effect of bilingualism on the development of cognitive ... · effect of bilingualism on the...

16
451 Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume. 8 Number 4 December 2017 Pp. 451-466 DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.24093/awej/vol8no4.31 Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign Languages College of Arts and Humanities Albaha University, Albaha City, Kingdom of Saudi Arabia Abstract The aim of this paper was to evaluate the contentions and evidence supporting two divergent views about the existence of advantages offered by bilingualism. It also considered whether a definite conclusion was possible. A qualitative research methodology using the published evidence was adopted. The published evidence supporting or rejecting the advantages of bilingualism was collected using search terms in the Google Scholar search engine. A total number of 64 papers were collected, among which only five challenged the claimed advantages enjoyed by bilingual individuals. The papers were classified by their orientation and discussed. Based on the points derived from the evaluation of evidence, some conclusions were drawn. The overwhelming research support in favour of the existence of supposed bilingual advantage tends to suggest a conclusion in favour of that hypothesis. However, the points raised by critics, such as small sample sizes, inadequate matching of other variables, as well as defective measurements and analysis cannot be ignored as they question the very validity of the studies which support bilingual advantage. Future research needs to pay more attention to these aspects. Keywords: advantages of bilingualism, bilingualism, bilingual acquisition, childhood bilingualism Cite as: Alshahrani, A. (2017). Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children. Arab World English Journal, 8 (4). DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.24093/awej/vol8no4.31

Upload: others

Post on 22-Jul-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

451

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume. 8 Number 4 December 2017 Pp. 451-466

DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.24093/awej/vol8no4.31

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children

Abdulaziz Alshahrani

Department of Foreign Languages

College of Arts and Humanities

Albaha University, Albaha City, Kingdom of Saudi Arabia

Abstract

The aim of this paper was to evaluate the contentions and evidence supporting two divergent views

about the existence of advantages offered by bilingualism. It also considered whether a definite

conclusion was possible. A qualitative research methodology using the published evidence was

adopted. The published evidence supporting or rejecting the advantages of bilingualism was

collected using search terms in the Google Scholar search engine. A total number of 64 papers

were collected, among which only five challenged the claimed advantages enjoyed by bilingual

individuals. The papers were classified by their orientation and discussed. Based on the points

derived from the evaluation of evidence, some conclusions were drawn. The overwhelming

research support in favour of the existence of supposed bilingual advantage tends to suggest a

conclusion in favour of that hypothesis. However, the points raised by critics, such as small sample

sizes, inadequate matching of other variables, as well as defective measurements and analysis

cannot be ignored as they question the very validity of the studies which support bilingual

advantage. Future research needs to pay more attention to these aspects.

Keywords: advantages of bilingualism, bilingualism, bilingual acquisition, childhood bilingualism

Cite as: Alshahrani, A. (2017). Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive

Processes among Children. Arab World English Journal, 8 (4).

DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.24093/awej/vol8no4.31

Page 2: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

452

Aim of this paper

There are two strongly held divergent views among researchers on whether bilinguals have

any advantage over monolinguals with respect to cognitive functions. One view, proposed by a

large group headed by Bialystok and her associates, presents evidence to show that attention

control and a superior capacity to analyse knowledge are two cognitive advantages of bilingualism.

The other group has obtained some recent evidence rejecting the idea of any cognitive advantage

deriving from bilingualism. The aim of this paper is to evaluate the contentions and evidence

proposed by the two groups and examine whether a definite conclusion is possible.

Background

Surprisingly, there is relatively little reliable research on the effects of bilingualism on

cognitive processes and development, especially in the case of children. This is a nascent area of

research and much needs to be done to answer a number of related questions. One of the

complexities involved in the research into bilingualism in children is the large array of disciplines

that such research involves, including psychology, education and linguistics.

In an edited book on cognitive aspects of bilingualism, Kecskes & Albertazzi (2007)

differentiated between the real world and the projected world of cognition. Although information

conveyed by a language needs to be about this inner projected (mental) world, this is not shown

well in the chapters of the book. Some unexplored areas included in the discussions are: the gender

system in bilingual minds, the concept of context and task synergism, blending, and the

relationship between lexical and ontological categorisations.

This paper is organised in the following manner. A brief outline of the research

methodology used for this paper is given in the next section. This is followed by a section on the

evidence in favour of bilingual advantage is discussed. The next section deals with the evidence

presented by the opposing group to show that no bilingual advantages exist. The relative merits of

the two arguments are discussed in the section following the two theories. Conclusions are drawn

in the final section.

Research Methodology

Since the topic is dealt by using evidence in favour of and against bilingual advantage,

research publications available on both sides of the argument were searched for in the Google

Scholar search engine using appropriate search terms. In the first stage, the papers available from

the first five web pages of the Google Scholar selection were downloaded and then classified based

on their contents, irrespective of the year (any time) of publication. In the second stage, more

recent papers were searched for, specifying a publication date range from 2013 to 2017. A total of

64 papers were obtained by these methods, of which 59 research works argued in favour of

bilingual advantage while the rest were counter to this theory.

The intention of this paper is to present a simple qualitative discussion rather than a

quantitative meta-analysis based on international reviewing standards like Cochrane.

