donders institute for brain, cognition and behaviour - noise … · 2009-03-06 · keywords: brain...

36
Noise Tagging as a New Auditory BCI-Paradigm: a Pilot Study Author: Joëlle Blankespoor 1 Supervisors: dr. J. Farquhar 1 , prof. dr. ir. P. Desain 1 , and prof. dr. S. Gielen 2 1 Nijmegen Institute for Cognition and Information, Radboud University, Nijmegen 2 Department of Biophysics, Radboud University, Nijmegen October 1 st , 2008.

Upload: others

Post on 27-Jul-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Noise Tagging as a New Auditory BCI-Paradigm:

a Pilot Study

Author: Joëlle Blankespoor1

Supervisors: dr. J. Farquhar1, prof. dr. ir. P. Desain1, and prof. dr. S. Gielen2

1 Nijmegen Institute for Cognition and Information,

Radboud University, Nijmegen

2 Department of Biophysics,

Radboud University, Nijmegen

October 1st, 2008.

Page 2: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Abstract

Brain Computer Interfaces (BCI) are intended to translate brain activity into computer actions

without intervention of any bodily movements. The main challenge in constructing a BCI

involves reliable signal detection to be used for single trial classification. In this paper, we

focus on developing a BCI using selective auditory attention. Frequency tagging is a way of

watermarking a carrier tone, by means of amplitude modulation, and results in an auditory

steady state response (ASSR). This paper investigates the use of a novel stimulus type for an

auditory BCI paradigm using pseudo-noise codes as amplitude modulators, called noise

tagging. This paper’s main hypothesis is that noise tagging offers a more robust signal

detectability than frequency tagging, which is used as the control condition. Two

experimental conditions were used: perceptual and attentional. The experimental results

confirmed that the noise tagged stimuli could be extracted successfully from the EEG signal

on a single trial basis in the perceptual condition, with averaged performances of 79 %. In the

parallel attentional condition the classification results were not consistent across subjects, but

comparable to the results of frequency tagging; highest classification results were around

70 %. Some suggestions for improvement will be discussed.

Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, auditory steady

state response (ASSR), selective auditory attention.

Page 3: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

1 Introduction

1.1 Controlling the world with thoughts: fantasy or reality?

Imagine that a few years ago you were paralyzed in a car accident .You can no longer walk or

move your arms, but you can still think. This might not seem like much, but it is enough to

perform a wide variety of physical tasks, if your brain activity is detected and interpreted by a

computer connected to a robot. Although it seems futuristic to think that it is possible to

control devices with brain activity, current research suggests that in the future this might

become possible. Even today, a modest form of controlling computers with brain activity is

possible. The so-called Brain Computer Interfaces (BCI) work from the principle that

recorded brain activity can be translated directly into computer actions without interference of

any bodily movements. A BCI system can be used, in very severe cases, to recover the

connection to the outside world of a motor-disabled person, caused by neuromuscular disorder

like amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), brainstem stroke or spinal cord injury (Wolpaw et

al., 2002; Lebedev et al., 2006). Other possible applications of BCI systems can be found in

for example: measuring vigilance during car driving (to prevent the driver from falling

asleep), playing a role in a gaming environment (Lalor et al., 2005), using neurofeedback to

treat children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders (ADHD) (Strehl et al., 2006).

In the current experiment I will investigate the neuronal response to a noisy auditory

stimulus, as distinguished from eliciting a neuronal response using a steady stimulus. In more

detail, an auditory steady state response (ASSR) in the brain can be elicited when presenting a

subject with an amplitude-modulated tone - that is, when a carrier frequency is modulated by

a lower frequency -, and this ASSR can be recorded using for instance an EEG or MEG

system. This paradigm can be used for a BCI based on selective attention, in which a user

directs his attention selectively to one out of two presented tones, both separated in pitch and

location. Each stimulus has a different frequency tag - a way of watermarking a carrier tone

by means of amplitude modulation - allowing identification of the neural response to the

stimulus in question. From analyzing the EEG-data it is possible to identify to which tone the

attention was directed, because there will be an increase in power of the neuronal signal in the

attended stream (Linden et al., 1987; Tiitinen et al., 1993; Ross et al., 2005; Skosnik et al.,

2007).

For the current experiment, a similar paradigm will be used, but with a different type

of stimulus: frequency tagged stimuli serve as control. Use is made of specially designed

Page 4: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

stimulus codes (called Gold codes; Gold, 1967), which contain a broad band of frequencies

and have low correlation between different codes. These pseudo-noise codes are used as

amplitude modulators – this is why this procedure is called noise tagging. The goal of this

project is to determine whether noise tagging has distinct advantages over frequency tagging,

and hence can be used in a BCI.

In the next sections a number of different topics related to the subject of this study will

be explained. In section 1.2 I will describe the current state of the art of BCI development,

and discuss a number of challenges that have to be addressed. Further, I will highlight the

pros and cons of recording brain activity using an EEG system. Section 1.3 deals with the

effects of selective attention on auditory signals in general. In line with this section, the results

of different experiments on the ASSR will be highlighted in section 1.4. This section will also

describe the theory concerning noise tagging in more detail.

1.2 BCI: overview and challenges

A wide variety of BCI methods exists, each with its own pros and cons. There are three

different BCI measurement methods: non-invasive, invasive and an intermediate invasive

technique. These categories will be discussed briefly below, followed by some of the

problems that emerge in using certain recording techniques. The possibilities and limitations

of using EEG in particular, as well as of the different kinds of cognitive tasks that are used to

generate a reliable signal, will then be discussed more thoroughly.

1.2.1 Invasive and non-invasive measurement methods

Invasive methods usually involve single- or multiple cell recordings, in which a number of

electrodes are implanted in the brain. One of the greatest advantages of invasive methods is

the relatively clean signal that is obtained, which is due to the direct contact with the area of

interest; different from EEG, there is no skull between the area of interest and the measuring

device to interfere with the signal. Another advantage of electrodes is its high precision of

implementation in the area of interest. One can for instance record from a population of

neurons, which code for spatial direction (Carmena et al., 2003). Another interesting example

is that of re-establishing the connection via electrodes in the brain to restore mobility, by

stimulating the muscles electrically based on the appropriate kinds of motor-related brain

activity detected by these electrodes (Santucci et al., 2005).

Page 5: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

However, invasive techniques have some major downsides, and I will discuss three of

them. As with any kind of surgery, there is a risk of infection when implanting electrodes.

Secondly, a major problem with implementation of electrodes is scar tissue buildup around

the electrodes, which makes them useless after a couple of months, as was found in monkeys

(Weber et al., 2000; Schwartz et al., 2007). Finally, another problem involves the risk of

damage to the implanted region or other areas, which might cause functional impairments.

These are some reasons why invasive methods are only sparingly used in human subjects, but

the results are highly promising for future work.

An intermediate invasive technique that is being researched is ECoG (electrocortico-

graphy). This technique is in fact similar to EEG, but now a pad with electrodes is below the

skull, therefore the signal has a higher spatial resolution and a better signal-to-noise ratio than

with EEG. As with all invasive techniques surgery risks are a concern. However, some of the

disadvantages of implanting electrodes in the brain are less of a problem with ECoG; for

example, with ECoG there is less risk of scar tissue buildup. This technique can be applied in

patients, but is too invasive to be used in healthy test subjects. This is why the vast majority of

BCI experiments uses non-invasive recording techniques.

In non-invasive ways, you can detect brain activity by means of a fMRI, MEG or EEG

system, but for practical reasons the latter is the best technique for BCI patients. An EEG

device is transportable, it does not need a shielded room, or magnets to be cooled, it has a

high temporal resolution and it is not very expensive (Lotte et al., 2007). In this study I will

focus on the use of EEG for a BCI system, and its possibilities and limitations. As noted

above, the most important property of EEG is its high resolution in the time domain. In the

spatial domain EEG is less specific, because of volume conduction, which is the process by

which an electric signal spreads out when it passes through different tissues. That is, EEG

localization techniques are highly complicated because of what is called the inverse problem;

a certain recorded scalp topography can be generated by an infinite number of different source

locations and combinations. Thus different neuronal sources might have the same recorded

scalp distribution of activity.

