effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

24
REVIEW Open Access Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions on academic and physical activity outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis Amanda Watson, Anna Timperio, Helen Brown, Keren Best and Kylie D. Hesketh * Abstract Background: Physical activity is associated with many physical and mental health benefits, however many children do not meet the national physical activity guidelines. While schools provide an ideal setting to promote childrens physical activity, adding physical activity to the school day can be difficult given time constraints often imposed by competing key learning areas. Classroom-based physical activity may provide an opportunity to increase school-based physical activity while concurrently improving academic-related outcomes. The primary aim of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to evaluate the impact of classroom-based physical activity interventions on academic-related outcomes. A secondary aim was to evaluate the impact of these lessons on physical activity levels over the study duration. Methods: A systematic search of electronic databases (PubMed, ERIC, SPORTDiscus, PsycINFO) was performed in January 2016 and updated in January 2017. Studies that investigated the association between classroom-based physical activity interventions and academic-related outcomes in primary (elementary) school-aged children were included. Meta-analyses were conducted in Review Manager, with effect sizes calculated separately for each outcome assessed. Results: Thirty-nine articles met the inclusion criteria for the review, and 16 provided sufficient data and appropriate design for inclusion in the meta-analyses. Studies investigated a range of academic-related outcomes including classroom behaviour (e.g. on-task behaviour), cognitive functions (e.g. executive function), and academic achievement (e.g. standardised test scores). Results of the meta-analyses showed classroom-based physical activity had a positive effect on improving on-task and reducing off-task classroom behaviour (standardised mean difference = 0.60 (95% CI: 0.20,1.00)), and led to improvements in academic achievement when a progress monitoring tool was used (standardised mean difference = 1.03 (95% CI: 0.22,1.84)). However, no effect was found for cognitive functions (standardised mean difference = 0.33 (95% CI: -0.11,0.77)) or physical activity (standardised mean difference = 0.40 (95% CI: -1.15,0.95)). Conclusions: Results suggest classroom-based physical activity may have a positive impact on academic-related outcomes. However, it is not possible to draw definitive conclusions due to the level of heterogeneity in intervention components and academic-related outcomes assessed. Future studies should consider the intervention period when selecting academic-related outcome measures, and use an objective measure of physical activity to determine intervention fidelity and effects on overall physical activity levels. Keywords: Classroom, Physical activity, Academic performance, Children, Schools, Intervention, Systematic review, Meta-analysis * Correspondence: [email protected] Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition (IPAN), School of Exercise and Nutrition Science, Deakin University, Geelong, Australia © The Author(s). 2017 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated. Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 DOI 10.1186/s12966-017-0569-9

Upload: others

Post on 10-Nov-2021

7 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

REVIEW Open Access

Effect of classroom-based physical activityinterventions on academic and physicalactivity outcomes: a systematic review andmeta-analysisAmanda Watson, Anna Timperio, Helen Brown, Keren Best and Kylie D. Hesketh*

Abstract

Background: Physical activity is associated with many physical and mental health benefits, however many childrendo not meet the national physical activity guidelines. While schools provide an ideal setting to promote children’sphysical activity, adding physical activity to the school day can be difficult given time constraints often imposedby competing key learning areas. Classroom-based physical activity may provide an opportunity to increaseschool-based physical activity while concurrently improving academic-related outcomes. The primary aim of thissystematic review and meta-analysis was to evaluate the impact of classroom-based physical activity interventionson academic-related outcomes. A secondary aim was to evaluate the impact of these lessons on physical activitylevels over the study duration.

Methods: A systematic search of electronic databases (PubMed, ERIC, SPORTDiscus, PsycINFO) was performed inJanuary 2016 and updated in January 2017. Studies that investigated the association between classroom-basedphysical activity interventions and academic-related outcomes in primary (elementary) school-aged children wereincluded. Meta-analyses were conducted in Review Manager, with effect sizes calculated separately for eachoutcome assessed.

Results: Thirty-nine articles met the inclusion criteria for the review, and 16 provided sufficient data and appropriatedesign for inclusion in the meta-analyses. Studies investigated a range of academic-related outcomes includingclassroom behaviour (e.g. on-task behaviour), cognitive functions (e.g. executive function), and academic achievement(e.g. standardised test scores). Results of the meta-analyses showed classroom-based physical activity had a positiveeffect on improving on-task and reducing off-task classroom behaviour (standardised mean difference = 0.60 (95% CI:0.20,1.00)), and led to improvements in academic achievement when a progress monitoring tool was used (standardisedmean difference = 1.03 (95% CI: 0.22,1.84)). However, no effect was found for cognitive functions (standardised meandifference = 0.33 (95% CI: -0.11,0.77)) or physical activity (standardised mean difference = 0.40 (95% CI: -1.15,0.95)).

Conclusions: Results suggest classroom-based physical activity may have a positive impact on academic-relatedoutcomes. However, it is not possible to draw definitive conclusions due to the level of heterogeneity inintervention components and academic-related outcomes assessed. Future studies should consider theintervention period when selecting academic-related outcome measures, and use an objective measure ofphysical activity to determine intervention fidelity and effects on overall physical activity levels.

Keywords: Classroom, Physical activity, Academic performance, Children, Schools, Intervention, Systematicreview, Meta-analysis

* Correspondence: [email protected] for Physical Activity and Nutrition (IPAN), School of Exercise andNutrition Science, Deakin University, Geelong, Australia

© The Author(s). 2017 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, andreproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link tothe Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver(http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 DOI 10.1186/s12966-017-0569-9

Page 2: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

BackgroundMultiple physical and mental health benefits can beattained when children participate in the recommended60 min per day of moderate- to vigorous-intensity phys-ical activity [1, 2]. Despite these benefits, populationbased-studies have reported that over 50% of children inAustralia and internationally are not meeting recom-mendations [3–6]. Schools are considered ideal settingsfor the promotion of children’s physical activity. Thereare multiple opportunities for children to be physicallyactive over the course of the school week, includingduring break times, sport, Physical Education classand active travel to and from school. Studies haveshown interventions targeting these discrete periodsmay be effective in increasing children’s physical ac-tivity levels [7, 8], with the potential to contribute toup to 50% of the physical activity required to meetphysical activity guidelines [9]. However, with limitedtime available during these discrete periods, additionalopportunities may be required in order for childrento achieve the recommended levels of physical activ-ity. Classroom-based physical activity provides anotherway for children to be active at school. This involvesclassroom teachers incorporating physical activity intoclass time through either integrating physical activityinto lessons (physically active lessons), or addingshort bursts of physical activity, either with curricu-lum content (curriculum focused active breaks) orwithout (active breaks).There is increasing interest from researchers and

education professionals about the potential forclassroom-based physical activity to positively impactacademic-related outcomes, including classroom beha-viour, cognitive function and academic achievement.While some teachers express concern that classroom-based physical activity may have an adverse effect onon-task classroom behaviour [10], emerging evidencefrom systematic reviews and meta-analyses suggest thatoverall physical activity may have a small positive effecton on-task classroom behaviour [11–17]. There is lessevidence on classroom-based physical activity.Narrative reviews [18–20], one systematic review [21]

and two meta-analyses [22, 23] have explored the impactof classroom-based physical activity interventions onacademic-related outcomes. However, these were narrowin scope, included few studies, and combined findingsamong primary and secondary school students, whichmay be problematic due to the difference in educationsettings.A systematic review of 11 studies concluded that

physically active lessons may have a positive effect, orno effect on academic-related outcomes [21]. However,that study did not consider other forms of classroom-based physical activity (e.g. active breaks), combined

findings among primary and secondary school students,and did not include a meta-analysis [21].A meta-analysis of four intervention studies found that

classroom-based physical activity had a positive effect onacademic-related outcomes (M = 0.67; 95%CI:0.26,1.09)[23]. Similar results were reported in a meta-analysis of24 intervention studies investigating the associationbetween different types of physical activity (e.g., du-ring recess or lunch vs. active breaks vs. physically ac-tive lessons) and school engagement (behaviour athome and at school, and emotions, e.g. lesson enjoy-ment) [22]. In that meta-analysis, overall resultsshowed physical activity had a significant positive ef-fect on school engagement (d = 0.28;95%CI:0.12,0.46)[22]. When broken down into type of physical acti-vity, active breaks (n = 4 studies) appeared to be themost effective type of intervention for improvingschool engagement (d = 0.55; 95%CI:0.02,1.06), com-pared with recess or lunch time physical activity(n = 3 studies; d = 0.26; 95%CI:-0.19,0.73) and physic-ally active lessons (n = 5 studies; d = 0.22; 95%CI:-0.21,0.66) [22]. However, results from those meta-analyses are limited by the small number of includedstudies [22, 23], the narrow range of potentialacademic-related outcomes assessed, the combinationof findings among primary and secondary school stu-dents [22], and their recency [23].The current paper aims to expand on findings from

these reviews by conducting a systematic review andmeta-analyses of the evidence of effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions (active breaks,curriculum-focused active breaks and physically activelessons) on a broad range of academic-related out-comes (classroom behavior, cognitive function andacademic achievement), specifically among primaryschool-aged children. A secondary aim is to examinethe effect of these interventions on children’s physicalactivity levels.

MethodsDefinitionsWhile there are no set definitions for classroom-basedphysical activity, the following definitions are providedin order to maintain consistency and clarity throughoutthe remainder of this systematic review.Classroom-based physical activity: physical activity

carried out during regular class time, and can occur ei-ther inside or outside the classroom (e.g. hallway, play-ground), and is distinct from school recess/lunch breaktimes. Classroom-based physical activity can take threeforms:

� Active breaks: short bouts of physical activityperformed as a break from academic instruction [24].

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 2 of 24

Page 3: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

� Curriculum-focussed active breaks: short bouts ofphysical activity that include curriculum content[25, 26].

� Physically active lessons: the integration of physicalactivity into lessons in key learning areas other thanphysical education (e.g. mathematics) [27, 28].

Academic-related outcomes: overarching term to en-compass factors associated with academic performanceat school. These can be grouped into three maincategories:

� Classroom behaviour: Observed behaviours that maypromote or interfere with learning in the classroom,including on-task behaviour [29] (e.g. concentratingon tasks assigned by the teacher), and off-task be-haviour (e.g. not concentrating on tasks assigned bythe teacher).

� Cognitive function: Mental process (e.g. executivefunction) that may influence academicperformance [29].

� Academic achievement: A child’s performance onschool-related tasks; often reported via classroomgrades, national standardised tests or progress moni-toring tools [29], as well as self-reported perceivedacademic competence [30].

Registration and protocolThis study followed the Preferred Reporting Items forSystematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA)recommendations for systematic review reporting, andwas registered with the International ProspectiveRegister of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) (record#CRD42016027294).

Search strategyStudies were identified through a systematic search offour electronic databases (PubMed, ERIC, SPORTDiscusand PsycINFO), first conducted in January 2016, and up-dated in January 2017 by one author (AW). The searchstrategy consisted of four elements (see Table 1). Thesearch was limited to peer-reviewed articles published inEnglish in all available years. ‘Grey’ literature, includingthe reference lists from the websites of two organisations(“Active Academics” and “Active Living Research”) in-volved in children’s physical activity research were alsosearched.

Inclusion criteriaA predetermined set of inclusion criteria were used toselect papers for this systematic review. Each study hadto meet the following criteria:

1. Intervention study design;

2. Investigated associations between classroom-basedphysical activity and at least one academic-relatedoutcome. Interventions involving strategies inaddition to classroom-based physical activity wereexcluded (to enable the effects of classroom-basedphysical activity to be isolated);

3. Study population included primary school-aged chil-dren (5–12 years);

4. Presented original data;5. Did not focus specifically on special populations (e.g.

overweight children).

Study selectionThe search yielded 7729 citations from electronic data-base records, and 17 from ‘grey’ literature (Fig. 1). Afterremoving duplicates (n = 500), the titles and/or abstractsof 7246 unique publications were screened by one au-thor (AW). A total of 101 publications were identified aspotentially relevant according to the inclusion criteria.Full texts of 98 of these 101 articles were obtained andreviewed independently by two authors to determine eli-gibility (AW, KB). Two full texts were conference ab-stracts only, and one full-text was unable to be retrieveddespite extensive librarian-assisted enquiries and emailsdirectly to the contact author. Of the 98 full-text articles,a total of 59 were excluded as not meeting inclusion cri-teria. Disagreements between the two reviewers wereresolved through discussion with all authors. Referencelists of included articles were also examined, however noadditional studies were identified. Thirty-nine uniquecitations satisfied the eligibility criteria and wereincluded in this systematic review.

