emotion regulation, executive functioning, and ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours...

117
EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND UNDERSTANDING OF INTENTIONALITY IN CHILDREN WITH A HISTORY OF PEDIATRIC CANCER BY ALEXANDRA MARIE DELONE A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of WAKE FOREST UNIVERSITY GRADUATE SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF ARTS Psychology August 2014 Winston-Salem, North Carolina Approved By: Deborah L. Best, Ph.D., Advisor Janine M. Jennings, Ph.D., Chair Lisa Kiang, Ph.D. Thomas W. McLean, MD

Upload: others

Post on 27-Jun-2020

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND

UNDERSTANDING OF INTENTIONALITY IN CHILDREN

WITH A HISTORY OF PEDIATRIC CANCER

BY

ALEXANDRA MARIE DELONE

A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of

WAKE FOREST UNIVERSITY GRADUATE SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES

in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

for the Degree of

MASTER OF ARTS

Psychology

August 2014

Winston-Salem, North Carolina

Approved By:

Deborah L. Best, Ph.D., Advisor

Janine M. Jennings, Ph.D., Chair

Lisa Kiang, Ph.D.

Thomas W. McLean, MD

Page 2: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

ii

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Thank you to my amazing family! Mom, Dad, Victoria, and Charles: I could

never have completed this program or project without your help and encouragement.

You are my favorite people in the whole world and I would be entirely lost and would

have much less fun without you!!

Preparing for this project was a “clan” effort! Victoria, thank you for being my

master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons!

My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles, Ashton, Brett, Elaine, Gail, Claire, Natalie,

Brooke, Molly, Rachel, and Zoe, thank you for spending multiple snow days coloring

pictures to my specifications. Charles and Gaily, you two were cast perfectly as the robot

and narrator! I wish you could have seen the kids smile as they thought they were

playing with a “real” robot and talking to the storyteller.

To my ribbon makers: Lauren, Stephanie, Julia, Natalie, and Jamie, your efforts

helped to sell this project to the kids who were unsure about playing with a stranger. All

of the kids loved picking and giving away their little surprise!

Thank you to all of the girls in Best Lab! You were so helpful in getting materials

organized and sharing your ideas. Kelsey, there is no way in the world that I could have

logistically completed this project without you! Plus, it was always so much more fun

when you were around! Thank you for willingly making early morning runs to the

preschools and hospital in hopes of nabbing a few kids.

Dr. Simonian, thank you for teaching me to love psychology and supporting me

throughout college and graduate school. Without you as an advisor, my life may have

taken a very different path!

Page 3: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

iii

Debbie, thank you for being a wonderful advisor throughout my time at Wake

Forest! I came to you with an inkling that I wanted to work with children with pediatric

cancer and you helped me explore this by supporting my summer away at St. Jude and

working overtime to design this project. I know it required so much extra work on your

part and I truly appreciate everything you did to make it happen.

Dr. McLean, a few months ago, I was a random student who sent you a request to

become involved in my research project. Not only did you respond positively, but you

have gone above and beyond the call of duty every time I have needed anything. Most of

all, thank you for sharing your kids with me. I have enjoyed every child and family I

have had the pleasure of meeting.

Janine and Lisa, thank you for all of the hours you gave up to make my project the

best it could be through your input and suggestions. Without you, I would not have

incorporated the ribbons. Although I can never get back the many hours of my life

devoted to crafting these prizes, they made all the difference by helping me win over

teachers, the clinic staff, and hesitant kids.

I wanted to pursue this project because of the phenomenal experience I had during

my summer as a Pediatric Oncology Education intern at St. Jude Children’s Research

Hospital. Dr. Gronemeyer and Dr. Phipps, you have devised a program that changes the

life of every young person who is lucky enough to participate.

Finally, I want to thank all of the children, families, doctors, nurses, and staff I

have had the privilege of meeting at St. Jude and at Brenner Children’s Hospital. Thank

you for your devotion to these kids and helping them fight. To the little ones, you are so

brave and an inspiration to others by showing what it means to never give up!!

Page 4: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF TABLES ...............................................................................................................v

LIST OF APPENDICES .................................................................................................... vi

ABSTRACT ...................................................................................................................... vii

INTRODUCTION ...............................................................................................................1

Cognition...................................................................................................................2

Family Emotionality .................................................................................................3

The Development of Emotion Regulation ................................................................5

The Development of Executive Functioning ............................................................7

The Development of Identifying Intentionality ......................................................10

The Impact of Pediatric Cancer on Developmental Processes ...............................13

METHOD ..........................................................................................................................17

Participants .............................................................................................................17

Parent Measures .....................................................................................................24

Child Tasks ............................................................................................................29

Procedure ...............................................................................................................33

RESULTS ..........................................................................................................................38

Differences on EF, Emot. Reg., and Identifying Intentionality .............................42

Differences in Parent Measures .............................................................................46

Hierarchical Regression Analyses .........................................................................52

DISCUSSION ....................................................................................................................58

REFERENCES ..................................................................................................................70

APPENDICES ...................................................................................................................77

CURRICULUM VITAE ..................................................................................................108

Page 5: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

v

LIST OF TABLES

1 Gender and Ethnicity of Hospital and School Samples ............................................18

2 Diagnoses for Children with a History of Pediatric Cancer .....................................19

3 Treatment Modalities & Intensity for Children with a History of

Pediatric Cancer ........................................................................................................20

4 Caregivers Enrolled on Study for Hospital and School Samples .............................22

5 Parent and Family Demographic Information for Hospital and School Samples .....23

6 Factor Analysis of Parent Protectiveness Subscale ................................................. 28

7 Summary Table of Hypotheses and Measures ..........................................................34

8 Means and Standard Deviations for Parent Measures: Hospital

and School Samples .................................................................................................40

9 Means and Standard Deviations for Child Tasks: Hospital and School Samples.....41

10 Summary Table of Hypotheses, Measures, and Results ...........................................51

11 Correlations between Variables Considered for the Family

Emotionality Variable ...............................................................................................53

12 Hierarchical Regression for Variables Predicting Zoo Secret Sharing Task ...........54

13 Hierarchical Regression for Variables Predicting Ribbon Secret Sharing Task .......56

14 Hierarchical Regression for Variables Predicting Zoo Secret Sharing Task ............57

15 Hierarchical Regression for Variables Predicting Ribbon Secret Sharing Task .......58

Page 6: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

vi

LIST OF APPENDICES

A Demographic Information ....................................................................................... 77

B Hospital Consent Form .............................................................................................78

C School Parent Letter and Consent Form ...................................................................83

D Parent – Family Expressiveness Questionnaire ........................................................85

E Parent – Emotion Regulation Checklist ....................................................................86

F Parent – Parent Emotion Checklist ...........................................................................87

G Parent – Child Health and Family Functioning Questionnaire .................................88

H Parent – Intensity of Treatment Rating .....................................................................89

I Child – Silly Sounds Task........................................................................................ 90

J Child – Secret Sharing Task .....................................................................................91

K Child – Identifying Intentionality .............................................................................93

L Correlations across Samples ...................................................................................105

M Correlations for Children with a History of Cancer................................................106

N Correlations for Children without a History of Pediatric Cancer ...........................107

Page 7: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

vii

ABSTRACT

This study aims to identify early developmental processes that may be affected by the

experience of pediatric cancer in young children, with particular focus on emotion

regulation, executive functioning, and identifying the intentions of others. Additionally,

it was hypothesized that parents of children with and without a history of pediatric cancer

would have differing perceptions of their children. Thirty-nine children with a history of

pediatric cancer and 40 children without a history of pediatric cancer, ages 4-7, were

recruited for the current study. The children completed 4 tasks: the Peabody Picture

Vocabulary Test, Silly Sounds, Secret Sharing, and Identifying Intentionality. Parents

were asked to complete brief questionnaires. Differences were found between various

parent measures as well as cognitive ability and identifying intentionality. However,

there were fewer differences than expected. Despite a potentially stressful and emotional

pediatric cancer experience, results from this study suggest that certain developmental

processes are unaffected by the child’s history of pediatric cancer.

Page 8: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

1

INTRODUCTION

In the United States, childhood cancer is the second leading cause of death for

children from birth to age 14, after accidental deaths (Murphy, Xu, & Kochanek, 2012).

However, medical advances in the last 50 years have changed the focus of pediatric

cancer care from dealing with a fatal illness to managing a chronic disease coupled with

uncertainty in survival (Armenian, Landier, Hudson, Robinson, & Bhatia, 2013;

Eisenberg, 1981). The national average for five-year event-free survival for children

diagnosed with pediatric cancers now exceeds 80% (Armenian et al., 2013). As the

number of pediatric cancer survivors increases, it is important to understand how the

experience of pediatric cancer affects developmental outcomes throughout child

development. This may be particularly important during the preschool years, where three

of the most common pediatric cancers (acute lymphoblastic leukemia (ALL), central

nervous system tumors, and retinoblastoma) have their highest incidences ("United States

cancer," 2009). Preschool age children diagnosed with pediatric cancer experience a

significant event during one of the most fundamental developmental periods and they

have a very high chance of surviving their disease. It is crucial to understand how their

overall disease experience may alter the course of their development. This study aims to

identify early developmental processes that may be affected by the experience of

pediatric cancer in young children.

It would be simplistic to examine treatment procedures and medical outcomes as the

sole source of developmental differences between children with and without a history of

pediatric cancer. Typical developmental influences, such as parental beliefs, are still very

much present. The interconnectedness of family members as well as the cultural

Page 9: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

2

variability in family beliefs and practices in multicultural societies, such as the United

States, have long been appreciated and assessed in health care settings (Harkness &

Keefer, 2000; Kazak, 1997). In families of children with a history of pediatric cancer,

parents no longer respond only to the child, but also to an impending health crisis.

Parents have to cognitively process the fact that their child has been diagnosed with a

serious illness. They then react to the emotions they experience when learning of

pediatric cancer, including fear (Ehiwe, McGee, Filby, & Thompson, 2012). The

family’s cultural beliefs about the illness and the medical context must then be integrated

with the medical procedures. Simultaneously, the typical parent-child relationship must

be maintained. Development during a health crisis is a very complicated process, with

multiple sources of influence, all of which could lead to different developmental

outcomes for the child. Gaining a better understanding of the way parents and the child

interact as they react to the abnormal stressor of their child’s pediatric cancer could

provide insight into potential developmental differences between children with and

without a history of pediatric cancer.

Cognition

The earliest survivors of pediatric cancer could celebrate the fact that they beat

their disease but were left with relatively extreme cognitive deficits, or a decrease of

around 30 IQ points (Cousens, Waters, Said, & Stevens, 1988). Early research quantified

these cognitive deficits by measuring school performance or IQ points, but did not

specify more subtle areas of cognitive functioning (Costa, 2010; Cousens et al., 1988).

Researchers and practitioners agreed that future treatment protocols should strive to

reduce cognitive deficits. Advances in medicine made this goal realistic as radiation

Page 10: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

3

therapy became used more sparingly and could be more targeted. Certain protocols, such

as ALL, were able to remove radiation therapy from standard cases (Campbell et al.,

2009).

Medical researchers continue to strive to improve pediatric cancer treatments to

maximize effectiveness against the disease and minimize impact to the child’s cognitive

ability. Still, integral treatment modalities, specifically cranial radiation therapy as well

as intrathecal chemotherapy, have been shown to cause long term cognitive changes in

survivors of pediatric cancer (Campbell et al., 2009; Carey et al., 2008; Conklin et al.,

2007; Lesnik, Ciesielski, Hart, Benzel, & Sanders, 1998). Today, children with CNS

treatment (intrathecal chemotherapy and cranial radiation) can develop problems with

visual processing, where they are less able to make sense of information presented

visually (i.e., interpreting a map). They can also develop difficulties with hand eye

coordination and subsequent visual-motor functions. Children who have received these

types of treatments can also develop attention difficulties and a decreased ability to

organize, hold, and manipulate information in their mind (Costa, 2010).

Although cognitive differences are readily reported, the typical effects are not as

severe as early treatment. A previous study reported no differences in teacher-reported

school performance, or enrollment in special education classes for pediatric cancer

survivors, including those who had received radiation therapy (Radcliffe et al., 1996).

Family Emotionality

Previous research highlights the importance of the parent child relationship in

shaping the child’s emotional development (Piotrowski, Lapierre, & Linebarger, 2013).

Through modeling and reinforcement children learn appropriate expressions and actions.

Page 11: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

4

Children’s developmental outcomes are impacted by differing parenting practices and

parents’ own emotional states (Piotrowski et al., 2013). Not only has this interrelation

between parent and child emotions been shown to occur in more typical interactions, it

has also been found specifically in emotional responses to pediatric cancer. The parents’

emotional response to the stressors of pediatric cancer has a direct effect on the child’s

response to their own disease and impacts the child’s functioning (Brown et al., 1993;

Patterson, Holm, & Gurney, 2004).

Coping with pediatric cancer is a highly emotional experience and it follows that

families dealing with chronic illness would have different daily emotional reactions than

families who are not dealing with such an event (Coffey, 2006). A meta-analysis aimed

to identify patterns of parents’ emotional experiences for families of children with a

variety of chronic illnesses. The analysis found that parents of children with a chronic

illness live in an almost constant state of worry. Additionally, they struggle with the

daily, complex caregiving responsibilities, leading to anger and sadness (Coffeey, 2006).

In addition to the emotions relating to the chronic nature of their child’s cancer, parents

of children with pediatric cancer who are currently on treatment have reported feeling

numb, overwhelmed, devastated, and helpless. Additionally, they feel a loss of control,

fear for the child’s death, grief for the loss of their child’s “normal” life, and some

parents even report feelings of guilt (Patterson et al., 2004). After the conclusion of

therapy, parents maintain fear of a relapse and general anxiety about their child’s future

(Patterson et al., 2004). The known bi-directionality of emotional experiences between

parents and children with pediatric cancer coupled with the documented negative

emotions felt by these families increase the importance of examining developmental

Page 12: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

5

differences in parents and children with and without a history of pediatric cancer.

Specifically, it is important to investigate whether the child’s experience with pediatric

cancer, including family emotions and treatment procedures, alters the development of

emotion regulation, executive functioning, and understanding of intentionality.

The Development of Emotion Regulation

Developmental psychologists define emotion regulation as children’s ability to

regulate their emotional experiences by monitoring their own expressive behavior

(Carlson & Wang, 2007). These behaviors alter the experience and expression of the

child’s emotions and are carried out to “meet situational demands and achieve personal

goals” (Blandon, Calkins, Keane, & O’Brian, 2008, p. 1110). The critical time period for

development of emotion regulation is from ages 4-7, which parallels the child’s

decreased likelihood of displaying negative affect (Blandon et al., 2008). Emotion

regulation has been linked to children’s effective stress coping, language development,

cognitive ability, academic achievement, social competence, adjustment, introversion,

and sympathy (Blandon et al., 2008; Carlson & Wang, 2007; Fox, 1994; Southam-Gerow

& Kendall, 2002). Children with deficits in emotion regulation tend to have more

behavioral problems and problems interacting with peers (Blandon et al., 2008). Parents’

behavior has been identified as a “key predictor” of children’s emotion regulation

(Piotrowski et al., 2013).

Caregiver and parental influences, such as attachment and parenting style, are

important mechanisms by which children develop emotion regulation (Blandon et al.,

2008; Piotrowski et al., 2013; Southam-Gerow & Kendall, 2002). Gross and Thompson

(2007) conceptualized the development of emotion regulation into five modalities:

Page 13: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

6

situation selection, situation modification, attentional deployment, cognitive change, and

response modulation. Parental implementation of three of these modalities is cited as the

first exposure children have to emotion regulation. Parents implement situational

selection with the enforcement of daily routines and appropriate emotional demands for

their young children. This progresses to situational modification as parents aid their

children with complex goals, such as completing a difficult puzzle. These modifications

reduce frustration and with proper parental encouragement, allow children to gain

increased self-sufficiency. Parents are also credited with laying the foundation for

cognitive change, which is defined as the emotional appraisal of the situation and one’s

ability to think differently about circumstances. Cognitive change occurs in three ways.

Parents provide information about the situation’s circumstances, verbalize causes of their

child’s emotion, and dictate behavioral rules, such as “big girls do not throw temper

tantrums” (Gross & Thompson, 2007). Knowing that parents of children with pediatric

cancer encounter different, and often more extreme, emotional experiences from parents

of children without pediatric cancer and because parental influence is the initial

mechanism of emotion development, it follows that the development of emotion

regulation may be altered for children with a history of pediatric cancer.

