cognitive rehabilitation for unilateral neglect

Upload: patricia-antequera-logopeda

Post on 28-Feb-2018

223 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    1/23

    Cognitive rehabilitation for unilateral neglect:Review

    Tom Manly

    Addenbrookes Hospital, Cambridge, UK

    Unilateral spatial neglect, a striking difficulty in paying attention to one side ofspace, is a common consequence of cerebro-vascular disease. Although mostpatients appear to recover from this deficit relatively quickly, chronic formsof the disorderusually resulting from right hemisphere damage and affectingleft spaceare associated with slowed motor recovery, poor response torehabilitation, and difficulties in many everyday activities. This article reviewsthe theoretical underpinnings andefficacyof rehabilitation techniques including;behavioural training in leftward visual scanning, eye-patching, encouragingmovement of the left limbs, and interventions designed to increase generalalertness.Recenthighly positive results frombrief adaptation trainingwithprism

    lenses are discussed. Neglect is a heterogeneous cluster of deficits that canarise following damage to a variety of brain structures. It is not yet clear whetherone rehabilitation technique will be appropriate for all patients/manifestations ofthe disorder, or whether combining different interventions may produce additivebenefits. There is growing evidence that chronic neglect is associated withindeed possibly fostered bylimitations in a number of non-spatial attentionalcapacities. Whether reduction of the spatial bias is sufficient, in itself, to improveoverall outcome for patients remains an open question.

    INTRODUCTION

    Unilateral spatial neglect is a curious difficulty in detecting, acting on or even

    imagining information from one side of space that cannot be fully explained by

    basic sensory loss. Numerous studies now show that its presence is associated

    Correspondenc e should be sent to Tom Manly, MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit,

    Box 58 Addenbrookes Hospital, Cambridge CB2 2EF, UK.

    Email: [email protected], Tel: + 44 1223 355 294, Fax: + 44 1223 516630

    NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION, 2002,12(4), 289310

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    2/23

    with grossly impaired everyday functioning and, importantly, with slowed

    recovery in apparently unconnected functions, including non-spatial attention

    and motor skills (Ben-Yishay, Diller, Gerstman, & Haas, 1968; Blanc-Garin,

    1994; Cherney et al., 2001; de Seze et al., 2001; Denes, Semenza, Stoppa, &

    Lis, 1982; Paolucci, Antonucci, Grasso, & Pizzamiglio, 2001a; Paolucci,

    Grasso, Antonucci, et al., 2001b; Robertsonet al., 1997; Rode,Rossetti, Badan,

    & Boisson, 2001; Sea, Henderson, & Cermack, 1993). Developing and

    evaluating interventions that might ameliorate the condition (or the effects of

    the condition on everyday activities) are therefore important clinical aims.

    Well over a thousand scientific papers on unilateral neglect have been

    published in the last 20 years, many focused on the careful analysis of different

    neglect phenomena and their relevance for understanding normal perceptionand attention. Although there is a degree of well-justified scepticism about the

    links between basic cognitive neuropsychological research and rehabilitation,

    it seems almost inconceivable that at least some of these findings would not

    provide useful pointers for intervention.

    UNILATERAL SPATIAL NEGLECT: PRESENTATION,

    AETIOLOGY, AND NATURAL HISTORYMost readers will be familiar with the striking presentation of unilateral

    neglect. Patients with apparently intact visual pathways1may fail to find visual

    targets presented on the left side of a page, may draw only the right half of a

    remembered image, copy the right half of a picture, or squash the numbers from

    1 to 12 into the right half of a drawn clock. They may fail to respond to auditory

    stimuli originating from the left, or to tactile stimuli presented to the left limb

    or the left side of a limb (Mattingley & Bradshaw, 1994). Famously, their

    recollection of well-known scenes (learned prior to the injury) may be strongly

    determined by their imagined point of view, omitting information on their left

    that they can subsequently recall when it is imagined on the right (Bisiach &

    Luzzatti, 1978). Motorically, patients may ignore their own left limbs or have

    difficulty reaching to the left (Mattingley & Driver, 1996; Mattingley, Phillips,

    & Bradshaw, 1994b). It is important to note, however, that patients vary enor-

    mously in the details of presentation and that neglect should not be viewed as

    a unitary disorder, but rather as a cluster of deficits characterised by lateralised

    spatial bias (Halligan & Marshall, 1994c; Robertson & Halligan, 1999). The

    implication of this diversity for rehabilitation (i e whether one technique

    290 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    3/23

    considered to dateindeed the majority of studies considered here have

    primarily examinedvisualneglect for an area immediately in front of the

    patient.

    Neglect has been observed following damage to a variety of brain regions

    including the parietal cortex (particularly the posterior region: Vallar & Perani,

    1986), frontal cortex (Damasio, Damasio, & Chang Chui, 1980; Mesulam,

    1981), basal ganglia (Bradshaw & Mattingley, 1995; Damasio et al., 1980),

    striatocapsular (Chung et al., 2000), putamen (Hier, Davis, Richardson, &

    Mohr, 1977), caudate (Kumral, Evyapan, & Balkir, 1999), and thalamus

    (Velasco, Velasco, Ogarrio, & Olvera, 1986). Recent analyses suggest that

    lesionswithin paraventricularwhite matter in the temporal lobe (Samuelsson et

    al., 1997) or superior temporal cortex (Karnath, Ferber, & Himmelbach, 2001)are most closely associated with the disorder. However, Hillis et al. emphasise

    that the location and extent of clearly lesioned tissue is not always the best

    predictor of disability (Hillis et al., 2000). In their study, the extent of

    blood hypoperfusion within cortical regions (the ischemic penumbra), in

    fact formed the better predictor of neglect. Given this diversity, neglect may

    be better understood as a cluster of lateralised spatial problems that can

    arise following damage toor dysfunction ina network of brain regions

    and normal psychological functions involved in the allocation of attention andrepresentation of space (Mesulam, 1981; Robertson & Halligan, 1999).

    Aspects of neglect have been reported in a surprisingly high proportion of

    stroke patients within the acute phase. When assessed 23 days post-stroke,

    Stone, Halligan, and Greenwood (1993) found evidence of neglect in 82% of

    right hemisphere patients and65%of left hemisphere patients (see also Stoneet

    al., 1991; Stone, Patel, Greenwood, & Halligan, 1992). The great majority of

    patients showing this acute form of neglect apparently recover rather quickly

    fromthe spatial biasalthough it should benoted that this is generally based ona relatively limited range of measures (Campbell & Oxbury, 1976).

    The group of patients who show more chronic forms of the disorder almost

    invariably have right hemisphere lesions and neglect left space. The reasons for

    this asymmetric recovery pattern remain controversial. It has been suggested,

    for example, that the left hemisphere is responsible for the allocation of

    attention to the right half of space, while the right hemisphere is capable

    of driving attention to the left or the right (Weintraub & Mesulam, 1987).