Data extractionPaper characteristics including country of study, studydesign, participant characteristics, intervention charac-teristics, academic-related outcome measures, physicalactivity measures, and results were extracted by one au-thor (AW). Interventions were then categorised as activebreak, curriculum focussed active break, or physicallyactive lesson intervention.

Methodological qualityTwo authors (AW, KB) independently assessed themethodological quality of the included studies using theEffective Public Health Practice Project (EPHPP) tool[31]. This six-component rating scale for interventionsassesses (1) selection bias; (2) study design; (3) con-founders; (4) blinding; (5) data collection methods; and(6) withdrawals and drop outs. Each component wasrated on a three-point scale as either strong, moderateor weak using the tool’s defined criteria. Based on theseratings, an overall methodological quality score wasgiven; either strong (no weak component ratings);

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 3 of 24

Page 4: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Table

1Articlesearch

term

sanddatabasessearched

Classroom

-based

Physicalactivity

Acade

mic-related

outcom

esStud

ypo

pulatio

nDatabasesearched

Classroom

[tiab]

ORbreak*[tiab]

OR

curricul*[tiab]

OR“activebreak”[tiab]

ORintegrat*[tiab]

ORlesson

*[tiab]

“Physicalactivity”[tiab]

OR“physically

active”[tiab]

ORexercis*[tiab]

OR

active[tiab]

ORactivity[tiab]

Educationalstatus[tiab]

ORed

ucationalm

easuremen

t[m

h:no

exp]

ORcogn

ition

[mh:no

exp]

ORAcade

mic[tiab]

OR“Grade

pointaverage”[tiab]

OR“Stand

ardisedtest

scores”[tiab]

OR“stand

ardizedtestscores”[tiab]

OR“test

scores”[tiab]

ORReading[tiab]

ORMath*[tiab]ORlearn

*[tiab]

ORgrade*[tiab]

ORliteracy[tiab]

ORnu

meracy[tiab]

ORacadem

ic[tiab]

ORattent*[tiab]

OR

Con

centratio

n[tiab]

ORbe

haviou

r[tiab]

ORbe

havior[tiab]

ORcogn

iti*[tiab]

OR“executivefunctio

n”[tiab]

OR“fluid

intellige

nce”[tiab]

ORachievem

ent[tiab]

ORlearning

[tiab]

Stud

ent[tiab]

ORStud

ent*[tiab]

ORchild[m

h]ORchild*[tiab]

ORclass*[tiab]

PubM

ed

classroo

mor

scho

olor

lesson

physicalactivity

orexercise

academ

icor

achievem

entor

cogn

itive

childrenor

child

orstud

entor

class

SPORTDiscus

ERIC

PsycINFO

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 4 of 24

Page 5: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

moderate (one weak component rating); or weak (morethan one weak component rating), following the tool’saccompanying instructions. Where disagreementsexisted, deliberation occurred until a consensus wasreached.

Meta-analysesMeta-analyses were conducted where there were at leastthree studies investigating the same broad outcome, i.e.classroom behaviour, cognitive function, or academicachievement. Due to heterogeneity across study designs,for inclusion studies were required to have a separatecomparison group (i.e. RCT or quasi experimental withcontrol group). Studies that used a within subject orcross over study design were therefore excluded frommeta-analysis.To avoid duplication of studies under a single out-

come, where studies reported intervention effects onmultiple measures for an outcome (this happenedonly for cognitive functions) [32, 33] a decision wasmade to include outcomes relating to executive func-tions, over memory. Executive functions, inhibition inparticular, have been shown to be consistently related

to academic achievement [34] and therefore wereconsidered salient to teachers. Thus, where inhibitionand memory were reported, only inhibition was in-cluded in the meta-analysis; where executive functionsand short term memory were reported, only executivefunctions were included in the meta-analysis.Typically higher scores were reflective of betteracademic-related outcomes. Where lower scoresreflected better academic-related outcomes thesescores were reversed.As academic achievement tools varied widely in qual-

ity, only studies using national standardised tests orprogress monitoring tools were included in the meta-analyses. Further, intervention effects on mathematicswere used when studies reported multiple subject assess-ments, as math was the most commonly reportedoutcome. Of the 39 studies included in this systematicreview, 16 were included in meta-analyses. Reasons forexclusion were: insufficient data for calculating effectsizes and authors did not respond to email requests foradditional data (n = 6), using a within subject or cross-over study design (n = 9), not including a separate com-parison group (n = 2), insufficient studies investigating

Fig. 1 PRISMA Flow Diagram showing flow of studies through the review process

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 5 of 24

Page 6: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

an outcome (n = 4), or only reporting results separatelyfor subgroups (e.g. BMI categories) (n = 2).

AnalysisMeta-analyses were conducted using Review Manager 5.3.The wide variation in interventions and academic-relatedoutcomes employed in the different studies warranted useof a random effects model. Effect sizes (standardised meandifference) were computed as the difference betweentreatment and control means.

ResultsOf the 39 studies identified, 19 examined the effect ofactive breaks [24, 26, 35–51], seven examinedcurriculum-focussed active breaks [25, 52–57], and thir-teen examined physically active lessons [27, 28, 32, 33,58–66] on academic-related outcomes. The majority ofstudies (n = 27) were published in or after 2014 [24, 26,32, 33, 36, 39–41, 43, 46–51, 57, 65, 66], and none be-fore 2006. Most (n = 18) were conducted in the USA[25, 36, 39, 40, 42, 44, 45, 51–55, 57–60, 64, 65], sevenin the Netherlands [32, 41, 49, 50, 61, 62, 66], four inAustralia [27, 28, 46, 47], three in Canada [24, 35, 43],two in Scotland [37, 38], and one each in South Africa[48], UK [63], Greece [56] Denmark [33], andSwitzerland [26]. Sample sizes ranged from 14 [60] toover 4500 participants [45], with sample sizes <300 inthe majority of studies (n = 28) [24–28, 33, 35, 39–41,43, 44, 46–51, 53, 55–57, 59–64]. Intervention periodsspanned from single lessons [49, 55, 59, 65] to 3 yearduration [58], with most lasting no longer than nineweeks (n = 23) [24–28, 33, 37–41, 43–46, 48, 50, 52,55–57, 59, 63]. Study information is presented in Table 2(active breaks), Table 3 (curriculum focused activebreaks) and Table 4 (physically active lessons).

Intervention contentThere was considerable variation across studies inintervention content. While most (12 out of 19) ac-tive break interventions featured basic aerobic move-ments that students could be performed in theirclassroom (e.g. jumping jacks), and required no set-upor equipment [24, 35–40, 42, 43, 45, 50, 51], otherswere performed outside the classroom (e.g. sportsfield) [26, 41, 46–48], and/or required additionalequipment (e.g. markers, skipping ropes, balls, exer-cise bands, dance videos, or specialised stacking cups)[41, 44, 46, 49]. One study utilised both cognitivelyengaging active breaks (i.e. physical activity combinedwith cognitive demand) and active breaks to exploreseparate and combined effects of physical activity andcognitive engagement on cognitive function [26]. Thetarget frequency, duration and physical activity inten-sity of the breaks varied, ranging from 4 min of

vigorous-intensity physical activity weekly [24, 43] to20 min of moderate intensity physical activity donetwice per day [49].There was more consistency in content across

curriculum-focussed active breaks, compared with theactive breaks without curriculum content. Allcurriculum-focussed active breaks featured physical ac-tivity integrated into a combination of key learningareas, including mathematics, language, science and/orsocial studies, and aimed to reinforce previously taughtlesson content [25, 52–57]. Further, most (5 out of 7)required daily participation in 10 to 20 min of physicalactivity [19, 52–54, 57]. When specified, participationwas required at a moderate-[56] or moderate-tovigorous-physical activity intensity [55], but intensitywas not specified in the majority (5 out of 7) of thesestudies [25, 52–54, 57].While curriculum-focussed active breaks aimed to

reinforce previously taught lesson content, physicallyactive lessons were used to teach new lesson content[27, 28, 32, 33, 58–62, 64–66]. These lessons predom-inately incorporated physical activity into mathematicsand/or language lessons, but some also incorporatedscience and/or social studies [27, 28, 32, 33, 58–62,64–66]. Lessons ranged in duration from 30 to60 min [27, 28, 32, 33, 60–64, 66] with most (8 outof 13) requiring participation three times per week[27, 28, 32, 33, 61, 62, 64, 66]. Other physically activelessons were described as single lessons as part ofpilot interventions [59, 63, 65], or stipulated physicalactivity time per week, rather than number of lessonsper week [58].

Intervention fidelityIntervention fidelity was reported in twelve studies.For the three active break interventions delivered byteachers, various measures of fidelity were used, how-ever, no study clearly reported compliance withimplementing active breaks daily or the number ofactive break sessions conducted. Active break inter-ventions delivered by research staff reported high fi-delity, showing most children achieved the requiredphysical activity intensity [39–41], or at least 50% ofeach intervention session was spent at the requiredintensity [46, 47].For physically active lesson interventions, teacher re-

ports showed they delivered lessons either as intended[27] or for at least 50% of the required minutes per week[58]. Similar to active break studies, when delivered byresearch staff, at least 60% of intervention lessons werespent at the required physical activity intensity [61, 62].No curriculum focussed active break study reportedfidelity.

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 6 of 24

Page 7: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Table

2Stud

iesinvestigatingtheeffect

ofactivebreaks

onph

ysicalactivity

andacadem

ic-related

outcom

esPape

r/coun

try

Stud

yde

sign

Samplesize

Age

Interven

tion

Duration

Delivery

Physical

activity

measure

Acade

mic

outcom

emeasure

Stud

yqu

ality

Results

Maet

al.,2014

[34]

Cana

da

With

insubject

Stud

ents:

n=44

Scho

ols:

n=2

Classes:

n=2

Years2

&4

FUNtervals=20s

VPAseparatedby

10srestrepe

ated

8tim

esDose:alternating

days

3weeks

Research

staff

Non

eOff-task

behaviou

r:direct

observation

Strong

Off-task

behaviou

rSign

ificantly

less

followingFU

Ntervals,

comparedwith

noactivity

cond

ition

-Year

4children:off-task

passive(ES=0.31);off-task

motor

(ES=0.48)

-Year

2children:off-task

passive(ES=0.74);off-task

verbal(ES=0.45)off-task

motor

(ES=1.076)

How

ieet

al.,2015

[31]

USA

With

insubject

n=96

Age

9to

12years

Years4

&5

BrainBITES(Better

IdeasThroug

hExercise)=5,10

and20

min

MVP

Aactivebreaks

Dose:1cond

ition

delivered

twice

perweek

4weeks

Research

staff

Interven

tion

fidelity:d

irect

observation

Executivefunctio

n:trailm

akingtest&

digitrecalltests

Mathe

matics:1-min

mathfluen

cytest

Mod

erate

Executivefunctio

n:no

differencebe

tweengrou

psMathe

matics:sign

ificant

improvem

entafter10-m

in(ES=0.24)and20-m

in(ES=0.27)activebreak,

comparedwith

sede

ntary

cond

ition

How

ieet

al.,2014

[30]

USA

On-task

behaviou

r:direct

observation

Mod

erate

On-task

behaviou

r:largest

improvem

entafter10

min

activebreak(d

=0.50)

Janssenet

al.,2014

[32]

Netherland

sWith

insubject

n=123

Age

10to

11years

Year

5

15min

active

breaks

ofvarying

PAintensities

(MPA

,VP

A,p

assive

break,

nobreak)

Dose:un

clear

4weeks

Research

staff

PAintensity

durin

gactive

breaks:

Accelerom

eter

Selectiveattention:

Testof

Everyday

Atten

tionfor

children

(TEA

-chtest)

Mod

erate

Selectiveattention:im

proved

mostafterMPA

cond

ition

(B=−0.59,95%

CI:−0.70,-0.49),

comparedwith

VPA(B

=−0.29,

95%

CI:−0.39,-0.19),p

assive

break(B

=0.27,95%

CI:−0.35,-

0.18)andno

breakcond

ition

s

Maet

al.,2015

[21]

Cana

daWith

insubject

n=88

Age

9to

11years

Years3to

5

FUNtervals=20s

VPAseparatedby

10srest,rep

eated

8tim

esDose:on

ce/w

eek

3weeks

Research

staff

Non

eSelectiveattention:

d2Testof

Atten

tion

Mod

erate

Selectiveattention:sign

ificant

improvem

entfollowing

FUNtervals,comparedwith

noactivity

cond

ition

Barnardet

al.,

2014

[39]

SouthAfrica

Quasi-

expe

rimen

tal

with

preand

posttesting

Stud

ents:

n=149

Scho

ols:

n=2

Classes:

n=6

Scho

olA

meanage:

7.33

years

Scho

olB

meanage:

7.47

years

2interven

tion

prog

rams:

*integ

rated-30

min

integrated

academ

icskillsandmotor

skill

prog

ram

*inten

sive

prog

ram

-30

min

physical

activity

prog

ram

Dose:3tim

es/w

eek

8weeks

Unclear

Non

eLiteracy:ESSIR

eading

andSpellingtests

Num

eracy:VA

SSI

MathSkillsTest

Mod

erate

Reading:

fortheintegrated

(26%

)andintensive(30%

)prog

ramstestscores

improved

butno

tsign

ificant.