Studying the development of emotion regulation in children with childhood

cancer could be of particular importance because parents of children who have

experienced childhood cancer are often more protective of their children than parents of

children without childhood cancer (Van Dongen-Melman & Sanders-Woudstra, 1986).

This overprotection could lead to more parental control, which has been linked to lower

levels of emotion regulation (Piotrowski et al., 2013). Parents’ behavior, emotions, and

Page 14: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

7

attitudes towards the child’s disease, as well as the child’s own emotional experience,

could lead to children with childhood cancer having an increased risk for developing poor

emotion regulation strategies.

The Development of Executive Functioning

Linked with emotion regulation is executive functioning, which incorporates

higher order, cognitive self-regulatory processes that facilitate the monitoring and control

of thought and action (Carlson, 2005). Specifically, executive function is “adaptive, goal

directed behavior that enables individuals to override more automatic or established

thoughts and responses” (Garon, Bryson, & Smith, 2008, p. 31). The development of

executive functioning allows children to solve novel problems (Garon et al., 2008; Zelazo

& Frye, 1998). Increased executive functioning abilities have been associated with

decreased internalizing and externalizing behavior, as reported by parents and teachers

over a 2 year period during early elementary school (Riggs, Blair, & Greenberg, 2003).

The basis for more complex executive functioning, including the earliest forms of

response inhibition, develops during the first year of life (Garon et al., 2008). The

preschool period (ages 3-5) is a significant developmental period for executive

functioning, as children gradually gain the more complex cognitive skills required to

successfully complete executive functioning tasks (Carlson, 2005; Garon et al., 2008).

Some executive functioning tasks are still too complex for 5 and 6 year olds (Carlson,

2005). This suggests that executive functioning is still developing into the early school

years (Garon et al., 2008). Executive functioning predicts problem solving abilities,

working memory capabilities, social competencies, inhibitory control, and overall self-

regulation (Carlson, 2005; Carlson & Wang, 2007). Deficits in the development of

Page 15: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

8

executive functioning have been associated with problems in planning, organization, and

social skills. People with deficits in executive functioning have difficulty focusing on

what is important. Additionally, poor executive functioning increases the difficulty by

which an individual is able to monitor his/her own behavior. Potentially due to some of

these difficulties, an association between people with ADHD and executive functioning

deficits has been found (Clark, Prior, & Kinsella, 2002).

The development of executive functioning begins in infancy. By preschool, core

components of executive functioning are developing. These executive functioning

building blocks are the initial cognitive processes needed to develop higher order

cognitive processes throughout childhood and into adulthood (Garon et al., 2008).

Children prepare for more goal directed behavior through increases in their ability to

focus on relevant information and dismiss irrelevant information. Once infants are able

to control their attention instead of simply looking at a stimulus because of its novelty,

infants can begin to develop the flexibility needed to shift their attention in line with the

demands of a task and the infant’s internal goals. Between 4 and 6 months, infants

develop the ability to shift attention between two stimuli (Garon et al., 2008). The next

important step in executive functioning development is the ability for children to inhibit

automatic responses. Response inhibition allows children to apply a rule to their

behavior. Earliest response suppression, where a child stops an activity they find

enjoyable at the request of their caregiver, occurs during the latter half of the child’s first

year of life. The child’s ability to stop a behavior after hearing the caregiver’s command

of “no” or “don’t” increases with age (Garon et al., 2008). Response inhibition has been

shown to increase from 40% of the time for 8 month olds to 90% in 33 month olds

Page 16: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

9

(Kochanska, 2002; Kochanska et al., 1998). As children move from infancy, through

toddlerhood, and into the preschool years, their ability to inhibit automatic responses

gradually increases. The last aspect of executive functioning to develop is the ability to

regulate their own behavior using mental representations (Garon et al., 2008). This

continues to be challenging as children move from preschool into early elementary school

(Carlson, 2005).

Because of the central role that executive functioning plays in cognitive

development, it would be important to examine how it relates to emotion regulation in

children coping with pediatric cancer. In healthy populations, correlations between the

development of executive functioning and emotional regulation have been found and

even go as far as to suggest that the development of emotion regulation is tied to the

child’s development of executive functioning (Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005).

Changes in executive functioning in children with a history of pediatric cancer

may stem from intrathecal chemotherapy and irradiation therapy, staples in treatment for

leukemia and brain tumors (Campbell et al., 2009; Carey et al., 2008; Lesnik et al., 1998).

A subset of children who receive this type of treatment experience reduced cortical white

matter volume, particularly in the right prefrontal cortex (Campbell et al., 2009; Carey et

al., 2008). Some facets of executive functioning, particularly working memory, have

been shown to differ in older children (10-20 years of age) with a history of pediatric

cancer (Campbell, Scaduto, Van Slyke, Niarhos, Whitlock, & Compas, 2009). Despite

significant differences between the children with a history of pediatric cancer and the

control group, both group means were within the average range of scores. This indicates

that even though children with a history of pediatric cancer may have lower executive

Page 17: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

10

functioning as it relates to working memory, their working memory ability is still intact

(Campbell et al., 2009). These studies were conducted on older children, most of who

were well off therapy. No studies could be identified that compared younger children

with a history of pediatric cancer to a control group on earlier and more basic

developments of executive functioning.

The Development of Identifying Intentionality

Related to the development of executive functioning is the social cognitive ability

to understand others’ intentions, beliefs, and desires, known as theory of mind (Garfield,

Peterson, & Perry, 2001). Children evaluate others based on their own understanding of

criteria they see as relevant (Berndt & Berndt, 1975). A person’s motive becomes a

relevant criterion for children when evaluating others during their early school years as

they develop a sense of other people’s intentionality (Berndt & Berndt, 1975). Goal

directed behavior that satisfies an individual’s desire can be called intentional actions.

Identifying intentionality is one’s ability to discern whether an actor performed an action

purposefully or by accident. Individual’s with the ability to identify the intentions of

others can examine identical actions by two different people, and from each person’s

point of view, differentiate the acts differently as either intended or accidental (Wellman

& Phillips, 2001). As children’s cognitive abilities and social understanding increase,

they are better able to separate the consequences of another’s behavior from their

intentions (Piaget, 1932). The ability to identify other’s intentions allows children to

move beyond a system of right and wrong based solely on the consequences of another’s

actions. However, this process develops slowing, beginning at a very early age. By the

time children reach 3 years of age, they have a rudimentary understanding of the mind’s

Page 18: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

11

causal power for individual’s actions (Moses, 2001). Still, 3-year-olds have very little

insight into the concept of intention due to their inability to construct a meta-

representational understanding that another’s belief differs from their own point of view

(Astington, 2001; Moses, 2001). At ages 5 and 6, children still tend to judge outcomes as

good or bad based on consequences, with little regard for other’s intention (Piaget, 1932).

Young children fully develop a sense of other people’s intentionality during their early

school years (Berndt & Berndt, 1975). Still there are some situations in which children

younger than early-school-age are able to account for the intentions of others in a given

situation. Successful development of the ability to identify the intentions of others aids in

the more complex development of social cognition (Malle, Moses, Baldwin, 2001). The

development of intentionality includes the development of smaller domains such as the

beliefs, desires, and awareness of other people. The development of the ability to

identify other’s intentions is not simply an analytical assessment of another’s mental state,

but includes affective and moral responses to both the person’s intentions and actions

produced as a result of their intentions (Malle et al., 2001). Without the fully developed

ability to identify the intentions of others, moral judgments of other’s actions could be

negatively impacted (Malle et al., 2001). This has the potential to create strain on

interpersonal relationships and have lasting consequences throughout the lifespan.

The key to the development of intentionality is the child’s ability to differentiate

between intentions and desires. Although children as young as one year of age may be

able to detect intentions, children younger than three are incapable of distinguishing

intentions from desires. To successfully develop the ability to understand other’s

intentions, infants must first develop a “like me” analogy that relates the infant’s actions

Page 19: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

12

with the actions of others (Malle et al., 2001; Meltzoff & Brooks, 2001). As the infant

begins to control their own behavior in a goal directed manner, they infer others’ goals

from observed actions (Malle et al., 2001). This progresses during the preschool years,

where children develop more mature ideas about the motivational states behind intentions.

However, preschool children still struggle understanding that another person’s beliefs

constrain their actions (Malle et al., 2001; Moses, 2001). This ability successfully

develops with the child’s theory of mind, around age 4 (Mueller, Lieberman-Finestone,

Carpendale, Hammond, & Bibok, 2012). Even after gaining an understanding that a

person’s actions are driven by their beliefs, it is not until 5 or 6 years of age that children

reliably distinguish intention and desire, understanding that the outcome of an action may

not be what the actor would like (Astington, 2001; Malle et al., 2001). Fully developing

a complex understanding of identifying intentionality is a gradual process, which can be

deemed complete when a child understands that people’s beliefs and intentions are but a

mental representation that mediates their actions (Perner, 1991 cited in Malle et al., 2001).

For children with cancer, understanding the intentions of others, particularly those

helping them deal with cancer, would be vitally important. It may be that the experience

of childhood cancer accelerates the development of understanding others’ intentionality.

Some pediatric cancer treatments have significant side effects, making children feel

worse after treatment than before. In spite of feeling worse in the short term, it could be

the case that children come to understand that medical professionals’ and parents’ efforts

are intended to make them better. No prior research examining pediatric cancer patients’

ability to identify the intentions of their caregivers could be identified. However,

healthcare teams make a great effort to explain to each child the nature of the disease and

Page 20: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

13

the treatment for it in an understandable way. Many major healthcare centers around the

United States employ Child Life Specialists. Facilitating communication between

healthcare professionals, parents, and the child is one of Child Life’s main goals

(www.childlife.org). It follows that having trained specialists facilitate conversations

about treatment could help children with pediatric cancer separate the doctor’s intentions

from treatment side effects. It would be important to see if children with pediatric cancer

are able to decipher the intentions of others at an earlier age than children without a

cancer diagnosis.

Impact of Pediatric Cancer on Developmental Processes

There has long been a call for investigations exploring the developmental impact

of pediatric cancer (Pai et al., 2007; Van Dongen-Melman & Saunders-Woudstra, 1986).

In reference to this topic, two opposing outcomes for children with a history of pediatric

cancer were theorized: either developmental processes would be unaffected by the

disease or the developmental process would in some way be altered, leading to atypical

development (Susman, Hollenbeck, Stope, Hersh, Levine, & Pizzo, 1980 cited in Van

Dongen-Melman & Sanders-Woudstra, 1896). Despite these suggestions, the

developmental consequences of pediatric cancer remain a relatively unexplored area of

psychological research.

This research question is even more important today, as survival, not death, is the

more prevalent outcome for a child diagnosed with pediatric cancer (Armenian et al.,

2013; www.stjude.org). It would be important to determine potential long term

developmental consequences of the experience of childhood cancer. Identifying potential

difficulties for this population could be a first step that may lead to interventions to

Page 21: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

14

minimize residual developmental effects resulting from the child’s disease experience.

This, in turn, could help practitioners to better understand and psychologically assist the

children and families under their care. To begin working toward those goals, this study

specifically aimed to:

1. Identify potential developmental differences in terms of emotion

regulation, executive functioning, and understanding of intentionality

between children who have experienced childhood cancer and those who

have not experienced a life threatening illnesses.

2. Examine parenting differences in terms of emotional expressiveness,

parent protection, and perception of child vulnerability between those who

are rearing a chronically ill child and parents rearing a healthy child.

3. Identify possible parental influences on children’s executive functioning,

emotion regulation, and understanding of intentionality, regardless of

illness history.

4. Examine whether parental influences on children’s executive functioning,

emotion regulation, and understanding of intentionality differ between

families with and without pediatric cancer experience.

It is hypothesized that…

1. In regards to the child measures…

A. Children with a history of cancer will have significantly lower

cognitive ability than children without a history of pediatric cancer.

Page 22: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

15

B. Children who have experienced pediatric cancer will have

significantly lower levels of emotion regulation and executive

functioning than children without a history of pediatric cancer.

C. Children with a history of pediatric cancer will have a significantly

higher level of understanding of other’s intentions than children

who have not experienced childhood cancer.

2. In regards to parent measures…

A. Families with a child with a history of pediatric cancer will express

significantly more emotions, both positive and negative, than

families with a healthy child.

B. Parents will encourage more emotion expression for children with

pediatric cancer but will be less willing to show emotion,

particularly negative emotion, to their child compared to parents of

children without a history of pediatric cancer.

C. Parents of children with a history of pediatric cancer will report

lower levels of emotion regulation in their children than parents of

children without a history of pediatric cancer.

D. Parents of a child with pediatric cancer will perceive significantly

higher levels of child vulnerability and display more parental

protectiveness than parents of a child without a history of pediatric

cancer.

Page 23: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

16

3. Significant correlations will be found between the child’s emotion regulation

and executive functioning and between the child’s executive functioning and

ability to identify intentionality.

4. The child’s performance on the emotion regulation tasks will be predicted by

family emotion expressiveness, parents’ perception of child vulnerability,

parental protectiveness, and scores on the child emotion regulation task.

Page 24: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

17

METHOD

Participants

Children with (N = 39) and without (N = 40) a history of pediatric cancer were

participants in this study. Forty children with a current diagnosis or a history of pediatric

cancer and their parents were recruited through the Brenner Children’s Hospital Pediatric

Hematology and Oncology Clinic in Winston Salem, NC. Although consent was

obtained for 40 children in the pediatric oncology clinic, one child and her mother were

unable to complete the measures because of their low English language proficiency.

Neither the mother nor the child was able to complete an entire section of the study.

Therefore, no information from this family was included. The final sample consisted of

39 children (18 girls and 21 boys) with a history of pediatric cancer. The ethnicities of

the children included in the study are listed in Table 1. Children from 4.00 years to 7.98

years of age were recruited. The average age of the pediatric cancer participants was

6.13 years (SD = 1.32). The average age of the children at the time of their diagnosis was

2.97 years (SD = 1.44), ranging from 0.04 years to 6.41 years. Information about the

child’s disease and treatment history was also collected. A complete list of the cancer

diagnoses can be found in Table 2. At the time the children enrolled in the study, 12

(30.8%) were currently on treatment and 27 (69.2%) children had completed their

treatment and were considered “off therapy.” The average time since diagnosis was 3.14

years (SD = 1.54), ranging from 0.15 to 5.97 years. A breakdown of the treatment

modalities experienced by the children with a history of pediatric cancer as well as an

intensity rating of the child’s treatment (ITR) can be found in Table 3. It should be noted

that ITR scores in this study were generated based on parent reports, not from a medical

Page 25: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

18

professional with access to the child’s medical chart. Because almost all children

diagnosed with pediatric cancer undergo surgery to place a central venous catheter (port),

it is probable that some parents who reported their child received surgery were referring

to port placement or removal, not surgery as part of the treatment regimen.

________________________________________________________________________

Table 1

Gender and Ethnicity of Hospital and School Samples

________________________________________________________________________

Hospital School

Gender

Girls 18 20

Boys 21 20

Ethnicity

Black/African American 5 2

Hispanic 7 0

White/ Caucasian 25 32

Other 2 6

Asian 0 2

White and Indian 0 1

White and Black 1 1

White and Hispanic 1 2

Page 26: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

19

________________________________________________________________________

Table 2

Diagnoses for Children with a History of Pediatric Cancer

________________________________________________________________________

Diagnoses Frequency

Leukemias 24

Acute Lymphoblastic Leukemia 23

Acute Myeloid Leukemia 1

Lymphomas 3

Anaplastic Large Cell Lymphoma 1

Burkitt’s Lymphoma 2

CNS, Intracranial, and Intraspinal Neoplasms 2

Medulloblastoma 1

Pilocytic Astrocytoma 1

Neuroblastoma and Peripheral Nervous Cell Tumors 1

Neuroblastoma 1

Renal Tumors 5

Wilms’ Tumor 5

Hepatic Tumors 2

Hepatoblastoma 2

Malignant Bone/Soft Tissue Tumors 2

Ewing Sarcoma 1

Infantile Fibrosarcoma 1

________________________________________________________________________

Page 27: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

20

Table 3

Treatment Modalities and Intensity for Children with a History of Pediatric Cancer

________________________________________________________________________

Modality Frequency

Chemotherapy 39

Surgery 27

Radiation 5

Bone marrow/stem cell transplant 1

Intensity of Treatment Rating (ITR-2)

Level 1: Least Intensive 4

Level 2: Moderately Intensive 22

Level 3: Very Intensive 9

Level 4: Most Intensive 4

Children and parents without a history of pediatric cancer served as the

comparison group for families with a child diagnosed with pediatric cancer. Forty

children without a history of childhood cancer and their parents were recruited from local

preschools and after school programs. Children in the preschools, as well as kindergarten

and first grade children in the after school programs, ages 4 – 8, were invited to

participate in the study. Twenty girls and 20 boys completed the study. The ethnicities

of the children without a history of pediatric cancer are listed in Table 1. The average

Page 28: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

21

age of the children without a history of pediatric cancer enrolled on study was 5.40 (SD =

0.969) and ranged from 4.04 to 7.58 years of age.