1.2.2 Mental tasks

Different BCI systems have been developed, varying from systems based on different sensory

modalities combined with selective attention, imagining movements, or systems using more

automatic processes. One of the most widely studied cognitive tasks is motor imagery,

Page 6: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

because the associated neuronal signals are relatively easy to detect (Babiloni et al., 2000;

Penny et al., 2000; Pfurtscheller et al., 1993).

A method that differs from using a specific cognitive task is that of the so-called

‘operant conditioning’ approach, in which the mental control skill is acquired subconsciously

through feedback, but the subject learns to deploy the skill voluntarily (Birbaumer et al.,

1999; Wolpaw., 1998). The subject may think about anything during the task as long as the

result is control of the cursor on the screen. In a way, this method is comparable to acquiring a

new skill, such as learning to ride a bicycle, where you learn to control the bicycle despite not

being aware of the exact nature of the neural activity or equilibrium dynamics involved in the

process.

Another possible BCI signal is the P300, which is a positive deflection (200-700 ms

after stimulus onset) as response to a rare stimulus surrounded by other random stimulus

events. The P300 can be successfully used to select items through a matrix speller to

communicate spontaneously (Nijboer et al., 2008a). Finally, using selfregulation of slow

cortical potentials (SCP) can also be used as a basis for a BCI, by giving the subject feedback

he can learn to voluntarily control the SCP (which can last several seconds) (Pham et al.,

2005).

When developing a BCI system a number of methodological issues need attention,

which are: 1) how to extract the right cognitive processes; 2) the amount of training time; 3)

the amount of data transfer; 4) how to deal with subject variability.

Firstly, the brain is never quiet; the challenge is to extract the features of interest while

ignoring other processes that are taking place. Different experiments have shown the

difficulty to exactly pinpoint the cognitive process taking place. Moreover, there might be

intermediate cognitive processes that cause changes in the recorded EEG signal, like

concentration, attention and difficulty of the task (Curran et al., 2003).

The second concern in developing BCI system is the amount of training time

necessary to let the subject acquire control over their EEG output: some research groups

reported that fairly extensive training (10-40 sessions) was required to improve accuracy of

EEG control (Curran et al., 2003).

Thirdly, the amount of signal (trial length) needed to reliably classify the right class is

another point of importance. In constructing a BCI we need to maximize both the number of

decisions per minute, and the signal reliability (successful classifications).

The last aspect is the effectiveness of different cognitive tasks as reliable sources of

signals in different subjects. A variability of fitness for specific tasks between individuals

Page 7: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

makes it almost impossible to come up with a one-task-fits-all paradigm. This is even more

problematic for people with specific disabilities. Tasks involving motor imagery might be

difficult to execute for a person who has been paralyzed for several years (Pfurtscheller et al.,

2000), and a visual task will be nearly impossible for someone who is visually impaired.

These limitations indicate the need to build a number of different BCI systems with a broad

range of reliable tasks (Curran et al., 2003).

One set of these tasks involves attending selectively to sounds. The auditory modality,

which we focus on in this paper, is well-suited to be used in a BCI system, for instance

because it is a phenomenologically prominent modality and it is one of the last remaining

modalities for ALS-patients. In the next section I will start the exploration of this topic with a

general description of auditory neuronal signals, as well as the influence of selective attention.

1.3 Attending to sounds

At any given moment a person might hear different sounds from different locations. In such a

natural environment, multiple sources confront the auditory system with different types of

information. Among the modalities present, it is important to selectively attend to one of the

sources in order to evaluate and process relevant stimuli. The process of separating a relevant

stream in the auditory modality from its surroundings is called auditory scene analysis (ASA;

Bregman, 1990). An often-used example of the separating process is the so-called cocktail-

party effect, in which a person in a crowded room wants to attend to the conversation in

which he is involved while ignoring other conversations taking place. Selective attention

serves as a vital mechanism to allow a subject to process relevant information. Yet, attention

is a process that is difficult to quantify in a straightforward manner because it never involves a

simple on or off structure; rather, it is a process involving many simultaneous intermediate

values. This notion is important when using a task design involving attention as in the current

experiment.

Studies focusing on the mechanisms of selective attention in the brain, using

functional imaging techniques, found activity in primary and secondary auditory cortices

(Alho et al., 1999; Jancke et al., 1999; Crady et al., 2000; Sevostrionov et al., 2002). Other

known effects of attentional modulation are a correlation between increasing neuronal

magnitude and increasing task difficulty (Spritzer et al., 1988; Boudreau et al., 2006),

although this correlation can be influenced by task design and behavioral strategies.

Page 8: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Although localization studies are useful in terms of knowing where in the brain

auditory processing takes place, functional imaging techniques are not precise enough to

uncover the millisecond time course in which auditory processing takes place. A large number

of studies focusing on auditory attentional processes used MEG/EEG to record information on

a fine-grained timescale. An often used signal is the transient event related potential (ERP),

using brief stimuli. Often studies effects the brain response for change detection, such as the

N1 (~ 100 ms) and the later (~100-200 ms) mismatch negativity (MMN). The MMN is an

example of an ERP, which is elicited when selective attention is directed to a target stimulus

surrounded by distractors, also referred to as the ‘oddball paradigm’. The MMN has received

a lot of attention because of its possible underlying basis as a ‘novelty detector’. This serves

to encode stimulus deviations, which can capture attention and can result in an behavioral

response (Fritz et al., 2007). These ERP components have used relatively short stimuli (for

instance tone pips), and have a long interstimulus interval (Skosnik et al., 2007).

As will be discussed in the next section, an alternative technique to examine

attentional processes is the generation of the auditory steady state response, for this one can

use continuous stimulus modulation. The latter aspect is important for BCI applications,

because the stimulus can be presented for multiple modulation periods, increasing the signal

strength.

1.4 Towards Noise Tagging

In the previous sections I discussed a few auditory signals, selective attention, different

cognitive tasks for BCI systems and the problems that emerge when developing a BCI system.

In this last introductory section I will discuss several aspects of frequency tagging and noise

tagging more thoroughly.

Frequency tagging utilizes the auditory steady state response (ASSR): the neuronal

response to a periodic auditory stimulation such as click trains, amplitude or frequency

modulations (AM or FM) of continuous sounds (Galambos et al., 1981; Hari et al., 1989; John

et al., 2003). The ASSR provides interesting means for investigating topics related to the

assessment of hearing thresholds (Galambos et al., 1981; Stapells et al., 1984), developmental

research (Maurizi et al., 1990; Boettcher et al., 2002; Rojas et al., 2006), levels of

consciousness (Pockett et al., 2002; Plourde et al., 2008), and selective attention (Linden et al,

1987; Tiitinen et al, 1993; Ross et al., 2005; Skosnik et al, 2007). Possible generators of the

Page 9: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

ASSR are the primary auditory cortex in Heschl’s gyrus and the thalamus (Ross et al., 2004;

Skosnik et al., 2007).

The steady state response is not bound exclusively to the auditory domain: the so-

called steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEP) can be recorded when presenting a

constantly oscillating visual stimulus. The SSVEP is also prone to (c)overt selective attention

(covert means that the stimulus is not the foveal center), and hence might be useful as a BCI

(Müller et al., 1998; Allison et al., 2008). Another useful signal for a BCI is the steady state

somatosensory evoked potential (SSSEP), elecited using vibratory stimulation on for instance

a finger tip (Müller-Putz et al., 2006).