Spelling:

fortheintegrated

(32%

)andintensive(47%

)prog

ramstestscores

improved

butno

tsign

ificant.

Num

eracy:fortheintegrated

(30%

)andintensive(21%

)prog

ramstestscores

improved

butno

tsign

ificant.

Hillet

al.,

2011

[29]

Cross

over

n=552

Age

8to

12years

10to

15min

MPA

activebreak.

2weeks

Not

repo

rted

Non

eAtten

tionand

executivefunctio

n:Mod

erate

Atten

tionandexecutive

functio

n:im

proved

onlyfor

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 7 of 24

Page 8: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Table

2Stud

iesinvestigatingtheeffect

ofactivebreaks

onph

ysicalactivity

andacadem

ic-related

outcom

es(Con

tinued)

Scotland

Years4to

7Dose:on

ce/day

foron

eweek,no

interven

tionin

the

second

week

pacedserialadd

ition

,size

orde

ring,

listening

\span,

digitspan

backwards

&visual

coding

thosereceivingthe

interven

tionin

week2(m

ean

difference=3.85,95%

CI=

0.26,7.44)

Schm

idt

etal.,2016

[26]

With

insubject

n=98

Year

510

min

activebreak

involvingrunn

ing

atdifferent

speeds

Dose:5different

days

over

3weeks

3weeks

Not

repo

rted

Non

eAtten

tion:d2

Test

ofAtten

tion

Mod

erate

Atten

tion:no

sign

ificant

improvem

ent

Aham

edet

al.,2007

[35]

Cana

da

Cluster

RCT

Stud

ents:

n=288

Scho

ols:

n=10

Age

9to

11years

Years4

and5

ActionScho

ols!

BC=15

min

MVP

Aactivebreak.

Dose:on

ce/day

16mon

ths

Teache

rHabitu

alPA

:Mod

ified

Physical

Activity

Questionn

aire

forChildren

(PAQ-C)

Mathe

matics,

Readingand

Lang

uage

:Canadian

Achievemen

tTest

Weak

Mathe

matics,Readingand

Lang

uage

(totalscore)

Alth

ough

controlschoo

lhad

sign

ificantlyhigh

erscores

atbaseline,no

sign

ificant

differencebe

tween

interven

tion(m

ean=1672

(9.6)andcontrolg

roup

s(m

ean=1688.6(16.6)

atfollow

up Physicalactivity:increaseby

47min/w

eekininterventio

nscho

ols(139

±62

vs92

±45,p

<0.001)

Carlson

etal.,

2015

[27]

USA

Quasi-

expe

rimen

tal

(nopre-testing)

Stud

ents:

n=1322

Teache

rs>:

n=397

Scho

ols:

n=24

Meanage:

8.8years

Years1to

6

10min

MVP

Aactive

break

Dose:Atleaston

ce/

day

8mon

ths

Teache

rScho

oldayPA

:Accelerom

eter

Classroom

behaviou

r:Teache

rrepo

rtWeak

Classroom

behaviou

r:Teache

rswho

repo

rted

implem

entin

gactivebreaks

repo

rted

fewer

stud

entswho

lacked

effortor

gave

upeasily

(β=−0.17,95%

CI:−.033,

−0.01),weremorelikelyto

agreethat

stud

entswork

improves

following

participationin

activebreaks

(OR=1.88;95%

CI:1.04,3.37),

andshow

edatren

dtowards

agreem

entthat

stud

ents

stay

ontask

moreafter

activebreaks

(OR=1.88;

95%

CI:0.98,3.61;p=0.056),

comparedwith

non-im

plem

enters

Physicalactivity:stude

nts

ofteache

rswho

repo

rted

ever

holdingactivebreak

had3.14

moreminutes

perdayof

MVP

Aandwere

75%

morelikelyto

have

met

the30

min

perdaygu

ideline

forMVP

Adu

ringscho

ol(OR:1.75;95%

CI:1.22,2.51)

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 8 of 24

Page 9: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Table

2Stud

iesinvestigatingtheeffect

ofactivebreaks

onph

ysicalactivity

andacadem

ic-related

outcom

es(Con

tinued)

Hillet

al.,

2010

[28]

Scotland

Cross

over

n=1224

Age

8to

12years

Years4to

7

10to

15min

MPA

activebreak.

Dose:on

ce/day

for

oneweek,no

interven

tion

inthesecond

week

2weeks

Not

repo

rted

Non

eAtten

tionand

executivefunctio

n:pacedserialadd

ition

,size

orde

ring,

listening

span,d

igitspan

backwards

&visual

coding

Weak

Atten

tionandexecutive

functio

n:im

proved

only

forthosereceivingthe

interven

tionon

week2

oftheinterven

tion(con

trol

grou

pmean=58.20(18.03)

vs.interventiongrou

pmean=60.19(19.38)

Katzet

al.,

2010

[33]

USA

RCT

n=1214

Years2to

4Activity

Burstsin

the

Classroom

=MVP

Aactivebreaks

totaling

30minspe

rday.

Dose:Leng

thand

numbe

rof

sessions/

daycouldvary

8mon

ths

Teache

rNon

eClassroom

behaviou

r:Workandsocialskills

compo

nent

ofInde

pend

ence

Scho

olDistrict(ISD)prog

ress

repo

rtcard

Mathe

maticsandEnglish:

Year

4:Missouri

Acade

mic

Perfo

rmance

Test

(MAP)

Years2–4:ISD

prog

ress

repo

rt

Weak

Classroom

behaviou

r:no

differencebe

tweengrou

psAcade

micachievem

ent:no

differencebe

tweengrou

psforMAPtestresults

(Year4

only),bu

tagreaterprop

ortio

nof

controlg

roup

stud

ents

(Years2to

4)show

edim

provem

entin

math

(28.6%

vs.20.8%

)and

reading(21.1%

vs.16.1%

)as

measuredviaISDrepo

rt,

comparedwith

interven

tion

grou

p

Lisahu

nter

etal.,2014

[38]

Australia

Quasi-

expe

rimen

tal

with

control

grou

p

Stud

ents:

n=107

Teache

rs:

n=6

Scho

ols:

n=1

Classes:

n=4

Age

approx.

10years

Year

5

ActiveKids,A

ctive

Minds

(AKA

M)=

additio

nal

30minsof

MPA

activebreak.

Dose:on

ce/day

2term

s/approx.

20weeks

Specially

employed

PEteache

r

Habitu

alandscho

oldayPA

:Ped

ometer

(Yam

axCW700)

Scho

oldayPA

ofat

leastMPA

:Accelerom

eter

(ActiGraph

)

Cog

nitivefunctio

n:Cog

nitiveAssessm

ent

System

Academicachievem

ent:

totalscorefor8

classroom

subjects

Classroom

behaviou

r:scho

olbe

haviou

rrecords

Weak

Nodifferencebe

tweengrou

psforanyof

theacadem

icou

tcom

esassessed

Physicalactivity:d

aily

step

sde

clined

from

pre-

(con

trol

=13,772;

interven

tion=12,447)to

post-

(con

trol

=12,046;

interven

tion=9702)forbo

thinterven

tionandcontrolg

roup

s

Whit-Glover

etal.,2011

[ 36]

USA

RCT

Stud

ents

n=4599

Scho

ols:n=8

Years3to

5InstantRecess

=10

min

MPA

active

break.

Dose:on

ce/day

8weeks

Teache

rPA

durin

gInstant

Recess

lesson

:Direct

observation

Classroom

behaviou

r:direct

observation

Weak

Classroom

behaviou

r:11%

increase

intim

espen

ton

-taskin

interven

tion,

comparedwith

controlg

roup

Physicalactivity:M

PAincreasedby

16%

andLPA

increasedby

51%

Wilson

etal.,

2015

[37]

Australia

With

insubject

Stud

ents:

n=58

boys

Scho

ols:

n=1

Classes:

n=4

Meanage:

11.2years

Years5&6

10min

MVP

Aactive

breakou

tsidethe

classroo

mDose:on

ce/day,3

times/w

eek

4weeks

Teache

rPA

intensity

durin

gactivebreaks:

accelerometer

Sustaine

dattention:

5-min

Psycho

motor

VigilanceTask

On-task

behaviou

r:direct

observation

Weak

Sustaine

dattention:no

differenceinterven

tiongrou

ppreactivebreak:mean=477

(285)v

s.po

stactivebreak:

mean=479(200)

Off-task

behaviou

r:no

difference:interven

tiongrou

ppreactivebreak:mean=13.6

(10.0)

vs.p

ostactivebreak:

mean=14.8%

(11.6)

Uhrich&

Swarm.,2007

[35]

Quasi-

expe

rimen

tal

Stud

ents:

n=41

Scho

ols

Age

10to

11years

Year

5

20min

ofsport

stacking

:using

both

hand

sto

stacka

6weeks

Research

staff

Non

eDecod

ingand

compreh

ension

skills:

Gates

MacGinitie

Weak

Decod

ingskills:no

difference

betw

eengrou

ps(F1,41

=0.03,

p>0.05)

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 9 of 24

Page 10: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Table

2Stud

iesinvestigatingtheeffect

ofactivebreaks

onph

ysicalactivity

andacadem

ic-related

outcom

es(Con

tinued)

USA

with

control

grou

pn=1

Classes

n=2

grou

pof

12specialized

cups

inpred

etermined

combinatio

nsDose:3tim

es/w

eek

ReadingTestFourth

Edition

(GMRT-4)

Decod

ingand

Com

preh

ension

skill

subtests

Com

preh

ension

skills:

Improvem

entin

interven

tion

grou

p,comparedwith

control

(F1,41

=4.54,p

<0.05)

Altenb

urget

al.,

2016

[49]

Netherland

s

RCT

Stud

ents

n=62

Scho

ols

n=5

*con

venien

cesample

Age

d10

to13

years

20min

MPA

active

breaks

comprising

vide

o-baseddance

activities

Dose:on

cepe

rday&

twicepe

rday

1day

Supe

rvised

byresearch

staff

PAintensity

durin

gactivebreaks:heart

rate

mon

itor

Selectiveattention:

SkySearch

subtest

oftheTestof

Everyday

Atten

tion

forchildren(TEA

-ch

test)

Weak

Selectiveattention:test

scores

better

after2

bouts(β

=−0.26

(95%

CI:-0.52,-

0.004),com

paredwith

one

bout

(β=0.06

(95%

CI:−0.23,0.36)

and

controlcon

ditio

n.Note:ane

gativebe

taindicatedabe

tter

attention

score

VandenBerg

etal.,2016

[50]

Netherland

s

With

insubject

Stud

ents:

n=195

Scho

ols:

n=3

Classes:n

=8

Age

10to

13years

Year

5&6

12min

MPA

active

breaks

=3cond

ition

s(aerob

ic,coo

rdinative

&streng

th-based

PA)