    Damage to the right hemisphere therefore leads to domination of a rightwardbias from the intact left hemisphere. The specialisation of regions of the left

    h i h f l (h l h l di l i h i h )

    COGNITIVE REHABILITATION FOR UNILATERAL NEGLECT 291

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    4/23

    attentional bias (e.g., towards right-sided targets on cancellation tasks) and

    failure todetect and orient to the wider spatial context. While helping to explain

    the chronic hemispheric asymmetry of neglect, these types of account are less

    useful in thinking about the presence of neglect in the acute stage in many left

    hemisphere patients, and the rapid recovery in some, but not all, patients with

    right hemisphere damage. An alternative (although not contradictory) view

    emphasises the role of damage to non-spatial right hemisphere capacities (see

    below) that may form the setting conditions that allow neglect to persist

    (Posner, 1993; Robertson & Manly, 1999). This account has drawn heavily on

    observed differences between right hemisphere patients who recover and those

    who do not (see Robertson & Halligan, 1999).

    The question of how patients recover is important in thinking about rehabili-tation. A number of studies suggest that some forms of apparently spontaneous

    recovery may in fact reflect behavioural compensation rather than a genuine

    reduction in the underlying spatial distortion. Goodale, Milner, Jakobson, and

    Carey (1990) analysed the reaching trajectories of right hemisphere patients

    who no longer showed neglect on standard bedside tests. Although the patients

    were able to reach towards targets with a similar accuracy to that of a control

    group, significant deviations into right space during the reach were still

    apparent. Similarly, a bias was apparent if the patients were given the morechallenging task of pointing to the midpoint between two objects (i.e., to a

    spatially-defined, rather than object-defined, region: see also Mattingley,

    Bradshaw, Bradshaw, & Nettleton, 1994a). Bartolomeo (2000) compared the

    performance of right hemisphere patients who currently showed neglect,

    patients who had recovered from neglect, and healthy age-matched controls on

    a computerised spatial task. As might be expected, while the neglect patients

    produced slow or inaccurate responses to left-sided targets, the recovered

    patients and the controls showed no consistent bias. When, however, theparticipants were asked to perform the task while simultaneously performing a

    second attentionally demanding (although non-spatial) test, a lateralised

    pattern re-emerged in the recovered patients (see also Robertson & Frasca,

    1992).The results suggest that, whatever the precise mechanism ofbehavioural

    adaptation in this group (e.g., compensatory voluntary leftward eye move-

    ments: Ladavas, Carletti, & Gori, 1994) these seem to require limited capacity

    top-down attentional resources. One obvious consequence of this is that

    patients with co-occurring difficulties in mobilising these resources are morelikely to show chronic spatial bias.

    If diff b i h ff i l

    292 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    5/23

    TRAINING LEFTWARD SCANNING AS A HABIT

    Many neglect patients can become aware of a left-sided stimulus or make some

    leftward eye or head movements if they are cued to do so (e.g., Riddoch &

    Humphreys, 1983). The difficulty is that such cuesas with simply informing

    the patient that they are missing things on the lefttend to have only very tran-

    sitory effects (Halligan & Marshall, 1994b). From at least the 1960s, therefore,

    therapists have tried to develop very systematic, behavioural programmes to

    encourage these behaviours. Perhaps the simplest example is in the context of

    reading. In reading text, neglect patients may miss the words on the left side of

    each line (see Young, Newcombe, & Ellis, 1991 for discussion of other forms

    of neglect dyslexia). A useful strategy is therefore to train patients to habitu-ally find the left side of the page before attempting to read each line (i.e., using

    the additional object on the left to re-frame the left side of the text line as an

    object on the right). This may be assisted byusing a highly salient cue (e.g., a

    red book-mark)as an attentional anchor at the left of the sheetand systemat-

    ically fading this cue as performance improves (Diller & Weinberg, 1977;

    Weinberg et al., 1977). Such training indeed appears to be successful in

    reducing left-word omissions. The problem is that very little spontaneous

    generalisation seems to occur. Lawson (1962), for example, describes how apatients improved readingwasso context-specific that it did not generalise to a

    different edition of the same book.

    Another quite widelyused technique has been to encourage patients to track

    a light that moves from right into left space, with the degree of leftward

    movement increasing as patients progress (Antonucci et al., 1995; Diller &

    Weinberg, 1977; Pizzamiglio et al., 1992; Ross, 1992; Wagenaar et al. 1992;

    Webster et al., 1984; Weinberg et al., 1977; Zihl, 2000). Again, although eye

    movements during the exercise improve, generalisation has been ratherdisappointing (Robertson, Gray, & McKenzie, 1988; Ross, 1992; Wagenaar et

    al., 1992; Webster et al., 1984). The results appear to concord with a general

    principle in rehabilitation that it is usually better to train real activities that are

    useful for patients (where a lack of generalisation is less important) rather than

    rely on transfer from abstract tasks (Wilson, 1996). Webster et al. (2001), for

    example, have shown that training that is specific to the task of navigating a

    wheelchair around complex environments produces very practical benefits.

    However, as neglect permeates so many aspects of a patients life, and trainingeach of these activities may be beyond the resources of most services, seeking

    h i d l i f i i b i l I i

    COGNITIVE REHABILITATION FOR UNILATERAL NEGLECT 293

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    6/23

    over a wide area) patients were initially given highly salient cues and therapist

    encouragement to look to the left. The cues were progressively faded as aware-

    ness of left space increased. As might be expected, performance on the trained

    tasks showed significant gains. Unlike previous studies, however, improve-

    ments were also noted on untrained tests and, crucially, on structured everyday

    activities. The reason for these positive results (which have been replicated in a

    fully randomised design: Antonucci et al., 1995) in comparison with previous

    studies is unclearalthough the authors suggest that the sheer duration of the

    training may have been a crucial factor.

    Many neglect patients show a curious lack of awareness foror outright

    denial oftheir disabilities (anosognosia). This has clear implications for their

    motivation to takepart in rehabilitationand their ability todetect progress. In aninteresting case study, Zoccolotti et al. (1992) have shown that the techniques

    developed in Rome (Antonucci et al., 1995; Pizzamiglio et al., 1992) can

    produce positive behaviour changes without necessarily effecting the patients

    acknowledgement of their difficulties.

    Although neglect can operate on object-based co-ordinates (the left side of

    an object being neglected regardless of the objects location: Driver &

    Halligan, 1991), for many patients the bodys midline appears to be a crucial

    marker of what is left and what is right. A potential alternative approach totraining leftward eye movements is, therefore, to train patients to rotate their

    torsos to the left in relation to their headposition. In this manner, when a patient

    looks straight ahead, more of the visual scene will fall to the right of the body

    midline. Remarkably, this simple intervention (trunk rotated left by 15) hasbeen shown to significantly reduce neglect (Karnath, Schenkel, & Fischer,

    1991). The difficulty is that, as with eye movements, patients are unlikely to

    spontaneously make such rotations, preferring to orient the eyes, head and

    trunk to the right.The rehabilitation studies discussed so far haveencouragedpatients to look

    (or turn) to the left duringsometrained activities. A different approach which,

    given the difficulty in achievinggeneralised results, has some merit, is to essen-

    tiallyforcepatients to look to the left duringallactivities.

    EYE PATCHING STUDIES

    Butter and Kirsch (1992) explored the value of using a patch to cover the right

    Gi h l i f h l i l ff d i

    294 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    7/23

    a dynamic relationship (activation in one hemisphere inhibits activity in the

    other). Damage to one hemisphere could therefore create an uneven competi-

    tion leading to a persistent orienting bias towards ipsilesional space (neglect).