Only children who spoke proficient English were included in the study. Children

from both the hospital and school samples were excluded from enrollment if the child had

significant intellectual disabilities or if they lacked sufficient mobility and/ or the verbal

ability needed to complete the tasks. Additionally, children who did not give verbal

assent were excluded.

In both samples, one caregiver was recruited along with the child. Sixty-five

mothers, 12 fathers, and 2 grandmothers completed the parent measures. The breakdown

of caregivers enrolled on study for families with and without a history of pediatric cancer

can be found in Table 4. The grandmothers who participated in the study did not have

primary custody of their grandchildren but each accompanied their grandchild the day the

child was recruited. Both had the power to sign consent forms and because they were

integrally involved in substantial caretaking responsibilities, it was believed that a valid

report could be obtained. All information collected from the grandmothers was included

in all analyses, with the exception of the average age of parents. Because the children’s

legal guardian was a parent(s) who was substantially younger than the grandmothers, a

more accurate assessment of the average age of the sample’s legal guardians was

obtained by excluding the two grandmothers. The age of parents of a child with a history

of pediatric cancer ranged from 23.97 to 51.97 years of age (M = 36.09, SD = 6.87). The

age of parents of a child without a history of pediatric cancer ranged from 24.95 to 58.98

(M = 38.90, SD = 7.41). The average number of children for families with and without a

history of pediatric cancer is 2.26 and 2.18 respectively. Specific family demographic

Page 29: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

22

information, including the highest level of education completed by the participating

parent or spouse, marital status, and number of children, for both samples can be found in

Table 5. The demographic information form completed by both hospital and school

families can be seen in Appendix A.

________________________________________________________________________

Table 4

Caregivers Enrolled on Study for Hospital and School Samples

________________________________________________________________________

Hospital School

Mother 29 36

Father 08 04

Grandmother 02 00

Group comparisons for basic demographic information, including child age,

parents’ level of education, parent’s age, and total number of children were calculated to

identify variables to be used as possible covariates in subsequent analyses. Parent’s age

and total number of children did not differ significantly between the hospital and school

samples. However, the two samples significantly differed on child age, t(77) = 2.78, p =

0.007, and parents level of education, t(75) = 1.71, p < .001. The children in the school

sample were significantly younger and had parents that were significantly more educated

than the children in the hospital sample. These differences were considered as possible

covariates in subsequent analyses.

Page 30: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

23

________________________________________________________________________

Table 5

Parent and Family Demographic Information for Hospital and School Samples

________________________________________________________________________

Hospital School

Caregiver’s Highest Level of Education

Some High School, no diploma 2 0

High School Diploma or GED 11 1

Some College Credit, no degree 7 3

Associates Degree 7 0

Bachelor’s Degree 5 16

Master’s Degree 4 12

Doctorate Degree 1 8

Caregiver’s Marital Status

Married 31 35

Divorced 3 1

Single 5 4

Number of Children

One 10 10

Two 12 14

Three 15 15

Four 1 1

Five 1 0

Page 31: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

24

This study was approved by the Wake Forest University Institutional Review

Board as well as the Wake Forest Baptist Medical Center Institutional Review Board and

Wake Forest Baptist Health’s Comprehensive Cancer Center Protocol Review Committee.

Additional permission was obtained from school directors before children and families

were approached and enrolled on study. IRB approved consent forms for recruitment at

Brenner Children’s Hospital’s Oncology Clinic and the local schools can be found in

Appendix B and Appendix C, respectively.

Measures

Parent Questionnaires

Family Expressiveness Questionnaire (Halberstadt, 1986): The revised 20 item

questionnaire asks parents to report how often immediate family members respond with

various emotions. The 20 items include 10 positive emotion items and 10 negative

emotion items. Examples: “Thanking family members for something they have done.”

“Showing dislike for someone.” See Appendix D for the full list of items. This

questionnaire is assessed using a 9 point Likert scale (1 = not at all frequently, 9 = very

frequently). Mean positive and mean negative expressiveness scores are calculated.

Higher scores indicate higher levels of emotional expressiveness. This measure has

shown sufficient reliability. Previous studies have found both subscales of this measure

to be reliable with Cronbach’s α between 0.75 and 0.90 (Halberstadt, 1986; Ramsden &

Hubbard, 2002). This measure also displayed sufficient reliability in this study. The

positive emotion subscale Cronbach’s α = 0.841 and the negative emotion subscale

Cronbach’s α = 0.837.

Page 32: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

25

Emotion Regulation Checklist (Shields & Cicchetti, 1997): Thirteen items from

the Emotion Regulation subscale ask parents to report their child’s ability to regulate

emotions. This subscale can be divided further with items addressing the child’s

negativity, affect, and emotion regulation. Because this study is focused on the child’s

overall ability to regulate his or her emotions, the scale’s total score is used in all

analyses. Examples of the items include: “Can say when s/he is feeling sad, angry or

mad, fearful or afraid.” “Is empathetic towards others.” See Appendix E for the

complete measure. This questionnaire is assessed using a 4-point Likert scale (i.e., rarely,

sometimes, often, and always). After reverse coding, item scores are summed with

higher scores indicating more emotion regulation. This measure yielded sufficient

reliability in past studies. Previous studies have reported Chronbach’s α between 0.85

and 0.96 (Ramsden & Hubbard, 2002; Shields & Cicchetti, 1997). Although not quite as

high as previous reports, sufficient reliability was also found for the current study

(Cronbach’s α = 0.786).

Parent Emotion Checklist (Nelson et al., 2012): This measure asks parents to

report how important it is for their children to express when they are happy, sad, angry,

and afraid. This measure is scored using a 4-point Likert scale (i.e., not important to very

important). Higher scores indicate parents feel that having their child express that

particular emotion is of increased importance.

The second part of this questionnaire asks parents to report how often they show

their child when they are happy, sad, angry, and afraid. The full measure can be found in

Appendix F. This is scored on a 4-point Likert scale (i.e., rarely to always) and higher

scores indicate increased frequency of expression of parent emotions to the child. To

Page 33: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

26

examine differences between positive and negative emotions, the average of sad, angry,

and scared are compared to the happy score. In past studies, the authors report high

internal consistency for both African American and European American samples

(Chronbach’s α = 0.96 for both ethic groups; Nelson et al., 2012). This measure was not

specifically tested on a Hispanic sample, therefore a previous measure of internal

consistency was not available for this ethnicity. Across all ethnicities, this measure

produced sufficient reliability in the current study as well. The Cronbach’s α = 0.922 for

the child negative emotion subscale and Cronbach’s α = 0.759 for the parent negative

emotion subscale. The scale’s overall internal reliability was also sufficient, Cronbach’s

α = 0.729.

Child Health and Family Functioning Questionnaire (Thomasgard & Metz, 1999):

Nineteen items from the parent protection and child vulnerability subscales are

implemented in this study. The full measure can be found in Appendix G.

The child vulnerability subscale assesses parental perceptions of their child’s

vulnerability to illness, both severe, chronic illnesses as well as common illnesses.

Additionally, there are items related to real or feared separation or loss (e.g., “I keep my

child from participating in activities because s/he might get or become ill”). This

subscale consists of 9 items total: one “yes” or “no” item (i.e., My child currently has a

chronic medical condition) and 8 items scored on a 4-point Likert scale (i.e., rarely to

always). The “yes/no” chronic illness item is scored separately. The other 8 items are

averaged to create a total score for child vulnerability. A higher average score for these

items indicates higher perceived vulnerability.

Page 34: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

27

The parent protection subscale was designed as an 11 item subscale that measured

the parents’ perception of child safety and autonomy. However, upon examination of the

individual items, two distinct factors measuring parent protectiveness and child

independence could be imagined. A factor analysis of the 11 items in the parent

protectiveness subscale confirmed the presence of two factors. The factor loadings for

the individual items can be found in Table 6.

This factor analysis was conducted using a promax rotation method to examine if

the factors would correlate with one another. There was not a significant correlation

between the two factors, r = 0.102. This provides more evidence that these items are

measuring two distinct constructs.

Based on the results of the factor analysis and to increase internal reliability, this

study broke the previously published parent protectiveness subscale into two distinct

subscales: independence and protectiveness. The newly created independence subscale

consists of 8 items, averaged for a total score measuring the child’s level of independence

(e.g., “My child can be trusted to do things alone”). Higher scores indicate more child

autonomy. The protectiveness subscale consisted of 4 items, averaged for a total score

measuring parent protection.(e.g., “When I am not with my child, I am worried that s/he

may get hurt or be involved in an accident). Higher scores indicate more parental

protective behaviors. The parent protection subscale and the independence subscale

were measured using a 4 point Likert scale (i.e., rarely to always).

The final item in the Child Health and Family Functioning scale, “It is difficult to

set limits on behavior,” was not previously included in a specific subscale. Therefore, it

will be assessed independently of the subscales in the current study.

Page 35: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

28

________________________________________________________________________

Table 6

Factor Analysis of Parent Protectiveness Subscale

________________________________________________________________________

Item Factor 1 Factor 2

I leave my child with anyone outside my family. .205 .594

My child deals with separation from me. -.006 .151

When I am not with my child, I am worried -.048 .824

that s/he may get hurt or be involved

in an accident.

I allow my child to choose his/her own clothing. .591 -.118

My child can choose the proper foods. .524 .087

My child can get along without me .562 -.211

My child can be trusted to do things alone. .775 .044

My child can bathe without help. .697 .040

My child is encouraged to express his/her own .645 .194

opinion, even when I disagree.

My child is allowed to refuse to eat certain foods. .353 -.205

I worry about my child’s safety when I am away -.067 .835

overnight.

My child can use the toilet without assistance. .393 .042

This questionnaire was originally given as an interview, which correctly predicted

parental perception of child vulnerability 77% of the time and parental protection 76% of

the time (Thomasgard & Metz, 1999). In the current study, this questionnaire did reveal

Page 36: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

29

sufficient reliability for the child vulnerability and newly conceptualized independence

subscale, Cronbach’s α = 0.812 and 0.713, respectively. Reliability for the parent

protectiveness subscale was not sufficient, Cronbach’s α = 0.525. To increase reliability,

one item (i.e., “My child deals with separation from me”) was dropped from the subscale,

increasing reliability to α = 0.656. Not only did dropping this item increase reliability,

but the dropped item was the only item in the parent protectiveness subscale asking about

the child, instead of the parent, providing reason to examine this item separately.

Intensity of Treatment Rating (ITR-2; Werba et al., 2007): Parents of children

with a history of childhood cancer were asked questions related to their child’s diagnosis

and treatment so the ITR-2 could be completed. Questions asked included diagnosis and

stage, whether or not the child was on treatment, whether or not the child had

experienced a relapse, and which treatment modalities had been used to combat disease

(e.g., chemotherapy, surgery, radiation, and transplant). The complete ITR measure can

be found in Appendix H. The experimenter evaluated the diagnosis and treatment

modalities on the ITR-2, which were then assigned a score between 1 and 4 for each child.

Higher scores indicate more intense treatment.

The authors of the measure report high inter-rater reliability for this scale (r =

0.87). Content validity is established, as ratings from teams of pediatric oncologists and

nurse practitioners found a strong correlation (r = 0.95) between the scores assigned by

the ITR-2, the current treatment protocols, and outcomes for pediatric cancer.

Child Tasks

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (Dunn & Dunn, 1959): This task assesses

proficiency of receptive English vocabulary and is a general measure of cognitive

Page 37: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

30

development. Children are shown pages with four drawings. The experimenter reads a

word and the child must point to the drawing that represents the word (e.g., empty, castle,

vehicle). There are 12 words in each set and the child can only move to the next set if he

or she misses fewer than 8 words in the set. Because of time constraints and to better

keep the child’s attention, the experimenter started one set above the recommended

starting point and skipped up to two sets if the child was performing well. These

alterations mean that the Peabody scores reported in this experiment may be slightly

inflated and should be interpreted as an approximation of the child’s cognitive ability.

Raw scores are calculated by subtracting the total number of errors from the

ceiling item in the last attempted set. Raw scores were then converted to a standard score,

using norms in the manual. Higher scores indicate higher cognitive ability. This is a

standard task used when measuring cognitive functioning and internal reliability is very

well established (Chronbach’s α = 0.91-0.95 for children age 4-7 for raw and

standardized scores; Dunn & Dunn, 1959).

Silly Sounds Task (Gerstadt et al., 1994): This task is similar to a Stroop task and

measures the children’s ability to suppress their prepotent response, which is an indicator

of executive function. First, five congruent trials are presented. The children are asked

to bark when they see a picture of a dog and to meow when they see a picture of a cat.

Next, 18 incongruent trials are presented. During the incongruent trials, the children are

asked to meow like a cat when shown a picture of a dog and to bark like a dog when

shown a picture of a cat. The identical pictures of a dog and a cat were used in both the

congruent and incongruent trials (Appendix I). In total, this task consisted of 23 trials

and children were given a 0 (incorrect response) or 1 (correct response) for each trial.

Page 38: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

31

Separate total scores for the congruent and incongruent trials were calculated by adding

together the child’s correct responses for each trial type. Congruent trials display the

child’s ability to make the correct animal sounds for dogs and cats and their

understanding of the task. Higher scores on the incongruent trials indicate higher

executive functioning.

Secret Sharing Task (Bethel et al., 2011): In order to test the child’s emotion

regulation, the experimenter asked the child to keep two secrets. In both secret keeping

tasks, the experimenter first defines a secret as, “something somebody tells you that you

are not supposed to tell anyone else.” Following the definition, the experimenter reveals

one of two secrets to the child. This task is repeated twice during the experiment so that

each child is asked to keep both secrets. These secrets were counterbalanced in the two

task orders used in the experiment.

Zoo Secret: The experimenter told the child that she is afraid of lions or tigers

(depending on task order) and reminded the child not to reveal the secret.

Ribbon Secret: The experimenter told the children that there is a ribbon to give

their doctor/nurse or teacher but that they cannot tell anyone about the ribbon because it

is a surprise.

After each secret is explained to the child, a talking, computerized robot is

presented on the computer and he asks five questions. Each question more explicitly asks

the child to reveal the secret (e.g., “What kind of animals do you think she might be

afraid of?”, “What kind of prize could we give?”). The complete script for this task can

be found in Appendix J. The number of questions required for the child to reveal the

secret, or if the child kept the secret throughout the task, is recorded from 1 (i.e., revealed

Page 39: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

32

secret after first question) to 6 (i.e., kept secret throughout the task). This task measures

the child’s ability to regulate emotions. Higher scores indicate better emotion regulation.

The creators of this task established validity for using a robot to prompt the child

instead of a second adult. Their research indicated that children responded to the robot

and to a second adult in a similar fashion by reporting a “moderate, positive correlation”

between the children’s responses to the robot and the second adult (r = 0.327, p = 0.042;

Bethel et al., 2011, p. 5). The authors also reported no difference between the level of

prompting required for children to reveal the secret to the robot or adult, F(1, 13) = 0.335,

p = 0.57); (Bethel et al., 2011).

Identifying Intentionality (Baird & Moses, 2001; Pellizzoni et al., 2009): The

child listens to five short stories about children who intentionally or unintentionally cause

their friends to feel happy, sad, angry, or scared. These stories are accompanied by

sequential cartoon images depicting the action of the story. Two sets of stories were

created: one with girl characters and one with boy characters. The girls heard stories

about the girls and the boys heard stories about the boys. The stories and all cartoon

images can be found in Appendix K.

During the task, children are asked to identify whether the antagonist meant to

make their friend feel happy, sad, angry, or scared and whether or not the friend believes

that the antagonist meant to make him or her feel happy, sad, angry, or scared. There was

one story for each of the negative emotions (sad, angry, and scared) and two stories for

happy. This task measures the child’s ability to identify other’s intentions. Each child

receives two scores on this task.