Page 3: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

453

The evidence in favour of the bilingual advantage

A large majority of the papers offering support for a theory of bilingual advantage belong

to the Bialystok group. For convenience, works supporting the likelihood of bilingual advantage

are discussed more or less chronologically, deriving mutual support from related works wherever

applicable.

Early works

Ianco-Worrall (1972) notes that bilinguals developed the ability to separate word sound

from word meaning earlier than monolinguals. The conclusion is derived from the results of a test

on 4-9 year old children. In this study, bilingualism is defined as dual language acquisition in an

environment of one child, from one language home. Out of three hypotheses connecting

bilingualism with various abilities, only one related to cognitive ability of a kind is verified in the

dissertation work of Ben-Zeev (1972).

In a very early study, Slobin (1973) proposes that children must have preliminary internal

structures to assimilate both linguistic and non-linguistic inputs. He argues that the development

of semantic intentions stems from general cognitive development. He uses linguistic input to

identify intended meaning, drawing on his theories about the nature of language use and general

cognitive-perceptive strategies. He also takes note of limitations enforced by operative memory.

In Slobin’s model, information from existing information structures are assimilated and absorbed

into new inputs. Increasing age expands these realms into new dimensions.

Cummins (1976) tries to resolve the inconsistency between earlier reports and current

reports regarding the relationship of bilingualism to cognitive consequences. Earlier studies have

reported a negative relationship between the two. The current results reported a positive

relationship. The contrasting results are partly due to the fact that the recent studies were conducted

on balanced bilinguals in what were claimed to be additive bilingual settings. In these cases, the

bilingual subjects had attained a high level of competence in their second language without

affecting their competence in their first language. In earlier studies, in bilinguals from minority

groups, the competency in the first language was being gradually replaced by competence in

second language. Thus, the bilinguals in these cases paid for their competence in the second

language by lowering competence in the first language. The author proposed a hypothesis Based

on this difference: the level of language competence attained by a bilingual child may mediate the

effects of the bilingual learning experience on cognitive growth. A threshold level of linguistic

competence may exist, beyond which the bilingual child needs to attain a certain level of both

competencies to avoid cognitive deficits and to allow potential benefits of bilingualism to affect

general cognitive functioning. There is no indication that the hypothesis was verified by any other

work.

The objective of the Irish study by Cummins (1978) was to assess the level of

metalinguistic awareness and the ability to evaluate contradictory and tautological statements

among children. A test to evaluate this was administered to grade Three and grade Six English-

Irish bilingual children and to control groups of unilingual children matched on Intelligence

Quotient, Socio-Economic Status, gender, and age. The findings showed a greater awareness of

certain properties of language and a better ability to evaluate contradictory statements among both

Page 4: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

454

grades of the bilingual children compared to the control group. Thus, it was proposed that

bilingualism can increase a child's metalinguistic awareness and promote an analytic orientation

to linguistic input.

In a later paper, Cummins (1979) further proposes the developmental inter-dependence

hypothesis. According to this hypothesis, “the development of competence in a second language

(L2) is partially a function of the type of competence already developed in L1 by the time that

intensive exposure to L2 begins” (p.3, abstract ). The threshold hypothesis proposed in the earlier

work was reiterated. Thus, the hypothesis states that “there may be threshold levels of linguistic

competence which a bilingual child must attain both in order to avoid cognitive disadvantages and

allow the potentially beneficial aspects of bilingualism to influence his cognitive and academic

functioning.” (p,3, abstract)The two hypotheses were combined to propose that “a cognitively and

academically beneficial form of bilingualism can be achieved only on the basis of adequately

developed first language (L1) skills.” (p.3, abstract).

In their work, Potter, So, Von Eckardt, & Feldman (1984) do not obtain support for a

simple word association hypothesis, but have found support for a meaning mediation hypothesis

to explain the association between equivalent words in the two languages of a bilingual person.

Thus it is suggested that the only connection between words in two languages is through an

underlying conceptual system to which pictured objects also have access. In this paradigm a certain

time seems to be needed to identify the word for a stimulus picture in the first language and

possibly also a certain time is needed to translate from the first to second language.

Cognitive aspects of bilingualism advantages

In one of E. Bialystok’s early works (Bialystok, 1987), she proposes a framework in which

metalinguistic awareness consisted of two processing components: the analysis of linguistic

knowledge, and the control of linguistic processes. In this model, global assessments of

metalinguistic ability by bilingual subjects are said to lead to conflicting results due to the

enhancement of only the latter of these processing components in a bilingual environment.

Attention control and the analysis of knowledge are two processes found to develop differently in

monolingual and bilingual children when solving linguistic problems (Bialystok, 1988).

According to the theory of cognitive complexity and control (CCC) proposed by Zelazo &

Frye (1997), preschool children lack the necessary conscious representation and executive

functioning to solve problems based on conflicting rules. Related to this, in a later work, Bialystok

& Majumder (1998) evaluate the effect of different degrees of bilingualism on the non-verbal

problem solving abilities of children in grade three. In their study, a French-English and a

Bengali-English bilingual group are compared with an English monolingual group. The problem

solving task is designed to measure the subject’s control of attention and analysis of knowledge.

