exploring the relationships among attachment, … · aline rodrigues department of applied...

159
EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, EMOTION REGULATION, DIFFERENTIATION OF SELF, NEGATIVE PROBLEM ORIENTATION, SELF-ESTEEM, WORRY AND GENERALIZED ANXIETY by Aline Rodrigues A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development Ontario Institute for Studies in Education University of Toronto © Copyright by Aline Rodrigues 2016

Upload: others

Post on 25-May-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, EMOTION

REGULATION, DIFFERENTIATION OF SELF, NEGATIVE PROBLEM ORIENTATION,

SELF-ESTEEM, WORRY AND GENERALIZED ANXIETY

by

Aline Rodrigues

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements

for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development

Ontario Institute for Studies in Education

University of Toronto

© Copyright by Aline Rodrigues 2016

Page 2: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

ii

EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, EMOTION

REGULATION, DIFFERENTIATION OF SELF, NEGATIVE PROBLEM

ORIENTATION, SELF-ESTEEM, WORRY AND GENERALIZED ANXIETY

Doctor of Philosophy (2016)

Aline Rodrigues

Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development

University of Toronto

ABSTRACT

This study attempted to provide preliminary empirical support for an emerging model of

generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) stemming from an emotion-focused therapy (EFT)

perspective, which views GAD as reflecting negative self-organizations and emotion

dysregulation as a function of attachment injuries (Watson & Greenberg, in press). It has done so

by examining negative early attachment experiences and attachment styles, as representing

attachment injuries; differentiation of self, negative problem orientation, and self-esteem, as

representing negative self-organization; and emotion dysregulation, and by investigating the

relationships among these variables and their contribution to generalized anxiety and worry.

Following on suggestion of previous research (Ruscio, 2002; Ruscio & Borkovec, 2004; Ruscio

et al., 2001) indicating that worry, while characteristic of GAD, exists independently of GAD

and therefore would benefit from being studied in its own right, this study examined worry and

generalized anxiety separately. Cross-sectional data was collected from 218 participants who

completed self-report questionnaires assessing demographics and variables of interest. Results

indicated that negative early attachment experiences are significantly and positively related to

generalized anxiety and worry. Findings from correlational analysis demonstrated that negative

early experiences with fathers were associated with worry in adulthood whereas negative early

experiences with both parents as well as perceptions of unfair family interactions were related to

Page 3: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

iii

generalized anxiety symptoms. Multiple mediation analysis using PROCESS (Hayes, 2012)

revealed a different pattern of mediation for insecure adult attachment and worry than for

insecure adult attachment and generalized anxiety. Negative problem orientation and

differentiation of self mediated the relationship between insecure adult attachment and worry,

whereas negative problem orientation and self-esteem mediated the relationship between

insecure adult attachment and generalized anxiety. Implications of these findings for the

understanding of GAD are discussed.

Page 4: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

iv

Table of Contents

List of Tables ................................................................................................................................ vii

List of Figures ................................................................................................................................ ix

Chapter 1: Literature Review .......................................................................................................... 1

Introduction ..................................................................................................................................... 1

GAD ........................................................................................................................................... 5

Worry ......................................................................................................................................... 7

Theoretical Perspectives on GAD and Worry .............................................................................. 12

The Avoidance Model of Worry .............................................................................................. 13

Intolerance of Uncertainty Model ............................................................................................ 19

Metacognition Model of Worry ............................................................................................... 20

The Emotion Dysregulation Model of Worry .......................................................................... 22

A New Formulation of GAD and Worry: The Emotion-focused Model ...................................... 25

Attachment Perspective on Worry ................................................................................................ 28

From Insecure Attachment to GAD and Worry: The Roles of Negative Self-organizations and

Emotion Dysregulation ................................................................................................................. 50

Negative Self-organizations ..................................................................................................... 51

Self-esteem ............................................................................................................................ 52

Negative Problem Orientation ............................................................................................... 55

Differentiation of Self ............................................................................................................ 59

Page 5: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

v

Emotion Dysregulation ............................................................................................................ 62

The Current Study ......................................................................................................................... 69

Hypothesis .............................................................................................................................. 701

Chapter 2: Method ...................................................................................................................... 711

Participants ............................................................................................................................. 713

Measure Assessing Demographic Information. ..................................................................... 733

Demographics Questionnaire .............................................................................................. 733

Measures Assessing Attachment Experiences ....................................................................... 733

Egna Minuen Betraffande Uppfostram – Short Form (s-EMBU, Swedish acronym for My

Memories of Upbringing; Arrindell et al., 1999) ................................................................ 733

Filial Responsibility Scale – Adult (FRS-A; Jurkovic, Thirkield, Morrell, 2001) .............. 744

Three-category Attachment Measure (Hazan & Shaver, 1987) .......................................... 755

Measures Assessing Self-organization ................................................................................... 755

Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965) .................................................... 755

Negative Problem Orientation Questionnaire (NPOQ; Robichaud & Dugas, 2005) .......... 766

Differentiation of Self Inventory - Revised (DSI-R; Skowron & Schmitt, 2003) ............... 766

Measures Assessing Emotion Regulation .............................................................................. 777

Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004) ....................... 777

Measures Assessing GAD and Worry .................................................................................... 788

Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire-IV (GAD-Q-IV; Newman et al., 2002) ....... 788

Page 6: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

vi

Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec, 1990) ... 799

Procedure ................................................................................................................................ 799

Chapter 3: Results ....................................................................................................................... 811

Data Analysis ......................................................................................................................... 811

Preliminary Analysis .............................................................................................................. 833

Analysis for Hypothesis 1 ........................................................................................................ 86

Analysis for Hypothesis 2 ........................................................................................................ 87

Analysis for Hypothesis 3 ...................................................................................................... 899

Analysis for Hypothesis 4 ........................................................................................................ 91

Chapter 4: Discussion ................................................................................................................. 977

Limitations and Future Research ........................................................................................... 109

Conclusion .............................................................................................................................. 111

References ................................................................................................................................. 1144

Appendice ............................................................................................................................... 14444

Appendix A – Informed Consent ....................................................................................... 14545

Appendix B – Demographics Questionnaire ........................................................................ 1466

Appendix C – Results of Reverse Mediation Analyses ....................................................... 1467

Page 7: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

vii

List of Tables

Table 1: Demographic Data for Sample ...................................................................................... 72

Table 2: Distribution of Adult Attachment Styles as a Function of Worry Level and Generalized

Anxiety .......................................................................................................................... 82

Table 3: Means, Standard Deviations and Group Differences for Memories of Upbringing, Filial

Responsibility, Emotion Dysregulation, Self-esteem, Negative Problem Orientation,

Differentiation of self, Worry, and Generalized Anxiety .............................................. 84

Table 4: Bivariate Correlations Between Memories of Upbringing, Filial Responsibility, Emotion

Dysregulation, Self-esteem, Negative Problem Orientation, Differentiation of self,

Worry, and Generalized Anxiety .................................................................................... 85

Table 5: Regression Coefficients for Predicting Generalized Anxiety from Negative Early

Attachment experiences .................................................................................................. 87

Table 6: Regression Coefficients for Predicting Worry from Negative Early Attachment

experiences ..................................................................................................................... 87

Table 7: Regression Coefficients for Predicting Adult Attachment from Negative Early

Attachment experiences .................................................................................................. 88

Table 8: Regression Coefficients for Predicting Emotion Regulation from Negative Early

Attachment experiences .................................................................................................. 90

Table 9: Regression Coefficients for Predicting Self-esteem from Negative Early Attachment

experiences ..................................................................................................................... 90

Table 10: Regression Coefficients for Predicting Negative Problem Orientation from Negative

Early Attachment experiences ........................................................................................ 91

Page 8: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

viii

Table 11: Regression Coefficients for Predicting Differentiation of Self from Negative Early

Attachment experiences .................................................................................................. 91

Table 12: Mediation of the Effect of Insecure Adult Attachment on Worry through Emotion

Regulation, Self-esteem, Negative Problem Orientation, and Differentiation of Self ... 93

Table 13: Mediation of the Effect of Insecure Adult Attachment on Generalized Anxiety through

Emotion Regulation, Self-esteem, Negative Problem Orientation, and Differentiation of

Self .................................................................................................................................. 93

Table 14: Mediation of the Effect of Insecure Adult Attachment on Emotion Regulation, Self-

esteem, Negative Problem Orientation, and Differentiation of Self through Worry .... 145

Table 13: Mediation of the Effect of Insecure Adult Attachment on Emotion Regulation, Self-

esteem, Negative Problem Orientation, and Differentiation of Self through Generalized

Anxiety ......................................................................................................................... 146

Page 9: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

ix

List of Figures

Figure 1: The EFT Model of GAD ............................................................................................... 26

Figure 2: Process Model of Affect Regulation in Anxious Attachment (Rodrigues & Watson,

2011) ............................................................................................................................... 64

Figure 3: Process Model of Affect Regulation in Avoidant Attachment (Rodrigues & Watson,

2011) ............................................................................................................................... 65

Figure 4: Mediation Model of Insecure Adult Attachment and Worry through Emotion

Regulation, Self-Esteem, Negative Problem Orientation, and Differentiation of Self ... 95

Figure 5: Mediation Model of Insecure Adult Attachment and Generalized Anxiety through

Emotion Regulation, Self-Esteem, Negative Problem Orientation, and Differentiation of

Self ................................................................................................................................. 95

Page 10: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

1

Chapter 1: Literature Review

Introduction

Since the publication of the revision of the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical

Manual of Mental Disorders (i.e., DSM-III-R; American Psychological Association [APA],

1987) in which the status of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) changed from that of a residual

category to that of a discrete diagnostic category, the psychological literature on GAD has grown

substantially (Heimberg, Turk, & Mennin, 2004). While still a relatively new diagnosis, GAD is

now recognized as a widely prevalent disorder associated with significant functional impairment

and high utilization of mental health services (Hoffman, Dukes, Wittchen, 2008; Kessler,

Walters, Wittchen, 2004). A growing body of empirical studies and other theoretical papers have

now emerged and have begun to create a more stable foundation of theory and knowledge

regarding the nature of this disorder (Howa & Antony, 2008). Much of this work has originated

within the cognitive school of psychology and has focused on the study of worry, which is an

essential feature of GAD (Aikins & Craske, 2001; Behar, DiMarco, Hekler, Mohlman, Staples,

2009).

Several cognitive models of worry have been proposed in the past few years and have

greatly advanced the understanding of GAD (Borkovec, Alcaine, & Behar, 2004; Dugas &

Robichaud 2007; Mennin, 2004; Newman & Llera 2011; Wells 2006). For the most part, these

models have converged on the notion that worry is a strategy used to regulate individuals away

from emotional distress but which directly contributes to anxiety. These models have identified a

number of faulty cognitive processes associated with worry, such as negative beliefs about

problems-solving abilities (Davey, 1994; Dugas, Freeston, & Ladouceur, 1997), metacognitive

Page 11: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

2

beliefs about the detrimental nature of worry (i.e., worrying about worry; Wells, 2006), and

intolerance of uncertainty (Dugas & Robichaud, 2007). Previous research has also highlighted

different emotion regulation deficits that help explain the use of worry as a strategy for coping

with emotional distress, such as difficulty experiencing and understanding intense emotions

(Mennin, Heimberg, Turk, & Fresco, 2002, 2005; Newman & Llera, 2011, 2014). In addition,

research has identified interpersonal deficits among chronic worriers, such as higher levels of

rigidity and distress in interpersonal relationships as compared to non-anxious individuals

(Newman, 2000; Roemer, Orsillo, & Barlow, 2002).

Empirical attention has been largely dedicated to the study of dysfunctional beliefs and

emotion and interpersonal deficits outlined in separate existing models of worry. However,

attempts have been made to integrate cognitive, emotion, and interpersonal correlates of worry

across models in efforts of improving the understanding and, most importantly, treatment of

GAD (Newman, Castonguay, Borkovec, & Molnar, 2004; Roemer & Orsillo, 2014). In fact,

some researchers have pointed out that more integrative as well as non-cognitive-behavioral

theoretical frameworks are needed to inform health care policy on the most appropriate forms of

psychological interventions in treating GAD (Cassidy, 2005, 2009; Hunot, Churchil, Teixeira,

Silva de Lima, 2007; Rapee, 1991).

One such integrative model of worry and GAD is the emerging work by Watson and

colleagues (Watson & Goldman, 2012; Watson & Greenberg, 2015, in press; Watson, 2015).

These investigators have recently developed an elegant, comprehensive theory of the protective

role of worry in GAD from an emotion-focused perspective. The authors propose that worry

represents a misguided attempt of the individual to protect his or her negative self-organization

Page 12: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

3

against overwhelming distress resulting from the activation of painful maladaptive emotion

schemes based on early insecure attachment experiences.

Watson and colleagues (Watson & Greenberg, 2012; Watson & Greenberg, 2015, in

press; Watson, 2015) place the concepts of negative self-organization and emotion dysregulation

at the heart of GAD and worry. Negative self-organization refers to individuals’ experience of

themselves as vulnerable, weak and at risk in their interactions with others and the world.

Emotion dysregulation refers to individuals’ inability to self-sooth or access adequate protection

or succor to regulate their emotions as they feel distressed. Both negative self-organizations and

emotion dysregulation are believed to originate from repeated early attachment injuries. These

injuries are viewed as leading to the development of maladaptive emotion schemes which

construct the self in a vulnerable fashion, thus cuing feelings of distress when activated. In light

of a vulnerable self-organization and in the absence of adequate emotion regulation capacities,

this distress overwhelms the individual who then relies on worry to protect the self from

shattering in the face of emotional pain.

Given its recent formulation, no empirical study has yet directly investigated the

propositions of the emotion-focused perspective on worry. However, because this perspective

shares some attributes with existing models of worry, such as seeing worry as an effort to cope

with difficult emotions, it has found indirect support from research findings showing that people

diagnosed with GAD and chronic worry have difficulty regulating emotions (Craske & Waters,

2005; Mennin et al., 2002, 2005). In addition, this perspective has found to be consistent with

other findings from attachment research highlighting the associations of GAD and chronic worry

with insecure attachment (Cassidy 1995, 2009). That said, no study to date has proposed to

examine the concepts emphasized in the emotion-focused formulation of worry and GAD in a

Page 13: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

4

more direct way, such as investigating the relationships among insecure attachment experiences,

negative self-organizations, and impaired emotion regulation among individuals with GAD.

The main purpose of this dissertation is to study the ideas articulated by the emotion-

focused perspective on GAD and worry. In order to do that, this study operationalized some of

the concepts of this perspective by focusing on early attachment experiences and attachment

styles as reflecting aspects of emotion schematic memories and attachment injuries; low

differentiation of self, low self-esteem, and negative problem orientation as reflecting aspects of

a negative self-organization and vulnerable sense of self; and affect dysregulation, and it sought

to examine the relationships among these variables and their contributions to both GAD and

worry. This study hypothesized that the concepts reflecting a negative self organization (i.e., low

differentiation of self, low self-esteem, and negative problem orientation) and emotion

dyregulation would mediate the relationships between insecure attachment and worry as well as

insecure attachment and GAD.

It is important to comment here that this study examined worry and GAD separately. The

reason for this comes from a growing body of research demonstrating that excessive worry,

while characteristic of GAD, also exists independently of GAD (Hirsch, Mathews, Lequertier,

Perman, & Hayes, 2013; Ruscio, 2002; Ruscio & Borkovec, 2004; Ruscio et al., 2001). These

authors argue that the nature of worry outside the context of GAD is still poorly understood and

understudied. They recommend that worry be studied in its own right so that researchers can

have a clearer picture of the features that distinguish the GAD presentation of worry and anxiety

from normal variants of these processes. As such, this dissertation examined GAD and worry

apart and tried to understand the factors that contribute to each independently and to both jointly.

Page 14: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

5

In the next sections a literature review on GAD and worry is provided. This review

presents the definition of worry and prevailing theoretical models of worry and GAD. Following

this review, an argument for the role of attachment injuries on the development of worry and

GAD is presented. Next, a review of psychological constructs reflecting aspects of a negative

self-organization (i.e., low differentiation of self, low self-esteem, and negative problem

orientation) as well as emotion dysregulation is provided and the link between these constructs

and insecure attachment presented. The hypotheses for this study are presented thereafter.

Findings from this study are presented and discussed from an emotion-focused perspective on

worry and GAD.

GAD

GAD is characterized by excessive and uncontrollable worry occurring more days than

not for at least six months (APA, 2013). The most recent edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical

Manual of Mental Disorders (i.e., DSM-5) adds that worrying in GAD is also pervasive – the

worry is about a number of events and activities – and it is associated with at least three of the

following symptoms: restlessness, fatigue, difficulty concentrating, irritability, muscle tension,

and sleep disturbance. Moreover, worrying in GAD is not better explained by another mental

health disorder or attributable to a medical condition or the physiological effects of a substance.

Additionally, the worrying in GAD leads to significant subjective distress and related

impairments in social, occupational, and other important areas of the individual’s functioning

(APA, 2013).

Epidemiological studies have placed the one-year prevalence of GAD in the Canadian

population at 1.1% (Public Health Agency of Canada, 2002). GAD has been associated with

long-term impairment, interpersonal difficulties, and health concerns (Waters & Craske, 2005). It

Page 15: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

6

is also highly comorbid with major depression and other anxiety disorders, such as panic

disorder and social phobia (Kessler, Walters, & Wittchen, 2004). This comorbidity is in turn

associated with greater functional impairment and health care use and cost (Hoffman et al., 2008;

Souetre, Lozet, Cimarosti, Martin, Chignon, Ades, et al., 1994). Stein and Heimberg (2004) have

found that GAD is associated with interpersonal problems even after the overlap with depressive

symptoms has been statistically controlled. GAD has been identified as one of the most difficult

disorders to understand and to treat, remaining one of the least successfully treated anxiety

disorders (Brown, Barlow, & Liebowitz, 1994; Roemer, Orsillo, Barlow, 2002; Waters &

Craske, 2005; Weston & Morrison, 2001).

Research has shown that about half of GAD patients receiving a course of cognitive-

behavioural therapy – the standard treatment for anxiety disorders – maintain high end-state

functioning at one-year follow-up (Borkovec, Newman, Pincus, & Lytle, 2002; Hanrahan, Field,

Jones, & Davey, 2013). One of the greatest difficulties in the treatment of GAD, relative to other

anxiety disorders, is believed to lie in the fact that worrying in GAD cannot be reliably tied to a

specific target of fear and anxiety (Roemer & Orsillo, 2005). Borkovec and colleagues

(Borkovec et al., 2002) argue that because the content of worry in GAD is diffuse, ever-

changing, and based on future-oriented apprehension in the absence of specific threat, it does not

lend itself as well to exposure therapy as other anxiety disorders. In addition, because worry in

GAD covers a wide-range of topics, it does not subject itself well to traditional cognitive

techniques, such as cognitive restructuring (Roemer & Orsillo, 2005; Mennin et al., 2004).

Although GAD is conceptualized as a discrete disorder whose symptoms are either

present or absent, Kertz and colleagues (Kertz et al., 2014) argue that it is best understood as a

dimensional construct. In their recent study investigating the latent structure of both worry and

Page 16: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

7

generalized anxiety in a diagnostically diverse clinical sample and using three taxometric

procedures, they found that a dimensional rather than categorical structure for both constructs

provided the best fit for their data. This finding indicates that ratings of worry or symptom

severity may be more useful in addressing worry and GAD. The authors add that their finding

has important implications for conceptualization models of worry and GAD because dimensional

constructs are “best explained by additive models in which various factors interact to explain

increasing or decreasing symptom severity along a continuum” (p. 13). In later sections, potential

explanatory factors of excessive worry will be discussed.

Worry

Much of the theory and research on GAD has focused on one of its essential features,

worry. Borkovec and colleagues (Borkovec, Robison, Pruzinsky, & Depree, 1983) were one of

the first researchers to study worry in its own right (independent from anxiety) and to provide a

formal definition of worry. They defined worry as “a chain of thoughts and images, negatively

affect-laden and relatively uncontrollable [which] represents attempts by the individual to engage

in mental problem-solving on issues whose outcomes are uncertain” (p.10). Borkovec (1994) has

extended this definition by stating that worry is predominantly a thought activity and is primarily

verbal-linguistic in nature. Several researchers have found support for that position, noting that

individuals report that their worries are predominantly composed of thoughts as compared to

images (Freeston, Dugas, & Ladouceur, 1996; Tallis, Davey, & Capuzzo, 1994). Freeston and

colleagues (Freeston et al., 1996) found in a sample of 502 individuals that worry consists of

approximately 70% thoughts and 30% images. They also found that excessive worriers reported

a greater percentage of thoughts in their worries in comparison to ordinary worriers. Borkovec

Page 17: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

8

and colleagues (Borkovec, 1994; Borkovec, Ray, & Stöber, 1998) explain that when individuals

worry, they talk to themselves about negative things that they are afraid might happen in the

future and verbally consider ways to cope with those situations. Worry thus appears to consist of

mostly verbal thought activity centered on an anxious apprehension of negative future outcomes

(Borkovec et al., 2004; Barlow, 2002).

Researchers (Borkovec et al., 2004; Stöber, 1998; Stöber, Tepperwien, & Staak, 2000;

Tucker & Newman, 1981) argue that worry involves the processing of anxiety-provoking

information at a conceptual but also abstract level. Borkovec (1985) explains that worry consists

largely of “what if” statements representing attempts at problem-solving which are not

necessarily accompanied by conceptual elaboration or further action on how to actually solve

problems. Stöber and colleagues (1998, 2000; Stöber & Borkovec, 2002) state that worrisome

thoughts are uncertain and unclear in nature, consisting of words and sentences characterized by

reduced concreteness. They argue that fearful imagery is indeed associated with worrisome

processes; however, because worrisome thoughts are verbally abstract, the fearful imagery

associated with them are reduced and less vivid. Borkovec and colleagues (Borkovec et al.,

2004; Borkovec et al., 1998) suggest that this is one of the main mechanisms by which worry is

reinforced and maintained. They argue that while on the one hand worrisome thoughts can prime

images of feared events, their lack of verbal concreteness allows individuals to avoid activation

of more vivid and thus emotionally distressing imagery.

Empirical evidence has shown that high levels of worry is also associated with

information processing biases (Cassidy, 1995; Hayes & Hirsch, 2007; MacLeod & Rutherford,

2004). Specifically, research on individuals with GAD has revealed that high worry is related to

attentional bias favoring encoding of threatening stimuli and to interpretative bias favoring

Page 18: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

9

threatening meaning of ambiguous information (Hayes & Hirsch, 2007). One common method

for examining attentional bias is the “Emotional Stroop Task” where individuals are presented

with words with different emotional valence (e.g., threatening words and non-threatening

words), displayed in different colors, and are required to name the colour of each word while

ignoring its content. Studies have found that GAD individuals are significantly slower to colour-

name threat related words, suggesting that their attention gets disrupted by the threatening

content of words (Hayes & Hirsch, 2007; MacLeod & Rutherford, 2004; Oates, Siegle, & Ray,

2011).

Another common task for assessing attentional bias is the “Dot-probe Task” in which

individuals are simultaneously shown on a computer screen pairs of words (e.g., one threatening

word and one neutral word) followed by a blank screen with a dot in the previous location of one

of the words (Hayes & Hirsch, 2007). Studies have found that GAD individuals have faster

reaction times than normal controls to dots that replaced threatening versus neutral words (Hayes

& Hirsch, 2007). This finding suggest that GAD individuals preferentially attend to threatening

stimuli, consistent with an attentional vigilance for threat. With respect to studies examining

interpretative bias on worry, it has been found that when individuals listen to threat-related and

neutral homophones (e.g., “slay/sleigh”) or to ambiguous sentences that could be interpreted in

either a threatening or non-threatening manner, GAD individuals are more likely than non-

anxious controls to produce threat-related spelling of homophones and to endorse threat

interpretations to ambiguous sentence (Cassidy, 1995; Hayes & Hirsch, 2007; MacLeod &

Rutherford, 2004). These findings are consistent with a threatening interpretation bias.

Recently, attempts have been made to elucidate the latent structure of worry. A growing

body of research (Kertz, McHugh, Lee, & Bjorgvinsson, 2014; Olatunji, Broman-Fulks,

Page 19: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

10

Bergman, Green, & Zlomke, 2010; Ruscio, Borkovec, & Ruscio, 2001) suggests that worry

involves a dimensional structure in which normal and pathological worry fall on opposite ends of

the same continuum. In two recent taxometric studies in a community sample of 1507

individuals and a college sample of 1220 students, Olatunji and colleagues (Olatunji et al., 2010)

found that the heterogeneity of worry (i.e., normal versus pathological) is best understood as

reflecting quantitative rather than qualitative differences among individuals. After examining

evidence from three taxometric methods, they concluded that worry does not demarcate a

discrete taxon but rather occurs to a greater or lesser extent in all people (Olatunji et al., 2010).

Worry thus reflects a normal human experience – aimed at mobilizing problem solving – which

can turn into a disturbance when it becomes excessive, uncontrollable and chronically present

(Borkovec et al., 1998).

Other empirical research on the occurrence and phenomenology of worry has focused on

comparing the frequency and content of worry between individuals with GAD and non-GAD

individuals (Holaway, Rodebaugh, & Heimberg, 2006). Studies have found that there are

similarities between normal worry and worry associated with GAD. For instance, the content of

worry appears to be similar across individuals, such that both GAD individuals and controls

identify family and interpersonal issues as common worry topics (Borkovec, Shadick & Hopkins,

1991; Kertz et al., 2014; Roemer, Molina, & Borkovec, 1997). In addition, duration of worry

episodes, aversion to worry, anxiety associated with resisting worry, and perceived likelihood of

worrisome outcomes have not been found to differ significantly between individuals with GAD

and non-GAD individuals (Craske, Rapee, Jackel, & Barlow, 1989). Despite these similarities,

however, these same studies and others have found that a few distinctions can be made between

normal and pathological worry. For the most part, pathological worry can be defined as

Page 20: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

11

excessive, pervasive, more unrealistic, and more uncontrollable compared to normal worry

(Mennin, Heimberg, & Turk, 2004). Research findings indicate that individuals with GAD worry

more frequently (Craske et al., 1989; Hirsh, Mathews, Lequertier, Perman, & Hayes, 2013),

worry about a wide range of topics as well as more miscellaneous topics (e.g., minor matters,

punctuality, home repairs; Borkovec et al., 1991; Hirsh et al., 2013), and find their worries more

difficult to control (Craske et al., 1989; Hirsh et al., 2013) than nonanxious individuals.

Research has also attempted to investigate excessive worry outside the context of GAD

(Hirsch, Mathews, Lequertier, Perman, & Hayes, 2013; Ruscio, 2002; Ruscio & Borkovec, 2004;

Ruscio et al., 2001). For instance, Hirsch and colleagues (Hirsch et al., 2013) recently compared

high worriers with and without a diagnosis of GAD and found that high worriers without GAD

reported less negative beliefs about worry and had a greater ability to control the intrusion of

distressing thoughts during a behavioural worry test than did high worriers meeting GAD

diagnostic criteria. In addition, they found that high worriers without a GAD diagnosis reported a

narrower range of worry topics and lower levels of anxious and depressed mood than did

individuals with a GAD diagnosis.

These findings are similar to earlier work by Ruscio and colleagues (Ruscio, 2002;

Ruscio & Borkovec, 2004; Ruscio et al., 2001) who found that high worriers without GAD

endorsed all symptoms of GAD but in lesser severity than individuals meeting GAD criteria,

even though they also reported their worry to be excessive and uncontrollable. Holaway and

colleagues (Holaway et al., 2006) suggest that worry within the context of GAD may encompass

additional factors to worry, such as different beliefs about worry and increased emotion

dysregulation, which may help differentiate it from high worry without GAD. The authors state,

Page 21: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

12

however, that the nature of high worry outside the context of GAD is still understudied and more

research examining high worry independently of GAD is warranted.

Worry is also prevalent across several psychological disorders other than GAD, and

hence is considered by many as a transdiagnostic construct (e.g., Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, &

Shafran, 2004; Starcevic, Berle, Milicevic, Hannan, Lamplugh, & Eslick, 2007). Many forms of

psychological dysfunction feature worry in their presentation, including depression, obsessive-

compulsive, and anxiety disorders (e.g., social phobia and panic disorder). In some clinical

presentations, worry may be confounded with other similar cognitive activities, such as in the

case of depression where worry and rumination are intimately related due to their shared

perseverative nature and negative content. Similarly, in obsessive-compulsive disorder there may

be an overlap between worry and obsessions, given that both can be experienced as

uncontrollable intrusive thoughts. In most cases, however, worry can be differentiated between

GAD and other anxiety disorders on the basis that worry in anxiety disorders tends to be more

content-specific in contrast to worry in GAD which tends to involve a wider range of topics

(Hirsch et al., 2013; Starcevic, 2010). In addition, worry is typically viewed as an activity

directed towards the future (i.e., anticipated threat) whereas other cognitive processes may be

past-oriented and involve themes of past failures and losses (e.g., rumination; Papageorgiou,

2006) or be more imagery-like and ego-dystonic (e.g., obsessions; Starcevic, 2010).