Page 40: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

33

The first score is a total knowledge score. The knowledge questions include what

object the antagonist has with them, whether or not the antagonist and their friend likes or

dislikes the object, how the friend would feel upon seeing the object, and whether or not

the antagonist knows how the friend will react to the object. Each child is asked a total of

24 knowledge questions. However, very few children were able to correctly identify the

drawing of the hamster. Consequently, that question was not included in the total

knowledge score. Therefore, children were assigned a total knowledge score out of 23,

with higher scores indicating greater knowledge of the stories’ plotlines.

Children also receive a total score for the test questions. Children earned a 0, 1,

or 2 points for each pair of test questions (i.e., Did the antagonist act on purpose? Did the

antagonist act on accident?). They received a score of 0 if they answered both questions

in the pair incorrectly, a score of 1 if they answered one question in the pair correctly,

and a score of 2 if they answered both questions in the pair correctly. Additionally,

children who had a consistent response pattern (i.e., all yes or all no responses for the test

questions in a particular story) were given a score of 0. Because there are 2 pairs of test

questions for each story, the child can earn a total test score of 20 points (4 points per

story for five stories). Higher scores indicate better understanding of intentionality.

In summary, the hypotheses for the current study and the measures that will be

used to test the hypotheses are included in Table 7.

Procedure:

Children with a history of cancer and their parents were invited to participate in

the research study during scheduled medical appointments. If the parent and child agreed

Page 41: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

34

________________________________________________________________________

Table 7

Summary Table of Hypotheses and Measures

________________________________________________________________________

Hypotheses Measure

Children with a history of pediatric cancer

will…

1. have lower cognitive ability

2. lower levels of emotion regulation

3. lower levels of executive functioning

4. better understanding of other’s

intentions

1. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test

2. Zoo and Ribbon Secret Sharing Tasks

3. Silly Sounds Task

4. Identifying Intentionality Stories

Families with a child with a history of

pediatric cancer will…

1. express significantly more emotions,

both positive and negative

2. Encourage more emotion expression

for children

3. Be less willing to show emotion,

particularly negative emotion

4. report lower levels of emotion

regulation in their children

5. perceive significantly higher levels of

child vulnerability

6. display more parental protectiveness

without a history

1. Family Expressiveness Questionnaire

2. Parent Emotion Checklist

3. Parent Emotion Checklist

4. Emotion Regulation Checklist

5. Child Health & Family Functioning

Questionnaire

6. Child Health & Family Functioning

Questionnaire

Page 42: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

35

to participate, the researcher thoroughly explained the experiment. Both parent and child

were given an opportunity to ask questions before obtaining written consent. If both

parents were present at the medical appointment, the researcher allowed the family to

decide which parent would participate. Once consent was obtained, parents were given

the packet of questionnaires and asked to complete the measures while the researcher

interviewed the child. The majority of parents completed the questionnaires quietly

while in the same room as the researcher and the child.

A separate consent letter and parent questionnaires were sent home with local

school children without a history of pediatric cancer. The parent questionnaire packet for

the parents of the school children was identical to the parent questionnaire packet

completed by parents of children with pediatric cancer with one exception. The parents

at the schools were not asked to complete an ITR-2, as it did not apply to their child.

Parents who were interested in participating in the study with their child were asked to

sign the consent form and complete the questionnaires before returning the form to the

child’s teacher. The researchers’ contact information was provided should any parents

have questions about the experiment. Children whose parents returned consent forms

were pulled from their classrooms during the school day or during their after school

program to complete the experiment.

All parents, both at the hospital and at the schools, were explicitly told that their

child may participate in the study even if they chose not to complete the parent

questionnaires. However, all but one parent at least partially completed the

questionnaires. Ninety five percent or 75 of the parents completed all measures. Of the

parents that did not complete all measures, 3 were from the hospital sample and 1 was

Page 43: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

36

from the school sample. Two of the parents did not complete the measures because of

difficulty with the English language, 1 parent missed the last page, and 1 parent cited a

lack of time to complete the measures. Parents completed the questionnaires in

approximately 10-15 minutes.

Once parental consent was obtained, the experiment was briefly introduced to the

child and the researcher asked for verbal assent. If the child gave assent, the researcher

proceeded with the experiment. There were identical procedures for children with and

without a history of pediatric cancer, with one exception. In one of the Secret Sharing

tasks, the children were asked not to reveal that the researcher had a ribbon for either a

doctor or nurse (i.e., children with a history of cancer) or their teacher (i.e., children

without a history of pediatric cancer).

The children’s tasks were presented in two orders to reduce order effects and to

account for fatigue. An almost equivalent number of girls and boys took form A and

form B. In both forms of the experiment, the Peabody Picture Vocabulary task was

administered first. In form A, the Peabody was followed by the Silly Sounds task, Zoo

Secret Sharing task, Identifying Intentionality stories, and, finally, the Ribbon Secret

Sharing task. In form B, the Peabody was followed by the Identifying Intentionality

stories, the Ribbon Secret Sharing task, the Silly Sounds task, and the Zoo Secret Sharing

task. All tasks, excluding the Peabody, were presented via a laptop computer to make the

stimuli more appealing and attention catching. The child’s responses were recorded by

the experimenter. The total experiment took approximately 20-30 minutes to complete.

At the conclusion of the experiment, the researcher thanked the child for participating and

the child was asked to choose a small gift. Children with a history of pediatric cancer

Page 44: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

37

were left in the care of their parents, and children without a history of pediatric cancer

were returned to their classrooms.

Page 45: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

38

RESULTS

The total scores for each measure were calculated according to the procedures

described in the method section. As mentioned, with respect to the basic demographic

variables, the sample of children with a history of pediatric cancer and the sample of

healthy children significantly differed on child age, t(77) = 2.78, p = 0.007, and parent

level of education, t(75) = 1.71, p < .001, the measure of family SES. Subsequent

analyses controlled for these variables if the possible covariates were significantly

correlated with the dependent variable. This was done to remove variance related to

demographic variables from the target analysis. There were no differences for any of the

measures based on the child’s gender; therefore, all further analyses did not differentiate

between boys and girls. Child’s age did produce significant differences in executive

functioning and identifying intentionality, which is to be expected since these abilities

continue to develop during this age period.

The hospital sample was examined for any possible differences based on the

children’s differing disease history. There were no significant differences on any of the

child tasks based on ITR scores. Because very few of the children with a history of

pediatric cancer experienced radiation therapy or a transplant and all of the children were

given chemotherapy, no separate analyses examining potential effects of these treatment

modalities could be performed. Additionally, it was difficult to distinguish children who

received surgery as part of their treatment regimen and children who only underwent

surgery only for port placement. As a result, no additional analyses were conducted

examining potential effects of surgery as a treatment modality. Considerations were

given to conducting analyses of individual diagnoses. To conserve power, this was not

Page 46: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

39

done because of the relatively small sample size. Therefore, all analyses examined the

effect of a history of pediatric cancer for the entire sample.

The means and standard deviations for the parent measures, calculated separately

for the hospital and school samples, can be found in Table 8. All data collected were

used in these calculations with the exception of parent age. The means and standard

deviations for child tasks, calculated separately for the hospital and school samples, can

be found in Table 9. All subsequent analyses in this study were run using two tailed tests

to provide more confidence in significant findings.

In addition to basic descriptive analyses, correlations were calculated between all

variables. Complete tables of correlations can be found in the appendix. Appendix H

displays all correlations across both conditions. Appendix I displays correlations for

children with a history of pediatric cancer and Appendix J displays correlations for

children without a history of pediatric cancer.

Although there were not significant correlations between child emotion regulation

and executive functioning or child executive functioning and identifying intentionality for

both groups, as hypothesized, interesting correlations did emerge. In the school sample,

there was a significant correlation between the parent report of emotion regulation and

the task measuring executive functioning (Silly Sounds incongruent trials) (r = .40, p

= .012). This correlation did not reach significance for the hospital sample. In the school

sample, there was a significant positive correlation between the child’s cognitive ability

(standardized Peabody score) and the parent’s report of their child’s emotion regulation (r

= .36, p = .025). A significant negative correlation was found between the child’s

cognitive ability and the parent’s perception of child vulnerability (r = -.35, p = .027) and

Page 47: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

40

parent protectiveness (r = -.48, p=.002). This pattern was not found for the hospital

sample.

________________________________________________________________________

Table 8

Means and Standard Deviations for Parent Measures: Hospital and School Samples

________________________________________________________________________

Hospital School

M SD M SD

Family Expressiveness Questionnaire

Positive 7.83 0.87 7.47 0.85

Negative 3.84 1.10 4.26 1.29

Emotion Regulation Checklist 40.43 5.21 41.13 5.29

Parent Emotion Checklist

Importance of Child Emotion Expression

Positive 3.57 0.69 3.60 0.59

Negative 3.77 0.40 3.75 0.59

Parent Emotion Expression

Positive 3.78 0.42 3.73 0.45

Negative 2.33 0.66 2.75 0.59

Child Health and Family Functioning Questionnaire

Child Vulnerability 2.08 0.55 1.35 0.19

Child Independence 2.77 0.52 2.79 0.44

Parent Protectiveness 2.66 0.79 2.04 0.57

Page 48: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

41

________________________________________________________________________

Table 9

Means and Standard Deviations for Child Tasks: Hospital and School Samples

________________________________________________________________________

Hospital School

M SD M SD

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test 106.62 14.42 117.05 11.18

Secret Sharing Task

Zoo Secret 4.05 1.18 3.58 1.03

Ribbon Secret 3.67 1.38 4.15 1.29

Identifying Intentionality

Knowledge Questions 20.11 3.05 20.73 2.03

Test Questions 8.77 5.42 9.58 4.48

Silly Sounds Task

Congruent Trials 4.53 0.86 4.63 0.54

Incongruent Trials 14.26 4.20 14.40 3.60

In both the hospital and school samples, almost all of the child measures were

significantly correlated with one another. (r = .32 to .59, p < .05) The exception to this

pattern was the zoo secret sharing task, which was not significantly correlated with any of

the other child measures in either the hospital or school sample. Greater emotion

regulation, as measured by the ribbon secret sharing task, is associated with greater

executive functioning (r = .37, p = .021) and greater ability to identify other’s intentions

for children with a history of pediatric cancer (r = .39, p = .015).

Page 49: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

42

For children with a history of pediatric cancer, higher cognitive ability was

associated with older age at diagnosis (r = .32, p < .047) while greater time since

diagnosis was associated with lower cognitive ability (r = -.33, p = .047). Child

independence was positively correlated with greater parent report of emotion regulation

(r = .39, p = .019), greater importance of emotion expression for both positive (r = .46, p

= .005) and negative emotions (r = .37, p = .026), and increased parental expression of

positive emotions (r = .35, p = .037) for children with a history of pediatric cancer.

Surprisingly, this pattern was not present for the school sample.

Not surprisingly, parents who report higher child vulnerability scores for their

children also report more parent protectiveness behaviors. This association holds true for

both healthy (r = .69, p < .001) and pediatric cancer (r = .65, p < .001) samples.

Although the correlations revealed various relationships between various variables,

computing t-tests, Hotellings T2, and regression models further tested the hypotheses and

clarified the results.

Differences for Children With and Without Pediatric Cancer on Executive Function,

Emotion Regulation, and Identifying Intentionality

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Task. It was hypothesized that there would be a

difference in cognitive ability between children with and without a history of pediatric

cancer. Children with a history of cancer were believed to have lower cognitive ability

than children without a history of cancer. This hypothesis was examined by conducting

an independent samples t-test between standardized Peabody Picture Vocabulary test

scores for each sample. Because parent’s level of education, the measure of family SES,

was significantly correlated with standardized Peabody scores, parent education was used

Page 50: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

43

as a covariate in this analysis. In keeping with the hypothesized result, there was a

significant difference between children with and without a history of cancer when

controlling for parent’s level of education, t(75) = 2.44, p = .017. Children with a history

of pediatric cancer performed significantly lower on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary task.

It is important to note that although children with a history of cancer have significantly

lower cognitive scores than children without a history of pediatric cancer, the means for

both samples of children were well within the normative range, 106.6 and 117.1

respectively. Additionally, the ranges for both samples (71-130 for cancer children and

83-135 for school children) show that there were children who had relatively low

performances and children who performed very highly in both samples. Because the

Peabody scores for the pediatric cancer children differed from the healthy children, the

standardized Peabody scores were considered as potential a covariate in subsequent

analyses examining child tasks. Again, standardized Peabody scores were used as a

covariate if it was significantly correlated with the dependent variable.

Secret Keeping Tasks. The second hypothesis predicted that children who have

experienced pediatric cancer would have significantly lower levels of emotion regulation.

Differences in emotion regulation between children with and without a history of

pediatric cancer were examined by conducting two independent sample t-tests between

the total scores for the ribbon and zoo secret sharing tasks. There was a significant effect

of cancer history on child performance for the zoo secret sharing task, t(73) = 2.00, p

= .050. Although there was a significant difference between children with and without a

history of pediatric cancer, it was in the opposite direction than hypothesized. Children

with a history of pediatric cancer had significantly higher emotion regulation. Unlike the

Page 51: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

44

zoo secret keeping results, there was not a significant effect of cancer history on child

performance for the ribbon secret sharing task, t(77) = -1.61, p = .112. These results did

not reach significance, but interestingly, the means for the ribbon secret sharing task are

in the opposite direction of the zoo secret sharing task. Although children with a history

of pediatric cancer performed better on the zoo secret sharing task, they performed worse

on the ribbon secret sharing task, albeit not significantly worse.

An analysis of variance was conducted to examine the possibility that the opposite

results for each task were the result of a significant interaction. A one-way between

subjects ANOVA was conducted to compare the effect of a history of pediatric cancer on

emotion regulation for performance on the zoo and ribbon secret sharing tasks. There is

no significant main effect for the zoo secret sharing task, F(1, 74) = 0.595, p = .443.

There is no significant main effect for the ribbon secret sharing task, F(1, 74) = .314, p

= .577. Additionally, there is not a significant interaction effect for the zoo and ribbon

secret sharing tasks, F(1, 74) = 0.003, p = .956. The opposite results for the zoo and

secret sharing tasks are not the result of a significant interaction between the two

measures of emotion regulation.

Silly Sounds Task. It was hypothesized that children with a history of pediatric

cancer would have significantly lower levels of executive functioning than children

without a history of cancer. Potential differences in executive functioning were

examined using the congruent and incongruent trials of the Silly Sounds task. First,

potential differences in task understanding were analyzed for children with and without a

history of pediatric cancer using the congruent trials of the Silly Sounds task. An

independent samples t-test revealed that when controlling for child’s age and

Page 52: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

45

standardized Peabody scores, there was no significant difference between children with

and without a history of pediatric cancer, t(73) = 0.667, p = .507. Both samples of

children preformed equally well on the congruent trials of the Silly Sounds task,

indicating no difference in their understanding of the directions or in their ability to make

the correct animal sounds. The means for both samples (Table 9) indicate that all

children performed very well on the congruent trials of the Silly Sounds task, on average,

missing less than 1 trial.

To examine differences in executive functioning, a subsequent independent

samples t-test was conducted comparing total scores for the incongruent trials of the Silly

Sounds task for children with and without a history of cancer while controlling for child’s

age and standardized Peabody scores. Contrary to hypothesized results, there was not a

significant effect of cancer history on child performance for this measure of executive

functioning, t(73) = 0.657, p = .513. Children with and without a history of pediatric

cancer have equivalent executive functioning abilities as it relates to suppressing a

prepotent response. Means from both samples (Table 9) indicate that children in both

samples performed well on this task, on average, missing fewer than 4 trials.

Identifying Intentionality Stories. Children with a history of cancer were

hypothesized to have a significantly higher understanding of other’s intentions than

children without a history of pediatric cancer. This was analyzed by examining total

knowledge and test question scores for the Identifying Intentionality task. First,

potential differences in basic story comprehension were assessed for children with and

without a history of pediatric cancer using performance on the knowledge questions. An

independent samples t-test revealed that when controlling for child’s age, parent

Page 53: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

46

education, and standardized Peabody scores, there was no significant difference between

children with and without a history of pediatric cancer on the total knowledge score, t(74)

= 0.078, p = .938. Both samples of children performed equally well on the knowledge

questions of the Identifying Intentionality task, indicating an equivalent understanding of

the plotlines. The mean knowledge scores for both samples was very high, indicating

that all of the children were able to comprehend and follow the action of the story, on

average, missing fewer than 3 knowledge questions throughout the entire task (Table 9).