    Unlike the visual pathways of the cortex, each colliculus primarily receives

    stimulation from the contralateral eye rather than hemifield. Preventing stimu-

    lation to the intact left superior colliculusbypatching the right eyemight, there-

    fore, release the right colliculus from this competitive inhibition and allow

    residual function to be better expressed (a benign form of the Sprague effect:

    Sprague, 1966).

    Butter and Kirsch asked 13 patients to wear right eye patches as they

    performed various spatial tasks. Significant reductions in spatial bias on those

    tasks were reported in 11 of the group. To potentially enhance this effect byincreasing stimulation to the right hemisphere, right eye patching was

    combined with a flickering visual stimulus on the left (dynamic left-sided

    visual cues had previously been shown to reduce neglect on computerised

    tasks). A study of 18 patients suggested that the combination of these tech-

    niques indeed produced greater reductions in neglect than either used in

    isolation.

    Subsequent work has provided rather more mixed results. Walker, Young,

    and Lincoln (1996) found improvements in three patients, no change intwo patients, andpoorerperformance in four of the patients in their study.

    Barrett, Crucian, Beversdorf, and Heilman (2001) also found that right eye

    patching could worsen neglect, and (in a single case study) that patching of the

    left eye could, bizarrely, produce improvements. The results stress the impor-

    tance of ruling out non-specific effects of interventions (e.g., heightened

    arousal caused by novelproceduressee later).They may also represent a case

    where one rehabilitation technique has considerable value for some patients,

    while being ineffective or counterproductive for others. Further work exam-ining which patients are likely to benefit (e.g., in terms of their presentation of

    neglect, their primary lesion location, time since injury or comorbidity) is

    required.

    Beis, Andre, Baumgarten, and Challier (1999) examined the effect of

    blocking information from the right hemifieldrather than the right eye. Patients

    were asked to wear glasses with the right half of each lens masked off for

    approximately 12 hours a day over 3 monthsthat is well over 1000 hours.

    When compared with right eye patching, the patients showed significantlyincreased spontaneous eye movements to the left and, encouragingly, signifi-

    i i i i i f d il li i Th i h i f

    COGNITIVE REHABILITATION FOR UNILATERAL NEGLECT 295

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    8/23

    external stimulation, is in a poorer position to competitively suppress residual

    activity in the right.

    RE-DISTORTING SPACE

    Prism lenses

    It has been demonstrated that some of the phenomena associated with neglect

    can be related to a distorted or rotated sense of ones location within space

    (Karnath et al., 1991). Accordingly, researchers have examined the effect of

    using interventions that, in healthy individuals, produce a similar distortion,and using these to correct the pathological bias of neglect.

    Wearing prism glasses creates an immediate distortion of space. If they are

    worn for long enough, however, the brain adapts (particularly if actions

    are performed). Subsequent removal leads to a refractory period during which

    the perception of space (as indexed by accuracy in reaching) is distorted in the

    opposite direction. In a randomised group design, Rossetti et al. (1998) asked

    patients to wear prism lenses that distorted space to the rightessentially

    giving them a form of hyper-neglect.After about five minutes of beingaskedtomake reaches towards targets, the lenses were removed.Thepatients showed

    the adaptation effect, their straight ahead reaches now being to the left of

    the pretreatment baseline. Remarkably, this brief exposure was sufficient to

    improve performance on neuropsychological tests relative to thecontrol group.

    These improvements were still apparent when both groups were re-tested

    2 hours later. Rossetti et al. suggested that the very strong error signal that

    resulted from the patients observing their own inaccuracy while wearing the

    prism lenses may have been sufficient to effectively re-set the perceptual/motoric representation of space.

    These positive effects have been replicated in a recent controlled study that,

    in addition to using a wider range of measures, examined the persistence of the

    gains at intervals up to 5 weeks post-treatment. Frassinetti et al. (2002) offered

    seven right hemisphere patients with chronic neglect 20 sessions of prism

    adaptation therapy (two per day over 2 weeks, each of 20 minutes duration). At

    the beginning of each therapy session, before wearing the prism lenses, the

    patients were asked to point to 60 visual targets presented at midline and tothe left and right of midline. Subsequently, when wearing the prism spectacles

    ( i 10 i h d hif ) h i i k d i 90

    296 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    9/23

    stages of each session revealed a significant leftward deviation in pointing

    attributable to prism adaptation for six of the seven patients.

    When compared with a pretreatment baseline, significant reductions in

    neglect were apparent on Conventional and the Behavioural subtests of the

    Behavioural Inattention Test (Wilson, Cockburn, & Halligan, 1987), a reading

    measure (Ladavas, Shallice, & Zanella, 1997), and tasks requiring patients to

    name objects located around a room and to reach for objects on a table. A trend

    for improvement was also apparent on the Fluff Test (in which patients,

    while blindfolded, are asked to find stickersplacedon the left sideof theirbody:

    Cocchini, Beschin, & Jehkonen, 2001). These improvements were apparent at

    re-tests administered two days, one week and five weeks after treatment.

    Evidence in support of a specific effect of the prisms camefrom a control group(based at another hospital, but otherwise matched on age, time since insult and

    Behavioural Inattention Test [BIT] performance) who showed no significant

    improvements despite being re-tested at the same intervals. The fact that

    performance gains were also absent in the treatedpatient whofailedto show the

    normal adaptation effects to the prisms is also consistent with a rather specific

    effect. Importantly, this study shows that the benefits of prism therapy are not

    limited to the control of motor behaviour but appear to exert a more general,

    higher level effect upon patients representation of space. If this pattern ofresults is apparent in other studies, this very practical intervention should

    clearly have a major impact on clinical approaches to neglect.

    Optokinetic stimulation, caloric vestibularstimulation, and neck muscle vibration

    Exposure to a background display of coherently moving dots causes involun-

    tary eye movements (nystagmus) and a distortion to the subjective midline.Pizzamiglio et al. (1990) have shown that the performance of neglect patients

    on spatial tests canbe significantly enhanced by such optokinetic stimulation

    although the effects appear to be broadly restricted to when the movement is

    actually present. Other routes to inducing temporary subjective shifts in

    midline include caloric vestibular stimulation (in which warm and cold water

    are used to create a temperature imbalance between the ears: Cappa, Sterzi,

    Vallar, & Bisiach, 1987; Rubens, 1985), and posterior neck muscle vibration

    (thought to create a motor illusion that the head is turned: Karnath, 1994;Karnath, Christ, & Hartje, 1993).

    A fi l h f h ff d h i li i f

    COGNITIVE REHABILITATION FOR UNILATERAL NEGLECT 297

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    10/23

    LIMB ACTIVATION

    The term attention is usually used with reference to some form of limited

    capacity selectionin other words mechanisms that can promote a subset of

    available stimuli to dominate awareness (e.g., because they are task relevant orinherently salient) while suppressing that which is irrelevant or unchanging. In

    accounting for why a potentiallyhighly parallel perceptual system is ultimately

    reduced toa narrow channelof awareness, a numberofauthorshave argued that

    its key role lies in allowing coherentaction(e.g., Allport, 1992; Rizzolatti &

    Camarda, 1987). As we are generally only capable of responding to one or two

    objects at a time (not least because of the limited number of limbs at our

    disposal), selection is a necessary aspect of the system. In line with this

    argument, our awareness of where things are may be modulated by what weare intending to do, or what we are intending to do it with. In the context of

    neglect, for example, Robertson, Nico, and Hood (1995a) found that spatial

    bias could be modulated by whether a patient intended to pick up, or to point to,

    an object.