Dose:on

ceoff

3days

Children

followed

pre-

recorded

vide

oof

active

breaksessions,

supe

rvised

byresearch

staff

PAintensity

durin

gactivebreaks:heart

rate

mon

itor

Inform

ation

processing

speed:

Letter

Digit

SubstitutionTest

Selectiveattention:

d2Testof

Atten

tion

Weak

Inform

ationprocessing

speed:

nochange

[F(1174)

=0.71,

p=0.040

Selectiveattention:

nochange

[F(1172)

=0.91,

p=0.34

Meadet

al.,2016

[51]

USA

Quasi-

expe

rimen

tal

with

preand

posttesting

Stud

ents:

n=81

Scho

ols:

n=1

Classes:n

=3

Year

6Age

11to

12years

3cond

ition

s-

implem

enteddu

ring

80min

mathclass

(2×5-min

active

breaks,sittingon

stability

balls

&tradition

alseated

lesson

)Dose:everyday

Unclear

Teache

rNon

eReading,

Mathe

matics

andScience:Minne

sota

Com

preh

ensive

Assessm

ents

Reading,

Mathe

matics

andLang

uage

:Measuresof

Acade

micProg

ress

Weak

Reading,

Mathe

maticsand

Science:no

differencebe

tweenactive

break(pretest:527.3(29.8)

vs.p

osttest(620.9(34.2)

and

seated

lesson

cond

ition

s(pretest:543.9(13.1)

vs.

posttest643.1(12.4)

Reading,

Mathe

maticsand

Lang

uage

:nodifference

betw

eenactive

break(pretest:219.7(14.0)

vs.

posttest(226.8(15.1)

and

seated

lesson

cond

ition

s(pretest:221.2(16.0)

vs.

posttest226.0(15.1)

Abb

reviations

PA:p

hysicala

ctivity

LPA:light

intensity

physical

activ

ityMPA

:mod

erateph

ysical

activ

ityintensity

MVP

A:m

oderateto

vigo

rous

physical

activ

ityintensity

VPA:vigorou

sintensity

physical

activ

ityRC

T:rand

omised

controlledtrial

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 10 of 24

Page 11: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Table

3Stud

iesinvestigatingtheeffect

ofcurriculum

focussed

activebreaks

onacadem

icandph

ysicalactivity

outcom

es

Pape

r/coun

try

Stud

yde

sign

Sample

size

Age

Interven

tion

Duration

Delivery

Physicalactivity

measure

Acade

micou

tcom

emeasure

Stud

yqu

ality

Results

Vazou

etal.,

2012

[44]

Greece

With

in-sub

ject

n=147

Years4to

610

minsMPA

active

breakincorporating

lang

uage

arts,m

ath

andsocialstud

ies

Dose:un

clear

2weeks

Senior

prim

ary

educationstud

ent

teache

rs

Non

eAcade

micmotivation:

IntrinsicMotivation

Inventory

Mod

erate

Acade

micmotivation:

Perceivedacadem

iccompe

tenceincreasedin

interven

tioncomparedwith

control.(F=4.87,p

<0.05)

Goh

etal.,

2016

[57]

USA

With

in-sub

ject

Stud

ents:

n=210

Classes:

n=9

Scho

ols:

n=1

Age

d8to

12years

Year

3to

5

Take

10!=

10min

activebreaks

incorporating

lang

uage

arts,m

ath,

science,socialstud

ies

andge

neralh

ealth

Dose:de

term

ined

byteache

r

8weeks

Teache

rNon

eOn-task

behaviou

r:direct

observation

Mod

erate

On-task

behavior:

sign

ificant

increase

inpe

rcen

ton

-taskbe

havior

from

pre-TA

KE10!(82.3±4.5)

topo

stTA

KE10!

(89.5±2.7)

Erwin

etal.,

2013

[41]

USA

Quasi-

expe

rimen

tal

Stud

ents:

n=29

Scho

ols:

n=1

Classes:

n=2

Meanage:

8.87

years

Year

3

20+minutes

active

breakincorporating

mathandreading

Dose:daily

20weeks

Teache

rInterven

tion

effectsof

PAon

academ

icou

tcom

es:

Pedo

meter

(Walk4Life,

LS2500)

ReadingandMathe

matics:

curriculum

based

measures(CBM

),teache

rrepo

rted

grades

&standardised

testscores

(T-PRO

,STA

Rand

Discovery

Education

Assessm

ent)

Mod

erate

Mathe

matics:high

erscores

ininterven

tioncomparedwith

controlg

roup

attim

e3

(Mdiff=10.87,p=0.003),

butno

ttim

e1(M

diff=2.75,

p=0.39)or

2(M

diff=2.16,p

=0.49)

Reading:

high

erscores

ininterven

tion

grou

pat

time1(M

diff=

79.46,p<0.01),tim

e2

(Mdiff=87.41,p<0.01),

time3(M

diff=92.46,

p<0.01)

Standardised

testscores:

Tren

dtowards

improvem

ent

Bailey&

DiPerna

.,2015

[40]

USA

Multip

lebaseline

design

Stud

ents:

Meanof

16Year

1and

14Year

2stud

ents

/classroom

Scho

ols:

n=1

Classes:

n=6

Years1&2

Energisers=approx.

10to

20min

active

breakincorporating

core

curriculum

Dose:tw

icedaily

5,7or

9weeks

Teache

rScho

oldayPA

:New

-Lifestyles

Accelerom

eter

(NL-800):

Interven

tionacceptability:

stud

ent

andteache

rqu

estio

nnaire

Weak

Interven

tionacceptability:M

ost

teache

rsstrong

lyagreed

(11%

)or

agreed

(89%

)that

Energisers

didno

tadverselyaffect

learning

.Mostteache

rsstrong

lyagreed

(11%

)or

agreed

(67%

)that

stud

entswerebe

tter

ableto

payattentionfollowingEnergisers

76%

ofstud

entsrepo

rted

beingableto

paybe

tter

attentionin

classfollowing

Energisers

Physicalactivity:intervention

sign

ificantlyincreasedscho

olbasedstep

s(ES=0.71

to1.26)

Fedewa

etal.,

2015

[42]

USA

RCT

Stud

ents:>

n=460

Scho

ols:

n=4

Years3to

55min

activebreaks

incorporatingcore

curriculum

,totaling

20minsPA

perday

Dose:daily

1year

Teache

rTo

explainvariance

influid

intellige

nce

andacadem

icachievem

entscores:

pedo

meter

(Walk4Life)

Fluidintellige

nce:

Standard

Prog

ressive

Matrices

ReadingandMathe

matics:

Measuresof

Acade

mic

Prog

ress

Weak

Mathe

matics:im

provem

entin

interven

tion,

comparedwith

controlg

roup

:t(33)

=2.17,

p=0.04

Reading:

improvem

entin

interven

tion,

compared

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 11 of 24

Page 12: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Table

3Stud

iesinvestigatingtheeffect

ofcurriculum

focussed

activebreaks

onacadem

icandph

ysicalactivity

outcom

es(Con

tinued)

with

controlg

roup

t(32)

=1.69,p

=0.10

Fluidintellige

nce:no

differencebe

tweengrou

pst(3

6)=0.23,p

=0.82

Grieco

etal.,

2009

[43]

USA

With

insubject

Stud

ents:

n=97

Scho

ols:

n=1

Classes:

n=9

Age

8to

10years

TexasI-C

AN=on

e10–15min

MVPA

activebreakincorporating

math,

lang

uage

arts,

science,socialstud

ies

andhe

alth

Dose:on

eofflesson

1-day

Teache

rTo

compare

PAam

ongBM

Igroup

s:pe

dometer

(Omron

HJ105)

On-task

behaviou

r:direct

observation

Weak

On-task

behaviou

r:slight

increase

afterinterven

tionlesson

comparedwith

control,

althou

ghno

tsign

ificant

(F1,94

=2.19,p>0.10)

Mah

aret

al.,

2006

[22]

USA

Cluster

RCT

with

multip

lebaseline

design

Stud

ents:

n=243

Scho

ols:

n=1

Classes:

n=15

Kind

ergarten

toYear

4Energisers=approx.10

minsactivebreak

incorporatingcore

curriculum

Dose:daily

4or

8weeks

Teache

rScho

oldayPA

:Pedo

meter

(Yam

axSW

-200)

On-task

behaviou

r:direct

observation(assessedin

2Year

2,and2Year

4classes)

Weak

On-task

behaviou

r:Im

provem

entin

interven

tion,

comparedwith

controlg

roup

(ES=0.60).Greatestim

provem

ent

inon

-taskbe

havior

forstud

ents

mostoff-task(ES=2.20).

Physicalactivity:intervention

grou

ptook

morein

scho

olstep

s,comparedwith

control

grou

p(ES=0.49)

Abb

reviations:

MVP

A:m

oderateto

vigo

rous

physical

activ

ityintensity

MPA

:mod

erateph

ysical

activ

ityintensity

PA:p

hysicala

ctivity

RCT:rand

omised

controlledtrial

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 12 of 24

Page 13: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Table

4Stud

iesinvestigatingtheeffect

ofph

ysicallyactivelesson

son

academ

icandph

ysicalactivity

outcom

es

Pape

r/coun

try

Stud

yde

sign

Sample

size

Age

Interven

tion

Duration

Delivery

PAmeasure

Acade

micou

tcom

emeasure

Stud

yqu

ality

Results

DeGreeff

etal.,2016

[32]

Netherland

s

RCT

Stud

ents:

n=499

Scho

ols:

n=12

Years2&3

Meanage:

8.1±0.7years

Fit&Acade

mically

proficient

atscho

ol=30

min

physicallyactive

(MVPA)mathand

lang

uage

lesson

sDose:3xpe

rweek

22weeks

peryear

scho

ol,w

ith1-year

and

2-year

follow

up

1styear

-interven

tion

teache

rs2n

dyear

–teache

r

Non

eExecutivefunctio

n:Inhibitio

n:Golde

nStroop

test

Working

mem

ory:

Digitspan

backward

&Visualspan

backward

Strong

Inhibitio

n:no

differencebe

tween

interven

tionM

=19.6(8.1)and

controlg

roup

M=19.9(9.5)

Digitspan

backward:no

difference

betw

eeninterven

tionM

=6.0

(2..2)andcontrolg

roup

M=6.2(1.9)

Visualspan

backward:

nodifferencebe

tweeninterven

tion

M=6.6(1.7)andcontrolg

roup

M=6.8(1.6)

Rileyet

al.,

2014

[24]

Australia

Cluster

RCT-

pilotstud

yStud

ents:

n=54

Classes:

n=2

Scho

ols:

n=1

Age

10to

12years

Years5&6

EncouragingActivity

toStim

ulateYo

ung

(EASY)Minds

=PA

integrated

into

existin

gmath

lesson

s,60

mins

per

lesson

Dose:3xpe

rweek

6weeks

Research

staff

Activelesson

and

scho

oldayPA

:Accelerom

eter

(GT3X)

On-task

behaviou

r:direct

observation

Strong

On-task

behaviou

r:Greater

durin

ginterven

tionlesson

s,comparedwith

control(19.9%

meandifference)

Physicalactivity:9.7%

increase

inMVPAacross

mathtim

eslot,

and8.7%

increase

across

scho

olday

Rileyet

al.,

2015

[23]

Australia

Cluster

RCT

Stud

ents:

n=240

Scho

ols:

n=8

Age

10to

12years

Years5&6

EASY

Minds

=PA

integrated

into

existin

gmathprog

ram,60mins

perlesson

Dose:3xpe

rweek

6weeks

Teache

rActivelesson

and

scho

oldayPA

:Accelerom

eter

(Walk4Life,LS,

2500)

Ontask

behavior:

direct

observation

Mathe

matics:

Prog

ressive

Achievemen

tTest

Strong

On-task

behaviou

r:13.8%

increase

ininterven

tion

comparedwith

controlg

roup

Mathe

matics:no

difference

betw

eengrou

psPh

ysicalactivity:2.6%

increase

inMVPAdu

ringmath

timeslot,and1.7%

increase

across

scho

olday

Don

nelly

etal.,2009

[45]

USA

Cluster

RCT

(pre-and

post-test)

Stud

ents:

n=1527

Scho

ols:

n=24

Years2&3

PhysicalActivity

Across

theCurriculum

(PAAC)

=MVPAintegrated

into

lang

uage

,math,

science

andsocialstud

ieslesson

sDose:90

min

perweek,

delivered

interm

itten

tlythroug

hout

scho

olday.