To examine differences in the ability to identify other’s intentions, a subsequent

independent samples t-test was run using total test pair scores for children with and

without a history of pediatric cancer. There was a significant effect of cancer history on

the children’s understanding of other’s intentions when controlling for child’s age, t(74)

= 2.20, p = .031. Contrary to the hypothesized results, children with a history of cancer

had a significantly lower ability to identify other’s intentions. Although children without

a history of pediatric cancer performed significantly better than children with a history of

pediatric cancer, on average, children in both samples correctly identified the intentions

of others less than 50% of the time (Table 9).

Differences Between Family Expressiveness, Parent Protectiveness, Child Health

and Family Functioning, Report of Child Emotion Regulation, and Importance of

Emotional Expression

Child Health and Family Functioning Questionnaire. Parents of a child with

pediatric cancer were hypothesized to perceive significantly higher levels of child

vulnerability and parent protectiveness and less child independence than parents of a

child without a history of pediatric cancer. Differences in parents’ perception of child

Page 54: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

47

vulnerability, child protectiveness, and child independence for children with and without

a history of pediatric cancer were also assessed using independent samples t-tests. When

controlling for the child’s age and parent’s level of education, there was an effect of a

history of cancer on the parent’s perception of their child’s vulnerability, t(72) = 5.51, p

< .001. Parents of children with a history of pediatric cancer perceived their children to

be more medically vulnerable than parents of children without a history of pediatric

cancer. It should be noted that even though there is a significant effect of the child’s

cancer history on parent report of child vulnerability, the means for samples are toward

the lower end of the scale. This indicates that, on average, parents feel their child is

“rarely” to “sometimes” vulnerable (Table 8).

In line with hypothesized results, scores from the parents of children with and

without a history of pediatric cancer revealed that there was an effect of cancer history on

the parent’s protectiveness of their child, t(72) = 2.27, p = .026. This analysis was

conducted while controlling for the parent’s level of education. Parents of children with

a history of pediatric cancer were more protective of their child than parents of children

without a history of pediatric cancer. Means from the samples indicate that, on average,

parents of children with a history of pediatric cancer “often” exhibit protective behaviors

while parents of children without a history of pediatric cancer, on average, exhibit

protective behaviors “sometimes” (Table 8).

Contrary to hypothesized results, there was no difference between the child’s

level of independence for children with and without a history of pediatric cancer while

controlling for the child’s age, t(72) = 0.451, p = .654. Both groups of parents allowed

their child to make age appropriate decisions, gave their child equivalent control over

Page 55: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

48

small choices, and believed their child was competent enough to carry out basic functions

without parental help.

Finally, differences between children with and without a history of cancer for the

last three items on the child health and family functioning questionnaire were examined.

An independent samples t-test controlling for child’s age and parent’s level of education

revealed that parents of children with a history of cancer significantly more often state

their child has a chronic illness, t(71) = 2.99, p = .004. There was no difference in how

difficult it is for parents to set limits on behavior, t(73) = 0.94, p = .348. The average

rating for this indicates that parents only “rarely” to “sometimes” have difficulty setting

limits on behaviors for children with a history of pediatric cancer (M = 1.65, SD = 0.716)

and children without a history of pediatric cancer (M = 1.50, SD = 0.647). Additionally,

there was no significant difference for how well the child deals with separation from their

parent, t(72) = 1.71, p = .092, while controlling for parent’s level of education. However,

these results can be qualified as marginally significant. Children with a history of

pediatric cancer “rarely” deal with separation from their parent (M = 1.22, SD = 1.20)

while children without a history of pediatric cancer “sometimes” dealt with separation

from their parent (M = 1.92, SD = 1.04).

Emotion Regulation Checklist. Parents of children with a history of pediatric

cancer were hypothesized to report lower levels of emotion regulation in their children

than parents of children without a history of pediatric cancer. This was analyzed through

an independent samples t-test. Contrary to hypothesized results, there was not a

significant difference between parent reported levels of emotion regulation for children

with and without a history of pediatric cancer, t(71) = 0.578, p = .565. There is not a

Page 56: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

49

significant effect of cancer history on the level of parent reported emotion regulation.

Means from both samples indicate that, overall, parents report their children are often

able to regulate their emotions (Table 8).

Family Emotion Expression. It was hypothesized that families with a child with a

history of pediatric cancer would express significantly more emotions, both positive and

negative, than families with a healthy child. This was analyzed using a Hotelling’s T2

test for the expressions of positive and negative emotions, measured using the Family

Expressiveness Questionnaire. Contrary to hypothesized results, there was not a

significant overall effect of the child’s cancer history on the family’s emotion

expressiveness, Wilks’ Lambda, F = 2.51, p = .088, for positive, F(1, 76) = 3.38, p =.070,

or negative emotions, F(1, 76) = 2.28, p =.135. However, results for the overall effect of

the child’s cancer history on family emotion expressiveness and positive emotion

expression within the family are trending toward significance. This difference is not

large enough to reach significance. Families of children with and without a history of

pediatric cancer express equivalent levels of positive and negative emotions. Overall,

families in both samples reported frequently expressing positive emotions and must less

frequently expressing negative emotions (Table 8).

Parent Emotion Checklist. Parent measures also looked at emotion expression

separately for the children and parents. It was hypothesized that parents would encourage

more emotion expression, both positive and negative, for children with a history of

pediatric cancer. To examine this hypothesis, we conducted a Hotelling’s T2 to examine

differences between parental importance of their child’s emotion expression for positive

(happy) and negative (sad, angry, and scared) emotions. There was not a significant

Page 57: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

50

overall effect of the child’s cancer history on the importance parents place on their child’s

emotion expression, Wilks’ Lambda, F = .296, p = .745. Additionally, there was no

effect for positive, F(1, 76) = 0.023, p = .881 or negative emotion, F(1, 76) = 0.259, p

= .612 expression. Parents of children with and without a history of pediatric cancer

place equivalent importance on their child’s emotion expression. Parents in both

samples believed it was very important for their child to express whatever emotion they

were experiencing (Table 8).

It was also hypothesized that parents of children with pediatric cancer would be

less willing to express emotions in front of their children. This effect was hypothesized

to be stronger for negative emotions than positive emotions. To examine this hypothesis,

a Hotelling’s T2 was conducted. There was an overall effect of the child’s disease history

when controlling for parent level of education, Wilks’ Lambda, F = 4.99, p = .009.

Contrary to hypothesized results, there was no difference in the amount of positive

emotions parents expressed in front of their children, F(1, 76) = 0.430, p = .514. All

parents equivalently expressed positive emotions very frequently in front of their children.

However, this was not the case for negative emotions. In line with hypothesized results,

parents of children with a history of pediatric cancer were less likely to express negative

emotions in front of their children, F(1, 76) = 8.55, p = .005. Parents of a child with a

history of pediatric cancer were less likely to express sadness, anger, or fear in the

presence of their child. Taken together, there is evidence that there was a significant

effect of the child’s cancer history on the parent’s emotional expression in front of their

child. However, this only holds true for negative emotions.

Page 58: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

51

________________________________________________________________________

Table 10

Summary Table of Hypotheses, Measures, and Results

________________________________________________________________________

Hypotheses Measure Results

Children with pediatric cancer

will…

1. have lower cognitive

ability

2. lower levels of emotion

regulation

3. lower levels of executive

functioning

4. better understanding of

other’s intentions

1. Peabody Picture

Vocabulary Test

2. Zoo and Ribbon Secret

Sharing Tasks

3. Silly Sounds Task

4. Identifying Intentionality

Stories

1. Hospital < School

2. Zoo: Hosp > School

Ribbon: No sig. dif.

3. No sig. difference

4. Hospital < School

Families with a history of

pediatric cancer will…

1. express significantly more

emotions, both positive

and negative

2. Encourage more emotion

expression for children

3. less willing to show

emotion, particularly

negative emotion

4. lower levels of emotion

regulation in their children

5. perceive higher levels of

child vulnerability

6. display more parental

protectiveness

1. Family Expressiveness

Questionnaire

2. Parent Emotion Checklist

3. Parent Emotion Checklist

4. Emotion Regulation

Checklist

5. Child Health & Family

Functioning Questionnaire

6. Child Health & Family

Functioning Questionnaire

1. No sig. difference

2. No sig. difference

3. Pos: No sig differ.

Neg: Hosp < School

4. No sig. difference

5. Hospital > School

6. Hospital > School

Page 59: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

52

Performance on Child Tasks for Emotion Regulation Predicted from Family

Emotion Expressiveness, Parental Importance of Child Emotion Expression,

Parental Frequency of Emotion Expression, and Parental Protectiveness

A hierarchical regression analysis predicting both groups of children’s scores on

each of the emotion regulation tasks (ribbon and zoo) was calculated. Step one included

demographic variables (child’s age and parent’s education) and Peabody Picture

Vocabulary Test scores. Step two included parent measure predictors, including a

composite variable of family emotionality (comprised of family expressiveness, parental

importance of child emotion expression, and parental expression of emotions), and parent

protectiveness. To verify the creation of a composite family emotionality variable,

correlations between all considered variables were run and are reported in Table 11. Step

three included the child’s scores on the executive functioning task (Silly Sounds

Incongruent trials).

The following variables were considered for the composite family emotionality

variable: family expressiveness of positive emotions (FEx Positive); family

expressiveness of negative emotions (FEx Negative); how important it is for the child to

show positive emotions (PEC Child Emot. Positive); how important it is for the child to

show negative emotions (PEC Emot. Negative); how often parents express positive

emotions in front of their children (PEC Parent Emot. Positive); how often parents

express negative emotions in front of their children (PEC Parent Emot. Negative). This

was done to conserve the power of the hierarchical regression analyses.

Page 60: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

53

________________________________________________________________________

Table 11

Correlations Between Variables Considered for the Family Emotionality Variable

________________________________________________________________________

1 2 3 4 5 6

1. FEx Positive -- -.148 .312** .302** .359** .088

2. FEx Negative -- -.014 .011 -.274** .196

3. PEC Child Emot. Positive -- .610** .196 .220

4. PEC Child Emot. Negative -- .167 .114

5. PEC Parent Emot. Positive -- .160

6. PEC Parent Emot. Negative --

Because parental expression of negative emotions did not correlate with any of

the other variables, it was removed from the composite variable and run in a separate

regression. The sum of the FEx positive, the reverse score of FEx negative, PEC positive

child emotions, PEC negative child emotions, and PEC positive parent emotions

variables were used as the composite family emotionality variable. Four hierarchical

regressions were conducted: predicting zoo secret performance from family emotionality

composite variable, predicting ribbon secret performance from family emotionality

composite variable, predicting zoo secret performance from PEC parent negative

emotions, predicting ribbon secret performance from PEC parent negative emotions. All

other predictors and steps were identical using the method described above.

Page 61: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

54

The first hierarchical regression consisted of three steps: a) standardized Peabody

score, child’s age, and parent’s level of education, b) family emotionality composite,

parent protectiveness, c) silly sounds incongruent trials predicting the child’s

performance on the zoo secret sharing task. Table 12 provides a summary of the

regression. None of the models in this analysis were statistically significant, indicating

parent protectiveness, family emotionality, and executive functioning are not good

predictors of the child’s performance on the zoo secret keeping task, measuring the

child’s emotion regulation. This hierarchical regression accounted for 3% of the

variability in the child’s performance on the zoo secret keeping task.

________________________________________________________________________

Table 12

Hierarchical Regression for Variables Predicting Zoo Secret Sharing Task (N = 79)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

Variable B SE β B SE β B SE β

Age .019 .121 .020 .019 .123 .020 .093 .152 .098

Education .003 .084 .005 .025 .089 .038 .031 .089 .047

Peabody -.01 .011 -.058 -.003 .011 -.03 .002 .013 .028

Protective .130 .213 .085 .146 .215 .095

Fam Emo .048 .054 .109 .053 .054 .119

SS Inc -.04 .045 -.13

R2

.004

.099

.021

.576

.031

.696 F change

Page 62: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

55

The second hierarchical regression consisted of the same three steps: a)

standardized Peabody score, child’s age, and parent’s level of education, b) family

emotionality composite, parent protectiveness, c) silly sounds incongruent trials. This

regression was predicting the child’s performance on the ribbon secret keeping task.

Table 13 provides a summary of the regression. None of the models in this analysis were

statistically significant, indicating parent protectiveness, family emotionality, and

executive functioning are not good predictors of the child’s performance on the ribbon

secret keeping task, measuring the child’s emotion regulation. This hierarchical

regression accounted for 10.3% of the variability in the child’s performance on the ribbon

secret keeping task.

The third hierarchical regression replaced family emotionality with parent

negative emotion expression in a three step regression: a) standardized Peabody score,

child’s age, and parent’s level of education, b) parent negative emotion expression, parent

protectiveness, c) silly sounds incongruent trials. This regression was predicting the

child’s performance on the zoo secret keeping task. Table 14 provides a summary of the

regression. None of the models in this analysis were statistically significant, indicating

parent protectiveness, parent negative emotion expression, and executive functioning are

not good predictors of the child’s performance on the zoo secret keeping task, measuring

the child’s emotion regulation. This hierarchical regression accounted for 5.3% of the

variability in the child’s performance on the zoo secret keeping task.

Page 63: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

56

________________________________________________________________________

Table 13

Hierarchical Regression for Variables Predicting Ribbon Secret Sharing Task (N = 79)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

Variable B SE β B SE β B SE β

Age .239 .141 .208 .258 .143 .224 .098 .170 .085

Education .064 .100 .079 .037 .105 .046 .020 .104 .025

Peabody .012 .012 .122 .007 .013 .073 -.004 .014 -.038

Protective -.251 .249 -.138 -.306 .248 -.168

Fam Emo .022 .064 .040 .013 .063 .023

SS Inc .087 .052 .250

R2

.049

1.23

.065

.579

.103

2.90 F change

The final hierarchical regression consisted of the same three steps as the third regression:

a) standardized Peabody score, child’s age, and parent’s level of education, b) negative

parent emotion expression, parent protectiveness, c) silly sounds incongruent trials. This

regression was predicting the child’s performance on the ribbon secret keeping task.

Table 15 provides a summary of the regression. None of the models in this analysis were

statistically significant, indicating parent protectiveness, negative parent emotion

expression, and executive functioning are not good predictors of the child’s performance

on the ribbon secret keeping task, measuring the child’s emotion regulation. This

hierarchical regression accounted for 11.8% of the variability in the child’s performance

on the ribbon secret keeping task.

Page 64: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

57

_______________________________________________________________________

Table 14

Hierarchical Regression for Variables Predicting Zoo Secret Sharing Task (N = 79)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

Variable B SE β B SE β B SE β

Age .025 .121 .027 .004 .122 .004 .064 .150 .067

Education -.01 .085 -.010 -.019 .090 -.03 -.014 .091 -.021

Peabody .004 .011 .052 .002 .011 .029 .002 .013 .022

Protective .148 .212 .097 .162 .214 .105

Par Neg .331 .208 .195 .325 .209 .192

SS Inc -.031 .044 -.11

R2

.005

.108

.047

1.47

.053

.481 F change

________________________________________________________________________

Table 15

Hierarchical Regression for Variables Predicting Ribbon Secret Sharing Task (N = 79)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

Variable B SE β B SE β B SE β

Age .232 .142 .201 .239 .142 .208 .073 .168 .063

Education .076 .102 .093 .024 .108 .030 .007 .106 .009

Peabody .011 .012 .116 .006 .013 .065 -.005 .014 -.052

Protective -.257 .249 -.141 -.313 .247 -.172

Par Neg .242 .248 .116 .257 .245 .124

SS Inc .091 .051 .262

R2

.048

1.180

.076

1.040

.118

3.170 F change

Page 65: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

58

DISCUSSION

The current study aimed to examine potential developmental differences between

children with and without a history of pediatric cancer. In particular, this study examined

cognitive ability, executive functioning, emotion regulation, and the ability to identify the

intentions of others. This project was approached from a family systems perspective and

aimed to account for the interactions between parents and their children. Some

significant differences between children with and without a history of pediatric cancer

were found. However, not all study hypotheses were supported.

It was first hypothesized that children with a history of pediatric cancer would

have significantly lower cognitive ability than children without a history of pediatric

cancer. Results from this study confirm this hypothesis and are consistent with previous

literature. Pediatric cancer treatments, including intrathecal chemotherapy and radiation

therapy, are known to cause cognitive deficits (Campbell et al., 2009; Carey et al., 2008;

Lesnik et al., 1998; Radcliffe et al., 1996). In the current study, a large number of the

children with a history of pediatric cancer received radiation therapy and or treatment for

ALL, which typically includes intrathecal chemotherapy (Campbell et al., 2009).