Theoretical Perspectives on GAD and Worry

Significant advances have been made in the theoretical understanding of GAD and worry

over the past two decades resulting in a number of models regarding the development and

maintenance of GAD and worry (Aikins & Craske, 2001; Behar et al., 2009; Heimberg et al.,

Page 22: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

13

2004; Rowa & Antony, 2008). This section reviews the prevailing models of GAD and worry

found in the literature.

The Avoidance Model of Worry

Borkovec and colleagues (Borkovec, 1994; Borkovec, Alcaine, Behar, 2004; Sibrava &

Borkovec, 2006) argue that worry functions as a cognitive avoidance strategy in response to

perceived threats. According to the authors, worry allows individuals to mentally search for ways

to avoid the occurrence of future perceived catastrophes and thereby avoid aversive images,

autonomic arousal, and negative emotions associated with those threatening perceptions.

Borkovec and colleagues offer a simple, evolutionary reasoning to support their model. They

argue that because the perceived threat does not necessarily exist in the present moment for the

individual, a flight-or-fight response is not available as an effective response to avoid that threat.

Therefore, the individual is left with only mental strategies to avoid or cope with feared events.

While engaged in worry, the individual is able to reduce somatic activation and emotional

processing triggered by the perception of threat and thus negatively reinforce the process of

worrying.

Borkovec and colleagues (Borkovec, 1994; Borkovec et al., 2004; Sibrava & Borkovec,

2006) assert that the immediate suppression of sympathetic reactions and experiencing of

distressing emotions to anxiety-provoking material serve not only to negatively reinforce the

worry process but also to strengthen the anxious meanings that those sympathetic reactions and

distressing emotions have for the individual. Over the long-run, worry thus becomes pervasive

and impairing. Given that access and exposure to threatening material is necessary for extinction

or adaptive emotional processing of fear responses to take place, worry in the long-run interferes

Page 23: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

14

with adaptive processing of distressing emotional material and thereby the individual continues

being confronted with that material in the future. In addition, given that the meaning of

autonomic reactions and distressing emotions get strengthened during the worrying process, the

individual eventually experiences a more intense experience of anxiety as result of worry.

Part of Borkovec and colleagues’ theory (Borkovec, 1994; Borkovec, Alcaine, Behar,

2004; Sibrava & Borkovec, 2006) is based on the emotional processing theory of the

maintenance and extinction of fear by Foa and Kozak (1989) which was based on Lang’s (1977,

1984, 1998) bioinformational model of emotion. Lang’s model is a network model of emotional

structures in memory. It proposes that emotional structures are cognitive representations

containing information about stimuli, responses (i.e., behavioral acts, physiological mobilization,

and expressive language), and meaning information of emotional experiences (Lang, Cuthbert, &

Bradley, 1998). Foa and Kosak (1986) distinguished between adaptive and maladaptive

emotional structures with a particular focus on fear. They noted that in adaptive fear structures,

associations between fear responses and meaning information reflect accurate representations of

reality whereas in maladaptive structures those associations distort reality and include excessive

response elements. They developed a theory of emotional processing in which the maladaptive

associations among stimuli, responses and meaning in fear structures are replaced with more

adaptive associations. They argue that for emotional processing to occur, fear structures need to

be accessed and fully activated (with activation of imagery and physiological arousal) for new

information to then disconfirm erroneous elements in those structures (Foa & Kosak, 1986).

Building on Foa and Kosak’s (1986) theory, Borkovec and colleagues (Borkovec, 1994;

Borkovec et al., 2004; Sibrava & Borkovec, 2006) hypothesized that worry, as an abstract

verbal-linguistic activity, functions to inhibit emotional processing by denying exposure to

Page 24: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

15

aversive image and associated negative emotion and thus preventing individuals from fully

activating their fear structures. The authors note that worry may initially appear effective at

reducing anxious arousal arising from activation of aversive images; however, it comes at the

high cost of adaptive emotional processing and extinction learning.

There are numerous studies that support the Avoidance Model of worry (see Sibrava &

Borkovec, 2006; Borkovec et al., 2004). For example, Butler and colleagues (Butler, Wells, &

Dewick, 1995) had participants watch a highly aversive film of a gruesome industrial accident.

Participants were then instructed to verbally worry about the film, visualize images from the

film, or simply “settle down.” The authors found that immediately after the task participants in

the worry and control conditions experienced significant decreases in anxiety, whereas the

imagery group remained anxious about the film. However, after three days of the study,

participants in the worry condition experienced intrusive thoughts about the film more frequently

than those in either the imagery or control conditions.

In another study examining the effects of worry and trauma recall among nonanxious

individuals and individuals with GAD and posttraumatic stress disorder, Behar and colleagues

(Behar, Zuellig, & Borkovec, 2005) found that worrying preceding an imagery-based trauma

recall exercise lessened the subjective anxiety experienced by anxious participants during the

exercise procedure. However, they found that trauma recall influenced the emotional experience

of subsequent worrying, such that anxious individuals experienced greater anxiety during the

worrying process if worry had preceded a trauma recall exercise. Altogether, these findings

suggest that worry helps individuals avoid anxious arousal and negative affect associated with

negative stimuli; however, the benefits of this avoidance appear to be limited, as worry later on

reinforced the arousal of negative stimuli.

Page 25: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

16

Additional support for the Avoidance Model come from laboratory studies demonstrating

the association between worry and decreased physiological arousal. Several studies have shown

that that anxious individuals who engaged in worrisome thinking prior to presentations of

anxiety-provoking imagery experienced lower cardiovascular activity in response to the imagery

as compared to individuals who engaged in either relaxed or neutral thinking (Bokovec & Hu,

1990; Borkovec, Lyonfields, Wiser, & Diehl, 1993; Pasley-Miklus & Vrana, 2000). Castaneda

and Segerstrom (2004), for instance, have shown that high-worry phobic individuals experienced

reduced physiological reactions to actual presentations of the phobic stimuli in comparison to

low-worry phobics. Earlier observations by Hoehn-Saric and colleagues (Hoehn-Saric &

McLeod, 1988; Hoehn-Saric et al., 1989) have also demonstrated that individuals with GAD

have a restricted range of variability on measures of autonomic arousal such as skin conductance

and heart rate.

In yet another study, Thayer, Friedman, and Borkovec (1996) investigated cardiovascular

responses of individuals with GAD and non-anxious controls during baseline, relaxation, and

worry conditions. These authors found that individuals with GAD demonstrated significantly

lower heart rate variability than non-anxious controls across all three experimental conditions,

suggesting that worry is related to deficits in vagal tone. Overall, these findings suggest that

worry is characterized by sympathetic inhibition and autonomic inflexibility. Borkovec and

colleagues (Borkovec et al., 1995) explain that worry mitigates physiological changes and thus

lessens the perception of somatic activation and the intensity of emotions associated with that

activation. They note that this lowered activation results in the rigidity of the individual’s

autonomic functioning which then compromises their ability to flexibly respond to moment-to-

moment demands in their environment. Friedman (2007) agrees with that position. He adds that

Page 26: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

17

worry ultimately compromises an individual’s ability to organize their physiological responses

and thus generate adaptive behavioral and emotional responses to perceived threatening

situations.

Drawing from Borkovec’s (Borkovec, 1994; Borkovec et al., 2004) conceptualization of

worry as a form of avoidance as well as the above mentioned research on the associations

between worry and decreased physiological arousal, Roemer and Orsillo (2002) have argued that

worry functions primarily to diminish internal distress. They assert that Borkovec’s avoidance

conceptualization of worry manifests through two different mechanisms: (a) superstitious

perceived avoidance of low-probability future negative events (or believing that worry reduces

the likelihood of negative outcomes), and (b) through experiential avoidance, including both

strategic and automatic avoidance of internal distress (e.g., worrying about minor matters to

decrease sense of worthlessness). The authors argue that the most striking form of avoidance in

GAD is experiential in that individuals with the disorder rely on worry strategically and/or

automatically to avoid unpleasant internal experiences. They note, however, that worry itself

eventually becomes an unwanted internal experience, prompting attempts to avoid it, which

paradoxically then increase its frequency. They suggest that approaches that emphasize non-

avoidance of internal experience (e.g., mindfulness techniques) are likely to reduce this negative

spiral that characterizes GAD.

More recently, Newman and Llera (2010; 2011; Llera & Newman, 2014) refuted the

findings of reduced physiological arousal associated with worry and proposed a modified version

of the Avoidance Model, called the Contrast Avoidance Model. In their model, worry is viewed

as a cognitive avoidance strategy in response to negative emotional contrasts rather than negative

emotional arousal per se. Whereas Borkovec and colleagues posit that worry functions as a

Page 27: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

18

means for managing negative emotional experience through the avoidance of internal emotional

arousal, Newman and Llera argue that worry helps anxious individuals maintain a sustained

negative internal arousal in order to then avoid increases in their negative emotions should

negative events take place. For the authors, the avoidance of negative emotional contrasts (from

baseline to negative) is what truly motivates anxious individuals to worry, as these individuals

are theorized to have developed strong aversive reactions to and to be highly sensitive to

negative emotional shifts in their experiences (Newman & Llera, 2011). Thus from this

framework, worry functions as a cognitive rehearsing mechanism that serves to first induce

negative emotional states in order to later facilitate the avoidance of sharp increases in negative

arousal associated with perceptions of future threat.

Newman and Llera (2011) indicate that because research has focused primarily on the

impact of worry on subsequent reactivity to exposure to emotions, it has concluded prematurely

that worry enables emotional avoidance. However, their emerging research on the Contrast

Avoidance Model and the experiential impact of worry itself, have shown that worry, in

comparison to relaxation and neutral strategies, in fact boosted negative emotionality in GAD

analogous and nonanxious individuals (Llera & Newman, 2010, 2014). Only when individuals

were already experiencing negative arousal during worry tasks and were exposed to fear stimuli,

did the authors find similar results to those reported in support of the Avoidance Model

(Newman & Llera, 2014).

In a recent study, Newman and Llera (2014) randomly assigned participants to engage in

worry, brief relaxation, or neutral inductions prior to sequential exposure to emotion-inducing

(e.g., fear and sad) film clips. During worry, the authors found that participants (both with GAD

and nonanxious) reported higher negative affect and marginally higher sympathetic arousal than

Page 28: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

19

did participants assigned to the relaxation and neutral inductions. However, participants in the

relaxation and neutral inductions groups experienced increased negative affect and stronger

sympathetic activation than participants in the worry group in response to subsequent fear and

sad film clips, whereas participants in the worry group did not experience greater contrasts. This

finding suggests that worry may lead to heightened negative emotionality in order to then

prevent further increase in arousal in the event of a stressor. The authors also found that GAD

individuals in comparison to nonanxious individuals reported that preceding worry was more

helpful than relaxation or neutral inductions in facilitating coping with the fearful or sad film

clips. This suggests that participants with GAD were more bothered by the negative emotional

contrasts than nonanxious individuals (Newman & Llera, 2014).

More research is still needed to replicate and confirm the tenets proposed by the

Contrasts Avoidance Model and to identify precisely the avoidant properties of worry, including

inconsistencies across studies on the physiological consequences of worry. By and large,

however, research findings on worry appear to converge on the position that individuals use

worry to avoid feeling vulnerable to negative or threatening experiences by dampening the full

emotional processing of those experiences.

Intolerance of Uncertainty Model

Dugas and colleagues (Koerner & Dugas, 2006; Dugas, Buhr & Ladoucer, 2004; Dugas,

Marchand, & Ladoucer, 2005) have proposed a model of GAD in which worry is largely driven

and maintained by an individual’s tendency to react negatively to uncertain or ambiguous

situations. The authors assert that individuals with GAD find uncertain or ambiguous situations

stressful and upsetting. In addition, these individuals believe that worry can either help them

cope with ambiguous or uncertain situations more effectively or prevent them from occurring

Page 29: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

20

altogether. Dugas and colleagues argue that because many events and situations in life are

characterized by ambiguity and uncertainty, individuals with a high disposition for intolerance of

uncertainty disposition are susceptible to ongoing worry and anxiety.

Dugas and colleagues have found that individuals with GAD or GAD analogues have a

lower threshold for uncertainty compared to nonanxious individuals and individuals with other

anxiety disorders (Dugas et al., 2004, 2005). Specifically, they observed that individuals with

GAD or GAD analogues have a heightened sensitivity to uncertainty-related information and

tend to perceive uncertainty in threatening and unpleasant terms (Dugas et al., 2004, 2005). In a

study using a gambling task to manipulate intolerance of uncertainty, Ladourceur and colleagues

(Ladourceur et al., 2000) demonstrated that increases in intolerance of uncertainty led to

increases in worrying. In another study examining GAD clients’ progress over the course of

cognitive-behavioural therapy revealed that changes in clients’ levels of intolerance of

uncertainty over the course of therapy preceded changes in the amount of worry they reported,

but the reverse relationship (i.e., changes in worry preceding changes in intolerance of

uncertainty) was not found. In yet another study examining the utility of a written exposure

condition relative to a control writing condition, improvements in worry were found to be

associated with preceding improvements in intolerance of uncertainty (Goldman, Dugas, Sexton,

& Gervais, 2007). Overall, there appears to be growing evidence suggesting that intolerance of

uncertainty may play a causal role in the development of worry in GAD.

Metacognition Model of Worry

Wells (1995, 2006) has proposed a metacognitive model of GAD that involves different

beliefs about worry. Wells suggests that there are two distinctive types of worry – Type 1 and

Page 30: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

21

Type 2 worrying. According to Wells, Type 1 worrying involves the positive metacognitive

appraisal of worry as a coping strategy to cope with possible negative outcomes. It consists of a

verbal contemplative process in which chains of catastrophizing and planning responses are

processed in the mind. This form of worry is considered to be common and not necessarily

pathological. Wells argues that by engaging in Type 1 worrying, individuals can contemplate

negative outcomes and begin to generate coping strategies to prepare for stressful situations. He

notes that although anxiety can arise from predicting negative outcomes, it eventually subdues

once coping options are generated in the mind.

Alternatively, Type 2 worrying consists of the negative metacognitive appraisal of worry

as uncontrollable and harmful to the individual. Type 2 worrying is generally activated when

Type 1 worrying has not proved effective and a felt sense that the individual is able to cope with

a particular stressor has not been achieved. In type 2 worrying, the individual believes that

worrying is uncontrollable and/or dangerous for their well-being. In this case, the individual

engages in negative meta-worry (i.e., negative interpretation of worry) in which the anxiety

arising from worrying is negatively interpreted as a sign of loss of control or physical and/or

psychological dysfunction. Wells argues that Type 2 worrying causes the individual to feel

threatened by worry, leading to an increase in anxiety and further worrying.

Studies have demonstrated that nonanxious individuals do not differ from individuals

with GAD in the endorsement of Type 1 worrying, but rather Type 2 worrying appears to be a

distinctive feature of individuals with GAD (Davis & Valentiner, 2000; Wells & Carter, 2001;

Wells & Papageorgiou, 1998). Several other studies have found that individuals with GAD

experience more Type 2 worrying as compared to individuals without a diagnosis of an anxiety

disorder (Cartwright-Hatton & Wells, 1997; Davis & Valentiner, 2000; Wells, 2005; Wells &

Page 31: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

22

Carter, 2001) and individuals with panic disorder (Davis & Valentiner, 2000; Wells & Carter,

2001), social anxiety disorder (Davis & Valentiner, 2000; Wells & Carter, 2001), and depressive

disorder (Cartwright-Hatton & Wells, 1997; Wells & Carter, 2001).

The Emotion Dysregulation Model of Worry

Drawing on theories that highlight the definition and function of worry, Mennin and

colleagues (Mennin et al., 2002, 2005; Mennin, Heimberg, Turk, & Carmin, 2004) developed a

model of worry that complements ideas presented earlier by Borkovec and colleagues as well as

recent formulations by Newman and Llera, and helps explain the reasons why worriers seek to

avoid their emotional experiences or emotional contrasts. Mennin and colleagues argue that

emotion dysregulation is a major feature in the psychopathology of worry such that individuals

worry as a result of their poor emotion regulation skills. According to the authors, individuals

with high worry are more likely to have heightened intensity of emotions, poor understanding of

their emotions, negative reactivity to one’s emotional state (i.e., fear of emotion), and

maladaptive emotional modulation responses. Having difficulties processing their emotional

experiences through the adaptive processes of attention, understanding, and experiencing, these

individuals make use of worry to avoid the distress associated with negative emotional arousal.

Mennin and colleagues add that although worry may be soothing in the short-run, in the long-run

it creates a self-perpetuating cycle of emotional dysregulation. That is because worry interrupts

adaptive emotional processing of distressing emotional material and paradoxically amplifies

emotional intensity, prompting the use of further worrying by the individual.

A growing body of research has begun to emerge confirming that individuals who

endorse symptoms of GAD report more intense emotional experience, poorer understanding of

Page 32: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

23

emotions, greater negative reactivity to emotional experience, and greater restriction in their

ability to self-sooth after negative emotions than nonanxious individuals (Mennin et al., 2005;

Salters-Pedneault, Roemer, Tull, Rucker, & Mennin, 2006). In a series of three studies, Mennin

and colleagues (Mennin et al., 2005) found that students with self-reported GAD and individuals

who met criteria for GAD displayed poorer emotional regulation skills compared to control

participants. In addition, they found that students with GAD reported greater physiological

symptoms and greater difficulty managing their reactions after an emotion-inducing exercise

than compared to controls (Mennin et al., 2005). Specifically, in their first study, they asked over

five hundred university students to complete self-report measures on emotional intelligence,

alexithymia, emotion expression, fear of emotions, worry, GAD, and depression. They found that

individuals with GAD reported greater deficits in their ability to understand (i.e., identify,

describe, differentiate) their emotional experiences and to clarify what motivational information

their emotions conveyed in comparison to control participants. Further, the authors found that

emotion dysregulation was associated with chronic worry even after known correlates of worry

(i.e., trait anxiety and depression) were controlled for in the analyses.

In their second study, Mennin and colleagues (Mennin et al., 2005) administered the

same aforementioned measures to a clinical sample of forty-two GAD patients. They found

similar results as study 1, suggesting that the emotion regulation difficulties found in study 1

were not an artifact to the presence of other emotional disorders in the sample of university

students. In their third study, Mennin and colleagues tested whether the experience of emotion

would elicit emotion regulation deficits in a sample consisting of over a hundred university

students with and without GAD. They asked GAD participants and controls to listen to mood-

inducting (i.e., anxious, sad, neutral) music and complete measures of subjective mood and

Page 33: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

24

somatic anxiety before and after the experimental musical induction. The authors found that

GAD participants, following a negative mood induction (either anxious or sad), reported greater

levels of self-reported physiological anxiety symptoms than did control participants. In addition,

GAD participants had more difficulty accepting their emotions and believed they had less

influence over their course in reaction to the negative mood inductions than did controls.

Mennin and colleagues concluded that individuals with GAD may have had difficulties

managing specific emotional states during mood-inductions due to their difficulties

understanding, reacting to, and managing their emotions, as found in studies 1 and 2.

Yamas, Hazlett-Stevens, and Borkovec (1997) also examined the responses of college

students with self-reported GAD on the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS) and found that they

endorsed greater difficulty identifying and describing emotions than control participants. In a

another study, Roemer, Salters, Raffa, and Orsillo (2005) found that experiential avoidance and

the fear of emotional experiences predicted the severity of GAD above and beyond worry itself.

Correspondingly, Salters-Pedneault and colleagues (Salters-Pedneault et al., 2006) investigated

emotion regulation deficits in chronic worry and GAD using a well-established and

psychometrically sound measure of emotion dysregulation (i.e., the Difficulties in Emotion

Regulation Scale, DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004). In a sample of 325 university students, they

found that with the exception of emotional awareness, individuals with chronic worry reported

difficulty with a range of emotion regulation competencies. These included: deficits in emotional

clarity, acceptance of emotions, ability to engage in goal directed behaviours when distressed,

impulse control, and access to effective regulation strategies. Of note, the relationship between

emotion regulation deficits and analogue GAD status remained significant after variance

accounted for by general affective distress was removed. Although research supporting the

Page 34: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

25

model of Emotion Dsyregulation is still growing, the findings emerging in the empirical

literature provide compelling convergent evidence for the role of emotion dysregulation in the

pathology of worry.

A New Formulation of GAD and Worry: The Emotion-focused Model

There is now a new model of worry and GAD stemming from the emotion-focused

therapy (EFT) perspective that complements as well as expands on some of the ideas presented

by existing models described earlier. Watson and colleagues (Watson & Goldman, 2012; Watson

& Greenberg, 2012; Watson & Greenberg, 2015, in press; Watson, 2015) have formulated a

model of GAD in which the disorder is viewed as reflecting the operation of core maladaptive

emotion schemes developed as a result of early attachment injuries. Emotion schemes are

understood as internal structures of experience made up of multiple levels of functioning (i.e.,

perception, sensation, cognition, memory, affect, physiological changes; Greenberg, Rice, &

Elliot, 1993). The authors assert that in GAD these schemes are centered on painful emotional

experiences of fear, sadness, and shame which originated from early attachment experiences of

abandonment, neglect, and abuse as well as intrusive caretaking and invalidation. These

maladaptive emotion schemes, which are on the edge of the individual’s awareness, contribute to

a negative self-organization which construct the self as scared and vulnerable. Watson and

colleagues posit that the painful emotions resulting from the activation of these schemes are also

experienced by the individual with GAD as a threat to the integrity of his or her vulnerable sense

of self, cuing overwhelming and undifferentiated distress associated with fears of disintegration

and annihilation. As a result of this distress, the individual reacts with worry and anxiety in an

attempt to protect the self from being overwhelmed by this distress and the associated painful

Page 35: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

26

emotions of when they felt as a child in similar situations. A diagram of this model if presented

below.

Figure 1. The EFT Model of GAD. Adapted with permission from Dr. Watson.

Watson and colleagues (Watson & Goldman, 2012; Watson & Greenberg, 2012; Watson

& Greenberg, 2015, in press; Watson, 2015) place negative self-organizations (based on core

painful emotion schemes) and the inability to regulate emotions at the heart of worry. Worry is

viewed as a misguided attempt of the individual to protect the self from falling apart or becoming

overwhelmed by painful emotions that he or she is unable to soothe. The authors explain that

individuals with GAD were repeatedly exposed to threatening and negative events without

adequate protection, soothing, and nurture. As a result, their emotion regulation capacities and

identity formation were compromised. In the absence of adequate emotion regulation capacities

and a resilient sense of self, these individuals did not have the resources to symbolize their

maladaptive emotion schemes of fear, shame and sadness in awareness. That means that

whenever painful emotion schemes get activated (in the present) or is anticipated to get activated

On Edge of

Awareness

Evoking Cues

Emotion

Scheme Vulnerable Self-

organization

Undifferentiated

Distress Anxiety Worry

In Awareness

Sadness Shame

Fear

Page 36: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

27

(e.g., through situations that resemble earlier events which led to developmentally significant

emotional injuries), individuals with GAD become aware of a secondary undifferentiated distress

that comes from those unsymbolized schemes and that they are unable to soothe. As noted

earlier, this secondary distress threatens these individuals’ vulnerable self-organizations such that

they fear they will disintegrate in the face of their emotional pain. Thus, these individuals worry

to preserve their self-organizations. However, because core painful emotions are left

unprocessed, worrying remains ongoing.

What distinguishes the EFT model from other existing models on worry and GAD is the

explicit integration of affective and developmental/interpersonal elements within the worry

process so that it is not seen as purely based on cognitive factors or on emotion regulation

deficits. This model proposes that the worry process in GAD contains representations of self

along with associated painful affect which originated in a relational context in childhood and is

not adequately processed and modulated by the GAD individual. Note that this model views the

self as a complex system composed of a set of elements that self-organizes according to

cognitive-affective experiential processes activated by internal and external stimuli (Watson &

Greenberg in press). In such a view, worry is much more than just a strategy to avoid negative

emotion but a complex process involving the overall organization of the self among several

modules (e.g., cognitive, affective, interpersonal/developmental). Thus, the avoidance of

emotions involved in the worrying process does not just reflect the avoidance of anxious affect

produced by an uncertain or threatening situation, but rather a proactive attempt by the individual

to protect his or her negative self-organization against, ultimately, unsymbolized, painful

emotion schematic memories.

Page 37: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

28

Because of its recent formulation, no empirical study has to date directly investigated the

premises of the emotion-focused perspective of GAD and worry. However, because this

perspective shares some similarities with existing models of worry, such as seeing worry as an

effort to cope with difficult emotions, it has found indirect support from existing research

findings showing that people diagnosed with GAD and chronic worry have difficulty regulating

emotions (Craske & Waters, 2005; Mennin et al., 2002, 2005). This perspective has also found

support from research highlighting the associations of insecure attachment histories with emotion

dysregulation (Lopez & Brennan, 2000; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007) as well as with GAD and

chronic worry (Cassidy 1995, 2009).

This dissertation seeks to add preliminary empirical support to the EFT model. There are

two particular propositions that that this study hopes to empirically address that have been

suggested as important within the emotion-focused view of GAD. The first proposition considers

the importance of insecure attachment in the etiology of GAD and worry. The second considers

the mediating role of negative self-organization and emotion dysregulation in the relationships

between insecure attachment and worry and insecure attachment and GAD. In the next section a

discussion of the role of insecure attachment on GAD and worry is provided.

Attachment Perspective on Worry

Bowlby’s theory (1969/1982, 1973) asserts that the protective and soothing functions of

responsive caregiving lead infants to construe positive representations of their environment and

emotional functioning which in turn provide them with a sense of safety to explore the world,

confidence in their self-value, as well as promote the development of adaptive emotion

regulation skills (e.g., support seeking, attention shifting, evocation of positive emotions, etc.).

Page 38: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

29

Bowlby described infants as having a repertoire of innate behaviours that motivates them to seek

and maintain proximity with caregivers. These behaviours, known as proximity seeking

behaviours, are intended to elicit protection and provision from caregivers in times of stress,

threat, or novelty.

According to Bowlby (1969/1982), if bids for proximity and comfort are answered

sensibly and consistently by the caregiver, the infant feels protected from danger and soothed

from anxiety. More importantly, the infant gains confidence in the availability of their caregiver

to regulate anxiety, and confidence in his or her own ability to manage anxiety through the

attachment relationship (Cassidy, 1994). Bowlby proposed that the internalization of positive

interactions with attachment figures, with its respective positive expectancies around caregiving,

forms a “working model” of secure attachment that provides the child with a secure base from

which to explore their surrounding environment as well as their affective experiences without

being overcome by fear.

Unfortunately, however, not all children receive sensible and consistent care. If the

attachment figure is unavailable, insensitive or inconsistent to the infant’s bids for proximity,

negative views of others and the world are formed based on the infant’s experiences of emotional

misattunement, unpredictability of care and exposure to physical danger. In addition, fear and

anxiety are not alleviated and the infant is required to learn affect regulation strategies other than

proximity seeking (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Main & Solomon, 1990; Mikulincer, Shaver, & Pereg,

2003). Given that in insecure attachment relationships infants are made to feel that their anxiety

is unwarranted or unmanageable, alternative emotion regulation strategies emerge in which the

infant either denies emotional distress and develops a distorted sense of self-reliance, or

overreacts to internal cues and hyperactivates the attachment relationship (Cassidy, 1994). To put

Page 39: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

30

it another way, the infant learns to remain cognitively and emotionally aloof in times of distress,

or to resort to intensive efforts to achieve love, care, and support to cope.

Several studies have found links between differences in the quality of early attachment

relationships and differences in internal working models as well as expression of emotions in

infants (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Cassidy, 1994; Kobak & Sceery, 1988; Main,

1990; Main, Kaplan, and Cassidy, 1985; Sroufe, 2000). Noteworthy among these is the “strange

situation” procedure devised by Ainsworth and colleagues (1978). The strange situation

procedure comprised of several episodes of interaction involving infants, mothers, and a stranger

in a toy-filled room. Of special relevance were two instances when the mother left the infant

alone with the stranger and then returned to be with the infant. By observing infants throughout

this procedure, Ainsworth and colleagues (1978) identified three styles of attachment that

reflected three different strategies infants used to seek and maintain proximity to their caregivers.

These styles included a secure attachment style, an ambivalent attachment style, and an avoidant

attachment style.

Ainsworth and colleagues (1978) observed that the expression of these three styles of

attachment behaviours was significantly linked to differences in the interactional patterns of the

infant-mother dyads. Emotion regulation was a determining factor in the differences between the

attachment styles. Infants with a secure attachment were found to engage easily in exploratory

play while in their mothers’ presence and became anxious when separated from them. They

sought proximity and were easily soothed upon their mothers’ return. These infants were thought

to have mothers who were responsive to their behavioural cues, such that they openly expressed

negative and positive affect when it occurred (Cassidy, 1994; Sroufe, 2000). Conversely, infants

with an ambivalent attachment style became unusually distressed and anxious with their

Page 40: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

31

mothers’ absence and were not easily soothed upon their return. Their response to their

caregivers’ return was one of proximity mixed with rejection and anger. These infants were

found to heighten affective experiencing in order to gain their mothers’ attention. This

hyperactivating strategy reflected an attempt to maximize care and protection in the context of

inconsistent and unpredictable caregiving (Cassidy, 1994; Sroufe, 2000).

