Transcript
Page 1: Drawing assessment and artistic skill

The Arts in Psychotherapy, Vol. 18, pp. 347-352. Q Pergamon Press plc, 1991. Printed in the U.S.A. 0197-4556191 $3.00 + .OO

DRAWING ASSESSMENT AND ARTISTIC SKILL*

FRANCES FISHER KAPLAN, DA, ATRt

The impact of art training on drawing assessment techniques has received scant consideration by art therapists. Some justification for this neglect can be made by pointing out that many of the expressive features of drawings- such as color use, quality of line, symbolic imagery, placement-are compara- tively independent of drawing skill. The formal ele- ments of a drawing, however, are also subject to interpretation. For example, the degree of differen- tiation or sophistication of a drawing is viewed as a reflection of psychological maturity in a number of rating schemes (Witkin, Dyk, Faterson, Goodenough, & Karp, 1962). It is in this last instance, logic tells us, that skill is most likely to influence interpretive results.

A review of the literature of drawing assessment reveals how others have dealt with-or failed to deal with-this concern. Among the psychologists who have developed and promoted projective drawing techniques, Machover (1949) has emphasized that lack of skill is no handicap whereas Hammer (1958) has provided assurances that art training does not mask the expression of valuable personal material. Harris (1963), on the other hand, has looked more closely at possible effects. He reports an experiment by Goodenough (1926) indicating that training in drawing the human figure can influence her Draw- A-Person (DAP) Test score. Nevertheless, he ap- pears to have accepted Goodenough’s dismissive statement that such training is not “commonly given” in school. Taking an opposing view, some psychol- ogists have conducted studies that have led them to

cite artistic skill as a serious threat to the validity of drawing tests. Whitmyre (1953), for example, has concluded that ratings of personal adjustment based on figure drawings really measure artistic ability. In addition, Sherman (1958) and Cressen (1975) have made similar judgments concerning other ratings us- ing the DAP.

Turning to the writings of art therapists does lit- tle to resolve these conflicting claims. Whether ex- plicating the art assessment process (Oster & Gould, 1987) or presenting research related to a particular procedure (Cohen, Hammer, & Singer, 1988), these otherwise thoughtful accounts tend to sidestep this particular problem.

Although the need to control for artistic ability has been emphasized by a few (Handler, 1984; Sims, Dana, & Bolton, 1983), the overall impres- sion conveyed by the literature is that drawing skill has an “all or nothing” effect on drawing analysis. The possibility that skill has a definite but limited influence is not sufficiently entertained. The purpose of this paper is to further investigate the effects of this variable. More specifically, it is to present a study that evaluates the impact of drawing training on the type of drawing rating likely to be sensitive to its influence: a rating of drawing differentia- tion designed to reflect level of psychological development.

The Study

The purpose of this study was to lay the ground- work for a nonverbal measure of ego development.

*This paper is based on a research project undertaken in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Arts in the School of Education, Health, Nursing, and Arts Professions of New York University. TFrances Kaplan, Book Review Editor for The Arts in Psychotherapy, is Coordinator of the Creative Arts Therapy Program at Hofstra University, Hempstead, NY.

347

Page 2: Drawing assessment and artistic skill

348 PRANCES FISHER KAPLAN

As mentioned above, others had constructed rating schemes based on formal drawing elements. These schemes were intended to measure certain develop- mental variables such as sexual differentiation (Swen- son, 1955; Swenson & Newton, 1955), intellectual maturity (Harris, 1963), and differentiation of body concept (Witkin et al., 1962). A similar approach might provide the key to assessing overall psycho- logical development. I was not particularly con- cerned about the effects of drawing skill but included an art training questionnaire in my procedure-just in case. I was indeed surprised when the impact of training turned out to be my most compelling result. Although I did obtain some modest correlations be- tween my rating scheme and a verbal measure of ego development (see below), the apparent strong interaction with previous art training indicated to me that some means to control for drawing skill would be needed to make this a fully valid scale.

Table 1. Demographic and diagnostic variables for sample groups

Method

Research Participants

The sample consisted of 59 adult male and fe- male psychiatric patients and 37 adult male and fe- male nonpatients. The patient portion of the sample was drawn from the population of a short-term pri- vate psychiatric hospital. Only patients from un- locked units were involved. (Patients on locked units were not included as it was important for partici- pants to be sufficiently oriented to follow the neces- sary directions and to give their informed consent.) The nonpatient portion consisted of two subgroups: 20 residents of a nonpsychiatric alcohol rehabilita- tion program attached to the hospital (designated re- hab residents) and 17 employees of an industrial research institution (designated AB employees). At the time of the study, average length of stay for pa- tients and rehab residents was about 4 weeks. The participants as a whole were predominantly white, middle-class suburbanites. The age range was 15- 76, with a mean age of 38.7. Demographic and di- agnostic variables for the sample groups are summarized in Table 1.