App

rox.10

minspe

rsession.

3years

Teache

rScho

olday,weekend

dayandweekday

PA:

ActiGraph

accelerometer

Academicachievem

ent:

subsam

ple(n=454)

WIAT-II-Astandardised

test(m

ath,reading,

spelling)

Strong

Acade

micachievem

ent:

improvem

entin

reading,

math

andspellingscores

from

baseline

to3yearsin

interven

tion,

comparedwith

controlschoo

lsPh

ysicalactivity:g

reater

scho

oldayPA

(12%

),weekend

dayPA

(17%

)andweekday

PA(8%)in

interven

tioncompared,

with

controlg

roup

Beck

etal.,

2016

[33]

Denmark

Cluster

RCT

Stud

ents:

n=165

Scho

ols:

n=3

Classes:

n=9

Year

1Group

A=grossmotor

movem

entsintegrated

into

60min

mathlesson

s,(e.g.skipp

ing,

craw

ling,

throwingwhilesolving

mathprob

lems)

Group

B=finemotor

movem

entsintegrated

into

60min

mathlesson

s(e.g.m

anipulatingLEGO

6weeks

Teache

rPh

ysicalactivity

intensity

durin

glesson

s:Com

bine

dhe

art

rate

(Polar

Team

2System

)and

accelerometer

(Minim

axXS4)-

Subsam

ple(n

=49)

Mathe

matics:

standardized

test

(nam

eno

tspecified

)

Mod

erate

Mathe

matics:change

sin

mean

mathpe

rform

ance

weregreater

forthegrossmotor

grou

p,comparedwith

finemotor

grou

pfro

mbaselineto

interven

tionen

d(1.87±0.71).How

ever

thisaffect

was

noteviden

tfro

mbaseline

to8weekfollow

up.

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 13 of 24

Page 14: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Table

4Stud

iesinvestigatingtheeffect

ofph

ysicallyactivelesson

son

academ

icandph

ysicalactivity

outcom

es(Con

tinued)

bricks

whilesolving

mathprob

lems)

Dose:3xpe

rweek

McCrady

Spitzer

etal.,2015

[47]

USA

Quasi-

expe

rimen

tal

Stud

ents:

n=14

Scho

ols:

n=1

Classes:

N=1

Age

6to

7years

Year

1

30–40min

mathand

lang

uage

lesson

using

ActiveClassroom

Equipm

ent

Dose:daily

9mon

ths

Teache

rScho

oldayPA

:Accelerom

eter

Acade

micachievem

ent:

Dynam

icIndicatorsof

BasicEarly

Literacy

Skills(DIBELS)-oral

readingfluen

cy,w

hole

words

read,correct

letter

soun

d

Mod

erate

Correct

letter

soun

d:children

ininterven

tiongrou

pshow

edgreaterim

provem

ent

(Mdiff=45

±34)compared

with

childrenin

thecontrol

grou

p(M

diff=15

±22)

Who

lewords

read:childrenin

interven

tiongrou

pshow

edgreaterim

provem

ent

(Mdiff=20

±14)compared

with

childrenin

thecontrol

grou

p(M

diff=7±9)

Oralreading

fluen

cy:n

odifferencebe

tween

interven

tion(M

diff=27

±27)

andcontrolg

roup

s(M

diff=19

±16)

Physicalactivity:46%

increase

ondays

used

activeclassroo

meq

uipm

ent,comparedwith

days

intradition

alclassroo

m

Mullend

er-

Wijnsm

aet

al.,2015a

[49]

Netherland

s

With

insubject

Stud

ents:

n=86

Scho

ols:

n=4

Meanage:

8.2years

Years2&3

Fit&Acade

mically

proficient

atscho

ol=30

min

physically

active(M

VPA)math

andlang

uage

lesson

sDose:3xpe

rweek

22weeks

Interven

tion

teache

rsNon

eOn-task

behaviou

r:direct

observation

Mod

erate

On-task

behaviou

r:high

erpo

stinterven

tion,

compared

with

postcontrollessons

(ES=0.41)

Graha

met

al.,2014

[46]

USA

Non

-rand

omised

controlled

trial

Stud

ents:

n=21

Scho

ols:

n=1

Classes:

n=1

Age

7–8years

Year

2JumpIn!=

PAintegrated

into

math

lesson

Dose:on

e-offlesson

1day

Teache

rand

researcher

Non

eMathe

matics:

postsession

know

ledg

equ

estio

nnaire

Weak

Mathe

matics:no

difference

betw

eeninterven

tion

(M=4.08)andcontrol

grou

ps(M

=4.25)

Mullend

er-

Wijnsm

aet

al.,2015b

[48]

Netherland

s

Quasi-

expe

rimen

tal

with

control

grou

p

Stud

ents:

n=228

Scho

ols:

n=6

Meanage:

8.1years

Years2&3

Fit&Acade

mically

proficient

atscho

ol=30

min

physically

active(M

VPA)math

andlang

uage

lesson

sDose:3xpe

rweek

21weeks

Interven

tion

teache

rsNon

eMathe

matics:

speedtest

arith

metic

Reading:

1-min

test

Weak

Mathe

matics:

-Year

3:interven

tiongrou

phadhigh

erscores,com

pared

with

controlg

roup

(F[1,99]

=11.72,p<0.05).

-Year

2:interven

tiongrou

phadlower

scores

compared

with

controlg

roup

(F[1109]

=12.40,p<0.05)

Reading:

-Year

3:interven

tiongrou

phadhigh

erscores,com

pared

with

controlg

roup

(F[1,98]

=6.97,p

<0.05).

-Year

2no

differencebe

tween

grou

ps(F[1109]

=0.72,p

=0.40)

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 14 of 24

Page 15: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Table

4Stud

iesinvestigatingtheeffect

ofph

ysicallyactivelesson

son

academ

icandph

ysicalactivity

outcom

es(Con

tinued)

Norris

etal.,2015

[50]

UK

Quasi-

expe

rimen

tal

Stud

ents:

n=85

Scho

ols:

n=2

Classes:

n=4

Age

9to

10years

Year

5

Lond

onOlympic

them

evirtualfield

trip

=30

mins

completingprom

pted

activities

(e.g.run

ning

100m

sprin

ton

thespot)

Dose:on

eofflesson

May

and

June

but

interven

tion

ranfor1-day

ineach

class

Teache

rActivelesson

PA:

Accelerom

eter

Lesson

conten

trecall:

10item

conten

trecallqu

iz

Weak

Con

tent

recallqu

iz:no

differencebe

tweengrou

psPh

ysicalactivity:increasein

interven

tiongrou

p

Reed

etal.,

2010

[51]

USA

Cluster

RCT;

pre-andpo

st-

test

Stud

ents:

n=155

Scho

ols:

n=1

Classes:

n=6

Age

9to

11years

Year

3

30minsPA

integrated

into

lang

uage

andmath

andsocialstud

ieslesson

s.Dose:3xpe

rweek

3mon

ths

Teache

rDIGI-WALKER

pedo

meter

SW200-

used

ininterventiongroup

torecord

steps

duringlesson

only

Fluidintellige

nce:

Standard

Prog

ressive

Matrices

Acade

micachievem

ent:

Palm

etto

Achievemen

tChalleng

eTests(Eng

lish,

math,

scienceandsocial

stud

ies

Weak

Fluidintellige

nce:high

erscores

ininterven

tion,

comparedwith

controlgroup

(M=36.66,p=0.45)

Socialstud

ies:high

erscores

ininterven

tion,

comparedwith

controlg

roup

(t=p=0.004)

Mathe

matics:no

difference

betw

eengrou

ps(t=1.107,p=0.09)

English:

nodifferencebe

tween

grou

ps(t=0.71,p

=0.0478)

Science:no

differencebe

tween

grou

ps(t=1.490,p=0.140)

Grieco

etal.,2016

[65]

USA

Mixed

factorial

design

Stud

ents:

n=320

Scho

oldistricts:

n=1

Classes:

n=20

Age

7to

12years

SpellingRelay=10–

15min

PAintegrated

into

spellinglesson

sde

livered

atdifferent

PAintensities

(seatedtradition

allesson

,seated

game,LM

PAgame

&MVPAgame)

1xlesson

per

cond

ition

Research

staff

Physicalactivity

intensity

durin

glesson

s:accelerometer

On-task

behavior:

direct

observation

Weak

On-task

behaviou

r:sign

ificant

increase

intim

eon

task

from

pre-

topo

st-LM

PAgame

(ES=0.43)andMVPA

game(ES=1.22)

Mullend

er-

Wijnsm

aet

al.,2016

[66]

Netherland

s

RCT

Stud

ents:

n=499

Scho

ols:

n=12

Years2&3

Meanage:

8.1±0.7years

Fit&Acade

mically

proficient

atscho

ol=30

min

physically

active(M

VPA)mathand

lang

uage

lesson

sDose:3xpe

rweek

22weeks

peryear

scho

ol,w

ith1-year

and

2-year

follow

up

1styear

-interven

tion

teache

rs2n

dyear

–teache

r

Non

eReading:

1min

test

Spelling:

spelling

scores

retrievedfro

machild

academ

icmon

itorin

gsystem

Mathe

matics:speedtest

arith

meticandge

neral

mathscores

retrieved

from

achild

academ

icmon

itorin

gsystem

Weak

Mathe

matics:interven

tion

grou

pshow

edgreater

improvem

entin

math

speedtest(ES=0.51)and

gene

ralm

athscores

(ES=0.42),

comparedwith

controlg

roup

Spelling:

interven

tiongrou

pshow

edgreaterim

provem

ent

inspellingscores

(ES=0.45),

comparedwith

controlg

roup

.Reading:

nodifferencebe

tween

grou

ps(t=0.00;p

=1.00)

Abb

reviations:

MVP

A:m

oderateto

vigo

rous

physical

activ

ityintensity

MPA

:mod

erateph

ysical

activ

ityintensity

PA:p

hysicala

ctivity

RCT:rand

omised

controlledtrial

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 15 of 24

Page 16: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Methodological qualityOf the 39 identified studies, most (36 out of 39) receiveda moderate [24, 26, 33, 38–41, 48, 53, 55–57, 60, 62], orweak quality rating score [25, 27, 28, 35–37, 42, 44–47,49–52, 54, 59, 61, 63–66]. Three received a strong qual-ity rating score [32, 43, 58]. Low to moderate qualityscore ratings were mostly attributable to not reportingor controlling for relevant demographic confounders,not reporting blinding of participants and researchers,and not reporting participant attrition. Further, for manystudies, authors did not report the rate of participant orschool participation. See Appendix A for further detailon quality assessment of included studies.

Academic-related outcomes: Classroom behaviourStudies assessed the effect of participation in these pro-grams on academic-related outcomes both immediatelyfollowing participation in a session (acute) and after alonger exposure (chronic; e.g. pre- and post- interven-tion periods spanning up to 8 months). Regardless oftype of classroom-based physical activity, the majority ofstudies (10 out of 12) showed participation in these pro-grams had an acute effect on improving on-task class-room behaviour [25, 27, 28, 39, 52, 57, 62, 65] andreducing off-task behaviour [36, 43] However, evidencein the few studies with longer term follow-up (2 out of 2studies) suggest that this improvement may dissipateover time, with no difference between groups whenchronic intervention effects on reported behaviour inci-dents were assessed [42, 47]. Due to few studies investi-gating chronic effects of classroom-based physicalactivity on on-task and off task classroom behaviour(<5) it was not possible to separate acute and chronic ef-fects in the meta-analysis. Results from the 4 includedstudies show classroom-based physical activity had apositive effect on improving on-task behaviour and redu-cing off-task behaviour (standardised mean differ-ence = 0.60 (95% CI: 0.20,1.00)) (see Fig. 2).

Academic-related outcomes: Cognitive functionStudies also assessed acute and chronic effects ofclassroom-based physical activity on a range of cognitivefunctions [24, 32, 37, 38, 40, 41, 46, 47, 49, 50, 54, 64].