In earlier studies, these capabilities have been shown to be different for monolingual and bilingual

children when it came to solving linguistic problems. Language proficiency tests show the French-

English group to be relatively more balanced bilinguals and the Bengali-English group to be

partially balanced bilinguals. The results show the balanced French-English group to be better in

solving non-linguistic tasks requiring the control of attention. However, there is no difference

among the three groups with respect to the ability to analyse representational structures. Thus,

Page 5: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

455

balanced bilinguals do appear to carry their linguistic advantages to non-linguistic tasks requiring

close attention.

In the studies of Bialystok (1999), she identifies analysis (manipulating representations)

and control (selective attention) as the two main components of language processing. One of these,

control, is shown to develop earlier in bilingual children than in monolinguals. The author also

identifies the role of attentional control in cognitive development, thus supporting CCC theory.

Depending upon discourse demands, at some level bilinguals need to control two languages

during speech. This affects their attentional networks. Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner

(2002) discuss three types of attentional networks among bilinguals. These three attentional

networks are: becoming alert, orienting and executive control. Bilinguals are faster in performing

tasks and specifically are more efficient at becoming alert and managing executive networks. More

content is added by bilinguals on presentation of an alerting cue and it is more useful to resolve

conflicting information. Their switching cost between trials is less than that of monolinguals. The

most efficient attention mechanisms appear to be produced by young adults who are at the peak of

their attention capabilities. In another work by Bialystok, Craik, Klein, & Viswanathan (2004),

the ability to control processing among bilinguals decreases less with age than among

monolinguals. Thus, bilingualism effectively helps to off-set age-related loss of the ability to

control some executive processes.

One experiment compares monolingual and bilingual children who either do or do not have

much experience with playing computer games. The experiment is done on specially designed

Simon tasks by (Bialystok, 2006). Video-game playing children are faster under most conditions.

They are even better when control conditions do not include conflict from irrelevant positions. On

the other hand, bilinguals are faster only when the most controlled attention is required to resolve

conflict between the position and the stimulus. The potential of experience to modify performance

and subtle differences in the processing of different versions of the Simon task are clear in these

results.

In the continuing work on bilingual development in relation to cognitive abilities, Bialystok

(2007) observes that the experience of controlling attention to two languages accelerates executive

control processes in early childhood. It continues to sustain the cognitive control advantages

through adulthood. During advanced age, it protects from the decline of these processes. In the

findings reported by Carlson & Meltzoff (2008), native bilingual children perform significantly

better on the executive function. They are better than both other groups, especially in the tasks that

appear to call for managing conflicting attentional demands (Conflict tasks), but not impulse

control (delay) tasks.

Based on a review and meta-analysis, Adesope, Lavin, Thompson, & Ungerleider, (2010)

find that bilingualism is associated with many cognitive outcomes. These included increased

attentional control, working memory, abstract and symbolic representation skills and

metalinguistic awareness. The term metalinguistic awareness refers to the ability to think about

the language. Even when bilinguals speak one language while suppressing the other language, both

Page 6: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

456

languages are active. Using many assessment tools, Hilchey & Klein (2011) observe higher

cognitive abilities among bilinguals than is the case with the monolingual group.

The findings of Morales & Bialystok (2013) show that working memory is better in

bilingual children, especially if the task contained additional executive function demands. In a

comparative study, all bilinguals perform executive control tasks similarly. Language tasks are

performed better by the bilingual groups whose instructional language is the same as the test

language. Thus, specific bilingual experience influenced performance in verbal tasks (Barac &

Bialystok, 2012). The results obtained by Morales, Calvo, & Bialystok (2013) confirm the

advantages of executive functioning and working memory among bilingual children compared to

monolinguals. The cognitive effects of bilingual advantages are highlighted by Kroll & Bialystok

(2013) as bilinguals outperform their monolingual counterparts on tasks that required ignoring

irrelevant information, task switching, and resolving conflict.

According to Macnamara & Conway (2014), experience in engaging general cognitive

mechanisms to manage two languages can explain the bilingual advantage. Also, bimodal (signed

language–spoken language) bilinguals do not exhibit this advantage due to the reduction of conflict

and control demands by distinct language modalities. The mechanism behind bilingual advantages

may arise from a combination of the magnitude of bilingual management demands and the amount

of experience managing those demands. In an experiment, bimodal bilinguals ae given the

experience of managing high bilingual management demands. These participants outperform

themselves from two years previously on demonstrating the cognitive advantages of bilingualism.

High socio-economic status and bilingualism are independently associated with better language

performance and executive function in a study by Calvo & Bialystok (2014).

In an investigation of US Latino students by Riggs, Shin, Unger, Spruijt-Metz, & Pentz

(2014), bilingualism significantly predicts an advantage in the summary executive function score

and working memory. Bilingual proficiency is positively related to executive function. While

controlling for a potential third variable in bilingualism, prospective associations between

bilingualism and executive function are demonstrated in this study, together with a significant role

for working memory in the relationship. In their study, Singh et al (2015) compare 114

monolingual and bilingual infants in a very basic task of information processing and visual

habituation at 6 months of age. The results show a generalized cognitive advantage in bilingual

infants that is broad in scope, early to emerge, and not specific to language.