Nonpatients

Rehab AR

Patients residents Employees Variable (?I = 59) (n = 20) (It= 17)

Age Range 15-76 26-63 21-60

Mean 37.3 46.5 34.7

Sex

Females 25 (42%) 6 (30%) 9 (53%) Males 34 (58%) 14 (70%) 8 (47%)

Education

High school or 28 (47%) 12 (60%) 3 (18%) less

College or more 30 (51%) 8 (40%) 12 (71%) Unknown 1 - 2

Diagnosis

Alcohol 24 (41%) 20 (100%) - dependence

Drug abuse/ 5 (8%) - -

dependence Mood disorders 18 (31%) - -

Psychotic disorders 5 (8%) - -

Other 7 (12%) - -

pletion Test of Ego Development (WUSCTED). De- veloped by Loevinger and co-workers (Loevinger & Wessler, 1970; Loevinger,Wessler, & Redmore, 1970), this measure is informed by theory that de- fines ego development as passage through a hierar- chy of invariant stages-resulting in changes in im- pulse control, moral style, interpersonal relations, conscious preoccupations, and cognitive complexity. Symbols and names for ego-development stages are as follows: I-l, autistic-symbiotic; I-2, impulsive; delta, self-protective; delta/3, ritualistic-traditional;’ I-3, conformist; I-3/4, self-aware; I-4, conscientious; I-4/5, individualistic; I-5, autonomous; I-6, inte- grated. These stages describe increasing levels of maturity with the I-6 stage representing an ideal state that few obtain. It is of note that the I-3/4 level has been found to be the modal level for adults (Holt, 1980).

Measures

The measure of psychological development em- ployed was the Washington University Sentence Com-

The first ego-level, I-l, is essentially preverbal and cannot be assessed by a written measure. The other nine levels can be scored by the WUSCTED. The WUSCTED consists of 36 sentence stems, scored by assigning an ego-level rating to each item re-

‘Robert R. Holt (undated) coined this name as Loevinger did not provide one for this stage.

Page 3: Drawing assessment and artistic skill

DRAWING ASSESSMENT AND ARTISTIC SKILL 349

sponse. A total protocol rating (TPR) is then derived from a distribution of these scores. The male and female versions of Form 11-68 of the WUSCTED were used. The resultant protocols were scored us- ing the published scoring instructions and item man- uals (Loevinger & Wessler, 1970; Loevinger et al., 1970) and some unpublished item manuals (Holt, undated). The TPRs were obtained by using the au- tomatic scoring rules. For the purposes of this study, a final score was then obtained by dividing the TPRs into three categories. A category rating of 1 was as- signed for TPRs of I-3 and below; a rating of 2, for I-314; and a rating of 3 for I-4 and above.

Research has indicated that construct validity for the WUSCTED is good (Loevinger, 1979). Further, interscorer reliability has been shown to be high. The median interscorer correlation for TPRs has been reported as .86 (Loevinger & Wessler, 1970). In this study, the TPR category ratings of the prin- cipal scorer (myself) produced an acceptable corre- lation of .90 when compared with those of another scorer.

The drawing measure consisted of the task for the Kinetic Family Drawing Technique (K-F-D) (Bums & Kaufman, 1972) along with a drawing rating scheme designed for this study. The K-F-D task was used because it offers sufficient complexity to give indications of drawing differentiation and integra- tion. The interpretative manual for the K-F-D was not used because it provides a largely impressionis- tic approach. A more objective scheme, based on formal rather than symbolic drawing elements, was developed. It involves assigning a rating for degree of drawing sophistication for each of two pictorial elements--form and space. A total rating, the draw- ing category rating, is obtained by combining the separate form and space ratings. Proceeding from least to most sophisticated, category ratings of 1, 2, or 3 are possible. (The process by which the rating scheme was developed has been detailed elsewhere- Kaplan, 1985.)

The drawing rating scheme was applied to the research sample by two raters working indepen- dently. These raters were art therapists who had been given some practice in using the rating scheme and who had achieved an acceptable interscorer re- liability of .77 for drawing category scores. Final drawing scores were obtained by averaging the two raters’ scores.