    The finding that has been most relevant to rehabilitation was that (at least

    some) neglect patients showed marked reduction in neglect if they used their

    left hands to perform a task (Halligan, Manning, & Marshall, 1990; Halligan &

    Marshall, 1989; Joanette & Brouchon, 1984; Joanette, Brouchon, Gauthier, &Samson, 1986). In a series of single case and group studies, Robertson and

    colleagues (Robertson & North, 1992, 1993, 1994; Robertson, North, &

    Geggie, 1992; Robertson, Tegnr, Goodrich, & Wilson, 1994a) subsequently

    showed that:

    1. The reduction invisualneglectdid not depend on the patient being able to

    see the moving left hand.

    2. The left hand did not need to be performing the spatial task for the effectto occur. Repeated finger movements of the left hand facilitated a purely

    perceptual test of naming letters in a spatial array.

    3. An interaction of movement of the left hand and thelocationof that

    movement to the left of the body midline appeared to be necessary to

    generate the effect (movement of the left hand in right space, or

    movement of the right hand in left space did not produce significant

    gains).

    4. Simultaneous bi-manual movements (regardless of the location of thehands) abolished any benefits of left hand movement on spatial

    298 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    11/23

    6. Although initial results in which the experimenter moved the fingers on

    the patients left hand suggested that passive movements did not produce

    benefitssubsequent research with larger amplitude passive movement

    of the arm has reported positive results (Frassinetti, Rossi, & Ladavas,

    2001)

    7. Left limb activation effects have not be found in all patients (see also

    Brunila et al., 2002)

    The results of these studies have been useful in clarifying the potential mecha-

    nisms of the left limb activation effect. The benefits of unseen movements,

    for example, clearly suggest that the effect is not simply one ofvisualcueing to

    the left. The failure to show benefits in the bi-manual movement conditionsimilarly suggest that a generalised alerting (caused by the difficulty or novelty

    of using the left hand) forms an unlikely account (see later for discussion of

    alerting effects on neglect). If there were a simple facilitation of awareness

    from intended movement in a particular location, it might be expected that

    moving therighthand within left space would also produce advantages

    which was not the case. It seems likely, therefore, that the additive effects of

    hand and location of movement are necessary, perhaps in inducing a more

    general activating effect within the right hemisphere (Robertson et al.,1994a; see also Kinsbourne, 1993).

    Many patients with neglect have dense left-sided hemiplegia. Left limb

    activation is therefore unlikely to hold much rehabilitative value for this group.

    For other patients, however, there may be under-use of residual function in the

    left limbs for attentional reasons (motor neglect: Sterzi et al., 1993). Encour-

    aging use of the left limb for these patients may produce benefits for both

    perceptual and motor function.

    Robertson et al. (1992) developed a portable neglect alert device to cuepatients to move their left hands. The device consisted of a hand-held button

    which, if not pressed within a particular interval (generally around 8 s) would

    trigger a buzzer. In a series of case studies using the device (Robertson, Hogg,

    & McMillan, 1998a; Robertson et al., 1992), improvements across a range of

    tasks (both spatial tests and everyday activities) were demonstrated. Encourag-

    ingly, for some of the patients, the improvements were well maintained

    following the end of formal training. A lasting effect was also observed by

    Wilson, Manly,Coyle, andRobertson (2000).Here, the patienthaddifficulty inadequately completing his morning self-care routine (often failing to wash or

    d h l f id fhi b d ) F ll i b li i d i hi h h b

    COGNITIVE REHABILITATION FOR UNILATERAL NEGLECT 299

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    12/23

    maintained for at least another 10 days, and the patient appeared to be making

    more spontaneous use of his left hand. It is possible, therefore, that the

    increased awareness of left space induced by the limb movement itself

    promotes greater use of the left hand,establishing a beneficial feedbackloop.

    If patients have some movement in their left hand or arm, and if the

    movement does not cause excessive discomfort, there are a number of advan-

    tages to this rehabilitation technique. Firstly, it is a very concrete activity to ask

    a patient toperform, and one that is clearly observable. It is therefore possible to

    get an estimate of whether it is likely to be beneficial in any given case through

    looking for improvements on spatial tasks concurrent with the onset of

    movement. It also lends itself to the automaticmonitoring and cueing described

    by Robertson et al. (1992) and to the verbal encouragement of therapists andcarers. Most importantly, it seems to lead to improvements in underlying

    spatial awareness that generalise to different tasks. Beneficial effects for the

    recovery of hemiplegic limbs have resulted from restraint therapy in which

    the patients unaffected limbs are temporarily restrained to prevent their use

    (Taub & Wolf, 1997). The value of these techniques for rehabilitation in

    neglect, through encouraging greater use of the left arm, has not yet been fully

    addressed.

    Although many of the studies reviewed in this article have focused on aspecific technique that allows clear interpretation of positive results, this

    should not be taken to imply that the best clinical outcome is likely to emerge

    from the application of anyof the techniques in isolation. Accordingly, Brunila

    et al. (2002) recently examined the effect of combining limb activation training

    with a progressive visual scanning programme. Four patients were encouraged

    to move either their left arm or hand (or if that was not possible, shoulder)

    during exercises including naming objects in a spatial array and cancellation

    tasks. Compared with a nine week repeated assessment baseline, the 12sessions of therapy produced significant improvements in reading, letter

    cancellation and in copying a complex figure, that were well maintained over

    the next 3 weeks. Again, the authors note considerable variability in the

    patients response, strongly suggesting that careful evaluation of the potential

    benefits of any technique for a given patient shouldbeexplored at the outset.

    TARGETTING NON-SPATIAL FUNCTIONS

    Right hemisphere lesions are associated with disproportionate problems with a

    l f i l i l i i ( i l d l i

    300 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    13/23

    Shallice, & McCarthy, 1987). There is, furthermore, a degree of convergence

    between neuropsychological and functional imaging literature on the particular

    importance of a right hemisphere fronto-parietal network in mediating these

    functions (Knight et al., 1981; Lewin et al., 1996; Pardo, Fox, & Raichle, 1991;

    Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Sturm et al., 1999; Wilkins et al., 1987). On this

    basis, it would be expected that many patients with persistent neglectwho

    almost always have right hemisphere lesionswould be vulnerable to these

    problems. This certainly seems to be the case. Many patients appear to be

    drowsy, unresponsive and to have difficulty in maintaining their focus on any

    task, regardless of its spatial content. These clinical observations have been

    confirmed in experimental studies. Robertson et al. (1997) asked a large group

    of patients (57 with right- and 30 with left-hemisphere lesions, mainly fromstroke), and age-matched controls, to perform a particularly tedious nonspatial

    tone counting measure (Robertson, Ward, Ridgeway, & Nimmo-Smith, 1994b;

    Robertson, Ward, Ridgeway, & Nimmo-Smith, 1996; Wilkins et al., 1987).

    Although all patients were mildly impaired relative to the control group, it was

    the right hemisphere patients with neglect who performed disproportionately

    poorlyindeed it was possible to make a good guess as to which right hemi-

    sphere patients would show neglect based on the score on this test alone.

    Similarly, it has been shown that neglect patients, when compared withrighthemispherepatients without neglect, have difficulty in mentally bridging the

    intervals between a cue and a target in reaction time tasks (Samuelsson et al.,

    1988).