Results showed active breaks had an acute positive effecton selective attention (3 out of 4 studies) [24, 41, 49].No acute effect was reported for sustained attention[46], information processing [50] or focussed attention,processing speed and accuracy [26], and no chronic ef-fect was reported for planning, attention, simultaneousor successive cognitive processes [47] or executive func-tion [32]. Acute intervention effects on executive func-tion were inconsistent, with no difference betweengroups reported in one study [40], while another re-ported improvements in executive function but only forthose receiving the intervention in the second week ofdelivery [37, 38]. Results were also inconsistent forchronic intervention effects on fluid intelligence, withone study reporting a significant improvement after3 months [64], while another reported no difference be-tween groups after 1-year [54]. Due to few studiesreporting chronic effects of participation (<5) results foracute and chronic studies were combined in the meta-analysis (5 studies). Results from the meta-analysis indi-cate classroom-based physical activity had no effect oncognitive function (standardised mean difference = 0.33(95% CI: -0.11,0.77) (see Fig. 3).

Academic-related outcomes: Academic achievementStudies assessed intervention effects on academicachievement using a range of academic assessmenttools, including standardised tests, progress monitor-ing tools, grades and content recall quizzes. Reportedeffects on academic achievement varied by interven-tion duration and the type of assessment tool used.Interventions of shorter duration tended to show im-provement in academic achievement if a progressmonitoring tool was used, but not if a national stan-dardised test was used. Seven out of 8 studies using aprogress monitoring tool reported significant improve-ment in academic achievement following interventionperiods ranging from 4 weeks to 1-year [40, 44, 53,54, 60, 61, 66]. In contrast, most (4 out of 7) studiesindicated no difference between groups followingintervention periods less than 1-year when nationalstandardised tests were used as the outcome measure[27, 42, 64, 66]. However, standardised test scores

Fig. 2 Forrest plot of the effect of classroom-based physical activity on classroom behaviour

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 16 of 24

Page 17: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

significantly improved following a 1-year [51] and 3-year physically active lesson intervention [58]. Theseresults were confirmed in the meta-analysis. Whenprogress monitoring tools were used (4 studies) asthe outcome measure, academic-related outcomesgenerally showed improvement (standardised meandifference = 1.03 (95% CI: -0.22,1.84)). However, whenmeasured using a national standardised test (6 stud-ies), academic-related outcomes generally showed noimprovement (standardised mean difference = −1.13(95% CI: -0.72,0.46)) (see Fig. 4).In addition to standardised tests and progress moni-

toring tools, a small number of studies (not includedin the meta-analysis) measured academic achievementvia grades, content recall quizzes and self-reportedacademic competence. Results were inconsistent. Onestudy reported no difference between groups forgrades across eight subjects (total score) following a20-week active break program [47], Another reporteda greater proportion of students in the control group

showed improvement in grades for math and reading,compared with an active break intervention group[42]. Other studies assessed academic achievement viacontent recall quizzes and perceptions of academiccompetence, with no difference between groups inmath and social studies scores following participationin single lessons lasting between 10 and 30 min [59, 63].Another study reported self-reported perceptions ofacademic competence improved during physically activelessons [56].

Dose response relationshipFour studies aimed to explore the optimal dose ofactive break (i.e. amount of physical activity requiredto confer academic benefits) required to provide max-imum effects on academic-related outcomes, by ma-nipulating intensity [41], duration [39, 40], andfrequency [49] of active break sessions. Howie andcolleagues [39, 40] compared 5-, 10- and 20-min ac-tive breaks with a 10-min no break condition. Results

Fig. 3 Forrest plot of the effect of classroom-based physical activity on cognitive function

Fig. 4 Forrest plot of the effect of classroom-based physical activity on academic achievement

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 17 of 24

Page 18: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

showed on-task classroom behaviour significantly im-proved after the 10-min active break condition [39]and math scores were highest after the 10-min(ES = 0.24) and 20-min (ES = 0.27) active break con-ditions [40]. Janssen et al. [41] compared selective at-tention scores across 15 min of each of the followingfour conditions: no break (continued with schoolwork), passive break (teacher read story), moderate-intensity active break (jogging, passing, dribbling), andvigorous-intensity active break (running, jumping,skipping) [41]. Results showed that selective attentionscores improved most after the moderate-intensity ac-tive break [41]. Altenburg and colleagues [49] com-pared acute effects of different frequencies (one perday vs. twice per day) of 20 min moderate-intensityactive breaks. Results showed significantly better se-lective attention scores for children who received thetwice per day frequency [49].

Physical activity outcomesEleven studies examined the effect of classroom-basedphysical activity interventions on children’s physical ac-tivity levels using a range of measures, including ques-tionnaire [35], direct observation [45], pedometer [25,47, 52], and accelerometer [27, 28, 36, 47, 58, 60, 63].Across most (10 out of 11) classroom-based physicalactivity interventions, small increases in physical activitywere reported [25, 27, 28, 35, 36, 45, 52, 58, 60, 63].Across studies there was a 2% to 16% increase in moder-ate- to vigorous- intensity physical activity during inter-vention lessons, [27, 28, 45, 60, 63], and 2% to 12%increase in school day moderate- to vigorous- intensityphysical activity [27, 28, 58]. However, as shown in Fig. 5results from 3 studies included in meta-analysis indicateclassroom-based physical activity did not affect physicalactivity (standardised mean difference = 0.40 (95% CI:-0.15,0.95).

DiscussionA systematic search of the literature found 39 studiesassessing the effect of classroom-based physical activityon academic-related outcomes, including classroom be-haviour, cognitive function and academic achievement.In the majority of studies, academic-related outcomesimproved following participation in classroom-basedphysical activity programs. These findings are gener-ally consistent with earlier reviews finding that overallphysical activity level was either positively associated,or was not associated with academic-related outcomes[14, 15, 17]. In addition, the interventions included inthe current review generally resulted in more physicalactivity.The finding that classroom-based physical activity

improves on-task or reduces off-task classroom

behaviour immediately following participation inintervention sessions is consistent with previous re-views of school-based physical activity. For example,systematic reviews of the effect of physical activityduring the school break time on academic-relatedoutcomes showed positive associations between par-ticipation in physical activity before class (e.g. duringrecess/snack time) and on-task classroom behaviourin subsequent lessons [17, 29]. Therefore, breaking uplesson time with physical activity offers a promisingstrategy to improve on-task behaviour. Further, phys-ically active lessons may provide a strategy to engagestudents in lesson content, which may lead to im-proved on-task classroom behaviour. However, thisassumption is purely speculative and further researchis needed to confirm this. One study reported a non-significant increase in on-task classroom behaviourafter intervention sessions, compared with control[55]. A possible reason for this finding may be thatthe sample size in that study (n = 97) may not havebeen large enough to detect a significant improve-ment. Few studies (n = 3) reported that classroom-based physical activity had no effect on classroombehaviour. The majority of these studies (2 out of 3)reported that, while behaviour incidents and off-taskbehaviour increased in both the intervention and con-trol groups, the increase was greater in the controlgroup, compared with the intervention group [46, 47].These findings may encourage teachers to considerimplementing classroom-based physical activity pro-grams by alleviating concerns about reducing on-taskbehaviour due to the disruption to the classroom rou-tine [10].While classroom-based physical activity showed rela-

tively consistent positive associations with classroom be-haviour, effects on cognitive function were inconsistent.A possible explanation for this finding may relate tothe variability in the quality of measures used. Overallresults showed studies that reported improvements incognitive function used measures with moderate tohigh levels of reliability and validity [67, 68]. In con-trast, studies reporting no improvement in cognitivefunction mainly used measures with lower levels ofreliability and validity [69–71]. It may be importantfor future studies to use tests of cognitive functionwith established validity and reliability.A further possible explanation for inconsistent ef-

fects on cognitive function may relate to the level ofcognitive engagement inherent in each type ofclassroom-based physical activity. It has been sug-gested that cognitively engaging physical activity (i.e.physical activity combined with cognitive demands)may enhance cognitive function to a greater degreethan non-cognitively engaging physical activity (e.g.

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 18 of 24

Page 19: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

repetitive exercise) [72]. As curriculum-focused activebreaks and physically active lessons can be consideredcognitively engaging physical activity, it could behypothesised that these types of classroom-basedphysical activity would lead to greater improvementsin cognitive function, compared with active breaksthat involve no cognitive content. While the majorityof physically active lesson and curriculum focussedactive break interventions (2 out of 3 studies) andonly half of active break interventions (5 out of 10studies) led to improvements in cognitive function,there were too few cognitively engaging interventionsincluded in the review to draw a definitive conclu-sion. The one study that compared cognitively en-gaging and non-cognitively engaging active breaks,showed an impact on cognitive outcomes for the cog-nitively engaging breaks group only, lending supportto this hypothesis [26]. Although not explicitly stated,many studies which do not purport to involve cogni-tively engaging physical activity involve some activitieswhich are likely to confer cognitive engagement e.g.hopping sequences to music [37, 38], and coordina-tive exercises [50]. Some of these report positive andsome null findings, yet it is difficult to ascertain theproportion of physical activity children were exposedto that was cognitively engaging. Future studies areencouraged to separate the effects of cognitively en-gaging and non-cognitively engaging physical activityon cognitive functions.In addition to the cognitive test used, results may

be dependent on the type of cognitive functionassessed. For example, classroom-based physical activ-ity appeared to have a particularly beneficial effect onselective attention [24, 41, 49], compared with othercomponents of cognitive function, including sustainedattention [46], fluid intelligence [54, 64], informationprocessing speed [50], and executive function [32, 37,38, 40]. However, a recent systematic review con-cluded that there is insufficient evidence to concludewhat specific cognitive functions are most affected byphysical activity [73]. Exercise-induced arousal mayprovide a further explanation for inconsistency infindings. This theory suggests that the heightened

level of arousal during physical activity facilitates cog-nitive function and that this effect may be moderatedby physical activity intensity [74]. However, while themajority of included studies reported a target physicalactivity intensity, few measured physical activity inten-sity during interventions precluding conclusions re-garding the role of physical activity intensity oncognitive function. Thus, the favourable effect ofphysical activity on selective attention indicated inthis review requires further research for confirmation.Nonetheless, should improvements in selective atten-tion occur, such as the ability to ignore distractionsthis may be of particular interest to teachers and mayprovide motivation to incorporate physical activityinto their classroom routine.In addition to classroom behaviour and cognitive

function, classroom-based physical activity may alsohave a positive effect on academic achievement.However, effects on academic achievement may bedependent on intervention duration and the type ofassessment tool used to measure academic achieve-ment. In the current review it appeared that inter-ventions of shorter duration were more likely toshow an improvement in academic achievement if aprogress monitoring tool was used, rather than anational standardised test. This may be becausecurriculum-based measures are sensitive to smallchanges in academic achievement, and can be admin-istered frequently (e.g. weekly) [75, 76], while stan-dardised tests are usually designed to be administeredless frequently (e.g. yearly), and are not sensitive toshort-term progress. Therefore, progress monitoringtools may be a more suitable choice to determineintervention effects on academic achievement in theshort-term. This finding has important implicationsfor future research, indicating it may be important toconsider intervention duration when selecting themeasure of academic achievement. Therefore, futureintervention studies may consider using a progressmonitoring tool for intervention periods less than 1-year, and standardised tests for intervention periodslonger than 1-year if academic achievement is theoutcome of interest.

Fig. 5 Forrest plot of the effect of classroom-based physical activity on physical activity

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 19 of 24

Page 20: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Other studies investigated the impact of differentdoses of classroom-based physical activity on academic-related outcomes. However, results are based on few(n = 4) heterogeneous studies which considered a lim-ited range of potential physical activity doses. Thus, fur-ther research is needed to be able to draw conclusionsregarding the minimal dose of active break required toimpact academic-related outcomes.Several studies aimed to explore the effect of

classroom-based physical activity on children’s phys-ical activity levels [25, 27, 28, 35, 36, 45, 47, 52, 58,60, 63]. Results from the meta-analysis showedclassroom-based physical activity did not affect phys-ical activity levels. However, as only three of the 11identified studies could be included in the meta-analysis these results should be interpreted with cau-tion, and further research is warranted. Findings fromthe systematic review consistently revealed small in-creases in physical activity in children participating inthe intervention, compared with students in the com-parison group. These findings are in line with resultsfrom another review reporting positive associationsbetween classroom-based physical activity interven-tions and children’s physical activity levels [21]. Whilepromising, it is possible compensation for this activityoccurs outside of school. However, with limited infor-mation available, it is difficult to make strong conclu-sions on this. Further, it can be difficult to implementphysical activity interventions in schools, often due toa lack of time associated with competing curriculumdemands [77]. However, classroom-based physical ac-tivity is unique from other forms of school-basedphysical activity (e.g. Physical Education class andschool sport) in that it does not compete for instruc-tional time (physically active lessons and curriculum-focussed active breaks) or requires only minimal timecommitment (active breaks). Thus, classroom-basedphysical activity may be a potentially appealing optionfor schools as it offers a time-efficient strategy to pro-mote physical activity.