Because of this, it would be surprising to find no overall cognitive differences between

the samples.

The significant, positive correlation between Peabody scores and older age at

diagnosis as well as the significant negative correlation between Peabody scores and time

since diagnosis provides further evidence of the developmental effects of cancer

treatment on cognitive ability. Children whose brains were more developed at the time of

diagnosis, indicated by their older age, may have experienced fewer cognitive deficits

Page 66: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

59

because the treatment side effects prevent brain development, without necessarily

destroying established neural networks. The significant negative correlation between

cognitive ability and time since diagnosis may indicate that cognitive deficits are not

immediate. Potentially, the cognitive deficits may worsen throughout the course of

treatment and beyond as a result of increased exposure to radiation or chemotherapy or as

a delayed effect, at least in this very young age group. Further longitudinal studies would

need to be conducted to substantiate these possibilities. Additionally, children without a

history of pediatric cancer may have fewer opportunities to attend school readiness

programs, as their time and resources are used combating their disease.

Even though this study found a significant difference in cognitive ability between

the two samples, practitioners, and parents can take solace in the finding that cognitive

scores for children with a history of pediatric cancer were well within established norms

for typical cognitive functioning. These are not the cognitive deficits seen with early

treatment protocols, even accounting for the possibility that the cognitive scores in this

study were slightly inflated. There is reason to believe that these children could grow up

to have intellectually stimulating lives and achieve their highest education and career

goals.

The second hypothesis predicted that children who experienced pediatric cancer

would have significantly lower levels of emotion regulation. This hypothesis was not

substantiated, as children with a history of pediatric cancer performed significantly better

on the zoo secret sharing task than did healthy children. Despite parent’s hesitation in

implementing strict behavior expectations, children with a history of pediatric cancer are

still learning to regulate their emotions (Young et al., 2002). It may be that these children

Page 67: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

60

are learning emotion regulation through treatment procedures. Even if the child is scared

or sad, they have to learn to cooperate with their doctors and nurses to complete treatment.

Because pediatric cancer is such an emotional experience, learning to lie still while their

port is accessed, no matter the number of tears that flow down their cheeks, may require

more emotion regulation than a healthy child who learns not to cry when they do not get

their way.

However, these results are complicated by the non-significant results of the ribbon

secret sharing task, where children with a history of pediatric cancer performed worse

than children without a history of pediatric cancer. Although these results were not

significant, it is important to try to understand why these two tasks, which were designed

to measure the same thing, produced group means for both samples in opposite directions.

There was an added element of restraint in the ribbon secret sharing task because

the children were holding an actual ribbon while speaking to the robot. Many of the

children in both samples revealed the secret by waving the ribbon in front of the robot,

which highlights that the children were restraining their mouths and their hands. It may

have been that the physical restraint of keeping the secret was more difficult for the

children with a history of pediatric cancer. There may have been times during treatment

that the children were too tired, did not feel well enough, or were still unable to verbally

communicate with caregivers. Under these circumstances, children may revert to

pointing or more non-verbal forms of communication. They may be more accustomed to

using these types of communication than children without a history of pediatric cancer.

In addition, children in the pediatric oncology clinic are frequently given presents, prizes,

toys, and beads. They are typically eager to show others what they have been given.

Page 68: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

61

Therefore, it would be more difficult for children with a history of pediatric cancer to

keep the ribbon secret than the zoo secret.

The children without a history of cancer may have more school experience than

children with a history of pediatric cancer. One of the main lessons in preschool and

early elementary school is learning what to do with your hands. Children are instructed

to have “quiet hands” that rest in the child’s lap and learn to keep their hands to

themselves. These lessons are less important in a family setting with less formal

instructions and with fewer children in the room. Therefore, if children with a history of

pediatric cancer did have less school experience than the children without a history of

pediatric cancer, they might not have learned these lessons as thoroughly. If these

lessons were learned by children in the school sample, it could help explain why children

without a history of pediatric cancer performed better on the ribbon secret sharing task

than the zoo secret sharing task.

It was hypothesized that children with a history of pediatric cancer would have

significantly lower levels of executive functioning than children without a history of

cancer. This hypothesis was not correct. There was no difference between children with

and without a history of pediatric cancer in terms of executive functioning as it relates to

the suppression of a prepotent response. This study provides preliminary evidence that

the child’s experience with pediatric cancer did not impact the development of their

executive functioning. This is extremely important as executive functioning was the

most basic process examined in this study and is the building block for more complicated

developmental processes including the ability to solve novel problems and decreased

internalizing and externalizing behavior (Garon et al., 2008; Riggs et al., 2003; Zelazo &

Page 69: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

62

Frye, 1998). Evidence from this study cannot address potential developmental

differences in the processes that build on the child’s executive function. For children

with a history of pediatric cancer, the executive functioning foundation for the more

advanced processes is intact and provides hope that these further abilities could develop

normally as well.

Children with a history of pediatric cancer were hypothesized to have a

significantly better understanding of other people’s intentions. This hypothesis was

incorrect. In fact, children with a history of pediatric cancer performed significantly

worse than children without a history of pediatric cancer on the identifying intentionality

task. Previous research has shown that children with a history of pediatric cancer

perceive that they have less control than children with diabetes, cystic fibrosis, or healthy

peers (Kellerman et al., 1980). It could be that this differing perception of locus of

control impacts their ability to identify the intentions of others. Children think very

referentially, basing much of their perception of others on their own thought processes

(Malle et al., 2001; Piotrowski et al., 2013). It could be that children who have a history

of pediatric cancer feel less in control of their own actions and less often feel as though

they act intentionally. This in turn would lead them to have greater difficulty identifying

the intentions of others.

Additionally, the clinic staff makes a point to explain the purpose of each

procedure to the child and family. This could limit the development of intentionality, as

the children never need to infer the intentions of others. They simply wait to be explicitly

told why people around them do what they do.

Page 70: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

63

Parents of a child with pediatric cancer were hypothesized to perceive

significantly higher levels of child vulnerability and parent protectiveness and less child

independence than parents of a child without a history of pediatric cancer. This

hypothesis was partially confirmed. Parents of children with a history of pediatric cancer

did perceive their children to be more medically vulnerable and displayed more

protective parenting behaviors than parents of children without a history of pediatric

cancer. This is in line with previous research. The degree to which parents of children

with a pediatric cancer diagnosis believe their children’s disease is life threatening

correlates with the behaviors the parents exhibit (Mereuta & Craciun, 2009). In fact,

parental perception of prognosis appears to be more predictive of parental behavior and

child outcomes than medical prognosis alone (Hauenstein, 1990; Mereuta & Craciun,

2009; Silver et al., 1998). Interestingly, the differences in parental behavior did not

carry over to child independence as there was no difference in the level of child

independence between the two samples. This could have been a function of the content

of the items. Most of the child independence items were related to basic self-care (Can

they use the toilet?) and rudimentary decision making (Can they choose their own

clothes?). These are appropriate independent behaviors for children in this age group.

The non-significant difference between samples might highlight that all children in this

age range, regardless of their cancer history, are not independent but very reliant on their

parents, as they should be. It may be that differences would emerge later in childhood

and it would be particularly interesting to look for potential differences in child

independence in the pre-teen age group.

Page 71: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

64

Additionally, parents of children with a history of pediatric cancer more often

stated that their children were currently experiencing a chronic illness. This seems very

intuitive. However, not all families of a child with a history of pediatric cancer listed

their child as chronically ill. As stated above, this is important because it provides

another measure of the parent’s perception of their child’s disease and can impact

parenting (Hauenstein, 1990; Mereuta & Craciun, 2009; Silver et al., 1998).

There were no significant differences in the two samples in terms of how difficult

it was to set limits on behavior or how well the child dealt with separation from their

parent. This was not in line with previous research. Establishing expectations for

behavior has been listed as a particularly difficult task for mothers of children with

pediatric cancer. Mothers interviewed in the Young et al. study (2002) reported feeling

like they were walking a thin line between being too harsh by applying normal rules of

conduct and being too lenient, which could lead to “spoiling.” However, this study did

not directly compare a sample of mothers of a child with pediatric cancer to mothers of a

healthy child, limiting direct comparisons to the current study.

Usually medical professionals advise parents to treat their sick child as normally

as possible, but parents often overprotect and over-indulge their ill child (Van Dongen-

Melman & Sanders-Woudstra, 1986). Because this study was conducted in the medical

clinic, parents may have been more likely to answer the questions in line with the doctors’

advice, not necessarily based on their own behavior.

The fact that many of the children in the current sample were off treatment may

also have influenced these results. Uncertainty for the future could be a driving force for

difficulty enforcing expectations. When speaking about the expectations for their child,

Page 72: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

65

mothers expressed concern about their child’s future, including significant physical,

psychological, or developmental impairments that could arise from their child’s cancer

experience (Young et al., 2002). Many of these fears have been dealt with by the end of

the child’s cancer treatment. Although many families are well aware of the possibility of

relapse, completing treatment alleviates initial uncertainty and would make it easier to

return to “normal” life, which could include more strict expectations and may account for

the non-significant results found in this study.

Parents of children with a history of pediatric cancer were hypothesized to report

lower levels of emotion regulation in their children than parents of children without a

history of pediatric cancer. This hypothesis was not supported by the results of this study.

There was not a significant overall effect of the parent’s report of their child’s emotion

regulation. These results contradict the results of the Zoo Secret Sharing task, which also

measures the child’s emotion regulation. Although there is no way to determine which

measure more correctly captures the child’s emotion regulation, this highlights a potential

methodological issue for future studies. More often, parents are asked to provide a report

of their child’s emotion regulation by completing a questionnaire. If parent reported

emotion regulation and emotion regulation tasks do not provide similar accounts, it must

be determined which method more accurately assesses child emotion regulation.

It was hypothesized that families with a child with a history of pediatric cancer

would express significantly more emotions, both positive and negative, than families with

a healthy child. This was not the case. There was also no difference in family

expressiveness for negative emotions. Although families of a child with a history of

pediatric cancer were trending toward expressing more positive emotions, the difference

Page 73: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

66

between the hospital and school samples was not significant. Increased positive emotion

expression in families of a child with a history of pediatric cancer, although not

significantly different from families with healthy children, is in keeping with an

expanding body of literature finding positive outcomes for children with a history of

pediatric cancer (Phipps et al., 2014). This includes findings of psychological growth,

better adjustment, and an increased endorsement of a positive outlook, fewer depressive

symptoms, and lower trait anxiety following the cancer experience (Phipps et al., 2014;

Phipps & Shivastava, 1997).

It was hypothesized that parents would encourage more emotion expression, both

positive and negative, for children with a history of pediatric cancer. Contrary to

hypothesized results, there was no difference between parental encouragement of

emotion expression for either positive or negative emotions between children with and

without a history of pediatric cancer. All parents greatly encouraged their children to

express whatever emotion they were experiencing. This may partially be a function of the

young age of all children in this study. The youngest children enrolled on study are just

starting to verbally express their emotions. It would be detrimental to the child for the

parents to discourage early emotion expression. Additionally, parents of children this age

are continuing to build trust with their child so that their child will feel comfortable

expressing themselves when more serious situations arise in the future. There may have

been differences in parental encouragement of child emotion expression if an older

sample had been used in this study. Parents of healthy children would be less worried

about developing their child’s ability to express themselves and probably a bit less

Page 74: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

67

interested in every emotion the child experiences. This could still be a major concern for

parents of a child with pediatric cancer.

Parents of a child with a history of pediatric cancer were expected to be less

willing to express their own emotions in front of their children. This effect was

hypothesized to be stronger for negative emotions. This hypothesis was partially

confirmed. While, there was no difference in the amount of positive emotions parents

expressed in front of their children, parents of children with a history of pediatric cancer

were less willing to express negative emotions in front of their children. Parents of

children with a history of cancer are most likely experiencing negative emotions as a

result of their child’s disease in addition to negative emotions surrounding more typical

parent stressors, such as interpersonal problems, employment concerns, or financial

issues. Although it is probably not beneficial for any child 4-7 years old to know the

intimate details of these topics, there might be a stronger desire on the part of the parents

of a child with pediatric cancer to keep a positive environment for their child in which

they can focus on fighting their disease. Parents of a healthy child might be more willing

to share struggles and stressors with their child because the other aspects of the child’s

life are secure.

Because there were a number of Hispanic children in the hospital sample and

none in the school sample, descriptive statistics for emotionality variables and cognitive

scores were examined. There was no reason to believe that the children’s ethnicity

greatly impacted scores on these variables. The presence of Hispanic children in the

hospital sample should not have impacted significant findings between the two groups.

Page 75: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

68

Finally, it was hypothesized that the child’s performance on the emotion

regulation tasks would be predicted by family emotion expressiveness, parents’

perception of child vulnerability, parental protectiveness, and scores on the child emotion

regulation task. This hypothesis was not supported by the results. None of the potential

predictors or overall conceptualized models significantly predicted the child’s

performance on either emotion regulation task. This model was designed using mainly

parent measures predicting child performance to better understand the interactions

between both parties.

Conceptually, these results are surprising. It has been well-documented that

family members psychologically influence one another (Hoekstra-Weebers, Wijnberg-

Williams, Jaspers, Kamps, & van der Wiel, 2012; Kazak, 1997; Kroneberger et al., 1996;

Pai et a., 2007; Phipps et al., 2006; Van Dongen-Melman & Sanders-Woudstra, 1986).

To understand the nature of psychological influences, it has been suggested that the

family should be treated as a single entity composed of individual but interrelated

components where more proximal components have greater influence on the child

(Kazak, 1997; Pai et al., 2007). There is no more proximal influence on the child than

parent psychological processes (Pai et al., 2007). Yet, in this study, the child’s

performance could not be predicted from parent psychological processes. These results

may not be consistent with previous literature because parents do their best to shelter

young children from the potential severity of any scary situation, which could include the

child’s disease. This is substantiated by results in this study that found parents of

children with pediatric cancer express fewer negative emotions in front of their child than

parents of healthy children. Many of the previous studies enrolled a much larger age

Page 76: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

69

range, often from elementary through high school or beyond. Parents may be sheltering

their children less as they grow up, causing the predictive power of the parent’s

psychological processing to increase over time, as information is shared more openly.

Additionally, the lack of an effect could result from the limited age range of participants

in this study. Because these processes are still developing in this age range, it might be

too early to see the full effects of the parental influence on the children’s behavior.

There were a number of limitations in this study. There was a relatively small

sample size and both samples contained some missing data. This is hard to avoid when

working with small children, as they get tired, may be in a bad mood, or decide they do

not want to complete a task. Increasing sample size would provide greater power and

more confidence in the results of the study. Another limitation is the conflicting reports

of child emotion regulation. It is hard to draw conclusions when two measures designed

to assess the same construct conflict with one another. Although the procedure for every

child was kept as consistent as possible, there was wide variability in the distractors,

settings, and interruptions. These distractions occurred in both samples and could have

differentially impacted the results.

Although a number of differences were found, there were fewer differences than

expected. Despite a potentially stressful and emotional pediatric cancer experience,

results from this study are in line with previous research, concluding that families of a

child with pediatric cancer are “normal families” dealing with extraordinary

circumstances (Kazak, 1997; Pai et al., 2007; Robinson et al., 2007).

Page 77: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

70

REFERENCES

Armenian, S. H., Landier, W., Hudson, M. M., Robison, L. L., & Bhatia, S. (2013).

Children's Oncology Group's 2013 blueprint for research: Survivorship and

outcomes. Pediatric Blood & Cancer, 60(6), 1063-1068.

Astington, J. W. (2001). The paradox of intention: Assessing children’s

metarepresentational understanding. In B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin

(Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 85-103).

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Baird, J. A., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Do preschoolers appreciate that identical actions may

be motivated by different intentions?. Journal of Cognition and Development,

2(4), 413-448.

Berndt, T. J., & Berndt, E. G. (1975). Children's use of motives and intentionality in

person perception and moral judgment. Child Development, 46(4), 904-912.

Bethel, C. L., Stevenson, M. R., & Scassellati, B. (2011, October). Secret-sharing:

Interactions between a child, robot, and adult. Systems, man, and cybernetics

(SMC), 2011 IEEE International Conference on (pp. 2489-2494). IEEE.

Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & O'Brien, M. (2008). Individual

differences in trajectories of emotion regulation processes: the effects of maternal

depressive symptomatology and children's physiological regulation.

Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 1110.

Brown, R., Kaslow, N., Madan-Swain, A., Doepke, K., Sexson, S., & Hill, L. (1993).

Parental psychopathology and children's adjustment to leukemia. Journal of

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(3), 554-561.