Monolinguals and bilinguals with or without reading difficulties are compared for

executive functioning by Jalali‐Moghadam & Kormi‐Nouri (2015). Reading difficulty reduces

executive control more prominently in bilingual children than monolingual children with reading

difficulty. Thus, although bilinguals show general superiority in executive functioning with respect

to normal reading, they fail to show superiority when they have reading difficulties. Executive

functioning is important for both reading skills and bilingualism.

The study reported by Poarch & Bialystok (2015) consist of 203 children, 8–11 years old,

who are monolingual, partially bilingual, bilingual, or trilingual performing a flanker task. The

results show that bilingualism affects multitasking. Bilingual children outperformed monolinguals

Page 7: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

457

on the conflict trials in the flanker task. This result confirms the existence of a bilingual advantage

in executive functioning. The inclusion of partial bilinguals and trilinguals set limits on the role of

experience. Partial bilinguals perform closer to monolinguals and trilinguals perform closer to

bilinguals. These results suggest that degrees of experience are not well calibrated to improve

executive functioning. Because both languages of bilinguals are constantly active, the model

hypothesizes that bilinguals need to manage attention to the target language and avoid interference

from the non-target language. This process is likely to be carried out by recruiting the executive

function (EF) system, a system that is also the basis for multitasking.

In a review, Bialystok (2015) argues that attention, as a component of executive function,

emerges early in the developmental process and initiates the differences in bilingual children from

their infancy.

Linguistic and other aspects of cognitive advantage of bilingualism

Lexical production in early sequential bilinguals (Spanish-English) is studied by Kohnert,

Bates, & Hernandez (1999). Spanish is learned as the first language at home. Formal English

learning starts at the age of five. Although both languages develop with age, the initial dominance

of Spanish in the youngest children gives way progressively (with age) to relatively balanced skills

in both Spanish and English in middle childhood and culminate in a clear English dominance

among adolescents and young adults. The response time for these lexical effects decrease with

increasing age. The developmental changes in the speed-accuracy trade-offs in the bilingual

condition can be due to a change in the ability to resist cognitive interference during word

production.

A common belief is that there is a stage among children learning two languages

simultaneously during infancy, at which, they cannot differentiate their two languages. Almost all

studies show bilingual children mixing elements from both languages. These results are assumed

to provide evidence for a unitary, undifferentiated language system, known as the unitary language

system hypothesis. However, Genesee (1989) questioned this assumption, pointing out that

bilingual children developed differentiated language systems from the beginning and could use

their developing languages in contextually sensitive ways. The possible role of parents was

suggested as an area for future research.

The objective of a study by Bialystok (2001) was to evaluate the differences in

metalinguistic development between monolingual and bilingual children in terms of their

awareness of words, their syntax and phonology. The difference between monolinguals and

bilinguals with respect to the types of tasks they perform better are identified. In all three tasks,

there was no uniform superiority demonstrated by either mono or bilingual children. Different

degrees of analysis and control affecting the tasks differently among either mono or bilingual

children may account for the observed variations.

From a comparison of English and Mandarin Chinese monolinguals and Mandarin-English

bilinguals, Goetz (2003) notes a significant superiority of bilinguals to monolinguals in all tasks.

Greater inhibitory control, greater metalinguistic understanding, and a greater sensitivity to

sociolinguistic interactions with interlocutors were proposed as reasons for the bilinguals being

Page 8: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

458

relatively superior. In a similar vein, in two studies involving identification of the alternative image

in a reversible figure, Bialystok & Shapero (2005) note that bilingual children were more

successful than monolinguals in seeing the other meaning in the images.

According to Bialystok (2007) some aspects of reading ability, notably phonological

awareness, are rooted in general cognitive mechanisms and transfer easily across languages,

whereas others, such as decoding, are more language dependent and language-specific and need

to be relearned with each new writing system. Writing systems and the differences between them

have a greater impact on children's acquisition of literacy than previously believed. Not

surprisingly, this relationship between writing and literacy has also been found to be related to

emerging ability with phonological awareness, and such factors have a subtle influence on

children's emerging concepts of print.

Based on their cumulative research, Bialystok, Craik, & Luk (2008) note that younger

participants performed certain tasks better than the older ones. This confirms the effect of the

ageing process. There is no difference between monolinguals and bilinguals regarding working on

memory tasks. Monolinguals perform better in lexical linguistic tasks. Bilinguals perform better

on executive control tasks. The author uses bilinguals from many languages with English as part

of the samples.

The benefits and costs of the bilingual experience on the cognitive and linguistic

performance of individuals across their lifespans are discussed by Bialystok & Craik (2010). The

costs are associated with bilinguals having lower formal language proficiency than monolinguals.

The benefits are related to the enhanced executive control in nonverbal tasks requiring conflict

resolution.

In the studies by Bialystok (2010), the bilingual advantage is found not only for

traditionally demanding conditions, but also in processing complex stimuli in those tasks requiring

executive processing components for conflict resolution. These includ switching and updating,

even when no inhibition appeared to be involved. The significant effect of bilingualism in eight-

year old children as shown by superior performances in spatial complex reasoning tasks in the

context of academic achievement is discussed by Greenberg, Bellana, & Bialystok (2013). A

significant level of problem-solving ability is implied in these performances, which is an essential

requirement of high academic achievement.