The precise mechanisms that give rise to the neuronal steady state response are a topic

of debate. Synchronisation (resonance) is a candidate for the explanation of the generation of

the SSR (Tanaka et al., 2008), especially in the gamma-band (around 40 Hz). This suggests

that neuronal units have an intrinsic firing rate that best resonates with stimulus frequencies

around 40 Hz. Moreover, experimental findings suggest that the response to a 40 Hz

modulated stimulus is two to three times larger in power compared to higher frequencies, of

70 Hz and above (Herdman et al., 2002; Petitot et al., 2005).

The term “noise” can have several different meanings. In general, noise refers to a

broadband and unpredictable signal. I will discuss three different ways in which noise can

play a role in neuronal systems: 1) noise as interfering with signal encoding; 2) noise as a

neuronal signal enhancer; 3) noise as a stimulus.

Firstly, recall that the purpose of this experiment is to test the effectiveness of noise

tagging instead of frequency tagging for a BCI, and this might be surprising to some. After

all, much of the activity in the brain is spontaneous, and might have nothing to do with actual

processing of environmental stimuli or motor actions that we are interested in for a particular

BCI task, hence this activity is often referred to as noise. In most studies, noise is considered

to disrupt and interfere with the encoding of relevant stimuli. Discussions in the literature

focus mostly on how reliable neuronal processing can compensate for neuronal noise.

However, evidence exists that reliable neural coding can make use of noise, instead of

being forced to compensate for it – this is the second noise type. Though it seems

counterintuitive, the addition of noise can actually lead to a larger response to a stimulus

(Shechter et al., 2006). When recording intracellularily, a current injection into a neuron will

cause a spike train. If a spike train is evoked by injecting a steady-state current the first

following action potentials are similar across multiple trials. But, the later action potentials

will have a greater variability across trials (Bryant et al, 1976; Mainen et al, 1995). However,

Page 10: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

when using the same method, but now with ‘frozen noise’ input (frozen refering to using the

same noise from a random distribution) one can cause reliable spiking (Bryant et al., 1976;

Mainen et al., 1995; Galán et al., 2008; Ermentrout et al., 2008).

Not only does noise enhance neuronal firing, as mentioned above, it also increases the

power of the phase synchrony of the ASSR as Tanaka and colleagues (2008) found. They

recorded subjects using a MEG system when AM tones were presented with white noise of

various intensities. A possible explanation of this phenomenon is stochastic resonance, which

refers to a nonlinear system showing a decrease in noise to signal ratio (N/S) when there is an

increase of the noise level in the input.

Thirdly, taking the idea of noise enhancing neuronal firing one step further, one might

argue that neuronal processing of a noise stimulus itself is also possible. The basic idea

behind noise tagging is that of spreading the signal’s power over a broad band of frequencies,

and this has several advantages. For instance, signal detectability will decrease little when

another source masks a particular frequency band. This advantage is important because signal

detection robustness is an important reliability aspect in BCI systems. By spreading the power

over multiple frequency bands the chances are maximized that some signal always remains to

be detected. Another aspect of this approach is that intersubjective variations are less

problematic: if the individual optimal frequency ranges vary, it is likely that each of these

ranges falls within the stimulus frequency range.

Frequency tagging stimuli have short repetion periods, increasing the risk of short

neuronal lags becoming difficult to distinguish from longer latencies, referred to as aliasing.

Temporal aliasing is unlikely to occur, because in noise tagging the stimulus period (in our

experiment ~2 sec) is much longer than the expected neuronal lags. In fact, the noise

modulator can be designed to last much longer.

Another important notion of these noise codes is that there is little crosscorrelation

between different codes. This property will make it easier for correlation analysis methods to

distinguish which stimulus code has been presented or attended. Moreover, autocorrelation of

one code, that is correlating the code with a timeshifted version of itself, is low except at time

lag zero. This latter aspect is for instance important when the brains neuronal response has

several different time lags.

The concept of using noise stimuli is inspired by the “spread spectrum” techniques.

Faced with the problem that wireless signals needed to be secured from “being disturbed,

intercepted, or interfered with in any way” Nikola Tesla came up with the “frequency-

hopping spread spectrum” idea in 1900. This works by rapidly switching a carrier among may

Page 11: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

frequency channels. Another application is the “direct-sequence spread spectrum” approach.

A normal narrow band signal is spread over a broad band of frequencies by multiplying it

with a “noise” signal. This noise signal is generated using pre-designed Golden codes. These

are generated using two pseudorandom number sequences, a set of predefined numbers, but

approaching all properties of being random (Gold, 1967; Hershey, 1982; Dixon, 1994).

Today, spread spectrum techniques are used in various wireless communication systems. If

the brain is conceived of as a signal processing device, these tried and true techniques might

also be useful as stimulus retrieval from neuronal signals.

1.6 Hypothesis

The challenge of extracting the noise codes back from the neuronal signal is a main goal of

the currently conducted experiment. To achieve this goal a new stimulus type is designed in

order to develop a more optimal BCI system based on selective auditory attention. The

hypothesis is that noise tagging offers significant advantages over frequency tagging as a way

to increase neuronal signal detectability in BCI applications.

2 Methods and procedure

2.1 Subjects

For the current experiment five subjects were asked to participate in the experiment. All

subjects were healthy adults (2 women, 3 men; aged between 22 and 35). All subjects were

free of neurological disorders, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and did not report

any hearing disabilities. Only three subjects had some prior experience with EEG recordings.

Subjects were informed about the global content of the study in advance.

2.2.1 Data acquisition

EEG recordings were acquired in an electromagnetically shielded and sound-attenuated cabin,

using 256 sintered Ag/AgCl active electrodes, amplified with a BioSemi ActiveTwo AD-box.

During electrode placement offset jitter and offset amplitudes values were kept below 0.2 mV

and 35 mV respectively. ActiView was used for data acquisition with a sampling rate of 2048

Hz. Data were stored for offline analysis and no further filtering was used at this stage.

Page 12: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

2.2.2 Experimental setup

Subjects were comfortably seated in a chair while watching a computer screen. Sounds were

presented on two loudspeakers in the right and left corner of the recording room. The sounds

were presented at a comfortable listening level for each subject (estimated at 70 dB SPL

(sound pressure level)). The grounding electrodes were placed on the forehead in order to

prevent formation of gel bridges (gel contact between two electrodes causing a short-circuit)

when a more central place on the head would have been chosen. Under the assumption that

eye movements and blinking artefacts would not threaten signal integrity at frequencies of

interest, no electro-oculargraphy (EOG) signals were recorded (see Fatourechi et al. (2007)

for a more extended overview about recording EOG in a BCI).

2.2.3 Auditory stimuli

The stimuli were generated by Matlab programs (version 7.5), with a sampling frequency of

44.1 kHz. The frequencies of both the carrier and the modulation signal were chosen such that

they contained an integer number of cycles in each modulation period. Moreover, the

modulators were matched to fit with the down-sampled sampling rate of the EEG recording

system.

For the stimuli two carrier signals at 512 and 768 Hz were used, approaching a saw

tooth function containing six harmonics. These carrier signals were amplitude-modulated with

a depth of 90 % with four different modulation patterns: two for frequency tagging, at 42 2/3

Hz (referred to as 42 Hz in this paper) and 64 Hz, and two for noise tagging at 128 Hz, called

code A and B (see figure 1 for the modulators and their spectra). All stimuli were ~2 sec in

duration and shaped by up-sampling the chosen bit-patterns and cosine filtering the rising and

falling edges for half of one cycle. The filter depended on the stimulus frequency. Effectively

this means that both frequency tags are similar to sine waves, and for the noise tags a

transition of zeros and ones looks like a sine wave.

Page 13: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Figure 1: Stimulus time-courses and spectra. On the left the time-course of each stimulus modulator is shown and on the right the spectrum of each modulator is plotted (note that only the first 200 ms of each stimulus is plotted).

2.3 Experimental design

EEG recordings lasted 1.5 hours. The experiment was divided into three different blocks: a

perceptual block and a parallel attentional block. The blocks were divided in different

sequences (~1 min), containing per sequence either noise or frequency tagging stimuli. Each

sequence was constructed out of multiple (20) trials/epochs lasting 2 seconds with small

silences of 250 milliseconds between the trials (see figure 2).