    Neglect is a rather volatile condition, changing in apparent severity not

    simply on different tasks but on the same task at different times (sometimes to

    the exasperation of experimental psychologists looking for reliable effects).

    This variability is consistentwith factors (suchas level of arousal/alertness)not

    simply being associated with neglect at a general level, but exerting adirectmodulatory influenceon spatial awareness2.

    This conclusion receives some support from a recent pharmacological study

    in which apparently recovered stroke patients (including three patients with

    COGNITIVE REHABILITATION FOR UNILATERAL NEGLECT 301

    2 This raises the intriguing possibility that other groups with low levels of alertness may also

    be vulnerable to spatial bias. In fact there is growing evidence that at least some children with the

    diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (in which poor sustained attention/lowarousal are central features) may show exactly this pattern of left-sided inattention (Nigg,

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    14/23

    right hemisphere lesions) were given midazolam (a GABA antagonist) that

    produces mild sedation. The period of the drugs effect (about 2 hours) was

    associated with significant increases in left neglect and, interestingly, in left

    hemiplegia (Lazar et al., 2002).

    Robertson and colleagues examined the link between arousal and neglect

    behaviourally using a prior entry computerised task. If healthy individuals

    are cued to attend to the side of a display onto which two simultaneous

    lateralised targets are subsequently presented, despite maintaining a central

    fixation, they will tend to report the stimulus on the attended side as having

    occurred firstin other words attention gives that stimulus prior entry into

    subsequent processing (Stelmach & Herdman, 1991). This technique has been

    used to calibrate the pathological attentional bias of neglect (Rorden,Mattingley, Karnath, & Driver, 1996). In their study Robertson, Mattingley,

    Rorden, and Driver (1998b)preceded occasional trials of the computerised task

    with a loud, alerting tone. Although the tone had no predictive value for the

    stimulus presentation order, its presence was associated with an abolitionor

    even reversalof neglect. Heilman, Watson, Valenstein, and Goldberg (1987)

    have suggested that there are two routes to developing and maintaining an

    alert or ready-to-respond state. The first is exogenously driven by the

    detection of intense, novel or salient stimuli. The second, suggested by recip-rocal cortical projections to the brain stem, involves the internal (endogenous)

    self-generation and maintenance of this state. Robertson et al.s results with

    loud tones suggests that, for neglect patients, the former may be relativelyintact

    while the second is deficient.

    Although it may be possible to use some form of external alerting during the

    performance of tasks (e.g., through presenting tones or the encouragement of a

    therapist of carer), as the technique appears to rely on the novelty as well as the

    salience of the tones, this may not be practical as a long-term solution. Twopotential alternatives for harnessing this effect are to try and train patients in

    self-maintaining an alert state during an important activity, and, given the

    results of Lazar et al. (2002), to use medication that boosts rather than reduces

    alertness.

    Robertson and colleagues (1995b)explored the first possibility, adapting a

    technique for the verbal regulationof attention firstdescribed byMeichenbaum

    and Goodman (1971; see also Luria, 1963; Meichenbaum & Cameron, 1973).

    During training, patients with neglect were asked to perform a number of rathertedious tasks (e.g., sorting coins or shapes). With their prior consent, every

    40 d h h i ld l d i b b i h d k d

    302 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    15/23

    the phrase out loud, and finally to internalise this instruction. Although there

    was no on-line monitoring (e.g., EEG) of the effect on alertness levels,

    improvements in performance on a nonspatial tone-counting sustained

    attention task, consistent with such enhancement, were found. Most import-

    antly, as a group the patients showed significant improvements on a number of

    untrained spatial neglect measures.

    MEDICATION

    In animals, behaviours analogous to unilateral neglect are associated with

    disruption to the dopamine system (Corwin, Burcham, & Hix, 1996). Fleet,

    Valenstein, Watson, and Heilman (1987) administered bromocriptine (adopamine agonist) to two patients. This stimulant was associated with

    improvements in some, but not all neglect measuresimprovements that

    tended to reverse when the medication was withdrawn. In a more recent case

    study, Hurford, Stringer, and Jann (1998) compared the effects of methyl-

    phenidate with bromocriptine. They report that, although methylphenidate

    produced benefits compared with the no-treatment condition, bromocriptine

    produced the stronger results. These preliminary studies suggest that stimulant

    medication may indeed have a role within rehabilitation for neglect, althoughclearly larger fully controlled trials are required.

    FUTURE DIRECTIONS

    Although most patients who show unilateral neglect in the acute post-stroke

    phase recover, the presence of chronic neglect in the less fortunate minority

    is associated with slowed recovery and poor outcome. This review has consid-

    ered broad approaches to the rehabilitation of neglect; training (or forcing)leftward scanning behaviour, prism adaptation, left limb activation; and inter-

    ventions designed to improve general alertness. Each has produced positive

    results that have shown some generalisation to untrained tasks. It is notable, in

    terms of the potential mutual benefits between cognitive neuropsychology and

    rehabilitation, that at a number of these approaches have emerged from studies

    of the underlying nature of the disorder (rather than direct treatment of the most

    salient symptoms)and have furthermore raised interesting questions for

    further academic study. A number of important questions, however, remain.In order for these effects to be considered rehabilitation, it is necessary to

    h fi l h h i i li h f k h i

    COGNITIVE REHABILITATION FOR UNILATERAL NEGLECT 303

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    16/23

    effects of distraction and dual tasking on apparently recovered neglect have

    been elegantly demonstrated by Bartolomeo (2000). Secondly, it is necessary

    to show that the effects last. The majority of studies have to date shown effects

    lasting for some weeks but the longer-term consequences of the interventions

    remain largely unknown.

    In discussing the negative consequences of neglect on recovery and

    outcome, the assumption has been that it is theneglectthat is mediating this

    effect. As discussed, however, neglect is associated with a cluster of other

    deficits, perhaps particularly with the nonspatial attention systems of the right

    hemisphere. Whether a reduction in the ostensive behavioural manifestations

    of neglectwithout improvements in these other systemsis sufficient to

    produce real changes in prognosis remains an open question. There is certainlygrowing evidence on the role of more general executive or attentional

    capacities in mediating recovery and functional adaptation following brain

    injury. It may well be that improvements in our understanding of these areas

    will offer new insights into the persistence and remediation of spatial and other

    deficits.

    In the majority of studies presented here, there has been considerable indi-

    vidual variation in responses to the interventions. It is also true that few of the

    studies have directly contrasted one technique with another at a group levelor examined possible additive effects of combining treatments. As discussed,

    neglect is a very heterogeneous condition that can arise following damage to a

    variety of brain structures. While the practicalities of applying the different

    approaches clearly suggests a sensible order in which to investigate their

    usefulness, an experimental suck-it-and-see approach with each individual

    patient remains the best policy.

    REFERENCES

    Allport, A. (1992). Attention and control. In D.E. Meyer & S. Komblum (Eds.),Attention and

    performance(Vol. XIV). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Antonucci, G., Guariglia, C., Judica, A., Magnotti, L., Paolucci, S., Pizzamiglio, L., &

    Zoccolotti , P. (1995). Effectiveness of neglect rehabilitation in a randomized group study.

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,17, 383389.

