bonus, alexander evan__the metronomic performance practice - a history of rhythm, metronomes, and...

597
THE METRONOMIC PERFORMANCE PRACTICE: A HISTORY OF RHYTHM, METRONOMES, AND THE MECHANIZATION OF MUSICALITY by ALEXANDER EVAN BONUS A DISSERTATION Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Music CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY May, 2010

Upload: nomopomo576

Post on 03-Aug-2015

139 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

THE METRONOMIC PERFORMANCE PRACTICE: A HISTORY OF RHYTHM,

METRONOMES, AND THE MECHANIZATION

OF MUSICALITY

by

ALEXANDER EVAN BONUS

A DISSERTATION

Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy

Department of Music

CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY

May, 2010

Page 2: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY

SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES

We hereby approve the thesis/dissertation of

_____________________________________________________

candidate for the ______________________degree *.

(signed)_______________________________________________ (chair of the committee) ________________________________________________ ________________________________________________ ________________________________________________ ________________________________________________ ________________________________________________ (date) _______________________ *We also certify that written approval has been obtained for any proprietary material contained therein.

Alexander Evan Bonus

Doctor of Philosophy

Dr. Mary Davis

Dr. Daniel Goldmark

Dr. Peter Bennett

Dr. Martha Woodmansee

2/25/2010

Page 3: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

Copyright © 2010 by Alexander Evan Bonus

All rights reserved

Page 4: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

i

CONTENTS

LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ii

LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . v

Preface . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi

ABSTRACT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xviii

Chapter

I. THE HUMANITY OF MUSICAL TIME, THE INSUFFICIENCIES OF

RHYTHMICAL NOTATION, AND THE FAILURE OF CLOCKWORK

METRONOMES, CIRCA 1600-1900 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1

II. MAELZEL’S MACHINES: A RECEPTION HISTORY OF MAELZEL,

HIS MECHANICAL CULTURE, AND THE METRONOME . . . . . . . . . . . . .112

III. THE SCIENTIFIC METRONOME . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 180

IV. METRONOMIC RHYTHM, THE CHRONOGRAPHIC BIAS,

AND THE SCIENTIFIC REDEFINITION OF MUSICIANS

AND MUSICAL ACTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 223

V. THE METRONOMIC INFLUENCE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 334

VI. THE METRONOMIC EDUCATION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 360

VII. THE ORIGINS OF A CHRONOGRAPHIC MUSICAL CULTURE . . . . . . . . 463

BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 539

Page 5: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

ii

FIGURES

Figure page

1.1. Johann Nepomuk Hummel’s pulse-sense diagram, 1828 23

1.2. John Wall Callcott’s weighted-accent notation, 1810 & 1817 24

1.3. Köhler’s rhythmical genealogy 25

1.4. Riemann’s graphical-rhythmical notations, 1884 26

1.5. Grove’s “Accent” in music of Beethoven, Haydn, and Mozart, 1879 27

1.6. Gottfried Weber’s “revulsive sensation” of rhythm 28

1.7. Joshua Steele’s accents, 1779 29

1.8. Liszt’s “a-rhythmical” melody in Christus 32

1.9. Tempo d’imbroglio examples from Haydn and Beethoven 35

1.10. Detail of the autograph manuscript, Beethoven’s Op. 111 39

1.11. Steele’s invisible accents in duple and triple meters 41

1.12. Caccini’s 1602 instructions on gestural, rhythmical embellishment 52

1.13. Christiani’s non-metronomic, “falling” rhythmical gesture 53

1.14. Mattheson’s poetical notation, 1735 & 1874 56

1.15. Christiani’s correlation between and musical rhythm 57

1.16. Poetical-musical gestures interpreted in Beethoven’s and Mozart’s music 58

1.17. Czerny’s bar-by-bar analysis of rhetorical-musical time 60

1.18. Caccini’s bar-by-bar analysis of rhetorical-musical time 61

1.19. Interpretations of eighteenth-century rubato from Türk’s Klavierschule 65

1.20. Czerny’s “four different ways of interpretation” for his “Andante” 68

1.21. An example of Hauptmann’s rhythmical schemas 71

1.22. Christiani’s example of “confused” music notation 74

1.23. Christiani’s example of improved rhythmical notation 78

3.1. The Greenwich Chronograph 187

3.2. Mercury contact metronome, 1884 202

Page 6: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

iii

Figure page

3.3. Mercury contact metronome, 1888 202

3.4. MIT observatory chronograph, 1900 206

3.5. Wilhelm Wundt’s electro-magnetic metronome 215

3.6. “Apparatus for the Serial Exposure of Nonsense Syllables” 219

4.1. Ebhardt’s chronographic piano-apparatus, 1898 268

4.2. Ebhardt’s chronographic record of rhythmic passages 270

4.3. Scripture’s chronographic testing of a gymnast-subject 274

4.4. Scripture’s chronographic testing of a conductor-subject 275

4.5. Scripture’s “Measuring the Simultaneity in Actions of a Piano-player” 279

4.6. Scripture’s chronographic record of a pianist’s performance 280

4.7. Scripture’s interpretation of a non-metronomic performance practice 281

4.8. Sears’ chronographic data underneath a notated Hymn, 1902 301

4.9. Seashore’s metronomic, musical-memory test, 1919 319

5.1. Liddell’s soundproof “Animal Room,” 1926 349

6.1. “Landing of Metronome and Sub-division” in Shedlock’s Music-land 368

6.2. General Metronome, King Harmony, and the Court of Music-land 370

6.3. Watson’s gymnastics studio performs “The Indian Club Race,” 1864 375

6.4. Example of Albert Ross Parsons’ musical-gymnastic exercises, 1886 384

6.5. “Teacher’s Pocket Metronome,” advertisement in Etude, c1890s 402

6.6. Detail, “Tests and Measurements in Child Study,” 1901 411

6.7. “Zeckwer Metronome,” U.S. Patent #360,550 416

6.8. U.S. metronome patent #923,094 by C. A. White & E. R. Hunter, 1908 422

6.9. Standard ergograph for endurance tests, 1914 434

6.10. Jaques-Dalcroze’s reinvented time-signature notation 445

6.11. Example of Jaques-Dalcroze’s Eurhythmics exercises 446

6.12. Jules Amar’s chronographic work-fatigue experiment, 1918 452

Page 7: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

iv

Figure page

6.13. “Apparatus for Testing the Aptitude of Keyboard Operators,” 1926 453

6.14. U.S. Patent #1,496,258, “Music Time Indicator,” 1923 461

7.1. Jaques-Dalcroze’s Eurhythmics students perform an exercise, 1913 472

7.2. Two of Bartók’s folk-music transcriptions in manuscript, 1906 478

7.3. Historical reconstruction of Bartók’s music room, c1940 480

7.4. Walt Disney’s apparatus for recording movie soundtracks, 1931 524

7.5. Two scenes from Walt Disney’s Music Land, 1935 526

7.6. Man Ray’s “Object Indestructible,” 1923-1975 537

Page 8: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

v

TABLES

Table page

4.1 Charles H. Sears, musical-chronographic data, 1902 294

4.2 Charles H. Sears, musical-chronographic data, 1902 294

Page 9: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

vi

Preface: Convergences In Time

Temporality and the elements intrinsic to temporality—which include duration, instant,

movement, speed, pulse, and rhythm—are not under the purview of any lone academic

discipline. Nor is temporality a subject strictly contained within academia itself.

Everyday, people recognize and function with their own senses of temporality, perhaps

unaware of the science of time telling, the multiplicity of clock technologies, or the

history of time-telling machines. One does not need to be a scientist, engineer, or scholar

of time to perceive and live with a sense of time. The same holds true for musical time.

Across the world, musicians from non-Western cultures have been performing highly

complex rhythms and rhythmical patterns for centuries—without ever reading a single

treatise on the subject or referencing a metronome. The reasons are obvious: temporality,

along with musical temporality, is not fully contained within text. Temporality transcends

text. Thus the present document takes this phenomenon as its basis: musical temporality

is first and foremost a manifestation of individual beliefs and actions that, due to the

variability of individual beliefs and actions, changes over time.

In the field of musicology as it concerns performance practices, all-encompassing

histories of rhythm are few and far between. Any detailed history of musical time

requires an epic scope, not only chronologically but also conceptually. For this reason, I

do not lay claim to a definitive realization of what musical time is for all places and ages,

for conceptions of time and musicality, as I argue in this preface and following chapters,

continues to change. I offer “a history of rhythm” in contrast to some previous studies

implicitly presenting “the history of rhythm.” The most significant models for a musical-

Page 10: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

vii

time history remain Curt Sachs’ Rhythm and Tempo (1953),1 and George Houle’s more

recent Meter in Music 1600-1800, published in 1987, but certainly the culmination of

decades of scholarship.2 The current study does not intend to overthrow the importance of

these works; indeed, they hold much primary-source evidence and commentary that

remain relevant today and will remain relevant into the future. But the current project

takes a different tack, recognizing that even these musical-time histories are tempered by

their authors’ own beliefs about “rhythm” and “tempo,” beliefs born out of a twentieth-

century bias for clockwork reference and regulation. This cultural-mechanical bias, how

it came about and why, forms the major narrative arc of the present history of musical

time. In order to answer a seemingly simple question—“What does the clockwork

metronome and beats-per-minute indications have to do with musical education,

performance practices, and research at all?”—this dissertation seeks to offer a history of

musical-time and mechanical tempo references that crosses centuries, nations, and most

importantly schools of thought. For the subject of “time,” as numerous histories of time

culture and technology have argued,3 is a vital aspect of human civilization that is

gleaned from the beliefs in “time” before all else.

1 See Curt Sachs, Rhythm and Tempo (New York: W.W. Norton & Co., 1953).

2 See George Houle, Meter in Music 1600-1800 (Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1987).

Stephen Hefling’s Rhythmic Alteration in Seventeenth- and Eighteenth-century Music (New

York: Schirmer, 1993) and Richard Hudson’s Stolen Time: The History of Tempo Rubato

(Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1994) are also essential source-studies dealing with more specific

aspects of “musical time” that this dissertation does not tackle in such great detail.

3 For two of the most prominent histories of time culture, see Gerhard Dohrn-van Rossum,

History of the Hour: Clocks and Modern Orders (Chicago: The University of Chicago Press,

1992) and David S. Landes, Revolution in Time: Clocks and the Making of the Modern World

(Second Edition, Cambridge: Harvard U. Press, 2000). For a more critical history with a

sociological perspective, see Lawrence Wright, Clockwork Man, the Story of Time, its Origins, its

Uses, its Tyranny (Reprint, New York: Marboro Books Corp., 1992).

Page 11: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

viii

The tensions that this history explores lie in the distinctions between the

mechanically objective versus the internally subjective reference, between the

externalized “truth” of time versus the immanent meanings of “musical time” as they

changed over the centuries due musicians’ changing relationships to tempo technologies.

Thus, a primary impetus for writing the present history comes from other texts that

explore, in Thomas Kuhn’s coinage,4 a modern “paradigm shift” towards positivism,

alongside the positivistic, scientific treatments of time and mechanical measurement in

general. In the 1940’s Max Horkheimer and Theodor Adorno recognized that modern

scientific methodologies, which they found to be a culmination of “Enlightenment”

ideals, transformed modern culture and seemingly homogenized the once-variable

activities of individuals—although the authors did not always specify how, why, or to

what extent this phenomenon occurred in twentieth-century music performances.5 The

present study seeks answers to these questions.

Recent science histories have documented reasons for modern society’s ever-

growing reliance on objective measurements, mechanical references, alongside the

increasing desire for temporal precision in many human activities.6 Perhaps the science

history most relevant to the present dissertation is Lorraine Datson and Peter Galison’s

4 See this influential science history, Thomas S. Kuhn, The Structure of Scientific Revolutions

(Third Edition, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1996).

5 See Max Horkheimer and Theodore Adorno, Dialectic of Enlightenment (1944), edited by

Gunzelin Schmid Noerr and translated by Edmund Jephcott (Stanford: Stanford University Press,

2002).

6 For an important collection of science-history essays that describe cultural trends towards

precision measurement, see M. Norton Wise, ed., The Values of Precision (Princeton: Princeton

University Press, 1995).

Page 12: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

ix

Objectivity (2007), which explores the trend of “modern” scientists in the nineteenth

century to view natural phenomena though more abstract visual schema and mechanical

measurements, ever-distanced from the “truth to nature” imagery and subjective

aesthetics of natural historians and philosophers educated in non-mechanical traditions.

Datson and Galison detail a “quest for objectivity” that refutes any trace of personal

experience in the scientific method; their history parallels closely—and serendipitously—

with the present study exploring the trend towards musical-temporal objectivity in

education and performance.7 Prior to Objectivity, science historian Theodore Porter

questioned the value of this “objective truth” defined by machines and mechanical

regulations in Trust in Numbers: the Pursuit of Objectivity in Science and Public Life

(1995). There, he often argues against “technoscience” as a practical and useful pursuit.

Porter occasionally criticizes the positivistic “scientific mentality” of the twentieth

century—seldom challenged within the self-vindicating culture of some academic

disciplines—which he considers to be problematic and incommensurate when compared

to subjective, sensory experience.8 These science historians’ assessments of a twentieth-

century “culture of objectivity” resonate in the methodology of present musicological

study.

That there has been a marked change in the performance “tempos” of Western

music during the twentieth century has been voiced by other musicologists and perceptive

7 See Lorraine Datson and Peter Galison, Objectivity (New York: Zone Books, 2007).

8 See Theodore Porter, Trust in Numbers: the Pursuit of Objectivity in Science and Public Life

(Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1995), ix, 12, 17, 213, 225-6, 230.

Page 13: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

x

cultural critics, most prominently by Richard Taruskin in his collection Text and Act:

Essays on Music and Performance (1995).9 In these various writings, he often expresses

a disparity between the positivistic interpretations of “early-music” ensembles and the

more “traditional” performance practices of symphony orchestras. But where Taruskin

often considers the primary culprits of fast, metronomic-sounding performances to be

from the so-called “authentic-performance” movement of the 1980s and 90s, I claim that

the aesthetics of musical-temporal objectivity—which often mistakes metronomic data

and regulation for absolute composer intention—conspicuously began during the early

twentieth century for Western musical culture at large. I argue that so-called “traditional”

and “early-music” musicians often share equally in this “metronomic metamorphosis”

(originally Anton Schindler’s assessment) whenever they mistakenly justified their

performances of Bach, Mozart, Beethoven, Wagner, and others through objective

“readings” founded solely on adherence to the printed page, which often results in

performances either “too fast” or “too slow” against an implicit metronomic standard.

While the present history largely ends in the mid-twentieth century, I suggest at the

conclusion of the dissertation that this metronomic metamorphosis continues to gain in

precision today within many prominent musical circles. For those who find Taruskin’s

observations compelling and justified, however, this dissertation might afford more

concrete evidence as to why performance practices of our day—for both “Classical” and

“early-music” genres—often sound so radically different in “tempo” than those from a

even century prior.

9 See Richard Taruskin, Text and Act: Essays on Music and Performance (Oxford: Oxford

University Press, 1995).

Page 14: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

xi

Where cultural histories are concerned, this dissertation takes as a model Stephen

Kern’s monograph The Culture of Time and Space, 1880-1918, which, through ample use

of historical images and source readings, vividly describes the emerging concepts of

“time” in modernity.10

The present history, likewise, attempts to construct a broad-

ranging discourse on musical time, detailing notable cultural-historical changes over

time.11

It approaches issues of musical time, alongside composers’ assumed temporal

intentions, through the language of those perceiving, practicing, and prescribing tempo,

movement, pulse, and rhythm for their respective ages and audiences.

In order to better contextualize a pre-metronomic ontology of historical musical

time, this study approaches temporality through various conceptual lenses. It alternately

takes on qualities of a reception history, a biography, a theoretical analysis, a technology

study, and an aesthetics tract. Since meanings of time change over time, this document

occasionally approaches the format of an historical “source reading” similar to the

musicological editions by Strunk and Donington, as well as the important philosophical

collection by Charles M. Sherover entitled The Human Experience of Time, the

10 See Stephen Kern, The Culture of Time and Space, 1880-1918 (Second Edition, Cambridge:

Harvard University Press, 2003).

The interdisciplinary methodology of this dissertation can be seen as a search for consilience in

the meaning of musical time as it relates to metronomic references and regulations. Perhaps

Laurens Perseus Hickok’s nineteenth-century definition of consilience is most pertinent to the

present cultural history: “THE CONSILIENCE OF FACTS. When facts, which have apparently a

very remote bearing from each other, and which at first seem widely disconnected, and would

induce the expectation that if they are ever made explicable it must be from reasons and

principles very diverse from each other, are yet found to leap together, as it were, in colligation

with facts more manifestly allied, and which may have already been brought together in an

induction, we have a case of what we here term the Consilience of Facts. The confidence in the

general law thus deduced is augmented in proportion to the number of the facts and the distance

whence they thus jump together within the same hypothesis.” See Laurens Perseus Hickok,

Rational Psychology or The Subjective Idea and Objective Law of All Intelligence (Auburn:

Derby, Miller & Co., 1849), 266.

Page 15: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

xii

Development of its Philosophic Meaning.12

Since much importance is placed on primary-

source passages in this dissertation, it is my hope that it can occasionally be referenced as

a performance-practice treatise on time and rhythm in its own right.

This seven-chapter dissertation seeks to document a cultural-historical

consilience, a convergence in two seemingly disparate performance practices. It is a

temporal connection that took a century to develop, between the time of the music studio

and the scientific laboratory, between the heartless clockwork click and the rhythmic

pulse of Western musical performances.

Chapter One has a dual purpose. First, it provides a brief introduction to the

culture of “time knowing” while exploring the dichotomy between words and what they

insufficiently signify for vastly more complex cultural beliefs and practices. Second, it

depicts some lasting traditions of musical time in theory and practice, unbroken into the

twentieth century: a “pre-metronomic” time of music, which is analyzed in both notation

and language. Through sources spanning the seventeenth through nineteenth centuries, I

argue that musical temporality and metronomic sound bore little resemblance to each

other, either in concept or actualization. This chapter presents the various subjective and

immanent values of musical time, which form a temporal ontology of pre-twentieth-

century music—one that existed prior to the widespread employment of simple

12 See W. Oliver Strunk, Source Readings in Music History, edited by Leo Treitler (New York:

Norton, 1998); Robert Donington, The Interpretation of Early Music (New York: Norton, 1992);

and Charles M. Sherover, ed., The Human Experience of Time, the Development of its

Philosophic Meaning (Reprint, Evanston: Northwestern University Press, 2001).

Page 16: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

xiii

pendulums and later clockwork metronomes. In the process, the chapter seeks to question

some assumptions regarding time, tempo, and rhythm, which find their basis in modern

mathematical-mechanical references to the printed page. Through a musical-temporal

hermeneutics spanning over four centuries, non-metronomic epistemologies of musical

time coalesce, which were accessed through subjective thought, sensation, and action—

not clockwork sound or motion. This introductory chapter presents shared values of a

long-standing Western musical culture in which an individual’s pulse was considered of

the greatest precision, in which a musician’s various rhythmic sensations conclusively

defined temporal truth.

Chapter Two references many of the same topics presented in the first chapter,

now through the prism of a nineteenth-century cultural history that highlights Johann

Maelzel, the most prominent champion of clockwork machines in the burgeoning

Industrial societies of Europe and America. This chapter explores a past culture in which

clockwork machines, including Maelzel’s iconic metronome, were still uncommon, seen

as uncanny, and found to be decidedly untrustworthy when compared to human thought

and action. Maelzel and his machines are placed against a contrasting culture, espoused

by playwrights, musicians, and cultural critics, who readily acknowledged that artificially

moving inventions failed to reflect the desired subjectivities found in living creativity. In

a history that spans over a century, I show how performers and pedagogues—intimately

aware of the pre-metronomic ontology of musical time analyzed in Chapter One—offered

ominous and damning assessments of Maelzel’s mechanical-performance culture, one

that eventually sprouted throughout the industrializing world. As documented in

following chapters, Maelzel’s once-uncustomary metronome would eventually stimulate

Page 17: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

xiv

a radical paradigm shift, redefining the theories and practices of musical time throughout

modern society.

Chapter Three points to the origins of that rhythmical paradigm shift for modern

performance culture through a science and technology history that runs chronologically

parallel to Chapter Two. Within the nineteenth-century astronomical observatory, the

germ of a new cultural tradition in time, rhythm, and action was planted, which displayed

contrasting temporal values, practices, and intentions for both clockwork machines and

people. This study provides a history of time telling in stark contrast to the previous

chapter; a scientific-cultural history in which clockwork machines were not only trusted

but also inseparable from a new methodology, a new field of research intended for new

modes of objectively “precise” action. In the experimental nineteenth-century sciences,

far afield from the composer’s desk or the performance hall, clockwork machines defined

new goals in time for once-subjective perceptions and actions. Yet the two contrasting

histories are very much connected through one significant commonality: Maelzel’s

metronome. This single invention—a technology shared in these parallel histories—

illuminates a cultural phenomenon pivotal to the present study: Different individuals or

communities, valuing different belief-systems in time, entrust identical machines with

different intentions. Thus, this chapter uncovers the foundation of a metronomic

performance practice in Western civilization, established by nineteenth-century scientists,

that less than a century later actively influenced twentieth-century students and

professionals in a uniquely modern performance culture.

Chapter Four documents how scientific methodologies and machines—

promoting metronomic time above all else—were first actively applied to musicians and

Page 18: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

xv

their performances in the latter decades of the nineteenth century. In seminal “rhythm”

studies, experimental scientists prescribed their laboratory culture, practices, and beliefs

in industrial-mechanical time and action (which, as witnessed in Chapter Three, spanned

nearly a century) directly to performers in controlled, reproducible settings. This

scientific “rhythm” research on living musicians exemplifies the very real hegemony—

alluded to throughout the dissertation—that temporal machines impose upon subjective

actions and perceptions. These new laboratory researchers regulated performers with

increasingly inhuman measures during mechanical-efficiency tests diametrically opposed

to the values of trained, skilled, and experienced musicians, whose subjective, temporal

beliefs were explored in the first two chapters. As this study charts in detail, late-

nineteenth-century laboratory scientists engendered a radically new meaning, practice,

and tradition of musical rhythm for the modern twentieth century—a metronomic

tradition that, as Chapters VI and VII document, some highly influential twentieth-

century musicians and pedagogues were instrumental in promoting.

Chapter Five continues the narrative of experimental science, technology, and

performance practices, while it furthers the evidence of a metronomic hegemony alluded

to in earlier chapters. Here, we find that the potentially damaging influence of the

clockwork metronome, claimed by nineteenth-century musicians and cultural critics, was

very much quantified by experimental scientists later in the century. The evidence

presented here strongly suggests that clicking metronome technologies exerted a real and

invasive influence upon performance practices. Maelzel’s machine, when used as a

regulative tool, created a new, mechanized species of musicality in human beings. This

chapter offers an historical analysis of the clockwork metronome and its lasting effects

Page 19: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

xvi

upon individuals’ notions of time, rhythm, and action, with findings that might prove

equally important for performers, teachers, as well as historians of science and music.

Chapter Six is a social-cultural analysis of nineteenth- and twentieth-century

pedagogies in relation to metronomes, and it represents the culmination of the myriad of

topics presented throughout this dissertation on mechanical motion and musical

movement. This chapter shows that the beliefs in time, metronomes, and educational

methods eventually created a new performance-practice tradition. The subjective and

variable culture expressed in the first two chapters recede in favor of the scientific-

industrial values expounded in Chapters Three to Five; the values of mechanical

efficiency, reproduction, and normalization first practiced in the experimental laboratory

were now prescribed for a new twentieth-century musical culture that paralleled the

pedagogical concerns of the larger Industrial World.

The final Chapter Seven first provides an overview of scientific pedagogical

methods as they disseminated throughout society by the 1920s. The remainder of the

chapter offers evidence to suggest that, through the increasingly popularized metronomic

education, a new metronomic notion of musicality emerged. This concluding chapter

documents influential twentieth-century musicians and researchers who redefined the

temporal values of performance for the music of all ages; they helped to found a new

scientific aesthetic, a new metronomic musicality in both theory and practice. The last

chapter is by no means an exhaustive survey of every significant composer-performer or

educator of the new century. Nevertheless, the examples given here provide compelling

evidence that many modern composer-performers and musicologists, who continue to

influence our notion of “musicality” in the Western tradition, embraced the “scientific”

Page 20: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

xvii

species of musical time popularized in their age, in contrast to the pre-metronomic times

of previous centuries. Evidence presented here runs in contradistinction to the historical

arguments offered in Chapters One and Two. Through accounts of those both for and

against this new precision-based musicality, the chapter depicts a culture in which

mechanical objectivity became a prevailing aesthetic, a culture in which metronomes

became unquestioned primary references for musical time, the infallible sources of

accurate rhythm, along with the definitive indicators of even pre-metronomic composers’

temporal intentions. In this concluding section, I suggest that these twentieth-century

models of education and aesthetics still inform many in our performance culture today, a

predominantly precision-based culture that continues to endorse a mechanized “time” and

“rhythm” significantly altered from past, pre-metronomic traditions.

Page 21: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

xviii

The Metronomic Performance Practice: A History of Rhythm,

Metronomes, and the Mechanization of Musicality

Abstract

by

ALEXANDER EVAN BONUS

Through the analyses of treatises, scores, letters, and technologies spanning four centuries, this

multidisciplinary history of rhythm charts the various, shifting meanings in musical time and

movement as pedagogies and performance practices became increasingly influenced by

clockwork machines—and Johann Maelzel’s metronome most conspicuously—over the course of

the modern age. Depicting how “musical time” constitutes an ever-changing belief system in

what “time” means, this study charts the ascendance of a new musical-temporal ontology brought

about by Western performance-culture’s increasing reliance on metronomes.

This history explains how scientific methodologies and machines—promoting

metronomic time above all else—were first actively applied to musicians and their performances

in the latter decades of the nineteenth century. The influential work of modern scientists,

pedagogues, and only later composers—with their precision-oriented beliefs in metronomic time

and rhythm—eventually helped to create a new performance-practice tradition, a new musical

culture in which mechanical objectivity became a prevailing aesthetic in the twentieth century.

Highlighting the writings of philosophers such as Mersenne, Diderot, and Rousseau; musicians

such as Quantz, Beethoven, and Stravinsky; scientists such as Wundt, Scripture, and Seashore;

and pedagogues such as A. B. Marx, Christiani, and Jaques-Dalcroze, the narrative explicates

how and why this temporal revision occurred, and what outcomes followed when scientific

modes of metronomic action were imposed upon past, subjective musical practices.

As this history of musical time, metronomes, and musicality uncovers, the very meanings

and cultural values underlying “rhythm” and “tempo” have palpably changed since the twentieth

century due to a heretofore-unacknowledged paradigm shift: a metronomic turn in which the

once-innate musical “beat” became both conceptually and audibly mechanized.

Page 22: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

1

CHAPTER I: THE HUMANITY OF MUSICAL TIME, THE INSUFFICIENCIES

OF RHYTHMICAL NOTATION, AND THE FAILURE OF

CLOCKWORK METRONOMES, CIRCA 1600-1900

Introduction: A Time before Metronomes

Malvolio: My masters, are you mad? or what are you? Have you no wit, manners,

nor honesty, but to gabble like tinkers at this time of night? Do ye make an

alehouse of my lady’s house, that ye squeak out your coziers’ catches without any

mitigation or remorse of voice? Is there no respect of place, persons, nor time, in

you?

Sir Toby: We did keep time, sir, in our catches. Sneck up!

—William Shakespeare, Twelfth Night, Act ii, Scene 3

The complex sensory phenomena commonly reduced to the term “musical time”

preceded the invention and use of the clockwork metronome. As Shakespeare reminds us

through the banter between Malvolio and Sir Toby, performers conceived and actualized

musical measures, movements, and rhythms long before mechanical clocks ever dictated

precise beats-per-minute clicks to music students and professionals across the world.

Prior to the large-scale manufacture and distribution of clockwork metronomes towards

the end of the nineteenth century, the human performance-act of music making—not to

mention speaking, acting, and dancing—bore little active relationship to the second-by-

second clicks of a mechanical time reference.

Perhaps due to modern societies’ overriding reliance on temporal precision—in

which “time” is indistinguishable from clocks, clockwork, and the base-60 numerical

scale—Shakespeare’s pun is not as blatantly obvious today as it was for Elizabethan

audiences, for the Bard juggles two opposing “times,” two distinct temporal

epistemologies well understood to pre-industrial cultures: one of the day, and one of

music. To sing a tune “in time” was not to understand the time of day through hours,

Page 23: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

2

minutes, or especially seconds, as referenced through the silent sundial, sandglass, or

typical Renaissance town clock—a non-clicking edifice with a single hand, hour dial, and

chime that bore little resemblance to the sound, vision, and portability of a clockwork

metronome circa 1897.1 Thus, while Sir Toby and his revelers lost track of the hour—a

common pre-Industrial problem in the darkness of night—he was justifiably offended by

Malvolio’s remarks. With clear ambivalence towards the clock, Toby’s group well knew

the other “time,” the musical time, which they faithfully maintained through a seemingly

innate, rhythmical sense of pulse and proportion, made all the more impressive given

their questionable states-of-mind.

As Shakespeare’s passage exemplifies, the term “time” often signifies many

conflicting or incommensurate meanings, which change through varying social contexts,

conventions, and practices. “Time” represents a fundamental set of values, culturally and

historically contingent, which are commonly shared amongst civilizations, communities,

or individuals. The argument between Malvolio and Sir Toby uncovers how different

communities or individuals think and function under these conflicting temporal value

systems. For Sir Toby, musical time was fundamentally a human process, unencumbered

and uninfluenced by constant mechanical rule, reference, or regulation. While Toby

rightly perceived the internal time-sense for his musical performance, Malvolio rightly

perceived that no such performance should be heard during an objectively late time at

night. Each character recognized his respective temporal construct, and neither one

misapprehended the “correct time;” they both understood the temporal truth as it related

1 See Gerhard Dohrn-van Rossum, History of the Hour: Clocks and Modern Orders (Chicago:

The University of Chicago Press, 1992) for a comprehensive history of public clocks, their

development across Europe, and time telling through the early-modern age.

Page 24: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

3

to their own perceptions and values, in their own contexts for their own purposes. One

valued the internal time for performing; the other valued the nightly time for sleeping.

This study documents the various, shifting meanings in musical time, rhythm, and

movement as performance practices became increasingly influenced by clockwork

machines over the course of the nineteenth century. In this history of rhythm—

documenting an intellectual and cultural shift in performance practices—I argue that true

or correct time is not merely an accurate or precise measurement based on objective

references; the present study enumerates how the phenomena of “time” is more

profoundly the personal or collective belief system in what “time” means. Indeed, it is an

anachronistic fallacy for moderns to believe that time always reduces to an unassailable

definition or objective constant based on reproducible data and mechanical

measurements—these are the exclusive temporal values of modern science, industry, and

education—a “time” referenced through modern clocks, watches, and metronomes. Only

in the modern age has “true time” become synonymous with exacting and precise

mechanical measurement. Yet as historians such as E. P. Thompson, G. J. Whithrow,

David Landes, Gerhard Van Rossum, and Peter Galison among others have shown, time

is much more than what precision machines tell us about the present, in our “time.”

Beyond clockwork measurements, “time” is an intrinsic trait of civilization; “time” is a

culture’s faith in temporality, one that alters over the ages. Indeed, “knowing the time”—

whether the time of day or the time of music—is more accurately considered “believing

in the temporal convention” being told. Thus, time can be likened to a secular religion;

yet it is a religion that offers its believers no conscious initiation, no clear right of passage

ritual, and no explicit tenets. We are born into our culture’s “time,” unconsciously and

Page 25: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

4

unquestioningly accepting of it. Time-belief demands no conversion; to those living

within its constructs, “time” is a worldview with little competition.

Consider the historical differences in the “time” of daily living: Ancient agrarian

societies believed in the sun and the seasons as time tellers for sowing and reaping,

alongside the spiritual and metaphysical explanations of the world. Medieval monastic

societies believed in the temporary canonical hours ordained by the Creator to structure

the opus Dei, and by extension, the Temporale and Sanctorale. Contrastingly, modern

societies—whether conscious of it or not—believe in the equal, mechanized hour,

minute, and second as dictators of the work day, and as controlling factors for

intercontinental travel, industrial production, global communication, and nearly every

other standardized activity under the sun. For the sixteenth century, local sundial time

told the true time; for the mid twentieth century, Greenwich Mean Time referenced

“definitive” global time zones; and in the early twenty first century, a Global-Positioning-

System seamlessly beams “accurate” time-data across wireless networks to individuals

around the world. These times exist within specific historical contexts; they are not truths

for all ages, people, places, or activities.

For both the time of day and music, tools and techniques considered most

necessary and convenient for the historical age and cultural activity help to define

temporality. Prior to the large-scale synchronization of civic clocks and the spread of

pocket watches in the latter half of the nineteenth century,2 clockwork machines were not

necessarily the most reliable references for the time of day or duration. Devices such as

2 See Peter Galison, Einstein’s Clocks, Poincaré’s Maps (New York: W.W. Norton and Co.,

2003) and Ian Bartky, Selling the True Time, Nineteenth Century Timekeeping in America

(Stanford: Stanford University Press, 2000) for comprehensive cultural histories of clock

synchronization in nineteenth-century Europe and America, respectively.

Page 26: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

5

gnomons, quadrants, sextants, sundials, sandglasses, water clocks, and bell towers all

successfully told various species time throughout history—marking either the duration of

an activity, the progress of the day or night, or a singular moment to reckon. Despite

these many temporal machines and methods available to past cultures—many of which

were readily trusted well through the nineteenth century—the initial time or tempo of

music was not defined by externalized means. Neither sandglasses, nor sundials, nor

sextants could define or document the motion of music, which was most often referenced

through the human pulse or tactus: the recurring rhythm of an individual’s heartbeat,

moving hand, or foot. Before the metronome, the time of music was definitively self-

referential.

Throughout history, external temporal tools, from ancient gnomons to modern

mobile phones, offer essential information for agrarian, civic, and industrial civilizations.

Time tellers, regardless of the specific era or device, afford standardized ways to

measure, quantify, and reproduce data about moments, durations, and—most importantly

for the present study—physical activities. To a great extent, time tellers make an

individual’s subjective perceptions about time unnecessary and negligible; one need not

guess the “time” to leave for work when a clock is present, or measure cooking “time” by

singing a popular hymn (a standard Renaissance practice) when a stopwatch is at hand.

Time-telling tools assist in better controlling and managing “time”—whatever the term

may signify in any given context; they dictate and describe activities with greater

uniformity and reproducibility, assisting organizations, communities, and individuals to

work with more efficiency and coordination. Yet, as time historians well recognize,

external time-tellers—with all of the importance they hold in the growth of

Page 27: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

6

civilizations—contrastingly sublimate interpretive and sensory assessments about time,

and negate individual perceptions for the sake of mass standardization, control, and

reproduction, values necessary for civic, commercial, and industrial functions. The

hegemony external time-tellers place over human perception and action is well

documented throughout Western history. By 1969 British sociologist Lawrence Wright

went so far as to describe modern society’s increasingly precision-oriented time culture

as an “intolerable chronarchy.”3 And regardless of the rising levels of precision in time

keeping, this hegemony—in which external machines redefine internal perceptions and

consequent actions—has been documented since antiquity. Consider Plautus’ indignation

against the most precise time-telling tool for Romans in the second century B.C.:

The Gods confound the man who first found out

How to distinguish the hours! Confound him, too,

Who in this place set up a sun-dial

To cut and hack my days so wretchedly

Into small portions. When I was a boy,

My belly was my sun-dial; one more sure,

Truer, and more exact than any of them.

This dial told me when 'twas proper time

To go to dinner, when I had aught to eat.

But now-a-days, why even when we have,

I can't fall-to, unless the sun gives leave.

The greater part of its inhabitants,

Shrunk up with hunger, creep along the streets.4

In chiding the hegemony that the lone sundial places on individual choice, Plautus

expressed a significant theme that runs throughout this present history of musical time-

telling, one that applies directly to metronomes: With increasing mechanical-temporal

3 See Lawrence Wright, Clockwork Man, the Story of Time, its Origins, its Uses, its Tyranny

(Reprint, New York: Marboro Books Corp., 1992), 7.

4 This quote appears in numerous clock and time histories including Daniel Boorstin, The

Discoverers (New York: Random House, 1983), 28; also Wright, 29.

Page 28: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

7

precisions and exactitudes, subjective qualities and perceptions of time become devalued

and even destroyed. As Plautus’ recounted, internal feelings once referenced time, even

his eating “time.” The “truth” and “perfection” Plautus once allied with personal

sensation and judgment receded in the face of greater mechanical and objective control.

For this Roman, the greater uniformity and precision seen in sundial time reinvented the

very nature of civic organization and Roman lifestyle. With the new sundial imposing a

reproducible solar measurement, once “true,” innate time telling became externalized.

Plautus’ ancient commentary allegorizes the fate of time-knowing in music as well. With

precise and reproducible mechanical measurements (which are becoming more precise

every decade), musical time has suffered the same fate as the time telling of day: the

subjectivities of intuitive temporality—the time Plautus understood before the sundial

and Sir Toby before the clock—has become increasingly subverted and simplified for the

sake of a new objective truth. Thus, this history of rhythm will show that metronomic

“time” devalues and disregards musical nuance, subjectivity, variability, and feeling once

recognized in past temporal practices. Clockwork metronomes imposed a mechanical

hegemony upon Western performance culture.

As this study charts in depth, only since the industrial age has modern societies’

temporal faith—for both the day and music—rested exclusively in the time told by

automatic machines in the form of synchronized clocks, precise watches, and now mobile

phones. The present study, perhaps for the first time in great detail, uncovers the

significant discrepancies that abound between modern and pre-modern performance

practices when telling the musical “time.” The clicks of quartz and computerized

metronomes, which now define modern musical seconds for the majority of music

Page 29: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

8

students and professionals, do not correspond to the once “natural” and “true” values of

living musical performance, first experienced through the human pulse and the tactus.

Just as Plautus’ historical sundial hour does not correlate with our Cesium-clock second,

the historical musical tactus does not correlate with the modern metronomic blip. We do

not share identical musical “times”—conceptually or practically—with past or non-

industrialized cultures.

It was only in the twentieth century when theorists, pedagogues, and

musicologists, guided by many centuries’ worth of accumulated data, began to view

musical proportionality—and by extension, tempo, pulse, and rhythm—as absolute and

objective measurements implicitly based on metronomic precisions and exacting

temporal constructs.5 These modern, scientifically inspired methods often failed to reflect

the act of musical performance itself or the historical performer’s role as the primary

agent in creating musical time. Thus, with the study of rhythm and tempo as a modern

science, musical “time” transitioned from a subjective process to an objective precision,

in which human agency became devalued, and in some cases discounted entirely. Modern

academics’ exceedingly literal interpretations of historical notation often reduced the

once-living variables of musical time to the tick-tock of clockwork.

The present study explicates how and why this temporal revision came about, and

what outcomes followed when modern mechanical measurements were imposed upon

past subjective practices. While Lewis Mumford’s often-cited assertion holds true—the

5 See notably, Eugène Borrel, “Les indications metronomiques…du XVIIIe siècle,” Revue de

musicology T.9c (1928): 149-153; and R. E. M. Harding, Origins of Musical Time and

Expression (London: Oxford University Press, 1938).

Page 30: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

9

primary tool of modern civilization is the clock6—I maintain that modernity is shaped not

only by clocks, but specific communities’ reapplications of clockwork technologies,

which then influence and alter human actions towards increasingly mechanical values.

New uses for clockwork technologies result in new, hegemonic impositions on past

practices. The increasing adherence and faith in objective, exacting clockwork “time” has

considerably revised and indeed mechanized modern musical “time” over the Industrial

Age. Given the research presented here, one can argue that twentieth-century musical

culture cannot completely recognize the misunderstanding between Malvolio and Toby,

since the current “times” of the clock and music are often considered as one and the

same.

To study time, musical or otherwise, is thus to study the ideas grounding “time,”

those temporal meanings referenced in historical and cultural contexts. In the pursuit of a

more nuanced, historical understanding of performance practices, one discovers that the

understanding of musical time cannot escape the cultural belief system of time itself.

Thus the essential questions that are pursued here: What are the appropriate and sufficient

belief systems of time within specific cultural and historical contexts? How do these

belief systems alter over the ages for different communities? What emerging concepts

stimulate these changes? And what effects do these conceptual changes have on

performance practices? The guiding methodology behind the present research seeks to

expose the musical “time” regarded of the greatest value for that age, community, and

6 See Lewis Mumford, Technics and Civilization (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1934) for

one of the first and most influential of twentieth-century histories on time telling and technology.

Page 31: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

10

performance context. “True” musical time is best sought through an intellectual and

cultural history untainted by modern temporal values—and modern temporal machines—

that cannot help but to revise and reduce historical and cultural conceptions of “time” for

modern intentions alone. (It goes without saying that Monteverdi, Lully, Bach, and

countless other musical masters needed no such metronomic certitudes for their musical

education, compositions, or performances.)

Applying anachronistic measurements and machines to past practices does not

lead to more profound knowledge, but only to mistranslations and misconceptions of past

temporal values. Indeed, it can be argued that the beats-per-minute descriptor is a failing

of the modern imagination when compared to past musical conceptions and practices; the

modern reduction of musical rhythm, meter, and tempo to clockwork clicks often reflects

the temporal limitations and constrictions of the modern age. Modern scholars,

pedagogues, and performers often ignore and discount historical-temporal meanings

when guided by this “metronome methodology”—the seemingly objective, scientific act

of temporal translation—which assumes that wristwatches, wall clocks, and metronomes

tell the same time as historic sundials, sandglasses, public clocks, or the individual’s

sense for pulse and proportion. Yet no modern machine, whether a wristwatch, mobile

phone, or a metronome, truly explains the “time,” the temporal meaning of musical

cultures that flourished prior to these technologies. As this study exposes, playing “in

time,” “in measure,” and “in rhythm,” means something drastically different today, for a

more scientific and precision-oriented culture, than it did prior to the twentieth century.

As Shakespeare and a host of others often recounted well through the nineteenth

century, musical performances maintained a separate species of temporality, with

Page 32: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

11

temporal references quite apart from sundials, clocks, or sandglasses. Before the

twentieth century, definitions and meanings of musical time most “accurately” and

“faithfully” relied on subjective, sensory, and personal measurements. Musical pulse and

movement comprised a uniquely human variety of time, gauged through subjective

senses, perceptions, and actions. Where musical time telling was concerned prior to the

twentieth century, external timekeepers such as sundials or sandglasses—not to mention

nineteenth-century clockwork metronomes—had little if nothing to do with the

practicalities of musical performance in rhythm, meter, and proportion.

The differences between the “time” of clocks and the “time” of music appear in

early-modern references across Europe. As with “time,” the term mouvement, which in

the present day is often misconstrued simply as a French-style rate of metronomic tempo,

contains various meanings, both mechanical and musical. Diderot’s Encyclopédie

distinguishes the mouvement of clocks and music using two separate definitions, one

unaffected by the other. Mouvement as a terme d'Horlogerie, references the various

components of a clock or watch, including a pendulum, escapement, dial, and pivots.

Alternately, mouvement en Musique again concerns qualities of liveliness or slowness

and the characteristics of each air—during each bar—which was most often related by

French or Italian words. It is significant to note that the Encyclopédie defines another

mouvement, that of rhetoric, as it concerns the fluctuating passions and emotions elicited

through skillful oration.7

7 Encyclopédie, Vol. 10, 842. “Mouvement, s. m. en Musique, est le degré de vîtesse ou de lenteur

qu'on donne à la mesure selon le caractere de l'air. Le mouvement s'exprime ordinairement par les

mots gai, vîte, grave, lent, &c. ou par les mots italiens allegro, presto, grave, adagio, &c. qui leur

correspondent. Voyez tous ces mots.”

Page 33: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

12

The early Encyclopædia Britannica (1778-83)—which owes much of its musical

content to the Encyclopédie—also makes a clear distinction in the entry on “Time”

between astronomical time, which necessitated precision clock measurements, and

musical time, which did not require machines at all, but instead relied upon subjective

characteristics, movements, expressions, through various degrees of liveliness or

slowness: qualities gleaned by creative interpreters that were unrelated to exacting,

reproducible clockwork quantities. The standard for measured musical “time” was, as it

always had been prior to the twentieth century, human in origin:

Time in music is concerned, either with respect to the general movement of an air,

and in this sense it is said to be swift or slow: or it is considered with respect to

the aliquot parts of every bar; these parts are marked by motions of the hand or

foot, and in a particular sense are called times8…all the bars should be perfectly

equal, and all the times contained in each bar perfectly equivalent to another.

Now, to render this equality sensible, every bar is struck, and every time

distinguished, by a motion of the hand or foot; and by these motions the different

values of notes are exactly regulated, according to the genius and character of the

bar.9

The “exact” equality of musical time—“perfectly regulated” by human movement and

sensation—was not the equality of mechanical time—perfectly regulated by redundant

“tick-tock” clockwork. Confirming the distinction, the Encyclopædia Britannica presents

an entirely separate discussion of “Time Keepers for finding the Longitude,” highlighting

John Harrison’s famed chronometer, the first portable machine that could maintain the

mechanical second with near-unerring constancy and precision during sea travel. It

dictated seconds accurately enough to be considered for maritime, mercantile, and

8 Encyclopædia Britannica; or, a dictionary of arts, sciences, &c. The second edition; greatly

improved and enlarged, Vol. 9 (Edinburgh, 1778-83), 8612.

9 Ibid., 8612-8613.

Page 34: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

13

military requirements. A supplement for insufficient onboard astronomical readings,

Harrison’s state-of-the-art chronometer was intended for naval activities far from the

values, contexts, and conceptions of eighteenth-century musical performers.10

Harrison’s

precise watch had nothing to do with the time of music. Yet by the modern age, as Lewis

Mumford acknowledged in 1944, “precision must be commonplace.”11

Thus it should be

recognized that mechanical precisions far exceeding the Harrison chronometer are

commonly available today in every store-bought quartz metronome, and ubiquitously

employed in some way around the world for modern music lessons, practice sessions, and

recording projects.

Prior to the modern world in which mechanical-temporal precisions became both

ubiquitous and commonplace, experienced performers’ practiced and perceived a

nuanced, living “time.” Prior to the modern belief that redundant, clockwork “time”

could and should regulate pulses, proportions, and rhythms—alongside harmonies,

melodies, ornaments, and gestures—of musical action, skilled performers willfully

created musical temporality. This chapter—an introduction to this modern history of

rhythm, metronomes, and musicality—reveals that faith in past musical time rested in

more variable, subjective perceptions and practices than could be gleaned through

clockwork. In opposition to the externally measured time of day, humanity thrived within

the time of historical music.

10 Ibid., 8613 -8616.

11

Quoted in M. Norton Wise, ed. The Values of Precision (Princeton: Princeton University Press,

1995), 353.

Page 35: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

14

On the Pre-Metronomic Pulse-Sense

From 1600-1900, music treatises and references such as the eighteenth-century

Encyclopédie and Encyclopædia Britannica define proportionality as the conceptual basis

of all mensural and metrical music in the Western tradition. These texts depict how

sounds divided into duple or triple patterns represented the rhythmical alphabet of

measured, musical time. But the measurer of that proportionality—the temporal agent—

made musical time audible. And it is historically obvious that these temporal agents, at

least through 1815, were not clockwork metronomes but performers themselves. For

three centuries at least, musical time originated through measures more vital than those

represented by notation, schematics, quantified data, or metronomic references. Musical

time was born of the subjective, sensory qualities intrinsic to living temporal agency.

The remainder of this chapter documents this largely forgotten, pre-metronomic

ontology of musical time, which ultimately relied not on a literal adherence to rhythmical

notation, mathematical computations, or mechanical measurements. Prior to the twentieth

century, musical temporality was ultimately defined through subjective perceptions and

actions, referenced through living variables of pulse, speech, physical sensation, and

gesture. These seemingly antiquated, anti-scientific notions do not suggest some vague

“musical-temporal chaos,” for historical documents instead expose many rich definitions

of rhythm far more practical than absolute formulas or schemas could offer: they relate an

aesthetics of time neglected in the modern age, in which subjective, rhythmical qualities

transcended the clicks of clockwork, proportional calculations, and even musical notation

itself. Indeed, as Sir Toby asserted, one could keep the time of tunes without keeping the

time of day.

Page 36: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

15

In his 1618 treatise Compendium Musicae, the young philosopher René Descartes

explained a “time” that was similarly conceived by Shakespeare’s fictional Sir Toby, an

actual temporal epistemology that extended well through the nineteenth century. At the

age of 22, Descartes described how musical proportionality was neither born of physics,

nor mechanics, nor was it the product of pure intellect:

Time in sound must consist of equal parts, for these are perceived most easily

according to [the sense of small divisions], or it must consist of parts which are in

a proportion of 1:2 or 1:3, this progression cannot be extended, for only these

relations can be easily distinguished by the ear…if time values were of greater

inequality, the ear would not be able to recognize their difference without great

effort, as experience shows.12

Descartes knew that the aural sense of pulse, the sonic perception of proportion, defined

musical time before notation ever existed. To prove this, Descartes writes out a figure

that was impossible to feel or sense as musical rhythm during his age. Descartes instructs,

“should I, for example, place five even notes against one, it would be almost impossible

to sing…[and] I cannot write such notes individually if the second [note] is a fourth [the

duration] of the first one:”13

| w q | For Descartes, this figure was an insensible rhythm, an indecipherable musical

proportion. No duple or triple feeling could be ascertained in what seems a clearly

performable figure—if modern, objective standards of mechanical-temporal addition

were applied. Descartes explained that the sonic experience of pulse defined both musical

12 René Descartes, Compendium Musicae (Ms., 1618; Utrecht: Jansson, 1650); translation by

Walter Robert ([Rome]: American Institute of Musicology, 1961), 13.

13

Ibid.

Page 37: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

16

time and musical notation. Thus “rhythm,” for his age, was not merely the objective,

mathematically precise notation and replication of discreet sound units. Active feelings

and perceptions delineated musical proportionality.

More than just hearing musical proportion, Descartes described musical time as

physical intuition, an internal rhythmical sense:

Singers and instrumentalists observe [proportion] instinctively, especially in

connection with tunes to which we are accustomed to dance and sway. Here we

accompany each beat of the music by a corresponding motion of our body; we are

quite naturally impelled to do this by the music.14

Descartes’ epistemology, one obscured in the twentieth century, valued subjective

perception and movement before temporal objectivity, calculation, or rhythmic

exactitude. This great philosopher of the mind so readily believed that musical time was

an instinctual phenomenon—set apart from notation, mechanics, and physics—that he

strikingly asserted:

It follows that even animals can dance to rhythm if they are taught and trained, for

it takes only a physical stimulus to achieve this reaction.

Descartes, who linked man’s existence with his profound ability to intellectualize,

ironically also linked man’s musical creativity to his primal, animalistic ability to “feel

the beat.” Nevertheless, he ascertained the fundamental living quality of musical

proportion that would reappear in performances over the ages—that sonic pulse-sensation

known as the measured or mensural accent. He states:

Since the sound is emitted more strongly and clearly at the beginning of each

measure, we must conclude that it has greater impact on our spirits, and that we

are thus roused to motion.15

14 Ibid., 14.

15

Ibid.

Page 38: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

17

For over three centuries, descriptions nearly identical to Descartes’ expose this sensory,

subjective reality of musical time. The eighteenth-century Chambers’ Cyclopædia—for

which Charles Burney wrote many of the musical entries—noted: “Rhythmus, in the

general, is perceived either by the eye or ear, and may be either with or without metre;

but the strict musical rhythm is only perceived by the ear, and cannot exist without it.”16

Nineteenth-century theorist Gottfried Weber also relates these long-standing temporal

values, unbroken since Shakespeare’s and Descartes’ age:

DIVISION IV. MUSICAL ACCENT. § LXVI. It is not alone the symmetry of the

exactly measured lengths of the times, that constitutes the essential nature and the

peculiar charm of the rhythmical arrangement; but our internal feeling superadds

still a certain other property. That is to say, we as it were involuntarily (and

instinctively) lay more stress on the first time...than on the following time [in a 2-

meter], or on the two following times [in a 3-meter]…which in fact gives definite-

ness, life, and meaning to the whole performance.17

Nineteenth-century pedagogue Adolph Bernhard Marx also recognized that rhythmical

proportionality was formed by an internal, non-notated sense for musical pulsation: “The

feeling of measure and sensation of rhythm—we repeat it,—are innate in every human

being gifted with understanding, but, like every other faculty, in different gradations.”18

Like Descartes, Chambers, Weber, and Marx, speech pedagogue Andrew Comstock

16 See Ephraim Chambers, “Rhythm,” Cyclopædia: or, an universal dictionary of arts and

sciences. In four volumes, Vol. 4 (London, 1786-88), [165]. Eighteenth Century Collections

Online. Gale. Case Western Reserve University. Accessed on 12 Oct. 2009

<http://find.galegroup.com/ecco/infomark.do?&contentSet=ECCOArticles&type=multipage&tab

ID=T001&prodId=ECCO&docId=CW3314759369&source=gale&userGroupName=cwru_main

&version=1.0&docLevel=FASCIMILE>.

17 Gottfried Weber, General Music Teacher: Adapted to Self-instruction, Both for Teachers and

Learners; Embracing Also an Extensive Dictionary of Musical Terms, trans. by James Franklin

Warner (Boston: J. H. Wilkins & R. B. Carter, 1841), 89.

18

Adolph Bernhard Marx, General Musical Instruction (Allgemeine Musiklehre), trans. by

George Macirone (London: J. Alfred Novello, 1854), 120.

Page 39: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

18

knew that human agency was the guiding principle underlying musical time, a distinctly

non-mechanical epistemology, as he certified in A System of Elocution:

It is rhythmical pulsation which enables a band of musicians to perform in

concert. It is this also which enables a company of soldiers to march

synchronously, and which governs the movements of the feet in dancing.19

This fundamental, commonly understood sensation of measured musical time was

variously described over three centuries. Christiani defined it as, “pulsation…formed of

the interchange of rising and falling, [which] is necessary to give a clear sense of the

rhythm, and lend to the [musical] movement its wavy outline.”20

Some English

pedagogues labeled it the Ictus metricus. “Arsis and thesis” was perhaps the most

common designation, owing to the relationship between tactus-reference and ancient

Greek poetic meters. Riemann called it the “agogic.” In the seventeenth and eighteenth

centuries, some French musicians considered it alternately an aspect of mouvement,

mesurée, or cadence. The eighteenth-century linguist Joshua Steele described it as

“emphatic and remiss.” Hummel marked it as a stress of “heavy and light:” a sensation

that did not reflect volume change. Hauptmann strikingly called it “the energy of

beginning.” Weber as the regular recurrence of “internal weight.” Perhaps the first

edition Grove Dictionary defined this essential, physical sensation of meter most directly

as the “quiet dwelling” upon the bar.21

19 Andrew Comstock, A System of Elocution (Philadelphia: E. H. Butler & Co., 1855), 63.

20

Adolph Friedrich Christiani, The Principles of Expression in Pianoforte Playing (New York:

Harper & Brothers, 1885), 33.

21

“Time,” in George Grove, ed., A Dictionary of Music and Musicians, Vol. IV (London:

MacMillan and Co., 1889), 120, n3.

Page 40: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

19

Yet in all these verbal descriptions, it becomes evident that this rhythmical sense

of pulsation, stress, or emphasis was not explicit in notation—it was an assumed element

of musical proportion that resided off the page, and only actualized in performance. The

eighteenth-century Encyclopædia Britannica confirms that, “of the different times

included in a bar, though all are equal [in notation], yet some more strongly, and sensibly

marked than others. This distinction is expressed in execution by emphatic or accented

notes, and by such as are unaccented or common.”22

Prior to the mechanical regulation of

musical time, there existed an experiential, interpretive element to the movement, pace,

and accent of meter; a sonic measure, an aural pattern of recurring rhythmical pulsation.

Well through the nineteenth century, the “beat” that defined the musical proportion was

first and foremost a physical, sensory process. George Grove’s first edition Dictionary of

Music and Musicians states:

BEAT. The movement of the hand or baton by which the rhythm of a piece of

music is indicated, and by which a conductor ensures perfect agreement in tempo

and accent on the part of the orchestra or chorus; also, by analogy, the different

divisions of a bar or measure with respect to their relative accent.23

With no discussion of the clockwork metronome as an intrinsic aspect of performing to

the beat, an ensemble’s “perfect agreement” in time should not be misconstrued as

“perfect mechanical consistency.” Indeed, a special term for a musician skilled in the

sense of musical time was commonly employed prior to the invention and application of

the clockwork metronome; a performer who “accurately” or “perfectly” expressed the

musical proportion, meter, and accent was considered a good timeist—pedagogues and

22 “Time,” Encyclopædia Britannica, 8613. Emphasis added.

23 “Beat,” Grove I (1879), 158. Emphasis added.

Page 41: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

20

critics used the term throughout the nineteenth century; it curiously and perhaps

ominously receded by the twentieth century.24

To be a timeist was to have an innate

rhythmical skill disassociated from mechanical precision. According to an early

nineteenth-century edition of the Cyclopædia; or, Universal dictionary of arts, sciences,

and literature this innate rhythmical sense was recognized since antiquity:

“Rhythm”…was the principal point in [ancient Greek] music, without which they

regarded melody as wholly unmeaning and lifeless. Hence Plato refuses the title

of musician to every one who was not perfectly well versed in rhythm, as we

should now to a bad timeist. It is of such importance, that, without it, music can

have no power over the human passions.25

A translation of August Heinrich Julius Lafontaine’s novel the Village Pastor (1810)

summarized these qualities in one character:

She was a perfect timeist, and had a great deal of expression both in her execution

on the instrument, and in singing; but when she played extempore, which she did

frequently, not having any written or printed music, the slow melancholy strain

was what she preferred.26

Linguistic philosopher Joshua Steele had much to say about the innate rhythmic sense

that made someone a skilled timeist. Steele, similar to so many musicians and theorists

over the centuries, found every bodily movement in some way rhythmical. Thus the

24 See, for instance, “TIMEIST. A performer who preserves a just and steady time,” in H. W.

Pilkington, A Musical Dictionary (Boston: Watson & Bangs, 1812), 77.