Infants with avoidant attachment were found to be unaffected by their caregivers’

presence or absence during the strange situation. Contrary to secure and ambivalent infants, they

displayed no distress in the absence of their caregivers and made no attempt at proximity upon

reunion. These infants were thought to have insensitive mothers who responded to distress or

vulnerability with punishment and rejection. Thus, it appeared these infants learned to use

deactivating strategies to suppress emotional arousal. In this way, these infants ensured sufficient

proximity to caregivers while also preventing caregivers from becoming alienated by the infants’

emotional responding (Cassidy, 1994; Sroufe, 2000). Interestingly, these infants’ heart rates

during the separation episode from their caregivers were later found to be as elevated as those of

visibly anxious but secure infants, and the rise in their level of cortisol (stress) hormone before

and after the strange situation procedure was significantly greater than that of secure infants

(Spangler & Grossmann, 1993; Sroufe & Waters, 1977). In addition, both avoidant and

ambivalent children (i.e., insecure children) showed inhibition in their exploratory play during

the procedure, suggesting that the inflexible use of either hyperactivating or deactivating emotion

regulation strategies not only resulted in dysfunctional emotions and physiological disturbance,

but also interfered with other important developmental activity (Cassidy, 1994).

Bowlby (1973) described several early negative experiences that could lead a child to

become uncertain about the availability of the caregiver and, in turn, develop chronic anxiety.

Page 41: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

32

These include major attachment-related problems, such as serious illness of a caregiver or

repeated extensive separations from a caregiver, which could lead a child to worry. Another

experience involves the death of someone close to the child, which may result in the child’s

increased worries that something may happen to a caregiver. Bowlby also identified repeated

daily experiences of rejection, particularly in times of stress when the child bids for proximity, as

precursors to anxiety. Of importance, Bowlby recognized that major trauma was not necessary to

lead to anxiety. He highlighted that repeated situations representing threats of abandonment or

separation (whether overtly or covertly) could contribute to a child’s increased fearfulness about

her well-being and/or the well-being of her caregiver (Cassidy, 1995).

Several early theorists proposed that insecure attachment would have a major role in the

development of chronic anxiety and possibly worry. For example, Horney (1950 as cited in

Crits-Christoph, Gibbons, & Crits-Christoph, 2004) suggested that negative attachment

experiences with caregivers (e.g., overprotection, domination, and indifference exchanges) lead

to a lack of confidence in the child (to elicit care) and in the availability of caregiver (to provide

care) which causes her to feel isolated and helpless, as well as to view the world as hostile.

Horney predicted that this child would develop “basic anxiety” which she then would attempt to

manage by creating an idealized self-image of herself. This idealized self, however, would never

be within reach and would lead to a neurotic conflict leading to further anxiety and concerns

about self and others.

Correspondingly, Sullivan (1953) postulated that poor relationships with caregivers and

the anticipated disapproval from them would often result in anxiety. Sullivan argued that early

insecure attachment experiences would increase a child’s need for security and thus compromise

the development of independence and self-esteem. According to Sullivan, insecurely attached

Page 42: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

33

individuals would have difficulties meeting their needs for security and personal satisfaction later

in life as well as developing a sense of agency, and as a result, they would experience anxiety in

times of distress. Klein (1975) discussed anxiety as originating from a child’s fear of not being

able to elicit the caregiver in times of need. For Klein, anxiety was intimately related to the

individual’s lack of confidence in their ability to sustain or repair relationships. Altogether, these

early theorists proposed that worry would thus originate from a more basic anxiety arising from

one’s lack of self-esteem and associated fear of not obtaining what one needs in relationships

(Crits-Christoph et al., 2004)

More recently, Newman and Erickson (2010) contended that GAD originates from

insecure attachment experiences of inconsistent caregiving where the child is pulled into a

developmentally premature state of needing to take care of themselves, the caregiver or both.

Sibrava and Borkovec (2006) proposed a similar hypothesis for the possible origins of worry.

They argue that having had unreliable and enmeshed caregiving, the child had to look out for

themselves and their caregiver. The child’s caregiving may go beyond mere concern about the

state of their relationship with the caregiver. The child may have to worry about the actual

emotional well-being of her caregiver such that the psychological responsibility of the caregiver

becomes the child’s own concern. In this context, the child feels that the world is a dangerous

place and that they have to anticipate the future for both themselves and the caregiver in order to

survive and obtain their caregiver’s love and approval. Correspondingly, Borkovec and Newman

(1999) suggest that individuals with GAD often had to give care to their caregivers rather than

receive care. As a result, they conclude that individuals with GAD may be attuned to other’s

agenda for attachment and “constantly anticipates threats as the necessary means to obtain love,

approval, and nurturance” (p. 446).

Page 43: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

34

Wolfe and colleagues (Wolfe, 2005; Wolfe & Sigl, 1998) have also proposed that the

etiology of worry lies on early experiences of self-endangerment (i.e., experiences associated

with separation, rejection, loss of a primary caregiver, intensely humiliating experiences or

disillusioning experiences of the hands of a primary caregiver) prime the infant to protect one’s

self-concept and self-image from painful shame-based emotions by interrupting sentient (i.e.,

immediate, in-the-moment) self-experiencing. According to Wolfe and colleagues, anxiety arises

from the interruption of sentient self-experiencing in which the individual shifts attention from

sentient awareness to reflexive (i.e., preconceived) awareness. The authors suggest that the

failure to process and modulate shame-laden self-endangerment experiences from the past, and

later negative sentient self-experiencing, results in the individual engaging in compulsive,

reflexive self-experiencing and thereby relying on worry as a primary defense to avoid the

awareness of feelings of inadequacy, inferiority and humiliation. Worrying in this light is

therefore viewed as a form of “obsessive cogitation” that originates from negative early

experiences in which immediate self-experience is ignored or interrupted and the individual

learns to respond only to preconceived notions about the self, others and the world (Wolfe &

Sigl, 1998).

As described earlier, Watson and colleagues (Watson & Goldman, 2012; Watson &

Greenberg, 2012; Watson & Greenberg, 2015, in press; Watson, 2015) have formulated the EFT

model of GAD in which the disorder is viewed as reflecting the operation of maladaptive

emotion schemes resulting from early attachment experiences of abandonment, neglect, abuse or

alternatively intrusive caretaking or invalidation. The authors argue that these insecure

attachment experiences – together with their sadness, fear, and shame responses – become

encoded as painful autobiographical memories and internalized into an overall painful

Page 44: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

35

maladaptive emotion scheme, characterized by a sense of vulnerability, which can be triggered in

adulthood. Watson and colleagues posit that the symptomatic worry and anxiety in GAD is

secondary to this basic sense of vulnerability, as the individual with GAD is cautious that core

painful schematic memories of early attachment injuries might be activated.

There are several reasons why the developmental roots of worry would lie in insecure

attachment (Cassidy 1995; Cassidy, Lichtenstein-Phelps, Sibrava, Thomas, & Borkovec, 2009).

Cassidy and colleagues observe that the nature of worry is inherently interpersonal. They note

that the most common themes in worry are associated with fears related to social evaluation and

interpersonal relationships. To support this position, they show that GAD has high comorbidity

rates with social phobia and that this is true for none of the other anxiety disorders. The authors

also argue that the emotional dysregulation difficulties that have been found to characterize

individuals with GAD, as predicted by the emotion dysregulation model of worry, are closely

linked to attachment.

Cassidy and colleagues (1995; Cassidy et al., 2009) posit that emotion regulation

capacities develop in the context of attachment relationships. They argue that it is within secure

attachment dyads that children learn that distressing emotions can be recognized, acknowledged,

tolerated, and recovered from rather than denied or avoided. The authors also cite evidence

demonstrating that although GAD occurs within families, it lacks a strong genetic component.

They review twin studies showing similar concordance rates for GAD and revealing no specific

genetic heritability for the disorder (Torgesen, 1983; Kendler, Walters, Neale, Kessler, Heath, &

Eaves, 1995 as cited in Cassidy, 2009). Finally, the authors argue that the diffuse nature of worry

parallels the type of anxiety that Bowlby proposed to be related to insecure attachment. They

note that Bowlby viewed the onset of anxiety as arising from an individual’s lack of confidence

Page 45: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

36

that the attachment figure would be available in times of need. According to Bowlby (1973), this

anxiety would develop no matter what the source of fear was and would engender in the

individual a generalized sense of threat and vulnerability, with no specific trigger (Cassidy,

1995). This anxiety would then become organized alongside the individual’s working models of

attachment relationships and the availability of attachment figures, and persist through

adulthood.

There has been some debate as to whether styles of caregiver-infant attachment remain

stable over the lifespan such that they would continue to influence current adult behaviours.

Bowlby’s (1979) asserted that attachment mechanisms influence relationships ‘‘from the cradle

to the grave’’ (p. 129). Some studies have found mixed support for this assertion (Lewis, Fering,

& Rosenthal, 2000; Weinfield, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2000). For example, Lewis and colleagues

(Lewis et al., 2000) have found no association between individuals’ overt attachment behaviours

at 1 year of age and attachment representations in adolescence (Lewis, Feiring, & Rosenthal,

2000; Weinfield, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2000). However, they found that individuals with negative

recollections of childhood, recorded in adolescence, showed insecure attachment representations

in early adulthood. In another similar study by Weinfield and colleagues (Weinfield et al., 2000),

the authors found no significant continuity in attachment representation in infancy (measured at

1 and 1.5 year of age) to early adulthood. However, findings revealed a significant relationship

between early negative life circumstances (e.g., childhood maltreatment, maternal depression,

and poverty) and insecure attachment representations in early adulthood (Weinfield et al., 2000).

Other studies examining the associations between individuals’ memories of childhood

and adult attachment styles have also provided support, albeit indirect, of attachment continuity

over the life span. For instance, Hazan and Shaver (1987) found that adults endorsing a secure

Page 46: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

37

attachment representation described having had caring and warm relationships with their

caregivers whereas individuals displaying an anxious and avoidant attachment representations

described their parents as intrusive, cold and rejecting. Brennan and Shaver (1998) also found

that insecure individuals described their parents as more rejecting relative to the secure group.

Studies reviewing the relationship between child-caregiver attachment and later romantic

attachment have also shown that child and adult attachment correlate at a moderate strength

(Crowell et al., 1999; Fraley & Shaver, 2000). Of note, although recollections of child-caregiver

attachment have been found to reflect past experiences as well as current circumstances (Henry,

Moffitt, Caspi, Langley, & Silva, 1994; Lewis, 1997), internalized views of attachment figures

have been found to be associated with adult functioning regardless of their veracity (e.g.,

Benjamin, 1996; Bowlby, 1973). Therefore, both actual and recalled early childhood

experiences remain an important focus of research when investigating attachment styles.

Empirical findings appear for the most part to converge on the notion that attachment

organization is stable over long periods of time, particularly from later childhood onwards

(Fraley, 2002; Hamilton, 2000; Waters, Merrick, Treboux, Crowell, & Albersheim, 2000). From

a more recent neuroscience perspective, researches (Cozolino, 2014; Hruby, Hasto, & Minarik,

2011; Schore 2003) support the plasticity of neurosystems (particularly the orbitofrontal system)

thought to be shaped by early attachment experiences; however, they also state that it is during

the first years of life that the basic structure for internal working models and emotion regulation

is formed. Overall, infant attachment does not appear to be necessarily deterministic of adult

attachment, especially when disruptive life events or changes in attachment relationships occur

(Cozzarelli, Karafa, Collins, & Tagler, 2003). However, given that new experiences are more

easily assimilated into one’s existent working models and that attachment styles give rise to self-

Page 47: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

38

fulfilling behaviours, attachment is usually thought to remain relatively stable over time (Collins

& Read, 2004; Hazan & Shaver, 1994).

Fraley (2002) concluded from a review of longitudinal attachment studies (dating from

1979 to 2000) that a prototype for close relationships arises in infancy and it remains stable

through adolescence and adulthood at a moderate strength. In his meta-analysis, he examined

whether a revisionist or a prototype model best explained attachment stability. The revisionist

model assumes that early attachment representations are revised and updated in response to

ongoing experience and consequently may or may not correspond to later attachment

representations. The prototype model assumes that early attachment representations are retained

throughout development and have an ongoing effect on attachment dynamics throughout the life

course. Fraley found that a prototype model of attachment stability provided the best fit to the

attachment data, suggesting that a prototype-like process contributes to continuity of attachment

across the life course. His estimated model indicated that early prototypes exert a moderate

influence on subsequent interactions (p = 0.39) and that these interactions are easily incorporated

into concurrent beliefs about the world (η = 1.00).

Fraley (2002) also found that his prototype model predicted that the continuity between

early attachment security and attachment security at any point later in the life course would be

equivalent to a correlation of approximately .39. Thus, internal working models of self, others,

and the world as well as emotion regulations strategies learned early in life likely continue at a

moderate level through adulthood, unless circumstances change or other experiences (e.g.,

psychotherapy) intervene (Sable, 2000; Wallin, 2007; Waters, Hamilton, & Weinfield, 2000).

Perhaps internal working models and regulatory strategies become more sophisticated with age,

Page 48: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

39

but guidelines for processing information and emotional responses remain over time (Watson,

2011).

Research on adult attachment behaviour grew out of Ainsworth et al.’s (1978) findings. It

began with the idea that attachment relationships developed in early childhood are carried into

adulthood and exert great influence on adults’ close relationships, in particular, romantic

relationships. Hazan and Shaver (1987) were the first to develop a self-report measure of adult

attachment. Paralleling Ainsworth et al.’s (1978) typology of infant attachment patterns, they

described three prominent styles of adult attachment: secure, anxious (or ambivalent), and

avoidant. Secure adults were shown to be comfortable with intimacy in relationship. They were

more likely than avoidant and anxious adults to view their partners as trustworthy and less likely

to worry about abandonment (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Anxious (or ambivalent) adults

tended to be nervous about relationships and desired greater intimacy than secure and avoidant

adults. They were more likely to fall in love at first sight and also to long intensely for their

partners’ reciprocation (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). In contrast, avoidant adults showed

difficulty trusting romantic partners and felt unease with intimacy. They felt uncomfortable

depending on their partners to provide care and were less likely than secure and preoccupied

adults to accept their partners’ faults (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Notably, these findings have

been replicated and extended by several investigators (e.g., Brennan & Shaver, 1995; Carver,

1997; Feeney & Noller, 1990; Kirkpatrick & Davis, 1994; Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992).

As research in adult attachment progressed, researchers have devised different ways to

measure adult attachment patterns. Going beyond Hazan and Shaver’s (1987) initial method of

measuring adult romantic attachment patterns, researchers have proposed additional and more

general categories of adult attachment (see Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007 for comprehensive

Page 49: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

40

review). For instance, Feeney, Noller and Hanrahan (1994) found evidence to divide the anxious

(or ambivalent) attachment pattern into two categories: those who primarily demonstrate need for

approval and those who are preoccupied with relationships. They also found support for breaking

down the avoidant attachment pattern into two subgroups: those who primarily show discomfort

with closeness and those who regard relationships as secondary to achievement (Feeney et al.,

1994).

Conversely, Bartholomew and Horowitz, (1991) proposed a four-category model of adult

attachment (secure, fearful, preoccupied, and dismissing) located in a two-dimensional space

defined by model of self and model of other. Specifically, Bartholomew and Horowitz extended

Hazan and Shaver’s (1978) classic styles of attachment by further dividing the avoidant category

into two categories: fearful-avoidant type (i.e., those who avoid relationships due to fear of

rejection) and dismissing-avoidant type (i.e., those who avoid relationships due to discomfort

with intimacy). Bartholomew and Horowitz also conceptualized adult attachment following

Bowlby’s (1973, 1988) ideas of internal working models of self and other. Individuals form

either a positive or negative view of self and other. Those with positive models of self and other

have a sense that they are worthy of care and expect others to be there for them in times of need.

These individuals with positive models of self and other tend to favour closeness in relationships

while those with negative models of self and others tend to avoid intimacy. Each of

Bartholomew and Horowitz’s (1991) adult attachment categories can be located within these

positive and negative regions of models of self and other.

Alternative to self-report measures, a separate avenue of research on adult attachment

was developed through use of narrative and interview methods. At about the same time Hazan

and Shaver (1987) began their work on adult romantic attachment, Main and colleagues (Main,

Page 50: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

41

Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985) devised the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI). In the AAI, adults

were interviewed about their early experiences with their attachment figures and asked to reflect

on childhood memories. The resulting narratives were analysed and adult attachment categories

were defined along certain dimensions. The adult categories corresponded to the infant

attachment classifications described by Ainsworth et al. (1978), including autonomous (similar

to secure), detached or dismissive (similar to avoidant), and enmeshed or preoccupied (similar to

anxious/ambivalent). Adults classified as autonomous are those able to speak freely and

coherently about their early attachment relationships. In contrast, adults classified as dismissing

are those who typically minimize the impact of early relationships on their current functioning.

Dismissing adults often speak vaguely or in idealizing ways about their parents and have

difficulty recalling childhood memories. Enmeshed adults, on the other hand, tend to become

easily overwhelmed by the task of reflecting upon their childhood experiences. Their narratives

are often long and are characterized by incoherencies and confusion. Like Hazan and Shaver’s

(1987) work, the AAI has been extended by other investigators (e.g., Fonagy, Steele, Steele,

Moran, & Higgitt, 1991).

Although research on adult attachment has often referred back to the different categories

of infant attachment, recent studies have shown that adult attachment can also be conceptualized

in terms of two dimensions: avoidance and anxiety (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998; Fraley &

Waller, 1998). The dimension of avoidance is characterized by a discomfort with intimacy and

interpersonal closeness, while the dimension of anxiety is characterized by a chronic fear of

interpersonal rejection and abandonment (Brennan et al., 1998). The idea behind this

dimensional model of adult attachment is that combinations of high and low scores on the

avoidance and anxiety dimensions contribute to an overall picture of attachment which removes

Page 51: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

42

the need for categorization. For example, secure attachment is defined by low attachment

avoidance and low attachment anxiety whereas fearful attachment is defined by high attachment

avoidance and high attachment anxiety.

Some authors have criticized the use of categorical methods in adult attachment research

(Fraley & Spieker, 2003; Ravitz, Maunder, Hunter, Sthankiya, & Lancee, 2010). However, both

categorical and dimensional methods for investigating attachment have been found valuable in

expanding our understanding of emotional aspects in relationships, as evidenced by the fact that

adult attachment patterns assessed by differing methods have been confirmed to be associated

with important relational adjustments (Shaver, Belsky, & Brennan, 2000). Also, the use of

categorical scales – including the original adult attachment measure by Hazan and Shaver (1987)

used in this study – continue to provide a practical framework upon which many researchers rely

when investigating significant attachment issues stemming from close relationships (Shi,

Wampler, & Wampler, 2014; Sroufe, 2003).

Until recently, empirical research had not focused on the association between insecure

attachment styles and worry. However, over the past decades a growing body of studies began to

emerge highlighting this link. Worry has been linked to experiences of traumatic events

involving significant others (Roemer, Molina, Litz, & Borkovec, 1996) and a history of disturbed

early environments, particularly for those who developed GAD before the age of 20 (Hoehn-

Saric, Hazlett, & McLeod, 1993). Eng and Heimberg (2006) have shown that undergraduate

students with self-reported GAD shared similar levels of attachment to peers and perceived

social support compared to control participants, but they reported greater attachment insecurity

with caregivers than controls. Mickelson and colleagues (Mickelson, Kessler, & Shaver, 1997)

have found that both anxious and avoidant insecure attachment styles are associated with GAD.

Page 52: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

43

Other researchers have gone one step further and tried to identify which specific characteristics

of insecure attachment experiences are linked to worry. For example, Bifulco and colleagues

(Bifulco, Kwon, Jacobs, Moran, Bunn, et al., 2006), have found that adult women with GAD

endorsed greater angry-dismissive attachment styles marked by high levels of avoidance, self-

reliance, mistrust, and anger which the authors argue may have been perpetuated by childhood

experiences of unmet needs and/or threats of separation from caregivers.

A few studies to date have also examined the associations of early attachment and

parenting experiences with worry or GAD in children and adolescents (Hale et al., 2006; Muris,

2002; Muris et al., 2000; Brown & Whiteside, 2008). Most of this work has used the children’s

version of the EMBU (Egna Minnen Betraffende Uppsfostran, My memories of upbringing;

Perris, Jacobsson, Lindstrom, von Knorring, & Perris, 1980) which is a retrospective self-report

measure of perceived parenting behaviours, as well as Hazan and Shaver's (1987) three-category

attachment measure. For instance, one study of a community sample of one hundred and fifty-

nine children (ages of nine to thirteen) found that negative perceived parental rearing behaviours

characterized by parental rejection, control, and fearfulness were found to be positively

associated with worry (Muris et al., 2000). The authors found that the more children perceived

their parents as rejecting and anxious, the higher their levels of worry (Muris et al., 2000). In

addition, children who classified themselves as avoidantly or ambivalently attached displayed

higher worry levels than children who classified themselves as securely attached (Muris et al.,

2000).

Another study with a clinically anxious sample of children and teens (eight of whom met

criteria for GAD) found significant correlations between parental rejection and worry (Brown &

Whiteside, 2008). Ambivalently attached children were also found to report more negative

Page 53: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

44

parental rearing behaviours and more worrying as compared to children with either secure or

avoidant attachment styles (Brown & Whiteside, 2008). No findings were reported for the

relationships between parenting behaviours and worry as well as attachment styles and worry for

the GAD sample alone (Brown & Whiteside, 2008). Correspondingly, Hale and colleagues

(Hale et al., 2006) have also found that parental rejection and parental alienation uniquely

predicted GAD symptoms in a sample of adolescents. The authors demonstrated that adolescents

who experienced feelings of isolation, anger, and detachment in their relationships with

caregivers (i.e., parental alienation) and perceived their caregivers as more rejecting (i.e.,

parental rejection), endorsed greater GAD symptoms.

To further investigate the relationship between attachment and worry, Cassidy and

colleagues (Cassidy et al., 2009) compared a group of sixty-nine adult therapy clients with GAD

to a matched nonanxious control group on a self-report measure of attachment (i.e., Perceptions

of Adult Attachment Questionnaire [PAAQ]; Lichtenstein and Cassidy, 1991 as cited in Cassidy

et al., 2009). The PAAQ consists of eight theoretically-based scales tapping both childhood

experiences of a) rejection/neglect, b) being loved, c) role-reversal/enmeshment, as well as

current state of mind in relation to attachment: d) vulnerable, e) balancing-forgiving, f) angry, g)

dismissing/derogating, and i) lacking in memory for childhood. Cassidy and colleagues found

that GAD clients reported significantly higher experiences of childhood rejection/neglect and

role-reversal/enmeshment, and significantly lower experiences of being loved and cared for by

their primary caregivers, in comparison to nonanxious controls.

In terms of current state of mind with respect to attachment, Cassidy and colleagues

(Cassidy et al., 2009) found that GAD clients reported significantly higher vulnerability toward

their mothers and also reported greater difficulty recalling childhood memories than nonanxious

Page 54: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

45

controls. Moreover, these PAAQ current state of mind subscales (i.e., vulnerable and lacking in

memory for childhood) were able to accurately predict diagnostic (i.e., GAD) status for nearly

three-quarters of the participants. The authors concluded that an anxious childhood based on

enmeshment and parental coldness and rejection could lead to generalized worries about the

future. They explain that a child with an insecure attachment bond to her caregiver would not be

able to count on the caregiver for care and protection, and in addition, would need to develop

avoidant coping mechanisms (e.g., worry) to manage the painful feelings associated with the

caregiver’s rejection. Moreover, the authors argue that the finding that GAD individuals endorse

greater feelings of anger/vulnerability towards their mothers and demonstrate a greater lack of

memory with respect to childhood experiences appears consistent with Borkovec’s

conceptualization that worry seems to serve the function of preventing the processing of

disturbing emotional material and of protecting individuals from more painful unresolved

attachment issues (Cassidy et al., 2009).

Additional support for the association between attachment and worry comes from studies

highlighting the similarities between the interpretative biases among individuals with GAD and

those with insecure attachment styles. As discussed earlier, individuals with GAD have been

found to display attentional and interpretative biases favoring the encoding of threatening stimuli

and threatening meaning of ambiguous information (Hayes & Hirsch, 2007). Bowlby

(1969/1982) suggested that insecurely attached individuals who lack a secure base from which to

explore the world would also display such vigilant monitoring of their environments and direct

attention to threat stimuli as well as interpret ambiguous situations as indicative of such

treatment.

Page 55: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

46

In a study investigating the association between negative childhood experiences and both

attention and interpretation biases for angry faces, Gibb and colleagues (Gibb, Schofield, &

Binghamton, 2009) found support for that position. The authors found that among a sample of

more than two hundred college students, those who reported a history of negative childhood

experiences preferentially allocated their attention to angry face, but not happy or sad faces. In

addition, these participants also exhibited increased sensitivity in the detection of angry

expressions at a lower level of emotional intensity in comparison to their secure counterparts.

Cassidy and colleagues (Cassidy, Kirsh, Scolton, & Parke; 1996) also found that insecurely

attached six-year old children were more likely to attribute hostile intent to a series of ambiguous

stories than were securely attached children. These findings suggest that the cognitive processes

characteristics of individuals with GAD are also the cognitive processes that arise in children and

young adults who are insecurely attached (Cassidy, 1995). As Cassidy (1995) notes, both GAD

and insecurely attached individuals view the world as a dangerous place and themselves as

vulnerable to threat.

The link between insecure attachment and worry is also implied from the association

between interpersonal problems and GAD. Research has shown that individuals with GAD score

higher than controls on a variety of interpersonal problems assessed by the Inventory of

Interpersonal Problems Circumplex (IIP–C; Alden, Wiggins, & Pincus, 1990; Horowitz, Alden,

Wiggins & Pincus, 2000) and report greater overall interpersonal distress than nonanxious

individuals (Eng & Heimberg, 2006; Pincus & Borkovec, 1994; Salzer, Pincus, Hoyer, Kreische,

Leichsenring, & Leibing, 2008). For instance, Salzer and colleagues conducted a cluster analysis

among seventy-eight individuals with GAD in order to identify subtypes of GAD clients based

on their interpersonal problems. They found four types of problematic interpersonal dispositions

Page 56: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

47

associated with GAD: (a) exploitation (e.g., letting others take advantage of them), (b) coldness

(e.g., keeping people at a distance), (c) nonassertiveness (e.g., having difficulties asserting

oneself to others), and (d) intrusiveness (e.g., having difficulties staying out of other people’s

business).

A later study from Przeworski and colleagues (Przeworski et al., 2011) also found those

same four interpersonal subtypes to be characteristic of individuals with GAD. However, in

Przeworski et al.’s study, clusters were more evenly divided among participants (with 27.7%

falling in the intrusive cluster, 31.9% in the exploitable cluster, 21.28% in the cold cluster, and

19.15% in the nonassertive cluster) whereas Salzer and colleagues found relatively small

percentages in the intrusive and non-assertive clusters (14.5% and 10.5%, repectively), over 50%

in the exploitable cluster (52.6%), and 22.4% in the cold cluster. Przeworkski and colleagues

also found that the differences in interpersonal functioning across individuals with GAD were

not accounted for by differences in DSM-IV Axis I symptoms (e.g., depression) and 95% of

individuals in the nonassertive cluster met criteria for an avoidant personality style.

Clusters of interpersonal problems are thought to originate from an individual’s

interpersonal learning history which manifests itself in large part in the individual’s negative

attachment experience (Horowitz, Rosenberg, & Bartholomew, 1993). For example, individuals

whose early experiences were characterized predominantly by enmeshment with attachment

figures are expected to develop difficulties accepting distance between them and important

others. Alternatively, individuals whose early experiences were characterized predominantly by

parental rejection are expected to develop a cold and avoidant style of interacting with significant

others. Przerworski and colleagues (Przeworski et al., 2011) did not find empirical support for an

association between interpersonal subtypes and attachment styles among individuals with GAD.

Page 57: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

48

However, the authors highlight that in their study there was a trend toward significance between

GAD-related interpersonal clusters and attachment subscales with a large effect size. Przerworski

and colleagues explain that it is possible that they did not have sufficient power in their study to

detect significant differences in attachment variables across interpersonal subtypes among their

samples. Clearly more studies are needed to elucidate this matter. However, it seems plausible to

assume that GAD is characterized by several interpersonal difficulties which appear to originate

in early interpersonal dynamics and could be further explained by attachment theory.

More recently, Koerner and colleagues (Koerner, Tallon, and Kusec, 2015) investigated

the associations between early maladaptive schemas and GAD symptoms. In a sample of one

hundred and thirty-eight university students, the authors found that maladaptive schemas

reflecting exaggerated self-standards (e.g., “one should not make mistakes”) and a need to place

others’ needs ahead of their own uniquely predicted greater GAD severity, after controlling for

trait anxiety and depressive symptoms. These findings were in line with Hazlett-Stevens’ (2008)

assertion several years ago that individuals with GAD not only have fundamental beliefs that the

world is dangerous and that one is unable to cope with adversity, but they also hold self-relevant

schemas that are maladaptive, including rigid self-expectations.