    Barrett, A.M., Crucian, G.P., Beversdorf, D.Q., & Heilman, K.M. (2001). Monocular patchingmay worsen sensory-attentiona l neglect: A case report.Archives of Physical Medicine and

    304 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    17/23

    Ben-Yishay, Y., Diller, L., Gerstman, L., & Haas, A. (1968). The relationship between

    impersistence, intellectual function and outcome of rehabilitation in patients with left

    hemiplegia.Neurology,18, 852861.

    Bisiach, E., & Luzzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral neglect of representationa l space.Cortex,14,

    129133.Blanc-Garin, J. (1994). Patterns of recovery from hemiplegia following stroke.Neuropsycho-

    logical Rehabilitation,4, 359385.

    Boller, F., Howes, D., & Patten, D.H. (1970). A behavioura l evaluation of brain scan results with

    neuropathalogical findings.Lancet,1, 11431146.

    Bradshaw, J.L., & Mattingley, J.B. (1995).Clinical neuropsychology : Behavioural and brain

    science . San Diego: Academic Press.

    Brunila, T., Lincoln, N., Lindell, A., Tenovuo, O., & Hmlinen, H. (2002). Experiences of

    combined visual training and arm activation in the rehabilitatio n of unilateral visual neglect:

    A clinical study.Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 2740.

    Butter, C.,& Kirsch, N. (1992). Combined andseparate effects of eye patching and visual stimula-

    tion on unilatera l neglect following stroke.Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,

    73, 11331139.

    Campbell, D.C., & Oxbury, J.M. (1976). Recovery from unilateral spatial neglect?Cortex,12,

    303312.

    Cappa, S.F., Sterzi, R., Vallar, G., & Bisiach, E. (1987). Remission of hemineglect and

    anosognosia during vestibular stimulation.Neuropsychologia,25, 775782.

    Cherney, L.R., Halper, A.S., Kwasnica, C.M., Harvey, R.L., & Zhang, M. (2001). Recovery of

    functional status after right hemisphere stroke: Relationship with unilateral neglect.Archives

    of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,82(3), 322328.Chung, C.S., Caplan, L.R., Yamamoto, Y., Chang, H.M., Lee, S.J., Song, H.J., Lee, H.S., Shin,

    H.K., & Yoo, K.M. (2000). Striatocapsular haemorrhage .Brain,123, 15801862.

    Cocchini, G., Beschin, N., & Jehkonen , M. (2001). The Fluff Test: A simple task to assess body

    representation neglect.Neuropsychological Rehabilitation,11, 1731.

    Corwin, J.V., Burcham, K.J., & Hix, G.I. (1996). Apomorphine produces an acute dose-

    dependent therapeutic effect on neglect produced by unilateral destruction of the posterior

    parietal cortex in rats.Behavioural Brain Research,79, 4149.

    Damasio, A.R., Damasio, H., & Chang Chui, H. (1980). Neglect following damage to the frontal

    lobe or basal ganglia.Neuropsychologia ,19, 123132.

    De Renzi, E., & Faglioni, P. (1965). The comparative efficiency of intelligence and vigilance tests

    in detecting hemispheric cerebral damage.Cortex,1, 410433.

    de Seze, M., Wiart, L., Bon-Saint-Come, A., Debelleix, X., de Seze, M., Joseph, P.A., Mazaux,

    J.M., & Barat, M. (2001). Rehabilitation of postural disturbance s of hemiplegic patients by

    using trunk control retraining during exploratory exercises.Archives of Physical Medicine

    and Rehabilitation,82(6), 793800.

    Denes, G., Semenza, C., Stoppa, E., & Lis, A. (1982). Unilateral spatial neglect and recovery from

    hemiplegia. A follow-up study.Brain,105, 543552.

    Diller, L., & Weinberg, J. (1977). Hemi-inattention in rehabilitation : The evolution of a rational

    rehabilitatio n program. In E.A. Weinstein & R.P. Friedland (Eds.),Advances in neurology(Vol. 10). New York: Raven Press.

    bl l b li h i di i d il

    COGNITIVE REHABILITATION FOR UNILATERAL NEGLECT 305

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    18/23

    Frassinetti, F., Angeli, V., Meneghello, F., Avanzi, S., & Ladavas, E. (2002). Long-lasting

    amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation.Brain,125, 608623.

    Frassinetti, F., Rossi, M., & Ladavas, E. (2001). Passive limb movements improve visual neglect.

    Neuropsychologia,39(7), 725733.

    Gainotti, G. (1972). Emotional behaviour and hemispheric side of lesion.Cortex,8, 4155.Goodale, M.A., Milner, A.D., Jakobson, L.S., & Carey, D.P. (1990). Kinematic analysis of limb

    movements in neuropsychologica l research: subtle deficits and recovery of function.

    Canadian Journal of Psychology,44, 180195.

    Halligan, P.W., Manning, L., & Marshall, J. (1990). Hemispheric activation vs. spatio-motor

    cueing in visual neglect: A case study.Neuropsychologia ,29(2), 165176.

    Halligan, P.W., & Marshall, J.C. (1989). Laterality of motor response in visuo-spatia l neglect : a

    case study.Neuropsychologia ,27, 13011307.

    Halligan, P.W., & Marshall, J.C. (1994a). Focal and global attention modulate the expression of

    visuo-spatial neglect: a case study.Neuropsychologia ,32, 1321.

    Halligan, P.W., & Marshall, J.C. (1994b). Right-sided cueing can ameliorate left neglect.Neuropsychological Rehabilitation,4, 6373.

    Halligan, P.W., & Marshall, J.C. (1994c). Spatial neglect: Position papers on theory and practice

    [special issue].Neuropsychological Rehabilitation,4(2).

    Heilman, K.M., Schwartz, H.D., & Watson, R.T.(1978). Hypoarousal in patients with the neglect

    syndrome and emotional indifference .Neurology ,28, 229232.

    Heilman, K.M., Watson, R.T., Valenstein, E., & Goldberg, M.E. (1987). Attention: Behaviour

    and neural mechanisms. In F. Plum (Ed.),Handbook of physiology(Vol. 5, pp. 461481).

    Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society.

    Hier, D.B., Davis, K.R., Richardson, E.P., & Mohr, J.P. (1977). Hypertensive putaminal hemor-

    rhage.Annals of Neurology,1, 152159.

    Hillis, A.E., Barker, P.B., Beauchamp, N.J., Gordon, B., & Wityk, R.J. (2000). MR perfusion

    imaging reveals regions of hypoperfusion associated with aphasia and neglect.Neurology, 55,

    782788.

    Howes, D., & Boller, F. (1975). Simple reaction time:Evidence for focal impairment from lesions

    of the right hemisphere.Brain,98, 317322.

    Hurford, P., Stringer, A., & Jann, B. (1998). Neuropharmacologi c treatment of hemineglect :

    A case report comparing bromocriptine and methylphenidate.Archives of Physical Medicine

    and Rehabilitation, 79(3), 346349.

    Joanette , Y., & Brouchon, M. (1984). Visual allesthesia in manual pointing: Some evidence for asensori-motor cerebral organization .Brain and Cognition,3, 152165.

    Joanette, Y., Brouchon, M., Gauthier, L., & Samson, M. (1986). Pointing with left versus right

    hand in left visual field neglect.Neuropsychologia ,24, 391396.

    Karnath, H.O. (1994). Subjective body orientation in neglect and the interactive contribution of

    neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation.Brain,117, 10011012.

    Karnath, H.O., Christ, K., & Hartje, W. (1993). Decrease of contralateral neglect by neck muscle

    vibration and spatial orientation of trunk midline.Brain,116, 383396.

    Karnath, H.O., Ferber, S., & Himmelbach, M. (2001). Spatial awareness is a function of the

    temporal not the posterior parietal lobe.Nature,411, 950953.

    Karnath, H.O., Schenkel, P., & Fischer, B. (1991). Trunk orientation as the determining factor ofthe contralateral deficit in the neglect syndrome and as the physical anchor of the internal

    306 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    19/23

    Kumral, E., Evyapan, D., & Balkir, K. (1999). Acute caudate vascular lesions. Stroke ,30,

    100108.

    Ladavas, E., Carletti, M., & Gori, G. (1994). Automatic and voluntary orienting of attention in

    patients with visual neglect: horizontal and vertical dimensions. Neuropsychologia, 32,

    11951208.Ladavas, R., Shallice, T., & Zanella, M.T. (1997). Preserved semantic access in neglect dyslexia.

    Neuropsychologia,35, 257270.

    Lawson, I.R. (1962). Visual-spatia l neglect in lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere: A study in

    recovery.Neurology ,12, 2333.

    Lazar, R., Fitzsimmons, B., Marshall, R., Berman,M.,Bustillo, M.,Young,W., Mohr, J., Shah, J.,

    & Robinson, J. (2002). Reemergence of stroke deficits with midazolam challenge.Stroke,

    33(1), 283285.

    Lewin, J.S., Friedman, L., Wu, D., Miller, D.A., Thompson, L.A., Klein, S.K., Wise, A.L.,

    Hedera, P., Buckley, P., Meltzer, H., Friedland, R. P.,& Duerk, J. L. (1996). Cortical localiza-

    tion of human sustained attention: Detection with functional MR using a visual vigilanceparadigm.Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography,20(5), 695701.

    Luria, A.R. (1963).Restoration of function after brain injury. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Manly, T., Robertson, I.H., & Verity, C. (1997). Developmenta l unilateral neglect: A single case

    study.Neurocase ,3(1), 1929.

    Mattingley, J.B., & Bradshaw, J.L. (1994). Can tactile neglect occur at an intra-limb level?

    Vibrotactile reaction times in patients with right hemisphere damage.Behavioural Neurology,

    7, 6777.

    Mattingley, J.B., Bradshaw, J.L., Bradshaw, J.A., & Nettleton, N.C. (1994a). Residual right

    attentional bias after apparent recovery from right hemisphere damage: implications for a

    multicomponen t model of neglect.Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,57,

    597604.

    Mattingley, J.B., & Driver, J. (1996). Distinguishing sensory and motor deficits after parietal

    damage: An evaluation of response selection biases in unilateral neglect. In P. Thier &

    H.O. Karnath (Eds.),Parietal lobe contributions to orientation in 3D space(pp. 309338).

    Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

    Mattingley, J.B., Phillips, J.G., & Bradshaw, J.L. (1994b). Impairments of movement execution

    in unilateral neglect: A kinematic analysis of directiona l bradykinesia .Neuropsychologia, 32,

    11111134.

    Mattingley, J.B., Robertson, I.H., & Driver, J. (1998). Modulation of covert visual attention byhand movement: Evidence from parietal extinction after right-hemispher e damage.

    Neurocase ,4, 245253.

    Meichenbaum, D., & Cameron, R. (1973). Training schizophrenic s to talk to themselves: a means

    of developing attentional control.Behaviour Therapy,4, 515534.

    Meichenbaum, D., & Goodman, J. (1971). Training impulsive children to talk to themselves: a

    means of developing self-control .Journal of Abnormal Psychology,77, 115126.

    Mesulam, M.M. (1981). A cortical network fordirected attention andunilateral neglect.Annals of

    Neurology ,10, 309325.

    Nigg, J.T.,Swanson, J.M., & Hinshaw, S.P. (1997). Covert spatial attention in boys with attention

    deficit hyperactivity disorder: Lateral effects, methylphenidat e response and results forparents.Neuropsychologia,35(2), 165176.

    COGNITIVE REHABILITATION FOR UNILATERAL NEGLECT 307

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    20/23

    Pardo, J.V., Fox, P.T., & Raichle, M.E. (1991). Localization of a human system for sustained

    attention by positron emission tomography.Nature,349, 6164.

    Pizzamiglio, L., Antonucci, G., Judica, A., Montenero, P., Rrazzano, C., & Zoccolotti, P.

    (1992). Cognitive rehabilitation of the hemineglect disorder in chronic-patient s with

    unilateral right brain-damage .Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14(6),901923.

    Pizzamiglio, L., Frasca, R., Guariglia, C., Incoccia, C., & Antonucci, G. (1990). Effect of

    optokinetic stimulation in patients with visual neglect.Cortex,26, 535540.

    Posner, M.I. (1993). Interaction of arousal and selection in the posterior attention network. In

    A. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness and control (pp.

    390405). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Posner, M.I., Inhoff, A.W., & Friedrich, F.J. (1987). Isolating attentional systems: A cognitive-

    anatomical analysis.Psychobiology,15, 107121.

    Riddoch, M.J., & Humphreys, G.W. (1983). The effect of cueing on unilateral neglect.

    Neuropsychologia,21, 589599.Rizzolatti, G., & Camarda, R. (1987). Neural circuits for spatial attention and unilatera l neglect. In

    M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Neurophysiologica l and neuropsychologica l aspects of neglect.

    Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Robertson, I.H.,& Frasca, R. (1992). Attentional load andvisual neglect.International Journal of

    Neuroscience,62, 4556.

    Robertson, I., Gray, J., & McKenzie, S. (1988). Microcomputer-base d cognitive rehabilitation of

    visual neglect: three multiple-baseline single-case studies.Brain Injury,2, 151163.

    Robertson, I.H., & Halligan, P.W. (1999). Spatial neglect: A clinical handbook for diagnosis and

    treatment. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Robertson, I.H., Hogg, K., & McMillan, T.M. (1998a). Rehabilitation of unilateral neglect:

    Improving function by contralesiona l limb activation.Neuropsychologica l Rehabilitation,

    8(1), 1929.

    Robertson, I.H., & Manly, T. (1999). Sustained attention deficits in time and space. In

    G.W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A.M. Treisman (Eds.),Attention, space, and action: Studies

    in cognitive neuroscienc e. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Robertson, I.H., Manly, T., Beschin, N., Haeske-Dewick, H., Hmberg, V., Jehkonen, M.,

    Pizzamiglio, L., Shiel, A., Weber, E., & Zimmerman, P. (1997). Auditory sustained attention

    is a marker of unilateral spatial neglect.Neuropsychologia ,35, 15271532.

    Robertson, I.H., Mattingley, J.M., Rorden, C., & Driver, J. (1998b). Phasic alerting of neglectpatients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness.Nature,395, 169172.

    Robertson, I.H., Nico, D., & Hood, B. (1995a). The intention to act improves unilatera l neglect :

    Two demonstrations .Neuroreport,7, 246248.

    Robertson, I.H., & North, N. (1992). Spatio-motor cueing in unilateral neglect: The role of

    hemispace, hand and motor activation.Neuropsychologia ,30, 553563.

    Robertson, I.H., & North, N. (1993). Active and passive activation of left limbs: Influence on

    visual and sensory neglect.Neuropsychologia ,31, 293300.

    Robertson, I.H., & North, N. (1994). One hand is better than two: Motor extinction of left hand

    advantage in unilateral neglect.Neuropsychologia ,32, 111.

    Robertson, I.H., North, N., & Geggie, C. (1992). Spatio-motor cueing in unilateral neglect: Threesingle case studies of its therapeutic effectiveness .Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and

    308 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    21/23

    Robertson, I.H., Ward, A., Ridgeway, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994b). Test of Everyday

    Attention. Bury St Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company.

    Robertson, I.H., Ward, A., Ridgeway, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1996). The structure of normal

    human attention: The Test of Everyday Attention.Journal of the Internationa l Neuropsycho-

    logical Society,2, 523534.Rode, G., Rossetti, Y., Badan, M., & Boisson, D. (2001). Role of rehabilitation in hemineglect

    syndromes.Revue Neurologique,157(5), 497505.

    Rorden, C., Mattingley, J.B., Karnath, H., & Driver, J. (1996). Visual extinction and prior entry:

    Impaired perception of temporal order with intact motion perception after unilateral parietal

    damage.Neuropsychologia ,35(4), 421433.

    Ross, F.L. (1992). The use of computers in occupational therapy in visual scanning training.

    American Journal of Occupational Therapy,46, 314322.

    Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., Pisella, L., Farne, A., Li, L., Boisson, D., & Perenin, M.T. (1998). Prism

    adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitate s left hemispatial neglect.Nature,395,

    166169.

    Rubens, A.B. (1985). Caloric stimulation and unilateral visual neglect. Neurology , 35,

    10191024.

    Rueckert, L., & Grafman, J. (1996). Sustained attention deficits in patients with right frontal

    lesions.Neuropsychologia,34(10), 953963.

    Samuelsson, H., Hjelmquist, E., Jensen, C., Ekholm, S., & Blomstrand, C. (1988). Nonlateralized

    attentional deficits: An important component behind persisting visuospatia l neglect?Journal

    of Clinical and Experimental Psychology,20(1), 7388.

    Samuelsson, H., Jensen, C., Ekholm, S., Naver, H., & Blomstrand, C. (1997). Anatomical and

    neurological correlates of acute and chronic visuospatial neglect following right hemispherestroke.Cortex,33, 271285.

    Sea, M.C., Henderson, A., & Cermack, S.A. (1993). Patterns of visual spatial inattention and their

    functional significance in stroke patients.Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,

    74, 355361.

    Sheppard, D.M., Bradshaw, J.L., Mattingley, J.B., & Lee, P. (1999). Effects of stimulant medica-

    tion on the lateralisation of line bisection judgements of children with attention deficit hyper-

    activity disorder.Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,66(1), 5763.

    Sprague, J.M. (1966). Interaction of cortex and superior colliculus in mediation ofvisually guided

    behaviour in the cat.Science ,153, 15441547.

    Stelmach, L.B., & Herdman, C.M. (1991). Directed attention and thedetection of temporal order.

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,17, 539540.

    Sterzi, R., Bottini, G., Celani, M., Righetti, E., Lamassa, M., Ricci, M., & Vallar, G. (1993).

    Hemianopia, hemianaesthesi a and hemiplegia after right and left hemisphere damage.

    Ahemispheric difference.Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 56,308310.

    Stone, S.P., Halligan, P.W., & Greenwood, R.J. (1993). The incidence of neglect phenomena and

    related disorders in patients with an acute right or left-hemispher e stroke.Age and Ageing,

    22(1), 4652.

    Stone, S.P., Halligan, P.W., Wilson, B., Greenwood, R.J., & Marshall, J.C. (1991). Performance

    of age-matched controls on a battery of visuo-spatia l neglect tests.Journal of Neurology,Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,54, 341344.

    COGNITIVE REHABILITATION FOR UNILATERAL NEGLECT 309

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    22/23

    Taub, E., & Wolf, S.L. (1997). Constraint induced movement techniques to facilitate upper

    extremity use in stroke patients.Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation,3, 3861.

    Vallar, G., & Perani, D. (1986). The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke

    lesions: A clinical/CT scan correlation study in man.Neuropsychologia ,24, 609622.

    Velasco, F., Velasco, M., Ogarrio, C., & Olvera, A. (1986). Neglect induced by thalamotomy inhumans: A quantitative appraisal of the sensory and motor deficits. Neurosurgery ,19,

    744751.

    Voeller, K.K.S., & Heilman, K.M. (1988). Attention deficit disorder in children: A neglect

    syndrome?Neurology,38, 806808.

    Wagenaar, R.C.,Wieringen, P.C.W.V.,Netelenboss, J.B.,Meijer, O.G., & Kuik,D.J. (1992). The

    transfer of scanning training effects in visual attention after stroke: five single case studies.

    Disability Rehabilitation,14, 5160.

    Walker, R., Young, A.W., & Lincoln, N.B. (1996). Eye patching and rehabilitation in unilateral

    neglect.Neuropsychological Rehabilitation,6, 219231.

    Webster, J., Jones, S., Blanton, P., Gross, R., Beissel, G., & Wofford, J. (1984). Visual scanningtraining with stroke patients.Behaviour Therapy,15, 129143.

    Webster, J.S., McFarland, P.T., Rapport, L.J., Morrilla, B., Roades, L.A., & Abadee, P.S. (2001).

    Computer-assisted training for improving wheelchair mobility in unilateral neglect patients.

    Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,82(6), 769775.

    Weinberg, J., Diller, L., Gordon, W., Gertsman, L., Lieberman, A., Lakin, O., Hodges, G., &

    Ezrachi, O. (1977). Visual scanning training effect on reading-relate d tasks in acquired right

    brain damage.Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,58, 479486.

    Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M. (1987). Right cerebral dominance in spatial attention: Further

    evidence based on ipsilateral neglect.Archives of Neurology,44, 621625.

    Wilkins, A.J., Shallice, T., & McCarthy, R. (1987). Frontal lesions and sustained attention.

    Neuropsychologia,25, 359365.

    Wilson, B.A. (1996). Cognitive rehabilitation : How it is and how it might be.Journal of the

    International Neuropsychological Society,3, 487496.

    Wilson, B., Cockburn, J., & Halligan, P. (1987).The Behavioural Inattention Test. Titchfield,

    UK: Thames Valley Test Company.

    Wilson, F.C., Manly, T., Coyle, D., & Robertson, I.H. (2000). The effect of contralesiona l limb

    activation training and sustained attention training for self-care programmes in unilateral

    spatial neglect.Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience,16(1), 14.

    Young, A.W., Newcombe, F., & Ellis, A.W. (1991). Different impairments contribute to neglectdyslexia.Cognitive Neuropsychology,8, 177191.

    Zihl, J. (2000).Rehabilitation of visual disorders following brain injury. Hove, UK: Psychology

    Press.

    Zoccolotti , P., Guariglia, C., Pizzamiglio, L., Judica, A., Razzano, C., & Pantano. (1992). Good

    recovery in visual scanning in a patient with persistent anosognosia .International Journal of

    Neuroscience,63(12), 93104.

    Manuscript received April 2002

    310 MANLY

  • 7/25/2019 Cognitive Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect

    23/23