25

See Abraham Rees, “Rhythm,” The cyclopædia; or, Universal dictionary of arts, sciences, and

literature. In forty-one volumes (Philadelphia: Samuel F. Bradford, and Murray, Fairman and Co.,

1805), [n.p.].

26

See Augustus La Fontaine, The village pastor and his children, translated from the German

(New-York: D. Longworth, 1810), 106-7.

Evidently, the term could describe dancers and orators as well. See, for instance, Sarah

Harriet Burney, Geraldine Fauconberg (Philadelphia: M. Carey and Son, 1817), 240: “The good

old-fashioned tune of ‘Come hast to the Wedding,’ being called, our dance began. William,

though perhaps wholly untaught, was far from being a clownish or perplexing partner: there is a

surprising degree of native ease, grace, and propriety in every thing he does; and he proved as

good a timeist as Caesar himself.”

Page 42: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

21

cultivation of rhythm was fundamentally a self-referential process. Hearkening to

Descartes’ epistemology, Steele reflects in Prosodia Rationalis:

Our animal existence being regulated by our pulse, we seem to have an intuitive

sense of rhythmus, as connected with, and governing, all sounds and all motions;

whence it follows, that we find all people feel the effects of rhythmus, as they do

those of light and warmth derived from the Sun; so that, without searching for the

reason, it has generally been passed over as a first principle, or self-evident truth.

The swing of the arm, and other such motions, made by public speakers, are

derived from their instinctive sense of rhythmus, and are, in effect, beating time to

their orations.27

Steele, throughout Prosodia Rationalis, suggested that Western rhythm had a long and

rich tradition, in which the human body was the primary reference for the insufficiently

documented practices of musical and spoken time. For Steele, as with Descartes before

him, rhythm was one of the most fundamental of human experiences:

Our breathing, the beating of our pulse, and our movement in walking, make the

division of time by pointed and regular cadences, familiar and natural to us. Each

of these movements, or cadences, is divided into two alternate motions,

significantly expressed by the Greek words arsis and thesis, raising and posing, or

setting down; the latter of which, coming down as it were with weight, is what we

mean to call heavy, being the most energic or emphatic of the two; the other,

being more remiss, and with less emphasis, we call light.

So when we lift our foot, in order to walk, that motion is arsis, or light;

and when we put it on the ground, in order to proceed, that aft of posing is thesis,

or heavy.28

As Steele recognized, to be a timeist was to hold an innate—and pre-metronomic—sense

of musical time, one that allowed for the subjective, creative interpretations of musical

accent, pulse, and proportion.

Confirming the subjective, sensory pulsation of musical time, early-modern

pedagogical treatises occasionally documented these internal, quiet, emphatic, or

27 Joshua Steele, Prosodia Rationalis, an Essay Towards Establishing the Melody and Measure of

Speech (London: T. Payne, 1779), 67.

28 Steele, 20.

Page 43: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

22

invisible accents, which every good timeist ought to recognize. Taken alongside standard

definitions of musical time, a wide variety of musical treatises attest to the insufficiency

of traditional notation in defining the fundamental sensory qualities of musical time,

rhythm, and proportion. They attest to a pre-metronomic ontology of musical time, in

which a successful timeist’s rhythmical feeling preceded mechanical rules or references.

In his Ausführlich theoretisch-practische Anweisung zum Piano-forte Spiel

(1828), Johann Nepomuk Hummel explicitly dictates for the musical novice, one who

strived to be a good timeist, where the heavy and light stresses fall in each meter.

Hummel also provides a telling graphic, a visual suggestion of physical weight, strength,

and velocity when beating time in two-, three-, and four-meters, both simple and

compound.

Page 44: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

23

Figure 1.1. The invisible weight of the musical pulse, made explicit by Johann Nepomuk Hummel. As

depicted, each meter is distinguished through the recurring sense of heavy and light. Hummel portrays the

standard knowledge of skilled musicians: measured music was conceived, heard, and performed with these

pulse-senses, which transcended rhythmical notation. From Johann Nepomuk Hummel, Ausführlich

theoretisch-practische Anweisung zum Piano-forte Spiel, volume 1 (Wein: Tobias Haslinger, 1828), 62-63.

It is significant to note that Hummel’s explication of musical weight, accent, and the

beating of time bares no relationship to his instructions on the metronome—mainly

derived from the Maelzel’s own words—that appears towards the very end of the

Page 45: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

24

composer’s treatise. (In his early London advertisements, Maelzel claimed that his

clockwork machine alongside his “Metronic Tutor” were “calculated to render the pupil a

steady timeist.”)29

Nevertheless, the implied sensory qualities of musical time preceded

and predominated the very new mechanical-tempo contrivance recently being introduced,

and critiqued, in the publications of Hummel and other skilled performer-composers.

Similar to Hummel’s important musical-time examples, Callcott’s popular A

Musical Grammar in Four Parts documented how repetitive-looking notations do not

contain mathematically or mechanically equal rhythms in performance; due to varying

degrees of emphasis within the bar, visually identical notes do not share audibly identical

“times.” As Hummel and Callcott both depicted, the meter or time signature implied the

auditory sensation of pulsation and accent—not the precise, mathematical equality of

every note within every bar.

Figure 1.2. With knowledge of the invisible, weighted accents common to all music in measure, Callcott

shows how seemingly identical musical passages take on completely different rhythmical qualities when set

in different meters. The capital “S” depicts the weightiest parts of each measure, in what amounts to a

minuet (3/4) and jig (6/8) rhythm, respectively. From John Wall Callcott, A Musical Grammar in Four

Parts, First American Edition (Boston: West & Blake and Manning & Loring, [1810]; Third Edition.

London: Robert Birchall, 1817), 258.

29 See [Classified Section,] Times, Feb. 19, 1817, 1.

Page 46: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

25

Intimately aware of the insufficiencies when relating the feeling of musical pulse to

notated rhythms, Christiani cited Köhler’s rhythmical-notational interpretations, in which

the traditional, sensory hierarchy of accents was likened to a husband-wife relationship,

extended through a rhythmic genealogy of sorts—with subdivided offspring.

Figure 1.3. Köhler’s rhythmical genealogy. Note that “m” and “f” are not volume indications, but signifiers

of the strong-weak, arsis-thesis, or “male-female” pairings commonly heard in rhythmical proportions.

From Adolph Friedrich Christiani, The Principles of Expression in Pianoforte Playing (New York: Harper

& Brothers, 1885), 48.

These subjective references for musical time were not lost on theorists later in the

nineteenth century, including Hugo Riemann, who was greatly concerned with the

problems of notating sensory, rhythmical nuances. He employed directional markings,

Page 47: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

26

not to be confused with dynamics or articulation marks, to express the weight and energy

of pulse for any given meter.

Figure 1.4. Two of Riemann’s graphical notations that depict the many nuances of rhythmical movement.

The indications above the note values represent directional “energy” (not dynamics) in which the most

emphasis or weight appears at the beginning of each bar. He also includes pause marks to express the

rhetorical qualities of breath, or lift, in any rhythmical arrangement. From Hugo Riemann, Musikalische

Dynamik und Agogik (Hamburg: D. Rahter, 1884), 71 (left) and 139 (right).

Similarly, the first edition Grove Dictionary depicted the weight of the musical accent,

fittingly enough, through heavily weighted strokes above the usually invisible metrical

pulse. The entry “Accent” also suggests that this temporal weight, a quality dissociated

from volume, was common to all music in classical traditions.

Page 48: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

27

Figure 1.5. Further depictions of the weighted pulse-sense in measured music. From the entry

“Accent” in Grove’s Dictionary of Music and Musicians, Volume 1 (1879), 15.

Gottfried Weber offered a striking confirmation of the non-notated feelings fundamental

to metered music by documenting how the visceral sensation of the invisible,

proportional accent was thwarted through dynamics. Weber depicted duple and triple

meters in which the internal emphases were occluded through loudly placed “weak

beats,” which opposed the customary sonic experience of musical time. He thus related:

What is here said of heavy and light parts of the measure is not to be so

understood as that a so called heavy or light part of the measure must really in all

cases be delivered more heavily and strongly (more forte) than the so called light

or weak part; we here speak rather of an internal weight which our rhythmical

feeling spontaneously gives to every heavy time. — Still however, so much as

this is true, that a kind of shock — a revulsive sensation is produced in our

feelings, if, on the contrary, a lighter time is rendered more prominent by a

greater external strength of tone than a time that is internally more heavy.”

Page 49: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

28

Figure 1.6. The “revulsive sensation” produced when uncommon volume changes counteracted the

traditional, non-notated pulse-accent. In the second bar of the bottom line, two volume indications are

absent, because the strong-weak pattern is already implied on those two notes through the time signature;

this is the one point when the volume corresponds to the invisible pulse-sense. From Gottfried Weber,

General Music Teacher, trans. by James Franklin Warner (Boston: J. H. Wilkins & R. B. Carter, 1841), 90.

Weber exposed the significant difference between the internal “weighted” sense of

musical time and an external emphasis created through volume. Accents, for Weber,

Steele, and the Grove Dictionary, were natural effects implied through the meter, whereas

strong dynamics—when placed on naturally weak beats—were often rhythmical

impositions, false accentuations that destroyed the metrical feeling (that sometimes were

intended by the composer). In Weber’s example the traditional feelings for musical time

were entirely effaced, thus creating an entirely uncommon and “shocking” rhythmical

aesthetic when compared to traditional, sensory epistemologies of musical time.

Steele, too, mentioned the essential distinction, seemingly known for ages, that

the weighted measure-accent and volume-emphasis were two different musical qualities:

Page 50: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

29

The affections of heavy and light were always felt in music, though erroneously

called by some moderns accented and unaccented; however, the accented, or

heavy note, was never understood to be necessarily loud, and the other necessarily

soft; because if it were so, there could be no occasion for separate directions,

where to apply the forte and piano, in as much as the affections of heavy and light

are continued in every cadence of every air, from the beginning to the end:

whereas the forte and piano are often applied directly contrary to heavy and light;

as in the following example, almost all the heavy notes are piano, and the light

notes, forte.30

Figure 1.7. Steele depicts how the invisible, arsis-thesis rhythm intrinsic to the bar need not correspond

with volume. He marks this passage with solid triangles ( ) that signify the strong pulse-accent, while the

figures underneath them ( ’ ) indicate softness, thus showing that the weight of the bar was a sensory-

temporal quality unrelated to dynamical strength. From Joshua Steele, Prosodia Rationalis, an Essay

Towards Establishing the Melody and Measure of Speech (London: T. Payne, 1779), 69.

Steele points to discrepancies, not in the traditional feeling of rhythm in speech and

music, but in the limited terminology and notational conventions at musicians’ disposal

for articulating the aural sensation of accent. For pedagogues concerned with musical

time, the complex practices that comprised an artistic performance were primarily non-

verbal and anti-notational. Words and ink dots were not enough to ensure the subjective

feeling of musical time. The end goal of musical time as suggested by many pedagogues

was expression, not exactitude, prompting Christiani to remark: “It has been said with

truth, ‘Upon accents the spirit of music depends, because without them there can be no

30 Steele, 68-9.

Page 51: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

30

expression. Without them, there is no more melody in song than in the humming of a

bee.’”31

As these sources relate, musicality, expressivity, and living rhythm ultimately

eluded any exacting description. Thus, the proportional rules of subdivision, as described

for centuries by theorists from Descartes through Riemann, were not, and could not be

performed as strict mathematical absolutes where human time-dictation and rhythmical

sensation were concerned. In support of an anti-scientific epistemology, the eighteenth-

century Encyclopædia Britannica criticized Rousseau’s concept that all note values

however minutely subdivided contained a strong-weak (thesis and arsis) rhythmical

quality. The source claimed that any such exacting distinctions mentioned by Rousseau

were impossible to perform as note-values became more rhythmically infinitesimal.

Before the metronome dictated the musical beat, this significant eighteenth-century

reference questioned how such human-derived rhythmical actions could achieve such

seemingly inhuman precisions:

[since] notes may be so minutely subdivided, as by the shortness of their duration

to be rendered incapable of emphasis; and we should be glad to know how

[Rousseau], either with his hand, his foot, or this thought, could distinguish the

perfect [strong, heavy] and imperfect [weak, light] times of a demi-semiquaver.32

Joshua Steele, in his Prosodia rationalis, offered answers as to why such rhythmical

precisions were both uncommon and nearly impossible to conceive through traditional

human references, such as the hand, foot, or intangible temporal conceptions. Steele

asserted late-eighteenth-century performers, when they perceived and actualized the bar

31 Christiani, 22.

32

See “Time,” Encyclopædia Britannica, 8613.

Page 52: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

31

through an initial pulse-accent, valued something other than the small and mathematically

equal subdivisions of notated rhythms. He stated, “when modern musicians refer to any

thing like a standard for time, it is to a maximum, which they suppose may be subdivided

to infinity by sub-duples or sub-triples.”33

He maintained that musicians defined time

through the larger feelings of pulse, not the mathematical equation of small metronomic

rhythms. Steele related a temporal epistemology drastically opposed to mechanical

precision or mathematical subdivisions, which instead relied upon broad rhythmical

sensations to define and dictate a more human variation of musical time. Such human

references to rhythmical movement, as Steele well understood, were inherently variable:

The beating of our pulse, which we must feel whenever we are silent and inactive,

prones us to rhythmical divisions even in the series of our thoughts; as soon as we

begin to move, our steps succeed in the government of rhythmical pulsation, and

the measure may then be at our option faster or slower; for while we were silent

and motionless, the measure of our thoughts must have been regulated by the

cadences or our pulse, which is an involuntary motion…If the step or pace

[governs the time or length of a cadence], then between walking and running there

is a latitude for great variety.34

If there was any temporal constant in the reference to pre-metronomic musical

performances, it was in this subjective sensation for accent, not in constant speed or

precise beat-per-minute regularity. Moreover, many sources noted that when the regular

pulse sensation was lacking, it elicited visceral problems of rhythmical perception for

both auditors and performers. Apart from notating the usually invisible accent for the

sake of education, the nineteenth-century Grove Dictionary documented a “curiosity” of

33 Steele, 131.

34 Ibid., 118-119.

Page 53: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

32

musical time from Liszt, in which mixed meters belied any regular pulse sensation. The

musical dictionary concludes:

It is impossible to reduce this passage to any known rhythm; but when the first

feeling of strangeness is past there is a peculiar and quaint charm about the music

which no other combination would have produced.35

Figure 1.8. A passage from Liszt’s oratorio Christus, which the first edition Grove dictionary exemplified

for being a curiosity of indecipherable rhythm; the unusual and strange pattern of musical meters, accents,

and gestures made any pulse-sense unrecognizable and ambiguous. From the entry “Accent,” Grove’s

Dictionary, Vol. 1 (1879), 15.

The terminology here is telling: “Rhythm” and “accent” were often synonymous or

interchangeable when defining musical time. Throughout the nineteenth century,

musical-temporal terminology remained exceedingly ambiguous, and in the entry

“rhythm,” the same publication concludes: “In short, Rhythm is the Meter of Music.”36

As Descartes mentioned centuries prior, perceiving meter through the sense of accent

made rhythm knowable, real, and expressive. Without musical accent there could be no

meter; without meter there could be no musical rhythm. Given Liszt’s Christus and other

a-rhythmical excerpts, in which no “rhythm” or accent was consistently decipherable, the

35 Grove, I (1879), 15.

36

See “Rhythm,” Grove, IV (1889), 123.

Page 54: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

33

Grove Dictionary politely suggested that they “are in no way to be recommended as

models for imitation.”37

The sensory curiosities of musical rhythm extended to odd meters such as 5/4—a

time that, for many pre-twentieth-century theorists, held no consistently conceivable

pulse-emphasis. While a metronomic-mathematic equivalence could certainly be adduced

from unusual meters such as 5/4 and 7/11, the fact that these meters could neither be

perceived nor actualized with any collective degree of understanding verifies that

physical sensations—not precise, metronomic calculations—grounded musical time prior

to the twentieth-century in both theory and practice. Indeed, Descartes mentioned the

sensory-temporal problems in executing such odd beat-groupings as early as 1618.

Steele, over a century and a half later, went so far as to state that “rhythmical time is only

capable of being generically divided either by the even number two, or by the odd

number three; but that the numbers seven, eleven, thirteen, seventeen, nineteen, &c., are

non-rhythmical divisors.”38

Prior to the twentieth century, notable pedagogues found odd

meters beyond the triple-bar to be interpretively problematic and rhythmically

ambiguous; they often heard these irregular bars as a non-sensory and anti-intuitive

collection of beats—lacking in any perceptible “rhythm.” For each bar of 5/4, for

instance, the measure could be ascertained in the following ways: with a sense of triple-

duple, duple-triple, a quintuple with initial accent, or as a uniformly monotonous

quintuple without an initial accent—which negated the purpose of the time signature to

37 Ibid. An acquaintance of the composer, Walter Damrosch attested that Liszt’s influence for

Christus was the flowing “rhythmical” qualities of non-metrical Gregorian chant. See Walter

Damrosch, My Musical Life (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, 1923), 49-50.

38

Steele, 128.

Page 55: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

34

begin with. For Weber, this 5/4 arrangement was highly unnatural to both hear and

perform:

Such a rhythm has an especially halting and dragging character, arising from the

fact that a grouping of this kind has too little of emphasis, that is to say, too many

light parts of the measure for one that is strong. In five-fold measure, e.g. only the

first part would be heavy, while all the following four would be light; and in

seven-fold measure there would be even six light parts to one heavy one, etc.

Such a scantiness of accented parts of the measure cannot be otherwise than

wearisome to the ear.39

Given the sensory problems with odd meters, Weber prophesized—for a musical culture

pre-dating ubiquitous metronomic references—“those species of measure will never meet

with general acceptance, or come into general use.”40

However, he considered that under

certain compositional instances, when special effects were required, the visceral

weirdness—the “unrhythmicalness”—of such odd meters could be used to an emotional

advantage. Through this realization, Weber again exposed the living fundamentals of

musical time:

In the course of a piece of music, our rhythmical sense sometimes tolerates

quintóles, septoles, and the like, in which, on account of the quickness of the

transition over them, the unrhythmicalness of the division is almost imperceptible,

(at least to the hearer, though by the player the difficulty, and I might say,

unnaturalness, of such quintóle or septole division is very sensibly felt.)41

Beyond uncustomary meters, the composer’s intentional muddling of the recurring pulse

sense was known as Tempo d'Imbroglio. Imbroglio was a rhythmical technique that again

attested to the expected, non-notated sensations of time—common to all measured

music—that could be thwarted for specific emotional-temporal effects. John Wall

39 Weber, 105.

40

Ibid., 106.

41

Ibid., 107.

Page 56: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

35

Callcott described the “embroiled” style, in which the prevailing pulse sensation overrode

the time signature—and the implicit accent indication: “To the same species of effect

which is derived from Emphasis, may be referred the Tempo d'Imbroglio (della

Confusione) of modern Music, in which the Music, although written in one kind of

Measure, is really performed in another.”42

But as with all terms concerning time and

rhythm, meanings change drastically over the decades. Weber later suggested imbroglio

and rubato shared temporal similarities due to the composer’s (not the performer’s)

skewing of the basic pulse sense, traditionally gleaned from the meter. Exemplifying a

minuet passage by Haydn, Weber relates, “Such revulsive, jerking, jolting rhythmical

movements frequently occur. They are sometimes designated by the expression tempo

rubato, i.e. stolen or robbed time, and also by the word confusion.”43

Figure 1.9. Two examples of Tempo d’imbroglio—the “revulsive” displacement of the traditional, non-

notated pulse-sensation. Above, a Haydn minuet, reprinted in Weber’s A universal dictionary of musical

terms, 49. Below, A passage from Beethoven’s Third Symphony reprinted in Christiani, 72. Weber

alternatively considered Haydn’s passage an example of tempo rubato, attesting to an imprecision or

generality in the use of these terms.

42 John Wall Callcott, A Musical Grammar in Four Parts, First American Edition (Boston: West

& Blake and Manning & Loring, [1810]; Third Edition. London: Robert Birchall, 1817), 261.

43 James F. Warner and Gottfried Weber, A Universal Dictionary of Musical Terms (Boston: J. H.

Wilkins & R. B. Carter, 1842), 49.

Page 57: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

36

Christiani confirmed, “An imbroglio is caused by irregular accentuation, sometimes by

syncopation, but more frequently by the intermingling of several voices, each voice

accenting its phrases independently, as though a room full of people were talking together

at the same time, the result being that the grammatical [invisible and weighted metrical]

accents are no longer to be detected by the ear. Hence, a rhythmical confusion.”44

Thus,

to perceive the visceral, rhythmical juxtaposition called “imbroglio” was to acknowledge

its distinction from the traditional pulse feeling, the sensory accent that was once

implied—but seldom notated—in measured music throughout the Western tradition.

The term for the traditional perception and actualization of musical meter in the

Western tradition was sometimes called tempo ordinario. As with “imbroglio” and even

“rubato,” the word “ordinario” did not simply reduce to a metronomic rate of speed; this

realization is especially apparent when considering that these terms existed before the

metronomic scale came into use. Tempo ordinario implied a “time” commonly sensed in

duple or triple meter; it was a rhythmical quality, not an exacting or consistent beats-per-

minute number. With this understanding of the term, Beethoven, in his final year,

considered metronome numbers as a necessary rehearsal expedient because performers

could not often glean this “ordinary” pulse-sense from the composer’s most challenging

later-period works. Additional guidance seemed warranted, since—as his creativity

demanded—Beethoven intentionally obscured or thwarted the metrical accent, the

implied and expected pulse-sense, altogether through techniques such as imbroglio,

44 Christiani, 72.

Page 58: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

37

rubato, phrase fragmentation, or syncopation.45

Clearly suggesting that compositions with

traditional meter-to-pulse relationships did not require mechanical guidance, Beethoven

wrote to Bernhard Schotts Söhne in December 1826, admitting “such things [metronome

markings] are certainly necessary…We can scarcely have any more tempi ordinari, for

one must follow the ideas of unfettered genius.”46

Indeed, Beethoven’s unique treatment of musical rhythm seemed difficult to

interpret for many during his age, and due to his “unfettered” or idiosyncratic meters,

phrase structures, and rhythmic gestures, an external measure may have provided a

“necessary”—but still incomplete—aid for novitiate performers of his more complicated

music. During the very same month in which he wrote the above comment, Beethoven

received a letter (dated December 12, 1826)—perhaps prompting the composer’s

realizations of a “metronomic necessity”—from Xaver Schnyder von Wartensee, who

attested to the temporal confusion engendered by some of Beethoven’s latest works.

Wartensee reports that within the variations of Beethoven’s Piano Sonata in C minor, Op.

111, certain time signatures “[produce] various opinions here [Frankfurt am Main?] and

occasions an aesthetic dispute.” The composer and educator Wartensee proceeds to

inquire about,

45 In at least one music publication, Beethoven used the term “ordinario” as a temporal indicator

(again, without metronome numbers). In his Serenade for flute, violin, and viola, Op. 25,

Beethoven states “Tempo ordinario d’un Menuetto,” which attests to the common pulse-sense

that this particular dance movement ought to project.

Ironically perhaps, the term “tempo ordinario,” by the twentieth century, had become

synonymous with 4/4, duple, or alle breve meters, not triple meters. See for instance, Grove

Dictionary of Music and Musicians V (New York: The Macmillan Company, 1910), 70.

Ludwig van Beethoven, Beethoven's letters: a critical edition, with explanatory notes by Dr. A.

C. Kalischer, translated by J. S. Shedlock, Volume 2 (London: J. M. Dent, 1909), 458.

Page 59: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

38

the second variation, which you designated 12/16 [actually 6/16]…and wrote

‘l’istesso Tempo,’ did you mean it in regard to the beat and want for the sixteenth

with the thirty-second to be played exactly as fast as the eighth with the sixteenth

in the [introductory] theme; or did you want the whole figure played just as fast as

[an eighth and sixteenth] where ‘l’istesso Tempo’ then does not designate

individual notes but the primary division of the bar. Likewise, in the third

variation, which is designated 12/32 in the figure where ‘l’istesso Tempo’ occurs

again, did you want the two notes [a 32nd

and 64th

] played exactly as fast as the

aforementioned figure [an eighth and sixteenth] in your theme;—or did you want

the whole passage played in the same time?47

Upon reading this letter, Beethoven may have recognized that certain levels of confusion

would always exist for those who could not ascertain a tempo-sense from his more

modern publications, and that many consumer-performers were not up to the rhythmical

challenges presented by his “genius.” Under these circumstances, metronome numbers

might have assuaged further inquiries similar to Wartensee’s. Yet, even with Beethoven’s

occasional statements in support of the beats-per-minute scale, contradictory evidence

abounds in the practice and publication of Beethoven’s music during his lifetime. For

instance, Wartensee did not request any metronomic indications or translations in the

above correspondence; he oppositely considered tempi though a more traditional

epistemology based on the printed meters, “the primary division of the bar,” and their

corresponding pulse-sense relationships. And while Beethoven told Bernhard Schotts

Söhne that the metronomic numbers were now “certainly necessary” for music that

extended beyond an ordinary pulse-sense, the majority of Beethoven’s compositions,

including the autograph manuscript of Op. 111,48

contained a host of Italian time and

47 See Theodore Albrecht, ed., Letters to Beethoven and Other Correspondence, Volume 2

(Lincoln & London: University of Nebraska Press, 1996), Letter 448, 159-160.

48

See Ludwig van Beethoven, Piano Sonata no. 32 in C minor, Op. 111, reproduction of the

autograph manuscript (New York: Dover Publications, Inc. 1968).

Page 60: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

39

affect words while failing to offer such “necessary” mechanical markings. In the end,

Beethoven never answered Wartensee’s or the aforementioned “disputers’” tempo

questions with metronome numbers or words49

and inconsistencies abound in interpreting

Op. 111 to this day.50

Figure 1.10. Detail of the autograph manuscript of Beethoven’s Piano Sonata no. 32, Op. 111, last

movement, which begins in an unusual meter of 9/16. Throughout the rhythmically and metrically

challenging set of variations, Beethoven provides ample Italian descriptions of tempo and affect, yet not

one metronome indication.

Decades prior to Beethoven’s unique rhythmical inventions and intentions as displayed in

his Op. 111, Joshua Steele presented one of the most telling notations of the traditional

sensory and subjective musical accent—likely alluding to Beethoven’s tempi ordinari—

unadorned by rhythmical complications such as imbroglio, rubato, syncopation, or

49 Albrecht, 161, fn2. Albrecht quotes Anton Schindler as confirmation.

Perhaps adding to the confusion, Schindler considered that works similar to Op. 111

necessitated playing in a “tempo di bravura,” which seemed to be a more unwavering and rapid

tempo than an ordinario. Schindler comments on this uncustomary and specific approach to

Beethoven’s music: “There are several [sonatas] in which a strict observance of time [tempo] is

indispensable; scarcely permitting, much less demanding, any deviation from regularity. Those

compositions require to be played in what is termed bravura style; they are Op. 106, 111, 57, and

some others.” Schindler also considered “bravura movements…few among [Beethoven’s]

compositions” and suggested these display “mechanical” effects opposite to those based upon

sentiment and expression. See Schindler, ed. Moscheles, 130, 107-108, 120.

50 For a comprehensive analysis and discussion of the interpretive issues, see Joanna Goldstein, A

Beethoven Enigma: Performance Practice and the Piano Sonata, Opus 111, American university

studies, Series XX (New York: Peter Lang, 1988).

Page 61: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

40

unusual time signatures. While he was ultimately concerned with applying musical-time

concepts to speech notation, Steele considered the fundamental duple or triple sense of

rhythm to be so ingrained in skilled performers that—if given a lengthy series of quarter

notes without a notated time signature—they would instinctually perceive the

proportional pulse of arsis and thesis—again without any visual guidance. Steele, like

Descartes, Weber, Marx, and Christiani, described a rhythmical sensation, an “instinctive

sense”51

inaccessible within the text, which was only realized through experience. Steele

states:

The metronomic appearance of the above passage is uncommon and striking: a redundant

succession of quarter notes with no notated meter, no prescribed weighted accent. For

eighteenth-century musicians, Steele’s example offered no “rhythm” to speak of, only a

general rate of movement gauged through a self-referential stride. Given this scant

printed information, Steele asserted that musicians’ natural rhythmic sense would

eventually lead to either one of two interpretations, printed below, in which the solid

triangles depicted the invisible weight, the “quiet dwelling” of the sensory accent, either

in a duple or triple rhythmical proportion. Due to the subjective, sensory foundations of

musical time, Steele depicted how visually redundant notes would “naturally” transform

into audibly rhythmical passages:

51 Steele, 220.

Page 62: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

41

If this series be repeated, without intermission, the singer or player will be led

instinctively to lay the emphatic pulses so as to divide into cadences or bars of

common measure, or [depending upon the length of the final note] the player will

be instinctively inclined to divide it into cadences of triple-meter. 52

Figure 1.11. Steele’s depiction of the invisible, weighted sense of meters, gleaned by skilled musicians out

of visually redundant series of quarter notes. The solid triangles depict the heavy tones of thesis. The dotted

triangles depict the light sense of arsis. Another degree, the two-dot symbol, is the “lightest” rhythm in this

triple meter. This common epistemology of musical time predates the metronome and the metronomic

regulation of musical time.

Steele, in order to express more directly the time of speech, exposed the time of measured

music in the process—the “time” of Shakespeare’s Sir Toby, Descartes, and Christiani: a

traditional and lasting epistemology in which meter and rhythm flourished far afield from

the tick-tock time of the clock. Steele’s two musical “times” depicted above were defined

not by mathematical or metronomic rates of speed, but by two alternate perceptions of

human pulse and rhythm. As others documented as well, the fundamentals of musical

time transcended notation, mechanical measurement, and even the composer’s intentions.

The various elements of musical time, including metric pulsation, accentuation, alongside

the effects of rubato and imbroglio were fundamental rhythmic expressions beyond the

52 Ibid., 219, 220.

Page 63: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

42

mere and limited conventions of notational and mechanical reference. For those who

actively sensed and experienced “rhythm,” the human subjectivities of pulse, nuance, and

movement grounded all of music notation. As Descartes through Christiani related—and

before the metronome helped redefine the musical measure with mechanical

consistency—rhythmical feeling trumped rhythmical notation. Prior to Maelzel’s

metronome, musical “time” was first and foremost within the timeist.

The Self-Referential Tempo

The sensory accent was the common guide and reference of measured, musical time. But,

as witnessed in Steele’s above example, meter and its necessary pulse-emphasis did not

always prescribe another quality of “tempo,” defined as the general pace of that recurring

emphasis. As noted for centuries, the primary techniques to find basic tempo-rates were

still self-referential, subjective, and personal. The traditional tactus reference—certainly

practiced since the Renaissance—made visible that recurring pulse-accent. Thus for

many musicians and theorists of pre-metronomic performance culture, conceptions of

accent, measure, and tempo were intricately linked through implicit or explicit human

tactus-indication. Considering the interconnectivity between pulse, meter, and tempo,

Christiani believed that since the musical accent was an absolute, albeit variable quality

of measured musical performance—and that all duple or triple meters should sound as

such—the pulse-sense alone set the quality and rate of movement for any given work. For

this late-nineteenth-century pedagogue, tempo and tempo variations were always

beholden to the traditional sense of pulse, meter, and the corresponding rhythms in any

composition:

Page 64: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

43

The rhythm is the chief carrier of the composer's idea, and cannot be

changed without impairing that idea.

The grammatical [weighted] accents, which follow the rhythm, cannot be

changed either.

If, therefore, time [tempo] and rhythm, or time and rhythmical accents, do

not agree…it must be the time [tempo] which is wrong. It may, in fact, be laid

down as a general principle, that

It is always the rhythm that decides the time [tempo], and not the time

which prescribes the rhythm.53

The audible rhythm as defined through the invisible accent, as Christiani explained,

projected the correct tempo. He alludes to the prevailing pre-metronomic aesthetic: tempo

was an experiential process, not a mechanical absolute dictated through printed tempo

indications, either through words or metronome numbers. (By the time of Christiani’s

publication, Maelzel’s metronome was being manufactured for nearly 60 years.) While

the pulse-accent was an expected quality, the speed or rate of movement was always

interpretive. Such realizations appear for nearly three centuries prior to Christiani’s

assertion: tempo was not an infallible rule imposed by words, machines, or numbers.

Beyond the sense of tactus or pulse as indicated by the meter, the most common

references for the rate of movement stemmed, yet again, from physical processes such as

walking, limping, or running—such prevailing tempo conceptualizations are well

documented, and exist apart from the musical accent. For instance, Michel Saint-Lambert

in his Principles of the Harpsichord remarked of musical movement: “For myself I have

fixed it as much as possible in the method book by the comparison I gave of a man

walking [to a quarter note] slowly, sometimes fast. But what I was able to communicate

53 Christiani, 88.

Page 65: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

44

by comparison is not definite enough.”54

Subjectivities were always present in tempo

reference, due to the self-referential nature of musical time and the insufficiencies of

musical notation, a fact which prompted Saint-Lambert to remark that “time

signatures…indicate the movement of pieces only very imperfectly” and that musicians

“all use the same terminology, but they do not all understand [affect words] in the same

way.”55

To find a tempo prior to the twentieth century was to find a “tempo

generalization” from the meter, melodic and harmonic contexts, occasionally aided by

words that conveyed emotions and actions, not mechanical precisions. Thus, as many

seventeenth- and eighteenth-century theorists asserted, the techniques that offered the

greatest ease and convenience in realizing general tempos during musical performances

were often preferred to complex tempo-machines, such as pendulums and later clockwork

metronomes, which defined pulse-movements with increasingly precise and more

inhuman exactitudes. Jean-Philippe Rameau, who well recognized and agreed with

Descartes’ rhythmical epistemology for measured music,56

considered self-referential

techniques standard, simple, and accurate enough for musicians to glean necessarily

subjective tempos:

54 Michel de Saint Lambert, Principles of the Harpsichord (Paris, 1702), translated and edited by

Rebecca Harris-Warrick (London: Cambridge University Press, 1984), 44.

55 Ibid.

56

Jean-Philippe Rameau, Traité de l'harmonie reduite à ses principes naturels, livre second

(Ballard: Paris, 1722), 150.

Page 66: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

45

We can hardly compare meter to anything simpler than the movements which are

natural to us; when these are repeated, they are always equal [regular]. We may

observe this in walking, provided that we walk naturally.

As the meter may be divided into several beats, each step taken in walking

may be considered to be one beat; just as we can walk faster or slower, the meter

may be faster or slower.57

Quantz similarly found that the ease of personal tempo-reference far outweighed the

incorporation of external, and often tedious new technologies, such as Loulié’s

experimental simple pendulum. In his Versuch, Quantz took great care to explain the

most sufficient human references for musical time:

The means that I consider most useful as a guide for tempo [in opposition to the

obscure, eight-foot chronomêtre of Loulié] is the more convenient because of the

ease with which it is obtained, since everyone always has it upon himself. It is the

pulse beat at the hand of a healthy person.58

Admitting to the long and trustworthy tradition of self-referential tempo dictation, Quantz

stated, “I cannot boast of being the first to come upon this device.” Similar to Rameau, he

suggested that musicians had the innate ability of finding what amounts to a personally

“correct” and “equal” tempo for any given work and performance. Due to individual

proclivities, Quantz explained that tempos would be slower or faster according to each

performer, and “it also would do no harm in any case if a melancholy person, in accord

with his temperament, were to play a piece moderately fast, but still well; and if a more

volatile person played it with greater liveliness.”59

Even with his knowledge of simple

pendulums and pendulum clocks, Quantz somewhat sarcastically announced:

57 Rameau, Traité de l'harmonie, translated by Philip Gossett (New York: Dover Publications

[1971]), 197-8.

58

Johann Joachim Quantz, Versuch einer Anweisung die Flöte traversiere zu spielen (Berlin: J. F.

Voss, 1752); translated by Edward R. Reilly as On playing the flute (Boston: Northeastern

University Press, 2001), 183.

59 Ibid., 289.

Page 67: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

46

Beyond this [pulse-reference], if someone could discover a simpler, more

accurate, and more convenient device for learning tempos and establishing them,

he would do well not to delay in communicating it to the public.60

While Quantz indeed desired more methodical and definitive methods for establishing the

basic tempos in modern compositions, he nevertheless recognized that the heartbeat-

reference of musical tempo displayed a high degree of variability, regardless of the

performer’s wellbeing. The pulse fluctuated naturally due to emotions, temperament, and

other personal traits. Quantz recited standard knowledge that the pulse was an

exceedingly variable organ, prone to every subjective whim, action, and inclination. Thus

he reticently acknowledged that tempos were always somewhat personal, due to

variations in human-derived rhythm:

One might object that the pulse beat is neither constant at each hour of the day,

nor the same in every person, as would be required to accurately fix tempos with

it. It will be said that the pulse beats more slowly in the morning before meal-time

than in the afternoon after meal-time, and still faster at night than in the afternoon;

likewise that it is slower in a person inclined to melancholy than in an impetuous

and jovial person.61

He nevertheless prescribed techniques that seemed exceedingly unnecessary and

inconvenient for skilled musicians such as himself: “if you take the pulse beat as it is

found from the midday meal till evening, and as it is found in a jovial and high-spirited

and yet rather fiery and volatile person, or, if you will permit the expression, in a person

of choleric-sanguine temperament, as your basis, you will have hit upon the correct pulse

beat.”62

Quantz related through his instructions that the heartbeat was no clockwork

60

Ibid.

61

Ibid., 288.

62 Ibid.

Page 68: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

47

metronome—far from it, as the nearly contemporaneous Encyclopædia Britannica also

considered the myriad ways in which the living pulse could fluctuate. A brief excerpt

explains:

PULSE, in the animal economy, denotes the beating or throbbing of the heart and

arteries…with regard to motion, the pulses are reckoned only four; great and little,

quick and slow. When quickness and greatness are joined together, it becomes

violent; and when it is little and slow, it is called a weak pulse…The pulses of

persons differ according to the largeness of the blood, the elastic force of the

canals; as also with regard to the sex, age, season, air, motion, food, sleep,

watchings, and passions of the mind. The pulse is larger and more quick in men

than in women, in the bilious and sanguineo-bilious, than in the phlegmatic and

melancholic…In children, the pulse is quick and soft; in adults, greater and more

violent. In the old, it is commonly great, hard, and slow…Intense speaking

increases the circulation, and consequently renders the pulse large and quick. In

watching, the pulse is more evident; in sleep, more slow and languid. After

drinking hot things, such as coffee and tea, or hot bath-waters, as well as after

meals, the pulse vibrates more quick. But nothing produces a greater change in

the pulse than affections of the mind.63

Beyond his knowledge that the human pulse was volatile and variable for every given

person and moment of the day, Quantz never conceived any heartbeat—even the stable,

jovial, or healthy pulse—to be a consistent mechanical regulator of rhythm, accent, or

tempo; traditional meanings of musical movement still held sway. Despite

misconceptions engendered through some twentieth-century tempo scholarship, he

outright acknowledged that the human pulse was not to be utilized as monotonous

clockwork for music making—an inconceivable practice for Quantz in any case:

63 See Encyclopædia Britannica, 6534-6535.

Page 69: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

48

I do not pretend that a whole piece should be measured off in accordance with the

pulse beat; this would be absurd and impossible. My aim is simply to show how

in at least two, four, six, or eight pulse beats, any tempo you wish can be

established, and how you can achieve a knowledge of the various categories of

tempo by yourself that will lead you to further inquiry. After some practice, an

idea of tempo will gradually so impress itself upon your mind that it will no

longer be found necessary always to consult the pulse beat.64

Quantz did not offer his instructions for finding basic tempos through the heartbeat as an

infallible scientific method for performance. Although he attested to the usefulness of

referencing a long-case clock as a last resort, Quantz still encouraged a great deal of

personal leeway in the establishment of any specific tempo, which he believed should

take into consideration the performance circumstances. In his Versuch, Quantz’s temporal

epistemology—despite the assertions of some twentieth-century scholars—was never

metronomic, or transferable to metronomic standards; he never suggested that a clicking

machine defined musical pulse or the rhythm of each musical measure—either in sonatas

or dance music. Rather he related that the human heartbeat, as it always had in measured

music, could generally establish the initial “true” (not a mechanically exact, precise, or

unflinching) tempo of an instrumental composition. Indeed, Quantz explicitly condoned

performers to vary the tempo for certain works, relating that “It is common knowledge

once or more times consecutively, particularly a fast piece…is played a little faster the

second time than the first.” For significant reasons he left undocumented in his own

compositions, Quantz instructs this temporal fluctuation was necessary so as not to “put

the listeners to sleep.”65

64 Quantz, trans. Reilly, 284.

65 Ibid., 289.

Page 70: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

49

Quantz did urge instrumentalists to rigorously adhere to French dance tempos for

the sake of dancers, but his techniques are telling: musicians, by “attending to the fall of

[dancers’] feet,” will attain an “accuracy of tempo.” Quantz describes a “time” equally as

volatile and variable during dance performances, stating: “It is well known that most

dancers understand little or nothing of music, and frequently do not know the tempo

themselves; for the most part they regulate themselves only by the mood at the moment,

or by their ability. Experience also teaches that dancers rarely require as lively a tempo at

rehearsals that take place in the morning before eating.”66

Yet again Quantz’s definition

of correct tempo was not in mechanical precision, but a far more complex, physical

correlation between the dancers’ physical movements and the musicians’ pulse-sense.

Despite the basics of music notation, these and many other sources relate a

complexity of experience beyond the reductions and limitations inevitable in any text,

complexities of meaning beyond any absolute mechanical calculations or measures of

time, beyond the fundamental schema of rhythmic proportionality. Weber recognized that

rhythmic basics he taught to amateur pianists did not encompass the reality of musical

time as actualized by performers and heard by audiences. Indeed, Weber forewarned his

readers that the comprehensive understanding of rhythm and tempo could not be

ascertained from pure theory or technical fundamentals alone, as he stated in his General

Music Teacher:

66 See Quantz, trans. Reilly, 289-290.

Page 71: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

50

The divisions of the measure, together with all the other greater and smaller

distinctions in rhythmical measurement, as we have thus far attended to them, are

properly the mere dry frame-work, that is to say, merely the measure of

rhythmical structures, but by no means those structures themselves; in the same

way as the yard, foot, inch, and line-measure, and the proportions according to

which an architectural work is measured, or the pattern or dimensions according

to which a pillar is measured, are not the structure or the pillar itself. In other

words, we have thus far directed our attention to the rhythmical measure merely

as measure.67

Beyond the Musical Measure

The self-referential tempo, the basic pulse-sense associated with physical activities such

as walking, limping, running, or tactus-marking were not the final answers to musical

time: these, of course, were only the fundamentals, the points of departure for more

complex creative practices. Aside from the human measure of musical proportionality

and pulse, more advanced subjective processes actively guided and shaped underlying

rhythmical sensations for any given composition, genre, age, and national style.

Since notation often failed to expose the accent or precise nature of tempo, it

likewise could not depict the many rhythmical nuances and gestures of musical time that

went beyond the meter and rate of movement for each and every bar. Over the centuries,

various theorists and musicians alluded to the fact that musical time for any pre-twentieth

century composition was far more complex than could be seen through notation, or

referenced through machines. While a comprehensive survey of the many qualitative

rhythmical techniques—specific to various repertoires, nations, and eras—cannot be

approached here, a few verbal and notation documents spanning three centuries attest that

musical time held far greater human complexities and subjectivities than could ever

67 Weber, 109.

Page 72: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

51

definitively, accurately, or absolutely be told through clockwork time, or beats-per-

minute dictations.

In the nineteenth century, Adolph Bernhard Marx expressed his own sense for

rhythm and alluded to the fact that musical time was a complex array of personal

perceptions only meagerly reflected in notation. In considering external references to

“rhythm,” Marx found that musical time was closely related not to clocks or metronomic

machines, but to the broad, recurring movements witnessed in the natural world. Perhaps

with an overt desire to romanticize all acts of musical creation, Marx recounted the

variable natural rhythms that most related to living performance—and most opposed

mechanical inventions—while offering his own subjective and volatile experiences of

pulse, proportion, and tempo:

In nature, we observe such a periodical succession of equal or unequal moments

of time and force in the beating of the pulse, in the rise and fall of the tide, in the

motion of the waves, &c. &c. In the doings of man, this succession reveals itself

in the form of "rhythm," with its two elements, time [duration] and ictus

(emphasis, accent). Every single moment is measured by these two elements. I

dwell upon a moment so long as it engages my interest and attention, or until I am

drawn away by another of greater force of attraction; I hurry from moment to

moment when a lively sensation impels me onward, and I expend a greater

amount of force and energy upon those moments which are to me the most

important. In this, we leave it out of consideration, for the present, whether in

dwelling a longer time and with greater emphasis upon certain moments, we are

guided by the intrinsic importance and objective contents of those moments (as in

prose declamation and in recitatives), or whether we do it merely for the sake of

variety or with a view to order and symmetry.68

In this passage, Marx expresses a significant and intrinsic element to pre-metronomic

rhythm: musical time, being self-referential, was always a living process informed by

individual choice. Active, motive expressions created musical time, which was often

68 Marx, 32-33.

Page 73: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

52

tempered through the interpreter’s gestural, linguistic, and rhetorical affinities. As Marx

explained, musical time was born of willful, continual action, and not a passive reaction

to external, unwavering regulations.

From the same century, yet with far more utilitarian intentions, Czerny also

described the subjective, willful choices that shaped the course of musical time:

When any musical idea, any group, or phrase, or passage, recurs in various places

of a composition, then the performer is not only at liberty, but it should be his

duty, to alter the mode of rendering at each repetition, in order to avoid

monotony.69

Some of these volatile, temporal qualities alluded to by pre-twentieth-century pedagogues

were heard and experienced through the creative practice of improvisation: immediate,

nuanced musical expressions that were seldom, if ever, depicted through ex post facto

notation. As early as 1602, Giulio Caccini documented how the performer’s willful,

musical gestures altered even the simplest of notated rhythmical passages for the sake of

variation—practices extending well beyond Descartes’ fundamental descriptions of

pulse-sense.

Figure 1.12. Caccini’s 1602 instructions on gestural, rhythmical embellishment. Transcribed and translated

in Strunk’s Source readings in music history, ed. Leo Treitler (New York: Norton, 1998), 614.

69 Quoted in Christiani, 265. It seems that musical monotony was so detested in classical

compositions and performance practices, that references such as H. W. Pilkington, A Musical

Dictionary (1812), 74, offer the term “Tautology: A tiresome repetition of the same passages.”

Page 74: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

53

Caccini’s rhythmical “falls” and “breaths” signify physical gestures, not mathematical

computations. And while the invisible pulse-accent was still implied, Caccini showed

how musicians could perform the rhythms between the musical pulse-sense in variable,

improvisatory ways; in practice, the sense of fall, the duration of breath—neither of

which imply mechanical precision—related rhythmical gestures beyond the fundamental

sense of proportionality.

While such improvisatory rhythmical embellishments had precedent in Italian

vocal and instrumental style, and can be heard in the diminution music of Bassano,

Rognoni, Virgiliano, dalla Casa, de Selma, and others, it is significant to note that the

treatment of rapid rhythmic passages as a series of non-calculable—and non-

metronomic—gestures extended well through the nineteenth century. Compare, for

instance, Caccini’s instructions (Fig. 1.12) to Christiani’s striking similar examples that

expose improvised, gestural “falls” and “breaths” in the “new music” of his century:70

Figure 1.13. Christiani’s depiction of a falling gesture as it contrasts from the notated indication

on the left. The rhythm as Christiani would have it performed on the right is decidedly non-

mathematical and non-metronomic. From Christiani, 157.

70 Also see Christiani, 174-175, for additional performance instructions that expose greater

inconsistencies between notation and the performance of nuanced, gestural rhythms.

Page 75: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

54

As Christiani explained centuries after Caccini, musical time as seen was not musical

time as heard. Even while these two treatises document very different compositional

genres, they share similar understandings of musical time, relating common rhythmical

qualities of accent, emphasis, breadth, and poise only actualized in willful performance.

They exposed that living gestural affects, whether in an early Italian monody or a

nineteenth century sonata, prevailed over the redundant look of notation.

The Linguistic Movement of Musical Time

Another long-lasting tradition, uncovering a more nuanced pre-metronomic

understanding of musical time, appears in the speech-epistemology of rhythmical

movement. As late as 1879, the Grove Dictionary confirmed the fundamental, self-

referential connections between musical and linguistic time:

As in spoken language certain words and syllables receive more emphasis than

others, so in music there are always some notes which are to be rendered

comparatively prominent; and this prominence is termed ‘accent’. In order that

music may produce a satisfactory effect upon the mind, it is necessary that this

accent (as in poetry) should for the most part recur at regular intervals. Again, as

in poetry we find different varieties of metre, so in music we meet with various

kinds of time.71

A century prior, the Encyclopædia Britannica likewise stated:

Times and bars in music answer the very same end as punctuation in language.

They determine the different periods of the movement, or the various degree of

completion, which the sentiment, expressed by that movement, has attained…the

first of these gradations [of sentiment] may be called a time which is likewise the

most convenient division of a bar or measure into its elementary or aliquot parts,

and may be deemed equivalent to a comma in a sentence; a bar denotes a degree

still more sensible, and may be considered as having the force of a semicolon; a

71 Grove, I (1879), 12.

Page 76: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

55

strain brings the sentiment to a tolerable degree of perfection, and may be

reckoned equal to a colon: the full period is the end of the imitative piece.72

As many late-sixteenth-century theorists of the Italian seconda pratica and French

musique mesurée traditions related, poetry and poetic recitation held deep connections to

musical performance since ancient Greece. From at least the eighteenth through the early

twentieth centuries, some pedagogical texts incorporated punctuation marks and poetical

symbols into typical musical notation—further highlighting the gestural nuances of light

and weight, arsis and thesis, rhetorical pause, stress, and movement that standardized

musical notation failed to convey.

A few notable examples suffice to confirm the linguistic-musical conceptions of

“time” described in the eighteenth-century Encyclopædia Britannica. In 1737, Johann

Mattheson annotated a typical French minuet with poetical figures, attesting to the

nuanced rhythm that transcended mechanical precisions and proportional exactitudes. He

applied asterisks underneath the half-notes to document an additional pulse-weight—a

“pathetic accent”—required for the graceful mouvement of the melody.

72 “Music,” Encyclopædia Britannica, 43-44, n4.

Page 77: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

56

Figure 1.14. Mattheson’s poetical notation for expressing the many nuances of musical movement.

Originally from Kern melodischer Wissenschafft (Hamburg: C. Herold, 1735) and reprinted in Lussy,

Traité de l'expression musicale, Deuxième Édition (Paris: Heugel et Cie., 1874), 26.

Mattheson again exposed additional elements of time, poise, breath, and emphasis that

were usually inaccessible through music notation. As a host of seventeenth- and

eighteenth-century musicians including Quantz, Rameau, and Rousseau, and others

skilled in the French style reiterated: rhetorical and gestural rhythmical qualities could

never fully be depicted in musical notation. This fact prompted François Couperin to

assert for the French-style compositions of his age:

In my opinion, there are defects in our method of writing music…we write

differently from the way we play, which is the reason why foreigners [Italians] play

our music less well then we play theirs.73

Evidence suggests, however, that seventeenth- and eighteenth-century French

publications were not the only repertoire performed differently than viewed.74

In

73 François Couperin, L'art de toucher le clavecin (1717), translated by Margery Halford (Van

Nuys, CA: Alfred Pub. Co., 1995), 49.

74

See Daniel Gottlob Türk’s Klavierschule, oder, Anweisung zum Klavierspielen für Lehrer und

Lernende: mit kritischen Anmerkungen (Reprint, Leipsig und Halle: Auf Kosten des Verfassers,

in Kommission bey Schwickert, 1802), in which Türk interprets the various ways to perform

ornaments and graces in late-eighteenth-century keyboard music by C. P. E. Bach, Mozart,

Page 78: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

57

following decades and in other lands, theorists applied linguistic notation to typical music

passages to express the often-hidden nuances of musical time. For example, Joshua Steele

in the late eighteenth century and Christiani in the late nineteenth century similarly

related the general qualities of metrical movement to strong-weak speech accents.

Figure 1.15. Christiani’s correlation between the movements of speech and the fundamental

movements of measured music. From Christiani, 38-39.

Christiani and the Grove Dictionary applied poetical indications to Mozart’s and

Beethoven’s thematic structures, further suggesting the long-standing traditions

connecting musical time, accent, and phrase to poetic meters and spoken inflections.

Clementi, and others.

Page 79: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

58

Figure 1.16. Examples of poetical-musical gestures interpreted within the phrase structures of Beethoven

and Mozart. Above, from Christiani, 207. Below, from the entry “Rhythm,” Grove, III (1880), 318.

Again, these poetical-musical notations—which were employed in education through the

late nineteenth-century and were occasionally printed in periodicals such as Etude

magazine—hint at rhythmical qualities born of willful performance practices, qualities

that transcended mathematical absolutes or mechanical references.75

As exposed through

these annotated poetical-musical examples, few if any commonalities were shared

75 As late as 1919 Paul Verrier considered spoken and musical time intricately intertwined. See

Paul Verrier, Essai sur les principles de la metrique anglaise, Three Volumes (Paris: H. Welter,

1909-1919).

Page 80: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

59

between the self-referential, linguistic movement of musical time and objective ticks of

clockwork rhythm.

The Active Rhetoric of Musical Time, or Tempo Rubato

Beyond the nuanced movement of metered music, further complexities often appeared in

the subjective treatment of musical time. Many composer, theorists, and performers

likened this enhanced musical-temporal expressivity to the art and act of rhetoric. As

documented at least as early as Joachim Burmeister’s Hypomnematum musicae poeticae

(1599),76

rhetorical effects further extended the possibilities of musical time, providing

volatile and expressive techniques that enhanced text. (It could be argued that rhetoric has

directly informed the structures, rhythms, and gestures of music compositions—and

likely performance practices—at least since the sixteenth-century Italian madrigalists

such as Verdelot and Arcadelt.) As Diderot’s Encyclopédie recounts, rhetoric contained a

subjective species of mouvement all its own, a movement allied with the passions.77

Thus

music treatises spanning three centuries occasionally explain and expose an even more

willful species of temporality in which rhetorical movement was a guiding principle

behind musical tempos, pulses, and rhythms. Piano pedagogue Czerny, for example,

found musical time in the piano compositions of his age so inaccessible through standard

76 While Burmeister’s musical-rhetoric concerned compositional techniques, often as they related

to text, it seems highly plausible that—in an age of self-referential musical time—they extended

to performance practices as well.

77 See Encyclopédie, Vol. 10: 842: “Mouvement, ou Émotion, en Rhétorique. Voyez Passion.”

Page 81: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

60

notation that he analyzed a living interpretation, bar by bar, in terms of rhetorical

movements and effects.

Figure 1.17. A passage from Czerny’s bar-by-bar performance analysis of rhetorical-musical time.

Reprinted in Christiani, 268.

Despite the continual changes in musical aesthetics and compositional practices

throughout history, and regardless of genre, era, or national style, an ontological fact

remained: skilled performers actively and definitively shaped musical time, pulse, and

movement prior to the twentieth century. One striking correlation of this assessment

appears as early as 1602, in Caccini’s Le nuove musiche, a treatise that documented

performance aesthetics nearly identical to Christiani’s Principles of Expression.

Page 82: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

61

Figure 1.18. A passage from Caccini’s bar-by-bar performance analysis of rhetorical-musical time from

1602. Transcribed and translated in Strunk’s Source readings in music history, ed. Leo Treitler (New York:

Norton, 1998), 615.

The anonymous treatise the Choragus (c1630) verified the fluidly of musical movement

in early-Italian monody when tempered by the performer’s willful rhetorical-musical

sense: “since the [singing] actor must stop to sigh at length as nature tells him and hold

on to the same note more or less according to the affect, he should not be tied to any one

Page 83: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

62

else’s measure [beat], but should freely follow the impulse of feeling, which is of great

importance in reciting [music] well.”78

It is also important to note that eighteenth- and nineteenth-century Italianate

recitative, born out of traditions of early monody, retained a rhythmical freedom unlike

that of metered music, grounded on a recurring and regular pulse-sense. This greater

rhetorical freedom offered by non-measured recitative was not lost on nineteenth-century

theorists such as Moritz Hauptmann, who explained the significant distinctions between

metered music and recitative as late as 1865. Writing to conductor Carl Kossmaly,

Hauptmann related that recitative should not be confined by strict tempo considerations,

referenced through time words, the recurring pulse-sense, or metronomic rules.

Hearkening back to traditions and practices of Caccini and the Choragus, Hauptmann

recounted, “Recitative has no metre, only rhythm and accent—a valuable and naturally

artistic quality, which ought not to be sacrificed to the tempo used to mark the beginning

of the actual piece, when it starts upon the course measured out for it. Here the old state

of things is certainly more poetical, and the new is very prosaic, as compared with it.

Recitative has many forms which ought to be retained: they were founded on vocal

expression by word of mouth, and grew up naturally of their own accord, with

Recitation.”79

More than any other musical styles, recitative and unmeasured recitative-

like passages in other genres projected a subjective “time” that was decidedly non-

mechanical in concept, sentiment, and practice.

78 Reprinted, translated, and edited in Strunk’s Source readings in music history, ed. Leo Treitler

(New York: Norton, 1998), 631.

79

Moritz Hauptmann, The Letters of a Leipzig Cantor, edited by Alfred Schöne and Ferdinand

Hiller, translated by A.D. Coleridge, Volume II (London: Novello, Ewer and Co., and Richard

Bentley and Son, 1892), 252.

Page 84: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

63

In defining the humanity of musical time through these sources, some

performance-practice questions linger: Did Caccini, the Choragus, Cernzy, Christiani,

and Hauptmann describe and endorse practices exclusive to their overly sentimental ages

and repertoires? Were they alone in condoning rhythmical flights of fancy—otherwise

known by that pejorative: the dreaded tempo rubato? Again, the problem can be partially

answered through the term tempo rubato itself, which has altered meanings alongside

“time,” “tempo,” and “rhythm” over the ages; Just as “time” is a relative indication based

on performance contexts and references, so too is the “stealing” of it. Eighteenth- and

nineteenth-century sources suggest there was no single rhythmical quality, no absolute

technique explaining what “rubato” actually signified. When considering Caccini,

Quantz, Christiani, and others, it seems that rubato—as “musical-temporal fluctuation or

variation”—was a common and desirable trait for a host of repertoires over the centuries.

Indeed, Paderewski was one of the last virtuoso performer-composers to maintain that

what many twentieth-century pedagogues and instrumentalists now derided as tempo

rubato, “is older than the Romantic school, it is older than Mozart, it is older than Bach.

Girolamo Frescobaldi, in the beginning of the seventeenth century, made ample use of

it.”80

Czerny’s contemporary Marx thought tempo rubato originated in French and

Italian vocal performances—both sacred and secular—from the mid-eighteenth century!

Yet he strongly believed rhetorical-rhythmical freedom and the historical tempo rubato

were two different considerations. In defining the “old” pejorative rubato, Marx cites the

80 See Louis C. Elson, Mistakes and Disputed Points in Music and Music Teaching (Philadelphia:

Theodore Presser Co., 1910), 96-97. Reprinted from Henry T. Fink, Success in Music and How

It’s Won (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, 1909).

Page 85: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

64

nineteenth-century musician who most exemplified the willful, rhetorical-temporal sense

cherished in his age:

The tempo rubato was a fashion of the eighteenth century, dating from the last

half, and came from the singers of Italy and France. It was intended to replace the

free, deep feeling which was wanting in the compositions themselves; this tempo

rubato was therefore an untruth, and was soon compelled to yield to the reaction

of reason. With this fashion (which amid other things became visible in

Pergolesi’s Stabat Mater) Beethoven had nothing in common; he followed

entirely the inner impetus—the demand of the thing—when he resorted to free

movement.81

As Marx’s discourse on “rubato” relates, one musician’s “theft” of time is another

musician’s “freedom” from time. By the end of the nineteenth century, pedagogue Franz

Kullak recounted Schindler’s definition of “free interpretation,” once heard in

Beethoven’s performances, as a necessary musical-temporal act of rhetoric:

Although a poet writes his monologue or dialogue in a regular, progressive

rhythm, the reciter must, none the less, observe certain divisions and pauses in

order to bring out the sense, even where the poet could not indicate them by

punctuation; and this style of declamation is equally applicable to music, and is

modified only by the number of participants in the execution of the given work.82

Schindler, similar to Marx, recognized that nineteenth-century “free interpretation” was

not the eighteenth-century tempo rubato once heard in opera buffa. By the end of the

nineteenth century, Franz Kullak, in his treatise Beethoven’s Piano-playing, admitted of

this earlier performance practice, “We do not know what the buffo singer of the period

81 Adolf Bernhard Marx, Introduction to the Interpretation of Beethoven Piano Works, translated

by Fannie Louise Gwinner (Chicago: Clayton F. Summy Co., 1895), 73n.

82

Quoted in Franz Kullak, Beethoven’s Piano-Playing, translated by Theodore Baker (New York:

G. Schirmer, 1901), 24. Kullak quotes from Life of Beethoven (third ed., 213) that by 1858,

Schindler had asserted, “at the present time the true conception of free interpretation is utterly

lost.”

Page 86: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

65

understood by ‘tempo rubato.’”83

Nevertheless, Kullak suggested that the “stolen” time

described by Türk and C. P. E. Bach might hold an answer to the eighteenth-century

interpretive practices. Kullak recounted that Türk’s tempo rubato comprised varieties of

voice separation—additionally nuanced, temporal qualities usually absent from

rhythmical notation.

Figure 1.19. Two performance interpretations of eighteenth-century tempo rubato. The original notation

appears on the left. From Türk, Klavierschule, Chapter, IV, Sect. 5, § 72. Reprinted in Franz Kullak,

Beethoven’s Piano-Playing, translated by Theodore Baker (New York: G. Schirmer, 1901), 25n.

Christiani cited three species of rubato: the first consisted of “any temporary retardation

or acceleration;” the second was the displacement of the invisible, weighted accent “by

which the time becomes robbed of its regular accents…(for example, syncopation).” He

defined the last rubato species as:

That capricious and disorderly mode of performance by which some notes are

protracted beyond their proper duration and others curtailed, without, however,

changing the aggregate duration of each measure, is a rubato. 84

Christiani’s third definition of rubato has two different species within itself, and one of

them is likened to voice separation: “It may be executed in two ways,” Christiani notes,

“1. Both hands in sympathy with each other, i. e., both hands accelerating or retarding

together. 2. Or, the two hands not in sympathy, i. e., the accompanying hand keeping

83 Kullak, 25n.

84

Christiani, 299.

Page 87: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

66

strict time, while the other hand alone is playing rubato. The latter way is the more

beautiful of the two, and is the truly artistic rubato.”85

From these texts, it seems that the free interpretation of musical time—beyond the

sensory foundations of pulse, accent, and meter—was common practice in the music of

many traditions. Any rhetorical movement or performance technique diverging from the

regular pulse-sense could be considered “in rubato.” Indeed, references such as

Pilkington’s Musical Dictionary (1812), described tempo rubato as intrinsic to all

rhetorically affected interpretations:

TEMPO RUBATO. An expression applied to a time alternately accelerated and

retarded for the purpose of enforcing the expression.86

It can be assumed from this source that rubato was no less desirable or unwarranted an

interpretive technique than playing strictly and unwaveringly to the musical pulse: this

opposing temporal quality was tempo giusto, and according to Pilkington’s Dictionary, it

elicited a different temporal effect for specific musical passages or sections:

TEMPO GIUSTO. In equal and just time. An expression generally applied to the

manner of performing a steady, sound movement, less directed to the feelings

than to the judgment; more scientific than impassioned.87

According to this early-nineteenth-century source, giusto—the strict adherence to the

metric pulse—was a no more necessary or implied performance practice than rubato.

Each was used according to the musical circumstance, the composer’s occasionally

explicit indications, and ultimately the performer’s self-referential and rhetorical sense of

85 Ibid.

86 Pilkington, 75.

87

Ibid.

Page 88: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

67

musical time. As Christiani recounted, even the strictness of a tempo-giuso performance

was an individual matter: ““Strict time-keeping involves naturally strict adherence to a

chosen tempo; though it does not imply that the tempo is the correct one. Thus, several

musicians may play the same piece in perfect time, yet, each one in a different tempo,

and neither in the composer's intended one.”88

Furthermore, as Pilkington’s Dictionary

suggested, Christiani considered rubato interpretations necessary for subjective,

rhetorically expressive performances:

Tact-freedom implies the faculty of hastening or slackening the tempo, knowingly

and intentionally, at such temporary moments as the character of the movement

not only suggests and justifies, but even requires. To which must be added the

ableness and certainty of returning to the normal tempo, whenever needed. This

shows expertness and masterly control.89

Christiani’s ultimate assessment was that artistry demanded a coexistence of rubato with

a strong sense of the tactus; A fine musician must understand and actualize both, thus

transcending the musical amateurism or vapid mathematical precision heard only with

beat-strictness: “Tact-freedom comprises all those transgressions and infringements of

strict time, including tempo, which are deliberately resorted to for the purpose of

imparting artistic variety to the stiffness of unchanging measured motion.”90

Caccini, Saint-Lambert, Christiani, Marx, H. W. Pilkington, and many others

consistently described musical time in these terms: as a living process, not a mechanical

precision. Thus, they implied that no document or machine could impart rhythmical

qualities to skilled musicians. In either a rubato or giusto performance, no metronome

88 Christiani, 260.

89

Ibid., 261.

90

Ibid., 263. See also page 300 for Christiani’s warning that neither technique should be

overdone.

Page 89: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

68

could define living “expressions,” “feelings,” or “scientific judgments”—all of these

temporal assessments were still under the purview of the skilled timeist.91

As these

sources show, before the mechanical age of the twentieth century, there indeed existed a

truth to musical time, but it was based upon subjective realities, not abstract theorems or

absolute measurements. For Marx, “feeling may have an individual right, ‘its own

rhythmic progress,’ [which] is not scientific.”92

Not even a composer’s intentions

regarding time and tempo were absolute, as Czerny proved by offering the musical

novice some temporal variations for his very own “Andante” composition:

Figure 1.20. Czerny’s suggestions for expressive interpretation, which assumes standard performance

practices in rhetorical time. He exposes how musical time involves willful processes, not mechanical

precisions. Note that not one indication is labeled either “tempo rubato” or “tempo giusto.” Reprinted in

Christiani, 265.

91 Beethoven stated as much, as Marx relates: “In the autograph copy of the song ‘Nord oder

Süd’… there is distinctly written in Beethoven’s hand, ‘100 according to Mälzel; but this can

refer to the first measures only, for sentiment has also its peculiar rhythm; but this cannot be

entirely expressed in this grade.’” See Marx, Introduction to the Interpretation of Beethoven

Piano Works, 74.

92

Marx, Introduction to the Interpretation of Beethoven Piano Works, 74.

Page 90: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

69

As the sources presented here confirm, the complete answers to musical temporality prior

to the twentieth century were found not in sight, but in sound. Rameau instructed on the

various speeds, meters, phrase structures, and beat-patterns of French airs, admitting:

“Every character and every passion has its own special movement, but this is more

dependent on taste than on rules.”93

Rameau, the renowned theorist of functional

harmony, maintained that no objective theory adequately defined the subjective qualities

of musical time. In matters of meter and time, musical taste trumped musical theory.

Christiani reiterated the longstanding aesthetic, that “As a rule, any group or figure, when

repeated, should be rendered in a different way, to avoid sameness.” He then described

how performers continually averted dreaded rhythmical stagnation through seemingly

anti-scientific practices: “Such alteration may be an augmentation or diminution of

emphasis, a lengthening or shortening of the extremes, etc… But the alteration may be

made in many other ways, as the occasion demands, and without following any

stereotyped rule.”94

Echoing composers of past centuries, Carl Maria von Weber understood that the

skilled performer held interpretive authority over the time, tempo, and rhetoric of even

the composer’s own works. Thus he too confirmed the necessary, underlying subjectivity

of musical time:

93 See Rameau, Traité de l'harmonie, livre second, 161: “Chaque caractere & chaque passion ont

leur mouvement particulier; mais cela dépend puls du goût que des Regles.”

94

Christiani, 159-160.

Page 91: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

70

Every singer imparts, though unconsciously, the coloring of his own individual

character to the dramatic character which he sustains. Thus two singers…will give

the same composition in a manner widely different…and yet both may do justice

to the composer, inasmuch as both mark the gradations of passions in his

composition, faithfully and expressively, according to the nature and degree of

power possessed by each.95

Even a group of instrumentalists gauged their musical time through these highly nuanced

rhetorical activities, which, according to C. M. v. Weber, no notation could possibly

document:

The movement ought not to be a tyrannical check—a driving mill-hammer, but

must be to the composition, what pulsations is in the animal economy. There is no

slow movement in which passages demanding acceleration do not occur. On the

other hand, there is no quick movement but what requires, in many passages,

moderate retardation. These changes, in particular cases, are absolutely necessary

to expression.96

Others in the nineteenth century recognized that the best ensembles had pronounced

rhetorical-rhythmical abilities. Take for example, theorist Moritz Hauptmann’s letter to

his mentor Ludwig Spohr on December 1, 1842:

We had a wonderfully good performance of your Weihe der Töne in the

Gewandhaus the other day. It is a favourite Symphony with our audiences here,

and you yourself would have been delighted…the ensemble is very satisfactory,

and the rhythmical nuances are as animated as those of good Quartet players.97

Even Hauptmann, who theorized upon the perfect architecture of musical phrases—

grounded, of course, on the invisible accent—recognized that musical notation did not

truly reflect musical time. What Hauptmann related to Sphor was a rhythm transcending

95 C. M. Von Weber, “Letter By C. M. Von Weber on the Performance of Dramatic Song,” The

Harmonicon V, part 1 (1827): 220.

96

Ibid.

97

Hauptmann, 193.

Page 92: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

71

both composer intention and Hauptmann’s own speculative rhythmic schemas. Even for

Hauptmann, living gestures presided over perfect symmetries.98

Figure 1.21. One example of Hauptmann’s rhythmical schemas that depict the usually invisible nuances of

musical accentuation for any given meter and phrase structure. From Moritz Hauptmann, The nature of

harmony and metre, translated and edited by W. E. Heathcote (London: Swan Sonnenschein & Co., 1888),

238.

98 Grove’s Dictionary also claimed that when this ensemble played in “strict” tempo, they were

not mechanically inflexible. Rhythmical nuances pervaded many of the Gewandhaus’ best

performances with Mendelssohn, who “held ‘Tempo rubato’ in abhorrence; yet he indicated

nuances of emphasis and expression—as opposed to the inevitable Accents described in the

forgoing article—with a precision which no educated musician ever failed to understand; and this

with an effect so marked, that, when even Ferdinand David—a Conductor of no ordinary

ability—took up the baton after him at the Gewandhaus, as he frequently did, the soul of the

Orchestra seemed to have departed…[Mendelssohn] knew how to beat strict Time with

expression.” See Grove, IV (1889), 125.

Page 93: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

72

Prior to the vast incorporation of the clockwork metronome into modern pedagogies,

musical time was fundamentally formed from the variable, self-referential senses,

perceptions, and actions of experienced performers. Indeed, Shakespeare, in As you Like

It, again expressed pre-twentieth century performance aesthetics, extending from Caccini

to Czerny to Paderewski—in which variable human movement was not considered

temporal thievery—when Rosalind proclaimed far from the town clock:

Time travels in divers paces with divers persons. I'll tell you who Time ambles

withal, who Time trots withal, who Time gallops withal, and who he stands still

withal.

Acknowledging the Limits of Musical Notation in Depicting

the Variable Qualities of Musical Time

The printed page seldom conveyed even the basic sensory qualities of time, affect,

movement, and gesture, and thus many acknowledged that rhythmical ambiguities would

always exist when performers read standard musical notation; these ambiguities were

especially problematic for the musical novice. Christiani confirmed: “Theorists say that

the irregular accents (emotional, aesthetical, rhetorical, or whatever name may be given

to them) have their root in the mood of the performer, and that can neither be anticipated

nor dictated. Moreover, lacking the appropriate musical indications for the emotions, a

composer cannot indicate them, hence the performer ought to divine them and accent

accordingly, although the accents are not indicated…This may be sufficient for the artist

or the master, but is quite inadequate for the majority of students and for teaching

purposes.”99

Over 150 years after Couperin assessed the problems between the page and

99 Christiani, 23.

Page 94: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

73

performance of French music, Christiani likewise concluded there was an “old looseness

in rhythmic notation”100

following classical keyboard traditions:

With all due respect to the great pianoforte composers, from Mozart to Chopin, it

is quite plain, that, in point of rhythmic notation, the old masters were often very

inexact and careless…Rhythmic interpretation, of Chopin's passages, for example,

is often a sore puzzle, and rarely quite clear to the average player. And how could

they be otherwise, seing [sic.] that frequently a whole passage is suspended, so to

say, on the hook of a single note: 101

We cannot blame Chopin for writing his passages and cadenzas in a loose,

indefinite manner, inasmuch as he followed the custom of his time, according to

which his notation was quite natural and correct. Hummel, his predecessor, was

much more inexact.102

Christiani also recognized that the invisible musical accent transcended composer

intention or the vagaries of musical notation. In Principles of Expression in Pianoforte

Playing, he cites musical examples by Chopin and Schumann in which the sensory accent

of meter was obscured through odd time signatures or phrases “confusingly” placed in

relation to the metric pulse. Christiani nevertheless believed that the skilled performer’s

innate sense for musical time would prevail over the visual ambiguities of text, stating:

A composer may succeed in deceiving the reader's eye, but he cannot deceive the

listener's ear. Accents on a melody so plain as in this prelude will naturally assert

themselves, and fall together with the strong part of the time, in spite of

antagonistic notation.103

100 Ibid., 53.

101

Ibid., 52.

102

Ibid., 55.

103

Ibid., 91.

Page 95: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

74

Figure 1.22. Above, Christiani’s example of “confused” music notation that obscures the performer’s

placement of sensory, musical accents. Below, Christiani publishes his own “natural notation” that allied

more closely to the rhythmic feeling of a triple meter, seemingly implied in Chopin’s melodic gestures.

Note that Christiani adds no metronome marks in the revision, only the emotional word “agitato.” See

Christiani, 89.

Marx—the theorist who codified Sonata form—also attested that the individual’s

rhythmical sense must transcend “a servile adhesion to the written directions.”104

Thus,

he strongly championed skilled performing “artists” of his age, because—similar to

compelling actors—they could creatively transcend the failings inherent in any printed

text, sonata publications included. In The Music of the Nineteenth Century and its

Culture, Marx asserted:

104 Marx, The Music of the Nineteenth Century and its Culture, 267.

Page 96: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

75

We have given and proved how…all writing, is necessarily defective and

insufficient. One who does not rely on his own feeling and perception, will not

gain the slightest security of correctness by limiting himself to written indications,

for he will at the same time be limiting himself to all their incompleteness and

poverty. Who would counsel an actor to recite his part in a lifeless way, just

because he might by instance take a false conception of it, or make an incorrect

accentuation?105

He concluded that machines such as “dynamometers” and metronomes must not

influence musical interpretations, rather, that “the feeling and perception of the executant

alone can supply all that in which written signs are defective.”106

Other notable theorists

throughout the nineteenth century recognized that the purely positivistic readings of

musical notation could not illuminate the nuanced qualities of rhythm; this was especially

true for older musical styles. Echoing Couperin’s realizations from the early eighteenth

century, the music researcher, editor, and composer Camille Saint-Saëns discussed the

peculiarities of eighteenth-century performance practices—primarily citing the French-

style vocal music of Handel and Rameau—in which sight and sound often failed to align.

Saint-Saëns keenly perceived a significant shift in rhythmical conception during his age:

In these days music is written almost exactly as it should be performed…In

former times, the ‘arithmetical’ value of the notes was not taken into account as it

is nowadays…there are innumerable signs the interpretation of which is

occasionally impossible, all contemporary methods indicating that they cannot be

described, and that to perform them one must have heard them sung by a

professor…a close study of these works has convinced me that the values of the

vocal parts are approximate, and that we must take into consideration,

declamation, not notation, if we are to interpret the melody part, and not merely

the recitative, in accordance with the real intention of the composer.107

105 Ibid.

106

Ibid., 257.

107

Camille Saint-Saëns, Outspoken Essays on Music, trans. Fred Rothwell (London: Kegan Paul,

Trench, Trubner & Co., 1922), 92, 94-95. It must be stated that “almost exactly” is yet another

culturally relative assessment.

Page 97: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

76

Saint-Saëns asserted that over the course of the late nineteenth century the actualization

of rhythm, gesture, and ornament had palpably changed through greater adherence to

more mathematical and precise rhythmical notations. These “contemporary methods” of

publication deemphasized the non-notated, rhetorical (“declamatory”) qualities once

provided by subjective interpretations alone.

The insufficiency and ambiguity that Marx recognized in music notation was

being reined-in by the last decades of the nineteenth century, made more definitive and

precise through revised rhythmical notation and corresponding educational techniques.

Waning from performance practices was the realization that rhetorical-musical time was

invisible, variable, and individual. Christiani confirmed Saint-Saëns’s assertion: rhythmic

notations were read in a more mathematical, positivistic light in their day, especially for

instrumental compositions, which now offered editorialized dynamics, articulations,

phrasings—with revised rhythms as well. As Christiani documents by the last decades of

the nineteenth-century, music publications—in part catering to the wave of amateur

consumer-musicians—left very little to individual, interpretive license:

Modern composers (whatever the contents of their compositions may be)

endeavor at least to make their rhythmic notation plain and accurate. Many of the

old abbreviations — for instance, the numerous signs for ornamentation — are

now generally written in full; and passages, cadenzas, arabesques, are no longer

left to the individual fancy of the interpreter, but are represented clearly and

unmistakably.108

Oddly perhaps, while Christiani championed skilled “artistic” performers for their

nuanced pulse-sense and rhetorical movement, he oppositely supported revised editions

for sake of the “average player,” in which rhythm became less interpretive and more of

108 Christiani, 53.

Page 98: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

77

what Saint-Saëns recognized as “arithmetical.” The interpretive freedom Marx so

cherished often seemed a nuisance to Christiani, who recounted: “We have to be very

thankful, that the old laxity of rhythmic notation, notably of grouping together a long row

of notes without any distinct division, is more and more disappearing.”109

Primarily for

the sake of modern students’ education, Christiani preferred revised publications for

“their modern accuracy,” a contrast from the “old laxity”110

of rhythmical notation

upholding the “old ad libitum style of interpretation.”111

With the trend towards precision-oriented editorial standards—seen through more

meticulously subdivided rhythmical notations—came the loss of the interpreter’s

“individual fancy” in musical time. Subjective “ad libitum” choice gave way to a greater

mathematical and eventually metronomic definitiveness. Christiani provides many

examples of modern rhythmic revisions, perhaps in an effort to stimulate music sales, in

which the modern editor, not the skilled timeist, becomes the primary temporal agent,

choosing the many now-specified nuances of rhythmical movement.

109 Ibid., 52.

110

Ibid.

111

Ibid., 53.

Page 99: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

78

Figure 1.23. One of the numerous examples Christiani provides of improved notational “accuracy” in

modern editions, in which rhetorical movements and rhythmical nuances are pre-interpreted by the editor,

in this case Hans von Bülow. Articulations, trills, phrasings, and proportional tempo equivalence—but not

metronome marks—are added to the once-improvisatory rhythmical passage, which is nearly

unrecognizable in the revision below. Christiani argues that these interpretations were necessary for the

“average player,” who was perhaps lacking in advanced rhythmical and rhetorical senses. Christiani, 59.

This modern trend toward the precise, rhythmical “pre-interpretation” of historical music

became so prevalent, and so detrimental to once-subjective rhetorical performances, that

Edgar F. Jacques considered modern editions such as these—alongside a newfound,

anachronistic adherence to metronome indications—were in no small part responsible for

the reduction of both singers’ and instrumentalists’ individual expression, invention, and

creativity in the concert hall by the last decade of the century. Speaking to the Royal

Musical Association on December 8, 1891, Jacques stated:

I need only ask you to remember how the progress [of music publication] has all

along been in the direction of definiteness…how, finally, marks of expression

were added and words used to indicate speed, these being supplemented later by

metronome-marks. We have, indeed, so clipped the wings of the executant that

we no longer expect extemporized cadenzas, either in an opera or in a concerto.112

112 See Edgar F. Jacques, “The Composer’s Intention,” in Proceedings of the Musical Association,

18th Sess. (1891 - 1892): 42.

Page 100: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

79

While Jacques lamented the loss of rhetorical musicality through increasingly precise,

positivistic treatments of rhythm, Christiani justified the necessity of these rhythmical

revisions due to the swell of mediocre performers, who perhaps had no comprehensive

training in the nuanced perceptions of musical rhetoric. For Christiani it was “better to

have all doubtful cases interpreted by a master rather than to leave them to the mercy of

every petty musician or amateur.”113

Christiani’s primary concern was in teaching amateurs, not artists, the basics of

expression in musical time. Most significantly, the influential pedagogue recognized that

the truly skilled musician would not take any editor’s prescriptions for rhythmical

interpretation literally or conclusively. As Christiani maintained, a skilled interpreter had

final authority over all notation and editorial revisions of musical time, since rhetorical

rhythmical choices remained in the experienced performer’s hands. Justifying his

proclivity for modernized editions, Christiani argues:

Let rigorous musicians grumble at such innovations, on the ground that those who

attempt to play Chopin should be sufficiently advanced to interpret his rhythm,

each one according to his own conception, thus leaving room for many different

ways, and not keeping to one particular idea or precept.

It may, however, be replied, that a player who is so far advanced in

maturity and intelligence, as to be able independently and correctly to interpret

Chopin, is himself the best judge whether or not he will accept Klindworth's, or

any other editor's, rhythmical interpretation.114

As Christiani explained, it was the musically ignorant masses that required rhythmical-

notational assistance, continuing, “those who are not so far advanced — and their number

113

Christiani, 61.

114 Ibid., 55.

Page 101: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

80

is by far the greater — will unquestionably derive a great benefit by having pointed out to

them positively what would otherwise have been doubtful.”115

For skilled musicians,

however, subjectivity in time still reigned beyond the modern educator’s penchant to

quantify rhythms with increasingly mathematical and metronomic precisions.116

For the

average mass of musicians who could not aspire to artistry, Christiani believed

rhythmical certitude was necessary beyond performer choice; an editor’s modernized,

rhythmical interpretation sufficed for amateur-consumers’ purposes. Christiani confirmed

the performance-practice distinctions between musical artists of classical traditions and

the modern, musical dilettantes of his day:

The only advantage which is to be recognized in free rhythmical interpretation

over positive reading, based upon precise [notated] rhythmical division, is that the

former (at the hands of a master) may be accomplished in many different ways;

while the latter is limited to the indicated way alone.117

In the first decades of the twentieth century, a waning collection of pedagogues still

confirmed the predominance of subjective, nuanced delivery—in which performers’

rhetorical-rhythmical sense shaped musical time beyond insufficient written indications,

bypassing those interpretative “limitations” seen in the published “way alone.” In

America, Oberlin Conservatory Professor Edward Dickinson continued to dispel the

fallacy that composers’ wishes ever fully resided in rhythmical notation. As late as 1911,

he too recognized, in an age when the clockwork metronome became a standard temporal

reference:

115 Ibid.

116

Somewhat contradictory, Christiani stated that, “Subjectiveness [emotion] and objectiveness

[intellect] — as the Germans are fond of saying — play a great role in the choice of tempo, and,

frequently, even a greater one than the composer's positive indication.” Christiani, 262.

117

Ibid., 61.

Page 102: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

81

That the composer should say to the player that here and here, and thus and thus

shall he make these expressive alterations of speed, is impossible. Rarely does he

attempt to do so. Here and there he will write ritardando or accelerando, but

precisely how much slower or faster, or exactly at what instant these changes

begin, cannot be indicated. In the wide spaces of the piece, however, no directions

are given. The composer implicitly says to the player: In the matter of tempo I put

myself in your hands, your musicianship is the arbiter; if my music sounds dull

and monotonous you must take a part or the whole of the blame, if otherwise a

goodly share of the honor shall be yours.118

The Failure of Metronomes to Relate the Humanity of Musical Time

The two-dimensional schema, the written page, or the ex post facto metronomic

indication did not encapsulate the “true” meaning of musical time well through the

nineteenth century, for—as only experience could prove—lifeless things did not regulate

the many nuances of rhythmical expression for skilled musicians. As over three centuries

of knowledgeable performers, composers, and theorists relate, metronomes—those time-

dictating machines, simple or complex—consistently failed to offer sufficient,

meaningful answers to the nuances of rhythm, tempo, accent, or meter. By historical

accounts—by historical temporal values—mechanical motion, seen either in the swing of

a pendulum or heard in the tick of clockwork, was not translatable or transferable to the

living experience of musical time. Metronomes projected a temporal epistemology in

conflict with traditional, self-referential understandings of musical time as sensed,

conceived, and practiced for centuries. Even on the most fundamental level, adhering to

external metronomic precisions and sensing the weighted pulse-accent were opposing

temporal epistemologies; Mechanical measurement did not equate to human perceptions

and movements.

118 Edward Dickinson, The Education of a Music Lover (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons,

1911), 118-120.

Page 103: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

82

In the 1630s, Marin Mersenne first theorized that the simple pendulum could

perhaps be a reference to musical performances. But in an age without trustworthy,

precise, or portable mechanical timekeepers, he also postulated that a person’s variable

heartbeat could judge the hour of day.119

As science historians are well aware, the

isochronism—or equal-swinging quality—of the simple pendulum was first discovered

by Galileo Galilei, who timed a moving lamp at the Pisa Cathedral to an exceedingly

familiar tempo source for musicians such as his father: the self-referential heartbeat. The

subjective pulse was a common and accepted temporal reference for Galileo’s day,

apparently for both music performances and empirical observation. Informed by

Galileo’s discovery, Mersenne eventually discounted his own tempo-pendulum theory,

recognizing that a subjective, sensory world ultimately defined time in music beyond the

mechanical isochronism of the pendulum. For Mersenne, his own musical-pendulum

concept was completely wanting as a practical solution:

Because [musicians] change the measure [pulse] many times, either binary or

ternary, in performing a single piece of music, [and] by hurrying or holding back

the lowering and raising [of the hand] according to the character and the words, or

the different passions of the text which they treat, it is difficult to apply any

certain rule if they do not use as many different [pendulum] strings as they wish

to have different meters [pulses].120

119 See Marin Mersenne, Traite' de l'harmonie universelle ou est conteu la Musique Theoretique

et pratique (Paris, 1627); “Critical translation of the second book” by John Bernard Egan (Ph.D.

diss., Indiana University, 1962), 43-4. Mersenne states, “It is certain that the beating of the pulse

can be low and deep, rapid or slow, small and feeble, or vehement, equal, or unequal, like sounds,

so that one could find all musical intervals in the different pulse of the same person at various

times, in diverse moods, health, and sickness. Some maintain that one can tell what time it is by

the movement of the pulse. The pulse quickens its movement from midnight on or from six

o'clock in the morning until noon.”

120

Marin Mersenne, Harmonie universelle (Paris: Cramoisy, 1636); translated by Carol

MacClintock in Readings in the History of Music Performance (Bloomington: Indiana University

Press, 1979), 154.

Page 104: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

83

The tactus—the movement of the hand itself—as Mersenne clearly explains, was not a

pendulum, and certainly not a ticking clock that measured proportions in any objective or

exacting fashion. Mersenne confirmed the rhetorical-temporal practices of his day: the

tactus was beholden to the will of the interpreter alone. The objective pendulum and the

subjective musical pulse portrayed two different “times.”

For a time-telling culture that recognized clear imperfections in personal time-

keeping machines, self-referential standards remained for many the most precise and

reliable references available, well through the eighteenth century. Etienne Loulié, the

“inventor”121

of the first seemingly practical tempo machine, the chronométre, also

acknowledged the preeminence of human interpretation, and suggested that his six-foot

simple pendulum, which he promoted in his 1696 treatise Eléments ou principes de

musique, was an still an approximation of the initial human tactus—a reference especially

necessary for the new and foreign music of his day. In Eléments, Loulié explains the

chronométre is, “an instrument by means of which composers of music will henceforth

be able to indicate the true tempo of their compositions and their Airs, will be able to be

performed in their absence as if they themselves were beating time.”122

Even the early Encyclopædia Britannica realized—over 150 years after

Mersenne and nearly a century after Loulié’s Elements—that the self-referential tempo

121 As early-modern inventor, Loulié promoted a design and description based largely on

Mersenne’s tempo-pendulum theories. He did not manufacture the chronométre for distribution

or sale.

122 Etienne Loulié, Eléments ou principes de musique, trans. and ed. Cohen (New York: Institute

of Mediaeval Music, 1965), 85. (Italics mine.) Loulié assumes the composer-performer to be the

primary dictator of tempos.

Page 105: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

84

was a more desirable, “precise,” and “sensible” standard for music making than a

swinging simple pendulum could ever offer:

It is a surprising phenomenon to observe with how much precision, by the

assistance of a little habit and practice, initiates may be brought to follow and

distinguish the times, with an equality so perfect, that no pendulum can vibrate

more justly than the hand or foot of a good musician, and that even the internal

perception of this equality is sufficient to conduct them and to answer with

accuracy every purpose of sensible motion; so that, in a concert, every performer

plays or sings the bar [the pulse-accent] with the utmost exactitude, without

hearing the time distinguished by any other, or distinguishing it himself any other

ways that by the succession of his own ideas.123

The Encyclopædia Britannica asserted that human time was more perfect, more “precise”

than mechanical motion. For a pre-industrial musical culture, the prevailing belief

seemed to be that the “precision” of qualitative rhythmic sensations, the “sensible

motion” of living musical time, trumped clockwork consistency. The “perfect” and

subjective “internal perceptions” of time formed through a “succession of ideas”

predominated over simple-pendulum oscillations in concept and practice.

These early-modern sources suggested that musical-temporal “perfection” was

inherent in human-derived references. If perfect early-modern tempos could be derived

from the physical movement and pulse-sense of living interpreters, then William Tans’ur,

in a telling irony, defined objective pendulum swings using astoundingly apt

comparisons:

123 “Time,” Encyclopædia Britannica, 8613.

Page 106: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

85

Four Pulses of a Pendulum [are] nearly the Space between the Beat of the Pulse

and Heart; (the Systole or Contraction answering to the Elevation or Lifting up of

the Hand, and its Diastole or Dilation, to the Letting it down, &c.) The like being

understood of the Pendulum both Course, and Recourse, is such a certain Space of

Time.124

In the early-modern age, the fundamental tempo reference was the individual pulse-sense,

a measure validated during the discovery of the isochronous pendulum swing by Galileo,

who only perceived this external, equal-moving phenomenon through the comparison

with his own heartbeat. Rousseau also described how the “perfect” technique for

dictating musical time stemmed not from any beat-per-minute designations or pendulum

swings, but from fundamental, long-understood physical processes:

Beating of time is the manner of marking the time by the motion of the hand or

foot, which regulates its duration, and by which all its measures of the same

degree are rendered, in the execution, perfectly equal in the chronic value of

time.125

For Rousseau and others in the early-modern age, the living pulse-sense contained the

appropriate and desirable temporal precision. Consequently, any externalized, mechanical

musical-temporal guide—whether it was a pendulum swing or a clockwork click—was

often looked upon as an impossibility for performance practices, seen as a less “accurate”

form of musical time and action, due to the stagnant or lifeless mode of tempo, pulse, and

rhythm that machines conveyed.

Thus, almost a century after Quantz stated that it was impossible to play exactly

under an exactly and continuously regulated pulse-standard, and that the chronométre

124 William Tans'ur, A new musical grammar, and dictionary: or, a general introduction to the

whole art of musick, (London: Robert Brown, for James Hodges, 1756), 49.

125 Jean-Jacques Rousseau, “Beating of Time,” translated in Appendix to Grassineau’s Musical

dictionary, selected from the Dictionnaire de musique of J. J. Rousseau (London: J. Robson

1769), 6.

Page 107: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

86

was an ineffectual substitute, Hector Berlioz similarly described the proper use of

Maelzel’s clockwork metronome—the first widely promoted and produced tempo

reference—as a mere, mechanized reflection of a more “perfect” living sense. Like the

late eighteenth-century chronométre, the more precise clockwork metronome was still a

substitute for initial, “human” beat indications; it was not to be mistaken for a mechanical

regulator, as Berlioz explains:

I do not mean to say…that it is necessary to imitate the mathematical regularity of

the metronome; all music so performed would become of freezing stiffness, and I

even doubt whether it would be possible to observe so flat a uniformity during a

certain number of bars.126

Despite the composer’s recognition that neither the metronome, nor any machine, was a

regulator of subjective musical time, misconceptions in the use of the machine abounded

during Berlioz’s lifetime and well through the nineteenth century. Richard Storrs Willis,

in an 1853 article “On Musical Tempo,” confirmed:

There are many persons, however, who mistakenly think that the intention of the

metronome is to have its unvarying beat followed throughout an entire piece,

denying all freedom to the play of feeling.127

The metronome projected an alternate species of “time,” an anti-human time now

imposed through clockwork regulation. Prior to the twentieth century, when the musical

126 Hector Berlioz, A Treatise on Modern Instrumentation and Orchestration, trans. Mary

Cowden Clarke, (London: Novello, Ewer, and Co., 1856), 246. As musical visionary, Berlioz was

one of the only composer-directors to claim a significant practical purpose and use for the

clockwork metronome. Berlioz personally employed metronomes during his opera rehearsals so

that offstage chorus directors—with meager musical abilities—would be more likely correspond

to the tempo of onstage performers. Such use of Maelzel’s metronome was uncommon, and

Berlioz admits that not many French opera companies employed his suggestions.

127

Richard Storrs Willis, “On Musical Tempo,” Musical World and New York Musical Times,

June 11, 1853, 82. After this initial caveat, the article provides an introduction in the use of the

metronome, derived from earlier documents, including Hummel’s and Maelzel’s own

instructions.

Page 108: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

87

performance adhered strictly to the beat—as the term guisto implied—tempo was still

connected to the pulse-sensation, not to mechanical references. At the end of the

nineteenth century, Franz Kullak reminds musicians that exactitude in musical time is a

culturally contextual variable, not a mechanical-temporal constant for all ages:

We, and probably Ries, too, understand by ‘strictness in tempo’ only the strictness

of a steady musician, and not an invariable and absolute coincidence with the

strokes of a metronome, hardly needs to be added. We are also well aware that

even the steadiest musician warms to his work, and calms down at a fitting

opportunity; and it may be said, in general, that occasional slight ritardandi at

transitional passages, on the resumption of the main theme, or before fermate, are

most to be recommended among all subjective deviations from the tempo, and

may be employed in concertos, in so far as they are applicable, in unaccompanied

passages.128

Marx also expressed the drastic differences between mechanical motion and musical

time:

In truth, however, music is little concerned with the mathematically exact division

of quarters. Her object is to excite and to manifest the emotions of the heart and of

the soul; and in that view the approximative directions of the composer are

appropriate for her and true to nature, than metronomic exactitude.129

He knew that suggestive words were more vital than metronomic rules due to the

inherent, desirable subjectivity displayed in self-referential tempos and rhetorical

performance practices:130

128 Kullak, 31.

129

Marx, General Music Instruction, 33.

130

Ironically, Beethoven and Maelzel first promoted the metronome, and more importantly the

metronomic scale, in order to replace traditional Italian tempo words, which Beethoven believed

no longer related to the true emotional character of his music. Yet, Beethoven recognized that

other time and affect words could never be done away with, since they still signified the

interpretive, qualitative “spirit” transcending quantified indications.

Writing to Ignaz Franz, Elder von Mosel in 1817, Beethoven explained: “To take one

example, what can be more absurd than Allegro, which really signifies merry, and how very far

removed we often are from the idea of that tempo. So much so that the piece itself means the very

opposite of the indication…But the words describing the character of the composition are a

different matter. We cannot give these up. Indeed the tempo is more like the body, but these

Page 109: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

88

Consequently the vague but less restrictive indications by means of general terms

appear to be more congenial than a rigorous subdivision into minutes and seconds

by the metronome. The musical executant, or the director of a grand performance,

must indeed endeavor to conceive and represent as faithfully and earnestly as

possible the spirit of the composition; hence, it is incumbent on him also to pay

the greatest attention to the time indicated by the composer.131

All aspects of musical temporality, as Marx reiterated, rested with the skilled performer,

who was the final temporal agent:

But all ultimately depends upon his [the musical executant’s or director’s] own

animus, and the degree in which the work identifies itself with his feelings; for

from his own inspired conception alone can it be rendered with animation and

effect; while, if performed according to mere abstract and mechanical rules, it

remains inanimate and unanimating.132

By 1827, C. M. v. Weber similarly considered the complete failure of metronomes and

metronomic indications to relate the complexity heard in human-derived musical time:

Such indications can be found only in the feelings of the performer, or of the

director; if they exist not in one of the two, the metronome is unable to supply the

want; all that this can do is, mechanically to prevent any gross mistakes. As to an

attempt to denote all the delicate shades of feeling, and the consequent

modifications necessary to give full effect to a performance, I have found every

endeavor fruitless, and have desisted from the task as hopeless.133

As accounts attest well through nineteenth century, skilled musicians neither completely

trusted nor desired temporal machines—the timekeepers of science, industry, and

travel—as musical references, since the sensory qualities of rhythm, pulse, and

proportion—the “wavy outline” of musical time—consistently trumped the “flat

certainly refer to the spirit of the composition.” See Ludwig van Beethoven, Letters, Collected,

translated and edited by Emily Anderson, Volume II (London: Macmillan; New York: St.

Martin's Press, 1961), 727, letter 845.

131 Adolf Bernhard Marx, Universal School of Music, translated from the Fifth Edition of the

Original German by A. H. Wehrhan (London: Robert Cocks and Co., 1853), 85.

132

Ibid.

133

C. M. von Weber, 220.

Page 110: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

89

stiffness” of clockwork. Affirmations of these human-temporal qualities even appeared in

middle-class America, in magazines such as Church’s Musical Visitor, in which amateurs

were reminded: “It should be borne in mind that the real use of the metronome, of any or

all descriptions, is to indicate the speed of the movement. A few swings of the tape or

pendulum are quite sufficient for this purpose. No good musician will need or use [the

metronome] for ‘keeping time.’”134

Marx listed a myriad of reasons why the machine was practically useless for all

skilled musicians’ sensibilities, citing Beethoven as the paradigmatic, nineteenth-century

musical “artist”:

However valuable the instrument [metronome] may be to the composer, for a safe

placing of the tempo desired by him, an absolute determination of the tempo is

not in accordance with the spirit of art. The tempo is determined not only by the

thought out of which a work of art has sprung, and by the manifold contents

thereof, but also by the temporary mood of the performer; the amount of vibrating

material (whether an orchestra is abundant or limited in numbers), the breadth of

the room in which the tone waves are to be developed, require consideration. This

probably explains (at least in part) the unanimous declaration of Czerny, Madame

von Ertmann, and others [that] Beethoven…played his compositions differently

every time.”135

It seems that this practice of unfettered rhythmic interpretation—beyond notation, beyond

mathematically precise readings, beyond metronomes—was afforded to all creative

musicians throughout Western history. Beethoven himself was quoted as expressing the

variability of tempo words in relation to musical movement. For instance, the term

Andantino is “sometimes…very nearly an Allegro; on the other hand, it is often to be

134 [Article 1, no title], Church’s Musical Visitor, January 1882, 102; http://www.proquest.com/

(accessed March 13, 2007).

135

Marx, Introduction to the Interpretation of Beethoven Piano Works, 68.

Page 111: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

90

played Adagio.”136

Metronomic indications, according to Ignaz Moscheles—an important

“metronomiser” of Beethoven’s published music—were never to be employed as

objective dictators for the living interpretation of Beethoven’s compositions. In his 1841

edition of Schindler’s The Life of Beethoven, Moscheles assumed:

The musical world knows that marking time by a metronome is but a slight guide

for performers and conductors. Its object is to show the general time of a

movement, particularly at its commencement; but it is not to be followed strictly

throughout; for no piece, except a march or a dance, would have any real life and

expression, or light and shade, if the Solo performer, or the orchestra under its

conductor, were strictly to adhere to one and the same tempo, without regard to

the many marks which command its variations…The player or conductor, who

enters into the time and spirit of the piece must feel when and where he has to

introduce the necessary changes: and these are often of so delicate a nature, that

the marks of the metronome would become superabundant, not to say

impossible.137

As these comments uncover, mechanical regulations were never intended to predominate

over musical time well into the nineteenth century. In the previous century, philosopher

and composer Jean-Jacques Rousseau—the son of an esteemed Swiss watchmaker—

likewise understood that the art of rhythm was not a mechanical activity; that musical

proportion was still tempered by human sensation and volition, not by clockwork

measurements. Citing nearly identical arguments as nineteenth-century theorists,

Rousseau stated in Europe’s largest compendium of knowledge to-date, under the

heading “Chronometer”:

136 Ludwig van Beethoven, Beethoven's letters: a critical edition, with explanatory notes by Dr.

A. C. Kalischer, translated by J. S. Shedlock, Volume 1 (London: J. M. Dent, 1909), 281.

137Anton Schindler, The Life of Beethoven: Including His Correspondence with His Friends,

Numerous Characteristic Traits, and Remarks on His Musical Works, edited by Ignaz Moscheles,

Volume II (London: Henry Colburn, 1841), 111fn.

Page 112: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

91

The connoisseurs in music…object…to all chronometers in general, that there is

not perhaps in an air two measures that are exactly of the same duration: there are

two things which necessarily contribute to relax or to augment the measure; one

is, the taste and harmony in the pieces of several parts, and the other, the taste and

surmise of harmony in the Solo. A musician, who is master of his art, has not

played four measures of an air before he seized its character, and gives himself up

to it: it is the pleasure of the harmony that alone directs him. Here he endeavors

after strong accords, and there he passes them slightly over; that is to say, he sings

or plays more or less slow from one measure to another.138

In Rousseau’s’ description, no trace of speculative philosophy is evident; he speaks of an

audible, subjective reality, in which the meaning of musical time rested on the variable

expressions of rhythm and pulse. Musical time was about subjective taste, and taste was

gained through personal education and experience alone. Consequently, Rousseau

criticized the purpose of any temporal machine. While he specifically cited Loulié’s

short-lived simple pendulum design from the late seventeenth century, Rousseau’s late

eighteenth-century comments applied to all mechanical measurers for music, which

would become increasingly precise over future decades:

Whatever instrument may be invented to regulate the duration of measure, it will

be impossible, tho' it should be of the most easy execution, that it should ever take

place in practice. The musicians, a confident set of people, and making, like many

others, their own taste the rule of what is right, will never adopt it. They will

neglect the chronometer, and will be only guided by the true character and the

true movement of the airs. Therefore the only good chronometer that it is possible

to have, is an expert musician, who has a fine taste; who has well considered the

music he is to execute, and knows how to beat the time. Machine for machine, it

is best to keep to this.139

Rousseau agreed with many philosopher-musicians before and after his age: the

rhetorical movements and pulses shaping musical time originated solely within the

138 Jean-Jacques Rousseau, “Chronometer,” translated in Appendix to Grassineau’s Musical

dictionary, selected from the Dictionnaire de musique of J. J. Rousseau (London: J. Robson

1769), 10. In part, Rousseau argues on behalf of Diderot.

139 Rousseau, trans. Grassineau,10-11.

Page 113: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

92

abilities of the skilled timeist. Yet, as twentieth-century culture attests, Rousseau’s

assertion did not hold forever; eventually Maelzel’s metronome would be on the pianos

of nearly every amateur, middle-class musician across the world. The metronome would

form a part of the daily practice routines of conservatory and professional performers in

Western orchestral traditions. Scholarly and critical writings would eventually place as

their point of reference the ability of musicians to perform under metronomic

“precisions” that were absolutely “intended by the (long-deceased) composer.” Through

nineteenth-century developments in music education and performance—as following

chapters show—Rousseau’s proclamation overturned completely: by the twentieth

century, many “classically trained” and “historically informed” musicians adopted

metronomes and their beats-per-minute indications, unfailingly and unquestioningly, as

the absolute dictators of time, tempo, pulse, and rhythm. As our contemporary musical

culture’s now overwhelming familiarity with metronomes attests, tempo-machines did

not keep to themselves.

The Pulse and the Pendulum in Nineteenth-Century Traditions

As Rousseau’s critiques allude to, a limited “chronométre tradition”—for which

twentieth-century performance-practice scholars such as Borrell, Harding, Sachs, and

Schwant consider the origins of a modern “clockwork metronome tradition”—followed

Loulié’s pendulum design through published writings by French Academy of Science

members Joseph Sauveur and Comte d’Onzembray, and only one contemporaneous

French music publication, Michel L’Affilard’s (1656-1708) fifth edition of Principes

Page 114: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

93

très-faciles (1705).140

Yet, if Loulié’s pendulum was a useful and desirable tool, one

corresponding to the widespread metronomic tradition of the twentieth century, then

historical musicology has yet to explain why no other Ballard music publication spanning

the eighteenth century offers markings or a comprehensive description for this machine.

As discussed in his acoustics treatise Système general des intervalles des sons

(1701), Sauveur’s “improvement” of the chronométre—which replaced Loulié’s non-

standardized pendulum ruler with a scale for “astronomical inches” most familiar to clock

builders and Academy members—followed the trends of seventeenth-century pendulum

scientists, most notably Galileo Galilei and Christiaan Huygens. As such, Sauveur’s

revised time-scale for the six-foot tempo-pendulum design was more speculative than

practical, especially since Joseph Sauveur himself, as sources thorough the nineteenth-

century relate, “was deaf, he had a false voice, and no appreciation of music,”141

and “he

had no musical voice nor ear [and] was obliged to borrow other people’s voice and

ears.”142

Nevertheless, even Sauveur, with his limited practical or sensory knowledge of

musical tone and time, recognized that the chronométre—when marked with precise

astronomical inches—was still not a clockwork astronomical regulator, but a more limited

140 Many of the vocal compositions found in L’Affilard’s fifth edition of Principes très-faciles

(1705) were revised versions of his previous edition (1701), which contained no tempo-pendulum

indications. (L’Affilard’s fourth edition appeared five years after Loulié published the

chronométre design.) L’Affilard’s works are airs de cour representative of the time. As skilled

French musicians may have recognized—and as Loulié’s own indications suggest—these airs do

not seem in particular need of pendulum numbers, since they were neither foreign nor unusual in

style, meter, or sentiment.

141 John Henry Pepper, Cycolpaedic Science Simplified (London: Frederick Warne and Co.,

1869), 514.

142

Charles Hutton, A philosophical and mathematical dictionary, volume II (London: Printed for

the author, 1815), 357.

Page 115: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

94

mechanical reflection of the living tactus in which “each vibration [of the pendulum] is

equal to the movement of the hand.”143

In the nineteenth century, Gottfried Weber recalls that a host of obscure tempo-

tools, including the chronométre, preceded the Maelzel metronome. None gained any

lasting success with either composers or performers, as Weber states, “various machines

have been invented and proposed since the seventeenth century, under the names of

Measure-measurers, Measure-clocks, Musical Time-measurers, Chronometers,

Rhythmometers, Metrometers, Measure-indicators, Tempo-indicators, and the like, which

were intended to strike quicker or slower, just according as they were regulated to this or

that particular number; and hence, in compliance with this plan, musical composers,

instead of employing the technical terms Allegro, Andante, and the like, were now, with

reference to these striking machines, hereafter merely to place the following signs at the

commencement of their piece of music.”144

Gottfried Weber’s recognition—that eighteenth-century “proto-metronomes,”

including Loulié’s chronométre, had heretofore failed in theory and practice—seems to

be justified given similar comments by Tomlinson, Quantz, and Rousseau over the course

of the early-modern age. English composer Thomas Wright, when promoting his own

variety of tempo-machine, also commented as late as 1795, “the elaborate Chronométres

of Monsieur Loulié, and Monsieur Sauveur, with the Metrometre of later invention, have

143 See Joseph Sauveur, Principes D'Acoustic et de Musique ou Systeme General des Intervalles

des sons (Paris, 1701). For an additional discussion see Robert Eugene Maxham, “The

contributions of Joseph Sauveur (1653-1716) to acoustics,” Vol. I (Ph.D. diss., Eastman School

of Music, 1976), 28-29.

144 G. Weber, General music teacher, 74.

Page 116: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

95

failed of success.” 145

The apparent failure of eighteenth-century tempo machines—not

only of Loulié’s chronométre but of later more scientific and precise “improvements”—

in becoming necessary or desirable aids to musical practice and pedagogy, is further

suggested by the clear absence of numerical-mechanical tempo data in the majority of

early-modern music publications throughout Europe. It seems that many skilled

musicians neither seriously considered nor utilized tempo-machines well through the

eighteenth century because of an inability for external, artificial swings to reflect living

references and musical practices—and not the inability of pendulums to exactly mimic

modern-day metronomes.

As this study explores, the incorporation of any technology into common usage is

never an instantaneous process; the acceptance of machines into society takes time, and

this is especially true for the clockwork metronome. The move towards mechanical

tempo reference was gradual, and evidence strongly suggests that before the clockwork

metronome became an accepted standard for music education, the homemade simple-

pendulum “metronome,” fitted with Maelzel’s beats-per-minute scale, seemed to be a

more convenient and inexpensive solution for early-nineteenth-century educators and

theirs students. For those amateurs still learning the sensory fundamentals of becoming a

good timeist, Maelzel’s tempo ruler did offer the most standardized method to-date for

describing the initial pace of uncommon, difficult, and new music. But many considered

the clockwork machine that dictated those values to be overly precise, expensive, and

potentially damaging to musical sensibilities. Thus, while Mersenne, Quantz, Rousseau,

145 See Thomas Wright, A Concerto for Harpsichord. Reprinted in R. E. M. Harding, Origins of

Musical Time and Expression (London: Oxford University Press, 1938), Plate 17.

Page 117: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

96

and the early Encyclopædia Britannica denied the practicality of the pendulum and

chronométre, some nineteenth-century theorists and pedagogues reconsidered the role of

older technology in practice, claiming that Maelzel’s new beats-per-minute scale was best

measured, not by Maelzel’s new and expensive metronome, but by a far less complicated

swinging pendulum—a basic machine first discounted by Mersenne in the 1630s and

later championed by Loulié and Sauveur at the end of the seventeenth century.

According to many nineteenth-century instructors, the simple pendulum had many

advantages over the clockwork metronome, as Weber explains: “such metronomical signs

are not only useless to all those who do not possess [Maelzel’s] machines, but also even

to those who have them, except in cases where the machine can be placed close by.

[Surmounting this problem] can in fact be done by using, instead of Maelzel's machine,

as can be done with entire satisfaction, merely a simple thread pendulum, i. e. any small

weight as e. g. a lead ball of any size that may be preferred, suspended by a thread; an

instrument, which every one can manufacture for himself in two minutes' time.”146

The

self-referential tempo was once the most simple, desirable, and common standard for

gauging musical time. And for a culture that still placed greater emphasis on subjective

interpretation in performance than mechanical objectivity, Weber claimed the self-built

simple pendulum was far more desirable than an exceedingly precise metronome. In the

eighteenth century, the pulse-sense prevailed over tempo-pendulum technology, yet in the

nineteenth century, the swinging pendulum prevailed over clockwork complexity. Weber

nevertheless related a musical and educational culture still largely unconcerned with

146 G. Weber, 75.

Page 118: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

97

mechanical-temporal precisions, as he encouraged students to construct a simple tempo-

pendulum:

This operation is the more easy, inasmuch as such a proceeding with the

pendulum requires throughout no particular exactness and care, farther than only

perhaps that one do not allow the pendulum to make too large or wide vibrations,

because in the case the ball imperceptibly retards its motion. On the other hand, it

is not at all necessary to measure the inches with very particular exactness; for

even a very considerable difference of length, as e. g. the difference between 15

and 16, amounts, musically considered, to a mere nothing; and even that between

15 and 17 or 18 is scarcely perceptible.147

Weber explains that mechanical “exactness” was unnecessary in relation to the known,

fluctuating temporality of music performances. While Maelzel’s standardized beats-per-

minute scale was helpful, Weber considered that the usefulness, accessibility, and

convenience of his own methods made Maelzel’s complicated machine practically

defunct:

We see from all this, with what perfect convenience a mere simple thread

pendulum can supply the place of a metronome; and that it would not therefore be

amiss, if the composer should write the corresponding lengths of pendulum

alongside his designation of lime according to the degrees of the metronome, as

e.g.

Andante, Mälzel metron. q = 60 (Pend. 38 Rhen. inch);

for, such a designation of time would be immediately practicable to every one, as

well by means of a simple thread pendulum, as by means of a metronome, and

might thus be understood by thousands of readers, players, or music directors, to

whom a designation of time written merely according to metronomical degrees is

unintelligible, for the want of an opportunity to consult the oracle of a metronome

or of a reduction table. One might, moreover, for the accommodation of those

who may not be acquainted with the inch used, or who may not have it

immediately at command, cause an inch scale to be printed in connection with this

designation of the time. Then it would be entirely immaterial, whether one should

use the Rhenish or the Parisian inch, the English inch, the French metre [mètres],

or whatever else, as a measure; for, a piece of music thus marked, carries with

147 Ibid., 76.

Page 119: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

98

itself, wherever it goes, its measure of time together with the measure of the inch

employed in designating the time.148

Perhaps unwittingly, Weber offered a pedagogical tempo-aid first propounded by

Mersenne in the early seventeenth century; first promoted by Loulié in the late

seventeenth century; discounted by Quantz, Rousseau, and the Encyclopædia Britannica

in the late eighteenth century; and only reconsidered in 1813. Between the lifetimes of

Mersenne and Weber—and despite Maelzel’s new and obscure invention—very little had

progressed in the technology of musical-time telling.

Marx confirmed that the simple pendulum was the most appropriate tool for an

average student who wished to find initial tempos in nineteenth-century music. Although

Marx rejected any metronome as the indicator of musical expressivity, he recognized that

the silent pendulum, as an initial pulse reference, was not to be utilized as a constant

mechanical regulator. Ease of use outweighed the precision of external tempo-tools, as

Marx relates: “A more simple contrivance, which is cheaper and less liable to get out of

order than the wheelwork of Maelzel, is the String Pendulum, recommended by Gottfried

Weber, consisting merely of a string divided into certain proportions, by means of knots,

&c. and to one end of which a small leaden weight is attached.”149

Marx’s contemporary,

the French pedagogue and musical conservative François-Joseph Fétis (1784-1871),

148 Ibid.

149

Marx, The universal school of music, 83. In a footnote to the page, Marx’s translator solicits

readers to purchase the now-“superior” clockwork metronomes produced and sold by the

publisher of this very edition, attesting to the strong commercial link between metronome sales

and music publications: “The translator takes great pleasure in directing the attention of

musicians and amateurs to the Metronomes (with or without bell) sold by Messrs. Cocks & Co.

He has inspected and tried a great number of them, and can testify to their elegance, superior

workmanship, and preciseness of action. (Vide also Hamilton's Treatise on the Metronome,

published by the same firm.)”

Page 120: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

99

while one of the most prominent champions of Maelzel’s clockwork metronome in the

early nineteenth century, too realized that “chronometers of the [simple pendulum]

kind…have the double advantage of being simple in their construction, and of trifling

expense.”150

Many pedagogues over the nineteenth century attested that the simple

pendulum—as a reference to the living, musical pulse—was no clockwork metronome. It

did not click or move perpetually, and because it was a visual aid, the pendulum swing

was only a brief reference that could not be adhered to while performing music—thus

leaving the process of musical time safely in the hands of the timeist. The simple

pendulum assisted amateurs in feeling the weighted pulse, not in regulating each internal

beat in every bar, in perpetuam. As a host of educators instructed, the pendulum was not

the a priori indicator of time—it visually suggested the sensory pulse, the self-referential

standard known before the pendulum or metronome ever existed. Such simple-pendulum

“metronomes” took on many forms over the century, and John W. Tufts claimed to invent

one popular design, the “pocket metronome,” by the 1860s. Intended specifically to assist

young amateurs, the pocket metronome—a bob with a retractable tape-measurer—closely

followed the concept of Weber’s chronometer. Tuft’s The Cecilian series of study and

song (1892) reports:

150 François-Joseph Fétis, Music explained to the world: or, How to understand music and enjoy

its performance, translated for the Boston Academy of Music (Boston: Benjamin Perkins, 1842),

37. An important proponent of the French conservatory system, Fétis seems to be unique in

stating his preference—in the third decade of the nineteenth century—for the “clicking”

Maelzel’s metronome above various simple-pendulum designs, which contrastingly “have the

inconvenience of not making sensible to the ear the tick or stroke of time.”

Page 121: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

100

Some thirty years ago the author devised a pendulum for singers and players, to

enable them to gain regularity and certainty in the different kinds of mensural

accent. Having proved its efficacy in doing this work in vocal and instrumental

practice, it was introduced in a series of exercises prepared for public schools.

This pocket metronome, now so generally used, has proved to be of great value;

in fact, in no other way can a true feeling for accurate time be so easily gained.151

Yet even the novice students were reminded well through the nineteenth century that

tempo machines did not portray musical reality. The pocket metronome could aid in

pointing the way towards the “mensural accent,” but the “true feeling for accurate time”

ultimately rested with the musician once the pendulum ceased. Pendulum swings and

metronome clicks did not define the many ineffable rhythmical nuances created by the

musical artist, which went beyond the composer’s initial tempo indication. John Taylor,

in The student's text-book of the science of music, recounted as late as 1876 how the

Maelzel metronome—a machine far more automatic than the simple pendulum—failed to

reference the true nature of musical time:

Although invaluable in cases of doubt as an undeviating standard, in the last

resort, of arbitration, and highly useful in the earlier stages of musical practice as

a means of engrafting a just and necessary sense of rhythmical duration or time

(considered from a mechanical point of view), still, as useless in determining the

numerous undefinable fluctuations and irregular rhythmical deviations

inseparable from the only true expression of a composition— that, viz., springing

from an appreciation of its character and scope, and having its origin in an ever-

prompting feeling—the introduction of the metronome has failed to abrogate the

employment of the older and (since more suggestive) truer method of indicating

musical face or rate of movement—the approximate method, viz., involved in the

use of certain verbal directions.152

Precise metronomic clicks or pendulum swings did not correspond in practice with the

pulse-sense, even for nineteenth-century amateur musicians. And throughout the

151 John Wheeler Tufts, The Cecilian series of study and song, Book VI (New York: Silver,

Burdett & Company, 1892): viii.

152

John Taylor, The student's text-book of the science of music (London: George Philip and Son,

1876), 132.

Page 122: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

101

nineteenth century, most skilled musicians educated in pre-metronomic traditions

acknowledged that these long-standing verbal approximations—however “imprecise”

when compared to standardized mechanical measurements—provided a “truer method”

for actualizing the subjectivities of musical time than precision clockwork could ever

offer.

Summarizing the Past, Pre-Metronomic Musical “Times”

These theorists and pedagogues—from Descartes to Christiani and beyond—all speak of

a time more nuanced, more variable, and ultimately more vital than any pendulum swings

or mechanical clicks could impart; nuances within diverse and respective repertoires that

included rhythmical emphases, anticipations, elongations, pauses, and delays; the hand

separation of soprano and bass voices; the “aspiration” and “suspension” of tones; the

free arpeggiation of notated chords; not to forget notes inégale; along with the fluctuation

of pulse-sense as each contrasting phrase, section, lyric, ornament, or harmony suggested.

These living qualities of musical time, while only implied through notation, were

eminently clear to knowledgeable performers, theorists, and pedagogues prior to the

twentieth century—and yet they remain qualities that defy simplified measurements,

reductive schemas, and mechanical certainties.

Reflecting upon the aesthetics of pre-industrialized culture, it seems entirely

logical for these theorists to assert that a rhythmic science could never exist: If thought

was variable; if speech was variable; if physical movement was variable; then so too was

the time of musical performance. In this cultural history of rhythm, basic questions

seldom posited by modern musicologists, theorists, pedagogues, or musicians themselves

must be pursued: How could performers conceive musical time in terms of a clockwork

Page 123: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

102

metronome when clockwork metronomes did not exist, or if they were not utilized to any

rigorous degree? Since, prior to the Mechanical Age, no scientific clock meaningfully

regulated a poem or play, how could a metronome truly dictate the time of a symphony or

sonata? Marx similarly questioned any non-experiential, reductive theorems of musical

time and rhythm. He seemingly upheld three centuries of performance-practice traditions,

reasoning:

If we consider the matter impartially and candidly, we shall acknowledge that,

continuous uniformity of any motion is quite as unnatural in music as in every

other department of human activity. And on this account: that the disposition and

excitement in us can never remain at any fixed point… the excitement and energy

of the executant, moreover, is either heightened in the progress of the

performance, or his fire and power are diminished. Can it be supposed for a

moment practical or psychologically possible, that, in all these phases, the

[musical] measure should remain stagnant and uniform?153

Prior to the twentieth century, performers and theorists readily understood that musical

proportion existed—in various repertoires from respective genres, composers, and

national styles—through the very palpable sensations of pulse and movement, with all of

the variations, subjectivities, and ambiguities these qualities suggested. Past beliefs in

musical temporality rested not on scientific ideals and certainties, but on human

perceptions gained from active experience. As Mattheson commented in Der

vollkommene Capellmeister: “The true mouvement of a musical work…is beyond words.

It is the ultimate perfection of music, accessible only through eminent experience and

talent.”154

The sensory foundations of musical meter—described as internal weight,

swing, arsis and thesis, the energy of beginning, the quiet dwelling upon the note, the

153 Marx, The Music of the Nineteenth Century and Its Culture, 261.

154

Quoted from Curt Sachs, Rhythm and Tempo (New York: W.W. Norton & Co., 1953), 321.

Page 124: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

103

invisible accent—all of these were only realized through the gestural, physical process of

musical performance itself.

Thus, for those concerned with performance realities, musical time was not a

precise science; musical time was an immediate and visceral form of human expression.

It was with these temporal values, restated by diverse and disparate theorists over the

ages—from Descartes to Rameau; Rousseau to Hauptmann—that Adolph Bernhard Marx

summarized the vitality of pre-twentieth-century musical time through this long-

recognized truth:

Rhythm is the expression of the will and pleasure of him who formed it; and we

recognize in it either the determined purpose of the artist, or his sensible delight in

a well-arranged and pleasing or significant succession of tonal quantities. Rhythm

in both forms is indispensible to music… Nor has music has ever been without

rhythm. Neither in the Gregorian cantus planus, nor during the period of mensural

music, neither in the nomos of the Greeks, nor in our modern recitative, nor in the

Ragneys [Ragas] of the Hindoo, has this element of life ever been wanting.155

For three centuries, the conclusion remained: The musician and the musician alone was

the measure of musical time. Yet Marx’s ontological definition of rhythm may strike

many moderns as flippant, imprecise, and ineffectual; since the twentieth century, Marx’s

“rhythm” seems to be the last definition that many twentieth-century scholars or

musicians would ever consider citing as fact.

“Time” is a cultural and communal belief, and so the “times” eventually change.

Modern performers, theorists, and scholars decidedly live within a different musical-time

culture than the one recounted by Marx. World-renowned conductors pride themselves on

authentically informed interpretations, based on exacting, “original” metronome marks—

155 Marx, The Music of the Nineteenth Century and Its Culture, 33.

Page 125: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

104

alongside a rigid adherence to such marks—for music by Beethoven, Berlioz, and

Wagner, although these very composer-performers considered playing to clockwork

references both an impossibility and abomination. Some professional “early-music”

ensembles perform the music of Vivaldi and Bach with such blindingly rapid and

constant metronomic speeds—and with such negligible senses of pulse, weight, or

rhetoric—that slavish practicing to an unwavering metronomic reference seems the only

technique that could have made such interpretations possible. In order to win a rarified

orchestra seat, conservatory instrumentalists—fulfilling both judges’ and their own

teachers’ expectations—mimic exacting gradations of metronomic time, showing how

“precisely” and “accurately” these applicants consistently maintain “tempo” indications

from music spanning from Beethoven to Mahler—even though Beethoven’s colleagues

and Mahler’s own piano-roll recordings strongly confirm that metronomic “precision”

had nothing to do with orchestral performance practices.

Professional teachers and performers likewise ask, “what is the tempo?” of pre-

twentieth century music by first asking, “how fast or slow is the metronome click?”—not,

“where does the invisible pulse-accent lie in relation to the harmony, meter, rhythmical

gestures, and rhetorical sentiments of any given composition?” Many singers believe that

mathematically gauged note-values on the page must prevail over the words, harmonies,

and characters conveyed in vocal repertoire spanning from sixteenth-century madrigals to

nineteenth-century oratorios. Since the 1930s, historical musicologists have cared more

about the “precise” interpretation of historical pendulum swings—so they might

“accurately” translate them to modern, metronomic beats-per-minute—while all but

neglecting the historical knowledge that subjective pulse-senses once guided the

Page 126: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

105

fundamental qualities of measured musical time. Quantz’s meager suggestions to

amateurs are treated as scientific proof of precise metronomic tempos, although Quantz

knew nothing of Maelzel’s future metronome and very little of Loulié’s obscure 1696

Chronométre. In his monumental book Rhythm and Tempo (1953), musicologist Curt

Sachs, while he often seemed skeptical of the metronome as continuous performance

aid,156

could not separate modern, beat-per-minute mechanics from his entire history of

musical time, stating, “ours is a mechanically counting notation,”157

that “the quarter note

[is] our motor unit,”158

while conversely claiming, “the [human] tactus was wholly

unconcerned with the actual rhythm, with grouping or accent…the tactus, does not, and

cannot, reflect the rhythm of a piece.”159

Few pre-metronomic composers have been spared this metronome methodology

in modern music publications, articles, and treatises. In 1984, modern-music specialist

Robert Craft, upon reviewing Rebecca Harris-Warrick’s translation of Saint-Lambert’s

Principles of the Harpsichord (1702), stated, “Saint Lambert’s unit of [tempo]

measurement must set a record for vagueness: ‘the step of a man of average height who

156 Sachs, 21.

157

Ibid., 168.

158

Ibid., 173. A similar statement appears on page 201.

159

Ibid., 242-3. In keeping with a twentieth-century tradition, Sachs often divorced musical time

from the living sensation of pulse heard in measured music. He believed of performance practices

existing centuries before objective, metronomic regulation and measurement that “the strength of

good beats…varied considerably according to the nature of a piece. But performers and listeners

were hardly aware of these minute shades and had no reason for mentioning them.” See Sachs,

256-7.

Page 127: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

106

walks one and a quarter leagues in an hour.’”160

Considering other historical instructions,

Saint Lambert’s self-referential tempo was perhaps the most quantitative and

reproducible for its time; it was, nevertheless, “vague” against the modern metronome,

the modern, mechanical treatment of tempo, and Craft’s modern performance practices.

In 2008, New York Times reviewer Bernhard Holland, detesting pianist Lang Lang’s

gestural performance style, suggested that “conservatories…hire time-and-motion

experts, professionals who could point out that the flailing arm, the bulging eye and the

balletic upper torso are extraneous work in a business best devoted to doing the most with

the least.”161

Holland espoused mechanical-industrial performance training—a clockwork

“time”-study—for “a business” of music making geared towards producing the printed

page with greater technical-rhythmical efficiency. Unbeknownst to this reviewer, and as

this study comprehensively documents, such mechanical-efficiency experiments in time

had been redefining modern music education and performance practices for over a

century.

In subsequent chapters, it will be shown that mechanical efficiency—only

espoused in pedagogy since the turn of the twentieth century—is often incompatible with

willful, creative, or nuanced musical performance. As Bernard Holland and Robert Craft

implied, the modern, temporal worldview often values reproducible efficiencies and

precisions above individual expressions; by extension, this industrial-scientific

160 Robert Craft, “Saint Lambert,” reprinted in Small Craft Advisories (New York: Thames and

Hudson, 1989), 147.

161

Bernhard Holland, “When Histrionics Undermine the Music and the Pianist,” New York Times,

February 6, 2008, http://www.nytimes.com/2008/02/06/arts/music/06look.html.

Page 128: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

107

worldview values metronomic time above the sensory and rhetorical qualities of

historical musical time. If time-tellers do impose hegemony over human thoughts and

actions, a “chronarchy” through continual mechanical reference, then the twentieth

century has indeed witnessed the results of this mechanical hegemony, manifested in a

metronomic performance practice based on ever-greater rhythmical precisions,

exactitudes, and consistencies.

Even Ludwig van Beethoven alluded to such a mechanical hegemony in the

potential employment of metronomes. During another partnership with Maelzel, this time

to promote the metronome as a standard for German musical education, Beethoven

acknowledged that the new device could be seen as an imposition upon current creative

and pedagogical practices. In 1817, when considering how to “bring the Metronome into

the so necessary general use,” Beethoven confided in von Mosel: “I have no doubt they

will call out that I am a tyrant.”162

Nearly half a century later, in 1863, Samuel Butler

prophesized that the hegemonic rise of machines—a “tyranny” only suggested by

Beethoven and Maelzel’s vision of selling a clockwork metronome to “every village

schoolmaster”—would continue to change the qualities of human life for future

generations. Highlighting the growing use of clocks and portable watches during his

century, Butler concluded a Darwinian machine-evolution was indeed occurring

throughout an increasingly industrialized civilization:

162 Beethoven, ed. Kalisher, II: 69. Beethoven’s justification for this potential mechanical

imposition: It “would be better than to accuse us of feudalism.”

Page 129: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

108

Day by day, however, the machines are gaining ground upon us; day by day we

are becoming more subservient to them; more men are daily bound down as

slaves to tend them, more men are daily devoting the energies of their whole lives

to the development of mechanical life. The upshot is simply a question of time,

but that the time will come when the machines will hold the real supremacy over

the world and its inhabitants is what no person of a truly philosophic mind can for

a moment question.163

It will be shown how the growing production, distribution, and use of the clockwork

machines directly corresponded to the ever-changing temporal beliefs in performance

practices. Butler’s prophecy seemed to culminate in twentieth-century performance

culture, in which mechanical precision in musical time is not only commonplace—it is

nearly unquestioned. In 1911, Edward Dickinson recalled the kernel of a more lasting and

profound hegemony to follow, when he succinctly stated: “Metronome marks are more

exact [indicators of tempo], but they are far from being an infallible reliance. At the most

they indicate the general movement of a composition, not the alterations that must

constantly occur. They often serve as fetters to players or conductors that submit to

them.”164

Yet what happens when the fetters of musical time become the standards of

authenticity? What performance practices result when the hegemony of metronomic

regulation, which critics recognized for centuries, becomes largely prescribed in

pedagogy and performance? What happens when “submission” to mechanical,

metronomic regulations—for the sake of modern performance-precision—becomes the

desirable aesthetic? The result, according to Theodore Adorno, was that individualistic,

temporal qualities were very much lost by the mid-twentieth century:

163 Originally from Samuel Butler, “Darwin Among the Machines,” Christ Church Press, June

13, 1863. Reprinted in Samuel Butler, A First Year in Canterbury Settlement, With Other Early

Essays, edited by R. A. Streatfeild (London: A. C. Fifield, 1914), 184-185.

164

Dickinson, 118.

Page 130: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

109

Today there is no music showing any trace of the power of the historical hour that

has remained totally unaffected by the decline of experience—by the substitution,

for “life,” of a process of economic adjustment dictated by concentrated economic

forces of domination. The dying out of subjective time in music seems totally

unavoidable in the midst of a humanity which has made itself into a thing—into

an object of its own organization.165

The following chapters document how this change first came about—how machines and

mechanical rhythm first became definitive references for once subjective acts—how the

hegemony, the “tyranny,” of metronomic time became commonplace. It is shown how,

over the course of a century, the willful fluctuations and intrinsic variability of musical

time came to be heard by many as thievery, unwarranted creativity, or error in relation to

objective mechanical rules; in which any excursion from the written page became

considered whimsical individuality in contrast to absolute composer intention. In brief,

this study charts the ascendance of a new rhythmical ontology, in which the internal

became externalized; the subjective became objectified. Throughout this intellectual and

cultural history, one finds the nuanced feelings for musical time receding in favor of the

precise reproduction offered by mechanical rhythm; faithfulness to composer-intention

becoming misconstrued as faithfulness to metronomic regulation; the rhythmical sense

subsuming to clockwork.

This profound and lasting paradigm shift, in which musical time transformed from

a subjective process to an objective regulation, is one nearly unacknowledged today. A

modern performance culture of efficiency, reproduction, precision, and positivism

prevails, valuing a clockwork species of musical “time” above the sound and sense of

musical pulsation. Yet again, Shakespeare prefigured this diametric opposition between

165 From Theodore Adorno, Philosophy of Modern Music (1948), reprinted in Daniel Albright, ed.

Modernism and Music (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2004), 77.

Page 131: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

110

innate musical perception and mechanical objectivity—alongside the results of

clockwork domination—when a distressed Richard II inquired:

Music do I hear?

Ha, ha! keep time. How sour sweet music is

When time is broke and no proportion kept!

So is it in the music of men's lives.

And here have I the daintiness of ear

To check time broke in a disorder'd string;

But, for the concord of my state and time,

Had not an ear to hear my true time broke.

I wasted time, and now doth time waste me;

For now hath time made me his numb'ring clock:

My thoughts are minutes…

Richard II’s personal-temporal crisis foreshadows a reality for twentieth-century

performance culture. His once-internal sense of time became externalized, regularized,

and mechanized—redundant clockwork time now controlled him, as his own living

movements lost their innate meaning and proportion; the monarch’s “times”—witnessed

in the intrinsic and the extrinsic—had submitted to the clock. The situation of many

modern musicians closely resembles Richard II’s conceptual predicament. Many

contemporary performers can attest to educational and rehearsal regimens in which their

subjective sensations and perceptions for rhythmical proportion, meter, and movement

are first and foremost measured and justified through precise, mathematical fractions of

objective time—no longer by silent, simple pendulum swings, but by unrelenting clicks

of metronomes. For professional music scholars and educators as well, “rhythm,”

“tempo,” and exacting metronomic reference are often conflated.

As this study uncovers, the very meanings and cultural values underlying

“rhythm” have palpably changed since the twentieth century due to a heretofore-

unacknowledged paradigm shift: a metronomic turn in which musical time has audibly

Page 132: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

111

and visibly mechanized. Today, a host of performers, theorists, pedagogues, and

musicologists readily profess an allegiance to modern temporal machines and precision-

oriented measurements, seemingly unaware that the historical and cultural contexts of

time, meter, and rhythm—espoused for over three centuries—have audibly and

conceptually faded in importance. In the present age, “good rhythm” is often an implicit

term for “precise metronomic regulation.” The subsequent chapters expose how, over the

course of the modern Industrial Age, musical time transformed into technological time—

as living rhythmical proportions and thoughts truly became clockwork minutes.

Page 133: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

112

CHAPTER II: MAELZEL’S MACHINES: A RECEPTION HISTORY OF

MAELZEL, HIS MECHANICAL CULTURE, AND THE METRONOME

Johann Nepomuk Maelzel moved to North America in 1825, not to manufacture or

promote the clockwork metronome as he had in Europe, but to continue his career as an

exhibitor of self-moving machines, or automata. Maelzel produced his unique automata

shows in cities across America including Boston, Philadelphia, Washington, Baltimore,

and New York.1 As nineteenth-century accounts attest, Maelzel’s most significant

contribution to the nineteenth century was not the simple invention of the metronome, but

the broad introduction of a new and unusual mechanical culture to a rapidly

industrializing civilization. His legacy is far more profound than one small machine

alone; for the nineteenth-century, Maelzel first exposed the promise, possibilities, and

pitfalls of the mechanized world in which we now reside. The deeper meaning behind

Maelzel’s most influential and lasting machine becomes evident when his mechanical

culture is viewed within nineteenth-century contexts, through recollections of his shows

and creations, and most significantly, in critiques of the metronome.

Before his death at sea in 1838, Maelzel offered a growing nation the visions of

mechanical wonder, mystery, and ingenuity. In the mechanical exhibitions of Maelzel,

spectators witnessed the intriguing, uncanny, and occasionally frightening; They viewed

clockwork automata behaving like people, performing activities once reserved for

humans alone, but acting with altogether inhuman qualities. A special pejorative was

1 Paul Metzner, Crescendo of the Virtuoso (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1998), 186.

Page 134: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

113

given to these qualities, automatical,2 which the widely popular Webster’s Dictionary of

1828 defined as, “1. Belonging to an automaton; having the power of moving itself;

mechanical / 2. Not voluntary; not depending on the will.” Western society consistently

considered all automata, and Maelzel’s machines especially, as soulless, thoughtless

entities, which lacked any sense of living motivation, volition, or spirit; self-moving

clockwork devices were invariably artificial in appearances and actions. The Webster’s

Dictionary of 1828 defines this quality:

ARTIFICIAL, a.

1. Made or contrived by art, or by human skill and labor, in opposition to

natural; as artificial heat or light; an artificial magnet.

2. Feigned, fictitious; not genuine or natural; as artificial tears.

3. Contrived with skill or art.

4. Cultivated; not indigenous; not being of spontaneous growth; as

artificial grasses.

Well through the nineteenth century, many in Western civilization perceived automata as

similar to mechanical time-tellers. As Derham’s and Sully’s important eighteenth-century

horology treatises on “artificial” clock-making remind us, mechanical devices made of

repetitive gear-work were a priori detached from natural time and motion.3 Clocks told

an artificial time unlike the time of the stars, the moon, the sun, or the tides. Just as early-

modern clocks told artificial as opposed to natural time, clockwork facsimiles of people

behaved with artificial as opposed to natural movements. Society clearly acknowledged

that the artificiality of clocks and other automata were the binary opposites of humanity,

2 The arcane but historically appropriate adjective “automatical” is employed throughout this

chapter, replacing the modern equivalent “automatic,” which has lost its pejorative meaning over

the twentieth century.

3 W. Derham, The artificial clock-maker. A treatise of watch and clock-work, shewing to the

meanest capacities the art of calculating numbers to all sorts of movement (London: printed for

James Knapton, 1714); and Henri Sully, Règle artificielle du tems (Paris: Gregoire Dupuis, 1717).

Page 135: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

114

and that the behaviors of these devices were antithetical to innate, internal,4 and natural

action. The 1836 Popular Encyclopedia unambiguously defined “automaton” as “a self-

moving machine, without life.”5 Thus, for Maelzel’s audiences, the puppetry of

automata-display exposed some very distinctive qualities that set artificial machines apart

from humanity.

Self-moving, mechanical artificiality was still uncommon for Maelzel’s America.

The 1828 edition Webster’s Dictionary did not even define the terms “minute”6 and

“second”7 with any acknowledgement to clock time. Indeed, Maelzel’s public found

precision-based mechanical time to be uncustomary and unnecessary in daily life. Single-

hand civic and church clocks were still regulated by the local time of the sundial each

4 “INTERNAL, a. [L. internus.] Inward; interior; being within any limit or surface; not external.

We speak of the internal parts of a body, of a bone, of the earth, &c. Internal excellence is

opposed to external. The internal peace of man, is peace of mind or conscience. The internal

evidence of the divine origin of the Scriptures, is the evidence which arises from the excellence of

its precepts and their adaptation to the condition of man, or from other peculiarities. 1. Pertaining

to the heart…2. Intrinsic; real; as the internal rectitude of actions…”Webster’s Unabridged

Dictionary (1828). Online http://machaut.uchicago.edu/?resource=Webster's (accessed September

20, 2007).

5 Thomas Thomson, Daniel Keyte Sandford, and Allan Cunningham, The Popular Encyclopedia,

Volume 1 (Glasgow: Blackie & Son, 1836), 355.

6 “MINUTE, a. [L. minutus.] 1. Very small, little or slender; of very small bulk or size; small in

consequence; as a minute grain of sand; a minute filament. The blood circulates through very

minute vessels. Minute divisions of a subject often perplex the understanding. Minute details are

tedious. 2. Attending to small things; critical; as minute observation.” Webster’s Unabridged

Dictionary (1828). Online http://machaut.uchicago.edu/?resource=Webster's (accessed September

20, 2007).

7 “SECOND, a. [L. secundus; L. sequor, to follow. See Seek.] 1. That immediately follows the

first; the next following the first in order of place or time; the ordinal of two. Take the second

book from the shelf. Enter the second house. And he slept and dreamed the second time. Gen. 41.

2. Next in value, power, excellence, dignity or rank; inferior. The silks of China are second to

none. Lord Chatham was second to none in eloquence. Dr. Johnson was second to none in

intellectual powers, but second to many in research and erudition.” Webster’s Unabridged

Dictionary (1828). Online http://machaut.uchicago.edu/ ?resource=Webster's (accessed

September 20, 2007).

Page 136: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

115

day. While the specialized fields of astronomy, cartography, and navigation did require

precision clocks that could faithfully measure and synchronize small durations of time,

scientific time telling carried little practical meaning or purpose for early nineteenth

century urban and agrarian communities. Until 1849, pocket-watches were primarily

imported into America as high-end luxuries,8 and even then, these were not ubiquitous

technologies like the wristwatches of today.9 Only after the mid-nineteenth century,

North American urban society began relying more heavily on synchronized clocks based

solely on artificial time. With the rapid growth of railroads, precise measurements of

five-minute intervals or smaller units of time became essential to safe and efficient

transportation. Accomplishing this task was not as simple as looking at the town clock.

Employing the latest technologies, astronomical observatories across the nation often

supplied the most precise, scientific standards of time for railroad companies. A

continental-wide system of mechanical timekeeping was not merely useful in the pursuit

of high-speed continental travel; it was a question of life and death.10

Nevertheless,

standardized time zones, initially conceived to organize the complex schedules of the

railroad industry, were not established as a national system in the United States until

8 See Carlene Stephens, On Time (Boston: Bullfinch Press, 2002), 51; and David S. Landes,

Revolution in time: clocks and the making of the modern world, Second Edition (Cambridge:

Harvard U. Press, 2000), 302, 307.

9 A telling and ironic anecdote from 1850 verifies: “When P. S. Bartlett boasted that his company

was making seven watches a day, his friend laughed, ‘Why, where could you sell seven watches a

day?’” From Harry C. Brearley, Time Telling Through the Ages (New York: Doubleday, Page &

Co., 1919), figure caption, between 176-177.

10

The rise of standardized time in North America is well documented in Ian R. Bartky, Selling

the True Time: Nineteenth-Century Timekeeping in America (Stanford: Stanford University Press,

2000).

Page 137: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

116

1883.11

Local, sundial time (that is, natural and variable time) was still the overriding

temporal reference for the American public during much of the nineteenth century.

Before the world was organized through time zones and precise train schedules,

and before pocket watches became the standard gentlemen’s accessory for urban life,

Maelzel was a seminal figure in the emerging culture of the machine. As nineteenth-

century readings expose, his self-moving inventions—indeed, all nineteenth-century

clockwork machines—had cultural meanings that transcended the scientific values of

unfailing consistency, exact replication, and precision measurement. Through his

exhibitions, audiences witnessed two opposing value systems in these automata, in which

the living traits of subjectivity, creativity, and sensation emerged as antitheses to the

artificially contrived action and appearance of clockwork motion. As many musicians,

pedagogues, and critics attested, Maelzel’s metronome directly related to his other

clockwork creations; nineteenth-century society perceived the metronome to be just

another of Maelzel’s inventions, just another product of his mechanical culture. For more

than a century, many attested that the metronome was a tool that audibly created musical

automata.

Criticisms of the metronome were nothing exceptional in the nineteenth century,

and they even appear from the earliest proponents of the device. Indeed, Ludwig van

Beethoven became one of the first protesters of Maelzel’s automatical creation. In an

often-overlooked statement regarding Maelzel’s tempo-clock, Beethoven wrote the

publisher Bernhard Schotts Söhne in 1826:

11 Landes, 304.

Page 138: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

117

This is to inform you that the quartet was delivered to Frank a week ago…The

metronome markings (the deuce take everything mechanical) will follow—

follow—follow—12

Beethoven mocked the incessant, unwavering, and monotonous metronome, which he

largely rejected by the end of his life. According to Schindler, Marx, and others

throughout the nineteenth century, Beethoven “finally declared, ‘No metronome at all!

Those who have a right feeling do not need it, and those who have not, will not be helped

by it.’”13

Schindler later commented that the metronome could never rule over

Beethoven’s notions of musical time; a proper metronome mark, at best, “leads the

intelligent musician by the right path into the spirit of the music.”14

According to

Schindler, the living, subjective realities of performance coupled with numerous

publication errors often thwarted Beethoven’s initial—and perhaps indefinite—

metronomic indications.15

Indeed it seems that the composer often found the machine a

12 Ludwig van Beethoven, Letters, collected, translated and edited with an introduction,

appendixes, notes and indexes by Emily Anderson, Volume III (London: Macmillan; New York:

St. Martin's Press, 1961), 1295, Letter 1498.

13

Adolf Bernhard Marx, Introduction to the Interpretation of Beethoven Piano Works, translated

by Fannie Louise Gwinner (Chicago: Clayton F. Summy Co., 1895), 68. Beethoven’s quote,

stemming from Schindler’s biography, was often repeated in sources throughout the nineteenth

century, including Franz Kullak, Beethoven’s Piano-Playing, translated by Theodore Baker (New

York: G. Schirmer, 1901), 22.

Anton Schindler, The Life of Beethoven: Including His Correspondence with His Friends,

Numerous Characteristic Traits, and Remarks on His Musical Works, edited by Ignaz Moscheles,

Volume II (London: Henry Colburn, 1841), 106 fn.

15

Schindler claimed “[during the winter of 1825-26] in my presence [Beethoven] ascertained that

the metronomic signs in the printed scores [of the symphonies] were faulty, in fixing the tempi

too quick; and, indeed, he declared that many of those metronomic signs were not authorized by

him.” See Schindler, 104-5; 116.

The possibility exists, however, that Beethoven changed his mind about metronome

indications once he recognized the poor results in practice. Schindler also recalled that Beethoven

occasionally marked the same music with different metronomic indications. See Schindler, 116-

117.

Page 139: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

118

total nuisance if one cites the numerous ex post facto metronomic “calculations”

Beethoven was asked to make at the behest of publishers, who often invented their own

markings. Their editorial license in printing metronomic tempos lasted throughout the

century, a practice which Schindler chided for causing “a Babel of confusion as to the

right feeling” in Beethoven’s music less than two decades after the composer’s passing.16

Evidence suggests that Beethoven often considered Maelzel’s metronome to be a

meager reflection of his own living pulse; the mechanical movement of the metronome

was a lifeless representation of his own heartbeat. “The metronome tempos will soon

follow,” Beethoven wrote to Bernhard Schotts Söhne on March 1825, “my own is ill and

has to recover its even steady pulse at the watch-maker’s.”17

Some communications make

Wagner had a similar experience and admitted in Über das Dirigiren: “In my earlier

operas I gave detailed directions as to the tempi, and indicated them (as I thought) accurately, by

means of the Metronome…In my later works I omitted the metronome and merely described the

main tempi in general terms, paying, however, particular attention to the various modifications of

tempo.” (Emphasis mine.) See Richard Wagner, On Conducting (Über das Dirigiren), translated

by Edward Dannreuther (London: William Reeves, 1897), 20-21.

In a letter dated August 16, 1850, Wagner reiterated to Franz Liszt that metronome

numbers did not supplant the prior, sensory understandings of musical time as actualized by

skilled performers—subjectivities not under the purview of composers’ intentions. In response to

Liszt’s request for additional metronomic references in certain passages of Lohengrin, Wagner

stated: “You ask me also for a few metronomical indications of the tempo. I consider this quite

unnecessary, because I rely in all things on your artistic sympathy so thoroughly as to know that

you need only be in a good humour with my work to find out the right thing everywhere; for the

right thing consists in this only: that the effect corresponds with the intention.”

Nevertheless, Wagner provided Liszt eight musical incipits with metronomic markings,

adding necessary verbal descriptions to achieve greater emotional and rhetorical nuance than ex

post facto numbers afforded. See Richard Wagner and Franz Liszt, Correspondence of Wagner

and Liszt, translated by Francis Hueffer, Vol. I (London: H. Grevel and Co., 1897), 83-85.

16 Schindler, 104-106; 111.

17 Ludwig van Beethoven, Beethoven's letters, a critical edition with explanatory notes by Dr. A.

C. Kalischer, translated with preface by J. S. Shedlock, Volume 2 (London: J. M. Dent; New

York: E. P. Dutton, 1909), 419.

Page 140: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

119

clear that Beethoven, late in life, found “metronoming” his own music to be a tedious,

complicated, and taxing process—entirely divorced from his compositional or

performance practices—when he wrote on May 20, 1826, “the metronome marks you

will receive in a week by post. It proceeds slowly, as my health requires care”18

and “as

soon as health permits me, you will also receive the metronome marks for the Mass” on

February 22, 1827.19

For Beethoven, perhaps the most prominent initial supporter of the

machine, metronomic indications never seemed to be intuitive, efficient, or easily

communicable measurements, even though he expressed their necessity in order for

musicians to better “follow the ideas of [his] unfettered genius.”20

As Schindler relates,

Beethoven eventually found the “metronoming [of his music] a mere ‘business’ matter,

and this view of the labor tended to increase his distaste for it.”21

Schindler concluded that the newly appearing metronome22

and its numbers were

directly antithetical to Beethoven’s deeper musical intentions: “Let Beethoven’s piano-

forte works be played according to the new metronomic directions, and it will soon be

perceived that no more opportunity is left for feeling and expression that the most rapid

18 See Beethoven, ed. Kalischer, 424.

19

Ibid., 465.

20

Ibid., 458. From a December 1826 letter to Bernhard Schotts Söhne in which Beethoven again

related the ex post facto process of metronoming his music: “The metronome marks will shortly

follow; do wait for them.”

21 Schindler, 116.

22 Beethoven attests to the relative obscurity of the machine within his lifetime when he wrote to

his nephew from Baden on September 14, 1824, “you could bring the metronome with you, it

can’t be got here.” See Beethoven, ed. Kalischer, 332.

This last phrase has occasionally been translated, “nothing can be done with it,”

suggesting that Beethoven had a metronome at home, but it was in disrepair. See for instance,

Beethoven's letters, 1790-1826, from the collection of L. Nohl. Also his letters to the archduke

Rudolph, translated by Lady Wallace, Vol. II (London: Longmans, Green, and Co., 1866), 168.

Page 141: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

120

fingering affords; and that this rule extends even to the execution of the Adagio.”23

What

Schindler and Beethoven perhaps found most deplorable in the metronome, being a

monotonous, invariable, and faulty machine, was its supreme artificiality when compared

to the complexity of human thought and expression. As this reception history of

clockwork machines demonstrates, Beethoven was only one in a long line of significant

critics who often rejected Maelzel’s artificial automata as soulless, self-moving objects

detached from the fundamental qualities of human spirit, intellect, and emotion.24

The World of Automata before Maelzel

Before Maelzel’s lifetime, sources attest that automata and mechanical time-tellers were

considered two sides of the same technology, since they both required clockwork to

function. For centuries, the most opulent public time-tellers featured ephemeral automata,

animated figures that adorned clocks for sheer visual delight and amusement. Yet the

term “automaton” had a twofold meaning; it could describe either these artificial

clockwork representations, or the clock itself. In a typical eighteenth-century definition,

any self-moving machine was an automaton, and “according to this description, clocks,

23 Schindler, 114.

24 Franz Liszt, writing the publishers Breitkopf und Härtel in 1863, reiterated Beethoven’s anti-

metronomic aesthetic. Recognizing that publishers’ commercial interests often stipulated the

incorporation of metronome numbers, Liszt recommended, “of the best of copyists it may be said

‘Better none,’ to use Beethoven’s words in pronouncing his verdict upon Maelzel’s metronome”

when publishing his piano scores of Beethoven’s nine symphonies. Nevertheless, Liszt, like many

nineteenth-century composers, often gave publishers full reign over the inclusion, and even

interpretation, of metronome indications. See Franz Liszt, Letters of Franz Liszt, collected and

edited by La Mara, translated by Constance Bache, Volume II (New York: Charles Scribner’s

Sons, 1894), 75.

Page 142: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

121

watches, and all machines of that kind, are automata.”25

Horology texts often used the

word “automaton” as a synonym for a clock, as exemplified in the title of John Ward’s

1714 watch-making treatise, A Practical Method To Discover the Longitude at Sea By a

New Contrived Automaton.26

Both time-tellers and representational automata were

species of clockwork. Thus, Western spectators often considered automata in the shape of

animals or humans not only to be similar to clocks, but also to be clocks personified.

Prior to the twentieth century, a host of sources described these clockwork

personifications in very specific contexts. Since automata were only mechanical

facsimiles—mere clockwork representations of man or beast—numerous writers defined

automata to be anti-human. René Descartes, in his Discourse on the Method (1637),

already noted the vast superiority and complexity of human actions over the artificial

limitations of automatical machines.27

In early nineteenth-century literature, too, the term

“automaton” consistently implied a figure with insufficient human qualities, an insult for

a person with lifeless characteristics or faulty intelligence. William Winstanley (fl. 1793–

1806), in his play The Hypocrite Unmask'd, considered an automaton to be a man

25 An historical miscellany of the curiosities and rarities in nature and art. Comprising new and

entertaining descriptions of the most surprising volcanos, caverns, cataracts, whirlpools, ... in

every part of the habitable world, vol. III (London: [1794-1800]), 281. Eighteenth Century

Collections Online. Gale.

http://find.galegroup.com/ecco/infomark.do?&contentSet=ECCOArticles&type=multipage&tabI

D=T001&prodId=ECCO&docId=CW101876105&source=gale&userGroupName=cwru_main&v

ersion=1.0&docLevel=FASCIMILE.

26

John Ward, A Practical Method To Discover the Longitude at Sea By a New Contrived

Automaton (London: James Woodward, 1714); See also John Imison, The school of arts; or, an

introduction to useful knowledge, being a compilation of real experiments and improvements, in

several pleasing branches of science… (London: printed for the author, and sold by J. Murray,

[1785?]), 215.

27

René Descartes, Discourse on the method of rightly conducting the reason, and seeking the

Truth in the Sciences, translated from the French (Edinburgh: Sutherland and Knox, 1850), 97.

Page 143: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

122

unmoved by nature or beauty, a hollow and expressionless figure.28

In his work A

Comedy, Entitled The Sprightly Widow (1803), American playwright John Minshull

confirmed the idea of the soulless, unfeeling machine:

But my good landlord—here—here—is her catalogue of

curiosities—If they don't move your risible faculties you are

an automaton. Ha! ha! ha! 29

The play Deaf And Dumb, Or The Abbe De L'Epee by Thomas Holcroft, (1745–1809)

described an automaton as a living imbecile, as one character chides, “you act remorse,

and feign this pity for / a thing—who, say the best, was but an idiot, an automaton.”30

Moreover, in his play Knave; or Not? (1798), Holcroft envisioned an automaton as a

figure lacking in will, manipulated through some form of external control, a living

puppet. As any viewer of a Maelzel exhibition could attest, a machine was always the

slave of its inventor. Holcroft’s character, in contrast, exclaims his humanity:

Peace! I will be no man's automaton:

I will hear no pleadings. I am the lord of my own

actions, and will be self-moved.31

28 “Well, Sir, as your interest seems to depend / much on his liberation, I will order his release; /

but pray Sir, what can have induced you / to place so much dependence on this piece of

automaton / buffoonery, or to expect any benefit from / his acting? He appears to me to possess

no one / single qualification for the stage.” See William Winstanley, The Hypocrite Unmask'd,

Act III (New-York: Geo. F. Hopkins, 1801). American Drama Full-Text Database.

http://ebooks.ohiolink.edu (accessed September 20, 2007).

29

John Minshull, A Comedy, Entitled the Sprightly Widow, Act III (New-York: Printed for the

Author, 1803.) American Drama Full-Text Database. http://ebooks.ohiolink.edu (accessed

September 20, 2007).

30

See Thomas Holcroft, Deaf and Dumb, or the Abbe de l'Epee, Act III (London, New

York: Samuel French, Samuel French & Son, 1801.) English Prose Drama Full-Text Database.

http://ebooks.ohiolink.edu (accessed September 20, 2007).

31

See Thomas Holcroft , Knave; or Not?, Act V (London: Printed for G. G. and J. Robinson

[etc.], 1798). English Prose Drama Full-Text Database. http://ebooks.ohiolink.edu (accessed

September 20, 2007).

Page 144: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

123

For many eighteenth- and early-nineteenth-century audiences, the clockwork facsimiles

of chirping birds, babbling children, concertizing musicians, and chess playing men,

allied these lifeless and soulless things with the craft of magic. Henri Decremps (1746-

1826), in his 1785 treatise The conjurer unmasked; or, la magie blanche dévoilée: being

a clear and full explanation of all the surprizing performances, described Mr. van Estin’s

Wunderkammer, which included a marvelous automatical organ that replicated a full

orchestra with “more precision in measure than you usually hear in instrumental

performances executed by common musicians.”32

The automatical-magical world

Decremps’ described was a direct precursor to the mechanical Wunderkammer Maelzel

established in early-nineteenth-century Vienna. Estin’s cabinet of curiosities also housed

a bird-machine that would imitate any tune it heard. During Decremps’ tour, a mysterious

mechanical chess player also made an appearance.33

After portraying the performances of

these automata in full, Decremps betrayed the secrets behind many of the more

spectacular illusions.

But even before Maelzel toured the continent with his machines, eventually to

arrive in a young nation, Americans also allied automata with illusion and magic. An

1805 treatise printed in Boston, Witchcraft, or the Art of Fortune-Telling Unveiled,

devoted an chapter to a “Reluminating Automaton” along with a metaphysical epistolary

of Kempelen’s chess player, the famed device Maelzel purchased in Vienna and

32 Henri Decremps, The conjurer unmasked; or, la magie blanche dévoilée: being a clear and full

explanation of all the surprizing performances exhibited ... (London: Printed for C. Stalker,

1785), 69.

33

Decremps, 97.

Page 145: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

124

exhibited in America two decades later.34

Automaton shows in American urban centers

were not uncommon prior to Maelzel’s 1825 arrival. As early as 1795, advertisements

appeared in Philadelphia touting “two artificial men, who are of the ordinary size of man,

perform feats of dexterity that surpass nature itself.”35

(Automata descriptions, as other

sources verify, regularly employed incredulous claims.) Broadsheets for a 1796 tavern

show in Providence printed the image of a “Chinese Automaton Figure” dancing on a

rope, triangle in hand, accompanied on one side by an ensemble of living musicians and

on the other by a barrel organ, an increasingly common mechanical instrument.36

While

Maelzel was not the first automata showman in America, he was the most successful, a

situation undoubtedly due to Maelzel’s three most significant talents: his

entrepreneurialism, ambition, and self-promotional skill. Through this powerful

combination of personal traits, Maelzel secured himself a reputation as mechanical

mastermind that lasted throughout the nineteenth century.

Skilled clock-makers and specialized court mechanics in the eighteenth century,

including the Hapsburgs’ own Wolfgang von Kempelen (1734-1804), gave rise to the

technology and display-culture of automata. Aside from their roles as utilitarian

engineers, these notable inventors fashioned reputations for themselves as mechanical

wizards and automata showmen. Besides Kempelen, clockwork mechanics including

34 William Frederick Pinchbeck, Witchcraft, or the Art of Fortune-Telling Unveiled (Boston:

Author, 1805), 42-46.

35

“A New entertainment, by Messrs. L'Égalité....” (Philadelphia: s.n., 1795), Early American

Imprints, 1st series, no. 29161 (filmed).

36

“Exhibitions, comic and experimental. At Mr. John Thurber's tavern, west side of the bridge.

This evening, for the first time, a Chinese automaton figure will perform several feats on the

rope...” (Providence: Printed by Bennett Wheeler, 1796), Early American Imprints, 1st series, no.

30298 (filmed).

Page 146: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

125

Jacques Vaucanson (1709-1782) and Henri-Louis Jaquet-Droz (1752-1791) devised

seemingly magical and miraculous automata displays that became significant curiosities

for privileged European audiences. Maelzel directly modeled his career on these

celebrated figures, surpassing his predecessors’ fame over the course of his adult life. By

visiting major cities in Europe and later North America, he introduced spectacular

displays of mechanical ingenuity to curious and astounded audiences on a global scale.

By the end of his picaresque life, Maelzel became the most renowned and influential

automata-showman of the nineteenth century.

Maelzel’s Mechanical Reputation

While Maelzel is known almost exclusively today as the inventor of the metronome, most

nineteenth-century sources attest that his fame and wealth were based on automata

exhibitions and sales, not his metronome enterprise.37

Late nineteenth-century music

scholar Henry Edward Krehbiel noted that Maelzel “as [he had] in Europe…depended for

a livelihood on exhibitions of his mechanical contrivances.”38

Moreover, Maelzel’s

notoriety was not due to his inventiveness as much as his entrepreneurialism and

showmanship. Indeed, numerous writers contest Maelzel’s actual mechanical skill, and

serious doubts still remain as to whether he invented any machines at all. Historian

Alexander Buchner has suggested that his brother Leonard Maelzel might have devised

37 Metzner, 183, 185, 186.

38

Henry Edward Krehbiel, Music and Manners in the Classical Period: Essays, Second Edition

(New York: Scribner & sons, 1898), 233. For a contemporaneous account of Maelzel’s

entrepreneurialism and wealth, see Karl Berhard, Travels through North America, During the

Years 1825 and 1826, Vol. 1 (Philadelphia: Carey, Lea &Carey, 1828), 197-8. Bernhard, “Duke

of Saxe-Weimer Eisenach,” failed to mention the metronome in his memoir of Maelzel.

Page 147: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

126

many of the early automata that Johann exhibited throughout Europe.39

And just as

Johann co-opted Winkel’s metronome, Maelzel merely assumed, copied, or bought many

of the renowned automata that became allied with his name. Maelzel’s most perplexing

automaton, the chess player known as the “Turk,” was purchased from Hapsburg

engineer Wolfgang von Kempelen, after the older inventor found the android too taxing

to display.40

By 1810, the Kaufman family of automata inventors had constructed a

similar if not identical android trumpeter.41

The Panharmonicon, Maelzel’s massive

mechanical orchestra, was only a more ostentatious and overwhelming barrel organ that

Leonard possibly built in 1804, a machine C. G. Hamilton likened to “a mammoth music

box.”42

Undoubtedly, American general-interest periodicals helped to establish Maelzel’s

fame as ingenious inventor during his lifetime. An 1827 North American article on the

history of Kempelen’s automaton chess player detailed the latest handler’s

accomplishments:

39 Alexander Buchner, Mechanical Musical Instruments (London: Batchworth Press [n.d.]), 79.

40

See Metzner, 183. Unlike Buchner, Metzner does not mention Leonard as an important

influence in Johann’s career.

41

See Buchner, 80, plates 49-52. Buchner states that the Kaufmann family took their automaton

trumpeter on tour between 1810-1812. Maelzel exhibited his trumpeter in Vienna by 1909

through 1813.

42 See Clarence G. Hamilton, How to Use the Metronome Correctly (Philadelphia: Theodore

Presser Co., 1916), 5.

Page 148: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

127

Mr. Maelzel has been for many years distinguished for his great mechanical skill.

The Panharmonicon, which formerly exhibited here, was made by him [unlike the

chess player]; he is likewise the inventor of the Metronome, an instrument by

which the time of music is accurately measured; it is not unknown here, and is

extensively used in Europe. He has also invented an apparatus which is attached

to a Piano Forte, by which any piece of music which is played on it, is at the same

time correctly written out. His speaking figures are of his own make and far excel

the attempts of Von Kempelen [the inventor of the chess player], although the

labours of the latter, were eminently successful.43

Placing Maelzel in the grand tradition of mechanical showmen, the writer clearly makes a

distinction between the automata that Maelzel truly invented and those he co-opted. Yet,

the reviewer seemed uninformed about the metronome itself, or its origins as a creation

of Dutch clockmaker Dietrich Winkel.44

The writer confides that the metronome was

little used in North America, but reports on “its extensive use on the continent.”

According to this early biography, the metronome was Maelzel’s least recognized

machine.

By the end of Maelzel’s life, his reputation as mechanical wizard grew seemingly

to mythical proportions. In February 1837, The Magazine of Useful and Entertaining

Knowledge considered all of his clockwork creations. In this literary cabinet of Maelzel’s

curiosities, the metronome accounted for one small portion of his ingenuity. The article

noted:

43 “Automaton Chess Player,” North American. Or, Weekly Journal of Politics, Science and

Literature, May 19, 1827, 1.

44

The history of Maelzel’s reported thievery of Winkel’s metronome is out of scope for the

present survey, but it is well represented in many nineteenth-century music references and

Beethoven biographies.

Page 149: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

128

Some of the most beautiful and splendid of automatic machines are the creations

of his genius… Of his inventions the following may be enumerated: viz.—The

automaton speaking figures, which articulate certain English and French words, in

the hands of any person; the panharmonicon-a magnificent instrument, composer

of all the pieces, rich, various and powerful as the are, of an entire military band;

the animated diorama of the cathedral at Rheims—a large and most superb

representation of the kind; the automaton trumpeter, of the size of a man; and

whose clarion notes cannot be equaled by those of any living performer—the time

from the nature of the mechanism, being absolutely perfect; the equestrian

automata, and the automatous slack rope dancers, which go through all the

difficult feats and surprising evolutions, both on ‘horse and foot’ of the circus and

amphitheaters, and with an agility, ease and gracefulness, so true to nature, as

scarcely to be credited by those who have not witnessed them; the melodium,

whose very name attests how exquisitely it is attuned to ‘sweet sounds;’ the

automaton charlatan, never exhibited, we believe, in America; the metronome, or

musical time-keeper, patented in Europe; and ‘last not least,’ that unique and most

masterly combination of music, mechanism and design, the grand and appalling

panoramic spectacle of the conflagration of Moscow.45

In this sprawling inventory, one can see Maelzel’s automatical culture in earnest: the

reader beholds the vision of the natural actions of man and beast reproduced in

clockwork. Speaking children, blazing trumpets, trotting horses, balancing acrobats, and

even burning cities, all contrived in mechanical facsimiles. As the reports describe,

Maelzel’s creations represented animals, people, and places of natural and civilized

world, reduced to artificial splendor.

When detailing these wonders, contemporaneous writers considered the

metronome to be a negligible contribution to Maelzel’s oeuvre. The device was eclipsed

in a list populated by Maelzel’s more wondrous mechanical entertainments. A brief

pedigree of the metronome—“patented in Europe”—again belied its unfamiliarity in

North America in 1837. The American public remained relatively unfamiliar with the

tempo-machine during his life for one significant reason: Maelzel never established a

45 “John Maelzel,” The American magazine of useful and entertaining knowledge, Feb. 1, 1837,

196-7.

Page 150: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

129

metronome factory in the United States as he had in France or England. Even after

Maelzel’s lifetime, sources attested to the continued scarcity of the device on the new

continent. An 1840 article in The Musical Magazine suggested, “the Metronome is a very

desirable thing, but it is at the same time rare in this country and very expensive.”46

As

late as 1874, while the American watch industry was in the midst of a boom,47

a Scientific

American reader inquired why no manufacturers of the metronome existed in America.48

Evidence of North American metronome companies only appear at the end of the

nineteenth century, when in 1897, an advertisement in the Musical Visitor announced the

John Church Company as the new national competitor within a market of expensive,

foreign-made metronomes.49

While American sources began to express greater familiarity with the metronome

by the middle of the century, Maelzel’s reputation was not defined by any single

automatical invention. For instance, a survey of various automata makers appeared in an

1852 article “Wonderful Toys” from The North American Miscellany and Dollar

46 “Substitute for Maelzel’s Metronome,” The Musical Magazine; or, Repository of Musical

Science, Literature and...Feb. 29, 1840, 67.

47

Landes, 344-5.

48

Scientific American, Feb. 21, 1874, 123. The suggestion was made to use Weber’s simple

pendulum as substitute for the clockwork metronome.

49

See Music Trades, “Metronomes,” The Musical Visitor 26 (Dec. 1, 1897): 335. “The John

Church Company lay claim to the distinction of being the first manufacturers of metronomes to

guarantee the little musical accessory as absolutely correct and perfect. The foreign-made

metronome seems doomed as the Church Company is making vast quantities of them, and at a

price below what the importer must pay for the foreign article without the guarantee. An

absolutely correct metronome is a necessity to every musician, but a poor one is about as useful

as wire strings on the bass drum.”

Despite this advertisement, enterprising American inventors continued to patent

alternative clockwork and electric metronomes during the late nineteenth century. Also, simple

pendulums called “metronomes” were widely disseminated through music periodicals and

publishers.

Page 151: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

130

Magazine, which praised Maelzel’s automaton trumpeter as being more skilled than any

living trumpeter of the age. The Panharmonicon, as this source suggests, was proof that

“a full orchestra of clock-work musicians is quite possible.”50

After mid-century, Maelzel

and his automatical machines were often discussed as precursors to a new, mechanically

inspired civilization. An Atlantic Monthly article entitled “The Human Wheel, its Spokes

and Felloes” from 1863 recalled Maelzel exclusively for his speaking automata. The

writer Oliver Wendell Holmes, who alluded to Maelzel’s works throughout the century,

championed the growing mechanical culture in America. Holmes imagined a future

where automata could imitate all aspects of humanity, including the ability to speak. “To

make a machine that articulates is not so easy,” Holmes said, “but we remember

Maelzel's wooden children.” In Maelzel’s automata, Holmes found the first successful

attempts to mimic life in mechanical form.51

Ironically, nearing the end of his own life in

50 See “Wonderful Toys,” The North American Miscellany and Dollar Magazine, Feb. 1, 1852,

103: “Maelzel, the inventor of the metronome, opened an exhibition in Vienna, in 1809, in which

an automaton Trumpeter as large as life, performed with surprising accuracy and power [after

being wound-up by its inventor…accompanied by a full band of living musicians…In the Journal

des Modes, whence this account is derived, it is declared that the tones produced by Maelzel’s

automaton were even fuller and richer than those got out of a trumpet by human lungs and lips;

because man’s breath imparts to the inside of the instrument a moisture which deteriorates the

quality of the tone.”

51

See Atlantic Monthly, Volume 11, Issue 67, May 1863. Gutenberg eBook #13026,

http://www.gutenberg.org (accessed September 20, 2007): “Talking seems the hardest to

comprehend [of all human action]. Yet it has been clearly explained and successfully imitated by

artificial contrivances…To whisper is to articulate without bleating, or vocalizing; to coo as

babies do is to bleat or vocalize without articulating. Machines are easily made that bleat not

unlike human beings…To make a machine that articulates is not so easy; but we remember

Maelzel's wooden children, which said, ‘Pa-pa’ and ‘Ma-ma’; and more elaborate and successful

speaking machines have, we believe, been since constructed.” Many reprints of this article

appeared through the end of the century.

In his Reminiscences, Benjamin Perley Poore describes Maelzel’s speaking dolls as

“small figures, when their hands were shaken, ejaculated the words, ‘Papa!’ and ‘Mamma!’ in a

life-like manner.” Gutenberg eBook #20290, http://www.gutenberg.org (accessed September 20,

2007).

Page 152: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

131

the 1890s, Holmes felt himself automatically imitating Maelzel’s decrepit, creaking, and

long-since departed chess player.52

A revised, posthumous image of Maelzel appeared in an 1855 article entitled

“Automata,” published in Ladies’ Wreath, a Magazine devoted to Literature, Industry

and Religion. The writer Annie Parker also espoused a new nineteenth-century

mechanical world. Unlike Holmes, she considered Maelzel’s most unassuming

clockwork invention, the metronome, to be of primary importance. Explaining why the

metronome surpassed Maelzel’s other machines in this emerging technological culture,

she drew the link between utilitarian devices of the nineteenth century and their

wondrous artificial precursors, stating that “Machinery and the mechanic arts in our day,

have been brought to such a wonderful degree of perfection that we are apt to overlook

the small beginnings from which this perfection sprung [sic.]. Among the causes which

have led to it, the making of automata is not the least important. In this utilitarian age, an

invention that does not serve some immediate purpose of beauty or use, is pretty sure to

be overlooked or forgotten.”53

According to Parker, Maelzel helped to found the modern,

52 Expressing a significant theme that recurs throughout this study, Holmes compared unthinking,

habitual behaviors to the actions of Maelzel’s automata, stating, “Every stage of existence has its

special trials and its special consolations. Habits are the crutches of old age; by the aid of these

we manage to hobble along after the mental joints are stiff and the muscles rheumatic, to speak

metaphorically,--that is to say, when every act of self-determination costs an effort and a pang.

We become more and more automatic as we grow older, and if we lived long enough we should

come to be pieces of creaking machinery like Maelzel's chess player,—or what that seemed to

be.” See Oliver Wendell Holmes, Sr., Over the Teacups (Cambridge: Riverside Press, 1891): 38.

53

See Annie Parker, “Automata,” Ladies’ Wreath, a Magazine devoted to Literature Industry and

Religion, Oct. 5, 1855, 360-362. She continues, “Early in the present century, Leonard [sic.]

Maelzel astonished the Parisians by the exhibition of his celebrated Panharmonicon [...] Maelzel

is not known merely as the inventor of this curious instrument. Had he given the world no more

useful proofs of his genius, he might well be forgotten. But he did great service to musical

science by the invention of the metronome. It is for this he is remembered and classed with the

most ingenious mechanicians of the century.”

Page 153: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

132

mechanical age. She argued that while his other trivial automata fell into obscurity, the

metronome survived for the utilitarian good of the new mechanically oriented society.

After describing the defunct Panharmonicon and its repertoire, she asserts that Maelzel

will be “remembered and classed with the most ingenious mechanicians of the century.”54

It is highly probable that Parker referenced the book Illustrious Mechanics of

Europe and America by the Frenchman Edward Foucaud. Maelzel’s biography from

Illustrious Mechanics, which appeared in an 1847 English edition, seems entirely

paraphrased by Parker. In Foucaud’s grandiose estimation, the inventor was destined for

the immortal annals of science due to his only “useful invention.” Yet Foucaud fully

acknowledged Maelzel’s renowned entertainment career, stating,

Such are Maelzel’s claims to the great reputation his name has obtained. But, to

speak the truth, if he had made nothing but automata, however ingenious and

curious the mystery of their construction, he would be now almost

forgotten…This species of curious ingenuity is not alone sufficient to immortalize

a man. But on the other hand, however simple a mechanism may be, if it is useful

to mankind, it is sure to perpetuate the inventor’s name, unless it originated in a

barbarous age…Maelzel will remain immortal through his metronome; even if

musical science should ever be entirely overturned by one of those revolutions

which sometimes take place in the regions influenced by the human mind, his

name will always maintain a place in the annals of general science.55

Parker seems ignorant of the true internal mechanisms comprising the Panharmonicon

and also confuses the Maelzel brothers. Leonard, too, was an automaton builder, but did not

travel as extensively as Johann. Alexander Buchner states, “The brothers are therefore very often

confused even in specialist works on the subject. It is in fact difficult to say exactly where the

work of one ends and the other begins.” See Buchner, 79.

54

Parker, 362.

55

Edward Foucaud, The Book of Illustrious Mechanics of Europe and America. Translated from

the French by John Frost (New York: Appelton & Co, 1847), 184-5. From Foucaud, Les artisans

illustres (Paris: Béthune et Plon, 1841). With more research, it may be argued that this French

biography was intended to stimulate sales of metronomes, which were primarily manufactured in

Paris during the time of this account.

Page 154: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

133

For some twentieth-century scholars of technology, the emerging value placed upon

useful inventions signaled the death of automata creation and display.56

As machines

became mundane parts of everyday life, the novelty once evoked in their public

exhibition waned. Clockwork automata, once mysterious spectacles, ceased to fire the

imagination in a world of fast trains, readily attainable pocket watches, household

musical-clocks, and powerful factory machines. Yet for some, Maelzel made this new,

mechanically aided civilization possible.

Despite Foucaud’s and Parker’s assertions, Maelzel continued to be recognized as

a mechanical showman and master illusionist throughout the nineteenth century. Circus

impresario P. T. Barnum recounted the encouragement he received from Maelzel, a

shrewd businessman who was for the young, impressionable Barnum, “the great father of

caterers for public amusement.”57

Likewise, Alexander Wheelock Thayer recalled

Maelzel in The Life of Beethoven:

The mechanician was not only a man of unquestionable inventive genius, but he

also understood the public; knew as by instinct how to excite and gratify curiosity

without disappointing expectation, and had the tact and skill so to arrange his

exhibitions as to dismiss his visitors grateful for an amusement for which they had

paid. He was personally both respected and popular.58

56 Paul Metzner charts the decline of automata culture later in history. He explains the early

twentieth century attitude that “Engineers have been less interested in how well a machine

imitates a human being than in how well it performs a certain function that human beings have

been accustomed to performing…People expect an electrically powered machine to accomplish

something, and admire it on the basis of what it accomplishes. They do not admire it any more if

in accomplishing its task it also looks or acts like a human being.” See Metzner, 210. The above

quotes suggest that this attitude towards mechanical utility began much earlier in the course of

both French and American modernity, during a self-dubbed “utilitarian age.”

57

See P. T. Barnum, The Autobiography of P. T. Barnum, Second edition (London: Ward and

Lock, 1855), 58. Recounted in Metzner, 186.

58

Alexander Wheelock Thayer, The life of Ludwig van Beethoven, edited, revised and amended

from the original English manuscript and the German editions of Hermann Deiters and Hugo

Riemann, concluded, and all the documents newly translated, by Henry Edward Krehbiel, Vol. 2

(New York: The Beethoven Association [1921]), 252. Lending credence to Thayer’s assessment,

Page 155: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

134

In contrast, John Delafield, in his study Mysticism and its Results; Being an Inquiry into

the Uses and Abuses of Secrecy, invoked Maelzel’s name with the history of public

deception. In Delafield’s memory, Maelzel remained an illusionist, a trickster for profit—

a figure that betrayed the intentions of a true mechanical scientist. Nowhere was the

“utilitarian good” of Maelzel’s metronome praised in Delafield’s 1857 commentary:

We arrive then at nearly the culminating point of Egyptian priestcraft, the days of

“wise men,” “sorcerers,” and “magicians.” Such men ever have, and we presume

ever will employ secrecy as the chief element of their clever jugglery…Maelzel's

automata, or Vaucanson's duck, will attract the world, when Bacon's, or Newton's,

or Laplace's works may remain in dust on the book-shelves. Human nature is

always the same, and thus it was in the days of Moses and Pharaoh. The wise

men, sorcerers, and magicians, held undisputed sway, not only over the

superstitions of the people, but over their educated monarchs and princes.59

Delafield perhaps recounted Maelzel’s exhibitions of the famed chess-playing Turk. Like

Kempelen before him, Maelzel hid breathing, thinking human being within a mechanical

frame, passing off a living player for an artificial masterwork. For almost a century, the

chess player represented an automatical illusion par excellence, recounted in diverse

sources including chess histories, fictions, a laudatory poem,60

journal articles (the most

Maelzel’s British contemporary Francis Joseph Grund recollected: “Mr. Maelzel, who exhibited

these wonders, was a very agreeable man, who, with a good-natured German smile always

reserved the first benches for the children, and regularly pampered them with sugar-plums. There

was, besides, mechanical ingenuity in the performance; and a problem to solve, which is always

interesting to Americans.” See Francis Joseph Grund, The Americans in their Moral, Social, and

Political Relations (Boston: Marsh, Capen, and Lyon, 1837), 78.

59

John Delafield, Mysticism and its Results; Being an Inquiry into the Uses and Abuses of

Secrecy (Saint Louis: Edwards & Bushnell, 1857), Gutenberg eBook #22314,

http://www.gutenberg.org (accessed September 20, 2007.)

60

See Hanna F. Gould, “To the Automaton Chess Player,” in Landmark Anthologies: Specimens

of American poetry (Boston: S. G. Goodrich & Co, [1829]). American Poetry Full-Text Database.

http://ebooks.ohiolink.edu (accessed September 20, 2007).

Page 156: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

135

famous of which was penned by a young Edgar Allen Poe),61

and even a memoir of

Napoleon.62

Historians, journalists, and critics remembered Maelzel throughout the century for

his many machines and methods; not only for wonderful automata and useful

metronomes, but also for his illusory performances and grand public deceptions. In his

memoirs, William Wirt, who served as Attorney General of the United States from 1817

to 1829, depicted Maelzel as both a complete shyster and a natural born orator, someone

who could convince the public of anything. Like Delafield, Wirt has no comment on

Maelzel’s scientific contributions to the future of society. Wirt infers, however, that

Maelzel would have made an exceptional politician, reflecting that

Everybody talks sense now-a-days. But how many are there who can talk

successful folly and gain the reputation of wisdom by it? That is a species of

mental legerdemain which puts a man on a level with the far-famed Maelzel, the

exhibitor of the Androides, compared with whom the Chief-Justice [John

Marshall] himself is but an everyday sort of man.63

61 Edgar Allen Poe, “Maelzel’s Chess Player,” Southern Literary Messenger II (1836): 318-326.

Many scholars consider Poe’s article to be his nascent attempt to establish the detective-story

genre.

62

Constant, Premier Valet de Chambre, Recollections of the Private Life of Napoleon, Vol. II,

trans. by Walter Clark. (Akron: Saalfield Publishing Co., 1907), 1895. “The Emperor made two

or three moves, and intentionally made a wrong one. The automaton bowed, took the piece, and

put it in its proper place. His Majesty cheated a second time; the automaton bowed again, and

took the piece. ‘That is right,’ said the Emperor; and when he cheated a third time, the automaton,

passing his hand over the chess-board, spoiled the game.” Also noted in Thayer, ed. Krehbiel, The

Life of Beethoven, Vol. II, 232.

63

John P. Kennedy, Memoirs of the Life of William Wirt, Volume II (Philadelphia: Lea and

Blanchard, 1850), 201.

Addressing Congress on October 11, 1837, Virginia representative John Robertson

compared an entire body of the U. S. legislative to Maelzel’s machines. Using the prevailing

automatical metaphors, Robertson railed, “Those who desert their own principles, and act in

opposition to their own judgments, are slaves, mere puppets, moved by the will of another.

Maelzel could construct a House of Representatives as fit to exercise the functions of

legislators—yes, sir, speaker, orators, and all.” See Register of Debates in Congress, volume XIV

(Washington: Gales and Seaton, 1837), col 1441.

Page 157: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

136

As nineteenth-century documents attest, Maelzel’s image was nothing if not protean. The

1884 and 1911 editions of the Encyclopedia Britannica unapologetically labeled Maelzel

an “imposter” for his sordid history with Winkel’s metronome.64

The Grove Dictionary

of Music and Musicians in 1889 posited that “Maelzel was evidently a sharp, shrewd,

clever man of business, with a strong propensity to use the ideas of others for his own

benefit,”65

while C. G. Hamilton, as late as 1916, likewise ascertained that Maelzel

“possessed a remarkable combination of inventive genius, business ability, and readiness

to appropriate unscrupulously to his own use the products of others’ brains.”66

Maelzel’s

obituary appearing in the Athenaeum perhaps treated him most harshly, concluding of his

life:

As a man, Maelzel seems to have been quarrelsome, extravagant, and

unscrupulous. He can only be ranked amongst those empirics whose cleverness

almost amounts to genius. Had he possessed a larger amount of culture and of

conscience, he might have done service to high Art.67

Regardless of Maelzel’s many posthumous visages, his death in 1838 signaled the true

downfall of automata-display. Maelzel’s self-fashioned career was singular in nineteenth-

century culture, and while other mechanical-showmen flourished for a time, no exhibitor

64 See The Encyclopedia Britannica. A Dictionary of Arts, Sciences, and General Literature,

Ninth Edition, American Reprint, (Philadelphia: J.M. Stoddart Co., Limited, 1884), XVI, 207. It

reads, “When the imposter revisited Amsterdam, the inventor [Winkel] instituted proceedings

against him for his piracy…” This article is reprinted verbatim at least through the 1911 edition of

the encyclopedia.

65

“Maelzel,” A Dictionary of Music and Musicians, edited by George Grove, D.C.L, Volume II

(London: Macmillan and co., 1880-9), 194-5.

66

See Hamilton, 5.

67

Reprinted in John Timbs, A Year-book of Facts in Science and Art (London: David Bogue,

1856), 94. The 1855 obituary is mostly likely confusing Leonard Maelzel with Johann, who died

in 1838.

Page 158: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

137

resonated in the public’s imagination like Maelzel. Barnum and other entertainers would

employ some of his methods, but Maelzel’s brand of performance-spectacle was never

again repeated. As few heirs to his career followed, Maelzel’s personal collection of

machines were sold, dispersed, neglected, or destroyed. Without a master, a venue, or an

accompanying orchestra, his famed automaton trumpeter no longer sounded. A fire that

gutted Philadelphia’s Chinese Museum in 1854 relegated the mysterious chess player to

the status of legend.68

Decimated in WWII, Maelzel’s gargantuan Panharmonicon—

always too cumbersome for traveling exhibitions—survived in another guise; his

mechanical orchestra was the precursor to the ubiquitous and utilitarian orchestrion,

which found a welcome place by the latter part of the nineteenth century in taverns,

department stores, amusement parks, and luxurious homes alike. While Maelzel’s

complex and non-duplicable clockwork inventions fell into obscurity, the metronome

continued to garner acceptance in the nineteenth century as a tool for children’s music

education. Ironically, Maelzel’s smallest, least spectacular, and least profitable automaton

in his lifetime became his most influential after his death, and today many recognize him

for this device exclusively.

While twentieth-century performers and researchers seemed to forget Maelzel and

his mechanical culture, sources in his own century attested to his unique influence:

Maelzel represented a culture of artificiality based on mechanical promotion and

exhibition, and many musicians and intellectuals from this industrializing civilization

recognized the common qualities that underlie all of Maelzel’s machines. Many in this

past Western culture—those who prized human volition, variability, and expression

above mechanical imitation, replication, and precision—perceived the clockwork

68 Metzner, 188.

Page 159: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

138

metronome similarly to all self-moving automata. They understood that Maelzel’s most

lasting machine also lacked the innate, volatile qualities of action and intellect that

comprised the very essence of humanity.

Maelzel’s Automaton Trumpeter and Panharmonicon

In eighteenth- and nineteenth-century literature, automata were invariably portrayed as

thoughtless, passionless figures. Not surprisingly then, nineteenth-century cultural critics

employed Maelzel’s famed automaton trumpeter in similar conceits. The performance

and history of the machine is well documented. In 1813, it débuted as soloist in high-

profile benefit concerts that included Beethoven, Hummel, Sphor, and other Viennese

musical greats. This hybrid man-machine concert, which included Beethoven’s “Battle

Symphony” (a work originally intended for Maelzel’s Panharmonicon), was so profitable

that Maelzel and the trumpeter traveled across Europe, replicating the performance-

spectacle with other live orchestras.

During the trumpeter’s premiere performances in London, listeners recognized

both Maelzel’s mechanical ingenuity alongside the machine’s musical failings.

According to the Times review printed in September 1818, the trumpeter did not have the

nuanced musical sense customarily heard in a human performance. With knowledge of

Maelzel’s reputation as “inventor of the metronome,” the British reviewer proclaimed of

Maelzel’s “automaton figure:”

Nothing can exceed the accuracy and neatness of the execution, or the steadiness

of the tone: in the rapidity with which the same note may be repeated in

succession, and in some passages of a similar nature, it surpasses the powers of

any living trumpeter; it fails only in expression, and in the swell of the note, a

defect which is common to all music produced by mechanism.69

69 The Times, Sept. 23, 1818, 2.

Page 160: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

139

The fact that the automaton was expressionless is immaterial to this reviewer, since

musical clocks and barrel organs regularly exposed the failings of machine-music to the

whole of English society. It was common knowledge that mechanical music was always

defective when compared to a human performance; any automaton, however spectacular,

“fails only in expression.” Among its deficiencies, the trumpeter could not dynamically

shape notes or phrases, which the English called “swell,” a practice that living performers

intuited without notated indications, at least since the time of Caccini.

The automaton’s repertoire, which comprised a finite selection of marches and

fanfares, did not require any interpretive or emotional complexity. The trumpeter played

strict military music with mechanical precision, steadiness, power, and speed. Not

surprisingly, it excelled in automatical qualities only. The trumpeter was, nevertheless, an

uncanny spectacle, and Maelzel’s London audience enthusiastically requested an encore.

What was astonishing to the reporter was that “After the ceremony of winding up [by

Maelzel], he [the automaton] complied with the wishes of the company present, and

repeated his elaborate performances without moving a muscle.”70

As Maelzel’s trumpeter crossed the Atlantic, so did its reputation as a musically

deficient mechanism. Accounts spanning the century attested that the once-amazing

automaton gradually lost its luster. With only the artificiality of Maelzel’s machine in

mind, the lawyer, diarist, and amateur musician George Templeton Strong (1820-1875)

expressed his distain for a human performer on December 30, 1842:

70 Ibid.

Page 161: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

140

As to Mr. Charles Braham, his voice is good and he manages it well, but

Maelzel’s automaton trumpeter has full as much expression. He looks as if he

were some great piece of clockwork wound up before the commencement of the

concert, and made to work itself into the room and emit musical sounds and then

stalk out again at intervals.71

For Strong, Maelzel’s automaton was the emblem for all that was unmusical. In a

germane comparison, Strong perceived that any lifeless human performer behaved and

sounded merely as “some great piece of clockwork,” a quality that bore a close

relationship to Maelzel’s musical timekeeper. The term later critics in the century would

use for this artificial quality was “metronomic.”72

The trumpeter was only one of Maelzel’s machines that analogized lifeless music

making in the nineteenth century. Like the automaton trumpet player, the

Panharmonicon was often praised as an automaton, but criticized as being insufficient

when compared to living musicians. While Maelzel’s trumpeter was an artificial

facsimile of one military musician, the Panharmonicon was the mechanical facsimile of

an entire military orchestra, replete with woodwind, brass, and percussion sections.

Thayer faithfully described the mechanics of the Panharmonicon as it originally appeared

71 Vera Brodsky Lawrence, Strong on Music, Volume I (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1988),

161.

Writer Edward Walter Hamilton, in his “monograph” Mr. Gladstone, commented upon

his subject’s skill as an orator, which bares striking similarity with Strong’s comment of Braham.

Hamilton described, “The pace at which he spoke was an even one. He could have spoken to a

metronome, though he had one pace for the House of Commons and another pace for the

platform. There was never a pause for want of an expression.” See Edward Walter Hamilton, Mr.

Gladstone (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, 1898), 4.

72

Other cultural critics analogized the automaton trumpeter as a pejorative throughout the

century. As late as 1862, a reviewer for the Atlantic Monthly criticized James Fenimore Cooper’s

narrative style, commenting, “[George] Washington, as shown to us in ‘The Spy,’ is a formal

piece of mechanism, as destitute of vital character as Maelzel’s automaton trumpeter.” See The

Atlantic Monthly 9 (January, 1862), Gutenberg eBook #13924, http://www.gutenberg.org

(accessed September 20, 2007).

Page 162: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

141

in the Viennese Wunderkammer of Maelzel.73

Long after the Maelzel’s European

displays, however, an American Panharmonicon show received a scathing critique in the

Western Recorder from 1825. The reporter first mocks an advertisement describing the

mechanism:

“This wonderful instrument is exhibited every evening at Knickerbocker Hall,

Albany. It performs by means of certain mechanical arrangements upon 206

musical instruments. Of these there are thirteen different kinds,” &c. So says the

Albany Daily Advertiser. How supremely ridiculous are all such pretentions!!—

Let the first novelty wear off, and it will be seen by every one, that the whole is a

mere burlesque on the very name of musical execution. Every man who wishes

well to music, ought to be indignant against such pretentions.74

This critic contended that the Panharmonicon, while novel, was in no sense musical.

Another overly enthusiastic description of the Panharmonicon appeared in Maelzel’s

obituary notice in the Gazette Musicale. The French article described that the machine

was exceptional for its “expressive play,” “extreme agility,” and “graceful manner.”

Writing for The Musical Magazine in 1839, a highly skeptical American reviewer

reported that the obituary reflected “whimsical specimens of French credulity, and are not

solitary indications of the degree of confidence that is to be placed in the matter-of-fact

statements of that paper.”75

The American reviewer distrusted both the specific

description of the Panharmonicon and the entire journal for personifying ad ridiculum

such an obviously artificial object, one that imitated a military band no less. If the

73 Thayer, ed. Krehbiel, II, 251. “The Panharmonicon combined the common instruments then

employed in military bands, with a powerful bellows—the whole thing being enclosed in a case.

The motive power was automatic and the keys were touched by pins fixed in a revolving cylinder,

as in the common hand-organ or music-box.”

74

“Panharmonicon,” Western Recorder, Aug. 23, 1825, 136.

75

“THE VIOLIN. Hogarth's Musical History,” The Musical Magazine; or, Repository of Musical

Science, Literature... Aug. 17, 1839, 270.

Page 163: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

142

writer’s opinion in the Western Recorder held true, the Panharmonicon was a “mere

burlesque on the very name of musical execution,” and although promotional material

would claim otherwise, the automaton was a machine bereft of any natural grace or

expression.

Audiences of the time could perceive that all of Maelzel’s machines, however

remarkable, were imposters of the thing itself; no automaton could replicate the living

qualities of human musicians. The difference was obvious for one New York Review

writer in 1838. Describing the ineffable artistry and subtle touch required to play the

piano, he exclaimed, “Compared with this, how dead, how destitute of interest is

mechanical music, even the wondrous melodium of Maelzel!”76

The Metronome as an Extension of Automatical Values

Like all automata, the metronome was a product of clockwork. Thus we find an early

nineteenth-century description of metronomic motion that seems to evoke the artificiality

of Maelzel’s other machines. The British periodical The Albion from 1829 printed a

sardonic review of a questionable, amateur house-concert where the “elderly ladies, in

particular, were enthusiastic…Their heads and fans had often, during the performance,

covibrated, like Maelzel’s Metronomes, with the rhythm of the livelier passages, which

seemed to call forth the remembrance of livelier days.”77

The writer portrayed the usually

placid and possibly oblivious spectators behaving very strangely for 1829 standards,

moving as if they were automatical machines. Such a depiction was unique, if not

76 [Book Review] “Gardiner’s Music of Nature,” The New York Review V (July, 1838): 46.

77

“Musical Outlines,” The Albion, A Journal of News, Politics and Literature, May 9, 1829, 384.

Page 164: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

143

obscure, as this writer clearly wished to amuse with witty analogies based on the latest,

fashionable technologies. Extending his mechanical metaphors, the reporter then

compared the two misguided recitalists to compensating balance-springs in the new

Breguet pocket watch.78

More practical comments regarding metronomic motion appeared in the following

decade in English-language sources. In these early critiques, we find that the metronome

was a tool that informed students not only with a tempo reference, but also with an

undesirable standard of automatical behavior. While interest in the clockwork metronome

was growing, some educators found that the machine caused unforeseen problems when

used constantly in practice. In The Zodiac, a Monthly Periodical, Devoted to Science,

Literature, and the Art from 1835, an anonymous writer suggested that reliance on the

machine created mental and physical issues for the inexperienced student, stating that “an

occasional recourse to a metronome will be a very great help to you, if you have one

within your reach; but the too frequent use of it will make you feel ill at ease when you

have to play without it.”79

Early in the nineteenth century, this writer acknowledged a

psychological crutch imposed by a new, and still unfamiliar, clockwork reference. His

comment was not an isolated critique of the machine—it was a standard assessment,

expressed throughout the century, of the noticeable drawbacks in educating with the

metronome.

78 Ibid., “Like a peculiar watch of Breguet’s invention, in which two distinct balances force each

other into simultaneous vibration so did these sisterly pair…”

79

“Musical Instruction,” The Zodiac, a Monthly Periodical, Devoted to Science, Literature, and

the Art...Dec. 1835, 86.

Page 165: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

144

Perhaps the most prominent early critic of the machine was none other than

Beethoven’s contemporary Johann Nepomuk Hummel (1778-1837). And while

Hummel’s 1828 pedagogical publication Ausführlich theoretisch-practische Anweisung

zum Piano-forte Spiel presented the clockwork metronome very early on to students—

through Maelzel’s own promotional language—the composer-performer Hummel

strongly warned musical students against the artificial rhythm imbued by the metronome.

As late as 1861, periodicals such as Dwight’s Journal of Music and the Saturday Evening

Post translated Hummel’s important caveats against the machine:

If one could play a piece through correctly, attending to the beating of a

metronome, which is scarcely possible, if he possesses any nervous sensibility,

the performance must necessarily bear a cold, hard and inexpressive character.80

Others continued to hear detrimental musical effects in using the metronome as the

century progressed. And as the device became more familiar, experienced musicians’

criticisms became more urgent.

In the early history of the device, teachers considered the new metronome to be a

useful aid in the growing field of middle-class musical pedagogy. Nevertheless, music

periodicals and reviews consistently sounded warnings against overusing the machine in

instruction. For many witnessing its influence firsthand, the metronome replaced the

internal sense of musical time with the externalized, artificial time of clockwork. Perhaps

the earliest and most comprehensive English description of the invention from a general-

interest publication appears in The Edinburgh Magazine and Literary Miscellany from

October, 1817. In “An Explanation of the Notation Employed in the Scale of Maelzel’s

Metronome,” the writer defines the techniques required translate Maelzel’s beats-per-

80 Quoted in “Musical Customs and Practices,” Saturday Evening Post, Sept. 28, 1861, 4; APS

Online.

Page 166: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

145

minute scale and describes the use of the machine to performers, students, and amateurs.

Most importantly, he explains the proper and improper place of the clockwork

metronome in living musical performance.81

The writer, known only as

“Philharmonicus,” verbally dissects the unfamiliar technology:

The Maelzel’s metronome we have seen is itself a species of pendulum, which is

made to vibrate different times by means of a sliding weight upon a portion of the

rod continued upwards beyond the point of suspension, and furnished with a

graduated scale upon which is marked the number of beats it makes, when the

upper edge of the weight is adjusted, so as to be opposite to it. It is furnished with

an escapement, and its motion is continued by a weight hung over a pully. The

scale extends from 50 to 160. This instrument answers its purpose in some

respects, but it is not without inconveniences. It is somewhat complex, and may

be put out of order. It is not portable, but requires to be fixed against a wall, and at

a considerable height.82

Also the constant loud ticking which it makes at every

beat, though perhaps esteemed an advantage by some, who cannot measure equal

portions of time in their mind, is disagreeable to those who have a real feeling for

music, and will render those who use it constantly, too mechanically uniform in

their performance, as it will not permit that judicious acceleration and retardation

81 The article also instructs on how to build a string pendulum, which the writer strongly suggests

is a more appropriate and convenient machine for initial tempo reference.

82

It becomes clear from this description that the writer speaks of a “metronome” resembling a

wall clock and not Maelzel’s more compact invention. The machine as described may therefore

be the earlier and more cumbersome design by Maelzel, otherwise a modified clock with

Maelzel’s scale. Despite the differences in appearance, the described machine functioned, and

more importantly sounded, with the same basic qualities as found in Maelzel’s more familiar

metronome.

The 1815 British patent announcement for Maelzel’s first metronome design is almost

comically ambiguous and suggests that the invention was not yet fully conceived or perfected:

“John Malzl, of Poland-street, Middlesex, Machinist; for an instrument or instruments, machine

or machines, for the improvement of musical performance, which he denominates a Metronome,

or musical time-keeper. Dated December 5, 1815.” See The Repertory of Arts, Manufacturers,

and Agriculture, Vol. XXVIII, Second Series (London: J. Wyatt, 1816), 127-8.

Probably due to the early obscurity of the invention in England, this patent description

appears verbatim in an 1816 review of music-publications by Ferdinand Ries, Beethoven’s

collaborator and confidant in London. Ries’ music, published by Clementi & Co., was some of

the first to include the unfamiliar metronome indications—along with the supposedly dreaded

Italian affect words that metronome numbers were intended to replace. See Sylvanus Urban,

“Review of New Musical Publications,” The Gentleman’s Magazine, Mar. 1816, 251.

Page 167: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

146

of the time according to the genius of the passage, in which a great deal of the

expression evinced by a performer of taste consists.83

As many other commentators on Maelzel’s machine suggested, the invention was a boon

to the unmusical student, those amateurs who had no innate sense of rhythm and were

hopeless in finding a musical pulse without external, mechanical assistance.

Nevertheless, “Philharmonicus” offers valuable insights into the performance practices of

his—along with Beethoven’s and Hummel’s— cultural age. Musical time, as he

explained, was essentially an internal phenomenon that the performer had sole authority

over. The writer contended—as so many other perceptive musicians over the century

would similarly explain—that the regular ticking of the automatical clock was the very

antithesis of human expression and musical taste. Mechanical “uniformity” negated the

sensory “judgment” of the performer, who was expected to vary the time over the course

of a composition as the quality of each phrase dictated. For those “who have a real

feeling of music”—as Philharmonicus was one of the first to acknowledge—the

metronome clicked out a time that most creative performers found highly “disagreeable”

with their very human sense of time, rhythm, and expression.

Nearly two decades later, the Musical Magazine reported in 1836, “the

importance of this little instrument in regulating the time of a movement is beginning to

be appreciated,” but provided the following caveat:

Our enterprising teachers should not be slow in availing themselves of such a help

as found in the Metronome. We would not recommend its constant use, lest the

style of the singers should become too evidently labored and mechanical, but

occasionally introduced it will be of great service.84

83 Philharmonicus, “An Explanation of the Notation Employed in the Scale of Maelzel’s

Metronome,” The Edinburgh Magazine and Literary Miscellany I (October, 1817): 223-4.

84

“The Metronome,” The Musical Magazine, Apr. 1836, 304. http://www.proquest.com/

(accessed April 13, 2007).

Page 168: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

147

The metronome, according to early critics, actually created a mechanical performance

practice when consistently referenced. Writers heard the qualities of mindless labor and

artificiality in the metronome, which seemed unlike the traditional perceptions of musical

movement. An 1840 article entitled “Substitute for the Maelzel’s Metronome” from the

Musical Magazine expressed a similar sentiment. The critic reminded students and

teachers that clockwork technology was an unnecessary musical-time reference, since the

simple pendulum was both appropriate and sufficient for finding starting tempos by

Maelzel’s beat-per-minute scale. As the writer recognized, the silent, swinging pendulum

defined musical time much differently than a perpetual, clockwork device. The simple

pendulum was not automatical. He concluded with an essential reminder:

We would make here one remark in regard to the proper use of Maelzel’s

Metronome. It is originally not designed as an instrument by which to keep the

time, but only as one to indicate it, and to use [it] in the former way, except in

exercises, for the mere purpose of learning to keep time, would take away the

spirit of the pupil’s performance, making it a mechanical affair, and him, if he

succeeds in conforming to it, a slave of time.85

As we will see throughout this study, critics continued to intuit a form of tyranny in the

self-moving, artificial time of the metronome. Much like the automaton was a mindless

slave due to its clockwork invariability, the student became a mindless slave under the

clockwork sound of the metronome. These criticisms appeared well into the twentieth

century: the metronome made once free-willed musicians into machines.86

85

“Substitute for Maelzel’s Metronome,” The Musical Magazine; or, Repository of Musical

Science, Literature...Feb. 29, 1840, 67. http://www.proquest.com/ (accessed April 13, 2009).

86

By 1854, Adolph Bernhard Marx had found technique-based instructional methods offered to

the rising wave of amateur, middle-class musicians typically created mechanical performers. Due

to the pressures demanded by this new culture of consumer-amateurism, the typical, lack-luster

music teacher perhaps unwittingly became the trainer of mindless young musicians. Marx

Page 169: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

148

For many who understood the physical and variable nature of musical time, the

metronome created a new and unwelcome performance practice, one based in artificial

and automatical behavior. Publishers and editors were no small part of the problem; the

publishing industry continued to encourage metronomic performance practices by adding

metronome numbers and promoting metronome use in new editions. This editorial trend,

which Schindler called “professional metronoming,” was especially prevalent for

Beethoven’s publications. Schindler scorned, “to here these Sonatas [Beethoven’s Op.

27] according to the metronomic signs affixed to them leads one to wish that all piano-

forte metronomers were put under the ban,” and suggested that these editors were

“perverters of all truth in expression [whose re-interpretation] threatens soon to bring all

genuine music under the domination of the superficial—if, indeed, it has not already

submitted to that authority.”87

As editor of Schindler’s text, Moscheles admitted that he himself was included in

the “metronomising” of Beethoven’s music yet attested to the worth of his metronomic

translations, stating “I have not merely listened to my own musical feeling, but been

guided by my recollections of what I gathered from Beethoven’s own playing.”88

It seems

described the dangerous results of what was effectively a cultural-social crisis in mass musical

education, during an age in which the clockwork metronome gradually found new, non-“artistic”

adherents. Marx explained: “the immediate consequence of this has been, as might be expected,

that the musical profession, like every other lucrative business, has attracted a host of

competitors, every one of whom again exerts himself in recruiting and sending out fresh legions

of amateurs…Every hour of the day, if possible, is occupied in giving lessons; and, in the few

spare moments, the over-taxed powers of body and mind are still further exhausted, in the

practice of all the new and fashionable things which every day brings forth. Art is made

mechanical, and as a mechanism transmitted to the people; not through the fault of the harassed

teacher, but in consequence of his false position.” See Adolph Bernhard Marx, The Music of the

Nineteenth Century and Its Culture (London: Robert Cocks, and Co., 1854), 74.

87 See Schindler, ed. Moscheles, 108-109.

88

Ibid., 106fn.

Page 170: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

149

that early-nineteenth-century metronome markings, whether from an editor or composer,

were largely grounded by subjective, ex post facto assessments and not objective

absolutes. In Schindler’s Life of Beethoven, Moscheles too recognized the subjective,

non-mechanical nature of musical time, and he could not comprehend the biographer’s

complete disdain for editorial metronome markings. While he was indeed a

“metronomising” editor, Moscheles believed that “the musical world knows, that marking

time by a metronome is but a slight guide for performers and conductors.”89

Moscheles’ understanding—that metronomic guidance was of slight and limited

importance—was not a universal truth for musicians in his age, or for ages to follow.

Many contemporaneous critics found that these metronomic values were becoming a new

reality in performance. In 1846, the Harbinger reviewer, similar to Schindler, suggested

that mechanical time was actively defiling the human qualities of musical expressivity:

To bang through an overture like a machine is not the thing; no machine has

whims and inflections, and therefore, it only makes cast-iron music…a mere

beater of time is worth nothing, but to embarrass all parties and to kill the music,

and Maelzel’s metronome were quite as good a thing and less expensive.90

As many suggested, the activities of the industrialized world—which functioned in

lockstep with automatical machines—had no place on the concert stage.91

Musicians with

89 Ibid., 111fn. Moscheles too recognized that performance practices, perhaps under the influence

of more metronomic, technical practice routines, created more mechanical interpretations. As a

Beethoven editor, he thus reasoned: “In some of [Beethoven’s] quick movements I have

purposely refrained from giving way to that rapidity of piano-forte execution, so largely

developed at the present time.” See Schindler, ed. Moscheles, 106-107fn.

90

“Musical Review,” Harbinger, Devoted to Social and Political Progress, Mar. 7, 1846, 204.

91

Susan Bogert Warner, in her 1852 novel Queechy, analogized the metronome as the antithesis

to natural music-making, writing “the canary might as well study Maelzel's Metronome…The

nature of the one must change or the two must remain the world wide apart.” For Warner, too,

innate musicality was “the world wide apart” from clockwork technology. See Susan Warner (aka

Elizabeth Wetherell), Queechy, Gutenberg eBook #8874, http://www.gutenberg.org (accessed

September 20, 2007).

Page 171: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

150

“cast-iron,” invariable, hard, and inexpressive qualities were considered just as useful as

Maelzel’s tempo-clock. As we recall, the contemporaneous critics of Maelzel’s other

automata concurred, “no machine has whims and inflections.” Thus, while George

Templeton Strong considered Maelzel’s automaton trumpeter a lifeless clockwork

performer, critics of the clockwork metronome, as early as “Philharmonicus” in 1817,

heard living musicians exhibiting the very same automatical qualities when Maelzel’s

machine guided their actions.

As amateur and professional musicians increasingly used the metronome to

reference tempo, critics began to discuss a performer’s ability to transcend metronomic

time and thus express something “anti-artificial.” Conductors, in particular, were

criticized throughout the nineteenth century when behaving like metronomes. The Times

pejoratively dubbed even Moscheles “a metronomic director” in 1841.92

Recognizing the

musical insufficiency of metronomic directing, others argued that a mechanical conductor

invariably created a mechanical orchestra; and that a music ensemble, under this slavery

of metronomic time, sonically transformed into a living Panharmonicon. In 1847, the

Harbinger magazine critic described how mechanical qualities were eroding the natural,

human nuances once heard in good orchestra performances. He maintained that the

emerging metronomic time-sense was entirely misguided and unlike anything that

composers, including Beethoven, could ever have intended. The reviewer scolds:

92

“Schindler’s life of Beethoven, edited by Moscheles,” The Times, April 10, 1841, 6.

Page 172: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

151

Let no orchestra attempt to play very fast, which is obliged to be merely

mechanical in its playing; in which there is not a pervading feeling of the

composition which dictates to every instrument, by a simultaneous instinct, when

to retard a little, and when to accelerate. No strict time-keeping by Maelzel’s

metronome can possibly produce a piece of music as it existed in the composer’s

mind, or fail to sacrifice its life and glow and meaning; and consequently that

swift rail-road speed, which does not yield to all the varying impulsations of a

controlling feeling, will express as little as a rapid locomotive with a long train of

dirt cars after it.93

The times were actually changing, and critics considered Maelzel’s metronome

instrumental in dictating a new paradigm of musical time through mechanical motion.

Even in literature, the radical, detrimental shift towards metronomic action was

expressed. In Menella Bute Smedley’s story Edith Kinnaird (c1845), one character

recites a well-worn poem with uncommon, anti-human performance practices: “Mr.

Dalton volunteered to read aloud Tennyson’s ‘Locksley Hall,’ which he delivered with a

pompous trepidation very fatal to the flow of the meter, to say nothing of the sentiment.

You might have kept time to his declamation with a metronome.”94

As both music

criticisms and fictional anecdotes suggest, the traditional practice of metric emphasis and

the “simultaneous instinct” of creative rhythm—which combined the qualities of accent,

movement, phrasing, dynamics, and expression—receded under the strict guidance of

mechanical time. The metronome, in opposition to the lived experience of musical and

spoken rhythm, enforced an expressionless value system that some listeners equated to

factory machinery and mass, industrial transportation––those utilitarian offspring of

Maelzel’s soulless automata. Yet experienced musicians could still intuit a distinction

between traditional music making and the “mere” artificiality of metronomic time. In

93 “Musical Review,” Harbinger, Devoted to Social and Political Progress, Feb. 27, 1847, 185.

94

Menella Bute Smedley, “The Maiden Aunt—No. V,” Sharpe’s London Magazine, Dec. 4,

1847, 92.

Page 173: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

152

1856, one such performer in the Philharmonic Society of New York praised a fine

conductor of pre-metronomic traditions, U. C. Hill, who “taught us the art of shades and

effects in music, and has rendered the metronome and all mechanical music hateful to us

for ever.”95

The Embrace of Maelzel’s Automatical Values in Industry

As cultural critics, music educators, and performers heard the sounds of trains and factory

machinery in Maelzel’s machines, it may come as no surprise that significant nineteenth-

century politicians and labor theorists found the artificiality of Maelzel’s automata to be

positive, influential symbols for the industrializing world. One notable example of

Maelzel’s cultural influence appears in a speech by Senator Daniel Webster (1782-1852)

from 1836. Possibly recollecting Maelzel’s Boston exhibitions, Webster described the

importance of machines in America’s new industrial society:

It is thus that the successful application of science to art increases the productive

power and agency of the human race…These automata in the factories and the

workshops are as much our fellow laborers, as if they were automata wrought by

some Maelzel into the form of men, and made capable of walking, moving, and

working, of felling the forest or cultivating the fields.96

While music critics heard the sound of monotonous labor and factory machines in the

metronome, Webster found that factory machines and human labor imitated Maelzel’s

automata. The politician envisioned a nation where automata and men work with the

95 “Music,” The Albion, a Journal of News, Politics, and Literature, Mar. 8, 1856, 115.

96

Daniel Webster, "Technical Progress and Prosperity," The Writings and Speeches of Daniel

Webster Hitherto Uncollected I (Boston, 1903), 63–78; in Annals of American History.

http://america.eb.com/america/ (accessed August 3, 2007).

Page 174: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

153

same movements and purposes.97

Webster was not concerned that factory machines did

not appear like Maelzel’s fabrications. The utility of automatical machines, despite

appearances, made them equal in value to living workers. Perhaps unwittingly, Webster

explained that machines and men, when used for the same industrial purposes, were

virtually indistinguishable from one another, and from Maelzel’s actual clockwork

creations.

The writings of other early nineteenth-century labor theorists strongly suggest that

Maelzel’s mechanical culture inspired the very notion that automatic machines could be

invented and employed in the new factories of England and France. One of the first

proponents of a Maelzelian factory system was Andrew Ure, in his 1835 text The

Philosophy of Manufactures: or An Exposition of the Scientific, Moral, and Commercial

Economy of the Factory System of Great Britain. Yet Ure recognized that automata—

Maelzel’s inventions specifically—presently failed to provide any lasting purposes for

society beyond fleeting entertainment and diversion:

The chess-player of M. Maelzel, now under exhibition at Paris, and

formerly shown in this country, has been often described. It imitates very

remarkably a living being, endowed with all the resources of intelligence, for

executing the combinations of profound study.

Raisin’s automaton harpsichord was found to contain an infant performer.

Self-acting inventions like the preceding, however admirable as exercises

of mechanical science, do nothing towards the supply of the physical necessities

97 Daniel Webster was not the first significant United States politician to describe an aspect of

American society using the term “automaton.” In a letter dated July 12, 1816, Thomas Jefferson

wrote to Samuel Kercheval, “And to preserve their independence, we must not let our rulers load

us with perpetual debt. We must make our election between economy and liberty, or profusion

and servitude…A departure from principle in one instance becomes a precedent for a second; that

second, for a third; and so on, till the bulk of the society is reduced to be mere automatons of

misery, to have no sensibilities left but for sinning and suffering.” It may be significant that at the

time of this speech, Maelzel was exhibiting automata in London. From Thomas Jefferson,

Memoirs, Correspondence, and Private Papers of Thomas Jefferson, volume IV (London:

Colburn and Bentley, 1829), 293–300.

Page 175: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

154

of society. Man stands in daily want of food, fuel, clothing, and shelter; and is

bound to devote the powers of body and mind, of nature and art, in the first place

to provide for himself and his dependents a sufficiency of these necessaries, with

which there can be no comfort, nor leisure for the cultivation of the taste and

intellect. To the production of food and domestic accommodation, not many

automatic inventions have been applied, or seem to be extensively applicable;

though, for modifying them to the purposes of luxury, many curious contrivances

have been made.98

Ure, similar to Daniel Webster, re-envisioned the ideal factory system to be one where

automatons and men labored together with the same actions. Ure’s modern factory, with

its human factory laborers, represented a mechanical collective that would rival even

Maelzel’s Panharmonicon:

But I conceive that this title [“factory”], involves the idea of a vast automaton,

composed of various mechanical and intellectual organs, acting in uninterrupted

concert for the production of a common object, all of them being subordinated to

a self-regulating moving force.99

Ure acknowledged, however, that these new labor concepts, based on repeating,

mechanical actions of men, women, and children would take time to implement, since

even factory workers in the early nineteenth century were not keen on behaving like

synchronized clockwork. He explains the real problems with the automatically structured

factory: “The main difficulty did not, to my apprehension, lie so much in the invention of

a proper self-acting mechanism for drawing out and twisting cotton into a continuous

thread, as in the distribution of the different members of the apparatus into one co-

operating body, in impelling each organ with its appropriate delicacy and speed, and

98 Andrew Ure, The Philosophy of Manufactures: or An Exposition of the Scientific, Moral, and

Commercial Economy of the Factory System of Great Britain (London: Charles Knight, 1835),

11. It seems that Ure did not realize Maelzel’s chess player, like Raisin’s harpsichord, also

contained a person within the pseudo-automaton. Ure did acknowledge, however, the truly

“productive” textile machines of famed eighteenth-century automaton creator Vaucanson. See

Ure, 12-13.

99

Ibid., 13-14.

Page 176: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

155

above all, in training human beings to renounce their desultory habits of work, and to

identify themselves with the unvarying regularity of the complex automaton.”100

For Ure,

the greatest problem in establishing the new industrial factory was in making men

metronomic.

Later nineteenth-century social critics often suggested that the mindless, slaving

automata that Webster and Ure once analogized were truly embodied in the new and

expanding industrial workforce. Human laborers took on the qualities of automata both in

action and appearance through repetitive, redundant, and soulless work. Likewise

suggesting Maelzel’s machine culture, George Fitzhugh explained how Ure’s speculative

theories became reality in an 1854 essay "The Failure of Free Society" stating, “division

makes labor ten times more efficient, but, by confining each workman to some simple,

monotonous employment, it makes him a mere automaton and an easy prey to the

capitalist.”101

Andrew C. Cameron also echoed this sentiment during an 1867 union

conference, maintaining, “The man who merely performs the monotonous functions of a

mere automaton, as thousands of our factory employees do from year to year, must

eventually descend both in the intellectual and social scale.”102

These social critics found

that men, women, and children—who lost the sense of their own interpretive will through

mindless repetitive actions—became mechanical. Cultural critics continued to express

similar perceptions regarding musicians when they applied the metronome to once

100 Ibid., 15.

101

George Fitzhugh " The Failure of Free Society," Annals of American History.

http://america.eb.com/america/ (accessed August 3, 2007).

102

Andrew C. Cameron, “The Problems and Prospects of Labor,” Annals of American History.

http://america.eb.com/america/ (accessed August 3, 2007).

Page 177: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

156

creative performance practices. As Cameron, Fitzhugh, and many critics of the

metronome suggested, a “man who merely performs monotonous functions” devolves

into “a mere automaton.”

Automatical behavior, once only dreamed of by Ure, became a positive value in

modern factory organization by the end of the century. In his “scientifically managed”

factory system, the industrial-labor mastermind Frederick Winslow Taylor recognized

that he trained human beings to act artificially. Through incessant drilling of workers’

actions, which were constantly gauged by the precise, artificial time of the stopwatch

down to fractions of a second, Taylor increased the productivity and efficiency of factory

laborers to unprecedented levels. Yet, Taylor found a common criticism with his

scientific training method:

Now, when through all of this teaching and this minute instruction the work is

apparently made so smooth and easy for the workman, the first impression is that

this all tends to make him a mere automaton, a wooden man. As the workmen

frequently say when they first come under this system, “Why, I am not allowed to

think or move without some one interfering or doing it for me!” The same

criticism and objection, however, can be raised against all other modern

subdivision of labor.103

In the industrial world, men seemed to be moving ever closer to the behaviors of

Maelzel’s automata. Acting in strict artificial time while performing mindless automatical

actions, humans started mimicking those inventions that many writers earlier in the

century considered insufficient when compared to humanity. But, while the factory was

embracing automatical culture for its efficiency, precision, and ease of production, the

world of music performance and education continued to voice protest regarding

Maelzel’s automatical metronome. In the metronome, critics continued to hear the very

103 Frederick Winslow Taylor, Scientific Management (Reprint, Westport: Greenwood Press,

1972), 125.

Page 178: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

157

same mindless, monotonous qualities of time and motion that Ure’s and later Taylor’s

modern factory system valued so highly.

Musical Artificiality Actualized in the Metronome

The 1884 edition of Encyclopedia Britannica acknowledged that the metronome, over the

course of the nineteenth century, became the primary external-reference tool for music

instruction in Europe.104

The metronome article, however, was no endorsement of the

device. Instead, the Britannica entry described the insufficiency of Maelzel’s automatical

creations in a world of living music performance. Concurring with pedagogues over the

century, the critical Encyclopedia Britannica writer argued that a simple pendulum, a tool

unrelated to the artificial time of clockwork, was vastly more appropriate in defining

musical tempos. He then railed upon the unoriginality and inconvenience of Maelzel’s

overly artificial creation, concluding:

Maelzel’s scale was needlessly and arbitrarily complicated, proceeding by twos

from 40 to 60, by threes from 60 to 70, by fours from 72 to 120, by sixes from

120 to 144, and by eights from 144 to 208. Dr. Crotch constructed a time-

measurer, and Henry Smart (the violinist, and father of the composer of the same

name) made another in 1821, both before that received as Maelzel’s was known in

England. In 1882 James Mitchell, a Scotsman, made an ingenious amplification of

the Maelzel clock-work, reducing to mechanical demonstration what formerly

rested wholly on the feeling of the performer. Although “Maelzel’s metronome”

has universal acceptance, the silent metronome and still more Weber’s graduated

ribbon are greatly to be preferred, for the clock-work of the other is liable to be

out of order, and needs a nicety of regulation which is almost impossible; for

instance when Sir George Smart had to mark the traditional times of the several

pieces of the Dettingen Te Deum,105

he tested them by twelve metronomes, no

104 General-interest periodicals continued to describe the metronome solely as a pedagogical tool,

not a professional practice aid. In 1905, an article entitled “The Romance of Automata” by Henry

Ridgely Evans states, “ Maelzel was the inventor of the Metronome (1815), a piece of mechanism

known to all instructors of music.” Emphasis added. See The Open Court, Mar. 1, 1905, 131.

105

Significantly, Smart knew the “traditional times” of Handel’s work without the aid of the

metronome.

Page 179: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

158

two of which beat together. The value of the machine is exaggerated, for no living

performer could execute a piece in unvaried time throughout, and no student

could practice under the tyranny of its beat; and conductors of music, nay,

composers themselves, will give the same piece slightly slower or quicker on

different occasions, according to the circumstances of performance.106

For this writer and many others in the nineteenth century, artificial clockwork placed an

external imposition on human action. The metronome, like Taylor’s stopwatch, imposed

a form of tyranny over the movements and thoughts of people. We recall that Beethoven

himself acknowledged the tyrannical nature of the device, even while endorsing its use to

fellow composers.107

The Encyclopedia Britannica entry confirmed that a free and

variable quality of musical time was the common practice. Recognizing that all good

performances necessitated subjective judgment, the critic realized that metronomic time

merely squashed creative freedom; its clockwork clicks effaced the natural subjectivity

inherent in musical time and expression. Thus the entry exposed a significant concept

underlying the use of the machine in the nineteenth century: The metronome did not

sufficiently explain or document how music moves. The device merely projected an

automatical constriction, a tyranny, upon the subjective movements and expressions of

living musicians.

By the late nineteenth century, metronomic time was an increasingly audible

quality in professional performances, and its seemingly hegemonic influence was heard

even in the most renowned musicians and ensembles. As critics continued to relate,

performers seemingly devolved into automata when guided by the time of Maelzel’s

106 The Encyclopedia Britannica, Ninth Edition, Vol. XVI (American Reprint, Philadelphia: J.M.

Stoddart Co., Limited, 1884), 207. This article is reprinted verbatim at least through the 1911

edition of the encyclopedia.

107

Beethoven, Letters, ed. Anderson, II, 727.

Page 180: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

159

metronome. For instance, a New York Times reviewer in 1894 described a concert where

“the dynamic gradations were attempted, though nearly all were made with abrupt

rudeness, but there were hardly any nuances of tempo at all. Mr. Damrosch beat off the

measures like a metronome, and the orchestra, following his lead, played like an

assembly of artisans.”108

This reviewer heard the qualities of repetitive, unthinking labor

in metronomic musical-time; the musicians sounded as if they were Taylor’s modern

factory workers—and by mechanical association, Maelzel’s historical automata.

The composer Camille Saint-Saëns expressed an opposite aesthetical point of

view in 1891, complementing a conductor, A. Vianesi, who “[possessed] the precious

quality of not conducting like a metronome, [giving] to my music the suppleness which is

essential to an artistic orchestra.”109

For both Saint-Saëns and the New York Times

reviewer, the metronome promoted the values of an artisan, in binary opposition to an

artist.110

The 1913 Webster’s Dictionary verifies the pejorative term and compares the

two opposing concepts, “where idea of mechanical conformity to rule…attaches to the

term artisan, [while] the ideas of refinement and of peculiar skill…belong to the term

108“The Symphony Society,” The New York Times, Jan. 4, 1894, 2.

109

Camille Saint-Saëns, “Letter 4—No Title,” Century Illustrated Magazine, Nov. 1891, 156.

110

In the nineteenth century, experienced musicians, pedagogues, and critics often invoked the

binary association between “artist and artisan” when describing their performance culture. By

1854, Adolph Bernhard Marx had considered the musical artist to contain a high capacity for

intuition, sensory perception, individualism, and creativity, while the mere performing artisan

could only reproduce lifeless products.

Describing the necessary values of musical education in The Music of the Nineteenth

Century and Its Culture, Marx, a vocal critic of metronomic interpretation, states: “I must once

more premise that all activity in art must be ‘artistic’ in its operation; that is to say, that it must

spring from individual will, reflection, and sympathy throughout all its manifestations. All

theorizing of every kind—that is to say, all teaching that does not appeal to intuitive perception of

the subject and active participation in it—is void, dead, and can only mislead.” See Marx, 265.

Page 181: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

160

artist.”111

Thus, for many, the clockwork metronome was a tool that developed musical

workers, not creative musicians.112

Another New York Times reviewer, summarizing Krehbiel’s publication Listening

to Music, maintained in 1897 that for an orchestra leader “to beat time might be the work

of the metronome, and so the orchestra would be brought down to the level of a

mechanism, to be set going with a crank.”113

The clockwork consistency of Maelzel’s

metronome resulted in an ensemble that again evoked two more of Maelzel’s machines:

the trumpeter and the Panharmonicon, those inexpressive and artificial military

musicians. One writer in 1905 perceived a similar mechanical aesthetic in an ensemble

directed by the notable American conductor, Theodore Thomas, admitting that he,

111 See Webster’s Unabridged Dictionary (1913), 86.

112

Venerable piano pedagogue Adolph Friedrich Christiani, in The Principles of Expression in

Pianoforte Playing (1884), offered perhaps the most detailed description of the qualities that

distinguish performing musical “artists” from common musicians. By scoring performers on the

presence or absence of four innate abilities—“Talent, Emotion, Intelligence, and Technique”—

Christiani printed a fifteen-point hierarchy of musicality and artistic development. Christiani

clearly consider those with “Talent” to be the highest order of musicians, even if they lacked

other innate qualities. Someone with all four innate qualities was considered to be an “Executive

artist, of highest order.” Someone only lacking in technique was a “non-executive artist; probably

first-class teacher.” Fourth in the list was the performer lacking in “Emotion,” who was listed

merely as an “Executant musician; probably scholarly and critical, but dry.” Seventh in the

artistic hierarchy of music performance was, “A virtuoso, without being either an artist or a

musician” and who lacked the capacities for both “Emotion” and “Intelligence.”

The lowest three musical types on Christiani’s hierarchical scale of artistry are telling:

One who lacks both “Talent” and “Emotion” is a “Scholarly executant, but cold;” Lower still is a

“Musical theorist,” someone who only contains “Intelligence” and nothing more. Finally, and

most significantly for the present study, stands the performer who only has “Technique,” whom

Christiani summarily discounts as a “Virtuoso of the music-box kind.”

See Adolph Friedrich Christiani, The Principles of Expression in Pianoforte Playing

(New York: Harper & Brothers, 1885), 16.

113 “Listening to Music,” New York Times, Mar. 6, 1897, RB2.

Page 182: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

161

seemed to me simply a kind of human metronome, beating time. I learned the

contrary on one occasion, when I was permitted to witness one of his private

rehearsals. At a certain point in the symphony, which the orchestra was playing in

perfect time and in perfect tune, but with a mechanical effect of crescendo and

diminuendo…he suddenly rapped on the music stand before him…with hand and

foot acting together he imitated the movement of an organ-grinder…The orchestra

repeated the passage with the spirit and fire, before lacking, infused into it. It was

a trifling incident, but a significant one.114

As we recall, nineteenth-century spectators consistently found artificial machines, both

their actions and appearances, to be wanting. The reviewer described a human

metronome as “simple,” in the same way an automaton was considered “mere.” Yet,

despite the insufficiency of metronomic time, comments continued to acknowledge the

rise of mechanical performance practices. A London reviewer for the Times, again in

1897, remarked on the unusual tempo-sense displayed in a performance of Berlioz’s

transcription of “Weber’s ‘Invitation’ [which] ended the programme; it was taken with a

strange absence of rhythmic elasticity, with almost metronomic regularity.”115

Berlioz

himself, by 1841, had realized that any music adhering to Maelzel’s metronome would

exhibit all the qualities of a “strange,” lifeless corpse:

I do not mean to say…that it is necessary to imitate the mathematical regularity of

the metronome; all music so performed would become of freezing stiffness, and I

even doubt whether it would be possible to observe so flat a uniformity during a

certain number of bars.

Much like the strangeness elicited by the visage of Maelzel’s chess player, automatical

time and motion appeared unnatural, dead, or altogether inhuman for nineteenth-century

composers and music critics alike. But this flat, uniform, and freezing-stiff performance

114 “Three Impressions of Theodore Thomas,” Outlook, Feb. 4, 1905, 316-17.

115

“The Lamoureux Concerts,” The Times, Mar. 29, 1897, 13.