It must be noted that the review on worry development focuses on the underlying

concept of attachment interactions. However, attachment is not the only influence on the

development of worry. Dispositional factors and temperament characteristics, such as

neuroticism, harm avoidance, and behavioural inhibition, may also have an impact on the

development of worry (Kertz & Woodruff-Borden, 2011; Mineka, Yovel, & Pineles, 2002;

Rapee, 2001). For instance, Hettema, Prescott, and Kendler (2004) found evidence that there is a

high degree of correlation between neuroticism and GAD (r = 0.80) and that these two

Page 58: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

49

presentations may involve some of the same genes, as evidence by their twin data. In another

study, Rettew and colleagues (Rettew, Doyle, & Kwan, 2006) assessed the relationship between

levels of the temperamental trait of harm avoidance and GAD diagnosis in 334 individuals, both

children and adults. Harm avoidance is widely recognized as a personality trait that denotes

behavioural inhibition due to anticipatory worry of future problems, low energy, and passive

avoidant behaviours and shyness with strangers (Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993). The

authors found that harm avoidance scores were highly predictive of GAD diagnoses in children

and adults.

Rapee and Szollos (1997 as cited in Rapee 2001) also found that compared with

nonclinical children, mothers of clinically anxious children reported that their children were

more difficult and fearful in their first two years of life. Clinically anxious children were

described as being more likely to cry, have difficulties sleeping, experience pain and gas, have

greater fears of strangers and the dark, and have greater difficulties setting into day care and

school as compared to their nonanxious counterparts. However, the authors found no significant

differences between anxiety disorders. Other studies have found strong relations between anxiety

disorders – although not specifically GAD – and the temperamental dimensions of behavioral

inhibition (see review by Degnan, Almas, & Fox, 2010). The term behavioural inhibition has

been used to describe individuals who are overly fearful and reticent when confronted with novel

situations and people (Kagan, 1994). Some of these studies have demonstrated that behavioural

inhibition is highly predictive of psychopathology, particularly anxiety disorders (Biederman,

Rosenbaum, Bolduc-Murphy, Faraone, Chaloff, Hirshfeld, et al. 1993; Schwartz, Snidman,

Kagan, 1999). For example, Biedeman and colleagues (Biederman et al., 1993) found that

Page 59: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

50

children identified as behaviourally inhibited at 21 months of age are more likely than

uninhibited children to develop an anxiety disorder 5 to 10 years later.

Temperamental characteristics have been deemed neither sufficient nor necessary to

determine an anxiety or GAD diagnosis (Kertz & Woodruff-Borden, 2011). For example, in the

aforementioned study by Rettew and colleagues (Rettew et al., 2006), the authors found that a

significant number of individuals (e.g., half of the children participants) with high harm

avoidance scores did not qualify for a GAD diagnosis, suggesting that while there are strong

associations between harm avoidance and GAD, high levels of harm avoidance are not decisive

in the formation of clinically significant anxiety symptoms. Other research has also shown that

approximately one-half to two-thirds of behaviourally inhibited children do not develop

clinically significant anxiety (Prior, Smart, Sanson, & Oberklaid, 2000; Schwartz et al., 1999).

Temperament appears to be one of many other important factors that interact to influence the

development of anxiety. Thus, worry is likely the result of a complex interaction among

biological, psychological and environmental factors, out of which attachment exert a meaningful

influence.

From Insecure Attachment to GAD and Worry: The Roles of Negative Self-organizations

and Emotion Dysregulation

As shown in the previous section, the literature has presented a consistent picture of the

associations between insecure attachment and GAD as well as worry. It has also highlighted

possible mediating mechanisms that could help explain those associations, such as emotion

dysregulation, interpersonal problems, and information processing biases. While existing models

of GAD and worry have identified attachment experiences and those possible mediating

mechanisms to be important in the development of GAD and worry, the EFT model explicitly

Page 60: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

51

articulated how attachment injuries can give rise to GAD and worry in its formulation. The EFT

model suggests that insecure attachment experiences may lead to GAD and worry through the

construction of negative self organizations and the inability of the individual to regulate

emotions (Watson & Goldman, 2012; Watson & Greenberg, 2015; Watson & Greenberg, 2015,

in press; Watson, 2015). Below is a review of the literature concerning negative self-

organizations and emotion regulation.

Negative Self-organizations

The EFT model of GAD and worry posits that one of the ways in which attachment

insecurity contributes to the development of GAD and worry is by engendering in individuals a

vulnerable sense of self. Watson and colleagues (Watson & Greenberg, 2012; Watson &

Greenberg, 2015, in press; Watson, 2015) described a vulnerable self-organization as involving

primarily, among other elements, a negative self-concept, a sense that the self is incompetent to

cope with challenges, and negative ways of relating to self and others. On the basis of this

formulation, this dissertation operationalized negative self-organization as encompassing low

self-esteem, a negative problem orientation, and low differentiation of self.

It must be noted here that Watson and colleagues (Watson & Greenberg, 2012; Watson &

Greenberg, 2015, in press; Watson, 2015) stress the important of viewing the self as a process,

rather than a “thing.” In such a view, a negative self organization is much more than just holding

low self-esteem, a negative problem orientation, and low differentiation of the self. It is a

complex, dynamic process composed of a set of organizations – generated by maladaptive

emotion schemes – which organize themselves at any moment according to cognitive-affective

processes activated by internal and external stimuli (Watson & Greenberg, in press). Because

Page 61: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

52

this process-structural perspective on the self can be difficult to capture in research using

measures that assess “static” constructs, this dissertation focuses on low self-esteem, negative

problem orientation, and low differentiation of self as variables that resemble part of the

attributes of a negative self organization articulated by the EFT model. Below is a discussion of

these constructs along with their relationship with insecure attachment and GAD as well as

worry.

Self-esteem. Self-esteem is one of the most researched constructs associated with the self

(Baumeister, 1997; Brown, 2014). Self-esteem has been defined as the “degree to which (we)

hold attitudes of acceptance or rejection toward (ourselves)” (Rosenberg, 1962, p.135) and a

subjective “evaluation of our worthiness as individuals” (Neff, 2011, p.1). It represents the

cognitive-affective evaluative component of our self-concept and it involves the concepts of self-

respect, self-liking, and self-acceptance (Rosenberg, 1965). Developmental theorists posit that

self-esteem is formed at an early age and is largely shaped by early interactions with caregivers

and peers (Harter, 2006). For the most part, children appear to learn to evaluate themselves

through the information they receive from important figures about their competencies and

acceptability and by comparing their characteristics and abilities with those of important others.

From an attachment perspective, children with secure attachment to caregivers are

expected to view themselves as worthy of love and care, and hence to have higher self-esteem in

comparison to children with insecure attachment bonds. Attachment theory also asserts that

attachment security is a source of support for self-exploration and for the mastering of

developmental tasks which promote feelings of competence and efficacy, and thus should be

linked to high self-esteem levels in children in securely attached dyads (Bowlby, 1969/1982,

1973). Studies have indeed confirmed that individuals reporting secure attachment with their

Page 62: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

53

caregivers experience high levels of self-worth (e.g., Armsden & Greenberg, 1987; Papini &

Rogman, 1992). In addition, secure individuals have been found to have a more positive,

coherent and well-organized self-structure (Mikulincer, 1995).

Brown (2014) argues that the affective quality of attachment dyads is particularly

relevant for the development of self-esteem and how one feels about him- or herself. He asserts

that feelings are at the very core of self-identity such that self-esteem is not purely the product of

cognitive self-evaluations, but rather a more general state of feeling or affection for the self

stemming from infancy, before cognitive capacities have developed to form a representation of

the self. Brown differentiates between two types of self-esteem – self-esteem originating from

affirmations of important others and self-esteem stemming from mastery – and argues that the

affective quality of attachment dyads has an influence on both types of self-esteem. He posits

that both types of self-esteem are rooted in the security of safe, warm, loving attachments in

infancy and early childhood. He notes that self-esteem developed from affirmation leads to a

sense of belonging whereas self-esteem that stems from mastery leads to feelings of joy and a

cognitive assessment of competence.

Research has shown that individuals with high self-esteem tend to have clear and

consistent self-knowledge, whereas individuals with low self-esteem have confused, fractured

and inconsistent views of themselves (Brown, 2014, Baumeister, 1997). Individuals with low

self-esteem have been found to be insecure about their ability to achieve success regardless of

their objective abilities and they tend to experience failure as more devastating than individuals

with high self-esteem (Brown, 2014; Baumeister, 1997). In a study investigating reactions to

failure among high and low self-esteem individuals, Brown and Dutton (1995) showed that while

people with low and high self-esteem both feel positive emotions and pride in response to

Page 63: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

54

success, and sadness in response to failure, only people with low self-esteem experience shame

and humiliation about the self in response to failure. The authors also found that people with low

self-esteem, regardless of competency levels, were motivated to avoid risks and protect

themselves from failure, as the experience of failure appeared more painful for them than for

those with high self-esteem (Brown & Dutton, 1995).

The role of self-esteem in the development and maintenance of psychopathology has

been studied mostly in relation to depression where poor self-esteem has been documented to not

only be strongly related to depressed mood but also to contribute to it (Orth & Robins, 2013;

Sowislo & Orth, 2013). The relationship between self-esteem and anxiety has only rarely been

investigated (Sowislo & Orth, 2013), and no study has looked specifically at the relationship

between low self-esteem and worry. In a recent meta-analytic study of 18 longitudinal studies on

the relationship between self-esteem and anxiety (as measured by general self-report measures

such as the Beck Anxiety Inventory), Sowislo and Orth (2003) found that unlike the findings for

the relationship between self-esteem and depression where the vulnerability model (i.e., low self-

esteem contributing to depression) is consistently supported, the relationship between self-

esteem and anxiety is symmetric and reciprocal, with small but significant, prospective effects

moving in both directions (i.e., low self-esteem contributing to anxiety and anxiety contributing

to low self-esteem).

Sowislo and Orth (2003) do not specify how low self-esteem might be related to different

forms of anxiety (e.g., social anxiety, worry, panic, phobias). However, they expect that low self-

esteem may be a stronger vulnerability factor for anxiety disorders that are highly comorbid with

depression, such as generalized anxiety disorder (Sowislo & Orth, 2013). Although the present

study makes no claims about the directionality of the relationship between low self-esteem and

Page 64: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

55

GAD as well as worry, it seeks to examine how these three constructs relate, and to investigate

their relationships with attachment. To date, no studies have conducted such examination.

Negative Problem Orientation. Another way in which attachment insecurity appears to

contribute to the development of GAD and worry is by engendering in individuals a sense that

they are incompetent to cope with adversity. Theoretical formulations from cognitive, EFT and

attachment perspectives all postulate that cognitive-affective processes – including beliefs about

self, others and the world as well as a felt sense of self as it interacts in the world – are largely

formed out of a child’s repeated experiences with their attachment figures and would have an

influence on the etiology of GAD and worry (Borkovec et al., 2004; Cassidy, 2009; Watson &

Greenberg, in press). If the child has experienced insecure attachment with the caregiver, he or

she would be expected to develop a view and sense of the world as a dangerous place and come

to believe that feared events are more likely to occur than they really are. Such a child would

also develop faulty beliefs and a negative sense of herself and, as a result, underestimate her

coping resources or view herself as incompetent to deal with perceived threat. This child would

thus be more likely to experience anxiety, including worry.

Cognitive theorists have for the most part emphasized the role of logical errors or faulty

thinking in the worry process (Borkovec et al., 2004; Dugas et al., 2004, 2005; Leahy, 2005;

Wells, 2006). They have argued that worry is driven by cognitive errors of probability

overestimation (i.e., assuming negative events are more severe and probable than they actually

are) and catastrophizing (i.e., assuming that outcomes will be less manageable than they actually

may be; Provencher, Freeston, Dugas, & Ladouceur, 2000). In addition, and as discussed in

previous sections, cognitive theorists have proposed that worry is linked to distorted beliefs

Page 65: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

56

about its usefulness (e.g., “worry will help me cope”) and a preference for negative outcomes

rather than uncertain ones (Dugas & Robichaud, 2007).

Berenbaum and colleagues (Berenbaum, Thompson, & Pomerantz, 2007) also found

support for the relationship between worrying and perceived threats and added that the perceived

costs of feared situations are related to worry severity. The authors asked a hundred and ninety-

seven college students to report their most common concerns and to rate how probable they

thought those concerns would actually happen. They also asked students to rate how upsetting

they would feel if their concerns actually happened. Findings from their study revealed that

individuals with high levels of worry exhibited high levels of perceived threat. Moreover, these

individuals also reported high levels of perceived distress associated with their concerns. The

opposite pattern of results was found for individuals with low levels of worry. The authors

concluded that chronic worriers tend to perceive their threats as more probable as well as costlier

(i.e., causing greater distress) than individuals who experience low levels of worry.

Cognitive researchers have also proposed that worry is associated with decreased

confidence in one’s ability to cope effectively with problems (Davey, 1994; Davey, Hampton,

Farrell, & Davidson, 1992; Dugas, Freeston, & Ladouceur, 1997). For instance, Davey (1994)

asked eighty-two university students to complete measures of worry and problem solving

abilities and confidence. He found that correlations between high levels of worry and problem

abilities were nonsignificant. However, increased worry was highly associated with poor

problem-solving confidence and poor perceived control over the problem-solving process (r =

.58 and .49, respectively, p < .001). According to Davey, “pathological worrying is not generated

by poor problem-solving skills per se, but by an individual’s lack of belief in the adequacy of

their problem-solving abilities and their ability to implement solutions” (Davey, 1994, p. 329).

Page 66: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

57

Building from that position, Dugas and colleagues (Dugas & Robichaud, 2007; Koerner

& Dugas, 2006) argue that individuals with chronic worry hold a negative problem orientation in

response to problems. Negative problem orientation refers to a set of dysfunctional attitudes and

perceptions related to the problem-solving process (Koerner & Dugas, 2006). Specifically,

negative problem orientation includes perceptions of problems as threats, a lack of self-

confidence in one’s problem-solving abilities, and negative views of the outcome of one’s

problem-solving efforts (D’Zurilla, Nezu, & Maydeu-Olivares, 1998 as cited in Koerner &

Dugas, 2006). Dugas and Robichaud (2007) note that negative problem orientation is one of the

constituents of the problem-solving process, which also include problems solving skills (e.g.,

defining a problem and formulating problem-solving goals, generating alternative solutions, and

choosing a solution). According to the authors, worrying is largely unrelated to this second

component of the problem-solving process which involves knowledge of problems-solving skills

(Dugas et al., 1997). They argue that individuals with chronic worry do know how to find

solutions to their problems. However, they posit that these individuals have difficulties arriving

at effective solutions because they view problems as threats, doubt their coping abilities, and are

pessimistic about the problem-solving outcome.

A growing number of studies have revealed a correlation between levels of worry and

negative problem orientation, supporting the position that individuals with chronic worry endorse

a negative problem orientation (Davey, 2006; Dugas & Robichaud, 2007). In one particular

study, using university students and self-report questionnaires, negative problem orientation was

found to make a specific contribution to the prediction of worry, such that the relationship

between worry and negative problem orientation was independent of other related personality

characteristics (e.g., pessimism, low self-mastery, and neuroticism; Robichaud & Dugas, 2005).

Page 67: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

58

In addition, negative problem orientation was found to be a stronger prediction of worry than of

depression, still accounting for 5.6% of the variance in worry scores as compared to 1.6% of the

variance in depression scores when all other related personality characteristics were taken into

account. Based on this finding, Robichaud and Dugas argue that negative problem orientation is

a sensitive and specific marker of chronic worry.

Studies with clinical (i.e., GAD) participants also demonstrated positive and strong

correlations between negative problem orientation and worry (Dugas et al., 1997; Dugas, Letarte,

Rheaume, Freeston, & Ladouceur, 1995). Similarly, studies using interview procedures instead

of self-report measure also yielded similar results. For instance, Robichaud and colleagues

(Robichaud, Dugas, & Radomsky, 2006 as cited in Dugas & Robichaud, 2007) asked

participants to describe in an interview setting the problems solving steps they would take to

resolve a real-life problem. They found that negative problem orientation interacted with

participants’ level of intolerance of uncertainty to determine participants’ ability to deal with

their problems. Results revealed that for participants with intolerance of uncertainty, negative

problem orientation was related to poor performance of problem-solving steps for their

problems. Given that individuals with GAD typically have high levels of intolerance for

uncertainty, Dugas and Robichaud (2007) argue that negative problem orientation may have a

significant role on these individuals’ ability to cope with difficulties.

Although researchers make no inferences about the etiology of negative problem

orientation, the set of beliefs that constitute this concept is very similar to the beliefs that Bowlby

postulated would originate from insecure attachment dyads (i.e., concerning a child’s perceptions

of problems or threats in her environment and her confidence to cope effectively with them;

Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1973). In addition, negative problem orientation appears to constitute one

Page 68: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

59

aspect of negative self-organizations, as proposed by the EFT model of GAD and worry,

characterized by the individual’s lack of confidence in his or her ability to cope with the

demands of life (Watson & Greenberg, in press). This study will investigate the relationships

among negative problem orientation, insecure attachment, worry, and GAD.

Differentiation of Self. The EFT model suggests that another feature of negative self

organizations is negative ways of relating to self and others (Watson & Greenberg, 2015, in

press). The intra- and inter-personal difficulties experienced by individuals with GAD may be

explained by these individuals’ difficulties managing relational distance (i.e., emotional

proximity and distance within relationships). These difficulties most likely originated in early

interpersonal contexts (either marked by enmeshment or neglect) in which individuals with GAD

were expected to put aside their needs and prematurely do the planning and caretaking of

themselves and/or their caregivers (Cassidy, 1995). These individuals may have not felt

sufficiently safe to distance themselves from their caregivers while maintaining their attachment

bond. This in turn may have compromised the development of their emotion regulation strategies

and their emerging sense of self as independent agents in their lives (Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist, &

Target, 2002; Schore, 1994).

Individuals with GAD may have learned to either reduce relational distance (i.e., having

no interpersonal boundaries) by taking on their attachment figure’s needs above of theirs or to

drastically increase relational distance (i.e., cutting off bonds) by physical and/or emotional

isolation in order to earn love and acceptance in their attachment dyads (Cassidy, 1995). As they

matured, these individuals may have had difficulty to differentiate themselves from important

others and to respond autonomously to their environments without using their relationships as an

emotion regulation process (Cassidy, 1995). As a result, they may have learned to negotiate

Page 69: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

60

interpersonal closeness and distance by moving emotionally and reactively into fusion (e.g.,

being intrusive or over-nurturing) or cut off (e.g., being cold) in important relationships.

This concept of relational distance has most often been discussed under Murray Bowen’s

family systems theory (Bowen 1978; Kerr & Bowen, 1988). In developing his theory, Bowen

(1978) coined the term differentiation of the self to refer to an individual’s ability to function

autonomously in the world while also remaining emotionally connected to a significant

relationship system (typically the family system; Kerr & Bowen, 1988). According to Bowen,

the process of differentiation requires that individuals separate themselves from emotional

attachments in their family systems without cutting off completely from family members. He

asserted that differentiation of self falls along a continuum from low (undifferentiated) to high

(differentiated) functioning. Individuals at the lower end of the continuum tend to have

difficulties negotiating and navigating emotional and physical closeness, and, particularly under

stress, may lose their sense of self in relationships or the connection to important others (Kerr &

Bowen, 1988). Conversely, individuals at the higher end of the continuum tend to experience

increased intimacy in interpersonal relationships along with a clear establishment of self-identity

boundaries and engagement in goal directed behaviours (Kerr & Bowen, 1988).

Bowen suggested that differentiation of self include both intrapsychic and interpersonal

aspects. The intrapsychic aspects involve cognitive and emotional processes and an individual’s

ability to distinguish and properly use both these processes (e.g., intellect and feelings) to

respond to their environments. The interpersonal aspects, on the other hand, involve an

individual’s ability to negotiate being close yet separate in important relationships. Building

from Bowen’s theory, Skowron and Friedlander (1998) further articulated the factors that

theoretically make up differentiation of self as Bowen proposed. They operationalized

Page 70: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

61

differentiation of self as having four domains: the “I position, emotional cutoff, fusion with

others, and emotional reactivity.

The “I” position is believed to be seen in more differentiated individuals who have a

defined sense of self and are capable of setting and keeping relationship boundaries, such that

they can adhere thoughtfully to their convictions even when pressured to do otherwise.

Individuals experiencing emotional cutoffs with others feel threatened by intimacy and adopt

behavioural defenses (e.g., overfunctioning, denial, distancing) for fear of interpersonal

engulfment. Those who are fused cannot separate themselves from their need for the acceptance

and approval of others. They tend to become overinvolved with others and attempt to over-

identify with closed others. Emotionally reactive individuals are those guided by their

emotionality. They tend to respond to the environment with emotional lability or

hypersensitivity. Skowron and Friedlander argue that high differentiation of self would be

manifested by the ability to take an I-position and make an intellectual decision even when

distressing emotions are present, rather than emotionally react to others and seek to cut them off

or to fuse with them.

Bowen proposed that an individual’s level of differentiation of self would have a number

of psychological consequences, for both interpersonal and intrapersonal adjustment (Bowen,

1976; Bowen & Kerr, 1988). Indeed, previous research has shown that differentiation of self is

associated with stress and coping (Murdock & Gore, 2004), separation anxiety (Peleg, Halaby, &

Whaby, 2006), college student stress and adjustment (Skowron et al., 2004), adult well-being

(Skowron, Holmes, & Sabatelli, 2003), social anxiety (Peleg-Popko, 2002), and marital

adjustment (Skowron, 2000).

Page 71: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

62

Although very few in number, studies have also demonstrated associations between

differentiation of self and attachment experiences (Skowron & Dendy, 2004; Thorberg & Lyvers,

2006). For instance, Skowron and Dendy (2004) examined whether lower differentiation of self

would be associated with greater adult attachment insecurity (i.e., attachment anxiety and

avoidance). In their study, more than two hundred adults completed online self-report measures

of adult attachment and differentiation of self. Their results confirmed significant relations

between low differentiation of self and insecure attachment. Hierarchical multiple regression

findings revealed that 40% of the variability in attachment anxiety scores and 62% of the

variability in attachment avoidance scores were shared with differentiation of self scores,

respectively. Greater attachment anxiety and avoidance were both associated with greater

emotional reactivity, emotional cutoff, fusion with others, and problems taking an “I” position

(Skowron & Dendy, 2004). No research to date directly tested Bowen’s proposition about

differentiation of self in relation to GAD and worry.

Emotion Dysregulation

Research accumulated thus far is consistent with the EFT proposition that emotional

dysregulation is one of the major risk factors for chronic worry and GAD (Mennin et al., 2002,

2005, Mennin et al., 2004). There is also ample evidence in the psychological literature

documenting the link between attachment injuries and emotion regulation skills (Cassidy, 1994;

Fuendeling, 1998; Mikulincer et al., 2003; Schore & Schore, 2008). Emotion regulation has been

defined as the “ability to tolerate, be aware of, put into words, and use emotions adaptively, to

regulate distress and promote needs and goals” (Elliot, Watson, Goldman, & Greenberg, 2004, p.

32). Watson and colleagues (Elliot, Watson, Goldman, & Greenberg, 2004; Kennedy-Moore &

Page 72: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

63

Watson, 1999; Watson & Prosser, 2004) suggest that the effective modulation of emotions

involves different but interrelated cognitive-affective processes.

For emotion regulation to occur effectively, Watson and colleagues (Elliot et al., 2004;

Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 1999; Watson & Prosser, 2004) state that individuals are required to:

(a) become aware of emotional arousal, (b) label their emotional experience accurately, (c)

consider their emotional experience valued and acceptable, (d) modulate their level of arousal

and expression in ways that enhance their functioning to meet their needs and goals, and (e)

reflect on their emotional experience to integrate it into other aspects of their selves and

environment. The key idea within this model is that the ability to engage in each of these

processes is thought to lead to effective emotion regulation. Conversely, skills deficits in

emotional processing and/or motivated efforts aimed at altering or suppressing one’s level of

engagement in these processes are expected to lead to disruptions and strains in the regulation of

emotional experiences (Elliot et al., 2004; Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 1999; Watson & Prosser,

2004).

Building on the Process Model of Affect Regulation developed by Watson and colleagues

(Elliot et al., 2004; Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 1999; Watson & Prosser, 2004), Rodrigues and

Watson (2011) have provided a practical, simplified illustration of how insecure attachment

experiences may lead to the development of poor emotion regulation through either heightening

or suppression of affective states. Figures 1 and 2 show how insecure attachment, characterized

by the two dimensions of attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance (Brennan, Clark, &

Shaver, 1998; Fraley & Waller, 1998), leads to the poor regulation of cognitive-affective

processes consisting of emotion regulation.

Page 73: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

64

Specifically, figure 1 shows that anxiously attached individuals having learned to

modulate their affective experiences from a perspective of vigilance and fear, tend to limit their

affect regulation skills to heightened negative affectivity, selective and/or distorted accessibility

to affect-related information, and dysfunctional rumination. Figure 2 shows that individuals with

avoidant attachment style, on the other hand, having learned to modulate their affective

experiences from a position of mistrust and self-reliance, tend to miss the adaptive aspects of

affect regulation by blocking affective experiences from awareness, denying affective

experiencing, repressing or suppressing negative emotional memories, and distancing themselves

from distressing cues. Broadly, these illustrations demonstrate that if a sense of security has

never been fostered within individuals, they remain in a state of hypervigilance or self-reliance

working to avoid what they perceive to be emotionally overwhelming stimuli.

Figure 2. Process Model of Affect Regulation in anxious attachment (Rodrigues & Watson,

2011).

Deficits in emotional processing and distorted ways of dealing with emotions Vigilance and Fear

Awareness Labelling Acceptance Modulation of

Arousal and Expression

Reflection

Poor Affect

Regulation

Heightened Negative

Emotionality

Difficulty Differentiating

Feelings

View emotions as

overwhelming/unmanageable

Modulates emotions

poorly Self-blame and

rumination strategies

Cognitive Rigidity

Selective

accessibility to emotional material

Page 74: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

65

Figure 3. Process Model of Affect Regulation in avoidant attachment (Rodrigues & Watson,

2011).

The association between emotion regulation and attachment depicted above has been well

documented in research (Fuendeling, 1998; Lopez & Brennan, 2000; Mikulincer & Shaver,

2007). The psychological literature, both theoretical and empirical, shows that there are

consistent patterns of association between adult attachment styles and emotion regulation. For

the most part, securely attached individuals have been found to display more adaptive emotion

regulation skills than insecure attached individuals as evidenced by their increased ability to

experience, symbolize, acknowledge, and express their affective experiences openly and flexibly

(Fuendeling, 1998; Lopez & Brennan, 2000; Meins, Harris-Waller, & Loyd, 2008; Mikulincer,

1997, 1998; Mikulincer & Orbach, 1995; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Montebarocci, Codispoti,

Baldaro, & Rossi, 2004; Searle & Meara, 1999).

Deficits in emotional processing and distorted ways of dealing with emotions Self-reliance and Mistrust

Awareness Labelling Acceptance Modulation of

Arousal and Expression

Reflection

Poor Affect

Regulation

Block affective experiences

from awareness

No consistent findings from the literature

Suppress, deny affective

experiences

Modulates emotions

poorly

Cognitive, behavioural,

emotional avoidance

Cognitive Rigidity

Selective

accessibility to emotional material

Page 75: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

66

Mikulincer and colleagues (Mikulincer, 1997, 1998; Mikulincer & Orbach, 1995;

Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007), in particular, have developed a consistent program of research

demonstrating the associations between emotion regulation and attachment organization. For

instance, Mikulincer and Orbach (1995) found that securely versus insecurely attached

individuals differ in terms of intensity of, attention given to and accessibility to affective

information. In their study, Israeli students were asked to recall early experiences of anger,

anxiety, sadness, and happiness while retrieval times were recorded. After recalling emotional

memories, subjects were then asked to rate the intensity of emotions felt in each recalled event.

Mikulincer and Orbach found that students with attachment avoidance were the least efficient at

recalling sad and anxious memories (i.e., they had the longest retrieval times), while students

with anxious attachment showed the highest accessibility for negative memories (i.e., they had

the shortest retrieval times). Securely attached students fell in between the two insecure

attachment groups in terms of accessibility of sad and anxious memories. Securely attached

students also took longer to recall negative rather than positive emotional memories, whereas

anxiously attached students took longer to recall positive rather than negative emotional

memories. Moreover, avoidant students rated emotional memories as less intense than did secure

individuals, whereas anxious students reported having experienced stronger emotions than secure

students. Memories of securely attached students fell in between the two insecure attachment

groups.

In another study, Mikulincer (1997) found that secure individuals were more tolerant of

ambiguity, more curious, and more open to new information when making social judgments than

individuals with insecure attachment styles. In contrast, insecurely attached individuals tended to

be uncomfortable with uncertainty and to rely on prior knowledge (to the exclusion of new

Page 76: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

67

information) when making social judgments. Mikulincer and Arad (1999) extended these

findings by examining attachment style differences in cognitive openness within the context of

close relationships. They found that when the participants’ descriptions of their partners’

behaviours were congruent with their expectations, both secure and insecure individuals did not

differ in cognitive openness. However, when information regarding their partners’ behaviours

was novel and incongruent with their expectations, secure individuals displayed more cognitive

openness than insecure individuals. Specifically, when their partners’ behaviours were

incongruent, secure individuals were more likely to revise and change their descriptions of their

partners than their insecure counterparts. Altogether, insecurely attached individuals appeared to

have more difficulty reflecting, processing and integrating incongruent information about

themselves and their environments than securely attached individuals.

