editors' notes: dimensions of early symbol use

6
editors’ notes: dimensions of early symbol use One burden of developmental psychology is to provide a positive characterization of the child at each stage of development. This challenge has proved easier to meet for some stages than for others. Thus there is widespread agreement that infancy is marked by the construction of practical intelligence and that the early school years feature the beginnings of conceptual thought. Finding a positive characterization of the years between infancy and the onset of school has proved more elusive, however; this stage is often viewed in terms of its lacks-the child is seen as illogical, egocentric, or intuitive. Slowly this situation is changing. First of all, there is increasing evidence that preschool children are capable of positive achievements. Claims that the child is incapable of taking another’s point of view or of classifying objects sys- tematically have proved unfounded. Given some flexibility in the administration of a task and some awareness of limitations in the child‘s information-processing capacity, a wide range of skills have been noted in children from two to five (see Borke, 1975;Bryant, 1974; Gelman and Gallistel, 1978;Rosch and Mervis, 1975). Even more encouragingly, there is growing acknowledgement that this period of life can be spoken of in terms of a single positive acquisition-symbol use. Such diverse authorities as Jerome Bruner, Alexander Luria, Jean Piaget, and Heinz Werner have noted that during this phase of life, the child becomes able to use various vehicles-both those devised by himself and those donated by the cul- ture-to capture aspects of his experience, to make reference, and to communi- cate messages to others with increasing clarity. While many now endorse this characterization, there is no consensus on the optimal conceptualization of the process of symbolic development. Among those who concur that symbolization is the central achievement of the preschool years, some authorities are loathe to posit connections among the various sym- bolic systems (Golomb, 1973;Goodnow, 1972;01son, 1970). In their estimation, each symbolic system has its own developmental trajectory; detailed descriptions of the development of, say, language are unlikely to exhibit significant parallels with the course of symbolic growth in other domains. An even more radical point of view has been propounded by transformational linguists (Chomsky, forth- coming; Fodor, 1975; Sperber, 1974). These authorities consider it false to group together the diverse vehicles used by human beings for referential ends. Proceed- ing from the conviction that the rules governing language are a class apart, these thinkers find no more basis for grouping together language and drawing than for conjoining language and eating or drawing and dueling. vii

Upload: howard-gardner

Post on 06-Jun-2016

216 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Editors' notes: Dimensions of early symbol use

editors’ notes: dimensions of early symbol use

One burden of developmental psychology is to provide a positive characterization of the child at each stage of development. This challenge has proved easier to meet for some stages than for others. Thus there is widespread agreement that infancy is marked by the construction of practical intelligence and that the early school years feature the beginnings of conceptual thought. Finding a positive characterization of the years between infancy and the onset of school has proved more elusive, however; this stage is often viewed in terms of its lacks-the child is seen as illogical, egocentric, or intuitive.

Slowly this situation is changing. First of all, there is increasing evidence that preschool children are capable of positive achievements. Claims that the child is incapable of taking another’s point of view or of classifying objects sys- tematically have proved unfounded. Given some flexibility in the administration of a task and some awareness of limitations in the child‘s information-processing capacity, a wide range of skills have been noted in children from two to five (see Borke, 1975;Bryant, 1974; Gelman and Gallistel, 1978;Rosch and Mervis, 1975). Even more encouragingly, there is growing acknowledgement that this period of life can be spoken of in terms of a single positive acquisition-symbol use. Such diverse authorities as Jerome Bruner, Alexander Luria, Jean Piaget, and Heinz Werner have noted that during this phase of life, the child becomes able to use various vehicles-both those devised by himself and those donated by the cul- ture-to capture aspects of his experience, to make reference, and to communi- cate messages to others with increasing clarity.

While many now endorse this characterization, there is no consensus on the optimal conceptualization of the process of symbolic development. Among those who concur that symbolization is the central achievement of the preschool years, some authorities are loathe to posit connections among the various sym- bolic systems (Golomb, 1973;Goodnow, 1972;01son, 1970). In their estimation, each symbolic system has its own developmental trajectory; detailed descriptions of the development of, say, language are unlikely to exhibit significant parallels with the course of symbolic growth in other domains. An even more radical point of view has been propounded by transformational linguists (Chomsky, forth- coming; Fodor, 1975; Sperber, 1974). These authorities consider it false to group together the diverse vehicles used by human beings for referential ends. Proceed- ing from the conviction that the rules governing language are a class apart, these thinkers find no more basis for grouping together language and drawing than for conjoining language and eating or drawing and dueling.

vii

Page 2: Editors' notes: Dimensions of early symbol use

Even among those who endorse the symbolization account, there are sig- nificant controversies. Until recently, the most prominent theorists (Bruner, 1966; Piaget, 1962; Werner and Kaplan, 1963) have viewed symbolization as “of a piece.” According to these authorities, once the child has grasped the possibil- ity of representing meanings, this insight should rapidly color the use of virtually any communicative material-language, marks, gestures. But it has become in- creasingly evident that the spread of representational skills is hardly quick or uniform. Language and symbolic play emerge early in the second year, but draw- ing and three-dimensional representation are products of the third and fourth year, while the conventionalized use of numbers and letters comes still later. The unevenness of these “symbolic discoveries” has made it increasingly difficult to embrace the notion of a single, unified semiotic function.

While the issues in this debate remain to be sorted out (see Gardner, forth- coming), one conclusion seems inescapable. The ratio of theorizing to facts is un- comfortably high in this area. Indeed, little is known about the domains and materials upon which any synthesis concerning symbolization would have to be based. We are just beginning to understand the developmental course of individ- ual symbol systems; we are even uncertain about how best to conceptualize such systems-whether a medium like language, for example, may not be better de- scribed in terms of more specific skills, such as sentence formation, storytelling, or metaphor making, each of which may have integral ties to other culturally de- fined media. Clearly, given our ignorance about the acquisition of individual grammars, we find ourselves in a weak position to assess the relations among symbolic systems.

Still confronting us are other equally deep and equally problematic ques- tions regarding the process of symbolization. We need to investigate the relation- ship between the course of symbolization and other fundamental areas of human development. Complementing the kind of inquiry undertaken by Piaget (1962, 1970), where the cognitive underpinnings of symbol use have been explored, we need to explore the social foundations out of which symbol use emerges (see Bates, 1476). And, as we move toward a less monolithic view of symbol use, we need to consider the possibility of diverse routes to the acquisition of symbolic competence. Until recently it was only in the realm of early language learning that findings suggesting individual routes to competence emerged (see Nelson, 1973). But now there is evidence that styles of symbol use may affect the man- ner in which individual children approach a range of symbolic challenges (see Gardner, 1976; Shotwell, Wolf, and Gardner, forthcoming; Wolf and Gardner, forthcoming).

These questions have prompted a number of researchers, coming from quite different perspectives, to investigate young children’s increasing mastery of symbol use. At Harvard Project Zero, with the aid of a grant from the Spencer Foundation, we have initiated a broad based inquiry into the nature and evolu- tion of early symbol use. Building upon the theoretical analysis of symbol sys-

Page 3: Editors' notes: Dimensions of early symbol use

tems proposed by Nelson Goodman (1968) as well as some early findings on the evolution of artistic skills (Gardner, 1973; Gardner, Wolf, and Smith, 1975), we have sought to assemble a full description of the routes to symbolic competence in the opening years of life. Nine first born children have been followed on a reg- ular basis for several years as they achieved initial competence in seven areas of symbolic functioning: language (specifically storytelling and metaphor), symbolic play, number, music, dance and movement, two-dimensional representation (drawing and mapping), and three-dimensional representation (clay and block building). As a parallel endeavor, a cross-sectional study of eighty-six children between two and five has also been undertaken. The two studies should yield a picture of symbolic competence which takes into account growth within indi- vidual media, relations among media, sources for symbolization in other domains of growth, and the possibility of diverse routes to symbolic competence. Our goal is a portrait of symbolization that is comprehensive in scope, accurate in detail, and applicable to the process of symbolization as it occurs in normal children.

Though our results have been presented to several audiences and published in various forums, we have not yet had the opportunity to bring together the diverse facets of our study. For this reason we are particularly grateful to Dr. William Damon for extending to us an invitation to edit a volume devoted to early symbolic competence in the child. We have taken this opportunity to de- scribe some initial findings from our studies of the first years of symbolization in a trio of media. We have included here reports on the first steps of symbol use in the realm of language (specifically, metaphor), three-dimensional representation (particularly block building), and music (specifically, the singing of simple tunes). (For reports on other media examined in our study, see Gardner and Wolf, in preparati0n;Rubin and Gardner, forthcoming;and the other articles cited above.) But clearly, a comprehensive description of the emergence of symbolization re- quires more than the output of a single laboratory or one program of research. Thus we are fortunate to have a number of colleagues whose work extends our own, complementing it with different questions, methods and perspectives. The opportunity to bring these various perspectives together has been a special re- ward of the present undertaking.

The present volume reflects the enormity of the task that occupies chil- dren between infancy and schooling. The articles are sequenced to suggest the evolution of symbol use from rather simple communication towards the sophisti- cated use of conventional representational systems. Consequently, we begin with Savage-Rumbaugh’s description, based on her work with primates of the social and cognitive foundations for the representation of meaning. Reciprocally, we conclude with Saxe and Shotwell’s portraits of children’s emerging abilities to apply the system of numerical representation to their experience.

The ordering of articles also reflects the chronology of milestones within the area of symbolic growth. The crucial foundation of communicative compe- tence, as described by Savage-Rumbaugh, provides the basis for initial symbol

Page 4: Editors' notes: Dimensions of early symbol use

X

use in the intimate sharing of experience through the grammars of language and pretense play (Schafer). In a second wave of symbolic use, children’s emerging ca- pacities to discern and capture likenesses among objects manifest themselves in initial attempts at representational drawing (Smith this volume) and metaphors basedupon perceptual resemblances (Winner and others). A third form of symbol use-that involved in the production of musical tunes @lcKemon)-reflects both an emerging sensitivity to patterned regularity and increasing mastery of the con- ventions of western music. Finally, we observe how symbolic capacities that c a p ture the attributes and likenesses of objects are reorganized by the tools of con- ventional notational systems like counting (Saxe, Shotwell).

Other themes can also be discerned. The important role occupied by the specific demands of each medium comes through with special force in the pieces by Smith, McKernon, and Saxe. The necessity for attending to the connections among media is underscored by Shotwell’s treatment of the staircase problem. The advantages of viewing symbolization from a cross-species perspective come through in Savage-Rumbaugh’s article, just as the effects of a radically different cultural context can be seen in Saxe’s account of numerical systems in Papuan New Guinea, Finally, the contributions made to early symbol use by affective concerns and by relations to significant others are sensitively conveyed in Schafer’s clinical observations.

The contributions which can emerge from a range of methods are sug- gested by the diversity of the articles collected here. Smith‘s paper, a case study of a single child making a single drawing, reveals the insights that can come from a child with given skills confronting a specific medium. Project Zero’s longitudi- nal study, documenting regularities in the detailed developmental trajectory of a small group of children, is complemented by Saxe’s cross-cultural investigation. We also note the roles of various models which may guide an investigation: Smith and McKernon proceed from a view of symbolic capacities as they are exhibited by mature artists; Schafer, from the vantage point of the expressive functions served by symbo1s;while Savage-Rumbaugh and Saxe highlight the need for inte- gration between personal knowledge and valid communication. So complex a phenomenon as symbolic development certainly deserves, and should eventual- ly be illuminated by, this range of investigative perspectives.

Just as our own study is not yet completed, the various contributions gathered here constitute only a small part of the puzzle of early symbolic devel- opment. Still, taken together, they suggest at least a first draft of findings that are likely to be the basis of more authoritative statements on this subject. We see a sensitive dialectic at work across diverse symbol systems-on the one hand, the child, with his emerging computing, calculating, perceptual, and motor capacities, and on the other, each medium of expression, with its specific potentials, limita- tions, biases, and flexibilities.

To some extent, the child’s resulting activities resemble patterns found in the adult. After all, the preschool child does count, create metaphors, and draw

Page 5: Editors' notes: Dimensions of early symbol use

xi

representationally. Yet what is striking here is the extent to which the child, at work solving the problems posed by each medium, is truly inventive. In case after case, one sees the child going beyond what he did before-and with equal stead- fastness, ignoring adult models-in order to pursue instead his own conception of what a problem is, what solution would be acceptable, and how best to realize it. To be sure, much of this problem solving occurs beneath the level of conscious awareness. Yet the factors which govern growth permit the child enormous and rapid progress as he learns the structure of the Western diatonic scale, the moves which permit graphic representation, the steps to a proper staircase. And some of the suggestive parallels across media-a beginning sensitivity to configurational properties by the age of two, a developing sense of how to organize component parts at the age of three, an incipient understanding of the notational systems of the culture at the ages of four and five-indicate that at least certain aspects of early symbol use transcend the parochialities of a single medium.

