weight by gestationalage in Canada. CMAJ. 1989;140:157–160, 165

51. Alexander GR, Himes JH, Kaufman RB, Mor J, Kogan M. A UnitedStates national reference for fetal growth. Obstet Gynecol. 1996;87:163–168

52. Ehrenkranz RA, Younes N, Lemons JA, et al. Longitudinal growth ofhospitalized very low birth weight infants. Pediatrics. 1999;104:280–289

53. Hamill PV, Drizd TA, Johnson CL, Reed RB, Roche AF, Moore WM.Physical growth: National Center for Health Statistics percentiles. Am JClin Nutr. 1979;32:607–629

54. Lothman T, Roche AF, Martorell R, eds. Anthropometric StandardizationReference Manual. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Books; 1988

55. Amiel-Tison C. Neuromotor Status. Boston, MA: Little, Brown &Company; 1987

56. Fenichel G. Clinical Pediatric Neurology: A Signs and Symptoms Approach.3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders; 1997

57. Swaiman K. Neurological examination of the older child. In: SwaimanK, Ashwal S, eds. Pediatric Neurology. Vol 1. 3rd ed. Boston, MA:Mosby; 1999:14–30

58. Baker LL, Stevenson DK, Enzmann DR. End-stage periventricularleukomalacia: MR evaluation. Radiology. 1988;168:809–815

59. Olsen P, Paakko E, Vainionpaa L, Pyhtinen J, Jarvelin MR. Magneticresonance imaging of periventricular leukomalacia and its clinicalcorrelation in children. Ann Neurol. 1997;41:754–761

60. Barkovich AJ, Kjos BO, Jackson DE Jr, Norman D. Normal maturationof the neonatal and infant brain: MR imaging at 1.5 T. Radiology.1988;166:173–180

61. Counsell SJ, Maalouf EF, Fletcher AM, et al. MR imaging assessment ofmyelination in the very preterm brain. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2002;23:872–881

62. van de Bor M, den Ouden L, Guit GL. Value of cranial ultrasound andmagnetic resonance imaging in predicting neurodevelopmental out-come in preterm infants. Pediatrics. 1992;90:196–199

SUPPLEMENT 1395 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 20: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

63. Wilson DA, Steiner RE. Periventricular leukomalacia: evaluation withMR imaging. Radiology. 1986;160:507–511

64. Johnson AJ, Lee BC, Lin W. Echoplanar diffusion-weighted imaging inneonates and infants with suspected hypoxic-ischemic injury: correla-tion with patient outcome. AJR Am J Roentgenol. 1999;172:219–226

65. Forbes KP, Pipe JG, Bird R. Neonatal hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy:detection with diffusion-weighted MR imaging. AJNR Am J Neurora-diol. 2000;21:1490–1496

66. Huppi PS, Murphy B, Maier SE, et al. Microstructural brain develop-ment after perinatal cerebral white matter injury assessed by diffusiontensor magnetic resonance imaging. Pediatrics. 2001;107:455–460

67. Wood E, Rosenbaum P. The gross motor function classification systemfor cerebral palsy: a study of reliability and stability over time. DevMed Child Neurol. 2000;42:292–296

68. Palisano RJ. Validity of goal attainment scaling in infants with motordelays. Phys Ther. 1993;73:651–658; discussion 658–660

69. Ment LR, Vohr B, Allan W, et al. Change in cognitive function overtime in very low-birth-weight infants. JAMA. 2003;289:705–711

70. Beery K. Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration. 4th ed. Parsip-pany, NJ: Modern Curriculum Press; 1997

71. Bracken BA. Bracken Basic Concepts Scale. Version 2. San Antonio, TX:The Psychological Corporation; 1998

72. Cohen M. Children’s Memory Scale. San Antonio, TX: The PsychologicalCorporation; 1997

73. Conners CK. Conners Continuous Performance Test for Windows [com-puter program]. Version 2. North Tonawanda, NY: International Psy-chological Services; 2000

74. Delis D, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal LearningTest-Children. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1994

75. Elliott C. Differential Ability Scales. Introductory and Technical Handbook.New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation; 1990