116

Hector Berlioz, A Treatise on Modern Instrumentation and Orchestration, trans. Mary

Cowden Clarke (London: Novello, Ewer, and Co., 1856), 246.

Page 183: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

162

practice was clearly on the rise by the end of the century. Again from 1897, another New

York Times critic heard an odd performance of Brahms’ Symphony no. 1, where in the

first movement, “There was no rubato at all, and much of the significance of the beautiful

phrasing was quite lost because Mr. Damrosch drove his orchestra ahead with the angular

rigidity of a metronome.”117

As Saint-Saëns’ compliment suggested, some notable conductors continued to

lead an orchestra unencumbered by artificial metronomic time. Yet, during the first

decade of the twentieth century, some critics found that such nuanced conducting became

a rarity. In 1907 one reporter for the International Year Book extolled the new conductor

of the Boston Symphony Orchestra: “Dr. Karl Muck, of Berlin, won the affection of all

admirers of orchestral music. He towers far above his predecessor, and the famous

orchestra, free from the dead weight of a mechanical metronome beat, played with all its

old-time sweep and passion to which audiences were accustomed under the batons of

Nikisch and Paur.”118

Indeed, in a telling assertion of twentieth-century performance

culture, the traditional, non-metronomic perception of musical movement was becoming

distinguished for being “old-time.”

The emergent epistemology of automatical-musical time not only altered

orchestra performances, but also solo recitals and choral concerts. Commenting on a

piano performance by Bernard Stavenhagen (1862-1914), a Times critic recounted in

1891 that “the metronomic accuracy of rhythm which now distinguishes Herr

Stavenhagen’s playing is very ill suited to the variations which form the opening

117 “The Symphony Society,” The New York Times, Jan. 23, 1898, 16.

118

Frank Moore Colby, editor, The International Year Book, A Compendium of the World’s

Progress for the Year 1907 (New York: Dodd, Mead, and Company, 1908), 523.

Page 184: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

163

movement”119

of a particular Beethoven sonata. Reviewing the 1905 Bristol Musical

Festival, another Times review noted that the choral director often “insists on a

metronomic regularity that can hardly have been intended. Surely the fine choral

recitatives…were not meant to be taken quite so inflexibly as they were taken today.”120

Even through the twentieth century, it seems, recitatives were still performed with a non-

metronomic time sense, employing the traditional knowledge of poetic meter and spoken

declamation, expressing the music in the “reciting style.”121

Critics of the metronomic time were especially resentful when the machine was

applied to the publications and performances of Beethoven. Georg Henschel (1850-

1934), the famed conductor and founder of the Boston Symphony Orchestra, was perhaps

the most outspoken detractor of the metronomic interpretations of Beethoven’s

symphonies. As editor of Sir George Grove’s published letters, Henschel described two

qualities in conflict: Maelzel’s automatical metronome and Beethoven’s musical spirit.

The tension between them was especially problematic in Beethoven’s grandiose Ninth

Symphony:

These marks had been sent by Beethoven—eight days before his death—to the

Philharmonic Society of London in his great anxiety to lessen the difficulties of

studying and performing his gigantic work. Interesting therefore as they may be to

the biographer, the historian, the student—to the public, I thought, it could be

nothing but distracting to state that the Adagio, for instance, of the Ninth

Symphony is supposed to be played at sixty beats, while the Andante alternating

with it should be played at sixty-three beats in the minute. Who could, I reasoned,

even supposing he had, by the help of the metronome, begun the Adagio at

exactly sixty beats—who could warrant that in the Andante he would not beat

more or less than exactly three beats per minute more? Surely not Beethoven

119 “Herr Stavenhagen’s Recital,” The Times, Nov. 25, 1891, 3.

120

“Bristol Musical Festival,” The Times, Oct. 12, 1905, 4.

121

See Webster’s Unabridged Dictionary (1913), 1198, for a common-usage concurrence.

Page 185: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

164

himself: for, godlike as are the revelations of his soul, it was human blood that ran

through the veins of his body.122

While critics and skilled musicians such as Henschel found the metronome an insufficient

tempo guide for publication, many also heard the machine actively affecting the once-

expressive interpretations of Beethoven’s monumental orchestral works. Ironically

perhaps, a 1911 performance of Beethoven’s Symphony no. 8—apocryphally inspired by

Maelzel’s clockwork metronome—was for one New York correspondent to the London

Times exceedingly mechanical as, “Not enough was made, for instance, of the delicious

ebb and flow of impulse in the whimsical ‘metronome’ movement. It was too

metronomic (and incidentally rather too fast throughout.)”123

Thus, many still believed in

the subjectivity of musical time, in which temporal nuance, variability, and expressive

movement were essential qualities to living performances. For this critic, even in

Beethoven’s supposed musical imitation of a metronome, a nuanced, human sense of

musical movement should prevail above the incessant, lifeless monotony of metronomic

time.

The Rejection of the Metronome in Music Education and Performance

Early in the nineteenth century, Maelzel’s metronome was initially championed in

pedagogical circles as a simple reference tool, but in the last decades of the century,

many skilled instructors considered its continual employment harmful to the goals of

music performance. By the 1880s, the metronome was no longer a novelty in some

122 Reprinted in C. A. B., “George Grove’s Analyses of Beethoven,” The Musical World, Nov. 3,

1888, 850.

123

“Music. Concerts from New York,” The Times, Nov. 25, 1911, 12.

Page 186: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

165

countries, and more musicians perceived that the machine was causing serious detriment

to subjective musical time. An 1897 Musical Visitor article by American composer,

pianist, educator, and theorist Edward Baxter Perry attested that individual choice and

experience must continue to define musical time, as it had for centuries. “Nine tenths of

the details and all of the finesse of interpretation,” he asserted, “must always be left to the

taste and intelligence of the individual player.”124

His sentiments echoed the beliefs of

Rousseau, Loulié, Couperin, and even Beethoven himself, who affirmed that skilled

performers imbue an ephemeral “spirit” into a performance, despite the visual limitations

of music notation. Using Chopin’s “Berceuse” as an example, Perry argued that

interpretive practices must oppose the influence of metronomic thinking. While equal,

rigid, and repetitive rhythms appeared in Chopin’s publication, Perry explained that

living practices inform musical time, since “no variation is even hinted at in the score, yet

surely no one but a wooden pianist, with a metronome for a heart, would think of playing

them exactly alike.”125

Perry heard wooden pianists trained by Maelzel’s metronome just as Frederick

Taylor witnessed wooden laborers trained with the stopwatch. Both accounts alluded to

the inhuman appearance, repetitive movements, and unthinking actions of people

subjugated by the constrictions of artificial time. For Perry, the metronome virtually

replaced a musician’s heart—the once standard reference for musical time—with

unfeeling clockwork. As critics even before Perry argued: the metronomic musician was

an automaton. In his 1902 Descriptive Analyses of Piano Works, Perry considered that

124 Edward Baxter Perry, “Rubato Playing,” The Musical Visitor, Aug. 1897, 202.

125

Ibid.

Page 187: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

166

“such a person, no matter how highly organized, or perfectly trained otherwise, is no

better than a machine. He does not live, he simply runs.”126

Perry reaffirmed in his 1910

pedagogical treatise Stories of Standard Teaching Pieces, where no discussion of the

metronome ever appears, that an innate, poetically derived understanding of musical time

“makes all the distinction between mere machine playing and artistic delivery.”127

As the century turned, some of the world’s most respected musicians argued that

metronomic time was immanently impinging upon the subjective nature of musicality.

The world-renowned pianist Josef Hofmann (1876-1957)—perhaps the first professional

instrumentalist to make a solo recording—warned that repetition, redundancy, and

mechanical consistency was to be seriously avoided in both performance and

instruction.128

As late as 1905, Hofmann brazenly proclaimed:

Never Play with a Metronome: You may use a metronome for a little passage,

as a test of your ability to play in strict time. When you see the result, positive or

negative, stop the machine at once. For according to the metronome a really

musical rhythm is unrhythmical—and on the other hand, the keeping of absolutely

strict time is thoroughly unmusical and deadlike.129

Musical rhythm continued to be a living quality for Hofmann, a musician of pre-

mechanical performance traditions. Indeed, he believed that the individual’s rhythmic

126 Edward Baxter Perry, Descriptive Analyses of Piano Works (Philadelphia: Theo. Presser Co.,

1902), 106.

127

Edward Baxter Perry, Stories of Standard Teaching Pieces (Philadelphia: Theo. Presser Co.,

1910), 181.

128

Hofmann argues: “Systemization is the death of spontaneousness and spontaneousness is the

very soul of art. If you play every day at the same time the same sequence of the same studies and

the same pieces you may acquire a certain degree of skill, perhaps, but the spontaneity of your

rendition will surely be lost. Art belongs to the realm of emotional manifestations, and it stands to

reason that a systematic exploiting of our emotional nature must blunt it.” Indirectly, he argues

against Taylorism and the procedures of Scientific Management.

129

Joseph Hofmann, “Playing the Piano Successfully,” The Ladies’ Home Journal, Mar. 1905, 9.

Page 188: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

167

sense could not translate into mechanical beats, movements, or measures.130

Hofmann’s

epistemology recalls the teachings of piano pedagogue Adolph Friedrich Christiani

(1836-1885) who warned, “the fact should never be lost sight of, that absolute strictness,

machine-like perfection in time-keeping, is not artistic.”131

As late as 1913, the Webster's

Revised Unabridged Dictionary recalls the common-usage of Christiani’s and Hofmann’s

expressive quality:

Rhythm, n. [F. rhythme, rythme, L. rhythmus, fr. GR. measured motion, measure,

proportion, fr. to flow. See Stream.] 1. In the widest sense, a dividing into short

portions by a regular succession of motions, impulses, sounds, accents, etc.,

producing an agreeable effect, as in music, poetry, the dance, or the like. 2. (Mus.)

Movement in musical time, with periodical recurrence of accent; the measured

beat or pulse which marks the character and expression of the music; symmetry of

movement and accent. Moore (Encyc. ) 3. A division of lines into short portions

by a regular succession of arses and theses, or percussions and remissions of

voice on words or syllables. 4. The harmonious flow of vocal sounds.132

Christiani’s 1885 treatise, The principles of expression in pianoforte playing, verified that

an interpretive, implied rhythmical hierarchy of meter, one directly akin to poetry, existed

in musical performance despite the visual limitations of music notation. These innate

rhythmic practices,

130 In 1901, Hofmann explained his anti-metronomic aesthetic in the same journal: “As to

rhythmics of performance: I must…remark that in performers of strongly pronounced

individuality such a thing as absolute rhythmics does not exist. By absolute rhythmics I mean the

correct time valuation of each individual note. For if the performer were to render everything

according to the time value of each individual note his performance would be exceedingly dull.”

See Hofmann, “Playing the Piano Correctly,” The Ladies’ Home Journal, Oct. 1, 1901, 18.

131 Adolph Friedrich Christiani, The principles of expression in pianoforte playing (Philadelphia:

T. Presser, [1885]), 260.

132

See Webster’s Unabridged Dictionary (1913), 1238-9.

Page 189: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

168

should pervade a musical composition, as the beating of the pulse pervades

whatsoever has life, because they give a swinging, undulating, human element to

a movement, which would be stiff and machine-like, lacking in pulsation and life,

without them.

This is why a music-box and a barrel-organ, though mechanically correct,

are so tiresome and monotonous.133

In 1917, composer Constantin von Sternberg (1852-1924), a staunch critic of both

modernism and modern mechanical time, was one of the last nineteenth-century-trained

musicians to reassert Christiani’s and Hofmann’s belief that the “invisible accent” of

musical meter was natural, innate, and intrinsic to meaningful interpretation. He stated

that “without it every distinction between the living musician and the mechanical self-

playing machine practically vanishes.”134

Consistent with centuries-old meanings of time and motion, nineteenth-century

musicians considered musical rhythm to be physically derived. Musical time continued to

be associated with gesture, movement, dance, and speech; it was a variable quality

containing “harmonious” and “agreeable” affects, proportionally “regular” in human

pulse and spoken meter, not mechanically precise in clockwork. In 1888, The Musical

World cited Henschel and others in their assertions that musical, dramatic, and poetic

rhythm was founded upon a completely different epistemology—an individualistic,

interpretive practice—distinctly opposed to the redundant sound of a clicking automaton,

“The metronome,” says Nottebohm, “has nothing to do with feeling. The

conception of the spirit of a tone-piece, the nuances in its motion, deviations from

the absolute and normal measure founded upon the rhythmical structure of the

piece, cannot be made dependent on a soulless clockwork, still less can they be

determined by such. The metronome is nothing but a help toward securing a time

which the composer had in mind”—a very weak one (Mr. Henschel adds) for

133 Christiani, 48-49.

134

Constantin von Sternberg, Ethics and Esthetics of Piano-Playing (New York: G. Schirmer,

1917), 50.

Page 190: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

169

those who cannot approximately find it through the character of the themes of a

musical work, the interpretation of which can as little be measured by the degrees

of a metronome as can the delivery by a reader or an actor of a poem, a

monologue in rhythmical verse.135

As analyzed in Chapter I, the subjective expression of musical time was a non-notated

performance practice essential to living music making, consistently likened to the time of

speech than to clockwork. The French baroque concept of mouvement and the twentieth-

century term “swing” in many senses equated to the nineteenth-century value of rhythm.

Thus, well through the nineteenth century, musical time was directly opposed to

artificiality; for those who could perceive difference, musical rhythm as actualized by

skilled and experienced performing “artists” was a non-automatical quality.

A 1912 article entitled “The Theory of Practicing,” published in The Youth’s

Companion, reaffirmed the distinctly human values of musical movement to the student

pianist:

To mention the ways in which you ought not to practice, it may be said first that

you should never allow yourself to be a mere machine. To become a human

metronome, or a mechanical device clicking out notes, it to build a wall across the

path of progress; to set a limit of proficiency beyond which you cannot pass. The

musician like the elocutionist, lessens his chances of success by the habit of

“parroting.”136

In 1910, an anonymous writer in the London Times succinctly explained the true utility of

the metronome, describing its defects in relationship to internalized understandings of

musical time. He summarized many of the reservations expressed in the past century

regarding automatical values when applied to creative performance:

135 C. A. B., “George Grove’s Analyses of Beethoven,” The Musical World, Nov. 3, 1888, 850.

136

“The Theory of Practicing,” The Youth's Companion, Feb. 1, 1912, 66.

Page 191: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

170

On the whole the dictum may be allowed to stand, with the insistently necessary

proviso that there are many different kinds of irregularity—and also many

different kinds of emotion. Or we might confine ourselves to a negative definition

and use the term “non-metronomic rhythm.” The Metronome is a useful aid for

unmusical children (though even then, unless it is set so as to synchronize with

the shortest notes played, all kinds of things may, and frequently do, happen

between its beats); but otherwise it is a pure snare and delusion.137

Like Joseph Hofmann and Edward Baxter Perry, this writer warned against the constant

use of any clockwork mechanism, which imparted an artificial hegemony over the

performer’s natural sense of musical time:

But it is necessary to try playing to a metronome and to feel its extraordinarily

hampering effect, in order to realize how non-metronomic (even if only slightly

so) good performances ordinarily are; and as soon as the metronomic yoke is

broken, however microscopically and momentarily, we have something which,

whether it be good or bad, is [known today as] tempo rubato.138

“Rubato” remained an insufficient term for what seemed to be a standard quality of

musical time. All human action, many argued, was in a type of rubato when compared to

an artificial clock reference. Louis Charles Elson explained, in his treatise Mistakes and

Disputed Points in Music and Music Teaching (1910), of “Tempo Rubato: This irregular

or, more properly, elastic tempo has many disputed points associated with it. Its very

name, ‘Rubato,’ is an error, since the time is not ‘stolen’ or even transferred from note to

137 “Rhythm and tempo rubato,” The Times, April 16, 1910, 13. The word delusion carries more

weight and meaning than commonly understood today. The critic’s wording is highly germane.

See the 1913 Webster’s Dictionary, 387: “Delusion n. [L. delusio, fr. deludere. See Delude.] 1.

The act of deluding; deception; a misleading of the mind. Pope. 2. The state of being deluded or

misled. 3. That which is falsely or delusively believed or propagated; false belief; error in

belief…Syn. -- Delusion, Illusion. These words both imply some deception practiced upon the

mind. Delusion is deception from want of knowledge; illusion is deception from morbid

imagination. An illusion is a false show, a mere cheat on the fancy or senses. It is, in other words,

some idea or image presented to the bodily or mental vision which does not exist in reality. A

delusion is a false judgment, usually affecting the real concerns of life. Or, in other words, it is an

erroneous view of something which exists indeed, but has by no means the qualities or attributes

ascribed to it.”

138

“Rhythm and tempo rubato,” The Times, April 16, 1910, 13.

Page 192: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

171

note.”139

Elson continued to describe this common performance practice with an

historical anecdote:

Tempo Rubato, therefore, means elasticity and not distortion. It is the very life-

blood of some modern [Romantic] music, as Chopin showed sometimes when

Mme. Dudevant caused him to play when he was not in the mood. He would then

perform one of his compositions in strict and exact time, and the guests would

soon perceive that he had given the body without the soul.140

Johannes Brahms expressed an equal resentment of the metronome and an identical

recognition that the phenomenon known by some as “rubato” was, in fact, the standard

practice of expressive or “soulful” performance. “The so-called ‘elastic’ tempo is

moreover not a new invention,” Brahms remarked, “‘Con discrezioné should be added to

that as to many other things.”141

In a letter to the eminent conductor, singer, and

composer Georg Henschel, Brahms continued to describe the use and value of the

metronome for his compositions:

Your question strikes me as rather indefinite,—whether the metronome marks

before the different movements of my Requiem should be strictly adhered to?

Why, just as well as those to be found before other music. I am of the opinion that

metronome marks go for nothing. As far as I know, all composers have as yet

retracted their metronome marks in later years. Those figures which can be found

before some of my compositions—good friends have talked them into me; for I

139 Louis C. Elson, Mistakes and Disputed Points in Music and Music Teaching (Philadelphia:

Theodore Presser Co., 1910), 95.

140

Ibid, 96.

141 A letter from Brahms to Henschel reprinted in Carl Van Vechten, In the Garret (New York:

Alfred A Knopf, 1920), 218.

Paderewski commented similarly to Brahms, that music performances throughout history

contained a temporal quality that many could describe as being in “Tempo Rubato.” He stated: “It

is older than the Romantic school, it is older than Mozart, it is older than Bach. Girolamo

Frescobaldi, in the beginning of the seventeenth century, made ample use of it.” See Elson, 96-97.

Reprinted from Henry T. Fink, Success in Music and How It’s Won (New York: Charles

Scribner’s Sons, 1909).

Page 193: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

172

myself have never believed that my blood and a mechanical instrument go very

well together.142

Clarence Grant Hamilton, an Associate Professor of Music at Wellesley College, in his

1910 treatise Piano Teaching, Its Principles and Problems, concurred with Christiani and

Brahms regarding the utility of the metronome in education and performance—and that

the phenomenon of rubato was part and parcel of the customary, “elastic” quality of good

musicianship. Furthermore, Hamilton’s instructions echoed Hummel’s words from over

80 years prior:

The pupil must be taught, however, that rhythmic verve is a more important factor

in determining this spirit, and that, therefore, a vital performance does not

necessitate the pushing of the speed to the metronome requirements. Disregard the

metronome marks in teaching, therefore, except as a general indication, and let

the rare of performance be kept rigidly within the pupil’s abilities.

It has been suggested that the metronome should be used only sparingly in

the performance of actual compositions (page 72). The reason for such restriction

is that the metronome produces an inflexible tempo, and that even in the most

formally rhythmic compositions some allowance should be made for variation in

this respect. The grace and artistry of personal performance is dependent upon the

subordination of all elements to the sense of expression, and it is this flexibility of

treatment which distinguishes the work of a pianist from that of a machine.143

Concurring with Brahms, Henschel, and a host of “artistic” musicians of past decades, the

anonymous writer of the 1910 Times article also found that the metronome merely

dictated an automatical mode of time while imposing an oppressive “yoke” upon creative

freedom—hampering both the composer’s and performer’s figurative “blood.” The

metronome was not intended for the interpretation of Brahms’ Requiem, Beethoven’s

142 C. A. B., “George Grove’s Analyses of Beethoven,” The Musical World, Nov. 3, 1888, 850.

Also Vechten, 218. Vechten claims that Brahms wrote this letter to Henschel. The Musical World

states that Brahms wrote about his metronome indications “at the request of a well-known

London conductor.”

Clarence Grant Hamilton, Piano Teaching, Its Principles and Problems (Boston: Oliver Ditson

Company, 1910), 85. Of rubato, Hamilton states: “The slight variation caused by the rubato

accent (page 78) is our first instance of tempo modification.”

Page 194: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

173

symphonies, or the meaningful performance of any composer’s music for that matter. “In

the proper and subtle sense of the words,” the Times writer explained, “free rhythm

applies to all music alike. It is not a morbid abnormality; it is the natural outcome of the

artistic temperament.”144

The article continues:

Many perhaps fail to grasp this [quality of tempo flexibility and rhythmic

variability in performance], simply through conceiving rubato only as something

uncommon and extreme; they do not see that the particular kinds of very palpably

flexible rhythm to which they would restrict the term are nothing but the natural

and inevitable extensions of a principle applicable, in a greater or lesser degree, to

the performance of 99 out of a hundred compositions of almost any age or style—

so long, that is to say, as the performer is a human being and not a barrel-organ.145

Conclusions in Automatical Culture

Living musicians were not automata, yet when guided by the metronome, musicians

sounded automatical. During Maelzel’s lifetime, to be mechanical meant to be “skilled in

making machines.” By 1913, a new and ominous usage of the term appeared in the

Webster’s Unabridged Dictionary, citing “Mechanical…3. Done as if by machine;

uninfluenced by will or emotion; proceeding automatically, or by habit, without special

intention or reflection; as mechanical singing; mechanical verses; mechanical service.”

As many argued, the metronome created mechanically sounding music, thus it

correspondingly changed an essentially human quality of musical time. As suggested by

the 1913 Webster’s dictionary, a musician under the influence of Maelzel’s metronome

was a “machine…3. A person who acts mechanically or at will of another.”146

A century

earlier, Maelzel’s audiences would have recognized such a figure as a mere automaton.

144 “Rhythm and tempo rubato,” The Times, April 16, 1910, 13.

145

Ibid.

146

See Webster’s Unabridged Dictionary (1913), 897, 906.

Page 195: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

174

From the outset, the metronome seemed to be an efficient tool for music

instruction. But over the decades, many heard metronomic time as totally inhuman and

lifeless when compared to traditional and internalized perceptions of musical movement.

When amateur or professional musicians required an external tempo reference, sources

over the century suggested the swing of the simple pendulum, which was silent, finite,

and exclusively visual. The pendulum did not keep musical time with clockwork

consistency. Due to its non-automatical swing, the pendulum was used—and could only

be used—as an initial tempo reference, a supplement for the very human pulse of musical

time. In 1889, a New York Times reviewer remarked, “no one who understands the design

and nature of the metronome marks expects a slavish, mechanical adherence to them, but

they are inserted for the purpose of giving a general indication of the rate of

movement.”147

Likewise, in Ethics and Esthetics of Piano-Playing, a treatise dedicated to

Joseph Hofmann, Constantin von Sternberg remarked in 1917, “it may be said that the

metronome is not intended to regulate the playing of a whole piece, but to give only a

general idea of its average speed. Granted!”148

Sternberg expressed a long-held

understanding of the metronome: The machine was a referential aid for traditional,

variable musical time; it did not define musical time in mechanical absolutes. More

significantly, he maintained that the metronome always failed to reflect the living

qualities of musical time, for compositions past and present. He continued:

147 “Amusements,” New York Times, Mar. 28, 1889, 4.

148

Sternberg, 81. British music critic Ernest Walker echoed Sternberg in 1930: “The rigidity of

the metronome we of course discount; the indication whatever it is, is understood to be both

approximate and fluctuating. Except for contrasts or other special effects, no performance worth

anything, of any music, remains mathematically level, either in time or tone, for more than a very

limited period. This we all take for granted.” See Ernest Walker, “Some Questions of Tempo,”

The Monthly Musical News (December, 1930). Reprinted in Ernest Walker, Free Thought and the

Musician (London: Oxford University Press, 1946), 134-5.

Page 196: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

175

We also have recalled to our minds that such terms as Adagio, Allegro, etc., are

not designations of speed but, primarily, of mood. Then, the metronome has been

“found out” as an artistic impossibility, and has been relegated to its proper

station as a sort of orthophonic adjunct to purely mechanical exercises.149

As Sternberg suggested, modern teachers of his age considered the metronome highly

useful for new pedagogies, grounded in repetitive exercises that drilled a student’s basic

technique. Yet, as a crude teaching tool, the metronome did not dictate a composer’s true

intentions, nor could it document a skilled performer’s nuanced interpretations.

Musicians trained in traditional temporal practices, like Henschel, Perry, Hofmann, and

Sternberg, never equated the pedagogical usefulness of the metronome with the historic

epistemologies of musical time. The efficiency of the machine in teaching fundamental

physical skills to amateurs did not relate to musical tempo and rhythm in performance.

As many over a century argued, the metronome only imparted an artificial and “slavish”

constriction, a “yoke” upon musical interpretation, one against the very nature of current

and past musical performance traditions. Skilled musicians explained that the objective,

externalized clockwork-click was irreconcilable with the subjective, internalized sense of

movement, meter, or pulse. Sternberg continued his assessment of the device:

A metronome should be in every teacher's studio, to be judiciously employed in

the rudimentary mechanical work; but as soon as his pupil touches a composition

away with it! For tempo is an individual matter; it must be felt, or it will not

convince.150

149

Sternberg, 44.

150

Ibid., 82. Sternberg acknowledged the binary opposition between internal, subjective musical

time and external clockwork: “What matters it whether a person who plays a piece without

understanding does it in the right or wrong tempo? I, personally, would ten times rather listen to a

wrong tempo with a conviction back of it, however erroneous it might be, than to a right tempo

dictated by a machine or, for that matter, by any influence extraneous to the player's mind.”

Page 197: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

176

As a host of comments in this reception history verify, a human uninfluenced by

subjective reason, spirit, or sensation became the incarnation of a Maelzel automaton—

the very definition of a mindless, heartless machine. Nineteenth-century critics,

performers, and pedagogues consistently found anyone habitually behaving with

automatical qualities was mere or insufficient in intellectual, physical, and expressive

capacity. In his Life of Beethoven, Schindler clearly recognized a crisis in performance

practices, a distinct change brought about by mechanical motion. “It must be borne in

mind that Beethoven’s instrumental music has undergone a metamorphosis,” Schindler

admitted, “it is necessary first to acquaint the reader that this metamorphosis relates

wholly and solely to metronomising, or the regulation of time by means of the

metronome.”151

As these many criticisms describe, Maelzel’s metronome made possible a

new aesthetic, an utterly insufficient and mechanical performance style for music

compositions never inspired by the beat-per-minute regulation of the machine’s click.

Maelzel’s career, taken in its entirety, was dedicated to one vision: the replication

of subjective human actions in the repetitive motion of clockwork. Nevertheless, for

nearly a century, the reception and recollection of Maelzel’s machines exposed them to

be mere mechanical copies of more complicated, living creations. Continual

acknowledgments described Maelzel’s trumpeter as a mere automatical soloist; the

Panharmonicon as a mere automatical orchestra; and the cold, unfeeling chess player as

an eerily human thinker (since, underneath the deceptive artificial façade, he was human).

As sources attest, the nineteenth century witnessed Maelzel’s clockwork copies not only

for their ingenuity and potential utility, but also for their overwhelming insufficiency

when compared to the complexity of human nature itself.

151 Schindler, ed. Moscheles, 97.

Page 198: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

177

Recognizing that automatical machines influenced and altered living rhythm and

musical time, a few perceptive musicians knew that Maelzel’s mechanical culture directly

extended to the metronome—that automata and tempo-clocks shared an identical origin,

both in design and application. Constantin von Sternberg intuited that Maelzel’s

renowned automata-career corresponded precisely to the mechanic’s posthumously

successful metronome, and in 1917 he offered a damning assessment of the tempo-

machine in a brief critical biography of Maelzel:

How any musician could ever play with a metronome, passes my humble

understanding. It is not only an inartistic, but a downright antiartistic instrument.

In order to prove this and to explain the vogue it has had in spite of it, we must

regard the inventor a little closer and consider the time in which the misfortune of

his invention happened. Maelzel was the son, not of a musician, but of an organ-

builder. His father delighted in contriving all sorts of queer “stops” which, of

course, no organist wanted or even accepted. These queer stops accumulated in

his shop until it was a regular museum of such musical eccentricities. His son

inherited this trait. He “learned” music, but never played in public or composed

anything. He did not even teach music, but only “gave lessons.” How much his

pupils could have learned from him we can infer from the fact that he spent all his

leisure time upon things that go against the very grain of every one who loves

music. He constructed all kinds of mechanical instruments, such as an automaton

trumpeter, a mechanical orchestra, a “panharmonium,” I believe also a

mechanical chess-player and the metronome!152

Despite Foucaud’s assertion in Illustrious Mechanics, Maelzel’s reputation as automata-

exhibitor would not be forgotten. For Sternberg and others spanning a century, the

metronome reflected the very same qualities witnessed in all of Maelzel’s self-moving

machines. The metronome was yet another Maelzel automaton, one that aided in the

actualization of automatical behavior.

Maelzel’s legacy is far more profound than his only remaining “useful” invention,

the metronome, might suggest. Maelzel influenced such varied and diverse figures as

152 Sternberg, 79-80. Sternberg continues: “From all this it is easily seen that the impulse leading

to the invention did not come from an artistic temperament.”

Page 199: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

178

Beethoven, Napoleon, Poe, Daniel Webster, P. T. Barnum, and Oliver Wendell Holmes

for one significant reason: Maelzel’s career, in its totality, was dedicated to the promotion

and exhibition of mechanical facsimile. Yet these important figures, along with many

other creative minds in the nineteenth century, found something deeper in his life’s work.

As insufficient copies of humanity, Maelzel’s devices represented for many the antitheses

of creativity, individuality, volition, and expression. Simply stated, Maelzel’s creations

represented the mechanization of life. In Maelzel’s values, thinkers such as Holmes,

Parker, Webster, Ure, and Taylor witnessed the possibilities of a mechanized future,

especially in industry and labor, where men could indeed act automatically through

methodically efficient and redundant training. Yet nineteenth-century performance

culture was not as enthusiastic regarding Maelzel’s machines, or the mechanization of

artistic creation. The sounds of automata were both mindless and monotonous, two

artificial values that did not serve the goals of musical expression or creation. For artistic

endeavors, automatical culture was nothing to be prized.

Yet, automatical music training expanded in the nineteenth century, as the

metronome defined a new clockwork paradigm of musical time that effaced the living

qualities of rhythm for amateur music students, and eventually professionals, across the

world. Through a slavish belief in mechanical time, living performers and ensembles

actually transformed into musical machines, as they mimicked metronomes, barrel

organs, wooden androids, and orchestrions. Indeed, the Encyclopedia Britannica found

that the clockwork metronome “reduced to mere mechanics what formerly rested wholly

on the performer’s feeling.”153

The metronome recast musicians—their nuanced

153 Speaking to the Royal Musical Association on December 8, 1891, Edgar F. Jacques

concluded similarly, that the adherence to metronome indications was in no small part responsible

Page 200: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

179

rhythmical actions and perceptions—into a shallow clockwork imitation. Subjectivity,

variability, and metric pulsation were lost and forgotten—“metamorphosed”—in the

heartless click of a tempo-clock. The metronome, therefore, was the very embodiment of

Maelzel’s mechanical culture: the metronome mechanized music making.

Perhaps musicians and scholars trained in twentieth-century traditions would have

no trouble concurring with nineteenth-century critics who heard lifelessness in Maelzel’s

clockwork trumpeter and Panharmonicon. Maelzel’s mechanical contrivances with their

redundant barrel-organ tunes could never have sounded with any semblance of human

spirit or expression. And yet today, countless performers, pedagogues, and researchers

continue to reference Maelzel’s other automaton, the metronome, exclusively for the time

of pre-twentieth-century music, while many even consider metronomic rhythm to define

the absolute movement and pulse of music performance itself. In every music

conservatory, studio, and concert hall, his mechanical creation continues to resonate in

some fashion. The next chapters document how Western society came to inherit and

embrace metronomic values after all, for the “far-famed” Maelzel did, over time,

successfully bequeath two lasting legacies of his automata-display culture to our modern

world: First, that the illusion of human expression and intelligence can be imparted into

mere, mindless automata; and second, that the delusion of artificial time can be slavishly

imposed upon expressive, thinking musicians through mere, automatical metronomes.

for the reduction of a performer’s individual expression, invention, and creativity in the concert

hall by the last decade of the century. Jacques notes: “I need only ask you to remember how the

progress [of music publication] has all along been in the direction of definiteness…how, finally,

marks of expression were added and words used to indicate speed, these being supplemented later

by metronome-marks. We have, indeed, so clipped the wings of the executant that we no longer

expect extemporized cadenzas, either in an opera or in a concerto.” See Edgar F. Jacques, “The

Composer’s Intention,” in Proceedings of the Musical Association, 18th Sess. (1891 - 1892): 42.

Page 201: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

180

CHAPTER III: THE SCIENTIFIC METRONOME

Chronography, the Clockwork Metronome, and the Personal Equation:

An Introduction

During Maelzel’s lifetime, the professional specialization that required the most

meticulous adherence to precise mechanical timekeeping, down to fractions of a second,

was not music composition, improvisation, teaching, or performance, but astronomical

observation. While skilled musicians judged musical time through a combination of

physical intuition, sensory perception, and living experience, professional astronomers

observed the sky with near inhuman precisions, gauging the movements of stars and other

heavenly phenomena through second-by-second reference. In the high-technology

observatory of the early nineteenth-century, astronomers made the most finite

observations and assessments of stellar transits, which they viewed through a high-

powered telescope and referenced by an ultra-accurate sidereal clock. Stoddard’s

Encyclopedia Americana (1884) explained the specialized astronomer’s complex series

of assessments and reactions—constituting his performance in precision, mechanical

time—typical for any stellar observation in the first half of the nineteenth century:

The usual plan for noting the time of any celestial phenomenon was for the

observer to listen to the beats of his sidereal clock; to keep count of the minutes,

and estimate the space passed over by the object during the interval between the

beats; to mentally divide this space into tenths, and record in his note-book the

minutes, seconds, and tenths, meanwhile counting the clock-beats that not one be

lost. All this must be repeated at the bisection of the object with each of the

several wires (spider-lines) in the held of view of his telescope, sometimes as

many as ten or more being employed. Even by this wearisome method skilled

observers, after years of practice, have been able to record the time of transits to

within one-tenth of a second, though it was more frequently to one-fifth of a

second.”1

1 J. M. Stoddart, Stoddart’s Encyclopedia Americana, Volume II (New York: J. M Stoddart,

1884), 88.

Page 202: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

181

With these exact scientific measurements, astronomers were, in essence, the time tellers

for the whole of modern civilization, mapping the heavens by hours, days, months, and

years and translating this data into time standards necessary for calendars, navigational

charts, and other systems of time-reckoning. Over the century, astronomical observatories

provided the ultimate reference for the time of cities, nations, and industries.

By the end of the eighteenth century, astronomical clocks were so ingeniously

constructed and meticulously maintained that significant errors seldom occurred in the

machines themselves. Although observatory clocks still needed careful calibration,

ultimately referenced through astronomers’ observations, the mechanisms were so regular

and precise that they typically did not lose more than a tenth of a second per day.2

Astronomers, however, were far more variable, and their non-mechanical perceptions of

stellar events, transits, and eclipses varied greatly depending upon the time, place, and

person; despite the infinitesimal accuracy required of their observations, astronomers

readings—their personal interpretations of natural phenomena—seldom corresponded

exactly to one another when measured against precision clock-time.

Astronomers dubbed the complex human process of perception and reaction when

recording an astronomical observation the “eye and ear method.” For scientific purposes,

in which objective certainties mattered above personal interpretations, nineteenth-century

astronomers began realizing this highly subjective method, hinging on an individual’s

variable senses of sight and sound, was insufficient for the vital requirements of their

science. More than 1/5 of a second difference between astronomers’ measurements of the

2 See G. J. Whitrow, Time in History (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1988), ii. Whitrow’s

chart of time-telling precision is based on the research of F. A. B. Ward.

Page 203: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

182

same stellar events displayed great personal errors, mistakes that could ultimately transfer

to official star charts, time tables, almanacs, civic clocks, and other essential time-

references. Thus, the eye and ear method created an important problem in science to be

solved: If heavenly bodies moved at an objective rate across the telescope, and the

sidereal clock was an objective, infallible temporal reference, then only the astronomer

was at fault in the scientific method of astronomical observation. In nineteenth-century

astronomy, the human observer, not the clockwork machine, represented the overriding

problem in performance practices.

British Royal Astronomer Nevil Maskelyne (1732-1811) first expressed the

“problem” of an astronomer’s personal perception in 1796 at the Greenwich Observatory.

Maskelyne’s assistant David Kinnebrook recorded times of transits up to eight-tenths of a

second different than Maskelyne. The differences between their measurements continued

to appear, and Kinnebrook failed to close the supposed margin of error. Maskelyne could

not abide such blatant mistakes from his assistant. (It seems that Maskelyne did not

consider himself as a source of error.) So in 1796 Kinnebrook was fired.

This story came to the attention of Friedrich Wilhelm Bessel (1784-1846),

German astronomer who founded the Königsberg observatory in 1813. The variables

exposed by the eye and ear method greatly concerned Bessel, who wished to quantify and

define the errors of human observation when compared to precise, mechanical time. By

1819, while Maelzel continued promoting a series of high-profile automata shows

featuring “Kempelen’s celebrated Automaton Chess Player, and Maelzel’s Automaton

Page 204: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

183

Trumpeter” in London at the admission price of “2s. 6d.,” 3 Bessel had begun

methodically recording human differences in fractions of mechanical seconds in order to

find astronomers’ individual errors, their differences in perceiving the same objective

events. Bessel, and all professional astronomers and experimental psychologists after

him, dubbed the human variation in time and action the personal equation—the errors

intrinsic (i.e. natural) to subjective human perception and reaction which were exposed

through methodical, mechanical testing.

Experimental psychologist Edward Wheeler Scripture colorfully retold this vital

discovery to the modern sciences in his text Thinking, Feeling, Doing (1895):

Finally, after more experience, astronomers in general reached the conclusion that

everybody disagreed with everybody else. Moreover, men who disagreed in one

way at one time would be likely to disagree differently at another time; so that a

man did not even agree with himself. As this was evidently not the fault of the

star, the conclusion was finally reached that each person had a peculiar error of

his own. This was called by the queer name, “personal equation.”4

3 See the advertisements, “Kempelen’s Celebrated Automaton Chess-Player,” Times Jan. 7, 1819,

1, col. A; and “Maelzel’s Exhibition,” Times, Feb. 24, 1819, 1, col. A. By June 1819, Maelzel

promoted “The ORCHESTRION, and a MOVING PANORAMA of the CONFLAGRATION OF

MOSCOW...The Orchestrion will perform, solely by mechanism, several grand Compositions by

Handel, Haydn, Beethoven, Cherubini, &c. daily, at 2, 3, and 4 o’clock.” See Times, June 3,

1819, 1, col A.

On April 16, 1819, Beethoven sent a letter to Ferdinand Ries, also in London, explaining

the tempi to the movements of Op. 106 with Maelzel’s metronome scale. Beethoven, Letters, II,

ed. Anderson, Letter 940, 806. There is a distinct possibility, therefore, that on his trips as famed

automata showman, Maelzel distributed (perhaps free of charge) new metronomes to Beethoven’s

collaborators and publishers. But the details of this history are not in the scope of the present

study.

One year prior, in 1818, Maelzel’s metronome appears in a list of French inventions

which also includes his Panharmonicon and another unnamed “instrument.” See J. R. Armonville,

Le guide des artistes or Répertoire des arts et manufanctures (Paris: Chaingnieau, 1818), 260,

261.

4 E. W. Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing (Meadville: The Chautauqua-Century Press, 1895),

40.

Page 205: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

184

With the initial hope of defining and reducing these personal-equation errors in

astronomy, Bessel discovered that no true objectivity was possible in an observer’s

perception of time and movement when gauged by the clock. Psychologist and historian

Edwin Boring recounted Bessel’s important contribution to the modern, nineteenth-

century sciences, stating, “Thus Bessel may be said to have discovered not only the

personal equation, but also its variability. He found a gross error, and he showed that one

could not reliably ‘calibrate the observer’ in order to correct for it.”5 Bessel could not

simply fix the errors of the personal equation even when he documented them. He found

with scientific certainty that astronomers did not, and could not, consistently conform to

clockwork precisions delineated by fractions of a second.

Since humans could not perform up to the goals of nineteenth-century astronomy,

which now required the observer to perceive and react far beyond normal tolerances of

either the mind or body, astronomers sought to solve the personal equation through

clockwork machines themselves. By the 1850s, while a middle-class musical culture

became more familiar with Maelzel’s metronome as a novel, and potentially useful

pedagogical tool for children and amateurs, a new metronomic machine for professional

astronomers—the observatory chronograph—had actively improved the science of

astronomy in the nineteenth century. The chronograph recorded and regulated the exact

time of the observer’s perception of a stellar transit; through the sound of metronomic

time, the chronograph ultimately corrected for the astronomer’s “problem,” his personal

equation during a performance.

5 Edwin G. Boring, A History of Experimental Psychology (New York: Appleton-Century

Company, 1929), 137.

Page 206: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

185

Engineers of The United States Coast Survey invented the observatory

chronograph in 1850, and the new process of high-precision chronographic recording was

initially dubbed “The American Method.”6 The Royal Observatory at Greenwich,

consistently at the forefront of precise time-telling practices, installed an American-style

chronograph in 1854, in what astronomers and science historians recognize as a major

development in time reckoning for Western civilization.7 The text Stargazing Past and

Present (1878) published an early image of the machine, which consisted of a pendulum-

clock dictating seconds-—a very large, ultra-accurate clicking metronome—with a

barrel-drum recording device.8 The chronograph recorded two events in time for any

astronomical reading: a temporal standard and a variable. One needle traced the

objective, mechanical second (and in later models, fractions of the second) defined by the

pendulum-clock through an unwavering pattern drawn on paper. The other needle printed

the interpretation of the observation: the astronomer’s reading of the heavenly transit,

referenced through the sound of ticking seconds, which he marked by a key-stroke,

electrically connected to the chronograph. Thus, an analysis of the readout showed the

6 E. C. Sanford, “Personal Equation,” The American Journal of Psychology 2 (November, 1889):

19-20. Also recounted in Boring, 139.

7 David S. Landes, in his highly important and influential study of time-telling Revolution in Time

(1983, reprint 2000), devotes little discussion to the nineteenth-century astronomical chronograph

and its impact. He opts instead to document the chronograph complication of nineteenth-century

pocket watches, the “stop-watch” feature used to time precise durations. Yet Landes failed to

realize that the handheld “chronograph (or sometimes chronoscope), whose primary function is

to measure elapsed time” of pulse-rates or horse races served a significantly different purpose

than the observatory chronograph, which acted as a second-by-second regulator and recorder of

action. The pocket-watch chronograph, which only became publicly available beginning in the

1860s, and the wristwatch chronograph of today are referential measurers of exact duration only

and do not structure or document the performer’s actions in precise, constant intervals of time.

See Landes, 3, 457 n35.

8 J. Norman Lockyer, Stargazing Past and Present (London: Macmillan and Co., 1878), 261.

Page 207: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

186

human’s interpretation, his errors due to the personal equation and when they occurred, to

within fractions of the invariable mechanical second.9 With a visible record of the

astronomer’s observation-performance in precision, repetitive time, the inconsistencies of

any individual’s perception and reaction could be accounted for and corrected by the

chronograph. The eye and ear method, along errors of personal equation, could now be

seen, controlled, and reduced with precise metronomic rhythm for the most precise

scientific observations. Thus astronomers’ performances in the Greenwich Observatory—

well before musicians’ performances in the concert hall and the music parlor—

necessitated constant, unwavering reference and strict regulation through metronomic

time.

9 Boring, 139-140.

Page 208: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

187

Fig 3.1. The Greenwich Chronograph. The right portion of the schematic depicts the pendulum clock

beating seconds, which was electrically recorded by the drum-barrel on the left. From Stoddart’s

Encyclopedia Americana (1884), 261.

Page 209: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

188

Stoddart’s Encyclopedia Americana (1884) recounted the boon of the

chronometer to astronomical observatories across the world, since the mechanical “time-

recorder” replaced the “eye and ear method, as it is called, which required the observer’s

powers of attention to be distracted beyond what they are able to bear.”10

The

encyclopedia continues:

The chronographic [method], which has almost entirely superseded the old “eye-

and-ear method,” relieves the observer of all anxiety regarding the time, the

clock-beats, and the making of the record in his note-book, and enables him to

give his undivided attention to the observation of the transits, which, under the

other method with its attendant difficulties, he was, to a certain extent, prevented

from doing.11

More than relieving the observer of any anxiety when thinking and acting with near

impossible mechanical precisions, the chronograph succeeded, according to Boring, in

making entirely negligible the effects of human perception itself. Indeed, observatories

employed the machine to “do away with the observer” in professional astronomy.12

Boring reports, “In the first two years of the use of the chronograph [1854-6], the

Greenwich observatory discovered that the instrument reduced the personal equation to

less than one tenth of a second, the goal of precision that the astronomers had originally

been seeking.”13

Through metronomic regulation, the chronograph reduced imprecise,

personal variations of time and perception to clockwork certainty. In nineteenth-century

astronomy first, metronomic time superseded and even subjugated personal

10 Stoddart, 88.

11

Ibid.

12

Boring, 133.

13

Ibid., 140.

Page 210: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

189

interpretations in sight and sound for objective, scientific aims. In 1889 the American

Journal of Psychology praised the introduction of the chronograph to the new nineteenth-

century sciences of physiology and experimental psychology, in laboratories where the

objective sound of clockwork ticked away, marking precision time for humans in a host

of scientific studies using a technique intended “to do away with the observer himself.”14

While astronomy first exploited the potential for constant metronomic regulation

in the astronomical observatory, the scientific applications of metronomic time—the vital

element of the chronographic method—expanded well into the twentieth century in ways

completely unrelated to the subjective, creative practices of skilled musicians from pre-

metronomic traditions. As evidence in the fields of both science and music pedagogy

strongly suggest, the metronome’s home in the second half of the nineteenth century—

more than on the skilled performer’s piano or the composer’s desk—was in newly

established university laboratories across the world. Indeed, the new nineteenth-century

scientists, those directly influenced by the chronographic method of astronomers,

embraced the metronome well before professional performers found the tempo clock of

any meaningful value as a regulator or reference for musical time and rhythm. Despite

Maelzel’s expressed intentions for the device, the clockwork metronome thrived within

an unlikely community, one separated from music pedagogy, composition, and

publication, as it became a regulatory machine intended for precision-oriented scientific

research and experimentation. Using the metronome as a modern laboratory apparatus,

scientists—not composers or experienced performers—first valued Maelzel’s machine as

a precision-oriented timekeeper and chronographic regulator. Employed by scientists for

14 Sanford, 24.

Page 211: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

190

precise temporal measurement, reference, and regulation in controlled settings, Maelzel’s

metronome became the scientific metronome: a chronographic apparatus dictating and

defining objective, invariable, and inhuman time for the quantification, reduction, and

even retraining of the various individual traits that comprise the personal equation.

In the second half of the nineteenth-century, scientific experiments in both Europe

and North America used the increasingly accessible metronome in the same fashion as

the large “seconds-clock” of the 1854 Greenwich chronograph. It is this scientific

metronome, applied for scientific ends, which provides us with the understanding of the

metronome as it became used in the last years of the nineteenth century, when both

scientists and pedagogues recorded and regulated a host of human activities through

metronomic rhythm—a modern trend that affected music instruction and performance

practices most conspicuously. Indeed, by the turn of the century, the scientific

metronome and the observatory chronograph were indistinguishable; fueled by efficient

scientific pedagogies, the metronome represented the infallible mechanical-temporal

reference for an industrializing culture, one that devalued and reduced the personal

equation in a variety of human performances.

The Clockwork Metronome Emerges in the Nineteenth-Century Sciences

Chronography, in the mid-nineteenth century, was a unique technical process to

professional astronomy, with the most exacting practices of mechanical time

measurement in relation to human action. Prior to the founding of experimental

psychology in 1875, the measurements of physical and mental action in constant

metronomic time seemed excessively precise for most professional activities.

Nevertheless, other nineteenth-century scientists readily considered Maelzel’s metronome

Page 212: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

191

a vital asset in regulating and recording a host of studies, and it seems that both

professional and amateur scientists embraced the metronome to a far greater extent—with

far less contention—than skilled musicians in the nineteenth century, many of whom

suggested that the simple pendulum was a vastly more appropriate mechanical pulse

reference. While creative musicians valued subjective interpretation in anti-metronomic

time throughout the century, many scientists realized the clockwork metronome provided

an objective, regular rhythm for their methodical and repetitive experiments.

Isolated evidence of the scientific applications of the metronome appear as early

as the 1830s, as some physiologists began rehabilitating patients for a variety of physical

and mental ailments by regulating their human movements to clockwork. One early

example is seen in the methods of renowned French doctor Armand Trousseau, who, in

the search to cure chorea, had his patients perform gymnastic exercises alternately to the

swing of a simple pendulum, a cuckoo clock, or click of the metronome.15

In 1858 the

specialist publication Half-Yearly Abstracts of the Medical Sciences makes clear,

however, that Trousseau’s most mechanized treatment was highly experimental, even

while chronography, at the time of this publication, was beginning to successfully efface

the errors of astronomers’ performances:

15 Armande Trousseau, Lectures on clinical medicine, translated by P. V. Bazire, Volume I

(London: Robert Hardwicke, 1867), 413-414, 428.

Page 213: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

192

M. Trousseau has found a very useful substance in the employment of a

metronome, the number of the oscillations of which can be regulated, each being

indicated by a clicking sound. If one of these be placed before the patient, the

sight16

and sound of the oscillations become of great utility in assisting him in

regulating the movements of the whole or a part of the body. It is, however, only

an adjuratory means, and is more useful in the case of tic, the patient being

directed to produce his tic voluntarily at the same time the click of the instrument

is heard. He becomes fatigued by this voluntary effort, and the tic is speedily

modified, though rarely cured.17

After Trousseau’s experiments, other doctors prescribed automatical actions for their

ailing patients. Perhaps inspired by the famed exhibitor of automata, physiologist M.

Colombat designed a clockwork machine that the American Journal of Dental Science

(1844) reported, “acts somewhat like Maelzel’s metronome, but is constructed on another

principle, and will go for several days without being wound up.” Colombat applied this

metronome-like device, the muthonome, in the chronographic treatment of stammering,

where the patients “give utterance at first to 60 or 80 in a minute, which number he

gradually increases to 160 or 180 in the same space of time.” 18

The machine, as this

source makes clear, was not Maelzel’s metronome and could run for an extended time

that no musician would consider an appropriate reference for expressive, meaningful

performance.

16 This comment verifies that Maelzel’s metronome was also a visual reference of pulse. The

swaying pendulum of the metronome had rhythmic significance beyond the machine’s audible

click.

17

W. H. Ranking and C. B. Radcliffe, editors, Half-Yearly Abstract of the Medical Sciences

XXVII (Jan.-June, 1858): 73. It seems that the metronome click, not the visual pendulum swing,

influenced the patient’s reactions. As Chapter V exposes, psychologists through the first half of

the twentieth century found that the monotonous metronome click had a definitive effect on

subjects’ mental and physical reactions.

18

American Society of Dental Surgeons, “French Medical Societies,” American Journal of

Dental Science IV (Dec., 1843): 127.

Page 214: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

193

Besides these seemingly unusual treatments, evidence strongly suggests that

British physiologists were the first to consistently and successfully employ Maelzel’s

metronome in the nineteenth-century sciences, most often for regulating nerve impulses

and breathing rates of animal subjects. British physiologist Benjamin W. Richardson, the

“Senior Physician to the Royal Infirmary for Diseases of the Chest,”19

had claimed to be

operating on animals with aid of an electrified metronome of his own design by 1853, the

same year that astronomers installed the chronograph at Greenwich. Richardson recalled

ten years later that his modified metronome apparatus (which again offered far greater

regulation than was possible in a wind-up metronome), “made its balance strike against a

spring on either side in perfect time and order. I connected two rods with this to which

the poles of a battery might be attached; when the battery was in action, and its poles

connected with the metronome, I set the instrument so that twenty, thirty, forty, or any

number of shocks, could be given per minute.”20

In the mid nineteenth century, the general sciences still found an electrical-current

metronome overly precise and unnecessary. The high-technology methods and

apparatuses of astronomers and physiologists did not serve scientists’ and engineers’

needs for measuring brief time periods during simple observations and experiments. As

the electrified clockwork metronome became desirable in the physiology laboratory,

Edward Charles Pickering, the Thayer Professor of Physics in the Massachusetts Institute

19 Benjamin W. Richardson, “Researches on the Treatment of Suspended Animation,” The British

and Foreign Medico-chirurgical Review XXXI (January-April, 1863): 478.

20

Richardson, 495.

Page 215: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

194

of Technology, advised in his Elements of Physical Manipulation (1873) the use of a

“Metronome Pendulum”21

for basic scientific research, suggesting:

Short intervals of time may be roughly measured by a pendulum, made by tying a

stone to a string, or better, by a tape-measure drawn out to a fixed mark…By

graduating the tape properly, we may readily construct a very serviceable

metronome…Where the greatest accuracy is required, as in astronomical

observations, a chronograph is used.22

Similar to Weber’s musical pendulum, Pickering’s “metronome” was merely a simple

pendulum with a graded measuring scale. As confirmed by many nineteenth-century

musical sources, the simple pendulum was a non-regulative tool—a finite visual

reference, not a ticking clock intended to artificially dictate and control action in

perpetuity. The metronome-pendulum displayed a different precision—a different

epistemology of rhythm viewed as a seamless movement—a silent, swinging pulse, far

less automatical or sonically intrusive than the chronograph. Significantly, Pickering

reminds the reader that nineteenth-century astronomy alone required the chronographic

method: the most objective, constant clockwork regulation for the most precise scientific

measurements. Thus it seems that well through the mid-nineteenth century both skilled

musicians and the typical home scientist did not need such high levels of mechanical

regulation in their endeavors; instead, they valued the simple pendulum as a brief

reference of time, not a regulator of action.

At the time of Pickering’s publication, non-specialists in science found the

clockwork metronome difficult to obtain, particularly in America where the metronome

21 Edward C. Pickering, Elements of Physical Manipulation (New York: Hurd and Houghton,

1873), x.

22

Pickering, 16-17.

Page 216: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

195

was an imported and expensive luxury compared with the simple pendulum. An 1874

Scientific American article suggested that the home scientist could easily construct a

metronome by “taking a cheap clock movement, and substituting for the pendulum a wire

with a sliding weight. Mark the wire with a file at the different points of graduation.”23

While failing to describe the specific purposes of the machine in relation to musical

education or performance, the periodical suggests that a clockwork metronome could be

made by anyone.

Throughout the last decades of the nineteenth century the metronome became a

more available and reliable apparatus, and a variety of scientists and engineers

increasingly employed the clockwork metronome for technical processes in ways entirely

unrelated to musical pedagogy, composition, or performance. In the 1880s even the U. S.

government acquired a Maelzel metronome for scientific purposes. A report of the

“Psychological and Anthropological Apparatus Now Accessible to Students in

Washington D.C., in the Office of the Surgeon General” listed a metronome with the “Set

of Instruments and Materials for Water Measurement” intended for educational training

and research.24

23 Scientific American XXX (Feb. 21, 1874): 123.

It seems that as early as 1816, the mechanically inclined British amateurs had been

constructing basic seconds-beating metronomes out of clocks for their practice. A music

aficionado Samuel Tozer recalled in Mechanics’ Magazine (Jan. 11, 1834) that, “The resident

inhabitants who played on that occasion, had had a rehearsal previous to our arrival, and they so

regulated the pendulum of a common wooden clock as to keep time for them, equal to the best

metronomes which are made by thee noted maker in John’s-street, Tottenham-court-

road…Previous to the Oakhampton occurrence, I was told of a country sawyer, at Crediton, that

he instructed his son on the violin by means of a wooden clock, such as in London is now to be

had for 5s.” See Samuel Tozer, “Cheap Metronomes or Time-Beaters,” Mechanics’ Magazine

XX no. 544 (Jan. 11, 1834): 255.

24

Joseph Jastrow; H. K. Wolfe; W. L. Hervey; Nicholas Murray Butler; William James; George

Trumbull Ladd; James McKeen Cattell; W. L. Bryan; E. C. Sanford; J. Mark Baldwin,

Page 217: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

196

Some technologically minded photographers, too, found Maelzel’s machine

useful when timing exposures in the darkroom.25

Yet British photographer James Cadett

stressed in 1880 that bell metronomes currently sold on the British market were

noticeably less accurate than other timekeeping technologies. Similar to the Scientific

American writer of six years prior, Cadett suggested an alternative, a homemade

“metronome” constructed from the workings of a cheap clock:

I have both used the metronome, and seen it used by others, with great success;

but it is very necessary to see that the metronome beats truly by comparing it first

with a clock. I prefer to let it beat two or three times to the second, and let the bell

indicate the seconds. I do not think that the metronome is reliable when beating

only once to the second. Another method, which is far cheaper and more correct,

is one adopted by a friend of mine. He took one of the cheap Swiss clocks which

may be bought for two shillings or less, and lengthened its pendulum with a piece

of wire till it beat dead seconds. Anything more effective or simple I have not

seen. It was done for timing ordinary exposures in the studio.26

While skilled artist-musicians discounted the metronome for its artificial precision—as

the antithesis to human creativity—technical aficionados such as Cadett criticized the

metronome for the opposite reasons: the standard commercially available metronome was

not mechanically precise enough for use as a constant temporal regulator. British Royal

“Psychology in American Colleges and Universities,” The American Journal of Psychology 3

(Apr., 1890): 280-1.