The gradual development of metalinguistic advantages is noticed by Bialystok, Peets, &

Moreno (2014) among children in a French immersion programme. Tasks requiring more

executive control, in terms of grammaticality judgments, appear later. Performance in some tasks

improve with age, but do not exceed the performance of monolinguals. For example, advanced

verbal fluency develop only when bilinguals reach the fifth grade.

According to Blom, Küntay, Messer, Verhagen, & Leseman (2014), bilingualism increases

working memory among children from low social economic status in a Turkish-Dutch population.

Working memory tasks involving both storage and processing increase independently of language,

but proficiency is also related to verbal tasks. These findings support the hypothesis that experience

Page 9: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

459

with dual language management influences the central executive control system that regulates

processing across a wide range of task demands.

Based on a research on bilingual deaf children, McQuarrie & Parrila (2014) conclude that

a signed-language phonological system facilitate the establishment of a “functional”

representational base to support reading acquisition for bilingual deaf learners.

According to Aguilar-Mediavilla, Buil-Legaz, Pérez-Castelló, Rigo-Carratalà, & Adrover-

Roig (2014), a sample of bilingual Spanish-Catalan children with specific language impairment

(SLI) exhibites a delay in processing abilities when they are aged six. Both comprehension and

letter identification are affected, but there is no problem with visual attention. Normally, at the

kindergarten stage, phonological awareness and verbal fluency can predict the reading outcomes

at school age.

In a research comparing the comprehension of active and passive English sentences in 7–

10 year old bilingual and monolingual children, Filippi et al. (2015), obtain results supporting the

association of bilingualism with better accuracy in comprehending syntactically complex

sentences in the presence of linguistic noise.

Based on a meta-analysis, Prevoo, Malda, Mesman, & van IJzendoorn (2016) propose a

task-dependent bidirectional transfer hypothesis. The hypothesis states that the strength of cross-

language transfer depends on the type of language proficiency task and the type of school outcomes

which are aimed for. Therefore, stimulation of oral language proficiency in both languages might

improve the school outcomes of bilingual children with immigrant backgrounds.

Inhibitory control

The results of three studies reported by Bialystok & Martin (2004) indicate that bilinguals

have a more sufficient inhibitory control to ignore impinging perceptual information compared to

monolinguals. However, they are not more skilled in representation, which confirms earlier

findings. The critical difficulty in solving this task is the lack of ability by some subjects to ignore

an obsolete display feature.

Cross-language activity, selection of the language to speak, planning of speech, language

selection and the word production steps of bilinguals’ speeches using a single language are

discussed by Kroll (2008). Language selection models hold that bilinguals develop the ability to

selectively use the intended language even if both languages are active. In an alternative model,

both languages compete for selection, requiring that cross-language activity modulates to select

the required language only. In this case, the non-target language needs to be inhibited. Some works

report on such inhibitory mechanisms. Three studies are done by Martin-Rhee & Bialystok (2008)

to assess the degree and type of inhibitory control through which bilingual children demonstrate

an ability to excel in tasks (Simon tasks as cited in Bialystok , Craik, Klein, and Viswanathan,

2004) by ignoring misleading perceptual cues. The studies involve the activation of inhibitory

controls leading to an inhibition of habitual responses. Bilingual children maintain their advantage

on tasks that require the control of attention but show no advantage on tasks that require the

inhibition of responses.

Page 10: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

460

MEG and genetic basis

In another study by Bialystok et al. (2005) magnet-encephalography (MEG) is used in

evaluating Simon tasks by Bilingual Cantonese–English, bilingual French–English and

monolingual English speakers. Any faster reaction time for congruent trials compared to

incongruent trials is recorded. The Cantonese group is faster than the other two groups, while the

differences between the other two groups are not significant. According to MEG data, all three

groups are characterised by the same pattern of activity, which involve signal changes in left and

medial prefrontal areas. However, the two bilingual groups differ from the monolingual group due

to greater activity in superior and middle temporal, cingulate, and superior and inferior frontal

brain regions located largely in the left hemisphere. In the case of monolinguals, faster reaction

times are associated with activation in the middle frontal brain regions. Thus, the management of

the two language systems leads to systematic changes in frontal executive functions.

Recent neuro-imaging results appear to show that bilingualism can compensate for the

degeneration of other cognitive functions associated with dementia and thus postpone the onset of

symptoms, (Bialystok, 2015). However, based on a study on 1,067 Spanish-English bilinguals in

the Washington/Hamilton Heights Inwood Columbia Aging Project (WHICAP), Zahodne,

Schofield, Farrell, Stern, & Manly (2014) report that bilingualism is not independently associated

with rates of cognitive decline or dementia conversion.

Using whole head magnetoencephalography (MEG), Ferjan Ramírez, Ramírez, Clarke,

Taulu, & Kuhl (2017) investigate brain responses to Spanish and English syllables in Spanish-

English bilingual and English monolingual 11-month-old infants. Monolingual infants are

sensitive to English. On the other hand, bilingual infants are sensitive to both languages. Neural

responses achieved by a slower transition from acoustic to phonetic sound analysis, together with

an adaptive and advantageous response to increased variability in language input, have been

responsible for this dual sensitivity of the bilingual brain. These bilingual neural responses extend

into the prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex, which may be related to the bilingual advantage in

executive function skills.