In regard to the association between emotion regulation and worry, it has been shown that

deficits in emotion regulation contribute to high worry (Mennin et al., 2002, 2005). Borkovec

and colleagues (Borkovec 1994; Borkovec et al., 2004) explain that the verbal-linguistic nature

of worry interferes with all the cognitive-affective processes of effective emotion regulation.

According to the authors, worry denies the individual full access to the experience of fear (with

all its referents – behavioural, cognitive, and emotional – including mental images and

unpleasant autonomic activation). Mennin and colleagues (Mennin et al., 2004) go further to

suggest that worry represents a “multi-component syndrome” involving both significant deficits

in emotional processing – characterized by increased sensitivity to negative emotion (e.g.,

experiencing negative emotions more intensively and as more aversive) – as well as motivated

efforts to control or supress emotional experience through over-reliance on “verbal thoughts” or

cognitive activity. Thus, from both the cognitive avoidance and emotion dysregulation

Page 77: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

68

perspectives of worry, individuals with chronic worry are believed to experience disruptions in

all steps in the process of effective emotion regulation. These disruptions interfere with the

experiencing of emotions which impact the individual’s well-being and serves to reinforce and

maintain their anxiety responses.

Empirical studies support the position that individuals with chronic worry have

difficulties with the cognitive-affective processes involved in the effective regulation of

emotions (Mennin et al., 2002, 2005; Roemer et al., 2005). As noted earlier, Mennin and

colleagues (2005) demonstrated that individuals with chronic worry report having more intense

emotional experiences, having difficulty naming and understanding emotions, difficulty

accepting their emotional experience (especially negative emotions), and greater difficulty self-

soothing after experiencing negative emotions than nonanxious individuals. Correspondingly,

Roemer and colleagues (2005) found that individuals with chronic worry exhibited greater

endorsement of their emotions as threatening and unwanted, which in turn was associated with

greater fear and avoidance of the experience of emotion. Individuals with GAD have also been

shown to view their worry as threatening and as having difficulties accepting the experiencing of

anxiety, which perpetuates a cycle of meta-worry or "worry about worry" (Wells, 2004).

Despite these associations between attachment and emotion regulation as well as emotion

regulation and worry, very little research has investigated the relationship among all variables.

There is one recent study by Marganska and colleagues (Marganska, Gallagher, & Miranda,

2013) which examined the relationship among emotion dysregulation, insecure attachment, and

GAD. Using self-report measures in a sample of over two hundred college students, the authors

found that anxious attachment styles (i.e., preoccupied and fearful avoidant attachment styles)

predicted symptoms of GAD. In addition, lack of acceptance of negative emotions, perceived

Page 78: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

69

limited access to effective emotion regulation strategies, and inability to refrain from impulsive

behaviours when distressed, fully mediated the associations between anxious attachment and

GAD symptomatology. Marganska’s study provides preliminary evidence that emotion

dysregulation skills (related to emotional acceptance and modulation) mediate the relationship

between attachment anxiety and worry. As noted earlier, however, the association between

attachment anxiety and worry has not been consistently demonstrated, as studies have also found

worry to be associated with dismissing attachment styles (Bifulco et al., 2006; Mickelson et al.,

1997). More research is necessary to fully understand attachment-related differences underlying

worry. The current study hopes to investigate the relationship between attachment and emotion

regulation in adults reporting worry.

The Current Study

The main purpose of this dissertation is to investigate the propositions articulated by the

EFT model of GAD which views the disorder as reflecting negative self-organizations and

emotion dysregulation as a function of insecure attachment experiences. To this end, this

dissertation operationalized some of the concepts of the EFT model, as outlined in figure 1, by

focusing on early attachment experiences and attachment styles as representing aspects of

emotion schematic memories and attachment injuries; low differentiation of self, low self-

esteem, and negative problem orientation as representing aspects of negative self-organizations

and a vulnerable sense of self; and emotion dysregulation. This dissertation sought to examine

the relationships among these variables and their contributions to worry and GAD.

Following on the suggestion of previous research demonstrating that worry, while

characteristic of GAD, exists independently of GAD and therefore would benefit from being

Page 79: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

70

studied in its own right (Hirsch et al., 2013; Ruscio, 2002; Ruscio & Borkovec, 2004; Ruscio et

al., 2001), this dissertation also sought to investigate the relationships among the aforementioned

variables and their contributions to worry, separately from GAD.

Hypothesis

1. Negative early attachment experiences would be significantly and positively associated

with GAD and worry.

2. Negative early attachment experiences would be significantly and positively associated

with insecure adult attachment style.

3. Negative early attachment experiences would be significantly and positively associated

with emotion dysregulation, low self-esteem, negative problem orientation, and low

differentiation of self in adulthood.

4. Emotion dysregulation, low self-esteem, negative problem orientation, and low

differentiation of self would mediate the relationships between insecure adult attachment

and GAD as well as adult attachment and worry.

Page 80: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

71

Chapter 2: Method

Participants

The sample consisted of 218 adults, 178 (81.7%) females and 40 (18.3%) males. Their

ages ranged from 18 to 63, with mean age of 30.76 (SD = 10.60). Their ethnic composition was

60.1% Caucasian, 13.8% East Asian, 11% South Asian, 4.1 % African, 3.7 % Latin/Hispanic,

1.8% Middle Eastern, and 5.5 % other. The majority of participants were single (60.1%) while

the remaining were married (22.5%), in a domestic partnership (13.8%), divorced (2.3%), or

separated (1.4%). In terms of their education level, 32.7% of participants had a university degree,

24.8% had a post-graduate degree, 21.6% had a high school diploma or GED, 13.8% had a

college degree, and 2.8% had completed grade school. Table 1 summarizes participants’

demographics information.

Data were originally collected on 326 adults, but as described above, only 218

participants completed the study. One hundred and eight participants (77 females and 31 males)

were excluded from analysis due to noncompletion (e.g., did not complete any measures beyond

the demographics section of the online survey). There was a significant gender difference

between participants who completed the study and those who were excluded from further

analyses [χ2 (1) = 3.96, p < .05], with more women completing the study. There were no

significant differences between participants who completed the online questionnaires and those

who were excluded from further analyses on remaining demographic variables: age, t(324) = -

.96, p = .34; ethnicity, χ2 (6) = 5.19, p = .52; marital status, χ2 (5) = 10.39, p = .06; and

education, χ2 (4) = 4.86, p = .30.

Page 81: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

72

Table 1

Demographic Data for Sample (N=218)

Characteristic N %

Gender

Male 40 18.3

Female 178 81.7

Race/Ethnicity

African 9 4.1

Caucasian 131 60.1

East Asian 30 13.8

Latin/Hispanic 8 3.7

Middle Eastern 4 1.8

South Asian 24 11

Other 12 5.5

Marital Status

Single 131 60.1

Married 49 22.5

Domestic Partnership 30 13.8

Separated 3 1.4

Divorced 5 2.3

Education

Grade School 6 2.8

High School Diploma or GED 47 21.6

College or Trade School 30 13.8

University Undergraduate Degree 81 37.2

Post Graduate Degree 54 24.8

Participants who agreed to participate in the study completed a series of questionnaires

assessing demographic information, memories and perceptions of attachment experiences with

caregivers, early life filial responsibility, adult attachment, differentiation of self, self-esteem,

negative problem orientation, emotion regulation, worry, and generalized anxiety.

Page 82: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

73

Measure Assessing Demographic Information.

The following questionnaire was created to assess participants’ demographic information.

Demographics Questionnaire. The demographics questionnaire (see Appendix B)

gathered general participant information such as age, gender, ethnicity, years of education, and

relationship status.

Measures Assessing Attachment Experiences.

The following measures were used to assess early attachment experiences (i.e., memories

and perceptions of childhood experiences with caregivers) and adult attachment style.

Egna Minuen Betraffande Uppfostram – Short Form (s-EMBU, Swedish acronym

for My Memories of Upbringing; Arrindell et al., 1999). The s-EMBU is a 23-item self-report

retrospective attachment inventory, designed to measure adults’ perceptions of their parents’

rearing style. The s-EMBU is a short version of the original 81-item EMBU (Perris, Jacobsson,

Lindstrom, von Knorring, & Perris, 1980) and assesses perceptions of parental rejection,

emotional warmth, and overprotection. Items are answered on a four-point Likert scale with

reference to each parent separately, ranging from 1 (“No, never”) to 4 (“Yes, most of the time”).

The rejection subscale refers to parental rearing that is characterized by physical punishment,

rejection of the child as an individual, hostility, derogation, and lack of consideration for points

of view and needs of the child. The emotional warmth subscale refers to an upbringing

characterized by warmth and loving attention, parent helping the child, aiding through problems,

and child’s ability to confide in the parent. The overprotection subscale refers to parental

behaviour indicative of excessive protection from negative experiences, high degree of

intrusiveness, high expectancy to know all about the child, and imposition of strict regulations

Page 83: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

74

and high standards of accomplishments. The s-EMBU is a valid and reliable instrument for

retrospective assessment of parental rearing behavior, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .75 to

.88 (Arrindell et al., 2001). In the current sample the s-EMBU was found to be reliable, proving

good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .87 to .94).

Filial Responsibility Scale – Adult (FRS-A; Jurkovic, Thirkield, Morrell, 2001).

Drawing from the original Parentification Questionnaire (Sessions and Jurkovic, 1982), the FRS-

A is a 60-item questionnaire that measures the subjective experience of caretaking responsibility

and fairness between child and parent(s) in the family of origin. The first part of the FRS-A,

consisting of 30 items, assesses for past levels of caretaking responsibilities and fairness in the

respondent's family of origin. The second part of the FRS-A, consisting of 30 items as well,

assesses for current levels of caretaking responsibilities and fairness in the respondent’s family

of origin. Part I of the questionnaire include items pertaining to the respondent’s “childhood

years” or the time when they were growing up in their families. The items in Part II refer to the

present, that is, the respondent’s “current” experiences in their families of origin. Because the

focus of this study was on experiences in childhood, participants only completed Part I of the

FRS-A which assesses past perceptions during childhood. Part I of the FRS-A is divided in three

subscales: instrumental caregiving (e.g., “I often did the family’s laundry”), emotional

caregiving (e.g., “I often felt like a referee in my family”), and unfairness (e.g., in my family, I

gave more than I received”). Past research supports the reliability and validity of the FRS-A

subscales, with internal consistency reliabilities reported ranging from .72 to .87 (Jurkovic et al.,

2001). In the current sample, the FRS-A was found to have good internal consistency

(Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .83 to .89).

Page 84: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

75

Three-category Attachment Measure (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). This is a 3-item

questionnaire designed to measure an individual’s attachment style. The three attachment styles

are: (1) avoidant, characterized as being afraid of intimacy, experiencing emotional highs and

lows during relationships, along with jealousy; (2) anxious/Ambivalent, characterized by strong

need for constant reciprocation and validation in intimate relationships, along with emotional

highs and lows; and (3) secure, characterized by friendly, trusting, and happy intimate

relationships. This measure assesses attachment qualitatively (categorically) and is forced-

choice. All three attachment styles are described briefly and respondents choose the style that fits

them best. The construct validity of this measures has been established in more than 30 studies

conducted since 1987 (see Shaver & Clark, 1994, and Shaver & Hazan, 1993, for reviews).

Measures Assessing Self-organization.

The following measures were used to assess self-organization, including a measure for

assessing self-concept (i.e., self-esteem), a measure for assessing a sense that the self is

incompetent to cope with challenges (i.e., negative problem orientation), and a measure for

assessing negative ways of relating to self and others (i.e., differentiation of self).

Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965). The RSES is a 10-item self-

report questionnaire that measures global self-esteem (e.g., “On the whole, I am satisfied with

myself”). Each item is rated on a four-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (“strongly agree”)

to 4 (“Strongly disagree”). The scale is considered a reliable and valid measure of global self-

esteem through all ages (Chiu, 1988; Gray-Little, Williams, & Hancock, 1997) and has

demonstrated good construct validity in correlations with a number of other measures of similar

constructs (Coopersmith, 1981; Crandall, 1973; Fleming & Courtney, 1984; Robins, Hendin, &

Page 85: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

76

Trzesniewski, 2001). The scale has high reliability including test-retest correlations in the range

of .82 to .88, and Cronbach's alpha for various samples in the range of .77 to .88 (Blascovich &

Tomaka, 1993; Robins, Hendin, & Trzesniewski, 2001; Rosenberg, 1986). In the current sample,

Cronbach’s alpha was at .90 indicating high reliability.

Negative Problem Orientation Questionnaire (NPOQ; Robichaud & Dugas, 2005).

The NPOQ is a 12-item measure that assesses the dysfunctional cognitive set of a negative

problem orientation. Participants rate each item on a 5-point Likert scale, according to how they

react or think when confronted with a problem. Sample items include ‘‘I see problems as a threat

to my well-being’’ and ‘‘I often see problems as bigger than they really are.’’ The NPOQ was

translated from the original French version of the measure, which was found to display excellent

internal consistency (α = .90), convergent and discriminant validity, and was unifactorial

(Gosselin, Pelletier, & Ladouceur, 2001). An initial psychometric evaluation of the English

version suggested that the NPOQ is also unifactorial, with excellent internal consistency

(Cronbach’s alpha = .92), high test–retest reliability (r = .80), and good convergent and

discriminant validity (Robichaud & Dugas, 2005). In the current sample, the NPOQ was found to

have good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .94)

Differentiation of Self Inventory - Revised (DSI-R; Skowron & Schmitt, 2003). The

DSI is a 46-item self-report measure based on Bowen’s (1976, 1978; Kerr & Bowen, 1988)

family systems theory. The DSI-R yields a total score as well as scores on four dimensions of

differentiation: emotional reactivity (ER), emotional cutoff (EC), fusion with others (FO), and I-

position (IP). ER refers to being unable to modulate intense emotional experience (e.g., ‘‘I’m

very sensitive to being hurt by others’’). EC assesses fear of intimacy and a tendency to

withdraw when stressed (e.g., ‘‘I tend to distance myself when people get too close to me’’). FO

Page 86: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

77

reflects the tendency to become overinvolved in relationships, taking on significant others’

values and beliefs without questioning (e.g., ‘‘I feel a need for approval from virtually everyone

in my life’’). Finally, the IP scale assesses the ability to maintain a clear sense of self in

interactions with others (e.g., ‘‘I usually do not change my behavior simply to please another

person’’). Items are rated on 6-point Likert-type scales. High scores represent greater

differentiation of self (i.e., lower emotional reactivity, lower emotional cutoff, less fusion with

others, but a greater ability to take an ‘‘I’’ position). The subscales have demonstrated good

convergent and divergent validity (Skowron & Schmitt, 2003). Construct validity for the DSI-R

was established in relation to measures of attachment security and individuation (Skowron &

Schmitt, 2003). Scores on the DSI-R has been predictive of marital adjustment among husbands

and wives, intergenerational intimacy, and levels of interpersonal problems (Skowron, Stanley,

& Shapiro, 2009). Internal consistency reliabilities reported by Skowron and Schmitt (2003)

range from .81 (IP) to .89 (ER), with a full-scale alpha of .92. In the current sample, the DSI was

found to have good internal consistency with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients greater than .84 for

each subscale and internal consistency of .92.

Measures Assessing Emotion Regulation.

The following measure was used to assess participants’ ability to regulate emotions.

Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004). The DERS

is a brief 36-item self-report questionnaire designed to assess multiple aspects of emotion

regulation. The measure yields a total score as well as scores on six scales derived through factor

analysis: (a) non-acceptance of emotional responses (e.g., ‘When I’m upset, I become

embarrassed for feeling that way’); (b) difficulties engaging in goal directed behaviour; (e.g.,

Page 87: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

78

‘When I’m upset, I have difficulty getting things done’), (c) impulse control difficulties (e.g.,

‘When I’m upset, I feel out of control’); (d) lack of emotional awareness (e.g., ‘I pay attention to

how I feel’); (e) limited access to emotion regulation strategies (e.g., ‘When I’m upset, I believe

that there is nothing I can do to make myself feel better’); (f) lack of emotional clarity (e.g., ‘I

am confused about how I feel’). Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert type scale, with 1 as

“almost never” and 5 as “almost always”. Higher scores indicate greater difficulties in emotion

regulation. Gratz and Roemer (2004) report Cronbach’s alpha coefficients greater than .80 for

each subscale and internal consistency of .90, as well as, good test-retest reliability and adequate

convergent validity with established measures of emotion dysregulation in non-clinical samples.

In the current sample, Cronbach’s alpha was greater than .80 for each subscale and .94 for the

full scale, indicating good internal consistency.

Measures Assessing GAD and Worry.

The following measures were used to assess GAD and Worry, respectively.

Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire-IV (GAD-Q-IV; Newman et al., 2002).

The GAD-Q-IV is a 9-item self-report diagnostic measure of generalized anxiety disorder based

on DSM-IV diagnostic criteria. The questions reflect the presence or absence of excessive or

uncontrollable worry within the last six months, as well as accompanying physical symptoms,

such as restlessness, sleep disturbances, irritability, or muscle tension. The GAD-Q-IV can also

be scored continuously using a sum total, with scores ranging from 0 to 13. This measure has

demonstrated good test-retest reliability, convergent validity and discriminant validity (Luterek,

Turk, Heimberg, Fresco, & Mennin, 2002; Newman et al., 2002).

Page 88: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

79

Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec,

1990). The PSWQ is one of the most widely used self-report measures of trait worry designed to

capture specific qualities of pathological worry such as excessiveness and uncontrollability. It

contains 16 items that focus on the frequency and intrusiveness of one’s worry. Each question is

scored on a 1 (“Not at all typical”) to 5 (“Very typical”) Likert scale. Scores range from 16 to 80,

with high scores representing more severe worry. A cut-off score of 62 is used to differentiate

individuals with clinical levels of worry (i.e., high worriers) from low-worriers (Behar, Alcaine,

Zuellig, & Borkovec, 2003; Fresco, Mennin, Heimberg, & Turk, 2003). This measure has good

internal consistency, good construct validity, and has demonstrated good test-retest reliability

over periods as long as 10 weeks (Meyer et al., 1990; Molina & Borkovec, 1994; Stöber, 1998;

Robichaud & Dugas, 2005). In the current sample, the PSWQ was found to have good internal

consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .94).

Procedure

Participants were recruited through several social networking websites (including Kijiji,

Craigslist and Facebook) where a link was posted for them to fill out a 30-40-minute online

survey (created and delivered on FluidSurvey.com). An information and informed consent sheet

was presented to participants upon entering the online survey website (Appendix A).

Participation in the study was voluntary and participants filled out the survey online at their

leisure. At the conclusion of the survey, participants were provided with a list of community

resources, including the researchers’ contact information in the event that they felt any anxiety,

or had any questions or concerns regarding the study. In addition, when participants completed

Page 89: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

80

the survey, they were given the option to enter into a draw for a prize in appreciation of their

time. Participants who chose to enter the draw were asked to provide their email address which

were kept separate from their survey data.

Page 90: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

81

Chapter 3: Results

Data Analysis

Prior to conducting the primary analyses, SPSS 22.0 was used to screen all variables for

accuracy of data entry, missing values, and to test for univariate normality. Distributions for the

sEMBU Maternal Rejection variable, sEMBU Paternal Rejection variable, and sEMBU Paternal

Overprotection variable were found to be moderately to highly skewed, with the majority of

participants reporting few early rejecting experiences with mothers and fathers, and few overly

protective and intrusive behaviours from fathers. In addition, the distribution for the GAD-Q-IV

variable was found to be somewhat bimodal, with clusters of participants reporting either lower

or higher generalized anxiety symptoms. To assess whether these deviations from normality

would affect subsequent data analyses in any meaningful way, Pearson and Spearman

correlations were computed and the significance of the differences between correlation

coefficients were calculated using the Fisher r-to-z transformation. No statistically significant

differences were found between Pearson and Spearman correlation coefficients, suggesting no

substantive differences between parametric and non-parametric correlation statistics. Given that

the methodology of this study was primarily correlational and no benefit was associated with

transformation of skewed and non-normal data (Nefzger & Drasgow, 1957; Norris & Aroian,

2004), original scores were used in analyses.

Univariate multicollinearity was also tested by screening for large bivariate correlations

between variables (a cutoff of r ≥ .80 is typically used; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007) and by

examining collinearity statistics. However, no evidence of multicollinearity was found.

Gender differences were not included as analyses given the disproportionate ratio of

female-to-male participants. Additionally, the three adult attachment styles (i.e., secure,

Page 91: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

82

avoidant, and anxious/ambivalent) derived from the Adult Attachment Scale were transformed

into a dichotomous variable representing attachment security and insecurity. This transformation

was due to the low frequency of the anxious/ambivalent attachment style (11% of the sample) as

compared to avoidant (46% of the sample) and secure (43% of the sample) attachment styles.

There was no evidence indicating that anxious/ambivalent and avoidant participants differed in

important ways with respect to the variables in this study.

Table 2 presents the distribution of participants according attachment organization in this

study as a function of worry and generalized anxiety group membership, prior to transforming

the adult attachment styles into a dichotomous variable. Note that participants in this study were

regarded as “high worriers” if their PSWQ total score was greater than or equal to 62 (Behar,

Alcaine, Zuellig, & Borkovec, 2003; Fresco, Mennin, Heimberg, & Turk, 2003). Participants

were categorized as meeting DSM-IV criteria for GAD using the GAD-Q-IV (to assess the

presence/absence of symptoms).

Table 2

Distribution of Adult Attachment Styles as a Function of Worry Level and Generalized Anxiety

Attachment Styles

Low Worriers

N High Worriers Only

N GAD analogues

N Row Totals

N

Secure 67 23 4 94

Avoidant 41 40 20 101

Anxious/Ambivalent 9 9 5 23

Column Totals 117 72 29 218

Page 92: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

83

Preliminary analysis involved examining descriptive data for childhood attachment

experiences, adult attachment, emotion dysregulation, negative problem orientation, self-esteem,

differentiation of self, worry, and GAD. Independent-samples t tests were used to compare

securely and insecurely attached individuals on all variables used in this study. One-way

ANOVAs were used to compare individuals with low worry, high worry and who meet

diagnostic criteria for GAD on all variables assessed here. Person’s correlations were also

performed to investigate the relationships among variables of interest. Linear regression analyses

were performed to evaluate hypotheses 1 – 3.

Next, multiple mediation analyses were conducted using PROCESS macro in SPSS

(Hayes, 2012) to assess pathways from insecure adult attachment experiences to worry and

generalized anxiety through emotion regulation, self-esteem, differentiation of self, and negative

poblem orientation. Regression analyses of total effect, direct effect, and bootstrapped bias-

corrected 95% confidence intervals of the indirect effect were computed with 5000 bootstrapped

samples, as recommended by Preacher and Hayes (2008). Confidence intervals that did not

contain zero indicated a significant indirect effect (mediation; Preacher & Hayes 2008).

Bootstrapping methods for examining mediating effects have been used in this study because

they offer increased statistical power (and control over the type I error rate) without assuming

multivariate normality in the sampling distribution (Mallinckrodt, Abraham, Wei, & Russell,

2006; Preacher & Hayes, 2008).

Preliminary Analysis

Descriptive statistics for memories of parental upbringing, filial responsibility, emotion

dysregulation, self-esteem, differentiation of self, negative problem orientation, worry, and

Page 93: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

84

generalized anxiety symptoms are presented for the total sample, and separately for attachment

and worry groups, in Table 3. Overall, securely attached individuals reported significantly more

positive memories of parental rearing than insecurely attached individuals, in terms of greater

maternal emotional warmth and less parental rejection and overprotection. The only exception to

this finding was perceptions of paternal emotional warmth, which did not differ significantly

between attachment groups. Securely attached individuals also perceived their childhood

interactions with families of origin as being significantly fairer as compared to insecurely

attached individuals.

Individuals who were in the GAD analogue group reported significantly more negative

memories of maternal and paternal rejection than individuals with high and low worry. Also,

individuals in the GAD analogue group perceived their childhood interactions with their families

of origin as being significantly less fair than high and low worriers. In terms of emotion

dysregulation, securely attached individuals and low worriers reported less difficulties in

emotion regulation than insecurely attached, high-worry, and individuals in the GAD analogue

group. Additionally, securely attached individuals and low worriers reported greater self-esteem

and higher levels of differentiation of self than their insecurely attached, high worry and

analogue GAD counterparts. Furthermore, securely attached individuals and low worriers

reported significantly less negative problem-solving beliefs than insecurely attached, high

worriers, and individuals in the GAD analogue group. Finally, securely attached individuals

endorsed significantly less worrying and generalized anxiety in comparison to insecurely

attached individuals.

Bivariate correlations were also calculated to examine the relationships among the

variables in this study. Results of correlational analysis presented in Table 4 indicate that

Page 94: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

83

generalized anxiety symptoms were positively and significantly correlated with memories of

parental rejection, memories of paternal overprotection, emotional dysregulation, perceptions of

unfair childhood interactions with family of origin, negative problem orientation, and worry, and

negatively correlated with maternal emotional warmth and differentiation of self.

Correspondingly, worry was positively and significantly correlated with paternal rejection and

overprotection, emotion dysregulation, and negative problem orientation, and negatively

correlated with self-esteem and differentiation of self.

Negative memories of parental upbringing (i.e., parental rejection and overprotection)

were positively and significantly correlated with adult attachment insecurity, negative problem

orientation, and emotional dysregulation, and negatively correlated with self-esteem. Memories

of maternal warmth was positively and significantly correlated with self-esteem and

differentiation of self, and negatively correlated with adult attachment insecurity, emotion

dysregulation and negative problem orientation. Memories of paternal overprotection were

positively and significantly associated with adult attachment insecurity and emotion

dysregulation, and negatively correlated with differentiation of self. With regards to

parentification dimensions, perceptions of unfair childhood interactions with family of origin

were positively and significantly correlated with adult attachment insecurity and emotional

dysregulation, and negatively correlated with differentiation of self.

Page 95: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

84

Table 3

Means, Standard Deviations and Group Differences for Memories of Upbringing, Filial Responsibility,

Emotion Dysregulation, Self-esteem, Negative Problem Orientation, Differentiation of self, Worry, and

Generalized Anxiety

Variable

Total

Sample

N = 218

M

(SD)

Securely

Attached

N = 94

M

(SD)

Insecurely

Attached

N = 124

M

(SD)

Low

Worriers

N = 117

M

(SD)

High Worriers

Only

N = 72

M

(SD)

GAD

Analogues

N = 29

M

(SD)

Memories of Upbringing

sEMBU Mother

Rejection 13.04

(5.77)

12.00

(5.31)

13.83*

(6.00)

12.84

(5.90)

12.32

(5.31) 16.21a

(5.62)

Emotional Warmth 17.13

(4.99)

18.19

(4.55)

16.33**

(5.18)

17.91

(4.82)

16.74

(5.29)

15.88

(4.85)

Overprotection 21.19

(5.76)

19.99

(5.34)

22.10**

(5.93)

20.80

(6.30)

21.26

(4.87)

22.96

(5.22)

sEMBU Father

Rejection 14.22

(7.99)

12.30

(6.73)

15.67**

(8.57)

13.33

(7.56)

13.90

(7.85) 19.67a

(8.69)

Emotional Warmth 16.56

(6.37)

17.50

(5.47)

16.18

(6.97)

16.75

(6.10)

16.69

(6.24)

15.13

(8.01)

Overprotection 20.11

(8.35)

18.13

(6.83)

21.62**

(9.07)

19.44

(7.96)

20.63

(8.27)

22.00

(10.27)

Filial Responsibility

Instrumental Caregiving 19.36

(8.35)

20.31

(9.23)

18.64

(7.58)

20.07

(8.36)

18.42

(8.40)

18.54

(8.16)

Expressive Caregiving 27.84

(9.51)

26.62

(9.49)

28.77

(9.46)

27.41

(8.96)

28.11

(9.98)

29.21

(10.96)

Past Unfairness 28.29

(9.87)

25.04

(9.26)

30.75**

(9.64)

27.42

(9.00)

27.75

(10.85) 34.33a

(9.33)

Emotion Dysregulation

DERS 93.84

(24.79)

81.04

(19.84)

103.51**

(23.82)

85.07

(21.54)

100.08

(24.08) 119.54a

(18.68)

Self-esteem

RSES 17.57

(6.23)

20.27

(5.04)

15.52**

(6.28)

20.25

(5.32)

15.06

(5.56) 17.57 a

(6.23)

Negative Problem

Orientation

NPOQ 31.66

(11.80)

26.21

(9.65)

35.78**

(11.64)

26.72

(9.83)

35.72

(11.08) 44.54a

(8.50)

Differentiation of Self

DSI-R 3.46

(0.73)

3.89

(0.62)

3.14**

(0.63)

3.76

(0.66)

3.20

(0.60) 2.72a

(0.54)

Worry

PSWQ 57.08

(14.49)

51.70

(14.07)

61.15**

(13.48)

46.72

(10.46)

69.63

(4.96)

72.08

(5.63)

Generalized Anxiety

GAD-IV 5.83

(4.16)

3.97

(3.48)

7.24**

(4.08)

3.73

(3.57)

7.50

(3.08) 11.50a

(0.68)

*p < .05, **p < .01 a Significant mean difference between High Worriers Only and GAD Analogues, p < .05

Page 96: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

85

Table 4

Bivariate Correlations Between Memories of Upbringing, Filial Responsibility, Emotion Dysregulation, Self-esteem, Negative Problem Orientation,

Differentiation of self, Worry, and Generalized Anxiety

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17

Memories of Upbringing

sEMBU Mother

1. Rejection --

2. Emotional Warmth -.34** --

3. Overprotection .50** .05 --

sEMBU Father

4. Rejection .41** -.28** .14* --

5. Emotional Warmth -.23** .40** -.10 .24** --

6. Overprotection .17* -.11 .31** .71** .49** --

Filial Responsibility

7. Instrumental Caregiving .04 -.08 .03 -.02 -.15* -.02 --

8. Expressive Caregiving .14* -.26** .00 .18** -.16* .07 .50** --

9. Past Unfairness .51** -.60** .23** .37** -.39** .15* .32** .56** --

Adult Attachment

10. AAS (insecurity) .16* -.18** .18** .21** -.07 .21** -.10 .11 .29** --

Emotion Dysregulation

11. DERS .20* -.19** .25** .19** -.14* .18** .00 .11 .25** .45** --

Self-esteem

12. RSES -.24** .29** -.14* -.21** .20** -.12 .03 -.10 -.31** -.38** -.65** --

Negative Problem

Orientation

13. NPOQ .27** -.20** .22** .20** -.15* .11 -.08 .05 .26** .40** .65** -.69** --

Differentiation of Self

14. DSI-R -.09 .17* -.17* -.17* .10 -.16* .03 -.13 -.25** -.51** -.72** .68** -.68** --

Worry

15. PSWQ .05 -.12 .10 .16* .00 .17* -.13 .04 .09 .32** .49** -.53** .56** -.58** --

Generalized Anxiety

16. GAD-IV .20** -.15* .09 .21** -.07 .14* -.03 .13 .21** .39** .55** -.55** .57** -.57** .66** --

*p < .05, ** p < .01

Page 97: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

86

Analysis for Hypothesis 1

The first hypothesis involved examining the relationship between negative early

attachment experiences and GAD as well as negative early attachment experiences and worry. It

was hypothesized that negative early attachment experiences, represented here by measures

assessing childhood memories of parental rejection, lack of emotional warmth, and

overprotection as well as filial responsibility, would be positively correlated with both

generalized anxiety symptoms and worry.

As indicated above and listed in Table 3, maternal rejection and lack of emotional

warmth, and paternal rejection and overprotection were positively and significantly correlated

with generalized anxiety symptoms. In addition, unfair early interactions with family of origin

were positively and significantly correlated with generalized anxiety. Linear regression analysis

was conducted to examine the relationship between those negative early attachment experiences

and generalized anxiety. Results from this analysis are listed in Table 5. Altogether, maternal

rejection and lack of emotional warmth, paternal rejection and overprotection, and past unfair

interactions with family of origin accounted for only 7% of the variance in generalized anxiety

scores, F(5, 212) = 3.24, p > .01. None of these early attachment experiences emerged

independently as significant predictors of generalized anxiety.

With respect to worry, only paternal rejection and overprotection were significantly and

positively associated with worry. To examine which of each of these early attachment

experiences would best predict adult worry, linear regression analyses were conducted and

results are presented in Table 6. When considered together, paternal rejection and overprotection

Page 98: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

87

accounted for only 3% of the variance in worry scores, F(2, 215) = 3.54, p = .03. None of the

predictor variables emerged independently as a significant predictor of worry.

Table 5

Regression Coefficients for Predicting Generalized Anxiety from Negative Early Childhood

Experiences

Negative Early

Childhood experiences Generalized Anxiety

B SE β R2

Maternal Rejection .062 .058 .086 .071

Maternal Lack of

Emotional Warmth -.018 .069 -.021

Paternal Rejection .050 .056 .095

Paternal

Overprotection .021 .049 .43

Unfairness .049 .039 .115

Table 6

Regression Coefficients for Predicting Worry from Negative Early Childhood Experiences

Negative Early

Childhood experiences Worry

B SE β R2

Paternal Rejection .163 .175 .090 .032

Paternal

Overprotection .179 .167 .103

Analysis for Hypothesis 2

The second hypothesis involved examining the relationship between negative early

attachment experiences and adult attachment. It was hypothesized that childhood memories of

parental rejection, lack of emotional warmth, and overprotection would be associated with an

insecure attachment style in adulthood. In addition, instrumental and expressive early filial

Page 99: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

88

responsibilities, and perceptions of unfair childhood interactions with family of origin would be

associated with insecure adult attachment style.

As mentioned above and presented in Table 3, insecure adult attachment style was

significantly positively correlated with memories of parental rejection and overprotection,

maternal lack of emotional warmth, and unfair filial responsibility experiences in childhood.

Those early attachment experiences accounted for 12% of the variance in insecure adult

attachment, F(6, 211) = 4.77, p > .001. Results of linear regression are presented in Table 7.

However, only unfair interactions with family of origin emerged as a significant predictor of

insecure adult attachment style over and above the other childhood experiences and memories, β

= .19, p < .05.

Table 7

Regression Coefficients for Predicting Adult Attachment from Negative Early Childhood

Experiences

Negative Early

Childhood experiences Adult Attachment

B SE β R2

Maternal Rejection .003 .007 .046 .119

Maternal

Overprotection .006 .007 .106

Maternal Lack of

Emotional Warmth -.007 .008 -.081

Paternal Rejection -.006 .008 -.056

Paternal

Overprotection .011 .008 .132

Unfairness .012 .005 .231*

*p < .05

Page 100: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

89

Analysis for Hypothesis 3

The third hypothesis involved examining the relationships between negative early

attachment experiences and emotion dysregulation, low self-esteem, negative problem

orientation, and low differentiation of self. It was hypothesized that negative early attachment

experiences characterized by childhood memories of parental rejection, lack of emotional

warmth, and overprotection would be associated with emotion dysregulation, low self-esteem,

negative problem orientation, and low differentiation of self. Correspondingly, instrumental and

expressive early filial responsibilities, and perceptions of unfair childhood interactions with

family of origin would be associated with impairments in emotion regulation, self-esteem,

problem orientation, and differentiation of self.

As shown in table 3, parental rejection and lack of emotional warmth, maternal

overprotection, and unfair interactions in the family of origin were significantly and positively

correlated with emotional dysregulation, low self-esteem, and negative problem orientation.

Paternal overprotection also correlated significantly and positively with emotion dysregulation.

Maternal lack of emotional warmth and overprotection, paternal rejection and overprotection,

and unfair interactions in the family of origin were positively associated with low differentiation

of self. As series of linear regression analyses revealed that early attachment experiences

accounted for 10% of the variance in emotion dysregulation scores, F(7, 210) = 4.51, p > .001

and maternal overprotection was a significant predictor of emotion dysregulation over and above

the other childhood experiences and memories, β = .19, p = .04. With respect to self-esteem,

none of the early attachment experience variables emerged independently as a predictor variable,

although together they accounted for 14% of the variance in self-esteem scores, F(6, 211) = 5.73,

p > .001. Similar results were found for negative problem orientation and differentiation of self,

Page 101: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

90

where none of the early attachment experience variables emerged as predictors variables

independently, but together they accounted for 12% and 9%, respectively of the variances in

negative problem orientation scores, F(6, 211) = 5.73, p > .001, and differentiation of self scores,

F(5, 212) = 4.01, p < .01. Results of linear regression analyses are presented on Tables 8, 9, 10,

and 11.

Table 8

Regression Coefficients for Predicting Emotion Dysregulation from Negative Early Childhood

Experiences

Negative Early

Childhood experiences Emotion Dysregulation

B SE β R2

Maternal Rejection -.243 .414 -.057 .131

Maternal Lack of

Emotional Warmth -.380 .461 -.076

Maternal

Overprotection .834 410 .194*

Paternal Rejection .241 .360 .078

Paternal Lack of

Emotional Warmth -.569 .386 -.146

Paternal

Overprotection .370 .385 .125

Unfairness .215 .235 .086

*p < .05

Table 9

Regression Coefficients for Predicting Self-esteem from Negative Early Childhood Experiences

Negative Early

Childhood experiences Self-esteem

B SE β R2

Maternal Rejection -.009 .096 -.008 .140

Maternal Lack of

Emotional Warmth .173 .109 .139

Maternal

Overprotection -.094 .084 -.087

Paternal Rejection -.117 .065 -.151

Paternal Lack of

Emotional Warmth .128 .081 .131

Unfairness -.059 .058 -.093

Page 102: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

91

Table 10

Regression Coefficients for Predicting Negative Problem Orientation from Negative Early

Childhood Experiences

Negative Early

Childhood experiences Negative Problem Orientation

B SE β R2

Maternal Rejection .142 .184 .069 .123

Maternal Lack of

Emotional Warmth -.164 .209 -.069

Maternal

Overprotection .302 .161 .148

Paternal Rejection .197 .125 .134

Paternal Lack of

Emotional Warmth -.182 .155 -.098

Unfairness .072 .112 .060

Table 11

Regression Coefficients for Predicting Differentiation of Self from Negative Early Childhood

Experiences

Negative Early

Childhood experiences Differentiation of Self

B SE β R2

Maternal Lack of

Emotional Warmth .009 .012 .064 .086

Maternal

Overprotection -.014 .009 -.109

Paternal Rejection -.002 .010 -.017

Paternal

Overprotection -.007 .009 -.083

Unfairness -.012 .007 -.164

Analysis for Hypothesis 4

The fourth hypothesis in this study involved examining the roles of emotion regulation,

self-esteem, negative problem orientation and differentiation of self as intervening variables in

the relationship between insecure adult attachment and worry, and insecure adult attachment and

generalized anxiety. It was expected that emotion dysregulation, low self-esteem, negative

Page 103: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

92

problem orientation, and low differentiation of self would mediate the relationship between

insecure adult attachment and worry, and insecure adult attachment and GAD symptoms. Results

of multiple mediations are presented in Tables 12 and 13, and Figures 3 and 4, for worry and

generalized anxiety, respectively. First, total, direct, and total indirect effects were calculated to

test the hypothesis that together, the set of proposed intervening variables mediate the effect of

insecure adult attachment on worry and generalized anxiety.

With respect to worry, mediation results indicated that the total effect of insecure adult

attachment on worry (β = .32, p < .001) became nonsignificant when mediating variables (i.e.,

emotion regulation, self-esteem, negative problem orientation, and differentiation of self) were

included in the model (direct effect of insecure adult attachment on worry: β = .01, p = .89).

Bootstrapping analysis revealed that the total indirect effect of insecure adult attachment on

worry through the four mediating variables was significant, with an unstandardized point

estimate of 9.19 and a 95% BC confidence interval of 6.47 to 12.22.

A similar finding was revealed with respect to generalized anxiety symptoms, where the

total effect of insecure adult attachment on generalized anxiety (β = .39, p < .001) became

nonsignificant when mediating variables were included in the model (direct effect of insecure

adult attachment on generalized anxiety: β = .09, p = .12). When all four mediating variables

(i.e., emotion regulation, self-esteem, negative problem orientation, and differentiation of self)

were included in the mediation model, bootstrapping procedures indicated that the total indirect

effect on generalized anxiety was significant, with an unstandardized point estimate of 2.47 and a

95% BC confidence interval of 1.80 to 3.21.

Page 104: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

93

Table 12

Mediation of the Effect of Insecure Adult Attachment on Worry through Emotion Regulation, Self-esteem,

Negative Problem Orientation, and Differentiation of Self

95% BC CI

Point estimate SE t p Lower Upper

Total effect 9.45 1.88 5.03 < .001 5.75 13.15

Direct effect .26 1.83 .14 .89 -3.34 3.86

Total indirect effect 9.19 1.48 6.48 12.24*

Specific indirect effects

Emotion Regulation .35 1.15 -1.88 2.68

Self-esteem 1.50 1.00 -.37 3.61

Negative Problem Orientation 2.85 1.09 .99 5.37*

Differentiation of Self 4.48 1.51 1.73 7.78*

Note. BC CI = bias-corrected confidence interval.

*p < .05 (significant indirect effect)

Table 13

Mediation of the Effect of Insecure Adult Attachment on Generalized Anxiety through Emotion Regulation, Self-

esteem, Negative Problem Orientation, and Differentiation of Self

95% BC CI

Point estimate SE t p Lower Upper

Total effect 3.26 .52 6.22 < .001 2.23 4.30

Direct effect .79 .51 1.54 .12 -.22 1.81

Total indirect effect 2.47 .36 1.80 3.21*

Specific indirect effects

Emotion Regulation .56 .32 -.04 1.25

Self-esteem .50 .27 .01 1.10*

Negative Problem Orientation .76 .31 .20 1.41*

Differentiation of Self .63 .39 -.12 1.43

Note. BC CI = bias-corrected confidence interval.

*p < .05 (significant indirect effect)

Page 105: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

94

To examine individual mediators in the context of a multiple mediation model, the

specific indirect effects for each presumed mediator variable (i.e., emotion regulation, self-

esteem, negative problem orientation, and differentiation of self) were also investigated. With

respect to worry, an examination of the specific indirect effects indicated that differentiation of

self and negative problem orientation emerged as significant mediators of the indirect path from

insecure adult attachment. Pairwise contrasts indicated that none of these mediators was of

significantly greater magnitude than the other. Paths a and b in Figure 4 indicate that greater

attachment insecurity in adulthood was related to lower differentiation of self and greater

negative problem orientation, which in turn were associated with higher levels of worry.

In the case of generalized anxiety, findings revealed that negative problem orientation

and self-esteem were significant mediators of the indirect path from insecure adult attachment.

Again, examination of indirect effect pairwise contrasts revealed that none of these mediators

was of significantly greater magnitude than the other. Paths a and b in Figure 5 shows that

greater attachment insecurity was related to greater negative problem orientation and lower self-

esteem, which in turn were associated with higher levels of generalized anxiety.

Page 106: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

95

Figure 4. Mediation model of insecure adult attachment and worry through emotion

dysregulation, self-esteem, negative problem orientation, and differentiation of self. Values

represent standardized regression coefficients. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001

Figure 5. Mediation model of insecure adult attachment and generalized anxiety through

emotion dysregulation, self-esteem, negative problem orientation, and differentiation of

self. Values represent standardized regression coefficients. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p <

.001

(Path a) (Path b)

Insecure Adult

Attachment

Self-esteem

Emotion Dysregulation

Negative Problem Orientation

Differentiation of Self

Worry

.01 (Path c’)

.32*** (Path c)

.45**

-.38**

-.51**

.40**

.03

-.14

.24**

-.30**

(Path a) (Path b)

Insecure Adult

Attachment

Self-esteem

Emotion Dysregulation

Negative Problem Orientation

Differentiation of Self

Generalized

Anxiety .09 (Path c’)

.39*** (Path c)

.45**

-.38**

-.51**

.40**

.15

-.16*

.23**

-.15

Page 107: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

96

To gain further understanding about the potential directionality of effects among

variables, this study also explored a series of alternative mediational models in which the

position of the mediator and the outcome variables were switched (i.e., reverse mediation;

Kenny, Kashy, & Bogler, 1998). In the reverse mediation models, worry and generalized anxiety

served as mediator variables while the constructs representing self-organization (i.e., emotion

regulation, self-esteem, negative problem orientation, and differentiation of self) served as

outcome variables (e.g., insecure adult attachment → worry → emotion dysregulation). Results

revealed that in all reverse mediational models, worry and generalized anxiety also partially

mediated the relationships of insecure adult attachment to emotion regulation, self-esteem

negative problem orientation, and differentiation of self, suggesting that there are bidirectional

associations of worry and GAD to the variables representing self-organization. Results of reverse

mediation analyses can be found on tables 14 and 15 for worry and generalized anxiety,

respectively, in the appendix section (i.e., Appendix C).

Page 108: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

97

Chapter 4: Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate the propositions of the EFT model of GAD

which views the disorder as reflecting negative self-organizations and emotion dysregulation as a

function of attachment injuries (Watson & Greenberg, 2012; Watson & Greenberg, 2015, in

press; Watson, 2015). To this end, this study operationalized some of the concepts of the EFT

model, as outlined in Figure 1, by focusing on early attachment experiences and attachment

styles as reflecting aspects of attachment injuries; low self-esteem, low differentiation of self,

and negative problem orientation as reflecting aspects of a negative self-organization, and

emotion dysregulation. This study sought to examine the relationships among these variables and

their contributions to both GAD and worry.

There were two main propositions that this study purposed to empirically address that

have been suggested as important within the emotion-focused perspective of GAD. The first

proposition considered the importance of insecure attachment in the etiology of GAD and worry.

It was asserted at the outset of this study that negative early attachment experiences would be

significantly and positively related to both GAD and worry. These hypotheses have been

substantiated, although the magnitude of correlations were modest. This study demonstrated that

GAD and worry correlated modestly with a number of negative early attachment experiences,

some of which were not found to be shared across the two constructs. Findings revealed that

paternal rejection and overprotection were positively correlated with worry whereas maternal

rejection, lack of emotional warmth, and overprotection as well as paternal rejection and

overprotection and unfair family interactions were closely related to generalized anxiety.

Page 109: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

98

The finding that negative paternal experiences were significantly and positively

correlated with both worry and generalized symptoms suggests that paternal experiences may

have an important impact on children’s psychological functioning with respect to these

constructs. It has been proposed that fathers have a crucial role in helping individuals develop

autonomy and a sense of personal agency, as fathers’ interactions with their children are

typically characterized by more play and other recreational (exploratory) activities and with

interactions associated with instrumental goals or mastery activities (Bogels & Phares, 2008;

Collins & Russel, 1991). In laboratory problem-solving tasks, fathers have been shown to display

more facilitative communications about tasks whereas mothers tended to be less consistent

(either directive or distracting) with respect to the laboratory tasks (Collins & Russel, 1991). It is

possible that fathers are more likely to exhibit behaviours that may influence worry by fostering

or dampening confidence in their children’s ability to cope with challenging tasks, which may

subsequently be associated with generalized anxiety. Clearly, more research is needed to

evaluate the differential impact of paternal and maternal caregiving on worry and generalized

anxiety.

It is important to note that the negative early experiences with caregivers assessed in the

current study explained only a small amount of variance in worry and generalized anxiety.

However, the findings from this study support previous research with stronger effects indicating

that childhood experiences marked by parental rejection, control and unfair interactions are

associated with worry and generalized anxiety (Brown & Whiteside, 2008; Kertz & Woodruff-

Borden, 2011; Van Eijck, Branje, Hale, & Meeus, 2012). In addition, when participants in this

study were classed as Low Worriers, High Worriers, or GAD Analogues, it was found that GAD

Analogues (i.e., those who met DSM-IV criteria for GAD using the GAD-Q-IV) reported

Page 110: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

99

significantly higher parental rejection and greater unfair interactions with caregivers as compared

to High Worriers and Low Worriers, suggesting that GAD may be associated with a more

upsetting developmental history.

This study also demonstrated that negative early experiences with caregivers are

associated with adult attachment insecurity. Specifically, participants who endorsed higher

attachment insecurity perceived their parents’ behaviours as characterized by higher rejection

and overprotection, and described their experiences in childhood as more unfair. Participants

with higher attachment insecurity also perceived their mothers as colder or lacking in emotional

warmth. Although the strength of the associations between negative early parental experiences

and insecure adult attachment were found again to be small in this study, they were nonetheless

consistent with the results of several previous studies that found moderate significant correlations

between adult attachment and early relationships (Hamilton, 2000; Pinquart, Feubner, & Ahnert,

2013; Waters, Merrick, Treboux, Crowell, & Albersheim, 2000).

The finding that perceived unfair interactions with family of origin emerged as a

significant predictor of insecure adult attachment style over and above other childhood

experiences suggests that perceptions of equity, reciprocity, and the extent to which individuals

receive appreciation for their caregiving activities within the family may have an important

influence in the development of representational models of attachment. It may be that

perceptions of unfair familial interactions engender in children a negative set of expectations

about self and others which they later incorporate into an insecure working model used to make

meaning of future relationships and ways of interacting.

Watson (2011, Watson & Greenberg, in press) suggests that negative early experiences,

such as unfair familial interactions, may contribute to more than just representational models of

Page 111: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

100

self and others. Watson asserts that childhood experiences contribute to the development of

emotion schemes which contain a large proportion of affective and non-verbal experiences.

According to Watson, emotion schemes incorporate information of all types and levels (i.e.,

body, movement, affect, motivation, and cognition) that altogether convey a “felt sense” of

oneself in relation to the world and others. This felt sense is an affectively-toned sense of self

coherence and agency which is primarily an experiential, not a cognitive, concept. From this

perspective, unfair familial interactions would not only lead to negative expectancies about self

and others, but also a felt sense of oneself as rejected and vulnerable coupled with feelings of

shame and powerlessness.

It has been suggested that perceived unfairness in interactions within the family is

associated with high levels of emotional distress and it also moderates the relationship between

caregiving activities within the family and behavioral self-regulation (suppression of aggression,

impulse control, and consideration of others), such that high levels of caregiving has been shown

to predict high levels of self-regulation but only when perceived unfairness was low (Kuperminc,

Jurkovic, & Casey, 2009). This means that caregiving that is perceived to be unfair within the

family may contribute to behavioral self-dysregulation as well as feelings of loneliness,

frustration, and anger. Children may learn to cope with these experiences by deactivating their

attachment needs and also by compromising their self-image (as unable to assert themselves

within the family) as opposed to that of caregivers, resulting in insecure working models of

others and self (West & Keller, 1991).

The fact that associations in this study were weaker than found in the literature (e.g.,

Bokhorst, Bakermans-kranenburg, Pasco, Van ijzendoorn, Fonagy, et al., 2003) could be

explained by the attachment questionnaire used in this study (Hazan & Shaver, 1987) which is

Page 112: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

101

brief and not usually thought to be related to child-parent interactions. This questionnaire

assesses adult attachment style more explicitly in the context of romantic relationships rather

than in relation to caregivers. With that in mind, it is notable that this study found an association

between childhood experiences and adult attachment.

This study also found, as predicted, significant and positive associations between

negative early attachment experiences and emotion regulation difficulties, low self-esteem,

negative problem orientation, and low differentiation of self. For the most part, parental

rejection, control, and lack of emotional warmth as well as unfair filial interactions were all

positively related to a moderate degree to adult emotion dysregulation, low self-esteem, negative

problem orientation, and low differentiation of self. Additionally, both maternal and paternal

characteristics showed similar associations with those adult variables.

In only one instance was there evidence for parenting-specific associations: Maternal

overprotection was found to predict adult emotion dysregulation above and beyond other early

rearing experiences and memories. This result suggests that maternal overprotective and

intrusive behaviours may be particularly relevant to one’s ability to regulate distressing

emotions. It has been suggested that overprotective and intrusive parenting styles may lead to

increased neurobiological activity and emotional overarousal in children which compromise their

attentional skills and subsequently their ability to learn effective regulatory strategies, including

engaging in goal directed behaviours when distressed (Eisenberg, Zhou, Spinrad, Valiente, Fabes

et al., 2005; Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson, 2007; Portman, 2009). Children raised

with overprotective and/or punitive mothers may also be more likely to model their caregivers’

behaviours, and in addition adopt a low perception of control of life events (i.e., external locus of

Page 113: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

102

control where life is perceived to be determined by factors outside of oneself), both of which

undermine adaptive learning and regulation (Portman, 2009).

Watson and Greenberg (in press) suggest that when children are overly protected by their

caretakers, an enmeshed relationship develops in which they are required to forfeit their needs in

the service of their caregiver’s needs for control and mastery. As a result, these children do not

develop adequate skills to care for themselves, nor do they develop an ability to soothe

themselves when faced with challenges, as they rely on others to have their needs met and

anxieties eased. The authors argue that these children’s sense of agency, mastery, and

competence becomes restricted contributing to the development of negative self-organizations

and emotion processing difficulties.

Rubin and colleagues (Rubin, Cheah, & Fox, 2001) add that overprotective parenting

prevents children from engaging in necessary self-initiated coping techniques (including

exploration and play) which make up part of their behavioural self-regulation system. They note

that lack of practice and experience in behavioural self-regulation can be detrimental to

children’s later emotional development, particularly to those who are by disposition poor self-

regulators, as they may not learn to overcome their dispositional vulnerabilities (Rubin et al.,

2001). On the other hand, parenting styles that encourage exploration and play enable the

development of self-regulation, emotional competence, and self-efficacy in children, as they are

able to learn that through exploration they can reduce their initial levels of emotional arousal

evoked by novel and unfamiliar situations (Rubin et al., 2001).

As suggested by the aforementioned findings, negative early attachment experiences may

contribute to a problematic concept of self and psychosocial adjustment. Individuals who

experienced parental rejection, control, lack of affection as well as unfair filial interactions may

Page 114: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

103

be more likely to have a poorly defined sense of self, characterized by deficits in emotion

regulation, low self-esteem, decreased confidence in problem-solving abilities, and difficulties

with autonomy and intimacy. While the associations between negative early attachment

experiences and emotion regulation, self-esteem, negative problem orientation, and

differentiation of self found in this study were not large, they are consistent with the ideas put

forth by the EFT model of GAD (Watson & Greenberg, 2012, Watson & Greenberg, 2015, in

press; Watson, 2015) and previous research (Bogels & Brechman-Toussaint, 2006; Portman,

2009; Schwartz, Thigpen, & Montgomery, 2006). The size of these associations, however,

implies that there may be other stressors that likely affect emotion regulation, self-esteem,

problem-orientation, and differentiation of self beyond the negative early attachment experiences

measured herein, such as dispositional factors.

The second main proposition that this study investigated pertained to the relationships

between insecure adult attachment and generalized anxiety as well as insecure adult attachment

and worry, and the mediating roles of emotion regulation, self-esteem, negative problem

orientation and differentiation of self in those relationships. As predicted, individuals with an

insecure adult attachment style reported greater worry and generalized anxiety as compared to

individuals with secure attachment. Also consistent with expectations, emotion regulation, self-

esteem, negative problem orientation, and differentiation of self showed significant correlations

with insecure adult attachment, worry, and generalized anxiety. In line with existing models of

GAD and worry as well as an emotion-focused perspective of GAD and worry, the findings

revealed that insecure adult attachment was positively correlated with emotion dysregulation and

negative problem orientation, and negatively correlated with self-esteem and differentiation of

self. Correspondingly, emotion dysregulation and negative problem orientation were positively

Page 115: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

104

correlated with worry and generalized anxiety whereas self-esteem and differentiation of self

were negatively correlated with worry and generalized anxiety.

The positive associations between adult attachment insecurity and generalized anxiety as

well as adult attachment insecurity and worry found in this study are consistent with other

studies investigating the relationship between adult attachment and anxiety disorders (Cassidy,

2009; Nolte et al., 2011; Picardi, Caroppo, Fabi, Proietti, Di Gennaro, et al., 2013; Van Eijck et

al., 2012). In these studies, individuals with anxiety disorders, including inpatients diagnosed

with generalized anxiety disorder, endorsed higher levels of attachment insecurity as compared

to healthy individuals and patients with other chronic health conditions. Indeed, Picard and

colleagues (2013) found associations of similar strength between attachment anxiety and

avoidance with anxiety disorders, suggesting that attachment insecurity in general has adverse

effects on one’s psychosocial development and functioning. Although associations between

insecure adult attachment styles did not differ among anxiety disorders (e.g., panic disorder,

social anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder), Picard and colleagues found that their

results were nevertheless independent of depression.

Given that worry and generalized anxiety disorder are characterized by cognitive and

emotional processes such as the tendency to process events as out of one’s control and

difficulties modulating affect, it is not surprising that this study found them to be significantly

and positively correlated with a negative problem orientation and emotion dysregulation. This

finding is consistent with all theoretical models on worry described in this study (Borkovec et al.,

2004; Mennin et al., 2002, 2005; Watson & Greenberg, in press) and previous research that has

observed worry to be associated with dysfunctional beliefs about distressful situations and one’s

ability to manage them, and emotion dysregulation (Davey et al., 1996; Dugas et al., 1997;

Page 116: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

105

Mennin at al.., 2002, 2005). However, this study has extended previous research by revealing

that self-esteem and differentiation of self also have a negative relationship with worry. This

finding lends support to the EFT model of GAD in which worry is viewed as a strategy for

overcoming a fragile and undifferentiated self (Watson & Greenberg, 2015, in press).

More importantly, this study provided support for the mediational processes suggested in

the EFT model to explain the link between attachment insecurity and GAD, including worry. Of

the four intervening variables examined, two were supported as full mediators of the relationship

between insecure adult attachment and worry as well as the relationship between insecure adult

attachment and generalized anxiety. With respect to worry, negative problem orientation and

differentiation of self were supported as full mediators of the relationship between insecure adult

attachment and worry. With regards to generalized anxiety, negative problem orientation and

self-esteem emerged as full mediators of the relationship between insecure adult attachment and

generalized anxiety. Interestingly, these findings suggest that there is a different pattern of

mediation for insecure adult attachment and worry than for insecure adult attachment and

generalized anxiety. Although negative problem orientation helped explain both relationships, it

appears that low differentiation of self may be particularly relevant in explaining attachment-

related differences in worry whereas low self-esteem may be an important contributing factor in

the insecure adult attachment and generalized anxiety relationship.