During each sequence the subject was instructed to look at a fixation cross on the

screen, to minimize the number of eye movements. Between each sequence the subject was

able to have a short break. The number of trials that were collected in the different

experimental conditions were: 150 trials in the perceptual and 100 trials in the attentional

frequency tagging data, 160 trials for both the perceptual and attentional noise tagging data.

Note that in subject 1, due to experimental error, the perceptual conditions has 110 trials and

behavioral data are not available.

Page 14: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Figure 2: Example of a sequence in the attentional condition. The subject is instructed to attend to the right sounds and count the number of low volume deviants. The deviants on the other side have to be ignored.

In order to minimalize the number of different experimental conditions the carriers

and their modulators were fixed, meaning that the 512 Hz carrier (modulated with code A or

the 42 Hz frequency tag) was fixed on the left side and the 758 Hz carrier with the other

modulators on the right side.

In the perceptual recording block a single auditory stream was presented on both

speakers. A simple counting task was used to make sure that the subject was paying attention;

in more detail the subject was instructed before every sequence to count one stimulus type.

In the parallel attentional block two different auditory streams were presented. The

subject was instructed to count the number of deviants (a lower amplitude stimulus) on the

attended side (right or left speaker), deviants were presented on both sides (probability of

occurrence 0.10). In both the perceptual and attentional block, subjects got feedback on their

performance after each sequence. The feedback was stored for later analysis of the behavioral

performances.

2.4 Data analysis

The main data analysis steps are shown in figure 3 and included: a number of pre-processing

steps (see section 2.4.1), correlation analysis (see section 2.4.2) and classification (see section

2.4.3). Further, the classification outputs were decomposed as is described in section 2.4.4.

Page 15: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Figure 3: Data analysis protocol. In this figure the main steps of the data analysis are shown. Starting with the raw data a number of pre-processing steps were used. The square boxes display the main procedure and optional steps are plotted in the oval boxes.

2.4.1 Pre-processing

The first step in the data analysis was to downsample the originally sampled data from 2048

to 512 Hz. The reason for this being no brain signals were to be expected above 256 (the

Nyquist frequency). After that, the right data segments with a length of 2.5 sec were epoched,

(2 sec stimulus plus an extra 250 ms pre- and post stimulus for analysis). Next, the bad

channels (high amplitude 50 Hz and/or offset jitter) were excluded from further data analysis.

Common average referencing (CAR) was used for re-referencing, this is subtracting the

average over the complete scalp from every single electrode at every point in time.

The last step in the pre-processing was applying a bandpass and a 50 Hz notch to the

data. The bandwidth of the applied bandpass was adjusted towards the most optimal response

for the data gathered in the experiment.

2.4.2 Data analysis procedures

After the pre-processing steps a number of different methods were tested to get the most

optimal performance. In short, these methods included: normalizing the spectrum; whitening

the signal; crosscorrelating the signal with the original stimulus modulators. The effects of

Page 16: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

each step were examined by comparing the output of the classifier, which will be explained in

more detail in section 2.4.3.

Using a broadband stimulus all frequencies are approximately equally distributed. It

was found that the “1 / f” power distribution of the EEG signal led to dominance of the lower

frequencies. Therefore, the averaged spectrum was calculated for every subject, and used to

normalize the 1 / f effect of the EEG spectrum.

Further, the use of a whitening function on the data was investigated, because such a

function gives all features in the data equal power, which could make it easier for the

classifier to pick the relevant features.

The next step in the analysis was the crosscorrelation computation, or convolution, of

every trial with its original modulator, as shown in figure 4. This works by sliding the original

stimulus modulator over the corresponding EEG epoch and at each timepoint computing the

correlation. Note, that the assumption of noise codes only having an autocorrelation at time

lag zero, and that there is little crosscorrelation between codes are essential.

Figure 4: schematic overview of cross-correlation analysis. This schematic overview illustrates how the correlation analysis was done. When presenting an auditory stimulus to the subject, EEG data was recorded and sliced out 250 ms before stimulus onset. After the pre-processing of the data the correlation was computed. As shown here, the original stimulus modulator was slid over the EEG signal for 0.5 seconds, and at each time-point a correlation value was computed. In the bottom box the resulting correlation values are plotted, with a peak after stimulus onset, indicated with the dotted line.

Page 17: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

2.4.3 Classification

The final step was to classify the crosscorrelation values with a linear logistic regression

classifier. A linear classifier groups items with similar features into groups, and bases its

decision on a linear combination of the features. This process can be described as splitting the

high dimensional input space with a hyperplane, for a two-class problem leaving points at

both sides of the plane to be classified as belonging to one class.

The performance of the classifier was assessed using a 30-fold cross-validation, where

the data are split into 30 equally-sized different ‘folds’. A different classifier was trained for

each fold as the testing set, the other 29 are used as the training set. The resulting cross-

validation estimates are a reliable but sometimes underestimated result of the dataset, because

the training is based on a subsample of the available data (Lalor er al., 2005; Bishop, 2008).

Since the trial length is relatively short, combining a few trials is an option to improve

classification accuracy. Moreover, it gives more information about relative strength of the

signal extracted. There are three ways in which one could combine multiple trials. The first is

averaging the EEG of multiple subsequent trials which increases the sensitivity for evoked

oscillations. The second is to combine multiple trials by concatenating them, which might

lead to an improved frequency resolution because the signal length increases. The last option

is to combine the single trial output of the classifier, which increases the probability that a

trial belongs to one class. According to Kallenberg (2007) all three methods seem to give

comparable classification improvements. It depends on the stimulus type which of these

methods performs best; in this experiment all stimuli are time-locked, therefore the first

method can be used.

2.4.4 Weight vector decomposition

The output of the classifier of weight vectors is further decomposed using singular value

decomposition, which is the process of decomposing the vector into several linearly

independent vectors. After this the strongest weights for each different stimulus dataset can be

used to generate a plot with the distribution over the scalp. This would make it possible to

infer if the responses to the different stimuli have the same spatial distribution. Moreover, one

might be able to infer if there is more than one source which is generating the neuronal

response. For more precise source localization one needs other methods like beamforming,

but these go beyond the scope of the currently conducted experiment.

Page 18: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

3 Results

3.1 Perceptual data

On both the frequency and noise tagging datasets the effectiveness of the data analysis

methods, described in the methodological section, were tested. These different steps were the

main part of the standard data analysis protocol and included: normalizing the 1/f-effect,

bandpassing with various bandwidths and a correlation analysis.

Classification performance throughout this text, is expressed as an averaged

percentage of correct classifications followed by, between brackets, the standard error (SE)

between subjects. For the frequency tagging data a number of analysis steps improved

classification performance significantly. Classification performance using the raw EEG time-

series was poor, 56 (3), when applying a bandpass between 30 Hz and 80 Hz classification

rates improved to 71 (8); this was most substantial in three of the subjects. Applying different

bandwidths did not further improve performance. Nor did normalizing of the data seem to

improve performance; in fact it slightly worsened the performance to 66 (5). By correlating

the original stimulus modulator with the data, classification rates improved to 86 (4). As one

would expect, combining the correlation analysis with either a bandpass, 87 (4), or the

normalizing approach, 88 (4), did not improve classification rates significantly (p > 0.1). This

means that only the correlation approach is useful for frequency tagged data, although either

bandpassing or normalizing the data might improve performance in some subjects. For

individual results see table 1.

Frequency tagging: perceptual

% (SE between folds) Mean (SE):

Noise tagging: perceptual % (SE between folds)

Mean (SE):

Subject 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1trial 98(1) 74(2) 87(2) 86(2) 93(1) 88(4) 94(2) 58(2) 69(2) 66(2) 78(3) 73(6) 2 trials 99(1) 76(3) 94(2) 89(3) 96(2) 91(4) 99(1) 62(3) 74(3) 70(4) 88(2) 79(7) 1 trial 97(1) 76(3) 76(2) 81(3) 89(2) 84(4) 84(2) 64(2) 55(2) 55(3) 65(4) 65(5) 2 trials 100 83(4) 83(3) 90(2) 93(2) 90(3) 92(2) 74(2) 60(3) 56(4) 75(3) 71(6)

Table 1: classification performances of the perceptual condition. The first two rows contain classification rates obtained using the standard data analysis protocol (bandpassing, norm-filtering and correlation analysis). The last two rows are the results obtained after whitening of the data. For each subject the averaged percentages for a 30-fold crossvalidation are given, and between brackets the standard error (SE) between different folds. The mean columns show the averaged percentages over subjects, and between brackets the SE.