Allelic differences between monolinguals and bilinguals for frequencies of the

DRD2/ANKK1 taq1A polymorphism noticed by Hernandez, Greene, Vaughn, Francis, &

Grigorenko (2015) also raises the possibility of genetic differences between the two groups.

Negative results

From a comparative study of the effect of bilingualism on the language proficiency and

reading skill development in two systems of writing, Bialystok, McBride-Chang, & Luk (2005)

observe that both language exposure and instruction are responsible for the development of

phonological awareness. However, once established, these skills can be transferred across

languages in the case of both bilinguals and second language learners. On the other hand, decoding

ability develops separately for each language as a function of both proficiency and the nature of

instruction in that language. This skill is not transferred to the other language. Thus, there is no

overall effect which is obvious from this research on bilingualism effect on learning to read. The

performance depend entirely on the structure of the language, proficiency in that language and

Page 11: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

461

instructional experiences with that writing system. The comparison involves monolingual English,

bilingual English-Chinese and Chinese children beginning to learn English as a second language.

In their study to evaluate the effect of bilingual advantage on inhibitory skills, Duñabeitia

et al. (2014) use verbal and non-verbal versions of the Stroop task. The results show that bilingual

and monolingual participants perform equally in the two types of tasks across all the indices of

inhibitory effect on the skills explored. The absence of difference extend to all the age ranges

tested. There is no modulatory effect from any of the independent factors. Thus, bilingual children

do not seem to exhibit any specific advantage in simple tasks which elicite inhibition as compared

to monolinguals.

Analysing executive function and cognitive reserve, Valian (2015) proposes that the term

“executive function” includes a complex set of cognitive processes, the components of which are

sometimes minimally correlated with each other, depending on the task. Bilingualism is

inconsistently correlated with superior executive function. There are non-linguistic ways of

improving executive function. Benefits from bilingualism, and all other cognitively challenging

activities, are inconsistent due to variations in the number and kinds of experiences of individuals

promoting superior executive function. The author contends that executive function is a narrow

interpretation how bilingualism hinders or helps cognition.

In a carefully matched large sample study, Antón et al. (2014) do not observe any bilingual

advantages at all. This finding supports the contention that the reported positive bilingual

advantages can be due to small or unmatched samples.

The meta-analysis of works done on bilingual advantage in executive functioning by Paap,

Johnson, & Sawi (2015) show that such advantages either do not exist or are noticeable only in

very specific and undetermined conditions. The studies conducted after 2011, either do not give

any result or utilise small sample sizes. Some others produce group differences when inappropriate

tests of the critical interaction or baselines are used. In a few others, demographic factors are

imperfectly matched. Also in a few other studies, questionable use of the analysis-of-covariance

is evident, apparently in order to control for problematic factors. Direct replications are under-

utilized and even if they are attempted, the results of seminal studies cannot be reproduced.

Measures and tasks used in the tests do not demonstrate convergent validity. Any significant

differences in performances may not have been able to differentiate any task-specific mechanism

from the domain-free abilities of executive functions. There is only modest support from brain

imaging studies in evaluating the bilingual-advantage hypothesis. This is mainly because the

neural differences do not align with the behavioural differences and/or the neural measures are

ambiguous with respect to the magnitudes required to cause increases or decreases in brain

performance. There are cumulative effects of confirmation biases and common research practices

which lead to a belief that a phenomenon exists when it really does not, or has inflated the

frequency and effect size of a genuine phenomenon that can emerge only occasionally and in

restricted and undetermined circumstances.

Page 12: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

462

Conclusion

The number of works reporting negative results is far outnumbered by the number of works

indicating positive results for the hypothesis of bilingual advantages. Although the tendency will

be to conclude that bilingual advantage exists, the points raised by the critics regarding small

sample size, inadequacy of matching other variables, as well as measurement and analysis

problems need to be considered seriously in future research on the topic. After all, in the issue of

supporting one side or the other, the validity of the research work is arguably the most important

consideration in accepting or rejecting any research conclusions.

About the Author:

Dr. Abdulaziz Alshahrani is an assistant professor of Applied Linguistics, graduated from the

University of Newcastle, Australia. He was admitted to the degree of MA with distinction in

Applied Linguistics from the same institution. His works are related to the fields of language

acquisition, psycholinguistics, the roles of the social variables and other topics. At the moment, he

works as an assistant professor at the Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Albaha University, in Saudi

Arabia.

References

Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and

meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research,

80(2), 207-245. doi:10.3102/0034654310368803

Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., Buil-Legaz, L., Pérez-Castelló, J. A., Rigo-Carratalà, E., & Adrover-Roig,

D. (2014). Early preschool processing abilities predict subsequent reading outcomes in

bilingual Spanish–Catalan children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Journal of

communication disorders, 50(April), 19-35. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.03.003

Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., . . .

Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from

children. Frontiers in psychology, 5(May), 398. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398

Barac, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingual Effects on Cognitive and Linguistic Development:

Role of Language, Cultural Background, and Education. Child Development, 83(2), 413–

422. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01707.x

Ben-Zeev, S. (1972). The Influence of Bilingualism on Cognitive Development and Cognitive

Strategy. University of Chcagi. Retrieved October 1, 2015, from

http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED104535

Ben-Zeev, S. (1977). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive

development. Child development, 48(3), 1009-1018. doi:10.2307/1128353

Bialystok, E. (1978). A Theoretical Model of Second Language Learning. Language Learning Ann

Arbor, Mich., 28(1), 69-84. doi:10.1111/j.1467-1770.1978.tb00305.x

Bialystok, E. (1987). Influences of bilingualism on metalinguistic development. Interlanguage

studies bulletin (Utrecht), 3(2), 154-166. doi:10.1177/026765838700300205

Bialystok, E. (1988). Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental

psychology, 24(4), 560-567. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.4.560

Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive Complexity and Attentional Control in the Bilingual Mind. Child

Development, 70(3), 636–644. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00046

Page 13: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

463

Bialystok, E. (2001). Metalinguistic aspects of bilingual processing. Annual Review of Applied

Linguistics, 21, 169-181. doi:10.1017/S0267190501000101

Bialystok, E. (2006). Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon

task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie

expérimentale, 60(1), 68-79. doi:79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cjep2006008

Bialystok, E. (2007). Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research.

Language learning, 57(S1), 45-77. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2007.00412.x

Bialystok, E. (2007). Cognitive Effects of Bilingualism: How Linguistic Experience Leads to

Cognitive Change. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10(3),

210-223. doi:10.2167/beb441.0

Bialystok, E. (2010). Global–local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children:

Beyond inhibition. Developmental psychology, 46(1), 93-105. doi:10.1037/a0015466

Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of

attention. Child development perspectives, 9(2), 117-121. doi:10.1111/cdep.12116

Bialystok, E. (2015). The impact of bilingualism on cognition. Emerging Trends in the Social and

Behavioral Sciences: An Interdisciplinary, Searchable, and Linkable Resource, 1-12.

doi:10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0340

Bialystok, E. (2017). The Bilingual Adaptation: How Minds Accommodate Experience.

Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262. doi:10.1037/bul0000099.

Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. (2010). Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind.

Current directions in psychological science, 19(1), 19-23.

doi:10.1177/0963721409358571

Bialystok, E., & Majumder, S. (1998). The relationship between bilingualism and the development

of cognitive processes in problem solving. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(1), 69-85.

doi:10.1017/S0142716400010584

Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence

from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental science, 7(3), 325-339.

doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00351.x

Bialystok, E., & Shapero, D. (2005). Ambiguous benefits: The effect of bilingualism on reversing

ambiguous figures. Developmental Science, 8(6), 595-604. doi:10.1111/j.1467-

7687.2005.00451.x

Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for

bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112(3), 494–500.

doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and

older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,

34(4), 859-873. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., & Pantev, C. (2005). Effect of

bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: evidence from MEG. NeuroImage,

24(1), 40-49. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, Aging, and

Cognitive Control: Evidence From the Simon Task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290-

303. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290

Page 14: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

464

Bialystok, E., Luk, G., & Kwan, E. (2005). Bilingualism, Biliteracy, and Learning to Read:

Interactions Among Languages and Writing Systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(1),

43-61. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0901_4

Bialystok, E., McBride-Chang, C., & Luk, G. (2005). Bilingualism, Language Proficiency, and

Learning to Read in Two Writing Systems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(4), 580-

590. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.4.580

Bialystok, E., Peets, K. F., & Moreno, S. (2014). Producing bilinguals through immersion

education: Development of metalinguistic awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(1),

177-191. doi:10.1017/S0142716412000288

Blom, E., Küntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being

bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish–Dutch children. Journal of experimental

child psychology, 128(December), 105-119. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.007

Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status

on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130(3), 278-288.

doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015

Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young

children. Developmental science, 11(2), 282-298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x

Cummins, J. (1976). The Influence of Bilingualism on Cognitive Growth: A Synthesis of. Research

Findings and Explanatory Hypotheses. The Ontario Institutefor Studies in Education,

Ontario, Canada. Retrieved October 1, 2015, from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED125311

Cummins, J. (1978). Bilingualism and the development of metalinguistic awareness. Journal of

cross-cultural psychology, 9(2), 131-149. doi:10.1177/002202217892001

Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual

children. Review of educational research, 49(2), 222-251. Retrieved April 6, 2017, from

http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED257312.pdf

Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras,

M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited. Experimental

psychology, 61, 234-251. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000243

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency

and independence of attentional networks. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 14(3), 340-

347. doi:10.1162/089892902317361886

Ferjan Ramírez, N., Ramírez, R. R., Clarke, M., Taulu, S., & Kuhl, P. K. (2017). Speech

discrimination in 11‐month‐old bilingual and monolingual infants: a

magnetoencephalography study. Developmental science, 20(1), 1-16.

doi:10.1111/desc.12427

Filippi, R., Morris, J., Richardson, F. M., Bright, P., Thomas, M. S., Karmiloff-Smith, A., &

Marian, V. (2015). Bilingual children show an advantage in controlling verbal interference

during spoken language comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(3),

490-501. doi:10.1017/S1366728914000686

Genesee, F. (1989). Early bilingual development: one language or two? Journal of child language,

16(1), 161-179. doi:10.1017/S0305000900013490

Goetz, P. J. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism:

Language and Cognition, 6(1), 1-15. doi:10.1017/S1366728903001007

Page 15: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

465

Greenberg, A., Bellana, B., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Perspective-taking ability in bilingual children:

Extending advantages in executive control to spatial reasoning. Cognitive development,

28(1), 41-50. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.10.002

Hernandez, A. E., Greene, M. R., Vaughn, K. A., Francis, D. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2015).