Campbell, L. K., Scaduto, M., Van Slyke, D., Niarhos, F., Whitlock, J. A., & Compas, B.

E. (2009). Executive function, coping, and behavior in survivors of childhood

acute lymphocytic leukemia. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34(3), 317–327.

Carey, M. E., Haut, M. W., Reminger, S. L., Hutter, J. J., Theilmann, R., & Kaemingk, K.

L. (2008). Reduced frontal white matter volume in long-term childhood leukemia

survivors: A voxel-based morphometry study. American Journal of

Neuroradiology, 29(4), 792–797. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A0904

Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in

preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595-616.

Carlson, S. M., & Wang, T. S. (2007). Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in

preschool children. Cognitive Development, 22(4), 489-510.

Page 78: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

71

Center for Disease Control and Prevention, (2009). United States cancer statistics.

Retrieved from website:

http://apps.nccd.cdc.gov/uscs/childhoodcancerbyprimarysite.aspx

Child Life Council: Mission, Values, and Vision. (2012). Retrieved from

http://www.childlife.org/The Child Life

Profession/ProfessionMissionVisionValuesPositionStatements.cfm

Clark, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. (2002). The relationship between executive function

abilities, adaptive behaviour, and academic achievement in children with

externalising behaviour problems. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry &

Allied Disciplines, 43(6), 785–796.

Coffey, J. S. (2006). Parenting a child with chronic illness: a metasynthesis. Pediatric

Nursing, 32(1), 51-60.

Conklin, H. M., Khan, R. B., Reddick, W. E., Helton, S., Brown, R., Howard, S. C., …

Mulhern, R. K. (2007). Acute neurocognitive response to methylphenidate among

survivors of childhood cancer: A randomized, double-blind, cross-over trial.

Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32(9), 1127–1139. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsm045

Costa, J. (2010). Neurocognitive effects of childhood cancer treatment. Chemo Fog (pp.

26-32). New York: Springer.

Cousens, P., Waters, B., Said, J., & Stevens, M. (1988). Cognitive effects of cranial

irradiaiton in leukemia: A survey and meta‐analysis. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 29(6), 839-852.

Dongen‐Melman, J. V., & Sanders‐Woudstra, J. A. R. (1986). Psychosocial aspects of

childhood cancer: A review of the literature. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 27(2), 145-180.

Dunn, D. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1959). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—third edition.

San Antonio, TX: Pearson.

Ehiwe, E., McGee, P., Filby, M., & Thompson, K. (2012). Black African migrants’

perceptions of cancer: Are they different from those of other ethnicities, cultures

and races?. Ethnicity and Inequalities in Health and Social Care, 5(1), 5-11.

Eisenberg, L. (1981). Foreword: Out of this nettle, danger, we pluck this flower, safely.

In G. Koocher & J. O'Malley (Eds.), The Damocles Syndrome (pp. xi-xv). New

York: McGraw Hill, Inc.

Five year cancer survival rates- 1962 vs. present. (n.d.). Retrieved from

https://www.stjude.org/stjude/v/index.jsp?vgnextoid=5b25e64c5b470110VgnVC

M1000001e0215acRCRD

Page 79: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

72

Fox, N. A. (1994). Dynamic cerebral processes underlying emotion regulation.

Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2‐3), 152-166.

Garfield, J. L., Peterson, C. C., & Perry, T. (2001). Social cognition, language acquisition

and the development of the theory of mind. Mind & Language, 16(5), 494-541.

Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A

review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31–60.

doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31

Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition

and action: Performance of children 312–7 years old on a stroop-like day-night

test. Cognition, 53(2), 129-153.

Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In

J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3 – 24). New York:

Guilford Press.

Halberstadt, A. G. (1986). Family socialization of emotional expression and nonverbal

communication styles and skills. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

51(4), 827.

Harkness, S., & Keefer, C. (2000). Contributions of cross-cultural psychology to research

and interventions in education and health. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology,

31(1), 92-109.

Hauenstein, E. (1990). The experience of distress in parents of chronically ill children:

Potential or likely outcome? Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19(4), 356-

364.

Hoekstra-Weebers , J., Wijnberg-Williams, B., Jaspers, J., Kamps, W., & van de Wiel, H.

(2012). Coping and its effect on psychological distress of parents of pediatric

cancer patients: A longitudinal prospective study. Psycho-Oncology, 21, 903-911.

Kazak, A. (1997). A contextual family/systems approach to pediatric psychology:

Introduction to the special issue. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 22(2), 141-148.

Kellerman, J., Zeltzer, L., Ellenberg, L., Dash, J., & Rigler, D. (1980). Psychological

effects of illness in adolescence. I. Anxiety, self-esteem, and perception of

control. The Journal of Pediatrics, 97(1), 126-131.

Kochanska, G. (2002). Committed compliance, moral self, and internalization: A

mediational model. Developmental Psychology, 38(3), 339.

Page 80: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

73

Kochanska, G., Tjebkes, T. L., & Forman, D. R. (1998). Children’s emerging regulation

of conduct: Restraint, compliance, and internalization from infancy to the second

year. Child Development, 69(5), 1378–1389. doi:10.2307/1132272

Kroneberger, W., Carter, B., Stewart, J., Morrow, C., Martin, K., Gowan, D., & Sender,

L. (1996). Psychological adjustment of children in the pretransplant phase of bone

marrow transplantation: Relationships with parent distress, parent stress, and child

coping. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 3(4), 319-335.

Lesnik, P. G., Ciesielski, K. T., Hart, B. L., Benzel, E. C., & Sanders, J. A. (1998).

Evidence for cerebellar-frontal subsystem changes in children treated with

intrathecal chemotherapy for leukemia: Enhanced data analysis using an effect

size model. Archives of Neurology, 55(12), 1561–1568.

doi:10.1001/archneur.55.12.1561

Malle, B. F., Moses, L. J., & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Introduction: The significance of

intentionality. In B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and

intentionality: Foundations of social cognition. (1-24). Cambridge, MA: MIT

Press.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2001). “Like me” as a building block for understanding

other minds: Bodily acts, attention, and intention. In B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, &

D. A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social

cognition (pp. 171-191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Mereuta, O., & Craciun, C. (2009). Parents' illness perceptions, maladaptive behaviors,

and their influence on the emotional distress of the child: A pilot study on a

Romanian pediatric cancer group. Cognition, Brain, Behavior: An

Interdisciplinary Journal, 13(2), 207-219.

Moses, L. J. (2001). Some thoughts on ascribing complex intentional concepts to young

children. In B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and

intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 69-83). Cambridge, MA: MIT

Press.

Müller, U., Liebermann-Finestone, D. P., Carpendale, J. I., Hammond, S. I., & Bibok, M.

B. (2012). Knowing minds, controlling actions: The developmental relations

between theory of mind and executive function from 2 to 4years of age. Journal

of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(2), 331-348.

Murphy, S., Xu, J., & Kochanek, K. Center of Disease Control, Division of Vital

Statistics. (2012). National vital statistics reports (Volume 60, Number 4).

Page 81: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

74

Nelson, J. A., Leerkes, E. M., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., & Marcovitch, S. (2012).

African American and European mothers’ beliefs about negative emotions and

emotions socialization practices. Parenting: Science and Practice, 12, 22-41.

doi:10.1080/15295192.2912.638871

Pai, A., Greenley, R., Lewandowski, A., Drotar, D., Youngstrom, E., & Peterson, C.

(2007). A meta-analytic review of the influence of pediatric cancer on parent and

family functioning. Journal of Family Psychology, 21(3), 407-415.

Patterson, J. M., Holm, K. E., & Gurney, J. G. (2004). The impact of childhood cancer on

the family: a qualitative analysis of strains, resources, and coping behaviors.

Psycho-Oncology, 13(6), 390–407.

Pellizzoni, S., Siegal, M., & Surian, L. (2009). Foreknowledge, caring, and the side-effect

effect in young children. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 289.

Phipps, S., & Srivastava, D. K. (1997). Repressive adaptation in children with cancer.

Health Psychology, 16(6), 521.

Phipps, S., Klosky, J. L., Long, A., Hudson, M. M., Huang, Q., Zhang, H., & Noll, R. B.

(2014). Posttraumatic stress and psychological growth in children with cancer:

Has the traumatic impact of cancer been overestimated? Journal of Clinical

Oncology, 32(7), 641-646.

Phipps, S., Larson, S., Long, A., & Rai, S. (2006). Adaptive style and symptoms of

posttraumatic stress in children with cancer and their parents. Journal of Pediatric

Psychology, 31(3), 298-309.

Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Harcourt, Brace &

World.

Piotrowski, J. T., Lapierre, M. A., & Linebarger, D. L. (2013). Investigating correlates of

self-regulation in early childhood with a representative sample of English-

speaking American families. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22(3), 423-436.

Radcliffe, J., Bennett, D., Kazak, A. E., Foley, B., & Phillips, P. C. (1996). Adjustment in

childhood brain tumor survival: Child, mother, and teacher report. Journal of

Pediatric Psychology, 21(4), 529-539.

Ramsden, S. R., & Hubbard, J. A. (2002). Family expressiveness and parental emotion

coaching: Their role in children's emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 30(6), 657-667.

Page 82: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

75

Riggs, N. R., Blair, C. B., & Greenberg, M. T. (2003). Concurrent and 2-year

longitudinal relations between executive function and the behavior of 1st and 2nd

grade children. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal

Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 9(4), 267-276.

Robinson, K., Gerhardt, C., Vannatta, K., & Noll, R. (2007). Parent and family factors

associated with child adjustment to pediatric cancer. Journal of Pediatric

Psychology, 32(4), 400-410.

Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of executive

attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental

Neuropsychology, 28(2), 573-594.

Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school-age children: the

development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental

Psychology, 33(6), 906.

Silver, E. J., Westbrook, L. E., & Stein, R. E. (1998). Relationship of parental

psychological distress to consequences of chronic health conditions in children.

Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 23(1), 5-15.

Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2002). Emotion regulation and understanding:

Implications for child psychopathology and therapy. Clinical Psychology Review,

22(2), 189-222.

Steliarova‐Foucher, E., Stiller, C., Lacour, B., & Kaatsch, P. (2005). International

classification of childhood cancer. Cancer, 103(7), 1457-1467.

Thomasgard, M., & Metz, W. P. (1999). Parent-child relationship disorders: what do the

child vulnerability scale and the parent protection scale measure? Clinical

Pediatrics, 38(6), 347-356.

Vernon-Feagans, L., & Cox, M. J. (Eds.). (2013). The Family Life Project: An

epidemiological and developmental study of young children living in poor rural

communities. New York, NY: Wiley.

Wellman, H. M., & Phillips, A. T. (2001). Developing intentional understandings. In B. F.

Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality:

Foundations of social cognition (pp. 125-148). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Werba, B. E., Hobbie, W., Kazak, A. E., Ittenbach, R. F., Reilly, A. F., & Meadows, A. T.

(2007). Classifying the intensity of pediatric cancer treatment protocols: The

intensity of treatment rating scale 2.0 (ITR‐2). Pediatric Blood & Cancer, 48(7),

673-677.

Page 83: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

76

Young, B., Dixon-Woods, M., Findlay, M., & Heney, D. (2002). Parenting in a crisis:

conceptualizing mothers of children with cancer. Social Science & Medicine,

55(10), 1835-1847.

Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1998). Cognitive complexity and control: II. The development

of executive function in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science,

7(4), 121–126.

Page 84: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

77

Appendix A

Demographic Information Collected

Please Answer the Following Questions:

Child’s Ethnicity: _____Black/African American _____ Hispanic _____ White/Caucasian

_____Other(please specify) ___________

What is the highest degree or level of school you or your spouse have completed?

____ Some High School, no diploma _____ High School Diploma or GED _____ Some

College Credit, no degree

____ Associates Degree ____ Bachelor’s Degree ____ Master’s Degree

____Doctorate Degree

What is your current marital status?

____ Married ____ Divorced ____ Single

Your date of birth: ___/___/______

Please list the gender and ages of your child’s brothers and sisters.

1. M F Age: ________ 3. M F Age: _________

2. M F Age: ________ 4. M F Age: _________

Page 85: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

78

Appendix B

Hospital Consent Form

Page 86: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

79

Page 87: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

80

Page 88: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

81

Page 89: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

82

Page 90: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

83

Appendix C

School Parent Letter and Consent Form

Page 91: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

84

Page 92: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

85

Appendix D

Family Expressiveness Questionnaire (Halberstadt, 1986)

Instructions

Please rate how often these responses occur in your family.

Please rate how often these

responses occur in your family.

Not at all

Frequently

Very

Frequently

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1 Showing forgiveness 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

2 Thanking family members 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

3 Praising someone for good work. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

4 Expressing anger at someone else's

carelessness.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

5 Pouting over unfair treatment by a

family member.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

6 Blaming one another for family

troubles.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

7 Crying after a disagreement. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

8 Showing dislike for someone. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

9 Expressing embarrassment over a

stupid mistake.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

10 Expressing excitement over future

plans.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

11 Expressing disappointment over

something that didn't work out.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

12 Telling someone how nice they look. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

13 Expressing sympathy for someone's

troubles.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

14 Expressing deep affection or love for

someone.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

15 Quarreling with a family member. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

16 Trying to cheer up someone who is

sad.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

17 Telling a family member how hurt

you are.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

18 Telling family members how happy

you are.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

19 Threatening someone. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

20 Saying "I'm sorry" when they were

wrong.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

Positive Emotions: 1, 2, 3, 10, 12, 13, 14, 16, 18, 20

Negative Emotions: 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 11, 15, 17, 19

High scores indicate more emotion expression.

Page 93: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

86

Appendix E

Emotion Regulation Checklist (Shields & Cicchetti, 1997)

Instructions

Please rate how often your child performs the following actions.

Rarely Sometimes Often Always

1 Is a cheerful child R S O A

2 Responds positively to neutral or friendly comments

from adults.

R S O A

3 Transitions well from one activity to another; does not

become anxious, angry, distressed, or overly excited.

R S O A

4 Can recover quickly from episodes of upset or

distress.

R S O A

5 Is easily frustrated. R S O A

6 Responds with positive emotions to neutral or friendly

comments by peers.

R S O A

7 Is able to delay gratification. R S O A

8 Can say when s/he is feeling sad, angry or mad,

fearful or afraid.

R S O A

9 Is overly excited when attempting to engage others in

play.

R S O A

10 Displays little emotion. R S O A

11 Responds with negative emotions to neutral or

friendly comments from peers.

R S O A

12 Is empathetic towards others. R S O A

13 Displays negative emotions when attempting to

engage others in play.

R S O A

Reverse Score: 5, 9, 10, 11, 13

High scores indicate better emotion regulation.

Page 94: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

87

Appendix F

Parent Emotion Checklist (Nelson et al., 2012)

Instructions

Please tell us what you think about when your child shows these emotions. Please mark

one of the choices.

Not

Important

Somewhat

Important

Important Very

Important

1 My child should show or tell me

when he or she is happy.

NI SI I VI

2 My child should show or tell me

when he or she is sad.

NI SI I VI

3 My child should show or tell me

when he or she is angry.

NI SI I VI

4 My child should show or tell me

when he or she is scared.

NI SI I VI

High scores indicate greater importance placed on child’s emotion expression.

Instructions

Please tell us how often you show these emotions in front of your child. Please mark one

of the choices.

Never Rarely Sometime

s

Always

1 How often do you show your child that

you are happy?

N R S A

2 How often do you show your child that

you are sad?

N R S A

3 How often do you show your child that

you are angry?

N R S A

4 How often do you show your child that

you are scared?

N R S A

High scores indicate parents express more emotion in the presence of their children.

Page 95: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

88

Appendix G

Child Health and Family Functioning Questionnaire (Thomasgard & Metz, 1999)

Instructions

Please rate your agreement with the statements in regard to your child.

1 My child currently has a chronic medical condition. Yes No

2 My child has been seriously ill. Rarely Sometimes Often Always

3 I have been afraid that my child might die from an

illness.

R S O A

4 It is difficult to set limits on behavior. R S O A

5 I leave my child with anyone outside of my family. R S O A

6 My child deals with separation from me. R S O A

7 When I am not with my child, I am worried that s/he

may get hurt or be involved in an accident.

R S O A

8 I worry that my child

A Is pale R S O A

B Has a sickly appearance R S O A

C Is blue around the mouth when crying R S O A

D Breathes too fast R S O A

E Gets sick very quickly R S O A

9 I keep my child from participation in an activity because

I am afraid s/he might get or become ill.