The final measure was the art training question-

naire. Research participants responded to the follow- ing questions by checking either “yes” or “no”:

1. Did you draw or do other artwork in elemen- tary school?

2. Did you have art instruction in junior high school?

3. Did you have art instruction in high school? 4. Did you have art instruction in college? 5. Did you have art instruction in any other

settings? 6. Did you have instruction in mechanical

drawing? 7. Do you ever draw or do other artwork for

your own amusement, or have you done so in the past (since childhood)?

Participants were assigned 1 degree of art training if they checked “yes” for question one only or if all questions were checked “no”; 2 degrees of art training if they checked “yes” for question one plus one other question; 3 degrees of art training for checking “yes” for the first question and two or more of the others.

Procedure

Patients and rehab residents were administered the three measures within regularly scheduled as- sessment groups for new admissions. The AB em- ployees completed the measures within one of two testing sessions. The WUSCTED and the K-F-D were given in counterbalanced order. The art train- ing questionnaire was always presented last. The in- structions for the drawing were to “draw a family doing something.” White drawing paper, pencils, and crayons and fine-line markers in assorted colors were provided for this purpose. The instructions for the WUSCTED were to “complete these sentences. ” Pens and the appropriate female or male versions of the test were distributed. All testing sessions were approximately one hour in length. When data col- lection was complete, the WUSCTED protocols and the drawings were scored independently without knowl- edge of the identity of the respondent.

Results

Product-moment correlations for ego-level and drawing sophistication scores resulted in weak sig-

Page 4: Drawing assessment and artistic skill

PRANCES FISHER KAPLAN

Table 2. Relationship between ego-level and drawing Table 4. Frequency of TPR category ratings for art ratings: Summary of correlation values for sample groups training groups (N = 96)

Sample groups N Product-moment correlations

Patients & nonpatients 96

Art training: 1 degree 29

2 degrees 29

3 degrees 38

Patients 59

Nonpatients 37

Rehab residents 20

AB employees 17

Males 56

Females 40

*p-C.05 **p<.o1

.24**

.07

.15

.44**

.17

.36*

.49*

.06

.26*

.20

Degree of art training 1

2

3

TPR scores

1 2 3

12 9 8

6 16 7

15 18 5

of drawing scores at different TPR levels (see Table 5), suggest that art training permits but does not in- sure top performance on the drawing task.

n&ant correlations for the total sample and for all males. A moderate significant correlation was ob- tained for the rehab subsample. These findings are summarized in Table 2. However, setting aside those with minimal art training (elementary school or less) enhanced most of the subsample correlations and resulted in two additional significant values-for all patients (I = .32, pc.05) and for male patients (r = .35, pC.05).

Finally, examination of the amount of art train- ing across subsamples revealed that 76% of patients, 65% of AB employees, and 55% of rehab residents had more than minimal training. Since AB employ- ees were the most highly educated (see Table 1) but not the most skilled in art, it appears that art train- ing is not simply a measure of overall education.

Discussion

Additional results provide a closer look at the ef- fects of art training. Tables 3 and 4 present fre- quency distributions of scores at different training levels. Comparing degree of art training and draw- ing scores produced a significant but weak correla- tion (I = .22, pc.05) and a marginally significant &i-square (x2 = 8.50, pc.10). On the other hand, both the correlation (r = - .08) and chi-square (x2 = 6.14, p>. 10) values for art training and TPR scores were nonsignificant. These findings, consid- ered in conjunction with the frequency distribution

The foregoing results offer evidence that art train- ing has a significant impact on particular types of drawing analysis. What makes these results interest- ing is that they suggest that a certain amount of training is a necessary but not suficient condition for rendering relatively sophisticated drawings. It is my speculation that drawing training (or practice) is necessary in order to acquire the “vocabulary” of representational concepts needed to construct well- differentiated and well-integrated drawings. If this is the case, it seems plausible that those with a high level of ego development will use this vocabulary to put together complex visual statements whereas those with a low level will not-just as a knowl- edge of verbal language, acquired through years of

Table 3. Frequency of drawing category ratings for art training groups (N = 96)

Table 5. Frequency of TPR and drawing category ratings (N = 96)

Degree of art training 1

2 3

Drawing scores 1 2 3

16 13 -

9 14 6

14 16 8

Degree of art training 1 2

3

TPR scores

1 2 3

19 14 6

11 23 9 3 6 5

Page 5: Drawing assessment and artistic skill

DRAWING ASSESSMENT AND ARTISTIC SKILL

training and experience, is a necessary but not suf- ficient condition for high-level performance on the ego-development measure. In sum, for those with little art training, artwork is not a valid measure of ego level.

