Transcript

Response to a critical situation during automated driving: can we take

drivers out of the loop?*

Tyron Louw, Natasha Merat, Georgios Kountouriotis, Ruth Madigan

Institute for Transport Studies, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK

// Introduction & Aims

• Endsley, M. R., and Kiris, E. O. (1995). The out-of-the-loop performance problem and level of control in

automation. Human Factors, 37(2), 381-394.

• Lee, J. D., Regan, M. A., and Young, K. L. (2008). Defining driver distraction. In M. A. Regan, J. D. Lee,

and K. L. Young (Eds.), Driver Distraction: Theory, Effects, and Mitigation(pp. 31–40). Boca Raton, FL:

CRC Press.

• Li, S. Y. W., Magrabi, F. and Coiera, E. (2012). A systematic review of the psychological literature on

interruption and its patient safety implications. Journal of the American Medical Informatics

Association: JAMIA, 19(1), 6–12.

• 30 Participants (39.2yrs ± 14.45)

• Driving experience: 20.17yrs ± 15.26

• Repeated measures, 3 X 3 mixed design

• Vehicle automation is likely to induce mind-

wandering, or stimulus-independent thoughts,

which can interfere with processing external

stimuli, such as roadway hazards (Li et al., 2012).

• This out-of-the-loop (OOTL) state presents an

issue to safety should the driver be called upon to

resume manual control (Endsley & Kiris, 1995;

Lee, 2013). But how does one study this given that

inducing the OOTL state is difficult?

// Results

// Methods

• There is also no objective measure of safety

and quality of the transition of control from

automation to manual driving

1. Can we induce the OOTL state by limiting

system and environmental information?

2. How do drivers make decisions and react in

the face of uncertain automation?

3. Are there other means of evaluating the

transition to manual driving?

Light FogHeavy Fog

30s

Automation On

Lead vehicle actionUncertainty Alert

EVENT START

Egovehicle

Lead vehicle

Screen Manipulation On

90s ≈30s

EVENT ENDScreen Manipulation Off

Non-critical Critical

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

≈ 20 mins

≈150s

// References

Critical Event = Lead vehicle braked at TTC of 5s.

Collision would occur unless driver intervened.

Steering Wheel

Colour

Automation

status

Grey Unavailable

Flashing green Available

Green Engaged

Flashing yellow Uncertain

Red Disengaged

• HMI: FCW & Automation Status • Inducing the OOTL state in automation:

• Schematic representation of each

discrete event

AutomationAutomation

Manual Manual

Critical

Event 1

Critical

Event 2

Critical

Event 1

Critical

Event 2Critical

Event 1

Critical

Event 2

Critical

Event 1

Critical

Event 2

Wit

hin

-Subje

cts

Facto

rs:

Dri

ve &

Event

Wit

hin

-Subje

cts

Facto

rs:

Dri

ve &

Event

Light Fog Heavy Fog

Between-subjects factor: Condition

Automation

Status

Hidden

Light Fog

Screen

Occlusion

Automation

Status

Hidden

Heavy Fog

Screen

Occlusion

Limited Visual Roadway

Information

Auditory and

haptic cues

still present

• Were drivers OOTL?

Percentage Road Centre during

screen occlusion

‘Peeks’ at hidden automation status

during screen occlusion

• What did they do?

In 16% of non-critical cases drivers disengaged

automation compared to 100% in critical cases

Automation Manual

Critical Event 1 Critical Event 2 Critical Event 1 Critical Event 2

Light Fog 7 (2) 10 (1) 11 (1) 12 (0)

Heavy Fog 9 (7) 9 (3) 11 (2) 11 (1)

Lane Changes and collision counts (in brackets)

• How did they do it?

• Automation (vs. Manual) =

↑ Lateral Acceleration (p=.005)

↑ Deceleration (p=.001)

↓ Time headway (p=.011)

*Paper to appear in the Proceedings of the Driver Distraction and Inattention Conference, Sydney 2015

Top Related