25

The relationship between the metronomic time, chronography, and photography—the technique

of chronophotography—is not in the scope of this survey. See Stephen Kern, The Culture of Time

and Space, 1880-1918 (Second Edition, Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 2003) for an

introduction to the history of precise, time-lapsed images in modern culture.

26

James Cadett, “ On the Establishment of a Standard Measurement of Sensitiveness, no II,” The

British Journal of Photography XXVII (Oct. 15, 1880): 496.

In the same edition, another writer suggests that a simple pendulum called a

“metronome” could be used to make an instant camera shutter, stating, “If any of my readers have

in their possession a common metronome for timing music, let them replace the lead ball at the

bottom by Mr. Beechey's apparatus, taking care that it has the same weight as the lead ball. In this

manner he will have in his possession an instantaneous shutter, with which he can regulate the

exposure from a second to a fraction of a second.” Ibid., 526.

Page 218: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

197

Engineer Allan Cunningham also recognized that Maelzel’s machine was neither the

most reliable nor most accurate timekeeper available for conducting precision-oriented

measurements. In 1881 he ranked timekeeping machines in relation to their accuracy and

sufficiency:

5°. Timekeeper. A half-second's chronometer is best. Next to this (and nearly as

good) a loud ticking clock (not ticking too quick for the car to follow) with

second's hand. Next a similar clock without second's hand, or else a metronome.

Next a stop-watch.

Next probably (but much inferior) a simple seconds or half-second's

pendulum. And last of all a common watch or clock provided with a second's

hand. [The pendulum may be improvised with a bullet and string: its proper

length must be found by comparison with a good watch or clock through at least a

minute].27

In both Cadett’s and Cunningham’s assessments we see how different professions with

different requirements placed different values on identical machines. For this civic

engineer, the least precise timekeeper was the simple pendulum, while the clockwork

metronome was a vastly better reference for hydraulic experiments (albeit not the best

machine available). Conversely, professional musicians of the time considered Maelzel’s

metronome overly precise, automatical, and artificial in relation to variable, human

performance practices. For many music pedagogues the simple pendulum remained the

most appropriate external time reference because it fundamentally referenced subjective

and physical phenomena, not artificial clockwork. Indeed, influential piano pedagogue

Adolph Friedrich Christiani, confirmed the scientific epistemology of time that grounded

Maelzel’s automatical machine:

27 Allan Cunningham, Roorkee Hydraulic Experiments ([n.p.]: Thomason College Press, 1881),

350.

Page 219: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

198

In principle, the metronome is, mathematically and astronomically, as correct as

any good clock is supposed to be.

The unit being always a minute.

A minute being the 60th part of an hour.

An hour, the 24th part of a day.

A day, the 365th part of a year.

And a year, the time it takes the earth to go around the sun.28

Writing in 1885, Christiani agreed with many scientists and engineers of the decade,

recognizing that the typical clockwork metronome was quite imprecise when compared

to household time-tellers. “In practice, however, a metronome is seldom as correct as a

good clock,” Christiani reiterated. He attested, however, that such chronographic

applications of the metronome had no place in music education and performance, and

commented—in contradistinction to mechanical engineers—“but that [level of accuracy]

is not necessary.”29

In the last years of the nineteenth-century, attitudes regarding the importance of

chronographic precision began to change, even in the field of music education.

Technically minded teachers, like chronographic scientists before them, began to view

the simple pendulum as insufficient when compared to automatical, clockwork

references. The values of scientific measurement and musical pedagogy, diametrically

opposed at least through the 1880s, would eventually merge with metronomic precision

by the turn of the century.

28 Adolph Friedrich Christiani, The Principles of Expression in Pianoforte Playing (New York:

Harper & Brothers, 1885), 262. N.B. He makes no mention of seconds, since the second

continued to be an unusually precise temporal measurement outside of astronomical observatories

and experimental laboratories.

29

Ibid.

Page 220: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

199

Over a decade after Maelzel’s death, clockwork and then electrically modified

metronomes became necessary apparatuses for the field of physiology, a medical science

that technology historians recognize as spawning a “laboratory revolution” in the mid-

century through the incorporation of novel automatic machines.30

In the 1860s, after

Richardson’s seminal experiments with his electrical-current metronome, physiology

departments across Great Britain and North America seemed to rapidly employ the

clockwork metronome during exploratory surgeries, most often on the muscle and nerve-

tissue of frogs.31

British researchers timed the effects of a poisoned frog by the click of

the metronome, as reported in 1867 by the Journal of Anatomy and Physiology.32

In

1876, Johns Hopkins University lists a “metronome, with electrical attachment, giving

contacts from 6 to 200 times in a minute” under a report of “the More Important

Physiological Apparatus in the Biological Laboratory.”33

The same year in Great Britain,

Physiologist James Dewar reported eye experiments where he timed a light source by a

30 Sven Dierig, “Engines for Experiment: Laboratory Revolution and Industrial Labor in the

Nineteenth-Century City,” Osiris 18 (2003): 118. Dierig’s article, while it details the

incorporation of chronographs in German physiology laboratories, makes no mention of the wide-

scale incorporation of electro-magnetic metronomes through the last decades of the nineteenth

century.

31

“Report on Physiology,” British and Foreign Medico-chirurgical Review LXV (Jan., 1864):

233.

32

Thomas R. Fraser, “On the Physiological Action of the Calabar Bean,” Journal of Anatomy and

Physiology I (Nov., 1886): 329.

33

Johns Hopkins University, “List of the More Important Physiological Apparatus in the

Biological Laboratory,” in Report of the President (Baltimore: William K. Boyle & Son, 1876):

62. These Hopkins experiments were reported in H. Newell Martin, “Respiration of the Frog,”

The Journal of Physiology 1 (London: Macmillan and Co, 1878-9): 159.

Page 221: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

200

metronome.34

By 1878 American doctors had timed frogs’ pulse rates during exploratory

operations to the metronome’s beats-per-minute.35

A Syllabus of a course of lectures on

physiology by John Scott Burdon-Sanderson, Professor at University College, London,

included a standard clockwork metronome as the timer for muscle-tissue experiments on

these animals.36

Also in 1878, Johns Hopkins University reported on frog experiments

where “the time in this case, as in all the others, was taken for each tracing separately by

an ordinary magneto-electric chronograph worked by a metronome.”37

Physiology sources also make clear that by 1875 the metronome’s artificial

rhythm had provided the best external regulator of breathing for unconscious animals.

Prior to the 1870s, however, some of the first attempts at artificial respiration in Germany

timed with a clockwork metronome proved to be less successful, since, as science

historian Sven Dierig relates, “no human assistant could retain the [metronomic] rhythm

of respiration throughout a long experiment.”38

But physiologists across Europe and

North America soon solved the problems of human action as part of a metronomic

(artificial) breathing apparatus. Isaac Ott, in the Philadelphia Medical Times (1875),

described an artificial respirator that was powered “at regular intervals, by an electro-

34 James Dewar, “On the Physiological Action of Light,” in Royal Institution of Great Britain,

Proceedings VIII (1875-1878): 147-8.

35

See, for instance, “A Contribution to Our Knowledge of Phytolacca Decandra, and of Grindelia

Rocusta,” in Atlanta Medical and Surgical Journal, J. G. Westmoreland, ed. (Atlanta: H. H.

Dickson, 1878): 478-9.

36

John Scott Burdon-Sanderson, Syllabus of a course of lectures on physiology, Second Edition

(London: K. Lewis, 1879), 133.

37

H. Newell Martin, “Respiration of the Frog,” in Studies from the Biological Laboratory I

(Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University, 1877-1878): 159.

38

Dierig, 127.

Page 222: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

201

magnet and metronome.”39

He recounted his physiological procedures at the University

of Pennsylvania in The Action of Medicines (1875): “By regulating the rapidity of the

metronome,” Ott reports, “the current of air passing into the chest of the animal can be

broken as often as is desired, the breaks being according to the normal respirations of the

animal operated on. This apparatus worked exceedingly rhythmically, and run for about

two hours.”40

Ott suggested that when electric current was not available, a standard

metronome should dictate the time for the technician, who would regularly pump air into

the unconscious animal during a surgical operation.41

Joseph Ketchum’s 1885 patent

filing for an “Apparatus for Producing Artificial Respiration,” offered a similar

suggestion: “A metronome may be used for a [human] patient to breath by and the

attendant to work by, so as to secure synchronous operations, if desirable.”42

39 J. Ott, “Original Communications. Physiological Action of Gelsemia,” Philadelphia Medical

Times, Jul. 31, 1875, 689. American Periodical Series Online,

<http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=726640772&sid=1&Fmt=2&clientId=3338&RQT=309&

VName=HNP>

40

Isaac Ott, The Action of Medicines (Philadelphia: Lindsay & Blakiston, 1878), 26-7.

41

Ott, The Action of Medicines, 27.

42

Joseph Ketchum, “Apparatus for Producing Artificial Respiration,” U. S. Patent #320,070

(Filed Apr. 1, 1885; Patented June 16, 1885): 3.

Page 223: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

202

Figs. 3.2 and 3.3. Mercury contact metronomes for experimental physiological surgeries. Left, from J.

M’Gregor-Robertson, The Elements of Physiological Physics (1884), 71. Right, from John Gray

McKendrick, A Text Book of Physiology (1888), 377.

While the metronome was a boon to physiology as an electro-mechanical

(chronographic) stimulant for muscles and lungs, we should note that Ott’s

“rhythmically” accurate apparatus was not “rhythmical” to the skilled music performer:

the scientific metronome, to the nineteenth-century musical artist, was a mere, unthinking

machine with no living “rhythm” whatsoever. This scientific “rhythm” was of a

decidedly different, metronomic species. Yet, such mechanical “rhythm” would be

explained and actualized in laboratories throughout the nineteenth century. This

experimental rhythm, exclusively defined and dictated by the metronome, eventually

became the fundamental rhythm of modern education as well.

In The Elements of Physiological Physics: An Outline of the Elementary Facts

(1884), J. M’Gregor-Robertson of the University of Glasgow documented the now-

standard apparatus used by physiologists across the world in muscle and nerve

experiments. He explains, “By an adaptation of the instrument used in music for beating

Page 224: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

203

time, the metronome, this can be done, and the rate of speed at which the shocks follow

one another can also be regulated by it to a large extent.”43

Likewise, John Gray

McKendrick’s A Text Book of Physiology (1888) describes and prints the image of a

“metronome, arranged for making and breaking an electrical current” as “the apparatus

employed in study of muscle” through a series of shocks.44

A Dictionary of Medical

Science (1895) briefly defines the metronome as an “instrument provided with clock-

work for measuring time in music; also employed to test hearing power.”45

Maelzel’s Metronome as Laboratory Chronograph

While physiologists first revised Maelzel’s metronome through electromagnets and

mercury contacts for the consistent regulation of shocks and pneumatic pumps, by the last

three decades of the nineteenth century the metronome had become the timekeeping

element of the new laboratory chronograph, the precision time regulator and recorder

once reserved for the astronomical observatory. The Standard Electrical Dictionary

(1897) confirms:

Metronome, Registering. A clockwork time-marking instrument arranged to

close, at any desired interval, the electric circuit of a chronograph.46

43 J. M’Gregor-Robertson, The Elements of Physiological Physics: An Outline of the Elementary

Facts (London: Cassell & Company, 1884), 71-72.

44

John Gray McKendrick, A Text Book of Physiology (Glasgow: James Maclehose & Sons,

1888), 377.

45

Robley Duglison, A Dictionary of Medical Science, Twenty-first edition (Philadelphia: Lea

Brothers & Co., 1895), 697. Duglison’s earlier editions of the Medical Lexicon from 1846

through 1874 do not include a definition of the metronome.

46

T. O’Conor Sloane, Standard Electrical Dictionary, Second Edition (New York: Normal W.

Henley & Co., 1897), 610.

Page 225: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

204

By the end of the century the metronome had also emerged as the standard time-element

for chronographic tests and experimental research in science classes across the world.

Winfred S. Hall’s Laboratory Guide in Physiology (1897) instructs the student or

professor on how to construct a simple chronograph using a contact metronome47

and

lists under “instruments for special use and for demonstrations” a scientific metronome

that can be purchased for 12 dollars.48

The 1899 British catalogue of Physiological

Instruments from the Cambridge Instrument Company listed three types of metronomes

under “time markers,” the most expensive of which has mercury contacts for running an

electrical current.49

The 1902 Technologisches Wörterbuch, a multilingual German

industrial-scientific dictionary, defined “Metronom” as a timekeeper used in the field of

physics.50

A 1907 German encyclopedia for pharmacists and apothecaries included the

definition of the clockwork metronome as well.51

Indeed, the metronome, applied as a

chronographic regulator in science, was not isolated to one nation or for one scientific

purpose. Even the University of Calcutta required a metronome for scientific lectures, as

47 Winfred S. Hall, A Laboratory Guide in Physiology (Chicago: Chicago Medical Book Co.,

1897), 319.

48

Ibid., 329.

49

Physiological instruments manufactured by the Cambridge Scientific Instrument Company

(Cambridge: Printed at the University Press, 1899), 26.

50

Egbert von Hoyer, Franz Kreuter, Technologisches Wörterbuch, Deutsch-englisch-französisch:

Gewerbe & Industrie (Wiesbaden: J. F. Bergman, 1902), 486.

51

Joseph Moeller and Hermann Thoms, Real-Enzyklopädie der Gesamten Pharmazie, Volume

VIII ([Leipsig]: Urban & Schwarzenberg, 1907): 668.

Page 226: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

205

reported in 1908.52

By the end of the nineteenth century, the metronome had increasingly

provided an objective rhythm for a host of scientific studies on time and action.

Given its scientific foundations in chronography, the clockwork metronome

continued to guide astronomers’ observations well through the nineteenth century,

especially during field research using the “eye-and-ear” method. Royal Astronomical

Society members recorded the January 22, 1898 solar eclipse in India with a Maelzel

metronome.53

Arthur G. Robbins reported on the total solar eclipse in Washington,

Georgia on May 28, 1900, as he “started simultaneously two stop-watches on the tick of

the chronometer, put on a pair of dark glasses, started the metronome and placed one of

the watches beside it on a box.”54

When another Royal Astronomical Society member

registered the total solar eclipse in Sumatra on May 17 and 18, 1901, the metronome

dictated the time within a portable astronomical chronometer. The Notices of the Royal

Astronomical Society reported on the procedure at the Sumatran recording station where

“Seven men from the ‘Pigmy’ [a military gunboat] also assisted; two counting seconds

with the aid of a metronome.”55

52 University of Calcutta, Calendar (Calcutta: Thacker, Sprink & Co., 1908), 327.

53

John Tebbutt, “Equatorial Comparison of Uranus…,” Monthly Notices of the Royal

Astronomical Society LVIII (Nov., 1897-Nov., 1898): 21-25.

54

Arthur G. Robbins, “The Eclipse Expedition of the MIT to Washington, Georgia,” Technology

Quarterly and Proceedings of the Society of Arts XIII (Sept., 1900): 172.

55

H. F. Newall, “Total Solar Eclipse of 1901 May 17-18,” Monthly Notices of the Royal

Astronomical Society LXII (Nov., 1901-Nov., 1902): 214-216; and W. F. Dyson, “Total Eclipse

of the Sun, 1901 May 18,” Monthly Notices of the Royal Astronomical Society LXII (Nov., 1901-

Nov., 1902): 240.

Page 227: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

206

Figure 3.4. MIT chronograph for observing the May 28, 1900 solar eclipse. The clockwork metronome

appears at the bottom left of the astronomical apparatus. From George L. Hosmer, “The Eclipse Expedition

of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology to Washington: Part II. Report of Observations,” Technology

Quarterly and Proceedings of the Society of Arts XIII (Sept., 1900): 165.

As its accuracy improved in the twentieth century, and as more manufacturers of

the machine appeared, the metronome became a necessary apparatus in the

comprehensive professional and educational laboratory. By the 1920s the metronome was

a readily accessible and affordable instrument for the home scientist as well. Ironically

perhaps, scientific sources seldom associated the machine with music, sound, or the

science of acoustics. The article “Starting the Small Physics Laboratory” (1923)

published in the Peabody Journal of Education lists the metronome in an inventory, not

under the “sound” category with resonator tubes, whistles, and tuning forks, but under the

Page 228: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

207

general heading “Mechanics of Solids” alongside other automatic machines such as a

rotator and a stop watch.56

From this brief survey into the scientific metronome, it becomes clear that the

uses of the machine diverged widely from Beethoven’s—and most nineteenth-century

composers’—original applications. Starting in the mid nineteenth century, scientists

found a new life for Maelzel’s automaton as part of the chronographic apparatus, and

thus the metronome rapidly disseminated throughout the fields of physiology, physics,

and astronomy well into the twentieth century. Yet, during most of the nineteenth

century, the scientific metronome had little to do with the creative will of composers or

the performance practices of experienced musicians. Indeed, skilled performers rejected

the machine for the very same reasons that scientists accepted it: the metronome offered

incessant rhythm and artificial regulation in time, far from the volatile qualities of human

perception, creativity, or expression. A benefit to scientific research and experimentation,

the metronome dictated automatical “rhythm” alone. For scientists, the objective

temporal reference was a vital aid to their novel empirical methods; yet, for many

experienced performers and composers, such artificial clockwork control represented

creative death itself.

Beyond the field of professional astronomy, the nineteenth-century science of

experimental psychology most actively relied on metronomic time for the research of

human action measured by the modern chronographic method. Laboratory psychologists

applied the machine quite differently from nineteenth-century physical scientists,

engineers, photographers, and even physiologists. Beyond classroom experiments in

56 Hanor A. Webb, “Starting the Small Physics Laboratory,” Peabody Journal of Education 1

(Sept., 1923): 90, 92.

Page 229: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

208

gravity, tests on hydraulic systems, darkroom exposures, or administering electric shocks

to dead frogs, the scientific metronome actively regulated the personal equation of

thought and action on living, conscious people. Experimental psychologists used

Maelzel’s machine not merely as an objective reference for objective phenomena; they

applied the metronome in a host of newly devised chronographic training-experiments.

For experimental psychologists the clockwork metronome was the sole rhythmic

reference against which they judged and regulated the “observer’s” (i.e. the laboratory

subject’s) every physical action and innate perception.

In the new nineteenth-century science of experimental psychology Maelzel’s

metronome became the “seconds-clock” of two chronographic machines in particular, the

kymograph and the ergograph. These recording and regulating apparatuses had become

essential tools in numerous university-funded laboratories across the world by the

1880s.57

The Dictionary of Philosophy and Psychology (1901) succinctly defines the

close relationship between the original astronomical chronograph, Maelzel’s clockwork

machine, and the modern apparatuses of experimental psychology:

The chronograph used in psychological experiment is some form of revolving

cylinder, such as the kymograph or polygraph…The time is measured by a

tuning-fork, metronome, or seconds pendulum, and recorded by means of

tambours or an electric circuit.58

The following three chapters of this study continue to explore how experimental

psychologists, like astronomers before them, researched the personal equation of

perception and action under the invariable control of metronomic time. As these scientists

57 Boring, 139.

58

James Mark Baldwin, ed., Dictionary of Philosophy and Psychology, Volume 1 (New York:

Macmillan and Co., 1901), 614.

Page 230: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

209

dictated and defined the complexities of the human experience within the rigid

framework of the chronographic method, their mechanical-scientific epistemology had

created a new standard of time and action for an industrialized culture by the twentieth

century. In this modern science, a paradigm shift occurred in the conceptualization of

musical rhythm—from a variable, subjectively sensed quality, to an objective truth,

exclusively heard through precise clockwork. Before composers or performers considered

the metronome to be the absolute reference of their creativity or expression, modern

experimental psychologists replaced the complex personal equation of musicality, in

theory, pedagogy, and practice, with monotonous clockwork clicks.

The Scientific Metronome in Experimental Psychology

Influenced by both the precision-oriented culture of professional astronomy, where

observers gauged perceptions of sight and sound upon the chronograph, and by

physiology, which utilized electro-magnetic metronomes to regulate experimental

surgeries, Wilhelm Max Wundt (1832-1920) founded the modern science of experimental

psychology at the University of Leipzig around 1875.59

With his new psychological

laboratory, which included a host of automatic machines, Wundt, his colleagues, and

students researched and reduced the personal equation of their subjects to second-by-

second clockwork precisions.60

59 O. L. Zangwill, “Wundt,” The Oxford Companion to the Mind Online. Boring dates the

founding of the Leipzig laboratory later in the decade.

60

Besides the astronomy and physiology, the industrializing landscape of Germany, Leipzig in

particular, may have influenced Wundt to incorporate and rely on new, automatic laboratory

machines. See Sven Dierig, “Engines for Experiment: Laboratory Revolution and Industrial

Labor in the Nineteenth-Century City,” Osiris 18 (2003): 116-134.

Page 231: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

210

Psychologist Carl Emil Seashore, perhaps the most prominent scientist working

on the problems of the musical-personal equation during the first half of the twentieth

century, recognized the traditions of his field. In Elementary Experiments in Psychology

(1908), he summarized the chain of influences that led modern psychology to research

and regulate human mental processes through precision, mechanical time:

Historically, interest in the reaction-measurement has passed through several

phases. It began over a hundred years ago in the study of the personal equation of

astronomers. Then the physiologists became interested in the measurement of the

speed of the nerve-impulse by this method. This roused the psychologists to the

measurement of the time of mental processes. At the present time the interest of

the psychologists centers upon its use as an aid in the analysis and synthesis of

action.61

In History of Experimental Psychology, psychologist and historian Edwin Boring also

acknowledged the origins of this uniquely modern science, along with its chronographic

methods, stating, “It is for this reason that experimental psychology can be said to have

grown in part out of astronomy: astronomy furnished it with a problem, some facts, a

method, and some apparatus.”62

The “problem” of experimental psychology was identical to Bessel’s problem first

recognized in the astronomical observatory: the personal equation, the individual’s errors

of perception when compared to an objective and precise clockwork reference. Where

astronomers improved the accuracy of observations through chronography, Wundt

proclaimed that his new science would extend the chronographic method to solve the

many other problems of human individuality:

61 Carl E. Seashore, Elementary Experiments in Psychology (New York: Henry Holt and

Company, 1908), 218.

62

Boring, 142.

Page 232: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

211

Psychology has to investigate that which we call internal experience, —i.e. our

own sensation and feeling, our thought and volition, —in contradistinction to the

objects of external experience, which form the subject matter of natural science.

Man himself, not as he appears from without, but as he is in his own immediate

experience, is the real problem of psychology.63

Thus Wundt—who originally trained and researched as a physiologist—sought to answer

these self-perceived problems of the human mind and body with a host of newly devised

experiments, using the apparatuses of physiology and the chronographic methods of

astronomy. Wundt believed the quantification of human subjectivity in precise,

mechanical time was well within the reach of his new science, stating by 1888 that,

When we learn how long it takes to perceive, to will, to remember, &c., are in

themselves of the same interest to the psychologist, as the distances of the stars to

the astronomer, or atomic weights to the chemist…these times are of further and

great use…in analyzing complex mental phenomena, and in studying the nature of

attention, volition, &c.64

According to Wundt, the human mind, even the individual’s will, functioned within

completely knowable laws and thus could be measured through precise clockwork

machines and methods. Unlike the metaphysical or “rational” psychologists of his day,

such as William James, Wundt—a strict empiricist—believed all things related to purely

quantifiable objects, that the fundamental “nature of volition,” expression,

communication, and intelligence could be registered completely through his new

laboratory procedures and machines, which ultimately could be reduced to definitive,

reproducible mathematical formulas and numerical data. Wundt explained the scientific

values beyond his objectivist brand of psychology:

63 Wilhelm Max Wundt, Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology, translated from the Second

German Edition by J. E. Creighton and E. B. Titchener (London: Swan Sonnenschein & Co.,

1907), 1.

64

James McKeen Cattell, “The Psychological Laboratory at Leipsic,” Mind 13 (Jan., 1888): 45.

Page 233: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

212

Experiment is accompanied by measurement, step by step. Weight and measure

are the great instruments of experimental research, and are always employed in

the search for exact laws. With experiment, weight and measure enter into the

science: for they give it a definite character. Measurement reveals the constants of

nature, the laws that regulate phenomena. The results of all measurement are

expressed in number. Numbers are not the object of measure; but they are the

indispensable means of arriving at its true object, for only numbers can reveal

law.65

It becomes clear from the inception of his Leipzig laboratory that Wundt’s experimental

psychology sought to completely objectify the seemingly ineffable processes of the

human mind. In studying mental phenomena through clockwork machines, Wundt’s

science was perhaps a response to physiologists’ strictly empirical research on bodily

phenomena, their mechanical measurements of blood circulation, musculature reactions,

and nerve responses. E. B. Titchener, Wundt’s early doctoral student at Leipzig and

highly influential North American psychology professor, argued that Wundt’s new

science was the next step in physiology, since experimental psychologists now had the

ability—using the very same apparatuses familiar in physiology—to chart the human

mind with a chronographic precision never before attempted.66

Titchener explained in A

Beginners Psychology (1916):

65 Théodule Ribot, German Psychology of To-day: The Empirical School, translated by James

Mark Baldwin (New York: Charles Scribner’s and Sons, 1886), 192.

66

American physiologists had worked with living human subjects in metronomic time at least by

1875. For example, weight lifting experiments at Washington University relied on metronomic

regulation in which “the beginning and close of this interval of work is marked by the sharp click

of a metronome, the time whose beat is t.” See F. E. Nipher, “On the Mechanical Work done by a

Muscle before Exhaustion,” American Journal of Science and Arts 9 (Feb., 1875): 142. APS

Online. Such studies continued throughout the twentieth century for industrial training purposes.

Page 234: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

213

Now it is possible to measure all these organic changes; to record the rate and

height of the pulse, for instance…physiology puts the necessary instruments at

our disposal. The observer may therefore be harnessed to some such system of

recording apparatus, and may be subjected to some pleasant or unpleasant

stimulus; he reports what he feels, and the experimenter is able to compare the

report with the record from the instrument.67

“The instrument” of time measurement that Titchener, Seashore, Boring, and Wundt all

allude to in their chronographic methods included the scientific metronome. In the

experimental-psychology laboratory, the metronome functioned alongside the

chronograph as the objective reference for researching the human mind and body. Thus

metronomic time aided in both recording and reducing human actions and feelings—all

“organic changes”—to scientific certainties. But the metronome served another purpose

in the experimental laboratory; the artificial rhythm of Maelzel’s machine occasionally

furnished as the “unpleasant stimulus” or distraction when testing mental and physical

reactions of laboratory subjects. As psychologists soon learned, the incessant sound of the

metronome clearly influenced more than just a subject’s immediate laboratory

performance; the sound of the clockwork metronome altered the mind and body over

time.

As an essential scientific apparatus, the metronome had a variety of applications

when researching and recording mental processes in the modern psychology laboratory.

Often psychology experiments employed a standard clockwork metronome, especially for

short tests on a subject’s near-term perceptions. Like physiologists earlier in the century,

however, experimental psychologists occasionally revised Maelzel’s metronome with

electrical contacts and battery power, so the machine could tick indefinitely for more

67 Edward Bradford Titchener, A Beginner’s Psychology (New York: The Macmillan Company,

1916), 82-3.

Page 235: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

214

controlled and consistent experiments—to better resemble an observatory chronograph.

Wundt first developed an electric metronome for his Leipzig laboratory, which was

depicted in his Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology. Clearly inspired by

physiologists’ alterations of the standard Maelzel machine, Wundt details his new

laboratory metronome for “Range of Consciousness” experiments:

Affixed to the pendulum of the metronome is a small iron plate, projecting on

either side. This is arranged between two electro-magnets, e1 and e2 in such a way

that the pendulum can be arrested or set swinging at any moment by the closing or

opening of a current passing through them from the battery k1. The current is

made by simply closing the key s with the left hand. In order to mark off for

perception the separate series of metronome-beats, we make use of a small

electric bell, g, supplied by a second current, k2. This current is made for a

moment, and then broken again by an instantaneous pressure upon the button of

the telegraph key t.68

Other psychologists altered the standard metronome in various ways for different

experiments. Wundt’s former student Titchener recommended that psychologists tamper

with the typical clockwork metronome to reduce the very distractive and prominent click,

which could negatively influence the subject’s reactions. He explains:

PRELIMINARIES. — The customary tick of the metronome, as it stands upon

table or piano, may be described as a noisy clang or a metallic clack. To deaden

the sound, i.e., to eliminate the clang elements, E [the experimenter] must remove

the floor of the clock-chamber (it comes away when a button is turned), and set

the instrument upon a layer of thick — felt. The tick then becomes a mere click or

crack.

The table on which the metronome stands must be placed in such a

relation to O [the observer, the subject] that there is no echo or reverberation from

the walls of the room. The ticks are to form a series of sharply separated dead

sounds.69

68 Wundt, Lectures On Human and Animal Psychology, 261.

69

Edward Bradford Titchener, Experimental Psychology: A Manual of Laboratory Practice,

Volume 1 (New York: The Macmillan Company, 1901), 175.

Page 236: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

215

Fig. 3.5. Wilhelm Wundt’s electro-magnetic metronome apparatus for “range of consciousness”

experiments. Reprinted from his published Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology, 261.

As the scientific applications of the metronome expanded, psychologists

increasingly abstracted the machine from its original purposes as an initial reference for

musical pulse; in their hands Maelzel’s metronome became an artificial sound-emitter

exclusively. Meanwhile, its pendulum swing became entirely devalued in scientific

experimentation. It seems that “sharply dead sounds” alone were a more appropriate

rhythmical reference for standardized experiments on the mind. Silent visual rhythm

played a minor role in the typical psychological investigation; because the simple

pendulum was non-automatical, it rarely if ever guided the scientific studies of attention,

fatigue, or consciousness. Indeed, Titchener, in one metronome-attention experiment,

Page 237: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

216

placed the metronome behind his subjects so they could perceive the unremitting sound

stimulus while being uninfluenced by the sight of the pendulum swing.70

The scientific metronome traveled widely and rapidly from Wundt’s laboratory as

the methods of experimental psychology expanded across Europe and America. A

University of Wisconsin inventory from 1890 lists a “Verdin rotating drum, Marey

tambour, Deprez signal, three metronomes” for testing the subject’s reaction time and

nervous system.71

Titchener, a primary proponent of Wundt’s science in North America,

recommended numerous types of metronomes when conducting experiments on

“Auditory perceptions, Memory, etc.” In 1900 he published metronome makers and

prices in an extensive list of scientific paraphernalia necessary for university

researchers.72

Likewise, an 1899 American Journal of Psychology laboratory checklist

includes the metronome in experiments of “Attention […] (b) range of attention

(counting of metronome beats and execution of several simultaneous acts).” Oddly

perhaps, this inventory did not list the metronome in experiments quantifying “Memory:

(a) visual memory of geometrical design, (b) memory of sentences, (c) musical

memory.”73

The Dictionary of Philosophy and Psychology (1901), however, defined the

metronome as a chief “apparatus for the study of rhythm and auditory span of

70 Titchener, Experimental Psychology, 177.

71

Jastrow, et al., The American Journal of Psychology 3 (Apr., 1890): 275-6.

72

Edward Bradford Titchener, “The Equipment of a Psychological Laboratory,” The American

Journal of Psychology 11 (Jan., 1900): 256. He lists: “56. 2 simple metronomes. Petzold…each

Mk. 7.50 / 57. Simple metronome. Willyoung…3.75 / 58. Metronome with mercury contacts.

Kronecher. Petzold…Mk. 36.00 / 59. Interrupter-clock, cased. Baltzar. Zimmermann…Mk.

170.00.”

73

Stella Emily Sharp, “Individual Psychology: A Study in Psychological Method,” The American

Journal of Psychology 10 (Apr., 1899): 345.

Page 238: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

217

consciousness.”74

As scientific sources attest, Maelzel’s metronome, when used with

chronographic methods, had little in common with most nineteenth-century composers’,

performers’, or musical pedagogues’ intentions for the machine, or their traditional

epistemologies of rhythm.

By the 1890s, metronomic time had regulated and recorded a host of human

activities in the psychological laboratory. Besides judging time intervals in attention and

memory experiments, the metronome set the determining pace in subjects of “memory

image,”75

the regularity of memory recollection,76

as well as reaction time.77

Among

many studies, Wundt analyzed a subject’s special sense in relation to metronomic time.78

Titchener codified one such experiment, which tested a subject’s visual reactions when

exposed to various light stimuli, invariably regulated by the metronome.79

As scientific texts and inventories confirm, the metronome was particularly

essential in memory, recollection, and response studies, some of them first devised in the

mid-1880s by psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850-1909). Using metronomic

regulation, Ebbinghaus’ experimental procedure required a subject to invariably repeat a

74 Baldwin, 611-614.

75

Arthur H. Daniels, “The Memory and After-Image and Attentions,” The American Journal of

Psychology 6 (Jan., 1895): 563. He summarized the work in Wundt’s laboratory on rhythm

perception with the metronome.

76

W. G. Smith, “The Relation of Attention to Memory,” Mind 4 (Jan., 1895): 62.

77

Edgar James Swift, “Disturbance of the Attention during Simple Mental Processes,” The

American Journal of Psychology 5 (Oct., 1892): 5.

78

See A. E. Segsworth, “On the Difference Sensibility for the Valuation of Space Distances with

the Help of Arm Movements,” The American Journal of Psychology 6 (Jun., 1894): 369-407.

79

Titchener, Experimental Psychology, 27.

Page 239: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

218

pattern of words or nonsense syllables, one after the other, testing the capacity for both

mental retention and quick, automatical reaction. An 1896 The American Journal of

Psychology article summarized Ebbinghaus’ original process:

[His self-experiments] were chiefly upon the memory span and the effects of

repetition. His method of learning the syllables was to read aloud in a monotonous

voice series of nonsense syllables of various lengths, regulating the rapidity of

reading by the strokes of a metronome, until the series could be just reproduced

without error.80

The recitation of nonsense syllables in metronomic time continued to be a standard

memory experiment well through the early twentieth century. In the text Experimental

Psychology, Titchener printed a figure of the metronome used in such experiments, an

“apparatus for the Serial Exposure of Nonsense Syllables.”81

Another prominent former

student of Wundt, Ernst Meumann, in The Psychology of Learning, further summarized

Ebbinghaus’ methods from 1885, explaining that “he controlled the rapidity of reading

and reciting by speaking in a tempo of ‘150 beats to the minute,’ having previously

practiced this tempo with a metronome or a watch. He memorized rhythmically, grouping

the syllables by threes and by fours and pronouncing the first, fourth, seventh, etc.—or

the first, fifth, ninth, etc., with a moderate accentuation.”82

80 Theodate L. Smith, “On Muscular Memory,” The American Journal of Psychology 7 (July,

1896): 456.

81

Edward Bradford Titchener, A Text-book of Psychology (New York: The Macmillan Company,

1916), 381.

82

Ernst Meumann, The Psychology of Learning, translated by John Wallace Baird (New York: D.

Appleton and Co., 1913), 162-3.

Page 240: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

219

Fig. 3.6. The “Apparatus for the Serial Exposure of Nonsense Syllables.” As depicted, the battery-

powered Maelzel metronome regulated the rate of this psychological performance-experiment to test

memory, recollection, and response time. Reprinted in Titchener, A Text-book of Psychology, 381.

Meumann’s comments again expose the scientific notion of “rhythmically”

infused action, qualities Meumann actively promoted through his own experiments,

which are analyzed in the following chapter. The unique “rhythm” of Wundt’s,

Ebbinghaus’ and Meumann’s experiments, keeping with a host of psychological studies

founded on chronography, was by very definition metronomic—a value antithetical to the

internal and subjective senses of pulse, proportion, and movement practiced by skilled

musicians. Chronographic scientists continually espoused a “rhythm” that reflected a

fundamental value shift in comparison to skilled musicians and educators of the past;

within psychologists’ laboratories thrived a reductive “rhythm” exclusively defined

through the sound of redundant, repetitive clockwork.

In 1927, The American Journal of Psychology reported on more precise methods

of the Ebbinghaus experiment, now a mainstay of psychology research:

Page 241: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

220

All presentation was aural. [The experimenter] read the series to the beats of a

metronome set at eighty per minute. Thus the interval between the beats was 3/4

of a sec., and the series most rapidly read were presented at this rate. Series more

slowly read were presented one to every second beat, one to every third beat, and

so on. Whatever the rate, the signal 'Now' was given two beats before the

beginning of each presentation and two beats after the end of each presentation

which was to be followed by another.83

Even for those unfamiliar with Ebbinghaus’ research methodologies, it becomes clear

that these memory studies were not simple, empirical observations of an individual’s

common mental abilities, but a more profound mental-physical training within the

confines of precise clockwork “rhythm;” similar to so many other psychological

experiments of the age, Ebbinghaus’ experiments were, to some extent unintentionally,

training sessions in mechanized reaction and response. Musical “artists” of the nineteenth

century would have recognized these experiments at face value—as a peculiar

performance practice in becoming automatical, in slavishly imitating Maelzel’s

automaton.

Ebbinghaus, and experimental psychologists before him, recognized that human

subjects could only perform with consistent metronomic rates and rigidities through

methodical, repetitive training. The results of numerous metronomic experiments on

physical and mental recollection show that for laboratory subjects (as well as experienced

musicians) these artificial performances were clearly not the normal course of living

action: to recollect and repeat in metronomic time took repetitive drilling, through a

recognizable process of habituation. French psychologist Théodule Ribot (1839-1916), in

his survey of the new science of experimental psychology, German Psychology To-day:

The Empirical School, reported that the subjects of Wundt’s laboratory usually failed to

83 Eleanor A. McC. Gamble, “A Study of Three Variables in Memorizing,” The American

Journal of Psychology 39 (Dec., 1927): 224-5.

Page 242: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

221

reproduce precise metronomic rhythm stipulated under chronographic guidelines. This

fact was most notable during the attention-consciousness experiments administered by

Vierordt:

To study the duration of these small intervals, Vierordt causes the subject to

attend for some time to the beating of the metronome; then he [the subject] is to

reproduce the beats as fast as he heard them. Now, the repeated intervals are too

short when the real intervals are long, and too long when the intervals are short.

The individual variations on both sides of the exact point are large.84

The scientific metronome, when applied with chronographic values and goals, provided a

new and artificial reference for newly devised human performances, which

correspondingly reduced the individual subject to the status of an automaton.

Psychologists consistently noticed problematic “individual variations” during

metronomic experimentation, yet over time, they also recognized that these personal

variations receded with incessant, mechanized training—just as it had for astronomers

using the chronograph. Ebbinghaus well understood that over time his methods effaced

the personal equation—the intrinsic variability of human action—to mere automatism,

concluding of his results, “There are great individual differences; but, in general, it is

only after a good deal of practice that the observer becomes the sheer mechanical

associator.”85

Experienced musicians of the century, acknowledging the inhuman

influence of the metronome upon human musicality, came to the very same conclusion as

experimental psychologists: “the observer may be trained.” 86

84 Théodule Ribot, German Psychology of To-day: The Empirical School, translated by James

Mark Baldwin (New York: Charles Scribner’s and Sons, 1886), 273.

85

Titchener, A Text-book of Psychology, 382.

86

Ibid.

Page 243: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

222

While it is not in the scope of this brief survey of the scientific metronome to

chart every use of the clockwork machine in the nineteenth-century psychology

laboratory—for its purposes are seemingly endless—the following chapters continue to

explore the numerous scientific applications of the device, as modern experimental

psychologists redefined the values of thought and action with an increasingly precise and

automatical “rhythm.” Chapter IV documents metronomic experiments on attention span

and musical performance; Chapter V exposes the effects of the metronome in behavioral

training; and Chapter VI explores metronomic tests of physical tolerances and child

normalcy as they relate to contemporaneous trends in music pedagogy. In their many

metronomic laboratory experiments—each founded on the chronographic methods of

astronomy—mechanical rhythm redefined the fundamentals of individual perception and

performance in time both for experimenters and their subjects. Indeed, the new paradigm

of rhythm first espoused by Wundt transferred to every act of humanity in his Leipzig

laboratory. Thus we find that in researching, documenting, and training physical and

mental phenomena with the sound of the clockwork metronome, Wundt and following

psychologists mistranslated musical time as being equitable or synonymous with

metronomic rhythm especially during sound-perception experiments; their mistranslation

of creative, subjective rhythm as an objective, automatical quality constituted a dramatic

value shift that served a reductive research methodology with lasting repercussions. By

the turn of the century this paradigm of laboratory “rhythm,” utilized first by

experimental psychologists, fueled the trend for more “scientific” pedagogies espoused in

the last years of the nineteenth-century, which now prescribed precision-based

metronomic time for a host of trainable performances.

Page 244: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

223

CHAPTER IV: METRONOMIC RHYTHM, THE CHRONOGRAPHIC BIAS,

AND THE SCIENTIFIC REDEFINITION OF MUSICIANS

AND MUSICAL ACTION

“Rhythm” and “time,” as this study continues to explore, are value systems that vary

according to people, places, and purposes. Well through the nineteenth century, the

automatical “rhythm” of the metronome was not the sensory, subjective rhythm of living

musicians. But as the metronome became an important chronographic tool for

physiologists and experimental psychologists, the objective “rhythm” of the scientific

laboratory rapidly emerged. This new rhythm had profound and lasting effects on musical

performance practices by the end of the century. Through scientific methods and

machines of the late nineteenth century, the time of clockwork and the time of music

converged ever closer.

As numerous recent histories of technology have shown, scientists and their

methods act as catalysts in the development of the most precise machines valued for the

most precise measurements. In fields such as surgery, chemistry, computing, and

especially time telling, scientists first require specialized equipment that helps to cut

smaller, view closer, count faster—or measure increasingly minute, equal fractions of

time. Thus, scientific values of “time,” when defined by artificial time tellers, are

traditionally the most precise for any given point in Western history. These temporal

values are as “precise” as the historical, communal, and cultural contexts stipulate. For

instance, in our present age, particle physicists use some of the most exacting time-telling

instruments, housed within sub-atomic super-colliders, currently in existence. Yet, these

precision tools have very specific and limited purposes. And they certainly have a

negligible place in most people’s daily time-telling epistemologies, since one does not

Page 245: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

224

need to measure the workday or quarter notes in picoseconds. For the nineteenth century,

the clockwork metronome was a similarly specialized time reference. When embraced by

the new experimental psychologists, it was an apparatus rigorously applied with similar

scientific intentions and values—providing the most objective, constant, and precise

temporal measurements then available to Western civilization. For the nineteenth century,

the clockwork metronome was a state-of-the-art regulator of scientific time and rhythm.

With their many psychological studies in precision-based rhythm, the new

scientists applied automatical machines to the study of human action in ways once

considered antithetical to humanity. This chapter explores the intimate link between their

scientific research, precision-oriented apparatuses, and the redefinitions of rhythmical

action in “time.” Through experimental psychologists’ chronographic studies, the once-

experiential qualities of creative “artistic” rhythm heard in the sensory accent, rhetorical

gesture, and other subjective techniques, became precisely mechanized through

speculative scientific theory. Originating in Wilhelm Wundt’s laboratory, this new

scientific culture redefined an individual’s rhythmical perceptions and actions with

mechanical precisions never before realized or desired by skilled performers.

Scientists altered the very meanings of “good rhythm” for living musicians

through the constant reference to metronomic apparatuses. For psychologists trained in

Wundt’s methodologies, the creative, interpretive time of musical rhythm ceased to hold

value; in their hands, the scientific metronome ruled over the once-subjective rhythms,

pulses, movements of musical time. In contrast to knowledgeable musical pedagogues

who espoused sensory and willful performance practices, Wundt’s school viewed the

human “observer”—both inside and eventually outside the laboratory—as a passive,

Page 246: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

225

reactive subject against precise, mechanical regulation. Consequently, by the 1890s,

musical performances in the psychological laboratory had transformed into

chronographic tests, as experimental psychologists maintained that exacting mechanical

time and metronomic rhythm provided the complete understanding of correct human

impulses and actions. Distanced from the temporal beliefs of Marx, Christiani, or Weber,

experimental psychologists’ modern rhythm studies indisputably turned musical time into

an automatical quality; in laboratories before on the concert stage, these scientists

reconceived musical rhythm within the absolute standard of metronomic sound. These

many experimental psychologists prefigured and first prescribed the precise mechanical

rhythm that most modern musicians practice, modern pedagogues teach, and modern

audiences hear today.

The Birth of Scientific “Rhythm”

Wilhelm Wundt practically invented the modern meaning and scientific application of

“rhythm” during his psychological investigations into the mental phenomena of attention

and consciousness. Perhaps inspired by notable physiologist Karl Vierordt—who

incorporated the metronome into human experiments by the 1860s—Wundt believed that

in testing a subject’s exposure to repetitive mechanical sounds, one discovered how long

the mind could hold a string of ideas, which pointed to a range of consciousness. The

metronome, and in following years more precise metronomic apparatuses, facilitated

these laboratory studies that spanned well into the twentieth century. As prominent

former student of Wundt, Titchener recognized that his teacher was “the first to propound

Page 247: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

226

a psychological theory of rhythm”1 based on these attention and consciousness

experiments. Here, in the mechanical laboratory of the first experimental psychologist,

the experience of musical time flipped to value automatons and automatical time.

Through Wundt’s new experiments, human motion was no longer the reference of

rhythm; instead, rhythm became a scientific rule regulated exclusively by objective,

mechanical means. Charles Samuel Myers described psychologists’ typical “Time and

Rhythm” studies in A Text-book of Experimental Psychology (1909), and he succinctly

defined this radically new temporal quality found in laboratory procedures, in which

“The simplest material for rhythm consists of a series of identical, regularly repeated, and

equally accented stimuli.” 2

The source of Wundt’s tradition of laboratory rhythm—without any ambiguities

or nuances—was the sound emitted from the scientific metronome. In 1901, Titchener

defined the essential, external “materials” employed over the past decade for

psychological experiments on the subject’s sense of “rhythm”:

1 Edward Bradford Titchener, Experimental Psychology: A Manual of Laboratory Practice,

Volume I (New York: The Macmillan Company, 1901), 355.

2 Charles Samuel Myers, A Text-book of Experimental Psychology (New York: Longmans, Green

& Co., 1909), 314-315.

By the end of the previous century, Myers had applied these typical metronomic

“rhythm” experiments to the indigenous populations of Murray Island to test the innate

“rhythmic” abilities of non-European peoples. Myers reports: “Twelve Islanders were tested for

their sense of rhythm; this was found to be remarkably accurate for 120 beats of the metronome

to the minute, and somewhat less so for 60 beats. Most of the subjects had a tendency to vary in

the direction of increasing the rate of the taps.” See C. S. Myers, et al. “Anthropological Reviews

and Miscellanea,” The Journal of the Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 29

(1899): 222.

Myers’ ethnographic research prefigures ethnomusicologists’ studies in following

decades that also relied upon the metronome to quantify rhythmic performances.

Page 248: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

227

The instruments which give the rhythm stimuli, the variously modified sound

series, must have a high degree of mechanical accuracy, and are correspondingly

expensive. The most useful appliance for investigation is, probably, Meumann’s

‘time-sense’ apparatus, consisting of Baltzar kymograph, time-disc, set of

contacts, and sound-hammers…The metronome recommends itself, for the initial

experiment, both by its cheapness and by its wide range of rate3…The metronome

is, in general, a very reliable instrument. Nevertheless, these rates should be

tested, on principle, by counting the beats with a stop-watch.4

Using his electrified metronome as the exclusive rhythmic stimulus, Wundt and his

students tested their subjects’ perception to the monotonous succession of mechanical

sounds at various rates of speed.5 Upon eliciting subjects’ reactions to the redundant

mechanical stimulus, these scientists found that the mind often attempted to order

incessant and equal metronomic ticks into groups, usually in patterns of two or three.

American psychologist William James (1842-1910) summarized the phenomenon:

Our spontaneous tendency is to break up any monotonously given series of

sounds into some sort of rhythm. We involuntarily accentuate every second, or

third, or fourth beat, or we break the series in still more intricate ways. Whenever

we thus grasp the impressions in rhythmic form, we can identify a longer string of

them without confusion.6

More than this phenomenon elicited by metronomic sound, when one sound in the

mechanical series was more prominent, accented by a bell for instance, the mind grouped

the softer clicks in relation to the loudest, even though every sound remained

automatically consistent. Thaddeus Bolton defined this mental phenomenon in the

3 Titchener, Experimental Psychology, 338.

4 Ibid., 339.

5 See, for another example, Wilhelm Wundt, Outlines of Psychology (Leipzig: Wilhelm

Engelmann, 1902), 230-236.

6 Sherover, 373. Reprint from William James, “The Perception of Time,” Chap XV, The

Principles of Psychology, Volume 1 (New York: Henry Holt and Co., 1890).

Page 249: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

228

American Journal of Psychology (1894), reiterating the scientific definition of rhythm

that was exclusively dictated through externalized, mechanized “impressions:”

We come now to the consideration of the nature of the rhythmical group. The

general principle is this: In a series of auditory impressions, any regularly

recurrent impression which is different from the rest, subordinates the other

impressions to it in such a way that that fall together in groups. If the recurrent

difference is one of intensity, the strongest impression comes first in the group

and the weaker ones after. If the recurrent difference is one of duration,7 the

longest impression comes last. These rules of course hold good only within the

limits spoken of above. When the impressions are uniform in length and intensity,

the mind enforces a grouping by giving fictitious values to the impressions,

generally with respect to intensity, but sometimes with respect to duration.8

This ability to mentally group incessant, automatical sounds became a trusted measure of

attention span in the laboratory; the longer that subjects were exposed to metronomic

rhythm, their ability to “fictitiously” group sounds into a discernable pattern diminished.

Wundt found that this phenomenon of mental grouping eventually ceased after forty

metronome clicks. Bolton recounted this initial discovery at Leipzig University:

In the work undertaken by Dietze in Wundt’s laboratory upon the Umfang of

consciousness, this rhythmical grouping of the sounds of the metronome was

observed and employed to determine the length of the mental span. The grouping

was accomplished [mentally from the subjects] by intensifying voluntarily certain

sounds and subordinating others to it. By grouping the sounds first by eight and

then the groups of eight by five, it was possible to grasp forty sounds…Wundt

refers this grouping to the ripening of the concept on the wave of apperception.9

Experimental psychologists considered the mental process of grouping redundant

artificial sounds into patterns standard knowledge, and they repeated and revised Wundt’s

initial metronome experiments well into the twentieth century. James Burt Miner in

7 Bolton makes clear on page 233 of his article that this lengthening is mechanically precise: “the

longer impression twice the length of the shorter.”

8 Thaddeus L. Bolton, “Rhythm,” The American Journal of Psychology 6 (Jan., 1894): 232-3.

9 Ibid., 205.

Page 250: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

229

Motor, Visual, and Applied Rhythms (1903) described a variant of the attention

experiment, in which the subject, as was the custom, passively perceived a rhythmic

grouping out of metronomic sound:

The subject was told to relax all his muscles as much as possible and then listen to

the metronome, with eyes closed. After he had listened awhile to the beats I asked

him if the sounds seemed to vary in any regular way. He generally at once noticed

the grouping. Having made sure that the subject perceived a subjective rhythm I

then watched for involuntary movements.10

“Subjective rhythm” as described by Miner and first practiced in Wundt’s experiments

was no longer related to any internal pulse, physical movement, or interpretive volition

whatsoever; the new psychologists’ “subjective rhythm” was indeed antithetical to the

type of rhythm espoused and experienced by skilled musical performers and pedagogues

throughout the nineteenth century. The species of rhythm that psychologists elicited in

the laboratory instead required the subject to “passively” listen to artificial, external

“impressions,” and then imagine an ordered pattern out of mechanical monotony. Herbert

Woodrow confirms in A Quantitative Study of Rhythm (1909) that, in experimental

psychologists’ attention research, all rhythmical phenomena (along with the definition

“rhythm”) reduced to chronographic explanation:

The following description of the method used in this research may be found rather

difficult to follow by those who are unaccustomed to the terminology of

rhythm…It should be remembered that in all the rhythms here dealt with every

second or every third [automatical] sound is either louder or longer than the

others. Also that rhythm is characterized by an apprehension of the sounds in

groups and that when there is no grouping [in the mind] there is no rhythm.11

10 James Burt Miner, “Motor, Visual, and Applied Rhythms,” (PhD Diss., Columbia University,

1903), 23-4.

11

Herbert Woodrow, A Quantitative Study of Rhythm, The Effect of Variations in Intensity, Rate,

and Duration (New York: The Science Press, 1909), 17. Woodrow introduces the scientific

theory of rhythm in Chapter I, exposing psychologists’ view that all creative human movement in

time is reducible to mechanical action and chronographic explanation:

Page 251: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

230

Woodrow certified the temporal truth defined by his scientific field: “Every one is agreed

today that the essential thing in the perception of rhythm is the experiencing of groups. It

is this experience of groups which distinguishes rhythm in the psychological sense of the

word from rhythm in the sense of the earth about its axis.”12

In keeping with his

discipline, Woodrow did not distinguish sensory musical rhythm from the purely

psychological “groupings” elicited through mechanical sound stimuli. He did, however,

recognize a wholly different “rhythm” in the rotation of the globe. For these scientists,

rhythm—as perceived by human-subjects—was a mentally “experienced” phenomenon

exclusively, called forth in the laboratory through objective, chronographic apparatuses.

Hearing the New, Best Source for “Rhythm”

Psychology texts document that the sound of the clockwork click, not the sight of the

pendulum swing, defined this new scientific species of rhythm. Titchener’s experiments

often effaced the visual element of rhythm entirely, as the psychologist placed the

subject’s back to the machine, so the only perceptible sense of rhythm was the succession

of artificial clicks emitted from an altered, muffled metronome. Using clockwork sound

as the paramount reference for rhythm, Titchener documented the procedure to elicit

“To produce an impression of rhythm, it is necessary to have a series of stimuli. These

stimuli may be sounds, as in the case of poetry and music, muscular contractions, as in

dancing and beating time, or lights and electrical shocks, as in some laboratory

experiments. The stimuli which give the impression of rhythm, whatever their nature,

may vary in intensity, in duration, and in quality, and may be separated by intervals of

varying length. A fundamental task of the experimental investigation of rhythm is to

investigate the part played by each of these factors. Only after each of them has been

studied separately, may we study the effect when two or more of them are simultaneously

involved, and when more complicated factors are introduced, as in melody and

harmony.” See Woodrow, 5.

12

Ibid., 53.

Page 252: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

231

“sensations” from the passive subject, who heard six different degrees of metronomic

speed, at 42, 48, 66, 92, 152, 200 beats per minute, respectively.13

The passive subject

had little recourse but to absorb the metronomic stimulus and then describe his sensory

and cognitive reaction to the various rates of artificial “rhythm.” How little this had in

common with musical creativity, interpretation, gesture, or rhetoric might be

overwhelmingly evident to musicians such as Wagner, Brahms, or Hofmann—or

countless other composer-performers who rejected mindless, metronomic thought and

action. Nevertheless, Wundt’s school continued to reduce the complexities of human

rhythm and sensation to reproducible methods and efficient metronomic explanations.

Occasionally, these attention experiments became tests in perceiving the

relationship between the specific sounds emitted from the metronome-apparatus itself. In

Experimental Psychology: A Manual of Laboratory Practice (1901), Titchener

documents another training process where hearing the synchronicity between the

metronomic bell and click became the subject’s true challenge:

EXPERIMENT (5). Seventh Law. — Materials: bell metronome.

(a) Set the metronome pendulum for a fairly rapid beat, e.g., 144 or 152 strokes in

the 1 min., and the bell for sounding at every sixth stroke.

After the experiment ends, the scientific researcher poses the following questions to the

observer:

13 Titchener, Experimental Psychology, 176. “EXPERIMENT (1). — … After the ready signal, E

starts the metronome at one of the two quickest rates (.39 or .30 sec. intervals), and lets it run for

45 sec. O is to give himself up, quite passively, to the impression of the sound series, and is to

describe what he hears. / The experiment should be repeated three times with each of the rates.

There must be a pause of at least 5 min. between series and series. The introspective record

should be made as full as possible. / EXPERIMENT (2). — The experiment is performed with

all six rates, taken in irregular order, and each twice repeated. / EXPERIMENT (3). —

Experiment (2) is repeated, but under slightly different conditions.”

Page 253: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

232

What time-relation does the ring bear to its corresponding stroke? Do the two fall

together? Or does the ring come before or after the stroke? (b) Set the bell for

sounding at every second stroke. How are the ring and its stroke heard? Is it

possible by shift of attention to shift the apparent time-relation of the two

simultaneous impressions?14

Besides scrutinizing the subject’s ear for mechanical sound-stimuli, these experiments

expose the practical problems in attending to the rhythm of Maelzel metronomes in

general, since the visual tempo of the pendulum oscillation and the click of the clockwork

did not necessarily correspond. Some nineteenth-century music references also indicated

that the metronome click actually sounded in-between the course of the pendulum swing,

and not at the end of the swing, where the visual pulse lay. Furthermore, this discrepancy

between the visual and audible rhythms varied depending upon the speed: the faster the

rate, the closer the click seemed to relate to the pendulum swing.