The finding that emotion dysregulation did not emerge as a significant mediator in the

multiple mediation model accounting for the relationship between insecure adult attachment and

worry as well as insecure attachment and generalized anxiety was somewhat surprising given the

large amount of studies documenting the associations of worry, generalized anxiety, and insecure

adult attachment with emotion dysregulation (Mennin et al., 2002, 2005; Mikulincer et al.,

Page 117: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

106

2003). A possible reason for this is that, at least in the case of worry, the differentiation of self

measure used in this study tapped into somewhat similar dimensions of emotion dysregulation

(i.e., emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff) that could be associated with worry. In fact,

differentiation of self and emotion regulation were found to be highly correlated in this study’s

sample.

In the case of GAD, as crucial as emotion dysregulation may be in the generation of this

disorder thorough its association with worry, it may be subsequent to a negative self-concept

lying at the core of this disorder’s pathology. This position is in line with the emotion-focused

perspective of GAD which proposes that the principal antecedent of worry in GAD is a

vulnerable self-organization constructed around unsymbolized experiences of sadness, shame

and fear (Watson & Greenberg, in press). It is interesting to note that of all variables used in the

study to represent a negative self-organization, self-esteem appears to be the one that

approximates the most to the idea formulated by the EFT model concerning a vulnerable self-

organization in which a felt sense of the self as weak and defective presides over other elements

(e.g., cognitive self-evaluations) relating to the self. In comparison to negative problem

orientation and differentiation of self, self-esteem is the construct that can better capture how one

generally feels about oneself (Brown, 2014). Not surprisingly, it emerged as a mediator of the

relationship between insecure attachment and GAD but not in the relationship between insecure

attachment and worry.

The fact that this study also found evidence for reverse mediations, that is, mediations by

worry and generalized anxiety on emotion dysregulation, low self-esteem, negative problem

orientation, and low differentiation of self suggests that these constructs (representing negative

self-organization) may have a reciprocal relationship with worry and generalized anxiety.

Page 118: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

107

Current theorizing and research on worry and GAD (Borkovec et al., 2004; Dugas & Robichaud

2007; Mennin, 2004; Newman & Llera 2011; Watson & Greenberg, in press; Wells 2006)

provide strong support for the notion that emotion dysregulation, low self-esteem, negative

problem orientation, and low differentiation of self are precursors of worry and GAD. However,

this study acknowledges the possibility that worry and GAD may precede and also be triggered

by aspects of negative self-organization. Recognizably, the use of cross-sectional data to explore

a set of associations for longitudinal propositions, such as those articulated by the EFT model

and existing models of worry, demands interpretative caution. That being said, Hayes (2013)

argues that such a data analytic approach is sometimes merited, particularly for hypothesis

generation and preliminary investigations. Future studies should examine the temporal

relationship between the constructs in this study in order to elucidate the question of causality.

There were other several findings revealed in this study that are worth noting. For

instance, the distribution of adult attachment styles in the current study (i.e., 43% secure, 46%

avoidant, and 11% anxious/ambivalent) was found unexpectedly to differ somewhat from

distributions found in previous large-sample studies assessing both college students and

nationally representative adults (Bifulco et al., 2006; Mickelson et al., 1997; Shaver & Hazan,

1993). In these prior studies, approximately half the number of avoidant individuals and twice

the number of anxious individuals were reported. In addition, in these studies, 50% to 60% of

their samples comprised secure individuals.

This discrepancy may have resulted from the online survey methodology employed here.

Although there are many benefits to online survey methodologies, selection bias is a limitation

that has to be taken into consideration when conducting online research (Wright, 2005). In the

case of this study, it is possible that individuals with an avoidant attachment style were more

Page 119: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

108

likely to access and complete the online survey than individuals with secure and/or

anxious/ambivalent attachment styles. The likelihood of this occurring could be questioned,

however, when one considers recent research suggesting that anxiously attached individuals

make greater use of social media than individuals with attachment avoidance (Oldmeadow,

Quinn, & Kowert, 2013). More research is needed to elucidate this matter.

Another possible explanation for the discrepancy in the attachment distribution found in

the present study is that individuals who worry – and perhaps would also be interested in

participating in a study about worry – are less likely to endorse an anxious attachment style. If

this were the case with this sample, then this finding would contradict postulations that anxious

attachment is more closely associated with worry and generalized anxiety than avoidant

attachment (Cassidy, 1995; Nolte, Guiney, Fonagy, Mayes, & Luyten, 2011; Warren, Huston,

Egeland, & Sroufe, 1997), and would provide further support to studies in which dismissing and

avoidant attachment styles were found to be associated with generalized anxiety (Bifulco et al.,

2006; Mickelson et al., 1997).

This study also revealed that the majority of individuals experiencing high levels of

worry do not meet the diagnostic criteria for GAD. About a third of this study’s sample (72 cases

or 33%) received worry scores indicative of clinical worry while a far smaller subset of

participants (29 cases; 13% of entire sample; 40% of high worriers) met diagnostic criteria for

GAD. This finding is consistent with a previous study by Ruscio (2002) which also found similar

proportions of high worriers and those who met diagnostic criteria for GAD relative to a larger

sample of over a thousand undergraduate students. Similar to Ruscio’s study, the findings from

this study suggest that GAD is more than just high levels of worry and that GAD symptoms

Page 120: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

109

associated with the distress or impairment caused by worry may be better at characterizing GAD

rather than excessive and uncontrollable worry.

The findings that high worriers and GAD analogues differ in emotion regulation skills,

self-esteem, negative problem orientation, and differentiation of self found here also suggest that

good adjustment with respect to these psychological constructs may be important in preventing

high worry from becoming clinically impairing. Replication of these findings and advances in

our understanding of the similarities and differences between high worry and GAD will be

crucial for both research and clinical perspectives in the preventive work of GAD.

Another unexpected finding in this study had to do with a small proportion of participants

(4 cases or 2%) who met criteria for GAD but endorsed a secure adult attachment style. This

would suggest that while there appears to be a relationship between insecure attachment and

GAD, attachment insecurity may not be necessary to determine the diagnostic status of this

disorder. Other factors such as specific life events and temperamental traits may have been

involved in the development of generalized anxiety symptoms among these subset of adults in

our sample. Another explanation may be that self-report biases (e.g., a preference for positive

attachment and parental rearing descriptions) may have impacted these individuals’ responding

style. Unfortunately, given the small number of cases, further investigations with this small

subset of participants were not rendered possible in this study. Future research would benefit

from further investigation on this topic.

Limitations and Future Research

Several limitations of this study must be considered when interpreting findings. A major

limitation is the correlational, cross-sectional design used here which did not permit causal

Page 121: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

110

statements about the links among variables. Although based on strong theoretical grounds, the

current study could not determine the direction of the relationships among variables.

Specifically, it is not clear whether variables representing negative self-organization (i.e.,

emotion dysregulation, low self-esteem, negative problem orientation, and low differentiation of

self) mediate the relationships between insecure adult attachment and worry and generalized

anxiety or vice-versa. This study explored reverse mediation models and found that there are

some bidirectional associations between variables representing negative self-organization and

worry and generalized anxiety. Future research should address this finding to better understand

the ways in which these constructs, as well as insecure adult attachment, are related.

Longitudinal and experimental studies examining the temporal development of attachment,

negative self-organization, worry, and generalized anxiety appear to be a logical next step in this

line of research.

Another limitation of this study is that all of its variables were assessed using solely self-

report questionnaires. It is possible that the significant correlations found here were confounded

by common method variance. Moreover, self-report questionnaires have been criticized in the

literature as a method for assessing implicit and unconscious processes, such as attachment

(Crowell & Treboux, 1995). This is especially relevant for this study because individuals with

chronic worry and generalized anxiety have been shown to have difficulties reporting on their

emotional and cognitive experiences (Mennin, Turk, Heimberg, & Carmin, 2004). Thus, their

self-report on internal processes presented here may be unreliable and may need to be interpreted

with caution. It would have been desirable to use additional and alternative measures to validate

participants’ self-report ratings (e.g., observation ratings). It would also have been beneficial to

include a larger or interview-based measure of attachment so that ratings would have been more

Page 122: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

111

reliable, and differences between anxious and avoidance insecure attachment could have been

investigated.

Lastly, the sample of this study was comprised predominately by females such that the

findings presented here may not generalize to more diverse demographic groups. Also

participants included in this study were recruited from the community and thus represent an

analogue sample experiencing worry and generalized anxiety symptoms. Although it is possible

that a number of these participants have clinical levels of worry and generalized anxiety (as

indicated by their scores on PSWQ and GAD-V-IV questionnaires), their responses may not be

generalized to clinical samples who have been diagnosed by more structured diagnostic methods

(e.g., interviews).

Future research should explore the associations between attachment and worry as well as

attachment and generalized anxiety using longitudinal designs in larger and more heterogeneous

samples with alternative measures of attachment styles and early childhood experiences as well

as worry and generalized anxiety. Furthermore, temperament, cognitive styles, and other

environmental factors have all been shown to be associated with worry and generalized anxiety

(Rapee, 2001; Roemer, Orsillo, & Barlow, 2002). Future work should investigate the predictive

value of these factors compared to insecure attachment.

Conclusion

This dissertation has attempted to investigate some of the propositions of an emerging

model of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) stemming from an emotion-focused therapy (EFT)

perspective, which views GAD as reflecting negative self-organizations and emotion

dysregulation as a function of attachment injuries (Watson & Greenberg, in press). It has done so

Page 123: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

112

by examining negative early attachment experiences and attachment styles, as representing

attachment injuries; differentiation of self, negative problem orientation, and self-esteem, as

representing negative self-organization; and emotion regulation dysregulation, and by

investigating the relationships among these variables and their contribution to generalized

anxiety and worry.

In agreement with the EFT model of GAD as well as existing research on worry and

GAD, the results of the current study suggest that rejecting and unfair early attachment

experiences may be characteristic of individuals with GAD. In addition, they demonstrate that

attachment insecurity and associated vulnerable self-states – marked by low differentiation of

self, low self-esteem, and negative problem orientation – as well as the inability to regulate

emotions may be useful to understanding worry and generalized anxiety. This study also

underlines the importance of negative self-processes concerning a sense of self as incompetent to

cope with challenges (i.e., negative problem orientation), relating poorly to oneself and others

(i.e., low differentiation of self), and as having little affection for one’s self concept (i.e., low

self-esteem) in the relationship between attachment insecurity and worry and generalized

anxiety.

The results presented here could be useful in informing therapeutic interventions tailored

specifically to worry and generalized anxiety disorder. Particularly with respect to assessment of

clients presenting concerns, this study suggests that clients who present in therapy as high

worriers may not only have difficulties finding solutions to their problems because of their

tendency to react to problems in a non-productive way, but they may also present with relational

impairments, such as difficulties creating or maintaining a clear sense of self in their

relationships with others. Likewise, assessment of clients presenting with generalized anxiety

Page 124: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

113

symptoms is likely to uncover poor problem-solving ability as well as important low self-esteem

issues. For the most part, treatment of chronic worry and generalized anxiety symptoms have

focused exclusively on decreasing clients’ level of worry (Dugas & Robichaud, 2007;

Ladouceur, Dugas, Freeston, Leger, Gagnon, et al., 2000; Leahy, 2006). However, this study

suggests that therapy for individuals presenting with worry and generalized anxiety symptoms

might also benefit from the assessment of clients’ attachment styles as well as self-differentiation

and self-esteem, all of which could potentially be emotionally-anchored on maladaptive emotion

schemes and a vulnerable self-organization.

Page 125: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

114

References

Aikins, D. E., & Craske, M. G. (2001). Cognitive theories of generalized anxiety disorder.

Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 24(1), 57-74.

American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders (3rd edition). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders (5th edition). Washington, DC: Author.

Ainsworth, M. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A

psychological study of the strange situation. Oxford, England: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Arrindell, W. A., Richter, J., Eisemann, M., Garling, T., Ryde´n, O., Hansson, S. B., et al.

(2001). The short EMBU in East-Germany and Sweden: a cross-cultural factorial validity

extension. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 157–60. doi:10.1111/1467-

9450.00226

Arrindell, W. A., Sanavio, E., Aguilar, G., Sica, C., Hatzichristou, C., Eisemann, M., Recinos, L.

A., Gaszner, P., Peter, M., Battagliese, G., Ka´llai, J., Van der Ende, J. (1999). The

development of a short form of the EMBU: Its appraisal with students in Greece,

Guatemala, Hungary and Italy. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 613–628.

doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00192-5

Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a

four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244.

Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Identity, self-concept, and self-esteem: The self lost and found. In R.

Hogan, J. Johnson, and S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook o f personality psychology (pp. 681-

710). Toronto: Academic.

Page 126: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

115

Behar, E., Alcaine, O., Zuellig, A.R., & Borkovec, T.D. (2003). Screening for generalized

anxiety disorder using the Penn State Worry Questionnaire: A receiver operating

characteristic analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 34(1),

25-43. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7916(03)00004-1

Behar, E., Zuellig, A. R., & Borkovec, T. D. (2005). Thought and imaginal activity during worry

and trauma recall. Behavior Therapy, 36(2), 157-168. doi: 10.1016/S0005-

7894(05)80064-4

Berenbaum, H., Thompson, R. J., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2007). The relation between worrying

and concerns: The importance of perceived probability and cost. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 45(2), 301-311.

Bergevin, M., Koerner, N., & Dugas, M. J. (2003, November). Fear of anxiety and experiential

avoidance in excessive worry: Specificity in a nonclinical sample. Poster presented at the

annual convention of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Boston,

MA.

Berry, K., Shah, R., Cook, A., Geater, E., Barrowclough, C., & Wearden, A. (2008). Staff

attachment styles: A pilot study investigating the influence of adult attachment styles on

staff psychological mindedness and therapeutic relationships. Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 64, 355-363. doi:10.1002/jclp.20456

Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J. F., Bolduc-Murphy, E. A., Faraone, S. V., Chaloff, J., Hirshfeld,

D. R., et al. (1993). A 3-year follow-up of children with and without behavioral

inhibition. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32,

814–821.

Page 127: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

116

Bifulco, A., Kwon, J., Jacobs, C., Moran, P. M., Bunn, A., & Beer, N. (2006). Adult attachment

style as mediator between childhood neglect/abuse and adult depression and anxiety.

Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 41, 796-805. doi:10.1007/s00127-006-

0101-z

Bogels, S. M., & Brechman-Toussaint, M. L. (2006). Family issues in child anxiety: Attachment,

family functioning, parental rearing and beliefs. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 834-

856. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.08.001

Bogels, S. M., & Phares, V. (2008). Father’s roles in the etiology, prevention and treatment of

child anxiety: A review and new model. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 539-558.

doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.07.011

Bokhorst, C. L., Bakermans-kranenburg, M. J., Pasco, R. M., Van ijzendoorn, M. H., Fonagy, P.,

Schuengel, C. (2003). The Importance of Shared Environment in Mother–Infant

Attachment Security: A Behavioral Genetic Study. Child Development, 74, 1769-1782.

doi:10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00637.x

Borkovec, T. D. (1985). Worry: A potentially useful construct. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 23, 481-482.

Borkovec, T. D. (1994). The nature, functions, and origins of worry. In G. Davey, & F. Tallis

(Eds.), Worrying: Perspective on theory assessment and treatment (pp.5-33). Sussex,

England: Wiley & Sons.

Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O. M., & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and

generalized anxiety disorder. In R. Heimberg, C. Turk, & D. Mennin (Eds.), Generalized

anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp. 77-108). New York, NY:

Guilford Press.

Page 128: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

117

Borkovec, T. D., & Hu, S. (1990). The effect of worry on cardiovascular response to phobic

imagery. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(1), 69-73. doi:10.1016/0005-

7967(90)90056-O

Borkovec, T. D., Lyonfields, J. D., Wiser, S. L., & Deihl, L. (1993). The role of worrisome

thinking in the suppression of cardiovascular response to phobic imagery. Behaviour

Research and Therapy, 31(3), 321-324. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(93)90031-O

Borkovec, T. D., Newman, M. G., Pincus, A. L., & Lytle, R. (2002). A component analysis of

cognitive-behavioral therapy for generalized anxiety disorder and the role of interpersonal

problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 288.

Borkovec, T. D., Ray, W. J., & Stöber, J. (1998). Worry: A cognitive phenomenon intimately

linked to affective, physiological, and interpersonal behavioral processes. Cognitive

Therapy and Research, 22(6), 561-576. doi:10.1023/A:1018790003416

Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & DePree, J. A. (1983). Preliminary exploration of

worry: Some characteristics and processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21(1), 9-

16. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(83)90121-3

Borkovec, T. D., & Roemer, L. (1995). Perceived functions of worry among generalized anxiety

disorder subjects: Distraction from more emotionally distressing topics? Journal of

Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26(1), 25-30. doi:10.1016/0005-

7916(94)00064-S

Borkovec, T. D., Shadick, R. N., & Hopkins, M. (1991). The nature of normal and pathological

worry. In R. M. Rapee, & D. H. Barlow (Eds.), (pp. 29-51). New York, NY, US: Guilford

Press, New York, NY.

Page 129: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

118

Bosquet, M., & Egeland, B. (2006). The development and maintenance of anxiety symptoms

from infancy through adolescence in a longitudinal sample. Development and

Psychopathology, 18, 517-550. doi:10.1017/S0954579406060275

Bourne, K., Berry, K., & Jones, L. (2014). The relationship between psychological mindedness,

parental bonding, and adult attachment. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory,

Research and Practice, 87, 167-177. doi:10.1111/papt.I2007

Bowen, M. (1978). Family therapy in clinical practice. NY: Jason Aronson.

Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.). New York: Basic

Books.

Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York:

Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss. New York, NY, US: Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development.

New York, NY, US: Basic Books.

Breitholtz, E., Johansson, B., & Ost, L. G. (1995). Cognitions in generalized anxiety disorder and

panic disorder patients: A prospective approach. Behavior Research and Therapy, 37,

533-544. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00147-8

Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult

attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson, & W. S. Rholes (Eds.),

Attachment theory and close relationships. (pp. 46-76). New York, NY, US: Guilford

Press.

Page 130: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

119

Brennan, K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1995). Dimensions of adult attachment, affect regulation, and

romantic relationship functioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(3),

267-283.

Brown, J. D. (2014). Self-esteem and self-evaluation: Feeling is believing. Psychological

perspectives on the self, 4, 27-58.

Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1992). Comorbidity among anxiety disorders: Implications for

treatment and DSM-IV. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60(6), 835-844.

doi:10.1037/0022-006X.60.6.835

Brown, T. A., Barlow, D. H., & Liebowitz, M. R. (1994). The empirical basis of generalized

anxiety disorder. The American journal of psychiatry, 151, 1272-1280.

Brown, T. A., DiNardo, P. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1994). Anxiety disorders interview schedule for

DSM-IV (ADIS-IV). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Brown, A. M., & Whiteside, S. P. (2008). Relations among perceived parental rearing behaviors,

attachment style, and worry in anxious children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(2),

263-272. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.02.002

Burt, A. (1986). Generational boundaries in children of alcoholics. Unpublished master’s thesis,

Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA.

Carlson, E. A. (1998). A prospective longitudinal study of attachment

disorganization/disorientation. Child Development, 69, 1107-1128. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8624.1998.tb06163.x

Carver, C. S. (1997). Adult attachment and personality: Converging evidence and a new

measure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 865-883.

Page 131: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

120

Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences of attachment relationships. Monographs of

the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2-3), 228-283. doi:10.2307/1166148

Cassidy, J. (1995). Attachment and generalized anxiety disorders. In D. Cicchetti, & S. Toth

(Eds.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology (Vol. 6): Emotion,

cognition, and representation (pp. 343-370). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester

Press.

Cassidy, J., Lichtenstein-Phelps, J., Sibrava, N. J., Thomas, Jr., C. L., Borkovec, T. D. (2009).

Generalized anxiety disorder: Connections with self-reported attachment. Behavior

Therapy, 40, 23-38. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2007.12.004

Castaneda, J. O., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2004). Effect of stimulus type and worry on physiological

response to fear. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18(6), 809-823.

doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2003.10.003

Chase, N. D. (1999). Burdened children: Theory, research, and treatment of parentification.

Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publication.

Chiu, L-H. (1988). Testing the test: Measures of self-esteem for school-age children. Journal of

Counseling and Development, 66(6), 298–301.

Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of

temperament and character. Archives of general psychiatry, 50, 975-990.

Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1994). Cognitive representations of attachment: The structure and

function of working models. In K. Bartholomew, & D. Perlman (Eds.), Attachment

processes in adulthood. (pp. 53-90). London, England: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Coopersmith, S. (1981). Self-esteem inventories. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Page 132: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

121

Cozolino, L. (2014). The Neuroscience of Human Relationships: Attachment and the Developing

Social Brain. New York: W. W. Norton & Co.

Cozzarelli, C., Karafa, J. A., Collins, N. L., & Tagler, M. J. (2003). Stability and change in adult

attachment styles: Associations with personal vulnerabilities, life events, and global

construals of self and others. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22, 315-346.

doi:10.1521/jscp.22.3.315.22888

Crandall, R. (1973). The measurement of self-esteem and related constructs. In J. Robinson, & P.

Shaver (Eds.), Measures of social psychological attitudes (pp. 45–168). Ann Arbor, MI:

Institute for Social Research.

Craske, M. G., Rapee, R. M., Jackel, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1989). Qualitative dimensions of

worry in DSM-III—R generalized anxiety disorder subjects and nonanxious controls.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27(4), 397-402. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(89)90010-7

Crowell, J. A., & Treboux, D. (1995). A review of adult attachment measures: Implications for

theory and research. Social Development, 4, 294-327. doi: 10.111/j.1467-

9507.1995.tb00067.x

Davey, G. C. L. (2006). A mood-as-input account of perseverative worrying. In G. C. L. Davey,

& A. Wells (Eds.), Worry and its psychological disorders: Theory, Assessment and

treatment. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Davey, G. C., Hampton, J., Farrell, J., & Davidson, S. (1992). Some characteristics of worrying:

Evidence for worrying and anxiety as separate constructs. Personality and Individual

Differences, 13, 133-147.

Page 133: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

122

Davey, G. C. L., Jubb, M., & Cameron, C. (1996). Catastrophic worrying as a function of

changes in problem-solving confidence. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20(4), 333-

344. doi:10.1007/BF02228037

Davey, G. C. L., Startup, H. M., MacDonald, C. B., Jenkins, D., & Paterson, K. (2004). The use

of ‘as many as can’ versus ‘feel like counting’ stop rules during worrying. Cognitive

Therapy and Research, 29, 155-169. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-3162-5

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a

multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113-

126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

Davis, R. N., & Valentiner, D. P. (2000). Does meta-cognitive theory enhance our understanding

of pathological worry and anxiety? Personality and Individual Differences, 29(3), 513-

526. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00211-1

Degnan, K. A., Almas, A. N., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Temperament and the environment in the

etiology of childhood anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 497-517.

DeKlyen, M., & Greenberg, M. T. (2008). Attachment and psychopathology in childhood. In J.

Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory: Theory, methods, and

clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press.

Doherty, N. A., & Feeney, J. (2004). The composition of attachment networks throughout the

adult years. Personal Relationships, 11, 469-488. doi:10.1111/j1475-6811.2004.00093.x

Dozier, M., Stovall-McClough, K. C., Albus, K. E. (2008). Attachment and psychopathology in

adulthood. In J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,

research, and clinical applications (pp. 718-744). New York: Guilford Press

Page 134: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

123

Dugas, M. J., Buhr, K., & Ladouceur, R. (2004). The role of intolerance of uncertainty in

etiology and maintenance. In Heimberg R. G., Turk C. L. and Mennin D. S. (Eds.),

Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp. 143-163). New

York: Guilford Press, New York, NY.

Dugas, M. J., Freeston, M. H., & Ladouceur, R. (1997). Intolerance of uncertainty and problem

orientation in worry. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21(6), 593-606.

doi:10.1023/A:1021890322153

Dugas, M. J., & Koerner, N. (2005). Cognitive-behavioral treatment for generalized anxiety

disorder: Current status and future directions. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An

international Quarterly, 19, 61-81. doi:10.1891/jcop.19.1.61.66326

Dugas, M. J., Marchand, A., & Ladouceur, R. (2005). Further validation of a cognitive-

behavioral model of generalized anxiety disorder: Diagnostic and symptom specificity.

Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19(3), 329-343. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.02.002

Dugas, M. J., & Robichaud, M. (2007). Cognitive-behavioral treatment for generalized anxiety

disorder: From science to practice. New York: Taylor & Francis.

Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A. & Liew, J. (2005) Relations

among positive parenting, children’s effortful control, and externalizing problems: a

three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76, 1055–1071.

Elliott, R., Watson, J. C., Goldman, R. N., & Greenberg, L. S. (2004). Learning emotion-focused

therapy: The process-experiential approach to change. Washington, DC, US: American

Psychological Association.

Page 135: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

124

Eng, W., & Heimberg, R. G. (2006). Interpersonal correlates of generalized anxiety disorder:

Self versus other perception. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20(3), 380-387.

doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2005.02.005

Erickson, T. M., & Newman, M. G. (2007). Interpersonal and emotional processes in generalized

anxiety disorder analogues during social interaction tasks. Behavior Therapy, 38(4), 364-

377. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.10.005

Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor of adult romantic

relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2), 281-291.

Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Hanrahan, M. (1994). Assessing adult attachment. In M. B. Sperling,

& W. H. Berman (Eds.), Attachment in adults: Clinical and developmental perspectives. (pp.

128-152). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

Flannery-Shroeder, E. C. (2004). Generalized anxiety disorder. In T. L. Morris, & J. S. March

(Eds.), Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents (pp. 125-140), 2nd ed. New York:

Guilford Press.

Fleming, J. S., & Courtney, B. E. (1984). The dimensionality of self-esteem II: Hierarchical facet

model for revised measurement scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46,

404–421.

Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Moran, G. S., & Higgitt, P. (1991). The capacity for

understanding mental stages: The reflective self in parent and child and its significance

for security attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 12, 201-218.

Fraley, R. C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: Meta-analysis and dynamic

modeling of developmental mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6,

123-151. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0602_03

Page 136: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

125

Fraley, R. C., & Spieker, S. J. (2003). Are infant attachment patterns continuously or

categorically distributed? A taxometric analysis of strange situation

behavior. Developmental psychology, 39, 387.

Fraley, R. C., & Waller, N. G. (1998). Adult attachment patterns: A test of the typological

model. In J. A. Simpson, & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close

relationships. (pp. 77-114). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

Fuendeling, J. M. (1998). Affect regulation as a stylistic process within adult attachment. Journal

of Social and Personal Relationships, 15(3), 291-322. doi: 10.1177/0265407598153001

Gosselin, P., Pelletier, O., & Ladouceur, R. (2001, July). The Negative Problem Orientation

Questionnaire (NPOQ): Development and validation among a non-clinical sample.

Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the World Congress of Behavioral and

Cognitive Therapies, Vancouver, BC.

Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and

dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in

Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26,

41–54. doi:10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94

Gray-Little, B., Williams, V. S. L., & Hancock, T. D. (1997). An item response theory analysis

of the Rosenberg Self Esteem Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23,

443–451.

Hale, W. W., III, Engels, R., & Meeus, W. (2006). Adolescents’ perceptions of parenting

behaviors and its relationship to adolescent generalized anxiety disorder symptoms.

Journal of Adolescence, 29, 407-417. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.002

Page 137: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

126

Hamilton, C. E. (2000). Continuity and discontinuity of attachment from infancy through

adolescence. Child Development, 71, 690-694. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00177

Hanrahan, F., Field, A. P., Jones, F. W., & Davey, G. C. (2013). A meta-analysis of cognitive

therapy for worry in generalized anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(1),

120-132. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.10.008

Harvey, A.G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behavioural processes

across psychological disorders: A transdiagnostic approach to research and treatment.

Oxford: Oxford University Press

Hayes, S., & Hirsch, C. (2007). Information processing biases in generalized anxiety disorder.

Psychiatry, 6, 176-182. doi:10.1016/j.mppsy.2007.02.003

Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational framework for research on

close relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 5(1), 1-22. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0501_1

Hazan, C, & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524.

Henning, E. R., Turk, C. L., Mennin, D. S., Fresco, D. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (2007).

Impairment and quality of life in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder.

Depression and Anxiety, 24(5), 342-349. doi:10.1002/da.20249

Hettema, J. M., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2004). Genetic and environmental sources of

covariation between generalized anxiety disorder and neuroticism. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 161, 1581–1587.

Hoehn-Saric, R., Haslett, R. L., & McLeod, D. R. (1993). Generalized anxiety disorder with

early and late onset of anxiety symptoms. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34, 291-298. doi:

10.1016/0010-440X(93)90013-T

Page 138: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

127

Hoffman, D. L., Dukes, E. M., & Wittchen, H. U. (2008). Human and economic burden of

generalized anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 25(1), 72-90.