Beyond the bilingual advantage: The potential role of genes and environment on the

development of cognitive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 35, 109-119.

doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.04.002

Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic

interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes.

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625-658. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7

Ianco-Worrall, A. D. (1972). Bilingualism and Cognitive Development. Child Development,

43(4), 1390-1400. doi:10.2307/1127524

Jalali‐Moghadam, N., & Kormi‐Nouri, R. (2015). The role of executive functions in bilingual

children with reading difficulties. Scandinavian journal of psychology, 56(3), 297-305.

doi:10.1111/sjop.12198

Kecskes, I., & Albertazzi, L. (Eds.). (2007). Cognitive Aspects of Bilingualism. Springer

Netherlands. doi:10.1007/978-1-4020-5935-3

Kohnert, K. J., Bates, E., & Hernandez, A. E. (1999). Balancing BilingualsLexical-Semantic

Production and Cognitive Processing in Children Learning Spanish and English. Journal

of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(6), 1400-1413.

doi:10.1044/jslhr.4206.1400

Kroll, J. F. (2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta

Psychologica, 128(3), 416–430. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.001

Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language

processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 497-514.

doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.799170

Macnamara, B. N., & Conway, A. R. (2014). Novel evidence in support of the bilingual advantage:

Influences of task demands and experience on cognitive control and working memory.

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 520-525. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0524-y

Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control

in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: language and cognition, 11(1), 81–

93. doi:10.1017/S1366728907003227

McQuarrie, L., & Parrila, R. (2014). Literacy and linguistic development in bilingual deaf children:

Implications of the" and" for phonological processing. American annals of the deaf, 159(4),

372-384. doi:10.1353/aad.2014.0034

Morales, J. C., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and

bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187–202.

doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002

Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning

either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances.

Cortex, 69(August), 265-278. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014

Poarch, G. J., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Developmental

Review, 35(March), 113-124. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.003

Page 16: Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive ... · Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes among Children Abdulaziz Alshahrani Department of Foreign

Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 8. Number 4. December 2017

Effect of Bilingualism on the Development of Cognitive Processes Alshahrani

Arab World English Journal

www.awej.org

ISSN: 2229-9327

466

Potter, M. C., So, K. F., Von Eckardt, B., & Feldman, L. B. (1984). Lexical and conceptual

representation in beginning and proficient bilinguals. Journal of Verbal Learning and

Verbal Behavior, 23(1), 23-38. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90489-4

Prevoo, M. J., Malda, M., Mesman, J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2016). Within-and cross-

language relations between oral language proficiency and school outcomes in bilingual

children with an immigrant background: A meta-analytical study. Review of Educational

Research, 86(1), 237-276. doi:10.3102/0034654315584685

Riggs, N. R., Shin, H.-S., Unger, J. B., Spruijt-Metz, D., & Pentz, M. A. (2014). Prospective

associations between bilingualism and executive function in Latino children: Sustained

effects while controlling for biculturalism. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health,

16(5), 914-921. doi:10.1007/s10903-013-9838-0

Singh, L., Fu, C. S., Rahman, A. A., Hameed, W. B., Sanmugam, S., Agarwal, P., . . . Rifkin‐Graboi, A. (2015). Back to basics: a bilingual advantage in infant visual habituation. Child

development, 86(1), 294-302. doi:10.1111/cdev.12271

Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. Studies of child

language development, 1, 75-208. Retrieved April 10, 2017, from

http://homepages.neiu.edu/~circill/bofman/ling450/cognitive.pdf

Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3-

24. doi:10.1017/S1366728914000522

Zahodne, L. B., Schofield, P. W., Farrell, M. T., Stern, Y., & Manly, J. J. (2014). Bilingualism

does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish-speaking immigrants.

Neuropsychology, 28(2), 238. doi:10.1037/neu0000014

Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1997). Cognitive complexity and control: A theory of the development

of deliberate reasoning and intentional action. In M. I. Stamenov (Ed.), Language

Structure, Discourse and the Access to Consciousness (pp. 113-153). John Benjamins

Publishing. Retrieved October 1, 2015, from

https://books.google.co.in/books?hl=en&lr=&id=zovd1I60zpYC&oi=fnd&pg=PA113&d

q=Zelazo+%26+Frye+(1997)+cognitive+complexity+and+control&ots=z4oabCi5g7&sig

=WN2DNB8U0nPlL1uXmVGkC85hIuA#v=onepage&q&f=false