LimitationsThe considerable variation between studies in studydesigns, intervention content and outcome assessmenttools make it difficult to draw definitive conclusions,as evidenced by the small proportion of studies thatcould be included in meta-analyses. For studies thatassessed intervention effects on physical activity, themajority compared physical activity levels during theclassroom-based physical activity session, with a trad-itional seated lesson [27, 28, 45, 47], or assessedintervention effects on school day physical activitylevels only [25, 27, 28, 36, 52, 60]. Therefore, it is un-clear if the increase in physical activity during these

sessions is compensated for by a reduction in physicalactivity at other times of the day. However, as inter-vention effects on improving on-task, reducing off-task classroom behaviour and cognitive functionappear to be primarily acute, this may not be a prob-lem for these outcomes. In addition, few studies usedan objective measure of physical activity intensity [27,28, 35, 36, 47, 58, 60, 63]. Thus, future studies usingobjective measures of physical activity are required todetermine intervention effects on overall moderate-to- vigorous-intensity physical activity, and to deter-mine intervention fidelity (i.e. if the required physicalactivity intensity is met) within the sessions. Lastly,given that the majority of included studies reportedsignificant improvements in academic-related out-comes, it is possible publication bias may haveimpacted the lack of published null associations.

ConclusionClassroom-based physical activity interventions mayprovide a practical, low-cost, and effective strategy to in-crease academic-related outcomes, particularly acutepositive effects on improving on-task and reducing off-task classroom behaviour and selective attention.Classroom-based physical activity could also have thepotential to increase children’s physical activity levels,however further research is needed to confirm this.Findings from this systematic review should be inter-preted with caution given the high number of includedstudies of low methodological quality, suggesting there isroom for improvement in classroom-based physical ac-tivity intervention study designs and reporting. This re-view has identified a number of areas for furtherresearch in order to increase understanding of the effectof classroom-based physical activity on academic andphysical activity outcomes. These include the need forfuture studies to use objective measures of physical ac-tivity, and to consider intervention duration when select-ing a measure of academic achievement. In addition,future studies should explore the effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions on specific cognitiveoutcomes, as well as the impact of different types ofphysical activity (aerobic versus anaerobic versus resis-tance training and cognitively engaging vs. non-cognitivelyengaging physical activity) on academic-related outcomes.Further, it is not clear if improvements in academic-related outcomes are a result of the physical activity or aresult of the break from academic instruction, thereforefuture research is encouraged to add an attention controlgroup. Lastly, it is recommended future studies use a stan-dardized measure of cognitive function with establishedreliability and validity to be able to make comparisonsacross studies.

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 20 of 24

Page 21: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

Appendix

Table 5 Quality assessment of included studies

Paper Selection bias Study design Confounders Blinding Data collectionmethods

Withdrawals& dropouts

Overall

Goh et al., 2016 moderate moderate strong moderate moderate weak MODERATE

Beck et al., 2016 moderate strong strong moderate weak strong MODERATE

De Greeff et al., 2016 moderate strong strong moderate strong moderate STRONG

Altenburg et al., 2016 weak strong strong moderate strong weak WEAK

Mead et al., 2016 weak strong weak moderate weak weak WEAK

Mullender Wijnsma et al., 2016 moderate strong weak weak strong strong WEAK

Van den berg et al., 2016 weak strong strong moderate moderate weak WEAK

Grieco et al., 2016 moderate strong weak strong moderate weak WEAK

Carlson et al., 2015 moderate weak weak weak strong strong WEAK

Ma et al., 2015 moderate moderate strong moderate strong weak MODERATE

Ma et al., 2014 moderate moderate strong moderate moderate strong STRONG

Howie et al., 2014 moderate moderate strong moderate strong weak MODERATE

Howie et al., 2015 moderate moderate strong moderate strong weak MODERATE

Janssen et al., 2014 weak moderate strong moderate strong weak MODERATE

Wilson et al., 2015 moderate moderate strong moderate weak weak WEAK

Hill et al., 2011 moderate moderate strong strong weak strong MODERATE

Hill et al., 2010 moderate moderate strong strong weak weak WEAK

Ahamed et al., 2007 moderate strong strong moderate weak weak WEAK

Whitt-Glover et al., 2011 moderate strong weak moderate weak strong WEAK

Uhrich & Swarm., 2007 moderate strong weak moderate strong weak WEAK

Katz et al., 2010 moderate strong weak moderate weak weak WEAK

Lisahunter et al., 2014 weak strong weak moderate strong weak WEAK

Bernard et al., 2014 moderate strong weak moderate strong strong MODERATE

Fedewa et al., 2015 weak strong weak moderate strong strong WEAK

Erwin et al., 2013 moderate strong weak moderate strong strong MODERATE

Grieco et al., 2009 moderate moderate strong moderate moderate weak MODERATE

Mahar et al., 2006 moderate strong weak moderate moderate weak WEAK

Bailey & DiPerna., 2015 moderate moderate strong moderate weak weak WEAK

Vazou et al., 2012 moderate moderate strong moderate strong weak MODERATE

McCrady-Spitzer et al., 2015 weak moderate strong moderate strong strong MODERATE

Norris et al., 2015 moderate strong strong moderate weak weak WEAK

Mullender Wijnsma et al., 2015a moderate moderate strong moderate moderate weak MODERATE

Mullender Wijnsma et al., 2015b moderate strong weak moderate moderate weak WEAK

Graham et al., 2014 weak strong and moderate weak moderate weak weak WEAK

Riley et al., 2014 moderate strong weak weak weak strong WEAK

Riley et al., 2015 moderate strong strong weak weak strong WEAK

Donnelly et al., 2009 moderate strong strong moderate strong strong STRONG

Reed et al., 2010 weak strong weak moderate strong weak WEAK

Schmidt et al., 2016 moderate strong strong moderate strong weak MODERATE

Overall ratingStrong = no weak ratingsModerate = 1 weak ratingWeak = 2 or more weak ratings

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 21 of 24

Page 22: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

AcknowledgementsAW is a recipient of a Postgraduate Scholarship from Deakin University.AT is supported by a National Heart Foundation of Australia FutureLeader Fellowship (Award 100046). KDH is supported by an AustralianResearch Council Future Fellowship (FT130100637) & Honorary NationalHeart Foundation of Australia Future Leader Fellowship (100370).

FundingN/A.

Availability of data and materialsN/A.

Authors’ contributionsAW conducted the database searches, screened titles, abstracts and full textsfor eligibility criteria, performed methodological quality assessments anddata extraction, and drafted the initial manuscript. KB reviewed full textsfor eligibility criteria, and performed methodological quality assessments.KDH, HB, and AT contributed to interpretation of the data, and madecontributions to multiple revisions of the manuscript. All authors readand approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information (optional)AW is an experienced primary school teacher, and holds a Master of PublicHealth from the University of Adelaide.

Ethics approval and consent to participateN/A.

Consent for publicationN/A.

Competing interestsThe authors declare they do not have any competing interests.

Publisher’s NoteSpringer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims inpublished maps and institutional affiliations.

Received: 29 September 2016 Accepted: 14 August 2017

References1. Janssen I, LeBlanc AG. Systematic review of the health benefits of physical

activity and fitness in school-aged children and youth. Int J Behav Nutr PhysAct. 2010;7

2. Okely T, Salmon J, Vella S, Cliff D, Timperio A, Tremblay M, Trost S, Shilton T,Hinkley T, Ridgers N, Phillipson L, Hesketh K, Parrish A, Janssen X, Brown M,Emmel J, Marino N, A systematic review to update the Australian physicalactivity guidelines for children and young people., Report prepared for theAustralian Government Department of Health, Editor, editors. Canberra.Australia: Commonwealth of Australia; 2012.

3. Active Healthy Kids Canada. Are we driving our kids to unhealthy habits?The 2013 Active Healthy Kids Canada Report Card on Physical Activity forChildren and Youth. 2013 26 March 2015]; Available from: http://hepac.ca/active-healthy-kids-2013/.

4. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Australian Health Survey: Physical Activity,2011-12. 18 March 2015. 2013; Available from: http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/[email protected]/Latestproducts/462FBA87B642FCA4CA257BAC0015F3CE?opendocument

5. Griffiths LJ, Cortina-Borja M, Sera F, Pouliou T, Geraci M, Rich C, Cole TJ, LawC, Joshi H, Ness AR, Jebb SA, Dezateux C. How active are our children?Findings from the millennium cohort study. BMJ Open. 2013;3(8):e002893.

6. Troiano RP, Berrigan D, Dodd KW, Masse LC, Tilert T, McDowell M. Physicalactivity in the United States measured by accelerometer. Med Sci SportsExerc. 2008;40(1):181–8.

7. Sallis JF, McKenzie TL, Alcaraz JE, Kolody B, Faucette N, Hovell MF. Theeffects of a 2-year physical education program (SPARK) on physical activityand fitness in elementary school students. Sports, play and active recreationfor kids. Am J Public Health. 1997;87(8):1328–34.

8. Ridgers ND, Stratton G, Fairclough SJ, Twisk JW. Long-term effects of aplayground markings and physical structures on children's recess physicalactivity levels. Prev Med. 2007;44(5):393–7.

9. Fairclough SJ, Beighle A, Erwin H, Ridgers ND. School day segmentedphysical activity patterns of high and low active children. BMC PublicHealth. 2012;12:406.

10. McMullen J, Kulinna P, Cothran D. Physical activity opportunities during theschool day: classroom Teachers' perceptions of using activity breaks in theclassroom. J Teach Phys Educ. 2014;33(4):511–27.

11. Haapala E. Physical activity, academic performance and cognition inchildren and adolescents. A systematic review. Baltic Journal of Health &Physical Activity. 2012;4(1):53–61.

12. Sibley BA, Etnier JL. The relationship between physical activity andcognition in children: a meta-analysis. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2003;15:243–56.

13. Fedewa AL, Ahn S. The effects of physical activity and physical fitness onchildren's achievement and cognitive outcomes: a meta-analysis. Res QExerc Sport. 2011;82(3):521–35.

14. Lees, C. and J. Hopkins, Effect of aerobic exercise on cognition, academicachievement, and psychosocial function in children: A systematic review ofrandomized control trials. Prev Chronic Dis, 2013. 10(10).

15. Singh A, Uijtdewilligen L, Twisk JWR, Van Mechelen W, Chinapaw MJM.Physical activity and performance at school: a systematic review of theliterature including a methodological quality assessment. Arch PediatrAdolesc Med. 2012;166(1):49–55.

16. Taras H. Physical activity and student performance at school. J Sch Health.2005;75(6):214–8.

17. Trudeau F, Shephard RJ. Physical education, school physical activity, schoolsports and academic performance. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2008;5:10.

18. Bartholomew JB, Jowers EM. Physically active academic lessons inelementary children. Prev Med. 2011;52(Suppl 1):S51–4.

19. Mahar MT. Impact of short bouts of physical activity on attention-to-task inelementary school children. Prev Med. 2011;52(Suppl 1):S60–4.

20. Donnelly, J.E. and K. Lambourne, Classroom-based physical activity,cognition, and academic achievement. Preventive Medicine, 2011.52(SUPPL.): p. S36-S42.

21. Norris E, Shelton N, Dunsmuir S, Duke-Williams O, Stamatakis E. Physicallyactive lessons as physical activity and educational interventions: asystematic review of methods and results. Prev Med. 2015;72:116–25.

22. Owen KB, Parker PD, Van Zanden B, Macmillan F, Astell-Burt T, Lonsdale C.Physical activity and school engagement in youth: a systematic review andmeta-analysis. Educ Psychol. 2016;51(2):129–45.