Holaway, R.M., Rodebaugh, T.L., & Heimberg, R.G. (2006). The epidemiology of worry and

generalized anxiety disorder. In G.C.L. Davey & A. Wells (Eds.) Worry and its

Psychological Disorders: Theory, Assessment & Treatment (pp. 3-20). London: John

Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Huth-Bocks, A. C., Levendosky, A. A., Bogat, G. A.,&von Eye, A. (2004). The impact of

maternal characteristics and contextual variables on infant-mother attachment. Child

Development, 75, 480–496. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00688.x

Johnston, W. M., & Davey, G. C. L. (1997). The psychological impact of negative TV news

bulletins: The catastrophizing of personal worries. British Journal of Psychology, 88(1),

85-91. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1997.tb02622.x

Jurkovic, G. J. (1997). Lost childhoods: The plight of the parentified child. Philadelphia:

Brunner/Mazel.

Jurkovic, G. J., Kuperminc, G. P., Perilla, J., Murphy, A. D., Ibanez, G., & Casey, S. (2004). The

role of filial responsibility and cultural ecological processes in school and social

adjustment of immigrant Latino adolescents. Journal of Primary Prevention, 25, 81-104.

Jurkovic, G. J., Thirkield, A., & Morrell, R. (2001). Parentification of Adult Children of Divorce:

A Multidimensional Analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30, 245-257. doi:

10.1023/A:1010349925974

Kasoff, M. B., & Pincus, A. L. (2002, August). Interpersonal pathoplasticity in generalized

anxiety disorder. Paper presented at the 110th Annual Convention of the American

Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.

Page 139: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

128

Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social psychology. In D.

Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, pp. 233–265.

New York: Oxford.

Kennedy-Moore, E., & Watson, J. C. (1999). Expressing emotion: Myths, realities, and

therapeutic strategies. New York: Guilford Press.

Kerr, M., & Bowen, M. (1988). Family evaluation: An approach based on Bowen theory. NY:

Norton.

Kertz, S. J., McHugh, R. K., Lee, J., & Björgvinsson, T. (2014). Examining the latent structure

of worry and generalized anxiety in a clinical sample. Journal of anxiety disorders, 28(1),

8-15.

Kertz, S. J., & Woodruff-Borden, J. (2011). The developmental psychopathology of worry.

Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 174-197. doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-

0086-3

Kessler, R. C. (2005). Evidence that generalized anxiety disorder is an independent disorder. In

D. Nutt, K. Rickels and D. Stein (Eds.), Generalized anxiety disorder: Symptomatology,

pathogenesis and Management (ed.), pp. 1-8. London, UK: Martin Dunitz

Kessler, R. C., Walters, E. E., & Wittchen, H. (2004). In Heimberg R. G., Turk C. L. and Mennin

D. S. (Eds.), Epidemiology. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press, New York, NY.

Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Davis, K. E. (1994). Attachment style, gender, and relationship stability: A

longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(3), 502-512.

Klein, M. (1975). Love, guilt, and reparation and other works 1921 – 1945. New York: Delacorte

Press/Seymour Lawrence.

Page 140: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

129

Koerner, N, & Dugas, M. J. (2006). A cognitive model of generalized anxiety disorder: The role

of intolerance of uncertainty. In G. C. L. Davey, & A. Wells (Eds.), Worry and its

psychological disorders: Theory, Assessment and treatment. Chichester, UK: John Wiley

& Sons, Ltd.

Kuperminc, G. P., Jurkovic, G. J., & Casey, S. (2009). Relation of filial responsibility to the

personal and social adjustment of Latino adolescents from immigrant families. Journal of

Family Psychology, 23, 14 – 22. doi:10.1037/a0014064

Ladoucer, R., Dugas, M. J., Freeston, M. H., & Eliane, L., Gagnon, F. Thibodeau, N. (2000).

Efficacy of a cognitive-beahvioral treatment for generalized anxiety disorder: Evaluation

in a controlled clinical trial. Journal of Consulting in Clinical Psychology, 68, 957-964.

doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.68.6.957

Ladouceur, R., Dugas, M. J., Freeston, M. H., Rhéaume, J., Blais, F., Boisvert, J., Blais, F.,

Boisvert, J. M., et al. (1999). Specificity of generalized anxiety disorder symptoms and

processes. Behavior Therapy, 30(2), 191-207. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(99)80003-3

Leahy, R. L. (2006). The worry cure: Seven steps to stop worry from stopping you. New York:

Three Rivers Press.

Lewis, M., Feiring, C., & Rosenthal, S. (2000). Attachment over time. Child development, 707-

720.

Lichtenstein, J., & Cassidy, J., (1991, April). The Inventory of Adult Attachment: Validation of a

new measure. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in

Child Development, Seattle.

Page 141: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

130

Lopez, F.G. (1995). Contemporary attachment theory: An introduction with implications for

counseling psychology. The counseling Psychologist, 23, 395-415. doi:

10.1177/0011000095233001

Lopez, F. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). Dynamic processes underlying adult attachment

organization: Toward an attachment theoretical perspective on the healthy and effective

self. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47(3), 283-300. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.47.3.283

Macatee, R. J., Capron, D. W., Guthrie, W., Schmidt, N. B., Cougle, J. R. (2015). Distress

tolerance and pathological worry: Tests of incremental and prospective relationships.

Behaviour Therapy, 46, 449-462.

MacLeod, C., & Rutherford, E. (2004). Information-processing approaches: Assessing the

selective functioning of attention, interpretation, and retrieval. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L.

Turk, & D. S. Mennin (Eds.), Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and

practice (pp. 109-142). New York: Guilford.

Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A

move to the level of representation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child

Development, 50, 66-104.

Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented

during the Ainsworth strange situation. In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti & E. M.

Cummings (Eds.), (pp. 121-160). Chicago, IL, US: University of Chicago Press, Chicago,

IL.

Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L.,& Fresco, D. M. (2002). Applying an emotion

regulation framework to integrative approaches to generalized anxiety disorder. Clinical

Psychology: Science and Practice, 9, 85-90. doi:10.1093/clipsy/9.1.85

Page 142: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

131

Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M. (2005). Preliminary evidence for

an emotion dysregulation model of generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research,

and Therapy, 43, 1281-1310. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.08.008

Mennin, D. S., Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., & Carmin, C. N. (2004). Focusing on the

regulation of emotion: a new direction for conceptualizing and treating generalized

anxiety disorder. In M. A. Reinecke, & D. A. Clark (Eds.), Cognitive therapy over the

lifespan: Evidence and practice (pp. 60-89). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation

of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 487-495.

doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6

Mickelson, K. D., Kessler, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1997). Adult attachment in a nationally

representative sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1092-1106.

Mikulincer, M. (1997). Adult attachment style and information processing: Individual

differences in curiosity and cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 72, 1217-1230.

Mikulincer, M., & Arad, D. (1999). Attachment working models and cognitive openness in close

relationships: A test of chronic and temporary accessibility effects. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 77, 710-725.

Mikulincer, M., & Orbach, I. (1995). Attachment styles and repressive defensiveness: The

accessibility and architecture of affective memories. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 68, 917-925.

Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and

change. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

Page 143: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

132

Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Pereg, D. (2003). Attachment theory and affect regulation: The

dynamics, development, and cognitive consequences of attachment-related strategies.

Motivation and Emotion, 27(2), 77-102. doi: 10.1023/A:1024515519160

Mineka, S., Yovel, I., & Pineless, S. L. (2002). Towards a psychological model of the etiology of

generalized anxiety disorder. In D. Nutt, K. Rickels, & D. J. Stein (Eds.), Generalized

anxiety disorder: Symptomatology, pathogenesis, and management (pp. 11-26). London:

Taylor & Francis Group.

Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Milne, B. J., Melchior, M., Goldberg, D., & Poulton R.

(2007). Generalized anxiety disorder and depression: Childhood risk factors in a birth

cohort followed to age 32. Psychological Medicine, 37, 441-452. doi:

10.1017/S0033291706009640

Montebarocci, O., Codispoti, M., Baldaro, B., & Rossi, N. (2004). Adult attachment style and

alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(3), 499-507.

Muris, P. (2002). Parental rearing behaviors and worry of normal adolescents. Psychological

Reports, 91, 428-430.

Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the

family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 361-

388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x

Muris, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., & Hulsenbeck, P. (2000). Worry in children is related

to perceived parental rearing and attachment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 487-

497.

Newman, M. G., & Erickson, T. M. (2010). Generalized anxiety disorder. In J. G. Beck (Ed.),

Interpersonal processes in the anxiety disorders: Implications for understanding

Page 144: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

133

psychopathology and treatment (pp. 235-259). Washington, DC, US: American

Psychological Association, Washington, DC. doi: 10.1037/12084-009

Newman, M. G., Zuellig, A. R., Kachin, K. E., Constantino, M. J., Przeworski, A., Erickson, T.,

& Cashman-McGrath, L. (2002). Preliminary reliability and validity of the generalized

anxiety disorder questionnaire-IV: A revised self-report diagnostic measure of

generalized anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy, 33, 215–233. doi: 10.1016/S0005-

7894(02)80026-0

Nolte, t. Guiney, J., Fonagy, P., Mayes, L., & Luyten, P. (2011). Interpersonal stress regulation

and the development of anxiety disorders: An attachment-based developmental

framework. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 1-21. doi:

10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00055

Olatunji, B. O., Broman-Fulks, J. J., Bergman, S. M., Green, B. A., & Zlomke, K. R. (2010). A

taxometric investigation of the latent structure of worry: dimensionality and associations

with depression, anxiety, and stress. Behavior Therapy, 41(2), 212-228.

Oldmeadow, J. A., Quinn, S., & Kowert, R. (2013). Attachment style, social skills, and Facebook

use amongst adults. Computers in Human Behaviour, 29, 1142-1149. doi:

10.1016/j.chb.2012.10.006.

Peasley, C. E., Molina, S., & Borkovec, T. D. (1994, November). Empathy in generalized

anxiety disorder. Paper presented at the 28th annual meeting of the Association for

Advancement of Behavior Therapy, San Diego, CA.

Peasley-Miklus, C., & Vrana, S. R. (2000). Effect of worrisome and relaxing thinking on fearful

emotional processing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 129-144.

doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00025-X

Page 145: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

134

Peleg, O., Halaby, E., & Whaby, E. N. (2006). The relationship of maternal separation anxiety

and differentiation of self to children's separation anxiety and adjustment to kindergarten:

A study in Druze families. Journal of anxiety disorders, 20, 973-995.

Peleg-Popko, O. (2002). Bowen theory: A study of differentiation of self, social anxiety, and

physiological symptoms. Contemporary Family Therapy, 24, 355-369.

Perris, C., Jacobsson, L., Linndström, H., Knorring, L. V., & Perris, H. (1980). Development of a

new inventory for assessing memories of parental rearing behaviour. Acta Psychiatrica

Scandinavica, 61(4), 265-274.

Pincus, A. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1994, April). Interpersonal problems in generalized anxiety

disorder: Preliminary clustering of clients’ interpersonal dysfunction. Paper presented at

the annual meeting of the American Psychological Society, New York.

Portman, M.E. (2009). Generalized anxiety disorders across the lifespan: An integrative

approach. New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-89243-6-3.

Przeworski, A., Newman, M. G., Pincus, A. L., Kasoff, M. B., Yamasaki, A. S., Castonguay, L.

G., & Berlin, K. S. (2011). Interpersonal pathoplasticity in individuals with generalized

anxiety disorder. Journal of abnormal psychology, 120, 286.

Provencher, M. D., Freeston, M. H., Dugas, M. J., & Ladouceur, R. (2000). Catastrophizing

assessment of worry and threat schemata among worriers. Behavioural and Cognitive

Psychotherapy, 28, 211-224.

Rapee, R. M. (2001). The development of generalized anxiety. In M. W. Vasey, & M. R. Dadds

(Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp.481-503). New York, NY:

Oxford University Press.

Page 146: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

135

Ravitz, P., Maunder, R., Hunter, J., Sthankiya, B., & Lancee, W. (2010). Adult attachment

measures: A 25-year review. Journal of psychosomatic research, 69, 419-432.

Rettew, D. C., Copeland, W., Stanger, C., & Hudziak, J. J. (2004). Associations between

temperament and DSM-IV externalizing disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of

Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 25, 383–391.

Robichaud, M., & Dugas, M. J. (2005). Negative problem orientation (part I): Psychometric

properties of a new measure. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(3), 391-401.

doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.02.007

Robins, R. W., Hendin, H. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2001). Measuring global self-esteem:

Construct validation of a single-item measure and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(2), 151–161.

Rodrigues, A., & Watson, J.C. (2010, June). An examination of clients’ attachment styles, affect

regulation, and outcome in process-experiential and cognitive-behavioral therapies.

Paper presented at the international meeting of the Society for Psychotherapy Research,

Asilomar, CA

Roemer, L., Molina, S., & Borkovec, T. D. (1997). An investigation of worry content among

generally anxious individuals. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185(5), 314-319.

doi:10.1097/00005053-199705000-00005

Roemer, L., Molina, S., Litz, B. T., & Borkovec, T. D. (1996). Preliminary investigation of the

role of previous exposure to potentially traumatizing events in generalized anxiety

disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 4, 134-138.

Page 147: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

136

Roemer, L., & Orsillo, S. M. (2005). An acceptance-based behavior therapy for generalized

anxiety disorder. In S. M. Orsillo & L. Roemer (Eds.), Acceptance and mindfulness-

based approaches to anxiety (pp. 213-240). NY: Springer US.

Roemer, L., Orsillo, S. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2002). Generalized anxiety disorder. In D. H.

Barlow (Ed.), Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and

panic (pp. 477-515). New York: Guilford.

Roemer, L., Salters, K., Raffa, S., & Orsillo, S. M. (2005). Fear and avoidance of internal

experiences in GAD: preliminary tests of a conceptual model. Cognitive Therapy and

Research, 29, 71-88. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-1650-2

Rubin, K. H., Cheah, C. S. L., Fox, N. (2001). Emotion regulation, parenting, and display of

social reticence in preschoolers. Early Education & Development, 12, 97-115.

doi:10.1207/s15566935eed1201_6

Ruscio, A. M., Borkovec, T. D., Ruscio, J. (2001). A taxometric investigation of the latent

structure of worry. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 413-422. doi:10.1037/0021-

843X.110.3.413

Sable, P. (2000). Attachment and adult psychotherapy. NJ: Jason Aronson.

Schore, A. N. (2003). Affect dysregulation and disorders of the self. New York: W. W. Norton &

Co.

Schwartz, C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of

inhibited temperament in childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1008–1015.

Page 148: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

137

Schwartz, J., Thigpen, S., & Montgomery, J. (2006). Examination of Parenting Styles of

Processing Emotions and Differentiation of Self. The Family Journal, 14, 41-48. doi:

10.1177/1066480705282050

Searle, B., & Meara, N. M. (1999). Affective dimensions of attachment styles: Exploring self-

reported attachment style, gender, and emotional experience among college students.

Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46, 147-158.

Shaver, P. R., Belsky, J., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). The adult attachment interview and self‐

reports of romantic attachment: Associations across domains and methods. Personal

Relationships, 7, 25-43.

Shaver, P. R., & Clark, C. (1994), The psychodynamics of adult romantic attachment. In J. M.

Masling & R. F. Bornstein (Eds.), Empirical perspectives on object relations theory.

Empirical studies of psychoanalytic theories (Vol. 5, pp. 105-156). Washington, DC:

American Psychological Association

Shaver, P. R., & Hazan, C. (1993). Adult romantic attachment: Theory and evidence. In D.

Perlman & W. Jones (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 4, pp. 29-70).

London: Jessica Kingsley.

Shi, L., Wampler, R., & Wampler, K. (2013). A comparison of self-report adult attachment

measures: How do they converge and diverge. Universal Journal of Psychology, 1, 10-

19.

Sibrava, N. J., & Borkovec, T. D. (2006). The cognitive avoidance theory of worry. In G. C. L.

Davey, & A. Wells (Eds.), Worry and its psychological disorders: Theory, Assessment

and treatment. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Page 149: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

138

Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support seeking and support giving

within couples in an anxiety-provoking situation: The role of attachment styles. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 62(3), 434-446.

Skowron, E. A. (2000). The role of differentiation of self in marital adjustment.Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 47(2), 229.

Skowron, E. A., Holmes, S. E., & Sabatelli, R. M. (2003). Deconstructing differentiation: Self

regulation, interdependent relating, and well-being in adulthood. Contemporary Family

Therapy, 25, 111-129.

Skowron, E. A., & Schmitt, T. A. (2003). Assessing interpersonal fusion: Reliability and validity

of a new DSI Fusion with Others subscale. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 29,

209–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-0606.2003.tb01201.x

Skowron, E. A., Stanley, K. L., & Shapiro, M. D. (2009). A longitudinal perspective on

differentiation of self, interpersonal and psychological well-being in young adulthood.

Contemporary Family Therapy, 31(1), 3-18.

Souetre, E., Lozet, H., Cimarosti, I., Martin, P., Chignon, J. M., Ades, J., et al. (1994). Cost of

anxiety disorders: Impact of comorbidity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 151-

160.

Spangler, G., & Grossmann, K. E. (1993). Biobehavioral organization in securely and insecurely

attached infants. Child development, 64, 1439-1450.

Sroufe, L. A. (2003). Attachment categories as reflections of multiple dimensions: Comment on

Fraley and Spieker (2003). Developmental Psychology, 39, 413-416. doi: 10.1037/0012-

1649.39.3.413

Page 150: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

139

Sroufe, L. A. (2005). Attachment and development: A prospective, longitudinal study from birth

to adulthood. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 349-367. doi:

10.1080/14616730500365928

Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct. Child

development, 48, 1184-1199.

Startup, H. M., & Davey, G. C. L. (2003). Inflated responsibility and the use of stop rules for

catastrophic worrying. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(4), 495-503.

doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00245-0

Stöber, J. (1998). Worry, problem elaboration and suppression of imagery: The role of

concreteness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 751-756.

Stöber, J., Tepperwien, S., & Staak, M. (2000). Worrying leads to reduced concreteness of

problem elaborations: Evidence for the avoidance theory of worry. Anxiety, Stress, and

Coping, 13, 217-227.

Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Norton.

Tallis, F., Davey, G. C. L., & Capuzzo, N. (1994). The phenomenology of non-pathological

worry: A preliminary investigation. In G. C. L. Davey & F. Tallis (Eds.), Worrying:

Perspectives on theory, assessment and treatment (pp. 61-89). London: Wiley.

Thompson, J. M. (1995). Silencing the self: Depressive symptomatology and close relationships.

Psychology of Women Quarterly, 19, 337–353. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1995.tb00079.x

Thorberg, F. A., & Lyvers, M. (2006). Attachment, fear of intimacy and differentiation of self

among clients in substance disorder treatment facilities. Addictive behaviors, 31, 732-

737.

Page 151: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

140

Tucker, D. M., & Newman, J. P. (1981). Verbal versus imaginal cognitive strategies in the

inhibition of emotional arousal. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 5, 197-202.

Turk, C. L., & Mennin, D. S. (2011). Phenomenology of generalized anxiety disorder.

Psychiatric Annals, 41(2), 72-78.

Van Eijck, F. E. A. M., Branje, S. J. T., Hale, W. W., III, Meeus, W. H. J. (2012). Longitudinal

associations between perceived parent-adolescent attachment relationship quality and

generalized anxiety disorder symptoms in adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 40, 871-883. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9613-z

Wallin, D. J. (2007). Attachment in psychotherapy. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

Warren, S. L., Huston, L., Byron, E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Child and adolescent anxiety

disorders and early attachment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 637-644.

Waters, A. M., & Craske, M. G. (2005). Generalized anxiety disorder. In M. M. Antony, D. B.

Ledlyey, & R. G. Heimberg (Eds.) Improving outcomes and preventing relapse in

cognitive behavioral therapy. New York: Guilford (pp. 77-127). New York: Guilford.

Waters, E., Hamilton, C. E., & Weinfield, N. S. (2000). The stability of attachment security from

infancy to adolescence and early adulthood: General introduction. Child Development,

71(3), 678-683. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00175

Waters, E., Merrick, S. K., Treboux, D., Crowell, J. A., & Albersheim, L. (2000). Attachment

security in infancy and early adulthood: A twenty-year longitudinal study. Child

Development, 71, 684-689. doi:10.1111/1467-8642-00179

Page 152: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

141

Watson, J. C. (August, 2015). Emotion focused therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Paper

presented to the 123rd Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association,

Toronto, Canada.

Watson, J. C., & Goldman, R. N. (2012, July). Experiential perspectives on anxiety. Panel

presented at the 10th Conference of the World Association for Person-Centered and

Experiential Psychotherapy and Counseling. Antwerps, Belgium.

Watson, J. C. & Greenberg, L. S. (July 2012). Working with generalized anxiety in EFT. Paper

presented at the 10th Conference of the World Association for Person Centered and

Experiential Psychotherapy and Counseling, Antwerp, Belgium.

Watson, J. C. & Greenberg, L. S. (Submitted). Emotion focused therapy for GAD. Washington,

DC. APA Books.

Watson, J. C., & Prosser, M. (2004). Observer-rated Measure of Affect Regulation (O-MAR).

Unpublished manuscript, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education of the University of

Toronto, Toronto, Canada.

Weinfield, N. S., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment from infancy to early

adulthood in a high-risk sample: Continuity, discontinuity, and their correlates. Child

Development, 71(3), 695-702. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00178

Weinfield, N. S., Whaley, G. J. L., & Egeland, B. (2004). Continuity, discontinuity, and

coherence in attachment from infancy to late adolescence: Sequelae of organization and

disorganization. Attachment & Human Development, 6(1), 73-97.

doi:10.1080/14616730310001659566

Page 153: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

142

Wells, A. (1995). Metacognition and worry: A cognitive model of generalized anxiety disorder.

Behavioural and cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 301-320.

doi:10.1017/S1352465800015897

Wells, A. (2006). The metacognitive model of worry and generalized anxiety disorder. In G. C.

L. Davey, & A. Wells (Eds.), Worry and its psychological disorders: Theory, Assessment

and treatment. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Wells, A., & Carter, K. (2001). Further tests of a cognitive model of generalized anxiety

disorder: Metacognitions and worry in GAD, panic disorder, social phobia, depression,

and nonpatients. Behavior Therapy, 32(1), 85-102. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(01)80045-9

Wells, A., & Papageorgiou, C. (1995). Worry and the incubation of intrusive images following

stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(5), 579-583. doi:10.1016/0005-

7967(94)00087-Z

Weston, D., & Morrison, K. (2001). A multidimensional meta-analysis of treatments for

depression, panic, and generalized anxiety disorder: An empirical examination of the

status of empirically supported therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,

69, 875–899.

West, M. L., & Keller, A. E. (1991). Parentification of the child: A case study of Bowlby's

compulsive care-giving attachment pattern. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 3, 425-

431.

Wittchen, H., Beesdo, K., & Kessler, R. (2002). The impact of generalized anxiety disorder. In

D. Nutt, K. Rickels, & D. J. Stein (Eds.), Generalized anxiety disorder: Symptomatology,

pathogenesis, and management (pp. 11-26). London: Taylor & Francis Group.

Page 154: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

143

Wolfe, B. E. (2005). Understanding and treating anxiety disorders: An integrative approach to

healing the wounded self. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Wolfe, B. E., & Sigl, P. (1998). Experiential psychotherapy of the anxiety disorders. In L. S.

Greenberg, J. C. Watson, & G. Lietaer (Eds.), Handbook of experiential psychotherapy

(pp. 272-294). New York: Guilford Press.

Wolkin, J. R. (1984). Childhood parentification: An exploration of long-term effects.

Dissertation Abstracts International, 45, 2707.

Wright, K. B. (2005), Researching Internet-Based Populations: Advantages and Disadvantages

of Online Survey Research, Online Questionnaire Authoring Software Packages, and

Web Survey Services. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 10: 00. doi:

10.1111/j.1083-6101.2005.tb00259.x

Yamas, K., Hazlett-Stevens, H., & Borkovec, M. (1997). Alexithymia in generalized anxiety

disorder. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Association for the Advancement

of Behavior Therapy, Miami, FL.

Page 155: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

144

Appendice

Page 156: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

145

Appendix A – Informed Consent

You are being invited to participate in a survey that examines the relationship between one’s childhood

experiences and how one behaves and feels as an adult, with a particular focus on worry. This study is

being conducted by Aline Rodrigues, Ph.D. student in Counselling & Clinical Psychology at the Ontario

Institute for Studies in Education, University of Toronto (OISE/UT) and Dr. Jeanne Watson, OISE/UT.

We hope that this study will result in improving our understanding of human development and the factors

that contribute to well-being in families and across the life-span.

Anyone over the age of 18 is invited to take part in this study. Your participation is voluntary and you

may refuse to participate or withdraw from the study at any time without any consequences. If you agree

to participate, you will be asked to respond to general background questions and eleven questionnaires

that require you to rate your childhood experiences with your parents as well as your general thoughts,

behaviours, and feelings as an adult. The survey will take approximately 30 to 40 minutes to complete.

Your answers will be kept confidential and no identifying information will be collected, except for your

email address (for the prize draw described below) which will be deleted before any analyses are done.

In appreciation for your participation, you will have the option of being entered in a draw for a chance to

win a $100 Amazon Online Gift Certificate.

There are no known risks associated with this study. However, some of the questions might lead you to

think about negative emotions or experiences. In the case that any questions raise personal issues that you

would like to discuss with a counsellor, you may contact us to direct you to a counsellor or you may use

the "counselling resource sheet” provided at the end of this survey.

If you have any questions about your rights as a research participant, you may contact the University of

Toronto Research Ethics Review Office by email at [email protected] or by phone at 416-946-

3273. You can also contact Dean Sharpe at 416-978-5585.

If you would like more information about the survey or how the data will be used, please contact:

Aline Rodrigues, M.A.

Email: [email protected]

Phone: (647) 298-4050

Dr. Jeanne Watson, C. Psych.

Email: [email protected]

Phone: (416) 978-0705

Below you will be prompted to indicate if you wish to participate in this study. Please click “I Consent”

to indicate that you agree to participate in this study, that you have read and understood the information

presented above, and that you are over 18 years of age. If you wish, you can print this information and

consent page for future reference. Thank you very much for your time!

Page 157: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

146

Appendix B – Demographics Questionnaire

1. What is your gender?

Male

Female

2. What is your age?

3. How would you describe your ethnicity? (Select one):

Native (e.g., First Nations, Metis)

Black African

Caucasian

Asian

South East Asian

Hispanic

Other _______________

4. What is your marital status?

Single

Married

Domestic Partnership

Separated

Divorced

Widowed

5. What is your highest level of education? (Select one)

Grade School

High School Diploma or GED

College or Trade School

Some University

University undergraduate degree

Post graduate degree

Page 158: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

147

Appendix C – Results of Reverse Mediation Analyses

Table 14

Mediations of the Effect of Insecure Adult Attachment on Emotion Regulation, Self-esteem, Negative Problem

Orientation, and Differentiation of Self through Worry

95% BC CI

Point estimate SE t p Lower Upper

Emotion Regulation (Outcome Variable)

Total effect 22.47 3.04 7.40 < .001 16.48 28.45

Direct effect 16.18 2.93 5.52 < .001 10.40 21.96

Total indirect effect 6.28 1.59 3.64 10.02

Self-esteem (Outcome Variable)

Total effect -4.74 .79 -6.00 < .001 -6.30 -3.18

Direct effect -2.90 .74 -3.91 < .001 -4.36 -1.44

Total indirect effect -1.83 .45 -2.88 -1.09

Negative Problem Orientation (Outcome variable)

Total effect 9.57 1.48 6.46 < .001 6.65 12.49

Direct effect 5.85 1.36 4.31 < .001 3.17 8.53

Total indirect effect 3.71 .86 2.22 5.66

Differentiation of Self (Outcome Variable)

Total effect -.75 .09 -8.76 < .001 -.92 -.58

Direct effect -.53 .08 -6.80 < .001 -.68 -.38

Total indirect effect -.22 .05 -.34 -.13

Note. BC CI = bias-corrected confidence interval.

Page 159: EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG ATTACHMENT, … · Aline Rodrigues Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto ABSTRACT This study attempted to

148

Table 15

Mediations of the Effect of Insecure Adult Attachment on Emotion Regulation, Self-esteem, Negative Problem

Orientation, and Differentiation of Self through Generalized Anxiety

95% BC CI

Point estimate SE t p Lower Upper

Emotion Regulation (Outcome Variable)

Total effect 22.46 3.03 7.40 < .001 16.48 28.45

Direct effect 13.83 2.94 4.72 < .001 8.04 16.62

Total indirect effect 8.64 1.82 5.62 12.99

Self-esteem (Outcome Variable)

Total effect -4.74 .79 -6.00 < .001 -6.30 -3.18

Direct effect -2.42 .75 -3.19 .001 -3.92 -.92

Total indirect effect -2.32 .49 -3.51 -1.50

Negative Problem Orientation (Outcome

variable)

Total effect 9.57 1.48 6.46 < .001 6.65 12.49

Direct effect 5.01 1.40 3.58 < .001 2.25 7.77

Total indirect effect 4.56 .92 3.01 6.70

Differentiation of Self (Outcome Variable)

Total effect -.75 .08 -8.76 < .001 -.92 -.58

Direct effect -.50 .09 -6.09 < .001 -.66 -.39

Total indirect effect -.25 .05 -.37 -.16

Note. BC CI = bias-corrected confidence interval.