76. Gioia G, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior Rating Inventory ofExecutive Function, Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological As-sessment Resources, Inc; 2000

77. Kaufman A, Kaufman NL. Interpretive Manual for the Kaufman Assess-ment Battery for Children. Circle Pines, MN: American GuidanceService; 1983

78. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp S. Neuropsychological Test. San Antonio, TX:The Psychological Corporation; 1997

79. McCarthy D. Manual for the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities. NewYork, NY: The Psychological Corporation; 1972

80. Klove H. Clinical neuropsychology. In: Forster FM, ed. The MedicalClinics of North America. New York, NY: WB Saunders; 1963

81. Roid G. The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales. 4th ed. Itasca, IL: RiversidePublishing Co; 2003

82. Sheslow D, Adams W. Wide Range Assessment of Memory and LearningAdministration Manual. Wilmington, DE: Jastek Associates; 1990

83. Wechsler D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-III. SanAntonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2002

84. Wechsler D. The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio,TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1999

85. Wechsler D. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 4th ed. SanAntonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2003

86. Colarusso RP, Hammill DD. Motor-Free Visual Perception Test Manual.Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 1972

87. Aylward G. The Bayley Infant Neurodevelopmental Screener. San Antonio,TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1995

88. Kaufman A, Kaufman NL. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test Manual. CirclePines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1990

89. Mullen E. Mullen Scales of Early Learning. AGS Edition. Circle Pines,MN: American Guidance Service; 1995

90. Greenberg L, Kindschi CL, Corman CM. TOVA: Test of Variables ofAttention. Los Alamitos, CA: Universal Attention Disorders, Inc; 2000

91. Roid G. The Leiter International Performance Scales. Woodale, IL: Stoelt-ing Publishing Company; 2003

92. Bricker D, Squires J. Ages & Stages Questionnaires (ASQ). Baltimore,MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co; 1995

93. Capute A. The Capute Scales. CAT-CLAMS. Instruction Manual. Balti-more, MD: Kennedy Fellows Association; 1996

94. Glutting J, Adams W, Sheslow D. Wide Range Intelligence Test. Wil-mington, DE: Jastek Associates; 2000

95. Msall M, DiGaudio K, Rogers BT, et al. The Functional IndependenceMeasure for Children (WeeFIM): Conceptual basis and pilot use inchildren with developmental disabilities. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 1994;33:421–430

96. Sparrow SS, Balla DA, Cicchetti DV. Vineland Adaptive Behavior ScalesInterview Edition: Survey Form Manual. A revision of the Vineland Social

Maturity Scale by E. A. Doll. Circle Pines, MN: American GuidanceService; 1984

97. Newborg JSJ, Guidubaldi J, Stock JR, Wnek L. Battelle DevelopmentalInventory and Recalibrated Technical Data and Norms: Examiner’s Manual.Allen, TX: DLM, LINC Associates; 1984

98. Haley SM, Coster WJ, Ludlow LH, Haltiwanger JT, Andrellos PJ.Pediatric Evaluation of Disability. Nashville, TN: Ellsworth and Vander-meer Press, Ltd; 1997

99. Stein RE, Jessop DJ. Functional status II(R). A measure of child healthstatus. Med Care. 1990;28:1041–1055

100. Fenson L, Dale P, Resnick J, et al. MacArthur Communicative Develop-ment Inventories. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing; 1993

101. Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III. Circle Pines,MN: American Guidance Service; 1997

102. Gardner M. Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. 2000 Ed. Wil-mington, DE: Wide Range, Inc; 2000

103. Brownell R. Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. 2000 Ed. Wil-mington, DE: Wide Range, Inc; 2000

104. Rice M, Wexler K. Rice/Wexler Test of Early Grammatical Impairment. SanAntonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2001

105. Semel E, Wiig EH, Secord WA. Clinical Evaluation of Language Funda-mentals III. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2003

106. Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery [computer pro-gram]. Version 1. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge Cognition;2002

107. Costello A, Edelbrock CS, Costello AJ. Validity of the NIMH Diagnos-tic Interview Schedule: a comparison between psychiatric and pediat-ric referrals. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1985;13:579–595

108. Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS, Irwin JR, Wachtel K, Cicchetti DV. TheBrief Infant-Toddler Social Emotional Assessment: screening for social-emotional problems and delays in competence. J Pediatr Psychol. 2004;29:143–155

109. Achenbach TM. CBCL 2–3. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont,Department of Psychiatry; 1992

110. Achenbach TM. Integrative Guide for the 1991 CBCL-4-18 YSR and TRFProfiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department ofPsychiatry; 1991

111. Connors CK. Conners Teaching Rating Scales Revised. San Antonio, TX:The Psychological Corporation; 1996

112. Connors CK. Conners Parent Rating Scales Revised. San Antonio, TX: ThePsychological Corporation; 1996

113. Kovacs M. The Children’s Depression Inventory. North Tonawanda, NY:Mental Health Systems; 1995

114. Reynolds CR, Kamphaus RW. Behavior Assessment Systems for Children.Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1992

115. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual ofMental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiat-ric Association; 1994

116. Gordon M. The Gordon Diagnostic System. Boulder, CO: GordonSystems; 1983

117. Kirby RS. A parable wrapped in an enigma. Population-based assess-ments of outcomes among high-risk neonates are even less achievablein the age of clinical informatics. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1999;153:789–792

118. Starfield B, Katz H, Gabriel A, et al. Morbidity in childhood—a longi-tudinal view. N Engl J Med. 1984;310:824–829

119. Starfield B, Hankin J, Steinwachs D, et al. Utilization and morbidity:random or tandem? Pediatrics. 1985;75:241–247

120. Ireys HT, Nelson RP. New federal policy for children with specialhealth care needs: implications for pediatricians. Pediatrics. 1992;90:321–327

121. Stein RE, Bauman LJ, Westbrook LE, Coupey SM, Ireys HT. Frame-work for identifying children who have chronic conditions: the case fora new definition. J Pediatr. 1993;122:342–347

122. Palta M, Sadek-Badawi M, Evans M, Weinstein MR, McGuinness G; forthe Newborn Lung Project. Functional assessment of a multicentervery low-birth-weight cohort at 5 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.1999;154:23–29

123. Jackson DJ, Lang JM, Ganiats TG. Epidemiological issues in perinataloutcomes research. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 1999;13:392–404

124. Hack M, Taylor HG, Klein N, Mercuri-Minich N. Functional limita-tions and special health care needs of 10- to 14-year-old childrenweighing less than 750 grams at birth. Pediatrics. 2000;106:554–560

125. Hack M, Flannery DJ, Schluchter M, Cartar E, Borawski E, Klein N.Young adult health and risk taking behavior of very low birth weightchildren (VLBW, �1.5kg) [abstract]. Pediatr Res. 2001;49:312A

1396 FOLLOW-UP CARE OF HIGH-RISK INFANTS by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 21: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

126. Hack M, Flannery DJ, Schluchter M, Cartar L, Borawski E, Klein N.Outcomes in young adulthood for very-low-birth-weight infants.N Engl J Med. 2002;346:149–157

127. Starfield B, Riley AW, Green BF, et al. The adolescent child health andillness profile. A population-based measure of health. Med Care. 1995;33:553–566

128. Stein RE, Westbrook LE, Bauman LJ. The Questionnaire for IdentifyingChildren with Chronic Conditions: a measure based on a noncategori-cal approach. Pediatrics. 1997;99:513–521

129. Starfield B. Child Health and Illness Profile. Adolescent Edition. Balti-more, MD: Johns Hopkins University; 2003

130. Stein RE, Silver EJ. Operationalizing a conceptually based noncategori-cal definition: a first look at US children with chronic conditions. ArchPediatr Adolesc Med. 1999;153:68–74

131. Langgraff J, Ware JE. Child Health Questionnaire. Boston, MA: HealthAct; 2003

132. Gill TM, Feinstein AR. A critical appraisal of the quality of quality-of-life measurements. JAMA. 1994;272:619–626

133. Fekkes M, Theunissen NC, Brugman E, et al. Development and psy-chometric evaluation of the TAPQOL: a health-related quality of lifeinstrument for 1–5-year-old children. Qual Life Res. 2000;9:961–972