Page 19: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

The averaged results for frequency tagging data are shown in figure 5. This figure

shows the cross-correlation values between the original stimulus modulators and the two

frequency tagging datasets. In order to visually inspect how strong the correlation values are

of the modulator with the EEG dataset, control plots are shown where the stimulus modulators

are correlated with the other datasets. From this figure it becomes clear that the averaged

correlation values are much stronger than the ones in the control plots. Furthermore, the

averaged correlation value of the 42 Hz trials is higher than the averaged value of the 64 Hz

trials.

Figure 5: averaged correlation values of frequency tagging. The averaged correlation values for subject 1, are plotted over time and across electrodes. The top two plots show the cross-correlation values with the datasets of the 42 Hz stimulus either correlated with the 42 Hz modulator (top left plot), or with the 64 Hz modulator (top right plot). The middle two plots show the correlation values for the 64 Hz stimulus, again correlated with the 42 Hz modulator (left middle plot) or the 64 modulator (right middle plot). In the bottom plots, the time-course averaged over channels is shown. Note that stimulus onset occurs at time point zero.

On the noise tagging dataset the different analysis methods performed differently in

some subjects. Classification performance on the not-normalized data was 60 (5).

Bandpassing the data between 30 and 80 Hz improved the classification performance in four

Page 20: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

out of five subjects, on average 68 (7). Only one subject 2 had a better classification

performance when the data were not bandpassed (70 versus 54 percent). All other

bandpassing widths that were tested did not lead to any improvements of the results. There is

an indication that correlating the data with the original stimulus modulators slightly improves

classification performance, on average 71 (7), this averaged percentage can be increased with

4 percent when excluding subject 2. Further, normalizing the data also improves classification

performance a little, 73 (6), and again this averaged percentage can be increased to 77 percent

when excluding subject 2.

In figure 6 the averaged results for noise tagging are shown. These have a peak

correlation value around 50 ms after stimulus onset. In addition, this figure shows that both

codes do not correlate with the other code datasets.

Figure 6: averaged correlation values of noise tagging. The averaged correlation values for subject 1, are plotted over time and across electrodes. The above top two plots show the cross-correlation values with the datasets of the A code either correlated with the A code modulator (top left plot), or with the B code modulator (top right plot). The middle two plots show the correlation values for the B code, again correlated with the A code modulator (left middle plot) or the B code modulator (right middle plot). In the bottom plots, the time-course averaged over channels is shown. Note that stimulus onset occurs at time point zero, and neuronal delays are ~50 ms.

Page 21: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

After the standard analysis protocol, whitening (as explained in section 2.4.2, this

gives all features equal power) was applied on the data, which did give some contradictory

results. For frequency tagging the whitening did on average not improve classification rates

significantly, 84 (4), (F=3.05, p>0.05), it was also inconsistent across subjects (see table 1),

slightly decreasing with 4 percent on average.

For the noise tagging data it did not make any significant change, 65 (5), (F=5.2,

p>0.05), decreasing in four subjects and only increasing performance in subject 2.

Combining two trials by averaging them prior to the correlation analysis did improve

the frequency tagging data significantly with 3 percent (F=9.8, p<0.05). For the noise tagging

the classification performance was 6 percent higher on average (F=24.9, p<0.01).

3.2 Attentional data

The attentional datasets were analyzed in the same way as the perceptual datasets. For the

frequency tagging dataset classification performance on average was 60 (2), but only two

subjects (1 and 2) were classified on or above 60 (67 percent and 60 percent respectively).

The three other subjects were classified slightly above chance level (58, 56 and 57 percent).

For noise tagging classification performance averaged 54 (1) percent. Only subject 1 was

classified significantly above chance level (57).

Frequency tagging: attentional % (SE between folds)

Mean (SE):

Noise tagging: attentional % (SE between folds)

Mean (SE):

Subject 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

1trial 67(3)** 60(4)* 58(3)* 56(3) 57(4) 60(2) 57(2)* 55(3) 53(3) 49(3) 55(3) 54(1) 2 trials 70(5)** 68(5)** 65(5)** 57(4) 57(7) 63(3) 57(4) 51(4) 54(3) 54(5) 55(4) 54(1) 3 trials 61(5)* 51(5) 56(5) 48(5) 62(5)* 56(3) 1 trial 61(4)* 71(3)** 53(4) 53(4) 55(3) 59(3) 64(3)** 57(3)* 54(3) 52(2) 61(2)** 58(2) 2 trials 70(6)** 76(5)** 51(5) 52(5) 58(5) 61(5) 67(4)** 57(4) 51(3) 56(4) 67(4)** 60(3) 3 trials 73(4)** 53(5) 54(6) 57(4) 71(5)** 62(4)

Table 2: classification performance of the attentional condition. As in table 1 the top rows are the classification rates obtained using the standard data analysis protocol. The last three rows show the results obtained including whitening in the data analysis. Note that for the frequency tagged data combining three trials was not possible because the dataset contained too few trials (see section 2.3). Significance levels are indicated as follows: ‘*’ refers to (p < 0.05) and ‘**’ refers to (p < 0.01).

In order to increase classification performance on both datasets, whitening of the data

and combining multiple trials were further exploited.

Page 22: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Combining multiple trials was done by averaging the EEG of two or three subsequent

trials. For the frequency tagging data a 20-fold crossvalidation was done because this dataset

consisted of too few trials for a 30-fold classification, and only a combination of two trials

was possible because of the smaller number of trials in this dataset. For this dataset combining

multiple trials did on average not improve classification performance, two combined trials

were classified on average at 63(3). However, this number is influenced by the two last

subjects (4 and 5) who were classified near chance level. Combining two trials did improve

classification for the first three subjects, to 70, 68 and 65 respectively, on average for these

subjects an improvement of 6 percent (see table 2).

For noise tagging combining three subsequent trials before classification, did not

improve classification rates on average, but two subjects (1 and 5) were now classified above

60 percent (61 and 62). The remaining three subjects were still classified on chance level.

Whitening of the frequency tagged data gave only a classification performance

improvement for subject 2, from 60 to 71 %. In three subjects it did not change classification

performance, but it did drop performance to chance level in subject 3. For the noise tagging

data, whitening did improve classification rates in the two subjects (1 and 5), that had

classification levels above chance level with the standard analysis methods, to 64 and 61 %,

respectively. Combining three subsequent trials improved the classification results further to

73 and 71 %, respectively.

3.3 Behavioral data

The feedback on each sequence in both experimental conditions is summarized in table 3. For

each sequence there were four answer possibilities, which means that if a subject gave a

random answer, performance would be 25 percent. All subjects performed quite well during

the perceptual condition. In the attentional condition two subjects (3 and 4) had lower

feedback scores compared to the other two subjects, especially for the frequency tagging

sequences.

Page 23: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Subject: 1 2 3 4 5 Mean Noise tagging - 71 92 63 96 81 Frequency tagging - 81 88 81 100 88

Perceptual condition Mean - 75 90 70 98 83

Noise tagging - 88 88 63 100 85 Frequency tagging - 90 60 40 90 70

Attentional condition

Mean - 88 77 54 96 79

Table 3: behavioral performance in both experimental conditions. For each subject the averaged feedback scores (expressed in percentage correct) on each sequence are shown. Due to experimental error no feedback scores are available for subject 1.

Figure 7: averaged weight perceptual condition. In the left top plot the strongest weight for code A is plotted, the right top plot shows the strongest weight for code B. The plots show a clear front to back distribution. The middle plot shows the timecourse of the strongest weight, for noise tagging averaged across subjects. On the left middle plot, the strongest weight for the 42 Hz stimulus is shown, on the right the strongest weight for the 64 Hz stimulus is plotted. The bottom plot shows the timecourse of the strongest weight, for frequency tagging. Note that the intensity of the colors reflects how strong the weight is, - the absolute value is important.