23. Erwin H, Fedewa A, Beighle A, Ahn S. A quantitative review of physicalactivity, health, and learning outcomes associated with classroom-basedphysical activity interventions. J Appl Sch Psychol. 2012;28(1):14–36.

24. Ma JK, Le Mare L, Gurd BJ. Four minutes of in-class high-intensity intervalactivity improves selective attention in 9- to 11-year olds. Appl Physiol NutrMetab. 2015;40(3):238–44.

25. Mahar MT, Murphy SK, Rowe DA, Golden J, Shields AT, Raedeke TD. Effectsof a classroom-based program on physical activity and on-task behavior.Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2006;38(12):2086–94.

26. Schmidt, M., V. Benzing, and M. Kamer, Classroom-based physical activitybreaks and Children's attention: cognitive engagement works! Frontiers inPsychology, 2016.

27. Riley, N., D.R. Lubans, K. Holmes, and P.J. Morgan, Findings from the EASYminds cluster randomized controlled trial: evaluation of a physicalactivity integration program for mathematics in primary schools. J PhysAct Health, 2015.

28. Riley N, Lubans DR, Morgan PJ, Young M. Outcomes and process evaluationof a programme integrating physical activity into the primary schoolmathematics curriculum: the EASY minds pilot randomised controlled trial. JSci Med Sport. 2015;18(6):656–61.

29. Rasberry, C.N., S.M. Lee, L. Robin, B.A. Laris, L.A. Russell, K.K. Coyle, and A.J.Nihiser, The association between school-based physical activity, includingphysical education, and academic performance: A systematic review of theliterature. Preventive Medicine, 2011. 52(SUPPL.): p. S10-S20.

30. Vazou S, Smiley-Oyen A. Moving and academic learning are not antagonists:acute effects on executive function and enjoyment. J Sport Exerc Psychol.2014;36(5):474–85.

31. National Collaborating Centre for Methods and Tools. Quality AssessmentTool for Quantitative Studies. 2008; Available from: http://www.nccmt.ca/resources/search/14.

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 22 of 24

Page 23: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

32. de Greeff JW, Hartman E, Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Bosker RJ, Doolaard S,Visscher C. Long-term effects of physically active academic lessons onphysical fitness and executive functions in primary school children. HealthEduc Res. 2016;31(2):185–94.

33. Beck MM, Lind RR, Geertsen SS, Ritz C, Lundbye-Jensen J, Wienecke J.Motor-enriched learning activities can improve mathematical performancein preadolescent children. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:645.

34. Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between executive function andacademic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative NationalSample. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21(4):327–36.

35. Ahamed Y, MacDonald H, Reed K, Naylor P-J, Liu-Ambrose T, McKay H.School-based physical activity does not compromise Children's academicperformance. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(2):371–6.

36. Carlson JA, Engelberg JK, Cain KL, Conway TL, Mignano AM, Bonilla EA,Geremia C, Sallis JF. Implementing classroom physical activity breaks:associations with student physical activity and classroom behavior. PrevMed. 2015;81:67–72.

37. Hill L, Williams JHG, Aucott L, Milne J, Thomson J, Greig J, Munro V, Mon-Williams M. Exercising attention within the classroom. DevelopmentalMedicine & Child Neurology. 2010;52(10):929–34.

38. Hill LJB, Williams JHG, Aucott L, Thomson J. And M. Mon- Williams, Howdoes exercise benefit performance on cognitive tests in primary-schoolpupils? Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2011;53(7):630–5.

39. Howie EK, Beets MW, Pate RR. Acute classroom exercise breaks improve on-task behavior in 4th and 5th grade students: a dose–response. Ment Healthand Phys Act. 2014;7(2):65–71.

40. Howie EK, Schatz J, Pate RR. Acute effects of classroom exercise breaks onexecutive function and math performance: a dose-response study. Res QExerc Sport. 2015;86(3):217–24.

41. Janssen M, Chinapaw MJM, Rauh SP, Toussaint HM, van Mechelen W,Verhagen EALM. A short physical activity break from cognitive tasksincreases selective attention in primary school children aged 10–11. MentHealth and Phys Act. 2014;7(3):129–34.

42. Katz DL, Cushman D, Reynolds J, Njike V, Treu JA, Walker J, Smith E, Katz C.Putting physical activity where it fits in the school day: preliminary results ofthe ABC (activity bursts in the classroom) for fitness program. Prev ChronicDis. 2010;7(4):A82.

43. Ma JK, Le Mare L, Gurd BJ. Classroom-based high-intensity interval activityimproves off-task behaviour in primary school students. Appl Physiol NutrMetab. 2014;39(12):1332–7.

44. Uhrich TA, Swalm RL. A pilot study of a possible effect from a motor taskon reading performance. Percept Mot Skills. 2007;104(3 Pt 1):1035–41.

45. Whitt-Glover MC, Ham SA, Yancey AK. Instant recess(R): a practical tool forincreasing physical activity during the school day. Prog Community HealthPartnersh. 2011;5(3):289–97.

46. Wilson, A.N., T. Olds, K. Lushington, J. Petkov, and J. Dollman, The impact of10-min activity breaks outside the classroom on male students' on-taskbehaviour and sustained attention: a randomised crossover design. ActaPaediatr, 2015.

47. Lisahunter R, Abbott D, Macdonald J. Ziviani, and M. Cuskelly, Active kidsactive minds: a physical activity intervention to promote learning? Asia-Pacific Journal of Health, Sport & Physical Education. 2014;5(2):117–31.

48. Barnard M, Van Deventer KJ, Oswald MM. The role of active teachingprogrammes in academic skills enhancement of grade 2 learners in theStellenbosch region. South African Journal for Research in Sport, PhysicalEducation & Recreation (SAJR SPER). 2014;36(3):1–14.

49. Altenburg TM, Chinapaw MJM, Singh AS. Effects of one versus two bouts ofmoderate intensity physical activity on selective attention during a schoolmorning in Dutch primary schoolchildren: a randomized controlled trial.Journal of Science & Medicine in Sport. 2016;19(10):820–4.

50. van den Berg V, Saliasi E, de Groot RH, Jolles J, Chinapaw MJ, Singh AS.Physical activity in the school setting: cognitive performance is not affectedby three different types of acute exercise. Front Psychol. 2016;7:723.

51. Mead T, Scibora L, Gardner J, Dunn S. The impact of stability balls, activitybreaks, and a sedentary classroom on standardized math scores. PhysicalEducator. 2016;73(3):433–49.

52. Bailey CG, DiPerna JC. Effects of classroom-based energizers on primarygrade Students' physical activity levels. Physical Educator. 2015;72(3):480–95.

53. Erwin H, Fedewa A, Ahn S. Student academic performance outcomes of aclassroom physical activity intervention: a pilot study. InternationalElectronic Journal of Elementary Education. 2012;4(3):473–87.

54. Fedewa AL, Ahn S, Erwin H, Davis MC. A randomized controlled designinvestigating the effects of classroom-based physical activity on children’sfluid intelligence and achievement. Sch Psychol Int. 2015;36(2):135–53.

55. Grieco LA, Jowers EM, Bartholomew JB. Physically active academic lessonsand time on task: the moderating effect of body mass index. Med SciSports Exerc. 2009;41(10):1921–6.

56. Vazou S, Gavrilou P, Mamalaki E, Papanastasiou A, Sioumala N. Doesintegrating physical activity in the elementary school classroom influenceacademic motivation? International Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology.2012;10(4):251–63.

57. Goh TL, Hannon J, Webster C, Podlog L, Newton M. Effects of a TAKE 10!Classroom-based physical activity intervention on third- to fifth-gradeChildren's on-task behavior. J Phys Act Health. 2016;13(7):712–8.

58. Donnelly JE, Greene JL, Gibson CA, Smith BK, Washburn RA, Sullivan DK,DuBose K, Mayo MS, Schmelzle KH, Ryan JJ, Jacobsen DJ, Williams SL.Physical activity across the curriculum (PAAC): a randomized controlled trialto promote physical activity and diminish overweight and obesity inelementary school children. Prev Med. 2009;49(4):336–41.

59. Graham DJ, Lucas-Thompson RG, O'Donnell MB. Jump in! An investigationof school physical activity climate, and a pilot study assessing theacceptability and feasibility of a novel tool to increase activity duringlearning. Front Public Health. 2014;2:58.

60. McCrady-Spitzer SK, Manohar CU, Koepp GA, Levine JA. Low-cost andscalable classroom equipment to promote physical activity and improveeducation. J Phys Act Health. 2015;12(9):1259–63.

61. Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Hartman E, de Greeff JW, Bosker RJ, Doolaard S,Visscher C. Improving academic performance of school-age children byphysical activity in the classroom: 1-year program evaluation. J Sch Health.2015;85(6):365–71.

62. Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Hartman E, de Greeff JW, Bosker RJ, Doolaard S,Visscher C. Moderate-to-vigorous physically active academic lessons andacademic engagement in children with and without a social disadvantage:a within subject experimental design. BMC Public Health. 2015;15:404.

63. Norris E, Shelton N, Dunsmuir S, Duke-Williams O, Stamatakis E. Virtual fieldtrips as physically active lessons for children: a pilot study. BMC PublicHealth. 2015;15:366.

64. Reed JA, Einstein G, Hahn E, Hooker SP, Gross VP, Kravitz J. Examining theimpact of integrating physical activity on fluid intelligence and academicperformance in an elementary school setting: a preliminary investigation. JPhys Act Health. 2010;7(3):343–51.

65. Grieco LA. E.M. Jowers, V.L. Errisuriz, and J.B. Bartholomew, Physically activevs. sedentary academic lessons: A dose response study for elementarystudent time on task. Prev Med. 2016;89:98–103.

66. Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Hartman E, de Greeff JW, Doolaard S, Bosker RJ,Visscher C. Physically active math and language lessons improve academicachievement: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2016;137(3):e20152743.

67. Brickenkamp R, Zillmer E. d2 Test of Attention, in d2 Test. Clark, Editors: P.L.Ackerman and E; 1998.

68. Manly T, Nimmo-Smith I, Watson P, Anderson V, Turner A, Robertson IH. Thedifferential assessment of children's attention: the test of everyday attentionfor children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2001;42(8):1065–81.

69. Neyens LGJ. and A.P. Aldenkamp. Stability of cognitive measures in childrenof average ability.

70. Nunnally, J.C., Psychometric theory. McGraw-Hill series in psychology. 1967:New York, McGraw-Hill [1967].

71. Roach GD, Dawson D, Lamond N. Can a shorter psychomotor vigilance taskbe used as a reasonable substitute for the ten-minute psychomotorvigilance task? Chronobiol Int. 2006;23(6):1379–87.

72. Best JR. Exergaming immediately enhances children's executive function.Dev Psychol. 2012;48(5):1501–10.

73. Lubans D, Richards J, Hillman C, Faulkner G, Beauchamp M, Nilsson M, Kelly P,Smith J, Raine L, Biddle S. Physical activity for cognitive and mental health inyouth: a systematic review of mechanisms. Pediatrics. 2016;138(3):1642.

74. Lambourne, K. and P. Tomporowski, The effect of exercise-induced arousalon cognitive task performance: a meta-regression analysis. Brain Res, 2010.1341(0): p. 12–24.

75. Madelaine A, Wheldall K. Towards a curriculum-based passage reading testfor monitoring the performance of low-progress readers using standardisedpassages: a validity study. Educ Psychol. 1998;18(4):471–8.

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 23 of 24

Page 24: Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions

76. Madelaine A, Wheldall K. Further progress towards a standardisedcurriculum-based measure of reading: calibrating a new passage readingtest against the new South Wales basic skills test. Educ Psychol.2002;22(4):461–71.

77. Naylor P-J, Nettlefold L, Race D, Hoy C, Ashe MC, Wharf Higgins J, McKayHA. Implementation of school based physical activity interventions: asystematic review. Preventive Medicine: An International Journal Devoted toPractice and Theory. 2015;72:95–115.

• We accept pre-submission inquiries

• Our selector tool helps you to find the most relevant journal

• We provide round the clock customer support

• Convenient online submission

• Thorough peer review

• Inclusion in PubMed and all major indexing services

• Maximum visibility for your research

Submit your manuscript atwww.biomedcentral.com/submit

Submit your next manuscript to BioMed Central and we will help you at every step:

Watson et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2017) 14:114 Page 24 of 24