134. Varni JW, Seid M, Rode CA. The PedsQL: measurement model for thepediatric quality of life inventory. Med Care. 1999;37:126–139

135. Rosenbaum P, Saigal S. Quality of life and pharmacoeconomics inclinical trials. In: Spilker B, ed. Measuring Health-Related Quality of Lifein Pediatric Populations: Conceptual Issues. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, PA:Lippincott-Raven Publishers; 1996:785–791

136. Eiser C, Morse R. Can parents rate their child’s health-related qualityof life? Results of a systematic review. Qual Life Res. 2001;10:347–357

137. Saigal S, Rosenbaum PL, Feeny D, et al. Parental perspectives of thehealth status and health-related quality of life of teen-aged childrenwho were extremely low birth weight and term controls. Pediatrics.2000;105:569–574

138. Theunissen N, Veen S, Fekkes M, et al. Quality of life in preschoolchildren born preterm. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2001;43:460–465

139. Eiser C. Children’s quality of life measures. Arch Dis Child. 1997;77:350–354

140. Eiser C, Mohay H, Morse R. The measurement of quality of life inyoung children. Child Care Health Dev. 2000;26:401–414

141. Juniper EF, Guyatt GH, Feeny DH, Griffith LE, Ferrie PJ. Minimumskills required by children to complete health-related quality of lifeinstruments for asthma: comparison of measurement properties. EurRespir J. 1997;10:2285–2294

142. Jenney ME, Campbell S. Measuring quality of life. Arch Dis Child.1997;77:347–350

143. Verrips GH, Stuifbergen MC, den Ouden AL, et al. Measuring healthstatus using the Health Utilities Index: agreement between raters andbetween modalities of administration. J Clin Epidemiol. 2001;54:475–481

144. Varni JW, Seid M, Kurtin PS. PedsQL 4.0: Reliability and validity of thePediatric Quality of Life Inventory version 4.0 generic core scales inhealthy and patient populations. Med Care. 2001;39:800–812

145. Vogels T, Verrips GH, Verloove-Vanhorick SP, et al. Measuring health-related quality of life in children: the development of the TACQOLparent form. Qual Life Res. 1998;7:457–465

146. Saigal S, Stoskopf B, Rosenbaum PL, Hoult LA, Furlong WJ, Feeny DH.Development of a multiattribute pre-school health status classificationsystem [abstract]. Pediatr Res. 1998;43:228A

147. Feeny D, Furlong W, Barr RD, Torrance GW, Rosenbaum P, WeitzmanS. A comprehensive multiattribute system for classifying the healthstatus of survivors of childhood cancer. J Clin Oncol. 1992;10:923–928

148. Torrance GW, Furlong W, Feeny D, Boyle M. Multi-attribute prefer-ence functions. Health Utilities Index. Pharmacoeconomics. 1995;7:503–520

149. Landry SH, Denson SE, Swank PR. Effects of medical risk and socio-economic status on the rate of change in cognitive and social devel-

opment for low birth weight children. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1997;19:261–274

150. Gross SJ, Slagle TA, D’Eugenio DB, Mettelman BB. Impact of amatched term control group on interpretation of developmental per-formance in preterm infants. Pediatrics. 1992;90:681–687

151. Doyle LW, Cheung MM, Ford GW, Olinsky A, Davis NM, Callanan C.Birth weight �1501 g and respiratory health at age 14. Arch Dis Child.2001;84:40–44

152. Buck GM, Msall ME, Schisterman EF, Lyon NR, Rogers BT. Extremeprematurity and school outcomes. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2000;14:324–331

153. Bylund B, Cervin T, Finnstrom O, et al. Very low-birth-weight childrenat 9 years: school performance and behavior in relation to risk factors.Prenat Neonatal Med. 2002;5:124–133

154. Gross SJ, Mettelman BB, Dye TD, Slagle TA. Impact of family structureand stability on academic outcome in preterm children at 10 years ofage. J Pediatr. 2001;138:169–175

155. Hall A, McLeod A, Counsell C, Thomson L, Mutch L. School attain-ment, cognitive ability and motor function in a total Scottish very-low-birthweight population at eight years: a controlled study. Dev MedChild Neurol. 1995;37:1037–1050

156. Saigal S, Szatmari P, Rosenbaum P, Campbell D, King S. Cognitiveabilities and school performance of extremely low birth weight chil-dren and matched term control children at age 8 years: a regionalstudy. J Pediatr. 1991;118:751–760

157. Pharoah PO, Stevenson CJ, Cooke RW, Stevenson RC. Prevalence ofbehaviour disorders in low birthweight infants. Arch Dis Child. 1994;70:271–274

158. Hack M, Breslau N, Aram D, Weissman B, Klein N, Borawski-Clark E.The effect of very low birth weight and social risk on neurocognitiveabilities at school age. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1992;13:412–420

159. Hack M, Taylor HG, Klein N, Eiben R, Schatschneider C, Mercuri-Minich N. School-age outcomes in children with birth weights under750 g. N Engl J Med. 1994;331:753–759

160. O’Callaghan MJ, Burns YR, Gray PH, et al. School performance ofELBW children: a controlled study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1996;38:917–926

161. Saigal S, Szatmari P, Rosenbaum P. Can learning disabilities in chil-dren who were extremely low birth weight be identified at schoolentry? J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1992;13:356–362

162. Avchen RN, Scott KG, Mason CA. Birth weight and school-agedisabilities: a population-based study. Am J Epidemiol. 2001;154:895–901

163. Resnick MB, Gomatam SV, Carter RL, et al. Educational disabilities ofneonatal intensive care graduates. Pediatrics. 1998;102:308–314

164. Seidman DS, Paz I, Stevenson DK, Laor A, Danon YL, Gale R. Neonatalhyperbilirubinemia and physical and cognitive performance at 17years of age. Pediatrics. 1991;88:828–833

165. Erickson A, Kallen B. Very low birthweight boys at the age of 19. ArchDis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1998;78:F171–F174

166. Aylward GP, Hatcher RP, Stripp B, Gustafson NF, Leavitt LA. Whogoes and who stays: subject loss in a multicenter, longitudinal fol-low-up study. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1985;6:3–8

167. Wolke D, Ratschinski G, Ohrt B, Riegel K. The cognitive outcome ofvery preterm infants may be poorer than often reported: an empiricalinvestigation of how methodological issues make a big difference. EurJ Pediatr. 1994;153:906–915

168. Broyles RS, Tyson JE, Heyne ET, et al. Comprehensive follow-up careand life-threatening illnesses among high-risk infants: a randomizedcontrolled trial. JAMA. 2000;284:2070–2076

169. Bednarek FJ, Weisberger S, Richardson DK, Frantz ID 3rd, Shah B,Rubin LP. Variations in blood transfusions among newborn intensivecare units. SNAP II Study Group. J Pediatr. 1998;133:601–607

170. Synnes AR, Chien LY, Peliowski A, Baboolal R, Lee SK. Variations inintraventricular hemorrhage incidence rates among Canadian neonatalintensive care units. J Pediatr. 2001;138:525–531

SUPPLEMENT 1397 by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 22: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

DOI: 10.1542/peds.2004-08662004;114;1377Pediatrics 

Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants

ServicesUpdated Information &

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/114/Supplement_5/1377including high resolution figures, can be found at:

References

#BIBLhttp://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/114/Supplement_5/1377This article cites 116 articles, 40 of which you can access for free at:

Subspecialty Collections

subhttp://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/fetus:newborn_infant_Fetus/Newborn Infantfollowing collection(s): This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the

Permissions & Licensing

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtmlin its entirety can be found online at: Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or

Reprintshttp://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtmlInformation about ordering reprints can be found online:

by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 23: Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants · Risk Factors for Preterm (

DOI: 10.1542/peds.2004-08662004;114;1377Pediatrics 

Follow-up Care of High-Risk Infants

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/114/Supplement_5/1377located on the World Wide Web at:

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is

1073-0397. ISSN:60007. Copyright © 2004 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print

the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois,has been published continuously since 1948. Pediatrics is owned, published, and trademarked by Pediatrics is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it

by guest on February 12, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from