R S O A

10 I still sometimes worry that my child may die. R S O A

11 I allow my child to choose his/ her own clothing. R S O A

12 My child can choose the proper foods. R S O A

13 My child can get along without me. R S O A

14 My child can be trusted to do things alone. R S O A

15 My child can bathe without help. R S O A

16 My child is encouraged to express his/ her opinion, even

when I disagree.

R S O A

17 My child is allowed to refuse to eat certain foods. R S O A

18 I worry about my child’s safety when I am away

overnight.

R S O A

19 My child can use the toilet without assistance. R S O A

Child Vulnerability: 2, 3, R5, R6, 7, 8a-e, 9

High scores indicate greater parental feelings of child vulnerability.

Parent Protection: R5, 7, 17

High scores indicate greater protective behaviors exhibited by parents.

Child Independence: 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 18

Higher scores indicate more child independence.

Page 96: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

89

Appendix H

Intensity of Treatment Rating (Werba et al., 2007)

Instructions

Please answer the following questions about your child’s cancer as accurately as possible.

Child’s Diagnosis: ____________________ Date of Diagnosis: ___/___/_____

Stage: 1 2 3 4 Relapse: YES NO

Is your child currently on treatment: YES NO

Please check all treatment procedures that your child has experienced.

_____ Chemotherapy _____ Surgery

_____ Radiation Therapy _____ Transplant

ITR-2 Scoring Levels 1-4

Page 97: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

90

Appendix I

Silly Sounds Task (Gerstadt et al., 1994)

Dog Picture

Cat Picture

Page 98: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

91

Appendix J

Secret Sharing Task (Bethel et al., 2011)

Zoo Task

Experimenter: Do you know what a secret is? A secret is something that somebody tells you that you

are not supposed to tell anyone else. I have a secret to tell you. I am afraid of (tigers or

lions) and I do not want anyone to know that I am afraid of (tigers or lions). This will be

our secret. Now you are going to play with my robot friend while I finish my work.

Robot:

1. I want to go somewhere special with her. Where do you think we should go?

2. I want to go with her to the zoo. What kind of animals do you think she would

like to see?

3. What animals do you think she might be afraid of?

4. I really like (secret animal: tigers or lions). I want to show her the (secret animal)

cage. Do you think that would be a good idea?

5. Do you think she might be afraid of (secret animal).

Ribbon Task

Experimenter: Do you know what a secret is? A secret is something that somebody tells you that you

are not supposed to tell anyone else. I have another secret for you. Look what I have.

(Let the child pick a ribbon.) I have a prize for you to give to your (doctor or

nurse/teacher) and I do not want anyone to know that we have these ribbons. It is a

surprise! This will be our secret. Now you are going to play with the robot again while I

finish my work.

Robot:

1. I want to do something special. What do you think we should do?

2. I want to do something special for someone. Do you think we could give

someone 2a prize?

3. What kind of prize should we give?

4. I really like giving prizes. We could say thank you to your doctors and nurses/

teacher by giving them something special. Do you think that would be a good

idea?

5. Is there a prize for your doctor or nurse/teacher for being the world’s best

caretaker/teacher?

Higher scores indicate greater emotion regulation.

Page 99: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

92

Picture of Animated Robot

Page 100: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

93

Appendix K

Identifying Intentionality (Pellizzoni et al., 2009; Baird & Moses, 2001)

(Correct responses are shown in parentheses)

Instructions

We are going to hear some stories about friends. During the stories, I am going to ask

you some questions about the children’s thoughts and feelings.

Stories

A girl named Emma (Jacob) is at her house.

A girl named Sophie (Alex) is at her house.

Look what Sophie has with her—she has a (frog).

Sophie loves frogs, but Emma hates frogs.

Can you remember— does Sophie love frogs? (yes) Does Emma love frogs? (no)

Sophie wants to bring the frog to Emma’s house. If Sophie brings the frog over, Emma

will be upset. She will be angry. Why will Emma be angry? (because she hates frogs).

Now listen carefully. Sophie knows that Emma hates frogs and that the frog will make

Emma angry.

Does Sophie know that Emma will be angry to see the frog? (yes)

Sophie brings the frog to Emma’s house, and she gets angry. Now I have a question for

you:

Test Questions: Did Sophie make Emma angry on purpose… did she mean to? (yes)

Did Sophie make Emma angry by accident… she didn’t mean to? (no)

Does Emma think that Sophie made her angry on purpose… did she mean to? (yes)

Does Emma think that Sophie made her angry by accident… she didn’t mean to? (no)

A girl named Abby (Anthony) is at her house.

A girl named Bella (Michael) is at her house.

Look at what Bella has with her- she has a (stuffed dog).

Bella loves this stuffed dog, and Abby loves this stuffed dog.

Can you remember—Does Bella love the dog? (yes) Does Abby love the dog? (yes)

Bella wants to bring the dog to Abby’s house. If Bella brings the dog over, Abby will be

very happy. Why will Abby be happy? (because she loves the dog).

Now listen very carefully. Bella knows that Abby loves the dog and that she will be

really happy when she sees the dog.

Does Bella know that Abby will be very happy to see the dog? (yes)

Bella brings the dog over to Abby’s house, and she is very happy. Now I have a question

for you:

Test Question: Did Bella make Abby happy on purpose… did she mean to? (yes)

Did Bella make Abby happy by accident…she didn’t mean to? (no)

Does Abby think that Bella made her happy on purpose… did she mean to? (yes)

Does Abby think that Bella made her happy by accident… she didn’t mean to?

A girl named Elizabeth (Josh) is at her house.

A girl named Ava (Billy) is at her house.

Look what Ava has with her—she has a (jump rope).

Page 101: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

94

Ava loves to jump rope, and Elizabeth loves to jump rope.

Can you remember— does Ava love to jump rope? (yes)

Does Elizabeth love to jump rope? (yes)

Ava brings the jump rope to Elizabeth’s house. While they were playing, Elizabeth broke

the jump rope. This made Ava sad.

Why was Ava sad? (Because Elizabeth broke the jump rope). I have a question for you:

Test question. Did Elizabeth make Ava sad on purpose… did she mean to? (no)

Did Elizabeth make Ava sad by accident…she didn’t mean to? (yes)

Does Ava think that Elizabeth made her sad on purpose… did she mean to? (no)

Does Ava think that Elizabeth made her sad by accident… she didn’t mean to? (yes)

A girl named Olivia (Ethan) is at her house.

A girl named Chloe (Chris) is at her house.

Look what Chloe has with her—she has a (hamster).

Now Chloe loves hamsters, and Olivia is afraid of hamsters.

Can you remember— does Chloe love hamsters? (yes) Does Olivia love hamsters? (no)

Chloe wants to bring the hamster to Olivia’s house. If Chloe brings the hamster over,

Olivia will be very scared.

Why will Olivia be very scared? (because she is afraid of hamsters).

Now listen very carefully. Chloe does not know that Olivia is afraid of hamsters and that

she will be scared when she sees the hamster.

Does Chloe know that Olivia will be very scared to see the hamster? (no)

So Chloe brings the hamster over to Olivia’s house, and she gets very scared.

Now I have a question for you:

Test question. Did Chloe make Olivia scared on purpose… did she mean to? (no)

Did Chloe make Olivia scared by accident…she didn’t mean to? (yes)

Does Olivia think that Chloe made her scared on purpose… did she mean to? (no)

Does Olivia think that Chloe made her scared by accident… she didn’t mean to? (yes)

A girl named Maddie (Matt) is at her house.

A girl named Natalie (Danny) is at her house.

Look what Natalie has with her—she has (ice cream).

Natalie loves ice cream, and Maddie loves ice cream.

Can you remember— does Natalie love ice cream? (yes)

Does Maddie love ice cream? (yes)

Natalie wants to bring the ice cream to Maddie’s house. If Natalie brings the ice cream,

Maddie will be happy. Why will Maddie be happy? (because she loves ice cream).

Now listen carefully. Natalie doesn’t know that Maddie loves ice cream and that the ice

cream will make Maddie happy.

Does Natalie know that Maddie will be happy to eat ice cream? (no)

Natalie brings the ice cream to Maddie’s house, and she is happy. Now I have a question

for you:

Test question. Did Natalie make Maddie happy on purpose… did she mean to? (no)

Did Natalie make Maddie happy by accident…she didn’t mean to? (yes)

Does Maddie think that Natalie made her happy on purpose… did she mean to? (no)

Does Maddie think that Natalie made her happy by accident… she didn’t mean to? (yes)

Page 102: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

95

Pictures Accompanying Stories

Girls Frog Story

Page 103: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

96

Boys Frog Story

Page 104: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

97

Girl Dog Story

Page 105: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

98

Boys Dog Story

Page 106: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

99

Girls Jump Rope Story

Page 107: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

100

Boys Jump Rope Story

Page 108: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

101

Girls Hamster Story

Page 109: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

102

Boys Hamster Story

Page 110: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

103

Girls Ice Cream Story

Page 111: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

104

Boys Ice Cream Story

Page 112: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

105

Page 113: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

106

bbb

Page 114: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

107

Page 115: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

108

ALEXANDRA (LEXIE) DELONE [email protected]

Graduate Education:

Masters in Psychology at Wake Forest University

GPA: 3.52

Undergraduate Education:

Honors College, College of Charleston, Charleston, SC

Bachelor of Science in Psychology, December 2011

GPA: 3.88

Psychology GPA: 3.79

Study Abroad: Semester at Sea through the University of Virginia, Fall 2010

Research:

“Emotion Regulation Executive Functioning, and Understanding of Intentionality in Children with a

History of Pediatric Cancer” Graduate Thesis Research with Dr. Deborah Best and Dr. Thomas

McLean, 2013-2014

o Examined potential differences between children with pediatric cancer and healthy children in terms

of self-regulatory behaviors, with particular emphasis on emotional regulation.

o Orchestrated all aspects of design and implementation of the study including obtaining IRB

approval, preparing materials, recruiting participants, analyzing data, and writing a thesis

“Parental Distress, Child Post Traumatic Stress Symptoms, and the Impact of Childhood Cancer”

Research with Dr. Sean Phipps, 2013

o Examined to what extent the experience of childhood cancer impacts the relationship between

parental distress and child PTSS using both child and parent reports.

o Research project conducted at St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital in Memphis, TN as part of a

summer internship.

“Healthy Preschoolers’ Perceptions of Illness” Graduate Research with Dr. Deborah Best, 2013

o Investigated the interaction between preschoolers’ knowledge, empathy, and prosocial behavior

regarding peers with various medical conditions

o Orchestrated all aspects of design and implementation of the experiment including obtaining IRB

approval, preparing materials, recruiting participants, analyzing data and completing a poster

presentation at Wake Forest University

“Music Preference and Focus of Listening Behavior” Research with Dr. Susan Simonian, 2011

o Investigated whether participants’ music preference affects their listening behavior in terms of

different genres

o Orchestrated all aspects of design and implementation of the experiment including obtaining IRB

approval, developing questionnaires, recruiting participants, analyzing data and writing a final

manuscript for submission to a peer reviewed scientific journal

“Effects of Music on Children and Adolescents” Independent Study with Dr. Susan Simonian, 2011

o Wrote a literature review paper on the importance of music in the lives of children and adolescents

focusing on the social psychological effects of music as well as the effectiveness of music therapy

for certain populations

Page 116: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

109

“Peer Pressure in a Local Coffee Shop” Research with Dr. Lisa Thomson Ross, 2009

o Conducted an experiment on the influence of company on the size and healthiness of coffee orders

o Prepared a poster that was presented at the College of Charleston School of Math and Science

Research Day 2009

Work Experience:

Pediatric Oncology Education Internship at St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital, Summer 2013

o Selected to participate in a summer research experience at St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital in

Memphis, TN.

o Worked 40+ hour weeks on clinical research protocols in the hospital’s psychology clinic

o Attended daily seminars on various topics related to pediatric cancer and the ongoing research at St.

Jude

Teaching Assistant at Wake Forest University, Fall 2012, Spring 2013, Fall 2013, Spring 2014

o Conducted lab sessions for an undergraduate research methods and statistics class

Intern, Carolina Autism in Charleston, SC, Fall 2011

o Conducted ABA therapy sessions with children with autism spectrum disorder as a line therapist

o Learned about and constructed appropriate learning aids for children of different ability levels

o Completed 120 hours on site

Pre-Teacher, REACH Program at the College of Charleston, Spring 2012

o Taught and mentored special-needs college students enrolled in College of Charleston’s REACH

program

o Responsible for lesson plans, leading classroom sessions, holding office hours, and offering overall

support for the students

Intern, the Pennsylvania Attorney General’s Office, Summer 2011

o Processed Home Improvement Contractor Program applications for the Department of Consumer

Welfare

Program Assistant for Volunteer and Community Service Coordinator on Semester at Sea, Fall 2010

o Assisted and organized any efforts relating to volunteer and community service

o Oversaw two organizations, Students of Service and Vicarious Voyage by arranging reflections

groups, seminars, and independent service opportunities as well as organizing correspondence

between SAS students and 55 classrooms in the US

Development and Communications Intern, Heads Up!, Summer 2009

o Planned the Project Fair for a non-profit committed to the children of the DC public schools

o Completed around 240 hours on site

The Fund for American Studies: The Institute on Philanthropy and Voluntary Service, Summer 2009

o Balanced 6 credit hours at Georgetown University, a 30 hour a week internship, and orchestrated a

philanthropy project, in Washington, DC

Sound Engineer at Studio H, Hershey Park, Hershey, PA, 2007-2011

o Use karaoke tracks and professional equipment to mix amateur CD and DVD recordings

Voice Coach at the Music for Life Studio, 2006-2008

o Taught voice classes to K-7th graders and prepared them for live performances

Volunteer Experience:

Activity Coordinator: Memphis Grizzlies House at St. Jude, Summer 2013

o Responsible for the planning and coordination of weekly activity nights at one of St. Jude’s Housing

Facilities, serving families battling childhood cancer

o Organized age appropriate crafts and games for the patients, siblings, and parents

Page 117: EMOTION REGULATION, EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING, AND ... · master artist and giving up hours and hours of your Christmas break to draw cartoons! My wonderful colorers: Victoria, Charles,

110

President: College of Charleston Volunteer Corps, Spring 2010 and 2011 and Fall 2011

o Arrange volunteer opportunities & activities to better the Charleston area and bring unity to the

students in Volunteer Corps

Relay for Life, 2009-2012

o Raise money for the American Cancer Society and participate in a 12 hour relay-walk

Habitat for Humanity 2006-2012

o Built more than 10 houses for Habitat chapters in Charleston, SC, Central PA, Ghana, and South

Africa

Elementary Children’s Tutor, Fall 2010

o Tutored children between the ages of 4 and 9, 1-2 hours a day while on Semester at Sea

o Facilitated both one on one and group activities relating to music, math, reading, cultural activities,

journaling, and scrap-booking

Volunteer/Service Opportunities on Semester at Sea, Fall 2010

o Volunteered with international and local charities while studying abroad with Semester at Sea

o SOS Children’s Village, Opportunity International, local schools, and orphanages

o Located in Morocco, Ghana, South Africa, and India

Vicarious Voyage Coordinator, Fall 2010

o Corresponded with 4 fourth grade classrooms while on Semester at Sea sending weekly emails and

pictures

o Prepared pictures & documents to be sent from SAS students to 55 different k-12 classes in the US

Leadership Activities and Organizations:

Honors Ambassador, 2010- 2012

o Meet with potential honors students, take them to lunch, show them around campus while answering

any questions they might have about CofC and the Honors College

Psychology Club, 2009-2011

o Learn about psychology related opportunities on and off campus as well as career options

Chorus President, 2007-2008

o Responsible for attendance, money allocation, students’ choral needs, & acting as a liaison between

students & director

Girl Scouts of America, 1996-2008

o Involved 1-12 grades, moving from Brownies through Senior girl scouts, earning my bronze and

silver awards

Honors and Awards:

PSI CHI Psychology Honors Society, inducted Spring 2011

Scholar at Sea Scholarship from the National Society of Collegiate Scholars, August 2010

Golden Key Honor Society, inducted in 2009

National Society for Collegiate Scholars, inducted in 2010

Phi Eta Sigma Honors Society, inducted in 2009

College of Charleston’s Dean’s List for the Fall 2008 and Spring 2009 Academic Year

Faculty Honors every semester at the College of Charleston

Presentations

“Parental Distress, Child Post Traumatic Stress Symptoms, and the Impact of Childhood Cancer”

August 14, 2013 at St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital