early word learning 1 early word learning and conceptual ... · early word learning 1 early word...

43
Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth to a new thought. 1 Sandra R. Waxman & Erin M. Leddon Northwestern University Synopsis Perhaps more than any other developmental achievement, word-learning stands at the very intersection of language and cognition. Early word-learning represents infants’ entrance into a truly symbolic system and brings with it a means to establish reference. To succeed, infants must identify the relevant linguistic units, identify their corresponding concepts, and establish a mapping between the two. But how do infants begin to map words to concepts, and thus establish their meaning? How do they discover that different types of words (e.g., “dog” (noun), “fluffy” (adjective), “begging” (verb) refer to different aspects of the same scene (e.g, a standard poodle, seated on its hind legs and holding its front paws in the air)? We have proposed that infants begin the task of word-learning with a broad, universal expectation linking novel words to a broad range of commonalities, and that this initial expectation is subsequently fine-tuned on the basis of their experience with the objects and events they encounter and the native language under acquisition. In this chapter, we examine this proposal, in light of recent evidence with infants and young children. Introduction Infants across the world’s communities are exposed to vastly different experiences. Consider, for example, one infant being raised in a remote region of the Guatemalan rainforest, another growing up in the mountains of rural Switzerland, and a third being raised in Brooklyn, NY. Each infant will live in a world that is unimaginable to the other, surrounded by objects and events that are foreign to the other, and immersed in a language that the other cannot begin to understand. Yet despite these vast

Upload: others

Post on 09-Aug-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development:

Everything had a name, and each name gave birth to a new thought.1

Sandra R. Waxman & Erin M. Leddon

Northwestern University

Synopsis

Perhaps more than any other developmental achievement, word-learning stands at the very

intersection of language and cognition. Early word-learning represents infants’ entrance into a truly

symbolic system and brings with it a means to establish reference. To succeed, infants must identify

the relevant linguistic units, identify their corresponding concepts, and establish a mapping between

the two. But how do infants begin to map words to concepts, and thus establish their meaning? How

do they discover that different types of words (e.g., “dog” (noun), “fluffy” (adjective), “begging”

(verb) refer to different aspects of the same scene (e.g, a standard poodle, seated on its hind legs and

holding its front paws in the air)? We have proposed that infants begin the task of word-learning with

a broad, universal expectation linking novel words to a broad range of commonalities, and that this

initial expectation is subsequently fine-tuned on the basis of their experience with the objects and

events they encounter and the native language under acquisition. In this chapter, we examine this

proposal, in light of recent evidence with infants and young children.

Introduction

Infants across the world’s communities are exposed to vastly different experiences. Consider,

for example, one infant being raised in a remote region of the Guatemalan rainforest, another growing

up in the mountains of rural Switzerland, and a third being raised in Brooklyn, NY. Each infant will

live in a world that is unimaginable to the other, surrounded by objects and events that are foreign to

the other, and immersed in a language that the other cannot begin to understand. Yet despite these vast

Page 2: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 2 differences in experience, infants across the world display striking similarities in the most fundamental

aspects of their conceptual and language development.

Within the first year of life, each of these infants will begin to establish systematic links

between words and the concepts to which they refer. On the conceptual side, they will begin to form

categories of objects that capture both the similarities and differences among the objects they

encounter. Most of these early object categories will be at the basic level (i.e., dog) and the more

inclusive global level (i.e., animal). Infants will begin to use these early object categories as an

inductive base to support inferences about new objects that they encounter. They will also begin to

relate categories to one another, implicitly, on the basis of taxonomic (e.g., dogs are a kind of animal),

thematic (e.g., dogs chase tennis balls), functional (e.g., dogs can pull children on sleds) and other

relations. Infants’ early object and event categories will provide a core of conceptual continuity from

infancy through adulthood.

Concurrent with these conceptual advances, infants in each community will make remarkably

rapid strides in language acquisition. Even before they begin to understand the words of their native

language, infants show a special interest in the sounds of language. Newborns respond to the emotional

tone carried by the melody of human speech (Fernald, 1992a, b), and prefer speech sounds to other

forms of auditory input (Jusczyk & Kemler Nelson, 1996; Vouloumanos & Werker, 2004, 2007).

Within the first six months, infants become perceptually attuned to the distinct prosodic, morphologic,

and phonologic elements that characterize their native language (Jusczyk & Kemler Nelson, 1996;

Kemler Nelson, Hirsh-Pasek, Jusczyk, & Cassidy, 1989; Morgan & Demuth, 1996; Shi, Werker, &

Morgan, 1999; Werker, Lloyd, Pegg, & Polka, 1996; see Saffran, Werker, & Werner, 2006 for a

review). By their first birthdays, infants begin to produce their first words. These early words tend to

refer to salient individual objects (e.g., “Mama”), categories of objects (e.g., “cup”, “doggie”), social

routines (e.g., “bye-bye”), and actions (e.g., “up”). Across languages, infants’ earliest lexicons tend to

show a “noun advantage”, with nouns referring to basic-level object categories (e.g., cup, dog) being

Page 3: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 3 the predominant form (see Bornstein, Cote, Maital, Painter, Park, & Pascual, 2004 for an excellent

cross-linguistic developmental review). By their second birthdays, most infants have mastered

hundreds of words of various grammatical forms (e.g., nouns, verbs, adjectives) and have begun to

combine these into short, well-formed phrases that conform broadly to the syntactic properties of their

native language.

This brief sketch illustrates several early milestones along the road of language and conceptual

development. Infants naturally form categories to capture commonalities among objects and events and

learn words to express them. What is perhaps even more intriguing is that these two advances do not

proceed independently. Instead, there are powerful implicit links between them.

Links between early language and conceptual development: A view through the lens of word-

learning

The links between early language and conceptual development are most clearly viewed through

the lens of early word-learning. Word learning supports infants’ subsequent discovery of the

fundamental syntactic properties of the native language (See Gillette, Gleitman, Gleitman, & Lederer,

1999; Snedeker & Gleitman, 1999; Pinker, 1984, 1989; Waxman, 1999a; Waxman, 1999b) as well as

the evolution of increasingly abstract conceptual representations. Moreover, from the onset of

acquisition, the process of word-learning involves powerful, implicit links between the linguistic and

conceptual systems. Even before infants begin to speak, novel words guide their attention to objects,

and highlight commonalities and differences among them (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Booth &

Waxman, 2002; Fulkerson & Haaf, 2003, 2006; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007; Graham, Kilbreath, &

Welder, 2004; Keates & Graham, 2008; Waxman & Markow, 1995; Welder & Graham, 2006; Xu,

1999). But these links are not as precise as those held by older children and adults. How does their

development proceed?

We have proposed that 1) infants across the world’s language communities begin the task of

word learning equipped with an initially general and universal expectation, and 2) this early

Page 4: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 4 expectation is then shaped by the structure of the particular language under acquisition. In our most

recent work, which will be reviewed here, we have sought to uncover the origin and unfolding of these

links (Booth & Waxman, 2003; Waxman & Booth, in press; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007; Klibanoff &

Waxman, 2000; Waxman, 1998; Waxman & Booth, 2000a; Waxman & Booth, 2001b; Waxman, Lidz,

Braun, & Lavin, in press; Waxman & Markow, 1995). To amplify this topic, we will discuss what it

takes to learn a word, to establish a mapping between the linguistic entities that we call words and the

corresponding entities and events in the world. We then go on to trace the origin and emergence of

these links in infants.

What does it take to learn a word?

Perhaps the most celebrated example of word-learning comes from Helen Keller’s

autobiography. As Keller recounts, “...my teacher placed my hand under the spout. As the cool stream

gushed over one hand she spelled into the other the word water, first slowly, then rapidly. I stood still,

my whole attention fixed upon the motions of her fingers. Suddenly I felt a misty consciousness as of

something forgotten – a thrill of returning thought; and somehow the mystery of language was

revealed to me. I knew then that ‘w-a-t-e-r’ meant the wonderful cool something that was flowing

over my hand. That living word awakened my soul, gave it light, hope, joy, and set it free! ... I left the

well-house eager to learn. Everything had a name, and each name gave birth to a new thought.”

(Keller, 1904 p. 22-23)

This memorable passage poignantly conveys the obstacles in first establishing a

correspondence between the abstract entities that we call words and their referents in the world. It also

conveys the power of such word-to-world mappings, once they are attained. But the scenario in this

passage differs in important ways from the more typical circumstances in which infants’ first words are

acquired. One important difference is the age of acquisition. Infants tend to produce their first words at

approximately one year of age. Keller, in contrast, learned water at approximately seven years of age.2

Page 5: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 5 A second difference is the extent to which names are deliberately “taught”. Psycholinguists and

anthropologists have conducted detailed observations of naming practices across cultures. In some

cultures (c.f., Western, college-educated communities), caretakers do name objects deliberately for

their infants, even before the infants themselves can speak. Yet in many other communities, (c.f.,

Kahluli, see Ochs and Schieffelin, 1984), caretakers refrain from speaking directly to infants until the

infants themselves have begun to speak. Clearly, then, infants can discover the meaning of novel words

even in the absence of direct tutoring.

A third relevant difference is in the presentation of the new word. In the typical course of

events, words are seldom, if ever, presented in isolation, as Keller’s tutor presented the word w-a-t-e-r.

Instead, words tend to be embedded in a fluent stream of continuous speech (e.g., “Look at the water!

Oooh…it’s so cold. Isn’t that cold water?”), leaving it to the infant to parse the novel word. How do

they succeed in these cases? At a most general level, it helps that infants devote special attention to

human speech, for this puts them in a good position to begin to single out the novel words (Jusczyk &

Kemler Nelson, 1996). Moreover, in many cultures, caretakers use a special speech register

(sometimes known as infant-directed speech or ‘motherese’) when addressing infants and young

children. Two characteristic features of this speech register -- exaggerated pitch contours and phrase

boundaries – help infants to identify words and phrases in the continuous speech stream (Gleitman &

Wanner, 1988).

A fourth difference concerns the identification of the referent of the novel word. Unlike Keller,

infants must identify the referent of a novel word amidst an ever-changing current of events. In many

cases, the referent may be absent entirely (e.g., “Let’s call Daddy”, uttered as the caretaker picks up a

phone), or may make only a fleeting appearance (e.g., “Look at the monkey”, uttered as a monkey

makes a brief appearance in its habitat at the zoo). And even if the referent is present throughout the

naming episode, there is no guarantee that the infant will be attending to it at the time that the novel

Page 6: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 6 word is introduced (e.g., “Go find your teddy-bear”, uttered as a caretaker tries (in vain) to pull the

infants’ attention away from the sleeping family cat).

The puzzle of word-learning

Thus, in the natural course of word-learning, an infant is faced with a difficult three-part

puzzle: (1) parsing the relevant word from the ongoing stream of speech, (2) identifying the relevant

entity in the ongoing stream of activity in the world, and (3) establishing a word-to-world

correspondence. To put matters more formally, successful word-learning rests on the infant’s ability to

discover the relevant linguistic units, the relevant conceptual units, and the mappings between them.

Notice also that each piece in the word-learning puzzle is itself dependent on infants’ ability to

recruit other perceptual and psychological capacities. Consider, for example, the ability to parse words.

We know that newborns prefer to listen to human speech—and particularly infant-directed speech —as

compared to other sources of auditory stimulation. However, the function of infant-directed speech

appears to change during the first year of life (Fernald, 1992b). Initially, infant-directed speech serves

primarily to engage and modulate the infant’s attention. Toward the end of the first year, “…words

begin to emerge from the melody” (Fernald, 1992a p. 403). By approximately 9 to 10 months, infants

become increasingly sensitive to the cues (morphologic, phonetic and prosodic cues) that mark word

and phrase boundaries (Jusczyk & Aslin, 1995; Kemler Nelson et al., 1989).

Infants’ growing sensitivity to these perceptual cues permit them to distinguish two very broad

classes of words: open class words (or, content words, including nouns, adjectives, verbs) and closed

class words (or, function words, including determiners and prepositions). Research using a preferential

listening task reveals that even 6-month-old infants prefer to listen to open class words (Gomez, 2002;

Shi & Werker, 2003). This preference is likely related to the fact that such words are perceptually more

salient: they receive greater stress and more interesting melodic contours than closed class words.

Since this preference exists well before infants begin to map words systematically to meaning, it is

reasonable to assume that it is perceptually based and independent of meaning. Yet this perceptually-

Page 7: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 7 based preference represents an important step on the way to word-learning, for it insures that infants

attend to just those words (the open class, content words) that are required if they are to anchor their

first word-to-world mappings. (Jusczyk & Kemler Nelson, 1996; Morgan & Demuth, 1996; Werker et

al., 1996).

Early word-learning also draws upon the infants’ perceptual and conceptual ability to identify

objects and events in their environment, and to notice commonalities among them. During the first

year, infants demonstrate a great deal of core knowledge about objects (Baillargeon, 2000; Spelke,

2000). They also form a repertoire of pre-linguistic concepts, including category-based (e.g., dog,

bottle) and property-based (e.g., red, soft) commonalities. Since many of these concepts are formed

before the advent of word-learning, it is reasonable to assume that they are independent of language

and are universally available. Each object and concept is, in essence, a candidate for a word’s meaning.

The infants’ task is to discover which candidate meaning maps to the word that they have parsed.

The third piece of the word-learning puzzle—grasping the symbolic and referential power of

words—further requires infants to draw upon fundamental notions related to human behavior: inferring

the goals and intentions of others (Waxman & Gelman, in press). For example, the ability to establish a

mapping between a word and its referent is predicated upon infants’ capacity to infer that the speaker

intended to name the designated object. By the end of the first year of life, infants have begun to make

such connections (Ahktar & Tomasello, 2000); for example, they spontaneously follow a speaker’s

line of regard to identify the object to which an adult speaker is attending (Baldwin & Baird, 1999;

Guajardo & Woodward, 2000).

In addition to these three central elements, successful word-learning requires infants to go

beyond a word-to-object mapping. To use a word consistently over time, infants must be able to store

in memory the correspondence between a word and its intended referent. They must also be able to

generalize a newly learned word appropriately beyond the individual on which it was taught. For

example, to be able to apply the word dog to a new, and (as yet) unlabeled object, that child must make

Page 8: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 8 an inference regarding its extension. Infants’ spontaneous extensions indicate that they do not merely

map words to the objects on which they were introduced. Infants go beyond word-to-object mappings

to establish word-to-category mappings (Gelman, 2006; Waxman & Lidz, 2006).

Different kinds of words highlight different aspects of a scene

To complicate matters further, many different words -- indeed many different types of words –

may be offered in a single naming episode. Importantly, each type of word highlights a different aspect

of the same observed scene and supports a unique pattern of extension. For example, in English, count

nouns (“Look, it’s a dog”) typically refer to the named object itself and are extended spontaneously to

other members of the same object kind (other dogs); proper nouns (“Look, it’s Zeus”) also refer to the

named individual, but these are not extended further. Adjectives can also be applied correctly to that

individual (“Look, it’s furry”); they refer to a property of the individual, and are extended to other

objects sharing that property. Verbs, in contrast, are used to describe the event, or the relation in which

the individual(s) are participating (“Look, it’s running”), and are extended to other relations of the

same type.

Considerable research has documented that by two-and-a-half to three years of age, children are

sensitive to many of these links between kinds of words and kinds of relations among objects, and

recruit these links in the process of word learning (For a review, see Waxman & Lidz, 2006). This

establishes that preschool-aged children have the linguistic capacity to distinguish among the relevant

syntactic forms (e.g., count noun vs. adjective) and the conceptual or perceptual ability to appreciate

many different kinds of relations among objects, and a tacit expectation that these linguistic and

conceptual abilities are interwoven.

The proposal: The acquisition of word-to world links

But how do infants acquire these specific word-to-world links? Which, if any, are available at

the very onset of lexical acquisition, and how are these shaped over the course of development? The

evidence discussed above suggests that infants begin with a perceptual preference for listening to open

Page 9: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 9 class words, with a repertoire of accessible perceptual and conceptual categories, and with a broad

expectation that novel (open class) words, independent of their grammatical form, highlight

commonalities among named objects. This initial link serves (at least) three essential functions. First,

with words directing attention to commonalities, this link facilitates the formation of an expanding

repertoire of concepts, concepts that may not have been detected in the absence of a novel word.

Second, this initial expectation supports infants’ first efforts to establish symbolic reference, to form a

set of stable ‘word-to-world’ mappings. Finally, and perhaps most radically, this initial expectation sets

the stage for the evolution of the more specific expectations linking particular types of words (nouns,

adjectives, verbs) to particular types of relations among objects (object categories, object properties,

event categories) in the native language under acquisition (Waxman, 1999b).

How might this evolution come about? Infants’ early expectation (that words refer to

commonalities) supports the establishment of a rudimentary lexicon. This lexicon serves as a base

upon which infants a) begin to tease apart the various grammatical forms presented in the language

under acquisition, and b) begin to detect the correlations between these emerging forms and their

meanings. We argue that infants’ initial expectation (linking words in general) to commonalities (in

general) will direct their attention to just the sorts of regularities in the input that will promote the rapid

discovery of the distinct grammatical forms present in the language under acquisition, and will support

the induction of more specific expectations.

The evidence: The evolution of infants’ word-to world expectations

To test this proposal, we must identify the expectation(s) of infants on the threshold of word-

learning, and observe how these are shaped in the course of acquiring their native language. The

sections that follow describe the influence of language on categorization in infants and young children.

The focus is on how infants and young children begin to map words to meaning, and how their

expectations vary according to the word’s grammatical category, be it noun, adjective, or verb. We

Page 10: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 10 examine the acquisition of words in each of these grammatical categories, considering evidence from a

variety of measures, including time-course analyses of children’s responses to a novel word. We

conclude with a discussion of new avenues for research on word learning.

Unifying features of the experiments

The experiments described in this section all share several important features. Each is

essentially a categorization task, tailored to suit the very different behavioral repertoires of infants vs.

young children. In each, the goal is to observe the influence of language on categorization. To do so,

we compare subjects’ performance in "neutral" conditions (involving no novel words), with their

performance when they are introduced to novel words. Because our goal is to examine an abstract

linkage between particular grammatical forms and particular types of relations, we introduce novel

words (e.g., blicket), rather than familiar (e.g., animal) words. This insures that the words themselves

carry no a priori meaning for the child. To examine the influence of grammatical form, we vary the

frame in which the novel word is embedded. We use short, simple syntactic constructions that 1) are

typical in infant- and child-directed speech, and that 2) provide unambiguous contextual evidence that

the novel word is either a count noun, an adjective, or a verb. In all conditions, these constructions are

presented to infants in a speech register known as “motherese” or infant-directed speech because

infants find it especially engaging. In the Novel Noun conditions, we introduce the novel word, saying,

for example, "This is a blicket." In the Novel Adjective and Novel Verb conditions, we present the

same word using a different frame, saying, for example, “This is a blick-ish one," or “She is blicking

the cup.” In the No Word control conditions, we engage the infants with infant-directed speech, bur

offer no novel word, simply saying, for example, "Do you like this?” or “Look at this". Performance

in this No Word control condition assesses how readily subjects form the various categories presented

in our tasks (e.g., dog, animal, purple, running). Performance in the Noun, Adjective and Verb

conditions assesses the role of naming in this important endeavor, and permit us to test the specificity

Page 11: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 11 of the relation between form and meaning. Because nouns, adjectives, and verbs alike can be used to

describe different aspects of the very same scene, this is an important control.

Naming and categorization

Prelinguistic infants: Cognitive consequences of naming. Recent research suggests that naming

has several cognitive consequences, even for infants who have not yet begun to produce words on their

own. For example, by 10 months, infants devote more attention to objects that have been named than

to objects that have been presented in silence (Baldwin & Markman, 1989). This raises two further

questions: First, does the increased attention stem from the general attention-engaging functions of

auditory stimuli, or does it reflect something special about words? Second, does naming promote

attention to a named individual only, or does it exert an influence beyond the named individual?

Several recent studies have compared the effect of novel words versus tone sequences on

infants’ categorization behavior, and these suggest that words are indeed special (Balaban & Waxman,

1997; Fulkerson & Haaf, 2003, 2006; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007). In one study, Fulkerson and

Waxman (2007) assessed 6-month-old infants’ performance when presented with words versus tones

in a novelty-preference task. During a familiarization phase, infants saw a sequence of colorful slides,

each depicting a different member of a basic-level category (e.g., dinosaurs). To examine the influence

of words, they randomly assigned infants to either a Word or a Tone condition. For infants in the Word

condition, a naming phrase (e.g., “Oh look, it’s a toma! Do you see the toma?”) accompanied the

familiarization trials. For infants in the Tone condition, a sine-wave tone (matched to the naming

phrase in amplitude, duration, and pause length) accompanied the familiarization trials. Next, infants

were presented with a test trial, including a) a new member of the now-familiar category (e.g., another

dinosaur) and b) an object from a novel category (e.g., a fish). Test trials were presented in silence.

The authors reasoned that if words focus attention on commonalities among objects, then

infants hearing words during familiarization should notice the commonalities among the

familiarization objects. In that case, the infants should reveal a preference for the novel test object

Page 12: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 12 (e.g., the fish). If this effect is specific to words, and not to auditory stimulation more generally, then

infants hearing tones during familiarization should be less likely to notice these commonalities and less

likely to reveal a novelty preference at test. This is precisely the pattern of results obtained. We can

therefore conclude that for infants as young as 6 months of age, there is indeed something special

about words, and that providing a shared name for distinct individuals highlights commonalities among

them.

In addition to supporting object categories, naming supports the process of object individuation.

Evidence suggests that 10-month-olds find it difficult to keep track of the unique identities of two

distinct objects (e.g., a ball and a duck), especially if these objects are presented in constant motion,

with one appearing and disappearing from one side of a screen, and the other appearing and

disappearing from the other side of the same screen (Xu & Carey, 1996). However, infants’ difficulty

tracking these distinct objects diminishes dramatically if each is labeled with a distinct name as it

emerges from behind the screen.

Together, these results reveal that naming has powerful cognitive consequences, even in pre-

linguistic infants. Naming supports the establishment of a repertoire of object categories and provides

infants with a means of tracing the identity of individuals within these categories throughout

development. These links appear before the advent of productive language.

Infants on the threshold of word learning: Changing expectations of word-to-world mappings.

In the previous section, we established that words have a unique influence on prelinguistic infants’

construals of individual objects and object categories. In this section, we trace the evolution of infants’

expectations regarding word-to-world mappings as they cross the important developmental threshold

of producing words on their own. At this developmental point, how do words influence infants’

attention to individual objects and object categories?

Words as invitations to form categories. One series of experiments examined the influence of

novel words on object categorization in 12- to 14-month old infants (see Waxman & Markow, 1995 for

Page 13: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 13 a complete description). We used a novelty-preference task (See Figure 1 for a sample set of stimuli

and introductory phrases). During a familiarization phase, an experimenter offered an infant four

different toys from a given category (e.g., four animals) one at a time, in random order. This was

immediately followed by a test phase, in which the experimenter simultaneously presented both a) a

new member of the now-familiar category (e.g., another animal) and b) an object from a novel

category (e.g., a fruit). Each infant completed this task with four different sets of objects. Two

involved basic level categories (e.g., horses vs. cats); two involved more abstract superordinate level

categories (e.g., animals vs. fruit). Infants manipulated the toys freely. Their manipulation served as

the dependent measure.

To test the influence of novel words, we randomly assigned infants to one of three conditions.

All infants heard infant-directed speech; what varied was the experimenter's comments during

familiarization. See Figure 1.

--------------------------

Insert Figure 1 here

--------------------------

We reasoned as follows: If infants detect the presence of the novel word, and if novel words

direct infants' attention to object categories, then infants who hear novel words during familiarization

should be more likely than those in the No Word condition to form object categories. Including both a

Novel Noun and Novel Adjective condition permitted us to test the specificity of infants’ initial

expectation. If the expectation is initially general, as we have proposed, then infants hearing either

novel nouns or adjectives should be more likely than those hearing no novel words to form object

categories.

For infants who had begun to produce words on their own, the data were entirely consistent

with this prediction. Interestingly, the facilitative influence of novel words was most powerful on

superordinate level trials. On basic level trials, all infants successfully formed categories. But on

Page 14: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 14 superordinate trials, infants in the No Word condition did not detect the commonalities. This difficulty

is likely due to the fact that there is considerable variation among category members at superordinate

levels, and as a result, the commonalities among them can be difficult to trace. However, infants who

heard novel words during familiarization (either count nouns or adjectives) detected the commonalities

among objects and successfully formed superordinate level object categories.

These results reveal that infants on the threshold of producing language can reliably detect

novel words presented in fluent speech, and that these novel words (both adjectives and nouns) direct

infants' attention to commonalities among objects. We have interpreted this finding as evidence that

words serve as “invitations to form categories” and have pointed out that this invitation has several

consequences. First, novel words invite infants to assemble together objects that might otherwise be

perceived as disparate entities. We suggest that words promote comparison among objects, and that

this process of comparison supports the discovery of other commonalities that might otherwise have

gone unnoticed (Booth, 2006; Chambers, Graham, & Turner, 2008; Gelman, 2006; Gentner & Namy,

1999; Keates & Graham, 2008; Welder & Graham, 2006).

Naming may also have dramatic consequences in situations in which infants have already

formed groupings and noticed (some of) the commonalities among objects. For example, although

infants in this series successfully formed basic level object categories (whether or not they were

introduced to novel words), their knowledge about these categories is not on a par with the knowledge

of an older child or adult. Even preschool-aged children lack detailed knowledge about most categories

(Gelman, 1996; Keil, 1994). Nonetheless, despite their relative lack of information, children seem to

expect that members of object categories share deep, non-obvious commonalities. Indeed, children

depend upon these to support inference and induction. We suspect that novel words are instrumental in

motivating infants and young children to discover the deeper commonalities that underlie our richly-

structured object categories (Ahn & Luhmann, 2004; Barsalou, 1983; Barsalou, Santos, Simmons, &

Wilson, 2008; Gelman, 1996; Gelman, Coley, & Gottfried, 1994; Gelman & Kalish, 2006; Gelman &

Page 15: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 15 Medin, 1993; Kalish & Gelman, 1992; Keil, 1994; Landau, 1994; Landau, Smith, & Jones, 1988;

Lassaline & Murphy, 1996; Macnamara, 1994; Markman, 1989; Medin & Heit, 1999; Murphy 2004).

Most importantly, the results of this series of experiments document that a link between word-learning

and conceptual organization is in place early enough to guide infants in their very first efforts to

establish word-to-world mappings.

Specifying the scope of infants’ word-to-world mappings. Having shown that words promote

the formation of object categories, our next goal was to capture more precisely the scope, power and

evolution of infants’ expectations in word-learning (Booth & Waxman, 2003; Waxman & Booth,

2001b, 2003). To do so, we extended the work described in the previous section in four ways. First,

we included a developmental component, comparing the performance of 11-month-old infants on the

very threshold of producing words with that of 14-month-olds whose lexicons already include a

modest set of entries. Second, we expanded the range of commonalities under investigation. In the

previously-described studies (Waxman & Markow, 1995), the only commonality among objects was

category-based (e.g., four animals, all of a different color). Here, we ask whether infants link novel

words specifically to category-based commonalities (e.g., animal), or whether they also link words to a

wider range of groupings including, for example, property-based commonalities (e.g., pink things,

lumpy things) (Waxman, 1999).3 Third, we considered the influence not only of nouns, but also of

adjectives, on infants’ categorization behavior.

A fourth goal in this series was methodological. All of the evidence reviewed thus far has been

based entirely on infants’ performance in novelty-preference tasks. In the current series, we asked

whether infants’ expectations would influence performance in a word extension task. Our goal here

was to bridge a methodological gap between research with infants and preschoolers. Novelty-

preference tasks have been successful with infants, but beyond 18 months of age, infants lose interest

in such tasks. Word-extension tasks have been successful with toddlers and preschoolers, but lack

Page 16: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 16 sensitivity with infants under 18-months, who have difficulty choosing systematically among objects

in forced-choice tasks.

To bridge this methodological gap, we developed a new method, which weds features of the

novelty-preference procedure with those of the word-extension paradigms. See Figure 2 for a

schematic description of the procedure and a summary of the instructions presented in each condition.

--------------------------

Insert Figure 2 here

--------------------------

In the Familiarization phase, an experimenter introduced infants in all conditions to four

objects, all drawn from the same object category (e.g., horses or animals) and embodying the same

object property (e.g., purple). These were presented in pairs, and infants manipulated them freely.

During the Contrast phase, the experimenter presented a new object (e.g., a brown rolling-pin), drawn

from a contrastive object category and embodying a contrastive object property. In the Test phase,

infants were presented with one familiar object (e.g., a purple horse), and one novel object. For half of

the infants in each condition (see below), this novel object was a member of a novel object category,

but embodied the same property (e.g., a purple plate). This constituted a Category test. For the

remaining infants, the novel object was a member of the same category as the familiarization objects,

but embodied a novel property (e.g., a blue horse). This constituted a Property test. Infants were first

permitted to play freely with the two test objects. Then, to assess word-extension, the experimenter

removed the test objects. At this point, she introduced a target object, drawn from the familiarization

set (e.g., a purple horse) and then re-presented the two test objects, asking the infant to give her one.

(See Figure 2). This word-extension task was presented a second time for each set of familiarization

objects. This permitted us to observe the consistency of infants’ responses. Infants completed this

entire procedure four times, with four different sets of objects, two representing basic level object

categories and two representing superordinate level categories.

Page 17: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 17 To trace the proposed developmental trajectory from an initially general expectation linking

open class words (either count nouns or adjectives) to commonalities among objects (either category-

based or property-based) to a more specific set of expectations, we compared the influence of novel

nouns and adjectives in the performance of infants at 11 and 14 months of age. If infants begin the

process of lexical acquisition with a broad expectation linking words (in general) to commonalities

among objects (in general), then for 11-month-olds, both nouns and adjectives should highlight both

category-based (e.g., animal) and property-based (e.g., purple things) commonalities. If this initial

expectation is refined once the process of lexical acquisition is under way, then for more advanced

learners, a different pattern should emerge: Different kinds of words should direct older infants’

attention to different aspects of the same experience: naming objects (with either a count noun or

adjective) should systematically influence infants’ construals of the very same set of objects (e.g.,

purple animals) either as members of an object category (animals) or as embodying an object

property (purple). Based on our previous work (Waxman, 1999b; Waxman & Booth, 2000a), we

expected that at 14 months, infants would have begun to distinguish count nouns (from among the

other grammatical forms) and to map these specifically to category-based, but not property-based,

commonalities. We expected that at this same developmental moment, infants’ expectations for

adjectives would still be quite general, directing their attention more broadly toward commonalities (be

they category- or property-based).

The results were consistent with these predictions. At 11 months, infants hearing novel words

(both nouns and adjectives) performed differently than those in the No Word condition. Infants

extended both novel nouns and adjectives consistently to the familiar test object (e.g., the purple horse)

on both Category and Property trials. This confirms that at the very onset of building a lexicon, (a)

novel (open class) words direct infants’ attention broadly to both category- and property-based

commonalities among named objects, and (b) this link is sufficiently strong to support the extension of

novel words. This outcome provides strong support for our proposal that infants on the very threshold

Page 18: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 18 of word-learning harbor a general expectation linking words (both nouns and adjectives) to

commonalities (both category- and property-based) among objects.

We also proposed that once word-learning is underway, a more specific pattern should emerge.

Evidence from 14-month-olds offers support for this developmental prediction. By 14 months, infants

were sensitive to the distinction between novel words presented as nouns as compared to adjectives,

and they recruited this distinction in mapping words to their meaning. They mapped nouns to category-

based, rather than to property-based, commonalities. However, their expectations regarding the

extension of novel adjectives were more general. Infants hearing adjectives were equally likely to

select the familiar object on both types of test trials (see Waxman, 1999b; Waxman & Booth, 2000a

for replications and extensions using various properties (e.g., color, texture)). Clearly, by 14 months,

infants have begun to distinguish among different kinds of words (nouns vs. adjectives) and recruit this

distinction in the service of mapping words to their meaning.

Additional support for this developmental trajectory comes from a recent study using a

precisely controlled automated method (Booth & Waxman, 2009). This method offers two advantages

over the studies described above, which all involved direct interaction between an infant and

experimenter: 1) it allows for tight control over the presentation of stimuli, and 2) it permits us to trace

the time course of children’s mapping of novel words to meaning (Fernald, Swingley, & Pinto, 2001;

Fernald, Pinto, Swingley, Weinberg, & McRoberts, 1998; Golinkoff, Hirsh-Pasek, Cauley, & Gordon,

1987; Swingley, Pinto, & Fernald, 1999). In this study, 14- and 18-month-olds watched a video that

mirrored the structure of the earlier studies. Their response was measured by examining the infants’

eye movements during the test period. Examining which of two images the infant paid more attention

to during this period provided an indirect measure of how they extended the novel noun or adjective.

Once again, infants at both ages in this study extended novel nouns to other members of the

same category, as opposed to items sharing the same property. Moreover, the time-course of their

extension was swift, occurring within two seconds of hearing the novel word. When the same words

Page 19: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 19 were presented as adjectives, infants revealed no preference for either category- or property-based

extensions. This finding lines up with the results of previous studies, which suggest the adjective-

property link may begin to emerge just after 18 months, or during the 18- to 21-month time frame

(Waxman & Markow, 1998).

The convergence between performance in this automated procedure and in the live tasks is

striking. Taken together, they show that clearly, 14-month-olds are sensitive to (at least some of) the

relevant cues (e.g., prosody, morphology, structural position within a phrase) that distinguish count

nouns from adjectives. Cues like these are sufficiently rich to support an emerging distinction among

major grammatical forms (Morgan & Demuth, 1996). Although infants’ grammatical distinctions are

certainly not as well-honed as those of adults, and although their knowledge of categories and

properties of objects is certainly not as rich, 14-month-olds do appear to share with adults an

expectation that different types of words (count nouns versus adjectives) refer to different types of

relations among objects.

Talking two-year-olds: beyond nouns and adjectives. Having examined the trajectory of

infants’ mapping of novel nouns and adjectives, we turn our attention now to the acquisition of verbs.

Verbs do not appear in appreciable number in infants’ productive lexicons until several months after

the appearance of nouns (between roughly 20 - 24 months) (For recent reviews, see Gleitman, Cassidy,

Nappa, Papafragou and Trueswell, 2005; Waxman and Lidz, 2006). This developmental phenomenon,

favoring the acquisition of nouns over verbs, suggests that these distinct grammatical forms differ not

only in the kinds of meanings they convey, but also in their underlying course of acquisition. In the

next series, we look at what it takes to learn a verb.

The conceptual underpinnings of verbs seem to be in place, at least in rudimentary ways, by the

end of the first year of life. By 8 – 12 months, infants are sensitive to certain fundamental components

of events, including animacy, agency, and cause (Buresh, Wilson-Brune & Woodward, 2006; Casasola

& Cohen, 2000; Gergely & Csibra, 2003; Gergely, Nádasdy, Csibra, & Bíró,1995; Gertner, Fisher, &

Page 20: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 20 Eisengart, 2006; Golinkoff & Hirsch-Pasek, 2006; Leslie & Keeble, 1987; Meltzoff, 2007; Muentener

& Carey, 2006; Sommerville, Woodward, & Needham 2005; Wagner & Carey, 2005). Between 12 and

24 months, infants demonstrate sensitivity to other key elements of events, including changes of state,

result, manner and path of motion (Bunger, 2007; Bunger & Lidz, 2004; Pruden, Hirsh-Pasek,

Maguire, & Meyer, 2004; Pulverman, Hirsh-Pasek, Pruden, & Golinkoff, 2006).

What this means is that infants’ relatively delayed acquisition of verbs must not be due to an

inability to represent the kinds of concepts that underlie verb meaning, but rather to other factors.

What might these factors be?

One possibility is that infants’ relative delay in acquiring verbs reflects the fact that the

meaning of a verb depends upon the arguments that it takes (and the relation among them). Simply put,

to identify the event labeled by a verb, learners depend upon the noun phrases that represent the event

participants and the linguistic relations among these phrases (Fisher, Hall, Rakowitz, & Gleitman,

1994; Gillette, Gleitman, Gleitman & Lederer, 1999; Gleitman, et al., 2005; Landau & Gleitman, 1985;

Lidz, Gleitman & Gleitman, 2003; Piccin & Waxman, 2007; Snedeker & Gleitman, 2004; Waxman &

Lidz, 2006). Therefore, without the nouns, it should be difficult for learners to identify the arguments

of a verb and impossible to identify the event labeled by the verb in that context. The acquisition of

verbs must therefore follow the acquisition of nouns.

This trajectory is supported by studies of toddlers’ vocabularies. Productive use of verbs to

refer to actions, mental states, and relations is often not established until 24 months, well after the

productive use of nouns (Fenson, Bates, Dale, Goodman, Reznick, & Thal, 2000). Around this age,

not only do toddlers produce verbs, but they seem to map novel verbs onto categories of events, taking

into account syntactic information, including the number and types of frames in which novel verbs

appear and the relations among the noun phrases in these frames, to narrow their hypotheses about

possible verb meanings (Akhtar & Tomasello, 1996; Bunger & Lidz, 2004; Fernandes, Marcus,

DiNubila, & Vouloumanos, 2006; Fisher, 2002; Gertner, Fisher, & Eisengart, 2006; Gleitman, 1990;

Page 21: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 21 Gleitman et al., 2005; Hirsch-Pasek, Golinkoff, & Naigles, 1996; Landau & Gleitman, 1985; Naigles,

1990, 1996; Naigles & Kako, 1993).

Nonetheless, the verb learning literature also reveals some astonishing failures, many of which

persist throughout the preschool years (Imai, Haryu, & Okada, 2005; Imai, et al., 2008; see also

Behrend, 1990; Kersten & Smith, 2002; Meyer et al., 2003). A review of the evidence suggests that

infants and young children succeed in verb-learning when the very same participant objects are present

in all the events, but encounter difficulty when there is a change in the event participants. This

suggests that infants and even young children are essentially “captured” by the participant objects and

have difficulty extending verbs beyond them.

Interestingly, infants appear to be captured by participant objects in laboratory tasks, but not in

real-world acquisition. Children are often observed extending verbs like “drink” to different beverages

(e.g., milk, water), “run” to different actors (e.g., dogs, people), etc. outside of the laboratory. How

can we account for this mismatch? What information is missing from laboratory tasks that hampers

children’s ability to learn novel verbs?

We examined infant verb-learning using the automated procedure described above (Waxman,

Lidz, Braun, & Lavin, 2009). Videos of dynamic scenes (e.g., a man waving a balloon) were presented

to 24-month-old infants. The study examined whether infants could construe these scenes flexibly,

noticing the consistent action (e.g., waving) as well as the consistent object (e.g., the balloon) and b)

whether their construals of the scenes were influenced by the grammatical form of a novel word used

to describe them (verb or noun). Infants were presented with the scenes in a Familiarization phase,

where the accompanying audio varied as function of condition. Afterward, during the Test phase,

infants saw two test scenes, presented simultaneously on either side of the screen. Both scenes featured

the same actor (e.g., the man) and the same object (e.g., the balloon) as in familiarization; what varied

was the event in which these two were involved. In the familiar test scene, the man performed the now-

Page 22: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 22 familiar action (e.g., man waving a balloon); in the novel test scene, he performed a novel action (e.g.,

man tapping a balloon). See Figure 3 for the audio presented in each phase of each condition.

Infants’ visual attention during each frame of the Test phase was analyzed, measuring attention

to the novel event (e.g. a man tapping a balloon) versus the familiar event (e.g. a man waving a

balloon). These time-course results revealed that infants were sensitive to the consistent events shown

during familiarization, as they exhibited a strong novelty preference (directing their attention to the

novel event) before hearing the test sentence that varied by condition. Once the infants heard the test

sentence (e.g., “Where’s larping?), their attention began to diverge. Children in the Verb condition

reliably directed their attention back toward the familiar event, or the event labeled with the novel verb

during familiarization (e.g. waving a balloon). Children in the Noun and No Word conditions

maintained their visual attention on the novel event. Note that both the novel and familiar events

included a balloon, making them both appropriate responses for the Noun condition. However, a

subsequent experiment using the same Familiarizaton phase, but changing the Test phase to include

events with the same action but different objects (e.g., waving a balloon versus waving a rake)

revealed that children could indeed appropriately extend a novel noun and direct their attention back to

the familiar object.

These results suggest that 24-month-olds’ representations of novel words are sufficiently

precise to permit them to map novel verbs to event categories (e.g., waving events) and novel nouns to

object categories (e.g., balloons). Once again the time-course of their mapping is swift, occurring

within about a second of hearing the novel word. These results beckon us to move beyond asking

whether or not infants can represent verb meanings, and to consider instead the conditions that support

their successful acquisition.

Avenues for future research

Page 23: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 23 The evidence discussed above has significantly advanced our understanding of children’s early

word learning, while simultaneously prompting myriad avenues for future research. Among them are

questions concerning how word learning proceeds in bilingual children, and what neuropsychological

studies can contribute to theories of word learning. We consider each of these in turn below.

Word-learning in bilingual environments. The previous sections highlight the many challenges word-

learning presents to infants and young children. The task for bilingual children is even more complex, as

they grapple with solving the puzzle of word-learning in more than one language. Nevertheless, the time-

course of bilingual children’s acquisition is similar to that of monolinguals (Holowka, Brosseau-Lapre, &

Petitto, 2002; Oller, Eilers, Urbano, & Cobo-Lewis, 1997; Pearson, Fernandez, & Oller, 1993), and their

early words and sentences closely resemble those of monolingual infants (Yip & Matthews, 2007).

Monolingual and bilingual word learning nonetheless differs in important ways (see Fernald,

2006; Werker & Byers-Heinlein, 2008 for recent reviews). Bilingual infants must separate the

linguistic input that they receive from their two languages, forming two distinct phonological systems

and two lexicons. An active area of research is the way the two languages might be represented and

processed in the brain. A recent neuropsychological study examined brain responses in 19- and 22-

month-old bilingual infants as they listened to known and unknown words in both of their languages

(Conboy & Mills, 2006). Differences were found not only the infant brain responses to known and

unknown words, but also in the infant brain responses to the two languages. Moreover, the timing and

distribution of brain activity linked to word-meaning differed for children’s dominant vs. non-

dominant language. These differences most likely reflect differences in the infants’ experience with

the two languages, rather than a general brain maturational mechanism.

The neuroscience of word-learning. Other recent neuropsychological studies contribute to our

understanding of the neural processes underlying word-learning in both bilingual and monolingual

environments, and support the behavioral findings detailed above in two main ways. First,

neuropsychological evidence shows that the infant brain responds to speech in a special way, distinct

Page 24: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 24 from other auditory signals. This dovetails well with evidence that infants map words (but not non-

speech stimuli like tones) to meaning (Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007). Second, there is evidence that the

infant brain responds in a specific way not only to speech sounds in general, but also to words in

particular. This specific response to the presentation of words provides the first neurological correlates

of word learning.

The evidence that even newborn infants process speech stimuli in a way that is distinct from

non-speech stimuli suggests that the human brain comes prepared to process human speech. In

particular, areas of activation observed in the newborn brain bear a striking resemblance to those for

adults. Two recent studies examined areas of brain activation in neonates (Pena, et al, 2003) and 3-

month-olds (Dehaene-Lambertz et al, 2002). These studies revealed that even in neonates there is a left

hemisphere bias when listening to language, a bias that is not observed when listening to backwards

speech or silence. Moreover, this left hemisphere bias was shown to emerge even when subjects were

presented with a language other than their own (Hespos, Ferry, Cannistraci, Gore, & Park, 2009;

Hespos, Park, Ferry, Lane, & Gore, 2009).

Recent work also shows that infants go beyond merely processing speech sounds in speech-

specific ways, but that they can also detect speech patterns, like the repetition of syllables (Gervain, et

al, 2008). Different patterns of brain responses emerged when neonates were presented with blocks of

syllables with immediate repetition (e.g., ABB) vs. no repetition. This indicates that the infant brain is

sensitive to speech structure, and suggests at least one way in which neuropsychological capacities

support the acquisition of words.

Beyond speech processing, neurospsychological research on word-learning primarily consists

of ERP studies that use the N400 as a method of testing children’s knowledge. The N400 is an ERP

response that is associated, in adults, with semantic integration. It is consistently observed after an

unexpected stimulus, as when a picture is paired with a mismatching names (e.g., pairing the word

Page 25: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 25 “dog” with a picture of a banana). (See Werker & Yeung, 2005 for a terrific overview of ERP

correlates to word learning)

The N400 response has been observed in several studies of infants and toddlers. In one study,

19-month-olds were presented with known words that either matched or did not match a sequentially

presented picture (Friedrich & Friederici, 2004). The words referred to the basic level name of the

object or an incongruent basic level name. As expected, the N400 emerged when the object names did

not match the object pictures. A later study obtained this same result with 14-month-olds (Friedrich &

Friederici, 2005). Studies featuring novel words have also revealed the N400 in infants and young

children (Mills, Plunkett, Prat, & Schafer 2005; von Koss Torkildsen et al., 2008).

In conclusion, the neuropsychological evidence converges well with the behavioral evidence on

word-learning. Recall that behavioral studies reveal that by 14 months, infants have mapped several

nouns to objects and object kinds. Correspondingly, neuropsychological studies show that when a

mismatch occurs between a known noun and picture of an object, the 14-month-old infant brain

‘notices’ the mismatch. As research in this field progresses, it will be interesting to see if

neuropsychological correlates are also evident in the infant brain’s response to words from other

grammatical categories, including adjectives and verbs.

Conclusions

In this chapter, we have focused on word-learning, asking what expectations, if any, infants

recruit in the process of establishing their first word-to-world mappings, and how these evolve over

development. We suggested that infants begin the task of word-learning equipped with a broad, initial,

and universally-available expectation that links novel open class words (independent of their

grammatical form) to a wide range of commonalities among named objects. We suggested that this

initially general expectation sets the stage for the evolution of more specific expectations, calibrated in

Page 26: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 26 accordance with the correlations between particular grammatical forms and their associated meanings

in the language under acquisition.

Infants’ performance provided clear support for this proposal. At 11 months, infants revealed a

broad initial expectation, linking words (count nouns and adjectives) to commonalities (both category-

and property-based) among named objects. By 14 months, infants’ expectations have indeed become

more fine-tuned. They distinguish nouns from adjectives, and treat this distinction as relevant when

mapping words to their meaning (mapping count nouns specifically to category-based, but not

property-based, commonalities among objects). Specific links between adjectives and properties of

objects continue to emerge toward the end of the second year of life. By this age, infants also begin to

map verbs to commonalities among events as well.

These results point to substantial continuity across development in the types of concepts we

form, and in the influence of naming in their acquisition. We see this line of work as providing

evidence that words are powerful engines for conceptual development: words advance us beyond our

initial groupings, fueling the acquisition of the rich relations that characterize our most powerful

concepts.

Finally, this developmental account has several distinct strengths. First, it embraces both the

importance of the expectations imposed by the learner, as well as the shaping role of the environment.

In the case of word-learning, this interplay between factors inherent in the child and factors within the

environment is essential. Infants across the world will encounter different objects, will acquire

different languages, and will be provided with different types of language input (Cole, Gay, Glick, &

Sharp, 1971; Laboratory of Comparitive Human Cognition, 1983).

Moreover, under our proposal, early acquisition is sufficiently constrained to permit infants to

form fundamental categories of objects and to learn the words to express them, and sufficiently flexible

to accommodate the systematic variations in the word-to-world mappings that occur across languages.

Notice that our view of constraints on acquisition is not an argument based entirely on assumptions of

Page 27: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 27 innate knowledge. Neither is it a polarized argument that locates the engine of acquisition solely within

the mind of the child. Rather, the idea is that in the process of word learning, infants direct their

attention toward precisely the sort of information and precisely the kinds of regularities in the

environment that will support foundational concepts and the acquisition of words to describe them.

Our proposal is flexible enough to account for the fact that infants naturally acquire a wide range of

human languages, and that these differ in the ways in which they recruit the particular grammatical

forms to convey particular types of meaning. We have suggested that the specific links between

particular grammatical forms and their associated meanings are calibrated on the basis of correlations

or regularities that are present in the language under acquisition. It therefore stands to reason that these

more specific links would not be available at the onset of word-learning, but instead would emerge

later, once the process of lexical acquisition is underway.

Finally, our proposal is a dynamic one: The initial expectations that we observe in infants at the

outset of acquisition are not rigidly fixed, exerting a uniform influence throughout development. On

the contrary, these expectations are shaped over the course of development in accordance with the

observed regularities in the language under acquisition.

Page 28: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 28 Literature Cited

Ahn, W., & Luhmann, C. C. (2004). Demystifying theory-based categorization. In L.

Gershkoff-Stowe & D. Rakison (Eds.) Building object categories in developmental time (pp. 277-300).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Akhtar, N. & Tomasello, M. (1996). Two-year-olds learn words for absent objects and actions.

British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 79-93.

Akhtar, N. & Tomasello, M. (2000). The social nature of words and word learning. In M.

Marschark (series ed.), Counterpoints: Cognition, memory, and language. Becoming a word learner: A

debate on lexical acquisition, 115–35. New York: Oxford University Press.

Baillargeon, R. (2000). How do infants learn about the physical world? In D. Muir & A. Slater

(Eds.), Infant development: The essential readings. Essential readings in development psychology (pp.

195-212). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers, Inc.

Balaban, M. T., & Waxman, S. R. (1997). Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-

old infants? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64(1), 3-26.

Baldwin, D. A., & Baird, J. A. (1999). Action analysis: A gateway to intentional inference. In

P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 215-240).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Baldwin, D. A., & Markman, E. M. (1989). Establishing word-object relations: A first step.

Child Development, 60(2), 381-398.

Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition, 11(3), 211-227.

Barsalou, L.W., Santos, A., Simmons, W.K., & Wilson, C.D. (2008). Language and simulation

in conceptual processing. In M. De Vega, A.M. Glenberg, & A.C. Graesser, A. (Eds.). Symbols,

embodiment, and meaning (pp. 245-283). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Behrend, D. A. (1990). The development of verb concepts: Children's use of verbs to label

familiar and novel events. Child Development, 61, 681–696.

Booth, A.E. (2006). Object Function and Categorization in Infancy: Two Mechanisms of

Facilitation. Infancy, 10(2), 145-169.

Booth, Amy E. & Waxman, Sandra R. (2002). Object names and object functions serve as cues

to categories for infants. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 948-957.

Booth, A. E., & Waxman, S. R. (2003). Mapping words to the world in infancy: On the

evolution of expectations for count nouns and adjectives. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4(3),

357–381.

Page 29: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 29 Booth, A.E. & Waxman, S.R. (2009). A Horse of a Different Color: Specifying with Precision

Infants' Mappings of Novel Nouns and Adjectives. Child Development, 80(1), 15-22.

Bornstein, M. H., Cote, L. R., Maital, S., Painter, K., Park, S.-Y., Pascual, L., (2004). Cross-

linguistic analysis of vocabulary in young children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italian, Korean,

and American English. Child Development, 75(4), 1115–1139.

Bunger, A. (2007). How we learn to talk about events: linguistic and conceptual constraints on

verb learning. PhD Dissertation. Northwestern University.

Bunger, A., & Lidz, J. (2004). Syntactic bootstrapping and the internal structure of

causative events. Proceedings of the 28th Annual Boston University Conference on Language

Development. Boston, MA: Cascadilla Press.

Buresh, J., Wilson-Brune, C., & Woodward, A. L. (2006). Prelinguistic action knowledge and

the birth of verbs. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R.M Golinkoff, (Eds). Action meets word (pp. 208-227).

Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Casasola, M., & Cohen, L. B. (2000). Infants' association of linguistic labels with causal

actions. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 155-168.

Caselli, M. C., Bates, E., Casadio, P., Fenson, L., Sanderl, L., & Weir, J. (1995). A cross-

linguistic study of early lexical development. Cognitive Development, 10, 159–199.

Chambers, C.G., Graham, S.A., & Turner, J. (2008). When hearsay trumps evidence: How

generic language guides preschoolers’ inferences about unfamiliar things. Language and Cognitive

Processes, 23 (5), 749-766.

Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J. A., & Sharp, D. W. (1971). The Cultural Context of Learning and

Thinking. New York: Basic Books.

Conboy, B.T. & Mills, D.L. (2006). Two languages, one developing brain: Effects of

vocabulary size on bilingual toddlers event-related potentials to auditory words. Developmental

Science, 9(1), F1-F11.

Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dehaene, S., & Hertz-Pannier, L. (2002). Functional neuroimaging of

speech perception in infants. Science, 298(5600), 2013–2015.

Fenson, L., Bates, E., Dale, P., Goodman, J., Reznick, J. S. & Thal, D. (2000). Measuring

variability in early child language: Don’t shoot the messenger. Child Development, 71, 323–8.

Fernald, A. (1992a). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals:

An evolutionary perspective. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind:

Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391-428). New York: Oxford University

Press.

Page 30: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 30 Fernald, A. (1992b). Meaningful melodies in mothers' speech to infants. In H. Papousek, U.

Jurgens, & M. Papousek (Eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental

approaches. Studies in emotion and social interaction (pp. 262-282). New York: Cambridge

University Press.

Fernald, A. (2006). When infants hear two languages: Interpreting research on early speech

perception by bilingual children. In P. McCardle & E. Hoff (Eds.) Childhood Bilingualism: Research

on Infancy through School-Age.

Fernald, A., Pinto, J. P., Swingley, D., Weinberg, A., & McRoberts, G. (1998). Rapid gains in

speed of verbal processing by infants in the second year. Psychological Science, 9, 228–231.

Fernald, A., Swingley, D., and Pinto, J.P. (2001). When half a word is enough: infants can

recognize spoken words using partial phonetic information. Child Development, 72, 1003-1015.

Fernandes, K. J., Marcus, G. F., DiNubila, J. A., & Vouloumanos, A. (2006). From semantics

to syntax and back again: Argument structure in the third year of life. Cognition, 100, B10-20.

Fisher, C. (2002). Structural limits on verb mapping: The role of abstract structure in 2.5-year-

olds' interpretations of novel verbs. Developmental Science, 5,56–65.

Fisher, C., Hall, G., Rakowitz, S., & Gleitman, L. (1994). When it is better to receive than to

give: structural and cognitive factors in acquiring a first vocabulary. Lingua, 92, 333–376.

Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). N400-like semantic incongruity effect in 19-month-

olds: Processing known words in picture contexts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1465-

1477.

Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Lexical priming and semantic integration reflected in

the event-related potential of 14-month-olds. NeuroReport, 16(6), 653-656.

Fulkerson, A. L., & Haaf, R. A. (2003). The influence of labels, non-labeling sounds, and

source of auditory input on 9- and 15-month-olds’ object categorization. Infancy, 4, 349-369.

Fulkerson, A. L., & Haaf, R. A. (2006). Does object naming aid 12-month-olds’ formation of

novel object categories? First Language, 26, 347-361.

Fulkerson, A. L., Waxman, S. R. (2007). Words (but not Tones) Facilitate Object

Categorization: Evidence From 6- and 12-Month-Olds. Cognition. 105(1) 218-228.

Gelman, S. A. (1996). Concepts and theories. In R. Gelman & T. Kit-Fong (Eds.), Perceptual

and cognitive development. Handbook of perception and cognition (2nd ed.) (pp. 117-150). San Diego:

Academic Press, Inc.

Gelman, S. A. (2006). Early conceptual development. In K. McCartney & D. Phillips (Eds.),

Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp. 149-166). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Page 31: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 31 Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., & Gottfried, G. M. (1994). Essentialist beliefs in children: The

acquisition of concepts and theories. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind:

Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 341-365). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Gelman, S. A., & Kalish, C. W. (2006). Conceptual development. In W. Damon & R. M.

Lerner (Series Eds.), & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2

cognition, perception, and language (6th ed., pp. 687-733). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Gelman, S. A., & Medin, D. L. (1993). What's so essential about essentialism? A different

perspective on the interaction of perception, language, and conceptual knowledge. Cognitive

Development, 8(2), 157-167.

Gelman, R., & Williams, E. M. (1999). Enabling constraints for cognitive development and

learning: A domain-specific epigenetic theory. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of Child

Psychology: Vol. 5. Cognition, perception, and language, Vol. 2.

Gentner, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2001). Individuation, relativity, and early word learning. In M.

Bowerman & S. Levinson (Eds.), Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development . New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Gentner, D., & Namy, L. (1999). Comparison in the development of categories. Cognitive

Development, 14, 487-513.

Gergely, G. & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naive theory of rational

action, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287–292.

Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G. & Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12

months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193.

Gerken, L., & McIntosh, B. J. (1993). Interplay of function morphemes and prosody in early

language. Developmental Psychology, 29(3), 448-457.

Gertner, Y., Fisher, C., & Eisengart, J. (2006). Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge

of word order in early sentence comprehension. Psychological Science, 17, 684-691.

Gervain, J., Macagno, F., Cogoi, S., Peña, M., & Mehler, J. (2008) The neonate brain detects

speech structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(37), 14222-14227.

Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A. (1999). Human simulations of

vocabulary learning. Cognition, 73(2), 135-176.

Gleitman, L. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition: A Journal

of Developmental Linguistics, 1(1), 3-55.

Gleitman, L. R., Cassidy, K., Nappa, R., Papafragou, A., & Trueswell, J. C. (2005). Hard

Words. Language Learning and Development, 1(1), 23-64.

Page 32: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 32 Gleitman, L. R., & Wanner, E. (1988). Current issues in language learning. In M. H. Bornstein

& M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (2nd ed.) (pp. 297-356).

Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2006). Introduction: Progress on the verb learning front.

In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs (pp. 3-28).

NewYork, NY: Oxford University Press.

Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K. M., & Gordon, L. (1987). The eyes have it:

Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 14, 23-45.

Gomez, R. (2002). Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science, 13

431–436.

Graham, S.A., Kilbreath, C.S., & Welder, A.N. (2004). 13-month-olds rely on shared labels

and shape similarity for inductive inferences. Child Development, 75, 409–427.

Guajardo, J. J., & Woodward, A. L. (2000). Using habituation to index infants' understanding

of pointing. Paper presented at the Presentation at the XIIth Biennial Meeting of the International

Society for Infant Studies, Brighton, UK.

Hespos, S.J., Ferry, A. L., Cannistraci, C., Gore, J., Park, S. (2009) Optical imaging on human

infants. To appear in A.W. Roe (Ed.) Imaging the brain with optical methods. Springer: New York.

Hespos, S. J. Park, S., Ferry, A., Lane, A.P. & Gore, J. (2009). An optical imaging study

oflanguage recognition in the first year of life. Manuscript in preparation.

Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. & Naigles, L. (1996). Young children’s use of syntactic

frames to derive meaning. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R.M. Golinkoff, (Eds.), The origins of grammar.

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Holowka S., Brosseau-Lapre F., Petitto L.A. (2002). Semantic and conceptual knowledge

underlying bilingual babies' first signs and words. Language Learning, 52(2), 205-262.

Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Theories

in cognitive psychology: The Loyola symposium. (331-368). Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.

Imai, M., Haryu, E., & Okada, H. (2005). Mapping novel nouns and verbs onto dynamic action

events: Are verb meanings easier to learn than noun meanings for Japanese children? Child

Development, 76(2), 340-355.

Imai, M., Lianjing L., Haryu, E., Okada, H., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R.M., & Shigematsu,

J. (2008). Novel noun and verb learning in Chinese-, English-, and Japanese-speaking children. Child

Development, 79, 979-1000.

Page 33: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 33 Jusczyk, P., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Infants' detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent

speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29(1), 1-23.

Jusczyk, P. W., & Kemler Nelson, D. G. (1996). Syntactic units, prosody, and psychological

reality during infancy. In J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from

speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 389-408). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,

Inc.

Kalish, C. W., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). On wooden pillows: Multiple classification and

children's category-based inductions. Child Development, 63(6), 1536-1557.

Keates, J., & Graham, S.A. (2008). Category labels or attributes: Why do labels guide infants’

inductive inferences? Psychological Science, 19, 1287-1293.

Keil, F. C. (1994). The birth and nurturance of concepts by domains: The origins of concepts of

living things. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in

cognition and culture (pp. 234-254). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Keller, H. (1904). The Story of My Life. New York: Doubleday, Page & Company.

Kemler Nelson, D. G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Jusczyk, P. W., & Cassidy, K. W. (1989). How the

prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning. Journal of Child Language, 16(1), 55-68.

Kersten, A. W., & Smith, L. B. (2002). Attention to novel objects during verb learning. Child

Development, 73(1), 93-109.

Klibanoff, R. S., & Waxman, S. R. (2000). Basic level object categories support the acquisition

of novel adjectives: Evidence from preschool-aged children. Child Development, 71(3), 649-659.

Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (1983). Culture and cognitive development. In

W. Kessen (Ed.), P.H. Mussen (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psychology; Vol. 1. History, Theory,

and Methods (4th ed.). New York: Wiley.

Landau, B. (1994). Object shape, object name, and object kind: Representation and

development. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research

and theory, Vol. 31 (pp. 253-304). San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.

Landau, B. & Gleitman, L. (1985). Language and experience: Evidence from the blind child.

Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical

learning. Cognitive Development, 3(3), 299-321.

Lassaline, M. E., & Murphy, G. L. (1996). Induction and category coherence. Psychonomic

Bulletin & Review, 3(1), 95-99.

Page 34: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 34 Leslie, A.M., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25,

265–288.

Lidz, J., H. Gleitman and L. Gleitman. (2003). Understanding How Input Matters: The

Footprint of Universal Grammar on Verb Learning. Cognition 87:151-178.

Macnamara, J. (1994). Logic and cognition. In J. Macnamara & G. E. Reyes (Eds.), The logical

foundations of cognition. Vancouver studies in cognitive science, Vol. 4 (pp. 11-34). New York:

Oxford University Press.

Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction.

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Medin, D. L., & Heit, E. (Eds.). (1999). Categorization. In D. Rumelhart & B. Martin (Eds.),

Handbook of cognition and perception (pp. 99-143). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Meltzoff, A.N. (2007). The ‘like me’ framework for recognizing and becoming an intentional

agent, Acta Psychologica 124(1), 26–43.

Meyer, M., Leonard, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Imai, M., Haryu, R., Pulverman, R.,

& Addy, D. (2003). Making a convincing argument: A cross-linguistic comparison of noun and verb

learning in Japanese and English. Poster presented at the Boston University Conference on Language

Development, Boston, MA.

Mills, D. L., Plunkett, K., Prat, C., & Schafer, G. (2005). Watching the infant brain learn

words: Effects of language and experience. Cognitive Development, 20, 19-31.

Morgan, J. L., & Demuth, K. (Eds.). (1996). Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to

grammar in early acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Muentener, P. & Carey, S. (2006). Representations of Agents Affect Infants' Causal

Attributions. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the XVth Biennial International Conference on

Infant Studies, Westin Miyako, Kyoto, Japan.

Murphy, G. (2004). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Naigles, L. (1990). Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. Journal of Child Language, 17,

357-374.

Naigles, L. (1996). The use of multiple frames in verb learning via syntactic bootstrapping.

Cognition, 58, 221-251.

Naigles, L., & Kako, E. (1993). First contact in verb acquisition: Defining a role for

syntax. Child Development, 64, 1665-1687.

Page 35: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 35 Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization. In R. Shweder & R.

LeVine (Eds.), Culture Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Oller D.K., Eilers R.E., Urbano R., Cobo-Lewis A.B. (1997). Development of precursors to

speech in infants exposed to two languages. Journal of Child Language, 24(2), 407-425.

Pearson B.Z., Fernandez S.C., Oller D.K. (1993). Lexical Development in Bilingual Infants and

Toddlers: Comparison in Monolingual Terms. Language Learning, 43(1), 93-120.

Peña, M., Maki, A., Kovacic, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F., & Mehler,

J. (2003). Sounds and silence: an optical topography study of language recognition at birth.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100 (20), 11702–

11705.

Piccin, T. B. & Waxman, S. R. (2007). Why nouns trump verbs in word learning: new evidence

from children and adults in the Human Simulation Paradigm. Language Learning and Development.

3(4), 295-323.

Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard

University Press.

Pinker, S. (1989) Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure.

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Pruden, S.M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Maguire, M.J., & Meyer, M.A. (2004). Foundations of verb

learning: Infants form categories of path and manner in motion events. In A. Brugos, L.

Micciulla & C.E. Smith (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th Annual Boston University

Conference on Language Development (pp.461-472). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

Pulverman, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Pruden, S., & Golinkoff, R. M., (2006). Precursors to verb

learning: Infant attention to manner and path. (pp.134-160) In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R. M. Golinkoff

(Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs. New York: Oxford Press.

Saffran, J.R., Werker, J.F., & Werner, L.A. (2006). The infant’s auditory world: Hearing,

speech, and the beginnings of language. In W.Damon & R.M. Lerner (Series Eds.) & R. Siegler & D.

Kuhn (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol.2, Cognition, perception and language (6th ed.,

pp. 58-108). New York: Wiley.

Shi, R., & Werker, J. F. (2003). The basis of perference for lexical words in 6-month-old

infants. Developmental Science, 6(5), 484–488.

Shi, R., Werker, J. F., & Morgan, J. L. (1999). Newborn infants' sensitivity to perceptual cues

to lexical and grammatical words. Cognition, 72(2), B11-B21.

Page 36: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 36 Snedeker, J., & Gleitman, L. (1999). Knowing what you know: Metacognitive monitoring and

the origin of the object category bias. Paper presented at the 24th annual Boston University Conference

on Language Development, Somerville, MA.

Snedeker, J., & Gleitman, L. (2004). Why it is hard to label our concepts. In D. G. Hall & S. R.

Waxman (Eds.), Weaving a Lexicon. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-

month-old infants' perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96, B1-B11.

Spelke, E. S. (2000). Nativism, empiricism, and the origins of knowledge. In D. Muir & A.

Slater (Eds.), Infant development: The essential readings. Essential readings in development

psychology (pp. 36-51). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers, Inc.

Swingley, D., Pinto, J. P., & Fernald, A. (1999). Continuous processing in word recognition at

24 months. Cognition, 71(2), 73-108.

Vouloumanos, A., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Listening to language at birth: Evidence for a bias

for speech in neonates. Developmental Science, 10, 159-164.

Vouloumanos, A., & Werker, J. F. (2004). Tuned to the signal: The privileged status of speech

for young infants. Developmental Science, 7, 270-276.

von Koss Torkildsen, J., Svangstu, J.M., Hansen, H.F., Smith, L., Simonsen, H.G., Moen, I., &

Lindgren, M. (2008). Productive vocabulary size predicts event-related potential correlates of fast

mapping in 20-month-olds Source. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(7) 1266-1282

Wagner, L., & Carey, S. (2005). 12-month-old infants represent probable endings of motion

events. Infancy, 7(1), 73–83.

Waxman, S. R. (1998). Linking object categorization and naming: Early expectations and the

shaping role of language, Vol. 38. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation

(pp. 249-291). San Diego: Academic Press.

Waxman, S. R. (1999a). The dubbing ceremony revisited: Object naming and categorization in

infancy and early childhood. In D. L. Medin & S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology (pp. 233-284). Cambridge:

Mit Press.

Waxman, S. R. (1999b). Specifying the scope of 13-month-olds' expectations for novel words.

Cognition, 70(3), B35-B50.

Waxman, S. R., & Booth, A. E. (2000). Distinguishing count nouns from adjectives: Evidence

from 14-month-olds' novelty preference and word extension. In Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston

University Conference on Language Development.

Page 37: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 37 Waxman, S. R., & Booth, A. E. (2001a). On the insufficiency of domain-general accounts of

word learning: A reply to Bloom and Markson. Cognition, 78, 277-279.

Waxman, S. R., & Booth, A. E. (2001b). Seeing pink elephants: Fourteen-month-olds'

interpretations of novel nouns and adjectives. Cognitive Psychology, 43(3), 217-242.

Waxman, S. R., & Booth, A. E. (2003). The origins and evolution of links between word

learning and conceptual organization: New evidence from 11-month-olds. Developmental Science,

6(2), 130–137.

Waxman, S. R., & Lidz, J. (2006). Early word learning. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.),

Handbook of Child Psychology (6th ed., Vol. 2).

Waxman, S. R., Lidz, J. L., Braun, I. E., & Lavin, T. (2009). 24-Month-Old Infants’

Interpretations of Novel Verbs and Nouns in Dynamic Scenes. Cognitive Psychology, 59, 67-95.

Waxman, S. R., & Markow, D. B. (1995). Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence

from 12- to 13-month-old infants. Cognitive Psychology, 29(3), 257-302.

Waxman, S. R., & Markow, D. B. (1998). Object properties and object kind: Twenty-one-

month-old infants' extension of novel adjectives. Child Development, 69(5), 1313-1329.

Welder, A.N., & Graham, S.A. (2006). Infants’ categorization of novel objects with more or

less obvious features. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 57-91.

Werker, J.F., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2008). Bilingualism in infancy: first steps in perception

and comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 144-151.

Werker, J. F., Lloyd, V. L., Pegg, J. E., & Polka, L. (1996). Putting the baby in the bootstraps:

Toward a more complete understanding of the role of the input in infant speech processing. In J. L.

Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early

acquisition (pp. 427-447). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Werker, J.F. & Yeung, H.H. (2005) Infant speech perception bootstraps word learning. Trends

in Cognitive Science. 9(11), 519-527.

Woodward, A. L., & Markman, E. M. (1998). Early word learning. In W. Damon, D. Kuhn, &

R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception and language (pp.371–

420). New York: Wiley.

Xu, F. (1999). Object individuation and object identity in infancy: The role of spatiotemporal

information, object property information, and language. Acta Psychologica, 102(2-3), 113-136.

Xu, F., & Carey, S. (1996). Infants' metaphysics: The case of numerical identity. Cognitive

Psychology, 30(2), 111-153.

Page 38: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 38 Yip, V. & Matthews, S. (2007). The Bilingual Child: Early Development and Language

Contact. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Page 39: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 39

Page 40: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 40

Figures

Figure 1. A schematic presentation of introductory phrases from Waxman & Markow (1995) and an example of a single stimulus set

_________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Familiarization Phase Test Phase Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 Animal Set: yellow duck green raccoon blue dog orange lion red cat red apple ________________________________________________________________________ __________________ Noun This one is. This one is See what I have? This one is See what I have? a(n) X a(n) X a(n)X Adjective This one is This one is See what I have? This one is See what I have? X-ish X-ish X-ish No Word Look at this. Look at this. See what I have? Look at this. See what I have? _________________________________________________________________________________________________________

Page 41: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 41

Figure 2. A schematic presentation of introductory phrases from Waxman & Booth (2001) and an example of a single stimulus set

Familiarization Contrast Test

Trial 1 Trial 2 Category Property

Purple Animal Set:

bear lion

elephant dog

red apple

purple horse vs. purple chair

purple horse vs. blue horse

Noun

These are blickets.

This one is a blicket & This one is a blicket

These are blickets.

This one is a blicket & This one is a blicket

Uh-oh, this one is not a

blicket!

Can you give me the blicket? Can you give me the blicket?

Adjective

These are blickish.

This one is blickish & This one is a blickish

These are blickish.

This one is blickish & This one is a blickish

Uh-oh, this one is not blickish!

Can you give me the blickish one?

Can you give me the blickish one?

No Word

Look at these.

Look at this one & Look at this one

Look at these.

Look at this one & Look at this one

Uh-oh, look at this one!

Can you give one? Can you give me one?

Page 42: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 42

Figure 3. A schematic presentation of introductory phrases from Waxman, Lidz, Braun, & Lavin (in press) and example of a single stimulus set.

Page 43: Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual ... · Early word learning 1 Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth

Early word learning 43

1 From Keller (1904). 2 Note, that this was, in fact, Keller’s second language. She had begun to acquire English before becoming deaf and blind. 3 This question itself hinges on there being a psychological distinction between object categories and object properties. Recent

approaches in cognitive psychology distinguish object categories (sometimes known as kinds or sortals) from other types of groupings (e.g.,

pink things, things to pull from a burning house) on at least 3 (related) grounds: Object categories (1) are richly-structured, (2) capture many

commonalities, including deep, non-obvious relations among properties (as opposed to isolated properties), and (3) serve as the basis for

induction (Barsalou, 1983; Gelman & Medin, 1993; Kalish & Gelman, 1992; Macnamara, 1994)(Medin & Heit, in press)(Murphy & Medin,

1985). Although infants and children lack detailed knowledge about most object categories, they clearly expect named object categories to

serve these functions (Gelman, 1996; Keil, 1994). In addition, there is now evidence for a psychological distinction between individual

properties and relations among properties in infancy (Bhatt & Rovee-Collier, 1997; Younger & Cohen, 1986). We selected color and texture

because these properties are perceptually salient to infants, and because these property-based commonalities typically do not underlie object

categories. We suspect that an object’s shape is more centrally related to category membership, particularly for simple artifacts and for animate

objects (Waxman & Braig, 1996).