Page 24: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

3.4 Weight decomposition

On both perceptual datasets the classifier weights were decomposed, as described in section

2.4.4, to see how the correlation values were distributed over the scalp. In figure 7 the

averaged strongest weight over all subjects is plotted for the perceptual condition. For both

noise codes a clear front to back distribution becomes visible. There is an indication that a

second, less powerful, source is present in this data, but the results are not consistent across

subjects (see appendix A for more details).

Figure 8: averaged weight for attentional condition. The strongest weight for all stimuli for subject 1 are plotted. See figure 7 for layout details.

Page 25: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

For frequency tagging, the scalp distribution is a bit different than for the noise

tagging, having a more central to back distribution. There is no clear indication that a second

source is present in the data (see appendix B).

The strongest weight of the attentional condition is shown in figure 8. Because only

subject 1 showed attentional modulation in both conditions, this subject was used for to

generate these plots.

4 Discussion

In the current experiment a new type of stimulus modulator was introduced, called noise

tagging. It was hypothesized to have distinct advantages over frequency tagging. The main

advantage was that of the spreading of power over multiple frequency bands, making signal

detection more robust to interfering signals in some frequency bands, an important feature in

BCI applications. Because no one has yet investigated a paradigm using noise stimuli in

humans, the main challenge was whether the noise codes could be extracted from the neuronal

signal. If this would be possible it was further hypothesized that noise tagging could serve as a

new BCI paradigm based on selective attention. Frequency tagged stimuli were used as a

control. The experimental results confirmed that the noise tagged stimuli could be extracted

successfully from the EEG signal on a single trial basis. Using selective attention to one out of

two auditory streams, it was investigated if noise tagged stimuli could be used for a BCI. The

experimental findings were not consistent across subjects: only two subjects were classified

above chance level in the attentional condition. Nonetheless, the results are comparable to

those of frequency tagging, where three subjects were classified successfully.

In this section the results will be discussed more thoroughly. Furthermore, in the next

section the limitations of and possible improvements for noise tagging as a BCI will be

discussed. In the third section a number of limitations of the current experiment and some

recommendations for further research will be discussed.

Although it is difficult to draw any strong conclusions based on the small group of

subjects, a number of interesting issues can still be discussed. If one looks at the difference in

classification performance between frequency tagging and noise tagging in the perceptual

condition, the latter classifies ~15 % worse than the frequency tagging data, even though this

dataset contained less trials. This means that frequency tagging is superior to noise tagging

because it elicits a stronger perceptual response. Moreover, high frequency tagging

Page 26: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

performance on the perceptual dataset seems to be a very good predictor for the performance

of the noise tagging data (see figure 9).

Figure 9: Relation between the responses (percentages correct classification) in the perceptual condition. Each data point represents the performance on both frequency and noise tagging of an individual subject.

There is a number of possible explanations for this relation. One is that the signal to

noise ratio is rather constant, being reflected in the amount of reliable signal that can be

extracted from that subject. Another possibility is that the same circuits underlie the

generation of the responses to both frequency and noise tagging. However, this latter

explanation might a topic of debate when looking at the attentional datasets performances.

For the attentional condition the superior performance of frequency tagging over noise

tagging seems to diminish. If one takes the best performances, three subjects can be classified

at 68 % on average for the frequency tagged data, that is when combining two trials. For noise

tagging in the attentional condition two subjects can be classified above chance, on average

67 % using two combined trials and whitening. These percentages are relatively close,

suggesting that frequency tagging, which had a better classification performance on the

perceptual data, somehow lacks this advantage in the attentional condition. Given the fact that

drawing any strong conclusions based on a few subjects is not possible, one could only

imagine a number of possible explanations for the found effects.

Page 27: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

A possible explanation is that too many subjects were classified at or slightly above

chance level, which makes it impossible to infer what is going on. Another reason might be

that the frequency tagged stimuli are harder to separate perceptually - due to their regularity

the subject has no cues other than the carrier and location by which frequency modulated

stimuli can be distinguished. The noise tagged stimuli seemed to be easier to attend to,

because their patterns made them recognisable. Moreover, in the current experimental design

the modulators were matched to be integers of the carriers, which were therefore half an

octave apart. This combination might have introduced perceptual mixing of both frequency

tagged stimuli, making it harder to separate them. The behavioral data of the attended

condition does not fully support this hypothesis. Subject 5 has high behavioral scores, but low

classification performances, and in subject 3 it is the other way around. In subject 2 and 4

behavioral data can be matched to classification performance.

Other possible explanations might be found in the underlying mechanisms that

generate the response. It might be the case that the frequency tagging data decrease non-

linearly in power due to interaction effects because of interference between the two stimuli.

Frequency tagged stimuli are static, hence more likely to exploit the similar neuronal

mechanisms, therefore more susceptible to interference. This is in contrast with noise tagged

stimuli which have dynamically changing properties. That is, noise tagging has the advantage

that is has a broad band of frequencies and is perhaps exploiting multiple neuronal

mechanisms at the same time.

As described in the introduction, frequency tagging stimuli respond best at frequencies

around 40 Hz. This preference might be explained by resonating neuronal circuits, that are

intrinsically tuned to this frequency range. Recall that in the current experiment the

modulators were at 42 Hz and 64 Hz. Though the first modulator frequency is in the preferred

frequency range, which was indicated by the stronger response in the perceptual condition, the

neuronal response to the 64 Hz stimulus was less strong. Although this reasoning is very

hypothetical, it might be true that the 40 Hz preference causes stronger responses in the

attentional condition. In the following section, the use of noise tagging for a BCI will be

evaluated.

4.2 Noise tagging as a new BCI paradigm?

Recall (from section 1.2.2) that when designing a new BCI system, there are several issues

that need to be addressed: 1) how to extract the right cognitive processes, 2) the amount of

Page 28: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

training time, 3) the amount of data transfer, 4) and how to deal with subject variability. In

retrospect, this experiment dealt with these issues in the following ways. To begin with, in

this experiment the stimuli were fixed, and extractable in all subjects in the perceptual

experimental condition. This means, that in the selective attention condition, only selective

attention is under the subject’s direct control. If subjects were capable of attending to the right

stimulus to the exclusion of everything else, classification performance should be perfect.

However, selective attention is subject to a wide variety of contextual influences, among

which concentration, task length and task difficulty (Curran et al., 2003). Even if one would

be able to perfectly attend to the stimuli, different mental strategies might be used in different

circumstances. For instance the subject could attend to the location or specific stimulus

characteristics like pitch. This illustrates some of the problems the classifier is faced with,

making it difficult to classify each trial.

The second problem was that in the current experiment subjects were tested only once,

so the training time was very short. Thus a subject was able to acquire control in the paradigm

used relatively quickly, which is an advantage compared to systems requiring weeks of

training. However, the behavioral data suggested that some subjects had difficulty in the

attentional condition, reflected in the low percentages of correct answers. Therefore, a few

training sessions might help to get the subject familiar with the task, and improve the

classification performance.

Thirdly, the number of succesful classifications is the feature that is probably most

important for a successful BCI system. Classification performance can be translated to an

information transfer rate. According to the formula provided in Wolpaw and colleagues

(1998) the averaged performance in both attentional conditions is comparable to a bitrate of

1.4 bits per minute (bpm).

The final problem a BCI system has to account for is subject variability. As discussed

in the introduction, a universally applicable BCI system can only be constructed by studying

BCI systems in various modalities, to determine which is least vulnerable to subject

variability. As such, a BCI system based on auditory signals is a useful contribution to the

range of BCI systems that are developed. In the current experiment the classification

performance of noise tagging was not superior to frequency tagging performance. Further

experiments on noise tagging, in auditory as well as other modalities, will have to prove its

use for a BCI. Some recommendations for changes in experimental design will be discussed

in the next section.