But alternative explanations for the results should be considered. First, Loevinger’s test, developed for use with healthy populations, may not be suitable for psychiatric patients. However, the similarity be- tween the distribution of ego-level scores for the patient subsample and Holt’s (1980) normative dis- tribution for nonpatients (x2 = 5.5 1, p> .OS) sug- gests otherwise-at least for these particular patients. Second, those undergoing psychiatric or rehabilita- tive treatment (82% of my sample) are probably ex- periencing disturbances in their interpersonal relation- ships. Consequently, producing a family drawing can be especially stressful. It may require a higher level of ego development to cope with such stress so that it does not interfere with task performance. Thus, lower-level individuals might experience skill regression on the K-D-F, but not on other drawing measures. Finally, alcoholism can result in neuro- logical deficits that also affect task performance. Of the nine participants with a high degree of art train- ing who received both low drawing and low TPR scores, five were alcoholics. These last two expla- nations, which may represent contaminating factors rather than alternatives, indicate directions for fur- ther research.

Before concluding, an additional finding should be addressed. Whereas 28% of those with the least art training scored at the highest TPR level, only 13% with the most training scored at this level (see Table 4). Does this mean that more training goes with lower ego-level scores? It is possible. Consider that the WUSCTED requires verbal skill and that visual-spatial skill is involved in drawing. Those who excel in the latter may develop it to the detri- ment of the former. This, if it involves sufficient numbers of people, is a primary reason for devising a nonverbal measure of ego development. Again, an area for further research is indicated.

Conclusion

The literature of art assessment has given insuffi- cient consfructive attention to the influence of art training. This study has been presented as a small

step in the direction of correcting this deficiency. I hope that others will carry forward a line of research that has the potential for increasing the validity of the art assessment process and, hence, for strength- ening the scientific foundation that supports art therapy.

References

Bums, R. C., & Kaufman, S. H. (1972). Actions, styles and symbols in kinetic family drawings (K-F-D): An interpretive manual. New York: BrunnerA4azel.

Cohen, B. M., Hammer, J. S., & Singer, S. (1988). The diag- nostic drawing series: A systematic approach to art therapy evaluation and research. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 15, 1 l- 21.

Cressen, R. (1975). Artistic quality of drawings and judges’ evaluations of the DAP. Journal of Personality Assessment, 39, 132-137.

Goodenough, F. L. (1926). Measurement of intelligence by draw- ings. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.

Hammer, E. F. (1958). The clinical application of projective drawings. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.

Handler, L. (1984). Anxiety as measured by the Draw-A Person Test: A response to Sims, Dana, & Bolton. Journal of Per- son&y Assessment, 48, 82-84.

Harris, D. B. (1963). Children’s drawings as measures of intel- lectual maturity. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.

Holt, R. R. (Undated). [Supplied preliminary WUSCTED male and female item manuals]. Unpublished test manuals.

Holt, R. R. (1980). Loevinger’s measure of ego development: Reliability and national norms for male and female short forms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 909-920.

Kaplan, F. F. (1985). Level of ego development as reflected in patient drawings. Dissertation Abstracts International, 46, 2166-2167A. (University Microfilms No. DA8522008)

Loevinger, J. (1979). Construct validity of the Sentence Comple- tion Test of Ego Development. Applied Psychological Mea- surement, 3, 281-311.

Loevinger, J., & Wessler, R. (1970). Measuring ego develop- ment (Vol. 1). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Loevinger, J., Wessler, R., & Redmore, C. (1970). Measuring ego development (Vol. 2). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Machover, K. (1949). Personality projection in the drawing of the human figure. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.

Oster, G. D., & Gould, P. (1987). Using drawings in assess- ment and therapy: A guide for mental health professionals. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

Sherman, L. J. (1958). Sexual differentiation or artistic ability? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 14, 170-171.

Sims, J., Dana, R. H., & Bolton, B. (1983). The validity of the Draw-A-Person Test as an anxiety measure. Journal of Per- sonality Assessment. 47, 250-257.

Swenson, C. H. (1955). Sexual differentiation on the Draw-A- Person Test. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 11, 3741.

Swenson, C. H., & Newton, K. P. (1955). The development of sexual differentiation on the Draw-A-Person Test. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 11, 417-419.

Page 6: Drawing assessment and artistic skill

352 FRANCES FISHER KAPLAN

Whitmyre, J. W. (1953). The significance of artistic excellence Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R. B., Faterson, H. F., Goodenough, D. in the judgment of adjustment inferred from human figure R., & Karp, S. A. (1962). Psychological a’iflerentiation. New drawings. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 17, 421-424. York: Wiley.


Top Related