Much of this is only intimated here, and much more analysis is required before we can reach firm conclusions about the ontogensis and development of symbolic competences. What has emerged, however, is a set of questions that demand to be answered and some sources of data that appear germane to the questions at issue. We hope that the publication of this volume, tentative and in- complete though it may be, will promote that scholarly dialogue which is essen- tial for and often precedes a significant synthesis.

Howard Gardner Dennie Wolf

references

Bates, E. Language in Context. New York: Academic Press, 1976. Borke, H. “Piaget’s Mountains Revisited: Changes in the Egocentric Landscape.”

Bruner, J. S. “On Cognitive Growth.” In J. S. Bruner;R. R. Olver, and P. M.

Bryant, P. Perception and Understanding in Young Children: A n Experimental

Chomsky, N. “Remarks.” In P. Piatelli-Piemonte (Ed.), Language and Cognition.

Fodor, J. The Language of Thought. New York: Crowell, 1975. Gardner, H. The Arts and Human Development. New York: Wiley, 1973. Gardner, H. “Promising Paths Toward Artistic Knowledge.” Journal of Aesthetic

Education, 1976,lO (3 and 41,201-207. Gardner, H. “Developmental Psychology after Piaget: An Approach in Terms of

Symbolization.” Human Development, forthcoming. Gardner, H., and Wolf, D. “First Drawings: Notes on the Relation Between Per-

ception and Production in the Visual Arts.” In D. Fisher and C. Nodine (Eds.), What Is a Picture? forthcoming.

Developmental Psychology, 1975 , l I , 240-43.

Greenfield, Studies in Cognitive Growth. New York: Wiley, 1966.

Approach. New York: Basic Books, 1974.

Cambridge: Harvard University Press, forthcoming.

Page 6: Editors' notes: Dimensions of early symbol use

Gardner, H., Wolf, D., and Smith, A. “Artistic Symbols in Early Childhood.” New York University Education Quarterly, 1975, 6 , 13-21.

Gelman, R., and Gallistel, R. The Child’s Understanding o f Number. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1978.

Golomb, C. “Children’s Representation of the Human Figure: The Effects of Models, Media, and Instruction.” Genetic Psychology Monographs, 1973, 87, 197-251.

Goodman, N. Languages o f Art. Indianapolis, Ind.: Bobbs-Merrill, 1968. Goodnow, J. “Rules and Repertoires, Rituals and Tricks of the Trade: Social

and Informational Aspects to Cognitive Representational Development.” In S . Farnham-Diggory (Ed.), Information Processing in Children. New York: Academic Press, 1972.

Nelson, K. “Structure and Strategy in Learning to Talk.” Monographs of the So- ciety for Research in Child Development, 1973,149.

Olson, D. Cognitive Development. New York: Academic Press, 1970. Piaget, J. Play, Dreams, and Imitation. New York: Norton, 1962. Piaget, J . “Piaget’s Theory.” In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s Manual o f Child

Psychology. Vol. 1. New York: Wiley, 1970. Rosch, E., and Mervis, C. B. “Family Resemblances: Studies in the Internal

Structure of Categories.” Cognitive Psychology, 1975, 7, 573-605. Rubin, S., and Gardner, H. “Once Upon a Time: The Development of Sensitivity

to Story Structure.” In S. Cooper and L. Odell (Eds.), Research on Responses to Literature. Urbana, Ill.: National Council of Teachers in English, forth- coming.

Shotwell, J., Wolf, D., and Gardner, H. “Styles of Achievement in Early Symbol- ization.” In M. Foster and s. Brandes (Eds.), Fundamentals of Symbolization, forthcoming.

Sperber,D. Rethinking Symbolism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1974. Werner, H., and Kaplan, B. Symbol Formation. New York: Wiley, 1963. Wolf, D., and Gardner, H. “Style and Sequence in Early Symbolic Play.” In

M. Franklin and N. Smith (Eds.), Early Symbolization. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, forthcoming.

Howard Gardner is codirector of Harvard Project Zero and a research associate of the Aphasia Research Center of the Boston Veterans Administration Hospital. He is the author of many articles and several books.

Dennie Wolf directs a longitudinal study of early symbolic development at Harvard Project Zero. In addition, she is the coauthor of a recent book on parent and child lifecyles, Ourselves and Our Children (Random House, 1978).