Page 29: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

4.3 Recommendations for further research

Overall, even the classification rates obtained after combining multiple trials are too low to

use in an online BCI. In this section, I will describe what could possibly explain this moderate

classification performance, also a number of possible improvements will be discussed. A

number of reasons could explain the poor average performance of the subjects in the current

experiment. Some subjects reported having difficulty attending to one side all the time.

Possibly the experimental task was not interesting enough to capture the subject’s attention

over the whole sequence. I would argue that when we make the task more interesting for the

subject, the attentional effects in the neuronal response will increase, as mood and motivation

are suggested to play in role in learning to control a BCI (Curran et al., 2003; Nijboer et a.,

2008b). Furthermore, the used attentional paradigm can be argued to be too different from the

natural situation when attention is needed. The natural function of attention, which is to

extract relevant information from the surroundings, is not exploited during the experimental

task. A possible implementation on an experimental level could be to choose a different

carrier (for instance an instrumental voice), or making a sequence meaningful as a whole (for

instance using a combination of increasing and decreasing carrier tones), or giving the subject

online feedback about their performance.

5 Conclusion

The main hypothesis associated with this experiment was that noise tagging would offer

distinct advantages over frequency tagging as part of an experimental BCI system. However,

this experiment demonstrated that the performance of the noise tagging paradigm did not

improve significantly over the performance of the frequency tagging paradigm. Still, this

experiment did show that noise tagging does work and as such offers new opportunities for

researchers in BCI to design a viable system; because this is such novel approach there is still

a lot of room for improvement. For fundamental neuroscience the expansion of the catalog of

stimuli retrieval paradigms is small but important next step in our exploration of the workings

of the (human) brain.

Page 30: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

6 Acknowledgements

I would like to thank my supervisors Jason Farquhar, Peter Desain and Stan Gielen for their

support and advice, and Rutger Vlek and Philip van den Broek for their valuable input.

7 References

Alho, K., Medvedev, S.V., Pakhomov, S.V., Roudas, M.S., Tervaniemi, M., Reinikainen, K.,

Zeffiro, T. & Naatanen, R. (1999). Selective tuning of the left and right auditory cortices during spatially directed attention. Brain. Res. Cogn. Brain. Res. 7(3): 335- 341.

Allison, B.Z., McFarland, D.J., Schalk, G., Zheng, S.D., Jackson, M.M. & Wolpaw, J.R.

(2008). Towards an independent brain-computer interface using steady state visual

evoked potentials. Clin. Neurophys., 119: 399-408.

Babiloni, F., Cincotti, F., Lazzarini, L., Millàn, J., Mourino, J., Varsta, M., Heikkonen, J.,

Bianchi, B. & Marciani, M.G. (2000). Linear classification of low-resolution EEG

patterns produced by imagined hand movements. IEEE Trans. Rehabil. Eng., 8:186-

188.

Bailey, B.J. (1984). Cochlear prosthesis implantation: review of the issues. JAMA. 251(24):

3282.

Bieser, A. & Muller-Preuss, P. (1996). Auditory responsive cortex in the squirrel monkey:

neural responses to amplitude-modulated sounds. Exp. Brain Res., 108 (2): 273-284.

Birbaumer, N., Ghanayim, N., Hinterberger, T., Iversen, I., Kotchoubey, B., Kübler, A.,

Perelmouter, J., Taub, E. & Flor, H. (1999). A spelling device for the paralyzed.

Nature, 398:297-298.

Birbaumer, N., Kübler, A., Ghanayim, N., Hinterberger, T., Perelmouter, J., Kaiser, J.,

Iversen, I., Kotchoubey, B., Neumann, N. & Flor, H. (2000). The thought translation

device (TTD) for completely paralyzed patients. IEEE Trans. Rehabil. Eng., 8:190-

192.

Bishop, C.M. (2008). Linear models for classification. Pattern recognition and machine

learning (ch. 4). New York; Springer Science.

Bregman, A.S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: the perceptual organization of sounds. MIT

press.

Bryant, H.L. & Segundo J.P. (1976). Spike initiation by transmembrane current: a whitenoise

analysis. J. Physiol., 260: 279-314.

Page 31: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Boettcher, F.A., Madhotra, D., Poth, E.A. & Mills, J.H. (2002). The frequency-modulation

following response in young and aged human subjects. Hear. Res., 165(1-2): 10-18.

Boudreau, C.E., Williford, T.H. & Maunsell, J.H. (2006). Effect of task difficulty and target

likelihood in area V4 of macaque monkeys. J. Neurophys., 96: 2377-2387.

Carmena, J.M., Lebedev, M.A., Crist, R.E., O’Doherty, J.E., Santucci, D.M., Dimitrov, D.F.,

Patil, P.G., Henriquez, C.S., Nicolelis, M.A.L. (2003). Learning to control a brain-

machine interface for reaching and grasping by primates. PLoS Biology, 1: 193-208.

Chi, T., Gao, Y., Guyton, M.C., Ru, P. & Shamma, S. (1999). Spectrotemporal modulation

transfer functions and speech intelligibility. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 106 (5): 2719–2732.

Curran, E.A. & Stokes, M.J. (2003). Learning to control brain activity: A review of the

production and control of EEG components for driving brain-computer interface (BCI)

systems. Brain and Cogn., 51: 326-336.

Dixon, R.C. (1994). Spread Spectrum Systems. Wiley - Interscience, 3rd edition.

Eggermont, J.J. (1994). Temporal modulation transfer functions for am and fm stimuli in cat

auditory cortex: effects of carrier type, modulating waveform and intensity. Hear. Res.,

74 (1-2): 51–66.

Ermentrout, G.B., Galán, R.F. & Urban, N.N. (2008). Reliability, synchrony and noise.

Trends Cogn. Sc., 624: 1-7.

Galambos, R., Makeig, S. & Talmachoff, P.J. (1981). A 40-Hz auditory potential recorded

from the human scalp. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 78(4): 2643-2647.

Grady, C.L., Van Meter, J.W., Maisog, J.M., Pietrini, P., Krasuski, J. & Rauschecker, J.P. (1997). Attention-related modulation of activity in primary and secondary auditory cortex. Neurorep., 8(11): 2511–2516.

Gold, R. (1967). Optimal binary sequences for spread spectrum multiplexing. IEEE

Trans. Infor. Theory, 13(4): 619–621.

Hari, R., Hamalainen, M. & Joutsiniemi, S.L. (1989). Neuromagnetic steady-state responses

to auditory stimuli. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 86(3):1033-1039.

Herdman, A.T., Lins, O., Van Roon, P., Stapels, D.R., Scherg, M. & T.W., Picton (2002).

Intracerebral sources of human auditory steady-state responses. Brain Topography,

15(2): 69-86.

Hershey, J. (1982). Direct Sequence Spread Spectrum Techniques. Aegean Park Press.

Fatourechi, M., Bashashati, A., Ward, R.K. & Birch, G.E. (2007). EMG and EOF artifacts in

brain computer interface systems: A survey. Clin. Neurophys., 118: 480-494.

Fritz, J.B., Elhilali, M., David, S.V. & Shamma, S.A. (2007). Auditory attention - focussing

Page 32: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

the searchlight on sound. Cur. Opinion in Neurobiol., 17: 437-455.

Galán, R.F., Ermentrout, B.G., & Urban, N.N. (2008). Optimal time scale for spike-time

reliability: Theory, simulations and experiments. J. Neurophys., 99: 277-283.

Jancke, L., Mirzazade, S. & Shah, N.J. (1999). Attention modulates activity in the primary and the secondary auditory cortex: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in human subjects. Neurosci. Lett., 266 (2): 125-128.

John, P.X., Dimitrijevic, A. & Picton, T.W. (2003). Efficient stimuli for evoking auditory

steady-state responses. Ear Hear., 24(5): 406-423.

Kallenberg, M. (2007). Auditory selective attiontion as a method for brain computer interface.

Nijmegen CNS Web Edition, 2.

Lalor, E.C., Kelly, S.P., Finucane, C., Burke, R., Smith, R., Reilly, R.B. & McDarby, G.

(2005). Steady-state VEP-based Brain-Computer Interface control in an immersive 3D

gaming enveronment. J. Appl. Sign. Process., 19: 3156-3164.

Lebedev, M.A. & Nicolelis, M.A. (2006). Brain- machine interfaces: past, present and future.

Trends Neurosci. 29(9): 536-46.

Liang, L., Lu, T. & Wang, X. (2002). Neural representations of sinusoidal amplitude and

frequency modulations in the primary auditory cortex of awake primates. J.

Neurophys., 87 (5): 2237–2261.

Linden, R.D., Picton, T.W., Hamel, G. & Campbell, K.B. (1987. )Human auditory steady -state evoked potentials during selective attention. Electroencep. Clin. Neurophys., 66(2): 145–159.

Lotte, F., Congedo, M., L´ecuyer, A., L’amarche, F. & Arnald, B. (2007). A review of

classification algorithms for EEG-based brain–computer interfaces. J. Neural Eng., 4:

1-13.

Mainen, Z.F. & Sejnowski, T.J. (1995). Reliability of spike timing in neocortical neurons.

Science, 268: 1503-1506.

Mauritzi, M., Almadori, G., Paludetti, G., Ottaviani, F., Rosignoli, M. & Luciano, R. (1990).

40-Hz steady-state responses in newborns and in children. Audiology, 29(6):322-328.

Müller, M.M., Picton, T.W., Valdes-Sosa, P., Riera, J., Teder-Salejarvi, W.A. & Hillyard, S.A.

(1998). Effects of spatial selective attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential

in the 20–28 Hz range. Cogn. Brain Res., 6(4): 249-261.

Müller-Putz, G.R., Scherer, R., Neuper, C. & Pfurfscheller, G. (2006). Steady-state

somatosensory potentials: suitable signals for brain-computer interfaces? IEEE Trans.

Rehabil. Eng., 14(1): 30-37.

Page 33: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Nijboer, F., Seller, E.W., Mellinger, J., Jordan, M.A., Matuz, T., Furdea, A., Halder, S.,

Mochty, U., Krusienski, D.J., Vaughan, T.M., Wolpaw, J.R., Birbaumer, N. & Kübler,

A. (2008a). A P300-based brain-computer interface for people with amyotrophic

lateral sclerosis. Clin. Neurophys., 119: 1909-1916.

Nijboer, F., Furdea, A., Gunst, I., Mellinger, J., MvFarland, D.J., Birbaumer, N. & Kübler, A.

(2008b). An auditory brain computer interface (BCI). J. of Neurosc. Methods, 167(1):

43-50.

Penny, W.D., Roberts, S.J., Curran, E.A. & Stokes, M.J. (2000). EEG-based communication:

a pattern recognition approach. IEEE Trans. Rehabil. Eng., 8:214-215.

Petitot, C., Collt, L. & Durrant, J.D. (2005). Auditory steady-state responses (ASSR): effects

of modulation and carrier frequencies. Intern. J. Audio., 44(10): 567-573.

Pfurtscheller, G., Flotzinger, D. & Kalcher, J. (1993).Brain-computer interface - a new

communication device for handicapped persons. J. Microcomp. Appl., 16:293-299.

Pfurtscheller, G., Neuper, N., Guger, C., Harkam, W., Ramoser, H., Schlögl, A., Obermaier,

B. & Pregenzer, M. (2000). Current trends in Graz Brain-Computer Interface (BCI)

research. IEEE Trans. Rehabil. Eng., 8:216-219.

Pham, M., Hinterberger, T., Neumann, N., Kübler, A., Hofmayer, N., Grether, A., Wilhelm,

B., Vatine, J.J. & Birbaumer, N. (2005). An auditory brain-computer interface based

on the self-regulation of slow cortical potentials. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair, 19:

206-218.

Ross, B., Picton, T.W. Herdman, A.T. & Pantev, C. (2004).The effect of attention on the

auditory steady-state response. Neurol. Clin. Neurophys., 22: 1-4.

Santucci, D.M., Kralik J.D., Lebedev M.A. & Nicolelis M.A. (2005). Frontal and parietal

cortical ensembles predict single-trial muscle activity during reaching movements in

primates. Eur. J. Neurosci., 22(6): 1529-1540

Schreiner, C.E. & Urbas, J.V. (1986). Representation of amplitude modulation in the auditory

cortex of the cat. I. The anterior auditory field (AAF). Hear. Res., 21 (3): 227-241.

Schreiner, C.E. & Urbas, J.V. (1988). Representation of amplitude modulation in the auditory

cortex of the cat. ii. comparison between cortical fields representation of amplitude

modulation in the auditory cortex of the cat. I. The anterior auditory field (AAF).

Hear. Res., 32 (1): 49–63.

Schwartz, A.B. (2007). Useful signals from motor cortex. J Physiol. 579: 581–601. Sevostianov, A., Fromm, S., Nechaev, V., Horwitz, B. & Braun, A. (2002). Effect of

Page 34: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

attention on central auditory processing: an fMRI study. Int. J. Neurosci., 112 (5): 587–606.

Skosnik, P.D., Krishnan, G.P. & O’Donnel, B.F. (2007). The effects of selective attention on

the gamma-band auditory steady-state response. Neurosc. Let., 420: 223-228

Spitzer, H., Desimone, R. & Moran, J. (1988). Increased attention enhances both behavioral

and neuronal performance. Science, 240: 338-340.

Stapells, D.R., Linden, D., Suffield, J.B., Hamel, G. & Picton, T.W. (1984). Human auditory

steady state potentials. Ear Hear., 5(2): 105-113.

Strehl, U., Leins, U., Goth, G., Klinger, C., Hinterberger, T. & Birbaumer, N. (2006).

Selfregulation of slow cortical potentials: a new treatment for children with Attention-

Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Trends Cogn. Sci., 3: 151-162

Tanaka, K., Kawakatsu, M. & Nemoto, I. (2008). Stochastic resonance in auditory steady

state responses in a magnetoenphalogram. Clin. Neurophys., 119: 2104-2110.

Thanos, S., Heiduschka, P. & Stupp, T. (2007). Implantable visual prostheses. Acta.

Neurochir. Suppl., 92 (2): 465-472.

Tiitinen, H., Sinkkonen, K., Reinikainen, K., Alho, J., Lavikainen, J. & Näätänen, R. (1993).

Selective attention enhances the auditory 40-Hz transient response in humans. Let. To

Nature, 364: 59-60.

Weber, R.E., Schwartz, A.B. (2000). Work toward real-time control of a cortical neural

prothesis. IEEE Trans. Rehabil. Eng., 8 (2): 196-198

Wolpaw, J.R., Ramoser, H., McFarland, D.J. & Pfurtscheller, G. (1998). EEG-based

communication: improved accuracy by response verification. IEEE Trans. Rehabil.

Eng., 6:326-333.

Wolpaw, J.R, Birbaumer, N., McFarland, D.J., Pfurtscheller, G. & Vaughan, T.M. (2002).

Brain-computer interfaces for communication and control. Clin. Neurophys., 113:767-

791.

Page 35: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Appendix A:

Figure 10: weight decompostion for individual subjects for code A. In this figure the four strongest weights (from left to right) of code A are plotted for each subject (top to bottom). The first weight is consistent across subjects, as already shown in figure 7. The second weight is less consistent across subjects, this is the same for the B code (not shown).

Page 36: Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour - Noise … · 2009-03-06 · Keywords: Brain Computer Interface (BCI), noise tagging, frequency tagging, ... in which the mental

Appendix B:

Figure 11: weight decompostion for individual subjects for the 42 Hz stimulus. In this figure the four strongest weights (from left to right) of the 42 Hz stimulus are plotted for each subject (top to bottom). The first weight is consistent across subjects, as already shown in figure 8. The second weight is not very consistent across subjects, this is the same for the 64 Hz stimulus (not shown).