An even more challenging aspect in referencing a typical bell metronome was that

the bell marking the metric pulse did not often sound simultaneously with the continuous

click marking the internal beats. This lack of synchronization within the metronome

itself, between the visual and audible tempos, created considerable confusion for those

who found a silent, simple pendulum a more faithful reflection of musical pulse and

physical movement. As late as 1908, psychologist Carl Emil Seashore noted in

Elementary Experiments in Psychology that due to this temporal discrepancy between the

metronome ring and click and “according to the direction of the attention, [the subject]

can make the one or the other appear to come first. One can make either the bell-stroke or

14 Ibid., 115.

Page 254: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

233

the click of the metronome arise first, although in a good instrument they really sound at

the same instant.”15

The typical inconsistencies found in “over the counter” metronomes seemed so

prevalent that Titchener felt it necessary to instruct experimental psychologists on the

proper way to shop for a scientifically acceptable machine:

The Instructor should, therefore, select the instrument himself from the music-

dealer’s stock, and not order at haphazard. There is a great difference between one

metronome and another, despite the sameness of make. An instrument whose

clacks sound approximately equal on the music-counter (and such an one should

be found among the first half dozen tried) will give still more nearly equal ticks in

the laboratory, when resonance is ruled out.16

Nineteenth-century performers and pedagogues found the referential pendulum swing

appropriate and acceptable when teaching novices in the subjective pulse-sense of

musical time. But, in order to study “rhythm” in psychologists’ laboratories, one needed

to hear the continual, “nearly equal ticks” and rings emitted from the best clockwork

metronomes available.

Training to Think and Act in Scientific Rhythm

Once the scientifically acceptable (the most chronographically precise and consistent)

metronome was procured, Titchener confirmed that these “rhythm” experiments required

a subject to accept a completely passive physical and mental attitude when

“experiencing” the laboratory phenomenon of “rhythm.” (Neither Wundt nor Titchener

devised attention tests to measure the subject’s interpretive rhythm while actively

15 Carl E. Seashore, Elementary Experiments in Psychology (New York: Henry Holt and

Company, 1908), 168.

16

Titchener, Experimental Psychology, 339. Titchener even commented that he “has not been

able to find a bell-metronome which answers the purpose” of some of his basic rhythmic-sense

experiments.

Page 255: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

234

performing works of music, poetry, dance, or drama.) In a Beginner’s Psychology,

Titchener explained, however, that these attention experiments in mechanical sound often

required the subject to do more than merely relax:

If you listen to a metronome beating, say, 15 in the minute, you will be able with

practice to hold six successive strokes in the focus of attention, but not more; if

you try to group the seventh stroke with the preceding six you become confused;

the series breaks, and cannot be welded together again. As the speed of the

metronome is increased, the beats fall of themselves into groups of twos and

threes; and you can still grasp and hold six of these rhythmical impressions. When

the speed has reached some 200 in the minute, the rhythmical grouping becomes

more complicated; as many as eight single beats may be bound together in a

rhythmical unit; and the attention is adequate, again after practice, to five of these

complex groups.17

More than just documenting the subject’s ability to mentally group metronomic clicks

into patterns, psychologists actively instructed their laboratory subjects to comprehend

various rhythmic groupings within the mechanical stimuli. Regardless of the individual’s

initial perceptions—or whether they perceived groupings or not—experimental

psychologists trained subjects to ascertain a sonic order within the continuous barrage of

clicks. Titchener described a learning process similar to Ebbinghaus’ memory

experiments, in which metronomic rhythm influenced and altered subjects’ very actions

and perceptions with repetitive practice over time. Thus, psychologists’ attention research

often reflected a more profound attention training, as subjects learned to interpret and

perform under artificial temporal guidance, not only for the mind but the body as well.

Underlying this training process was scientists’ belief that rhythmic perception originated

through the objective metronomic click alone. Throughout his career, Titchener

17 Edward Bradford Titchener, A Beginner’s Psychology (New York: The Macmillan Company,

1916), 103-4. Emphasis added.

Page 256: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

235

continued to document the methods and machines essential to these attention-training

experiments:

Set a metronome beating, with an interval of about a quarter of a second between

stroke and stroke. Try to throw the beats into all the different possible rhythms,

trochaic, iambic, etc. You will find it quite easy to change from rhythm to rhythm,

especially if you use movement to assist you,— moving foot or hand when the

beats come which you wish to emphasize. Then see how complex a foot or

measure you can construct in the various rhythms.18

The individual’s mental ability for “rhythm” merely rested on his response and reaction

to automatical sound. In stark contrast to past practices of musical performers and

pedagogues, Titchener even referenced once-traditional rhythmic epistemologies solely

through the unwavering click of the metronome. The subjective tactus and flow of music

and poetry now subsumed to clockwork. In the attention experiments of psychologists,

values of time and rhythm radically turned to favor Maelzel’s invention; as a

chronographic time source, the automaton became the very foundation of human rhythm,

thought, and movement.19

More than being disinterested empirical experiments, these “rhythm” studies

defined additional personal-equation problems that needed solving through unerring

mechanical reference: what could the mind comprehend—and for how long—when it is

exposed to exacting and unwavering metronomic time. Psychologists’ attention-span

18 Edward Bradford Titchener, An Outline of Psychology (New York: The Macmillan Company,

1902), 184.

19

Titchener, An Outline of Psychology, 184. Titchener applied the findings of attention span

studies, not only to the rhythms of poetry and music, but to the compositional forms of poetry and

music as well. Through the research of metronomic “rhythm,” he justifies the creation of artistic

works in which, “The result, we may note, agrees very well with the canons of musical and poetic

composition. The musical phrase never contains more than six measures, and the poetical line or

verse never contains more than six feet; a seven-measured phrase or a seven-footed line falls to

pieces, ceases to be unitary. The rhythmical wholes of a higher order, the period in music and the

stanza or strophe in poetry, never contain more than five phrases or verses; as a rule, neither

contains more than four.”

Page 257: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

236

experiments directly complemented their research on labor fatigue. In physical-motion

studies, they tested the duration that a subject could maintain a constant rate of weight

lifting (often by an index finger); in mental-attention experiments, they tested the

duration that a subject could maintain a constant series of rhythmic groupings. Each type

of training-study challenged natural human tolerances; scientists regulated laboratory

subjects’ performances through repetitive and invariable metronomic clicks, eventually

finding the mechanical instant when humans faltered.

Training to Feel in Scientific Rhythm

Not only for action and attention experiments, psychologists used the metronome to call

forth Wundt’s theoretical range of human emotions, in which “We can distinguish six

types or classes of these sense-feelings: the agreeable and disagreeable, the exiting and

subduing, and the straining and relaxing.”20

While Wundt’s “tridimensional theory of

feeling” aroused some contention in the field, many experimental psychologists trained

under his scientific theory based human sensation on these wholly limited, dualistic

measures.21

With their dramatic value shift favoring chronography over human action,

some psychologists found that the metronome stimulated this myopic emotional range

within passive laboratory subjects. Certainly a strong influence on Wundt’s

“tridimensional theory of feeling,” Vierordt performed the earliest research on human

20 Titchener, A Beginner’s Psychology, 81.

21

For a discussion and critique see Edward Bradford Titchener, A Text-book of Psychology (New

York: The Macmillan Company, 1916), 250-257.

Page 258: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

237

time perception and sensation with the sound of the metronome.22

In 1890 William James

reported:

Vierordt listened to series of strokes performed by a metronome at rates varying

from 40 to 200 a minute and found that they very naturally fell into seven

categories from ‘very slow’ to ‘very fast.’ Each category of feeling included the

intervals following each other within a certain range of speed, and no others. This

is a qualitative, not a quantitative judgment—an aesthetic judgment, in fact. The

middle category, of speed that was neutral or, as he calls it, ‘adequate,’ contained

intervals that were grouped about 0.62 second, and Vierordt says that this made

what one might almost call an agreeable time.23

With the scientific knowledge that constant metronomic sound exposed human emotions

in the laboratory, Titchener even justified musical aesthetics through Wundt’s scientific-

mechanical bases of feeling:

The straining and relaxing feelings are dependent upon the temporal course and

succession of sensations; the interminable pedal-point in Eb with which Wagner

begins the Ring sets up a feeling of tension which is relaxed when the Bb is

added, only to grow again, and again relax when new tones are introduced; and if

you follow the strokes of a slow-beating metronome you get a similar alternation

of the two sense-feelings.24

With a machine now informing aesthetic judgments for these experimental scientists,

Titchener considered the sensation of strain and relaxation elicited by Wagner’s

harmonies equal to the click of the metronome. In the mechanical laboratory of

experimental psychology, metronomic rhythm indeed seemed to be the sole reference for

all sonic and sensory phenomena, to the extent that even Wagner’s sense of tonality could

be explained through an objective, metronomic equivalent.

22 See Karl Vierordt, Der Zeitsinn nach versuchen (Tübingen: H. Laupp’schen Buchhandlung,

1868).

23

James, 618-619.

24

Titchener, A Beginner’s Psychology, 82.

Page 259: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

238

For Wundt himself, it clearly seems that living rhythms lost their subjectivity,

variability, and individuality—both in theory and practice. Neither rhythm, time, nor

emotions were innate, active, or experiential phenomena—instead, “rhythm” represented

the wholly passive subject’s reactions and impulses to metronomic exposure. The

laboratory subject’s armchair reactions to external and automatical sound “impressions”

logically translated to the perception of subjective time, as Wundt states in Outlines of

Psychology:

When the feelings produced by rhythmical impressions become somewhat more

intense, as is usually the case, especially when the rhythm is connected with

sensational contents that arouse the feelings greatly, the feelings of rhythm

become in fact emotions. Rhythms are for this reason the important means both in

music and poetry of portraying emotions and arousing them in the auditor.25

Skilled individuals’ nuanced and variable performances of poetry, dance, or music held

no value in Wundt’s concept of emotional response; since rhythm was purely external

and metronomic—a sound to perceive and react to—the experiential rhythm witnessed in

creative human activity did not factor into his “rhythms” of poetry or music. The

“auditor” of artistic rhythm, as tested in Wundt’s laboratory, was merely a passive subject

exposed to externalized and constant “impressions.” Redefining the audible qualities of

rhythm, Wundt devalued the lived experience of the creative artist, opting instead for the

regulated, objectified time used during chronographic experimentation.

While he often neglected to specify that the metronome afforded a primary source

of rhythm, Wundt further defined the mind’s exposure to rhythm as an absolute

psychological process of feeling, a reactionary phenomenon seemingly divorced from the

seminal acts of artistic creation; rhythm was ultimately an objective quality that

25 Wundt, Outlines of Psychology, 187.

Page 260: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

239

constantly influenced the passive spectator. Rhythm, as a scientific rule, was an external,

objective imposition upon the subjective mind. Thus, metronomic “impressions”

remained a prominent notion throughout Wundt’s theory of rhythm, and in his widely

published Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology, he explains:

The feeling of rhythm, which is the single psychological motive in dancing, and

ranks with harmony and disharmony as a psychological motive in musical

composition, contains the elements both of expectation and satisfaction. The

regular repetition in rhythmical sense-excitation makes us expect every

succeeding stimulation, and the expectation is immediately followed by

satisfaction. Rhythm therefore never involves strain, or if it does, it is simply bad

rhythm.26

Redefining artistic rhythm through the limited binaries of good and bad, relaxation and

strain, pleasing and displeasing, as dictated by his psychological spectrum of emotions—

called forth in his laboratory through the binary “tick-tock” of the metronome—Wundt

continues:

In pleasant rhythms satisfaction follows expectation as quickly as possible. Every

impression arouses the expectation of another, and at the same time satisfies the

expectation aroused by its predecessor, whose temporal relations it reproduces.

Rhythm, that is, is an emotion compounded of the emotions of expectation and

satisfaction. A broken rhythm is emotionally identical with disappointment.27

It will forever remain a mystery whether Beethoven or Berlioz would subscribe to

Wundt’s definition of musical rhythm, but given the evidence from the first two chapters,

it seems unlikely that any nineteenth-century composer would explain rhythm using

Wundt’s epistemology. In Beethoven’s lifetime, the Allgemeine musikalische Zeitung

often criticized the composer’s unique rhetorical penchant for unexpected rhythmic

26 Wilhelm Wundt, Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology, translated from the Second

German Edition by J. E. Creighton & E. B. Titchener (London: Swan Sonnenschein & Co.,

1907), 376-7.

27

Ibid., 377.

Page 261: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

240

motives and uncustomary time signatures as being highly “bizarre” or difficult, and only

disappointing to the extent that Beethoven’s music tried the amateur performer’s limited

abilities and interests.28

Indeed, Moritz Hauptmann, the professional Leipzig singer,

composer, and influential theorist, expressly stated in a letter dated October 28, 1848 that

the fashion of modern composers, which perhaps still included Beethoven, to break from

traditional symmetric forms of metrical phrasing actually caused many listeners clear

feelings of contentment, “satisfaction—nay, in many instances pleasure out of” such

unexpected rhythmic inventions.29

Gottfried Weber, in contrast, associated unexpected

syncopations in music—strong-beat juxtapositions against the regularly perceived

metrical pulse—with the visceral feelings of “shock” and “revulsion,” but not of

disappointment.30

Yet nowhere in Wundt’s scientific aesthetics do composers’ creative

styles—or, for that matter, skilled performance artists’ interpretations—alluded to by

28 For instance, AmZ published the following review of Beethoven’s Opus 12 keyboard sonatas in

June, 1799:“It is undeniable that Mr. van Beethoven goes his own way. But what a bizarre,

laborious way! Studied, studied, and perpetually studied, and no nature, no song. Indeed, to put it

precisely, there is only a mass of learning here, without good method. There is obstinacy for

which we feel little interest, a striving for rare modulations, a repugnance against customary

associations, a piling on of difficulty upon difficulty so that one loses all patience and

enjoyment.”

Reprinted in Wayne M. Senner, ed., The Critical Reception of Beethoven’s Compositions

by His German Contemporaries, Volume 1 (Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1999), 145,

Letter 65.

29

Moritz Hauptmann, The Letters of a Leipzig Cantor, edited by Alfred Schöne and Ferdinand

Hiller, translated by A.D. Coleridge, Volume II (London: Novello, Ewer and Co., and Richard

Bentley and Son, 1892), 66. The classicist Hauptmann also criticized these new compositional

trends, chiding modern composers’ for their break from the “traditional” metrical symmetry of

classical musical phrasing. He stated, “We live surrounded by a crust of prose, which it is hard to

break through…Modern works are conspicuous for want of rhythmical perfection.”

30 Weber contended that the “Syncope, like rhythmical inversion, produces a species of shock or

revulsion in our feelings, from the circumstance that the stress of voice falls upon the light

portion of the measure involved in the syncopated form, whereas no stress occurs on the heavy

portion.” See Gottfried Weber, General Music Teacher: Adapted to Self-instruction, Both for

Teachers and Learners; Embracing Also an Extensive Dictionary of Musical Terms, trans. by

James Franklin Warner (Boston: J. H. Wilkins & R. B. Carter, 1841), 118.

Page 262: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

241

Weber, Hauptmann, Marx, Christiani,31

and many others, appear as primary evidence of

rhythmic thought, sensation, or action. Again, Wundt conveniently and necessarily

omitted human artists’ expressive intentions, their creative authority and agency—their

personal equations—from his absolute, laboratory-inspired laws of scientific rhythm.

The New Science of Musical Time

Once Wundt and his school redefined human rhythm, thought, action and emotion

through limited perceptions and reactions to automatical sound, the father of

experimental psychology seamlessly redefined musical time through the clockwork

metronome. The infallible regulator in his laboratory now became the temporal constant

of musical interpretation and performance.32

Through Wundt’s attention studies, the

31 We must recall Adolph Friedrich Christiani’s strikingly antithetical, anti-scientific

understanding of artistic rhythm from 1885. In explaining the difference between positive (strong,

heavy) and negative (weak, light) grammatical accents, Christiani asserts:

“In fact, any kind of diversion — is generally a welcome change, a relief. Harmony, after

discord, is a new pleasure; sunshine, after rain, gives fresh enjoyment. And so with

rhythm.

A break in the rhythmic form gives more real animation to a movement and

stronger evidence of artistic spirit, than strict observance of uniformity, or of positive

rules, could possibly do.

Contrast, not uniformity, is a condition in every work of art. The petty artist, the

mere scholar, will keep within the boundary of traditional rules; the great artist, the

creator, the genius will go beyond them.”

See Adolph Friedrich Christiani, The Principles of Expression in Pianoforte Playing (New York:

Harper & Brothers, 1885), 69.

32

According to contemporaneous French psychologist Théodule Ribot, Wundt justified his

mechanical-mathematical values of once-creative rhythm, in part, through the theories of ancient

Greek geometry, which sought to define “perfect” proportions of the body, the heavens, and

harmonic relationships. Wundt’s chronographic science, however, extended these theoretical

concepts of proportionality to the actualization of rhythms using precision machines entirely

unknown to the ancients. In the years following Wundt’s founding of the Leipzig laboratory,

Ribot recognized this somewhat anachronistic justification “suffices to indicate Wundt’s

conception of aesthetics. In the order of auditory sensation, he studies similarly the three factors,

rhythm, melody, and harmony; and deduces their mathematical conditions.” Wundt assumed that

the perfect theoretical proportions of musical intervals relate to the human perceptions and

Page 263: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

242

metronome became the laboratory chronograph of all musical thought and action. In

Outlines of Psychology, Wundt exposes this foundational value shift where the

traditional, subjective time of musical performance quietly transformed into the objective,

mechanical rhythm of psychological experimentation:

When the [metronomic] rate passes the upper limit of about one every 0.12 sec.,

the formation of distinctly defined temporal ideas is impossible because the

attention can not follow the impressions any longer. The most favorable rate is a

succession of [metronomic] strokes, one every 0.2 — 0.3 sec. With this rate and

with the simplest rhythm of 2/8 time which generally arises of itself when the

perception is uninfluenced by any special objective conditions, as a rule, 8 double

or 16 single [metronomic] impressions can be just grasped together. The best

rhythm for the perception in one group of the greatest possible number of single

impressions is the 4/4-measure with the strong accent on the first stroke and the

medium accent on the fifth. In this case a maximum of five feet or forty single

impressions, can be grasped at once. If these figures are compared with those

obtained when the scope of attention was measured, putting simple and compound

temporal impressions equal to the corresponding spacial impressions, we find that

the scope of consciousness is about four times as great as that of attention.33

For Wundt and his school, the redundant metronomic rhythm of his attention experiments

precisely equated to musical meter. In a radical epistemological shift that served his

chronographic method, Wundt conflated precise clock time and the constant stimulus of

metronomic “impressions” with his reinterpretation of a now objectified, mechanized

musical meter and measure. In Wundt’s mind, the metronome was no meager initial pulse

reference—it was a chronographic regulator for all musical perceptions and

performances.

actualizations of musical rhythm, as Ribot summarized, “The important point is the method; it is

briefly this. We make a physiological and physical analysis of sensations which produce an

aesthetic effect, fix this analysis with numbers, and thus derive a law.” See Théodule Ribot,

German Psychology of To-day: The Empirical School, translated by James Mark Baldwin (New

York: Charles Scribner’s and Sons, 1886), 229.

In the twentieth century, Jaques-Dalcroze would offer similar justifications for modern

rhythmic action, as a neo-classical ideal of motion applied through his scientific pedagogy of

Eurhythmics.

33 Wundt, Outlines of Psychology, 236.

Page 264: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

243

Wundt’s chronographic concept of musical meter directly transferred to a new,

mechanical interpretation of music notation, in which each metronome click precisely

defined the movement of the invariable eighth note—for each and every time signature

commonly seen in print. Music meter became part and parcel of the attention experiment;

out of the repetitive stream of clicking eighth notes, variations of volume-intensity alone

created the phenomenon of mental groupings. Wundt thus redefined a new law of musical

pulse, a once sensory epistemology that Riemann previously considered the “agogic

accent.” Through chronography, Wundt literally mechanized musical time and notation:

If we denote the metronome-strokes by quavers, and the bell-signal by an accent

placed above them, an experiment consisting of two successive series may be

represented in this way:— 34

Wundt continued to represent his modern chronographic values through conventional

music notation, further transferring the active techniques of speech and music

performances to the limited perception of metronomic rhythm in his mechanical

laboratory:

If we give ourselves quietly up to the apprehension of the [metronomic]

impressions, we observe that the separate strokes are not all alike, even though

they are really and objectively perfectly equal. We alternately accentuate and slur

them, just as we do in marking time in speech, by a voluntary and regularly

alternating intensity of accentuation. If we denote the accented impressions by

points placed above them, we have the two series of our former figure as they

usually occur in reality [in the mind]:— 35

34 Wundt, Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology, 262.

35

Ibid., 263.

Page 265: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

244

A certain degree of complexity may result simply from the effort to hold as many

impressions as possible together in consciousness. You may quite easily obtain

the following system, e.g., in which the different degrees of accentuation are

again denoted by points, the strongest by three, the next by two, and the weakest

by one,— 36

In Wundt’s attention-study examples, music notation was reduced to invariable

metronomic precision. Wundt mistranslated the redundant succession of metronomic

clicks—expressed in automatical eighth notes with the occasional artificial bell-accent—

directly into traditional, physically actualized musical and poetic meters:

If we look at the metrical forms employed in music and poetry, we find again that

the limit of three degrees of accent is never exceeded. The absolute amount of

accentuation may, of course, be very different in different cases. But in immediate

perception these different degrees are always arranged in three principal classes,

which alone are of any real importance in metrical division as a basis of

classification for rhythmical forms…a time like the 6/4 is one of the most

complex of the rhythms employed in music. Its scheme is the following:—

It contains, you see, only twelve simple impressions.

37

In Wundt’s novel metronome experiments, new musical values emerged; the perception

of metronomic impressions in the laboratory became the reductive “time” of living

musical performance.

Once Wundt assumed an eighth-note metronomic regularity for all time signatures,

regardless of affect, melodic, harmonic or formal structure, without reference to physical

36 Ibid., 264.

37

Ibid., 265.

Page 266: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

245

or rhetorical gesture, he notated a new precision-oriented quality for musical time, a

lifeless automatical underpinning of note values in which the subjective sense and

sensation of human pulse and movement, traditionally gleaned from the musical meter,

conceptually died.

Scientific Interpretations of Musical Performances and Performers

Wundt’s fallacy in transferring the rhythm of attention-span studies to musical time can

be explained thusly: usually, in any given composition in the Western music tradition,

more than 40 distinct rhythmic “impressions” exist, and co-exist in various parts—and

yet sources throughout history readily recognize the existence of musical rhythm (albeit

of a non-metronomic variety). No evidence over the course of history confirms a wide-

scale problem of mental perception, in which experienced musicians, dancers, or poets

consistently failed to mentally “grasp” a succession of complex rhythms—involving, for

instance, hocket, counterpoint, diminution, syncopation, tempo rubato or imbroglio—

during performances.

Wundt explained this problem away by reducing the idea of musical meter to a

single voice in a constant succession of metronomic eight notes, commenting: “As a

matter of fact, however, music and poetry never push their use of this aid in the formation

of easily comprehended ideational series to the extreme limit of conscious grouping.

Each member in a rhythmical series must be referred to its predecessors, and for this to

be done with pleasure and without effort, it is necessary that the grasp of consciousness

be not too heavily taxed.” In other words, musical time—that is, Wundt’s revision of

Page 267: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

246

musical time as an equal and repeating series of metronomic “impressions”—was never

rhythmically complex enough to burden musicians’ minds.38

The reason that rhythmically varied and complex compositions existed and could

be performed without the benefit of Wundt’s chronographic procedures may seem

obvious: the physical, experiential qualities of musical time had little to do with passive

exposure to metronomic clicks. The rhythmic groupings elicited from automatical,

artificial machines in Wundt’s laboratory had little to do with the lived practices of

creative performers and their physical actualization of musical and poetic meter outside

of laboratory confines. As we continue to find, musical rhythm as practiced (and

explained) by skilled performers was not accurately accounted for in Wundt’s

methodology, which instead entrusted the metronome to reduce and redefine values of

rhythm once solely founded on sensory human experience.

Wundt was not the only scientist attempting to create theoretical connections

between musical and poetic rhythm, intangible mental perceptions, and objective

mechanical motion. While skilled nineteenth-century musicians knew how to play

together “in time” without the constant regulation or reference to the metronome, some

scientists realized that this process was due to more complex understandings of rhythm—

which pendulums, metronomes, and chronographs failed to account for—consisting of

internalized anticipations and perceptions of pulse, spoken rhetoric, and physical

movement. Ribot’s 1886 English edition of German Psychology To-day offered one

scientist’s conjecture on why metronomic time in Wundt’s laboratory had little to due

with musical time in living performance:

38 Ibid., 265.

Page 268: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

247

“It is certain,” says Wolf, “that at the moment of the passage, the observer does

not hear the stroke of the pendulum, but an internal stroke which his thought

substitutes for it, just as the musician does not wait for the stroke of the director's

baton, but catches himself the rhythmic advance of the measure.”39

It is striking that this early experimental psychologist compares the laboratory subject’s

tests by a metronome to the professional musician’s performance by the conductor’s

baton—a significantly different temporal reference. Wolf likened the director’s baton to

the accuracy and precision of the clockwork metronome, implying that the musician’s

performance on stage was identical to the subject’s chronographic test in the laboratory.

The reality, however, was even more complex and nuanced than many psychologists

including Wolf recognized, since knowledgeable musicians of the century, as Chapters I

and II demonstrated, valued conductors who displayed more than mere metronomic

technique, directors who could influence a body of instrumentalists to act as one nuanced,

expressive soloist.

William James, a philosophic-scientist distinctly separated from Wundt’s

empirical school but certainly informed by it, realized that perceptions of time, as a

general phenomenon, represented more complex qualities than could be accounted for

through artificial laboratory machines and methods. Indeed, James instinctually

understood that the mechanical constructs and measurements of time—which the

industrializing civilization increasingly relied upon—bore little relation to living action,

either to bodily movements or mental perceptions (not to mention music performances).

James echoed centuries-old understandings: time perception was a subjective

phenomenon, an individual sense. And like the other subjective senses, such as touch,

sight, and smell, the mind did not quantify relationships, variations, or proportions with

39 Ribot, 253.

Page 269: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

248

mechanical exactitudes or mathematical expressions. In the Principles of Psychology

(1890), James voiced this temporal epistemology still prevalent in pre-twentieth century

society:

The durations we have practically most to deal with—minutes, hours, and

days40

—have to be symbolically conceived, and constructed by mental addition,

after the fashion of those extents of hundreds of miles and upward, which in the

field of space are beyond the range of most men’s practical interests

altogether…To realize an hour, we must count ‘now!—now!—now!—now!—’

indefinitely. Each ‘now’ is the feeling of a separate bit of time, and the exact sum

of the bits never makes a very clear impression on our mind.41

Even through the 1880s, intellectuals such as James still considered precise, instant-by-

instant mechanical rhythm an abstraction to the experiential, sensory flow of time and its

movement. Precise artificial time—the chronographic reference in Wundt’s laboratory—

did not equate to a living reality. James described the concept known to metaphysical

philosophers as the “specious present:” the experiential perception of time that includes

the subjective sense of an intangible moment, a fleeting instant, contextualized by the

expectation of the future and the recollection of the past.42

Thus, in 1890, while Wundt

continued drilling subjects to perceive and react within the monotonous rhythm dictated

by the metronome, James contended that this precise laboratory procedure “never makes

a very clear impression on our mind” when applied to daily life and the experiential

40 N.B. James does not mention seconds, since the mechanical second was still an overly precise

construct to be a living perception at the time of his writing.

41

From William James, Principles of Psychology, Vol. 1 (New York: Henry Holt and Compnay,

1890), 611. Reproduced in Charles M. Sherover, ed., The Human Experience of Time, the

Development of its Philosophic Meaning, (Reprint, Evanston: Northwestern University Press,

2001), 372-3.

42

For a further explanation of the “specious present,” see James, 609-613. James illustrates, “In

short, the practically cognized present is no knife edge, but a saddle-back, with a certain breadth

of its own on which we sit perched, and from which we look in two directions into time.”

Page 270: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

249

feeling of time flow. He writes this comment, of course, before the clockwork metronome

became the universal temporal standard in the American middle-class musical household

or public-school classroom.

Other scientists in the 1880s, those unaffected by the high-technology

chronographic laboratory of experimental psychology, realized that the metronome could

not absolutely define the living realities of musical time or clarify the vagaries of music

notation. Reporting in The English Mechanic and World of Science (1884), William John

Grey, an analytical chemist and frequent contributor to the periodical,43

made a novel

musical experiment, quantifying with a metronome various music tempos in print in the

hopes of ascertaining scientific certainties regarding performance practices. (Whether

Grey used a simple pendulum or a clockwork metronome is uncertain.) “It occurred to

me some years ago,” he writes, “to endeavor to make a table of the metronome

equivalents to the various expressions used in music for indicating the rate of speed; but

on examining into the subject I found considerable disagreement, and abandoned the

attempt.”44

Years later, he realized the errors of his scientifically oriented methodology,

stating, “The results are not very concordant, and seem to indicate that the words are used

somewhat at random, and perhaps may be regarded as referring to style, rather than

time.”45

He inadvertently discovered, as musicians of the century already acknowledged,

that affect words, which required personal interpretation for each composition, were

43 See The English Mechanic and World of Science 53 (April 24, 1891): 188; and (May 8, 1891):

233.

44

William Grey, “Time,” The English Mechanic and World of Science 39 (May 9, 1884): 218.

45

Ibid.

Page 271: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

250

incommensurate with simplified, referential metronome numbers.46

Grey’s scientific

assertion:

On the whole, the conclusion we must come to is that the word indications of time

are not very reliable for translations into the beats of the metronome, and they

would seem unnecessary when the time is given by that instrument, except for the

benefit of those who have got one.47

By 1891, Grey’s summation had resonated with the readership of The English Mechanic

and World of Science, where some voiced considerable disagreement regarding the

proper use of the metronome in music. For these advocates of scientific methods, the

problem was how to apply the objective reference of mechanical time to subjective

reality of musical performance. Indeed, the very definitions of time, movement, and

rhythm seemed at odds in their discourse. According to one respondent, “Time is the

speed at which any music is played, and is governed by the expressions Andante, Largo,

&c., and by the metronome mark where given. The rhythm is the swing of the music, and

The “Metronome” entry in the first edition Grove Dictionary (1880) is highly critical as well of the of beats-per-minute translation of affect words, which began appearing on the more recently designed metronomes. The reference argued that the two epistemologies—mechanical indications and expressive words—are incommensurate with one another: “Some Metronomes are marked with the words Andante, Allegretto, Allegro, etc., in addition to the numbers. This is a new, and utterly useless contrivance: for it is evident, that, if q =100 be held to indicate Moderato, h =100 will stand for Allegro, and e=100 for Largo. The word Moderato, therefore, without the Minum, Crotchet, or Quaver, to qualify it, means nothing at all; and it is absurd to encumber the scale with it, or with any other technical terms whatever.” See George Grove, ed. A Dictionary of Music and Musicians, Vol. II (London: Macmillan and Co., 1880), 320.

47 The English Mechanic and World of Science 39 (May 9, 1884): 218.

Grey’s assessment bares close similarity to Gottfried Weber’s comment on the

impracticality of the clockwork metronome during Maelzel’s lifetime. Weber felt, “It is to be

regretted that the presuppositions in this case are so numerous, and particularly that such

metronomical signs are not only useless to all those who do not possesses these machines, but

also even to those who have them, except in cases where the machine can be placed close by.”

See Gottfried Weber, Godfrey Weber's General Music Teacher: Adapted to Self-instruction, Both

for Teachers and Learners; Embracing Also an Extensive Dictionary of Musical Terms,

translated by James Franklin Warner (Boston: J. H. Wilkins & R.B. Carter, 1841), 75.

Page 272: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

251

corresponds with the meter in poetry.”48

H. Brocklehurst concluded, similarly to skilled

musicians of the century, that the metronome was an initial pulse reference, not a

regulator, and he urged English Mechanic readers not to neglect the essential factor that

defines musical time:

The speed at which hymn tunes are played is governed by the taste and experience

of the organist, and is one of the safest guides in judging the capability and

discrimination of the performer.49

A later commentator to the periodical—and probably a trained musician—writing under

the pseudonym “Organon,” confirmed and elaborated upon Brocklehurst’s traditional

nineteenth-century aesthetic:

It is the rhythm that has to be studied, the mere “time” [tempo] may be anything

you please, though, as a rule, it is only courteous to adopt that indicated by the

composer. In the Hymns A[nglican] and M[ethodist] the “time” is rarely

indicated, and the organist is left to suit his taste: but do, please, understand that if

you wish to play “music” you must attend to the accent—the ictus metricus, so to

speak. The bars are simply used as guides. A real musician could play without

bars at all.50

Regardless of the comments of contemporaneous musicians, engineers, and scientists in

other fields, Wundt’s empirical school of experimental psychology continued to

misapprehend this intrinsic variability of artistic rhythm in favor of what can be

recognized as the chronographic bias: their overriding belief that, in measuring the

“incessant now” of automatical time and action, scientists efficiently solved the

“problems” of human variability for “operations” and “observations” as diverse as

manual labor, memory recollection, and even music performance.

48 The English Mechanic and World of Science 53 (May 8, 1891): 232.

49

Ibid.

Ibid., 253.

Page 273: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

252

Conceiving Musical Chronography and the Chronographic Musician

The chronographic method, as early as it appeared in the astronomical observatory,

represented a dualistic belief system that assumed the following: the chronograph was the

constant, absolute reference and regulator, while the human observer was the variable to

be strictly regarded against that mechanical standard. First in the astronomical

observatory and then in Wundt’s laboratory, the human performer was held accountable

to the exacting mechanical time and rhythm given by the chronographic apparatus. In this

light, the observer (i.e. the subject) was an imperfect machine, replete with errors in

reaction and response to the chronograph; his individual temporal perceptions and

subsequent physical actions exposed his fallible personal equation. Starting with Wundt’s

new science, the musician was considered under these same guidelines—as a mere, faulty

performer when referenced against mechanical time. It was the psychologist’s duty to

uncover these performer errors, and indeed, even correct for the musician’s personal

equation. Given their complete reliance on laboratory apparatus and trust in time-telling

technologies, these new psychologists had little interest in proving the ephemeral,

nuanced, and variable qualities of musical time, which defied the reductive methods of

their science and transcended the repetitive rhythms of their laboratory equipment.

In 1895, Wundt’s former student Edward Wheeler Scripture (1864-1945)—in

stark contrast to William James’ past comments on the abstractions of mechanical time—

exposed this modern conceptual bias towards a time controlled by automatical machines,

one that invariably defined individual creativity and expression in the industrialized

world:

Page 274: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

253

Time, the great independent variable, is the only force over which we can gain no

control. Man can annihilate space and fight power with power, but—tick, tick,

tick—the little watch counts off the seconds, not one of which can be hindered

from coming or be recalled when past.51

Scripture was one of Wundt’s most successful students, and he later became director of

Yale’s department of experimental psychology. Throughout his professional career,

Scripture maintained a keen interest in solving the “problems” of efficient rhythmic

action under the seemingly uncontrollable “tick, tick, tick” of chronographic reference.

While Scripture heard live orchestra performances displaying slightly more complex

temporal qualities than those dictated by laboratory methods and metronomes, he also

likened the conductor’s baton to a monotonous chronograph, one in which the orchestra

musician strove precisely to conform to, all while factoring in the personal equation—the

human errors of actualizing the mathematically correct time of musical performance. In

his 1895 article “Some Principles of Mental Education,” Scripture explains, “A player in

an orchestra or a band must not be ahead or behind the others. He must not be irregular.

He must, however, have a constant error in being just as far behind [the] baton of the

leader as the others are.”52

Poetic meter, expressive contrasts, rhetorical gestures—or

even the comments of skilled musicians regarding their own experiences—were not

factors in Scripture’s assessments of “regular” musical performance and subsequent

musician “error” represented by their non-metronomic musicality.

Unchecked by chronography, an individual’s experiential knowledge reeked of

the variable, personal equation. Wundt and his school, in order to first define and then

51 E. W. Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing (Meadville: The Chautauqua-Century Press, 1895),

61.

52

E. W. Scripture, “Some Principles of Mental Education,” The School Review 3 (Nov., 1895):

542.

Page 275: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

254

solve these chronographic problems of musical performance, consistently devalued the

fundamental humanity of the individual musician. In his Outlines of Psychology, Wundt

scientifically diminished the education of the performing instrumentalist:

This “enigma of the instincts” ceases to be an enigma when we come to look upon

instincts, as we have done above, as special forms of impulsive action, and

consider them as analogous to the simple impulsive acts of men and animals, for

which we have a psychological explanation…Such reduction can be easily

observed in the case of man, as, for example, in the habituation to complex

movements in learning to play the piano.53

Wundt unapologetically reduced musicians to the psychologist’s scientific and

technological ideal: the human as mere machine, an automaton with blind instincts and

habitual impulses, lacking in forethought or reflection. In one of his published lectures,

Wundt further described lay musicians (perhaps with musical abilities similar to his

own), who functioned like typical subjects in his laboratory:

The novice at the piano must strain his attention upon every note, in order to find

the appropriate key; the practised player translates the printed page mechanically

into the proper movements. Any movement that has become altogether habitual is

made instinctively. An impulse of will is, of course, necessary at the outset; but its

effect extends to a whole series of actions, and each particular one takes place

without effort and without knowledge: the series once started is continued to its

end with the same unconscious certainty and purposiveness as the reflex.54

53 Wundt, Outlines of Psychology, 314.

54

Wundt, Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology. 394-5. Wundt’s belief in the mechanized

musician perhaps originates from his physiological training, in which human actions were

considered the habituated outcropping of muscular reactions to nerve impulses. For instance,

William Carpenter in Principles of Mental Physiology (first published in 1874) states nearly

identically of impulsive action: “60. Thus, then, while the Human organism may be likened to a

keyed instrument, from which any music it is capable of producing can be called-forth at the will

of the performer, who may compare a Bee or any other Insect to a barrel-organ, which plays with

the greatest exactness a certain number of tunes that are set upon it, but can do nothing else.” See

William Carpenter, Principles of Mental Physiology, Fourth edition (New York: D. Appleton and

Company, 1883), 61. Also see 217, 218 for more theories on the automatic musician. James

reprints parts of Carpenter’s discussion in The Principles of Psychology, 117.

Page 276: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

255

In describing learned and habitual activities, Wundt exemplified mediocre, amateur

musicians—students that may have been more familiar to him than performing musical

“artists,” such as Chopin, Schumann, Liszt, or Brahms. To Wundt, the “practiced”

musician was distinguished from a “novice” by being perfectly automatical. He

considered a trained musician’s skill to manifest in blind, will-less, and mechanical

actions—unconscious reflexes made apparent only to the experimental psychologist.

Wundt’s disparaging attitude towards all musicians resonated throughout his

scientific discipline, well into the twentieth century. Ernst Meumann, one of Wundt’s

most influential students, viewed musicians—experienced performers no less—nearly

identically to his teacher’s mechanical ideal. An American Journal of Psychology (1902)

article explained Meumann’s conception of the living performer:

The high degree of accuracy with which a musician can produce a required

fraction of a standard [time] interval when it is part of a musical score must be

ascribed largely to this motor appreciation, i.e. to the facility with which a

rhythmic movement becomes automatic. In playing with both hands another

means of control is possibly furnished by the aid one hand may give to the other.

For example, when the right hand plays a complicated passage, the left supported

by its automatism can preserve the original rhythm, marking for the right hand the

essential points of each measure. Counting introduces still another motor

procedure, which becoming quickly automatic acts as a steadying influence. The

movements of the director have a similar result. The effect of all this automatism

is to unburden the attention so that it may concentrate itself upon the musical

purport of tones.55

Meumann’s non-experiential understanding of musicians and musicality confirms

Wundt’s laboratory culture, in which chronographic practices informed and influenced all

aspects of time and action. Under psychologists’ chronographic bias, the ideal musical

performer—like the ideal laboratory subject— habituated through repetitive, precise

55 Charles H. Sears, “A Contribution to the Psychology of Rhythm,” The American Journal of

Psychology 13 (Jan., 1902): 30.

Page 277: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

256

training to passively react in small gradations of automatic time with mindless,

“unburdened attention.” These scientists wholly devalued the traditional musical

techniques perceived and actualized by skilled performers while they embraced the two,

mutually exclusive values of scientific chronography: To believe in the absolute truth of

their chronographic method, psychologists needed to believe that living individuals—

regardless of personal intention, intelligence, and expression—held intrinsic and

disdainful “errors” when compared to their chronographic apparatus.

While Wundt could not compose, improvise, or perform the music of others with

any semblance of experiential knowledge, he scientifically presumed to define the trained

pianist’s abilities, in which,

In most cases there are no acts of intelligence involved at all, but only

associations; and in any case intelligent action must have been reduced to

association before it could became mechanized. The piano-player has first of all

to form a stable association between the printed note and the movement of touch.

But this association gradually lapses from consciousness, and the interconnection

of movements becomes purely mechanical.56

Wundt considered musical ideas and actions completely quantifiable with scientific

certainty; his was a radically differing conception of music creation than any skilled

nineteenth-century performers, teachers, or composers would have recognized or

condoned. Without learning music to any high degree, Wundt was certain that the

musical “instinct” could be fully explained and reduced with new scientific methods and

mechanical apparatus. Indeed, Wundt reduced all of music history and creation to blind

ritual, naïve and simple “ceremonial games,” which are:

56 Wundt, Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology, 398.

Page 278: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

257

Religious ceremonies secularized for the purposes of amusement. Here belong the

dance, the march, and many children’s games, in which the essential factor in the

enjoyment is the pleasure aroused by regular rhythmical movements performed in

unison, especially when accompanied by music or singing. Music itself is nothing

but a game of this order, so highly developed that it has grown to be a form of

art.57

Perhaps also influenced by the physiological pedagogy of gymnastics, Wundt’s

drastically revised (and highly subjective) epistemology of human creativity represented

a distinct paradigm shift in which automatical machines infallibly dictated time and

rhythm for musical, and indeed all cultural performances. This epistemology is again

described through one simple concept: the chronographic bias.

Upon reading the wealth of laboratory research by experimental psychologists on

the mental phenomena of rhythm, it seems safe to assume very few of these new

scientists had any experiential knowledge in the fields of artistic performance,

composition, improvisation, speech, drama, or dance. Rather, many of them seemed to

have a markedly second-hand understanding of musical creation and living rhythm.

Often, they gained their musical knowledge exclusively through passive listening. Edwin

Boring in A History of Experimental Psychology alluded to Wundt’s disdain for the

musician and musical experience during a scientific disagreement with the psychologist

Carl Stumpf, a trained performer and founder of “tone psychology.” Wundt discounted

Stumpf’s experiments on sound perception, believing that his colleague’s musical

education actually detracted from objective, scientific research. Indeed, Wundt

considered his own lack of musical expertise to be a scientific virtue. Boring paraphrased

Wundt’s self-assessment: “He relied on laboratory results with apparatus and the

57 Wilhelm Max Wundt, Ethics: An Investigation of the Facts and Laws of the Moral Life,

translated from the Second German Edition (1892) by Titchener, Gulliver, and Washburn, Vol. 1

(London: Swan Sonnenschein & Co., Lim., 1902), 209-210.

Page 279: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

258

psychophysical methods. Whatever is obtained under unprejudiced, carefully controlled

experimental conditions must be right, Wundt virtually said.”58

Perhaps Wundt failed to

realize that his sole reliance on chronographs and metronomes to solve the problems of

the human mind reflected, in itself, a prejudiced condition.

One of Wundt’s most influential students, Titchener admitted to his own musical

inexperience in endnotes to a published lecture: “I have practically no gift of musical

composition, and my skill as a performer is below zero.”59

Nonetheless, Titchener

considered himself to be skilled enough to claim judgments over musical creativity and

aesthetics. He founded his authority upon his upbringing, being raised after all in a

middle-class “musical family,” where he “was fortunate enough to hear a great deal of

the best piano music in my childhood.”60

He never claimed to perform “the best” music

in his youth; he only passively listened to other amateurs in familiar surroundings. (It

would be enticing to discover whether his “musical family” made extensive use of the

clockwork metronome.)61

Titchener reflects, “My musical endowment…consists in a

58 Edwin G. Boring, A History of Experimental Psychology (New York: Appleton-Century

Company, 1929), 354-5.

59

Edward Bradford Titchener, Lectures on the Experimental Psychology of the Thought-

Processes (New York: The Macmillan Company, 1909), 205.

60

Ibid.

61

Invoking the increasingly popular mid-nineteenth-century “musical” family as depicted by

Titchener—along with the typical dearth of musical talent contained therein—Adolph Bernhard

Marx, the Founder of the Berlin Conservatory, was highly critical of the new trends of middle-

class music pedagogy, which specifically arose in Germany during Wundt’s youth. Marx believed

that the educational models intended to instruct the masses in the technical skills of piano-

playing—due to overriding social and economic interests—devalued an individual’s creative and

“artistic” potential through routine practice and performance.

In The Music of the Nineteenth Century and Its Culture (1854), Marx rails at the culture

in which Wundt and later Titchener formulated their sense of musical aesthetics: “Look at our

domestic music! It is scarcely necessary to ask: who is musical? but, rather, who is not? In the so-

called higher or more refined circles of society, music has long been looked upon as an

Page 280: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

259

quick and comprehensive understanding of a composition, a sort of logical and aesthetic

Einfühlung, an immediate (or very rapid) grasp of the sense and fitness of the musical

structure.”62

Titchener never discussed the measures or qualifications by which he

deemed music compositions to be “fit” or “sensible.” Yet this comment reveals

Titchener’s highly personal aesthetics and education: he perceived music composition,

and by extension music performance, as he would the “sensibility” of a subject’s mind

and the “fitness” of his body, through a physiological-psychological examination.63

While lacking in musical expertise, experimental psychologists—given their training,

traditions, and environment—seemed to have an intimate understanding primarily of the

chronographic method: the controlled and reproducible research of human perception and

action guided by metronomic rhythm within the confines of their laboratory.64

indispensable branch of education. In every family it is cultivated, if possible, by all the members,

without particular regard to talent or inclination…we have to take into account that these lessons

and hours of practice have to be wedged in between the hours of continuous school and study;

and that this press of occupation does not even leave the necessary leisure for the full inward

apprehension of art itself; leaving out of the question the harmonic development of the entire

man…But where necessity or love of gain, and not a natural inclination and pure delight in art,

has been the moving spring, there industry and conscientiousness, being called forth, not by a

love for the thing itself, but by a feeling of duty and necessity, can at best be but of an external

nature; there may be found diligent study, but no deep interest—much practice, but all

mechanical and abstract.” See Adolph Bernhard Marx, The Music of the Nineteenth Century and

Its Culture (London: Robert Cocks and Co., 1854), 73, 74.

62 Titchener, Lectures, 205.

63 Ibid. Titchener admits: “There is thus a fairy close analogy between my apprehension of music

and the visual schematizing of arguments which is described in the Lecture. It would be

interesting to know whether the correlation is at all general.”

64 Titchener did acknowledge that rhythms occurred in nature without reference or regulation of

the metronome. In An Outline of Psychology (1902), he reiterates many of the physical paradigms

of rhythmic action in pre-modern societies: walking, breathing, poetic meter, all in a non-

mechanical arsis and thesis. Nevertheless, he faithfully follows Wundt’s modern metronome-

methodology of rhythm, as he judges: “Sounds are, indeed, better material for the idea of rhythm

than are tactual complexes; for the limbs are fixed to the trunk, and can therefore do no more than

oscillate to and fro, pendulum fashion, giving of necessity the most rudimentary form of

rhythm,—beat’ beat, beat’ beat,—whereas a series of sounds can be divided into groups of any

Page 281: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

260

Wundt indeed recognized that rhythm was relatable to bodily movements, but for

him the body was completely analogous to an automatic machine. Thus, living movement

in “rhythm” logically reduced to simple chronographic explanation, as Wundt states in

Outlines of Psychology, “The mechanical properties of the limbs are important

physiological bases for the rise of these ideas…we have movements in which the

voluntary energy of the muscles is operative only so far as it is required to set the limbs

oscillating in their points and to maintain this movement—rhythmical movements.”65

Through his extensive and influential publications, Wundt continued to refashion

“rhythmical” action with chronographic intentions. His interpretations of a living

individual’s “rhythmical movements” consistently employed terminology once reserved

for lifeless mechanical timekeepers:

With rhythmical movements the case is different. Their significance for the

psychological development of time ideas is due to the same principle as that

which gives them their importance as physiological organs, namely, the principle

of the isochronism of oscillations of like amplitude…Every single period of

oscillation in such a movement is made up of a continuous succession of

sensation which are repeated in the following period in exactly the same order.66

Given the psychologist’s exclusive understanding of rhythm as a perception and reaction

to external, mechanical stimulus—primarily in the repetitive, periodic rhythm of the

scientific metronome—Wundt again described the paradigmatic human, and human

musician, as a perfect automaton who projected automatical rhythms based in exactitude,

complexity. The rhythm: beat” beat beat, beat’ beat beat, beat” beat beat, beat’ beat beat, could

not be formed from tactual impressions.” As Titchener states, the “better” stimulus for “rhythm”

is founded on metronomic sound, not visual pulse or movement. He then translates these auditory

impressions, following Wundt’s epistemology, into music notation, in which the eighth-note

assumed metronomic consistency. See Titchener, An Outline of Psychology, 182-3.

65 Wundt, Outlines of Psychology, 159. Wundt’s original emphasis makes the relationship

between automatical machines and the epistemology of scientific “rhythm” all the more striking.

66

Ibid., 160.

Page 282: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

261

precision, and isochronism. Indeed, Wundt seemed to describe the perfectly moving

human as the perfectly regulated clockwork metronome.

To summarize, Wundt’s experimental science represented the origins of a radical

value shift for individual performances through the precise relationship to chronographic

reference. His laboratory experiments, especially on the research and training of rhythmic

perception, began a tradition through which later psychologists continued to

misapprehend the living practices of the musician, musicality, and musical rhythm.

Beginning in the 1890s, notable European and American psychologists, working under

Wundt’s methodologies and values, applied this new, objective “rhythm” to the scientific

study of living performers themselves. In the process these researchers further redefined

musicality through increasingly precise chronographic rules and regulations. In

laboratories across Europe and America these scientists would judge to ever-greater

extents the living rhythms of music performance under the precise mechanical guidelines

of chronography, where, not surprisingly, they always found the individual musician—

his personal equation—technically flawed.67

67 Adolf Bernhard Marx expressed starkly opposite values of musical artistry, performance, and

aesthetics in the earlier decades of the nineteenth century. Marx acknowledged the ultimately

complex and irreducible qualities of creative human thought and sensation in The Music of the

Nineteenth Century and Its Culture (1854). He indirectly argued against what would, by the end

of the century, become the chronographic study of the musician and living musicality:

“The human mind is furnished, by means of the senses, with a multitude of facts and

phenomena. Of these it retains and assimilates a greater or smaller number according to

its necessity and power; it distinguishes those that are dissimilar, recognizes and

compares those that are alike, or have some features in common, and draws conclusions

from all these observations. Thus it gradually expands, becomes more active, and

acquires what we call consciousness, both of itself and of the world around. It is,

however, to be observed, that no individual thing or isolated fact can, by itself, become

the object of mental contemplation; inasmuch as every thing that exists, or every fact that

takes place, is not only itself a compound of many things or facts, but also stands in close

relation with a number of other objects and phenomena. Thus, e.g. man appears to be an

individual being; and yet how many different things are to be distinguished in this being!

Page 283: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

262

The Living Musician, the Experimental Psychologist,

and the Laboratory Chronograph

It was one matter for Wundt to idealize the perfect person as a precise clockwork

machine and the typical musician as a blind automaton. It was yet another matter when

experimental psychologists—beginning with Ernst Meumann in Europe and Thaddeus

Bolton and Edward Wheeler Scripture in North America—actively employed the

chronographic method on trained musicians, testing their subjects musical abilities

through the exactitudes offered only through precision time-keeping machines. Guided

by Wundt’s scientific values, these and other psychologists not only revised the values of

musical rhythm towards a mechanized ideal, but invented a new performance practice, a

new goal for the musician, one corresponding with Wundt’s imaginary clockwork

creatures, who moved rhythmically based on “the principle of the isochronism of

oscillations of like amplitude.”

Once experimental psychologists defined rhythm through the chronographic bias,

human volition and expressive variation diminished in the mind of the researcher,

student, and faithful believer of modern science. A brief example from the modern

pedagogue William Morris Patterson and his book The Rhythm of Prose: An

Experimental Investigation of Individual Difference in the Sense of Rhythm (1916),

exposes how, by the second decade of the twentieth century, the non-mechanical nature

of human action was seen as a flaw through the prism of chronographic study. Reiterating

His body with all its powers and organs, his mental endowments, intellectual

developments, character, inclinations and manners! And then, how manifold are his

relations to the outer world, to his family, his friends, or his enemies, to his nation, and

the different ranks and classes of people, to his times, and to a thousand minor things and

conditions! The mind cannot possibly be satisfied with the contemplation of individual

objects or acts, or even with a number of isolated things or phenomena.” See Marx, 22.

Page 284: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

263

the ideas of experimental psychologists before him, Patterson affirms, “Rhythm is thus

regarded as first of all an experience, established, as a rule, by motor performances, of

however rudimentary a nature.”68

In justifying these scientific values of rhythm, he relied

heavily upon Margaret Keiver Smith’s study Rhythmus und Arbeit (1900), imprinted

under Wundt’s Philosophie Studien. A student of Meumann in Zurich, Smith grounded

her labor experiments on the metronome, which also informed her understanding of

artistic rhythm. In Rhythmus und Arbeit, she affirmed the now-standard scientific

aesthetic that “in the arts (namely in poetry, music, and perhaps dance) where the rhythm

is mathematically determined, is found to be of very great regularity and precision.”69

Further exposing her chronographic bias, Patterson recounts:

One interesting result of Miss Smith’s research is that the observers think they are

working to time-beats when they are not. The exactness with which their

movements and the beats of an accompanying metronome coincide varies

according to the individual difference of the subjects.70

As Patterson reported, Smith’s isolated laboratory experiments proved that subjects,

guided only by a physical sense of rhythm, did not naturally or usually move with

metronomic rhythm. Smith’s chronographic conclusion: the subjects’ movements were

not “rhythmical” —the very same quality skilled musicians would have recognized as

automatical or metronomic. A skilled musician, a good “timeist” of the nineteenth

century, could alternatively have argued that Smith’s Rhythmus und Arbeit study showed

68 William Morrison Patterson, The Rhythm of Prose, An Experimental Investigation of Individual

Difference in the Sense of Rhythm (New York: Columbia University Press, 1916), x. Patterson

cites the definition given by experimental psychologist C. A. Ruckmick in a 1913 rhythm study.

69

Margret Keiver Smith, Rhythmus und Arbeit (Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann, 1900), 165. “Bei

der Kunst aber (nämlich bei der Poesie, der Musik und auch vielleicht bei dem Tanz), wo der

Rhythmus mathematisch bestimmtwird, ist eine viel größere Regelmäßigkeit und Genauigkeit zu

finden.”

70 Reported in Patterson, 27.

Page 285: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

264

that subjects’ own internal perception faithfully guided their time-beats—they held a

physically-derived, subjective sense of pulse and movement. Those without a metronome

as reference may have also observed that the actions of these subjects indeed contained

pronounced bodily rhythm. Yet, the subjects’ belief in their own rhythmic performance,

or the visual rhythm seen in their actions, did not factor into the scientific method at all—

scientists’ chronographic bias trumped the experiential, innate rhythm of recurring human

movement.

Psychologists’ devaluation of non-metronomic (i.e. human) rhythm can be again

witnessed in the anthropological research of Charles Myers, who tested indigenous

peoples in his modern Western values of chronographic time and action before 1899.

Myers, like Smith, considered the variable nature of human rhythm, when mechanically

unfettered, to be either faulty or non-existent. The Journal of the Anthropological

Institute of Great Britain and Ireland reports:

Mr. C. S. Myers contributed some observations on Savage Music, dwelling on the

interest of savage music for the anthropologist as contrasted with that of the

musician…The characteristic feature of Murray Island music was a lack of

rhythm in the now obsolete tunes that were reproduced in the phonograph. In the

respect of the complexities of rhythm in other races, Mr. Myers said that from his

own observations on the Malays of Sarawak, there were grounds for suspecting

the futility of search after quarter-tone music, owing to irregularity of

intonation.71

Researchers including Meumann, Smith, Myers and others expressed little regard for the

cultural tradition, location, or the creative intention of their subjects, opting instead to

devalue the variable qualities of chronographically uncontrolled human performances.

Their subjects’ rhythm, in both musical and physical activities, seldom aligned with any

See C. S. Myers, et al., “Anthropological Reviews and Miscellanea,” The Journal of the

Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 29 (1899): 223.

Page 286: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

265

“exact” mechanical values because the “rhythm” psychologists sought could only be

known in observatories and laboratories through scientific methods and apparatuses.72

Eventually pedagogues such as Mary Hallock, Patterson, and Jaques-Dalcroze,

directly informed by the chronographic bias of experimental psychology, wholly

considered “the exactness” of an external metronomic reference to be the definitive and

correct reference of “time-beats,” and thus the absolute truth of “rhythmical”

performance. Again, living rhythm reflected a problem of imprecision, inefficiency, and

error for the psychologist and the modern pedagogue, and in Patterson’s own words, an

intrinsic, in fact troublesome, “individual difference of the subjects.”

Fueled by Wundt’s research into a subject’s mental “rhythmic groupings” of

metronome clicks, Ernst Meumann’s seminal work on the science of musical rhythm, his

University of Leipzig Ph.D. thesis Untersuchungen zur Psychologie und Aesthetik des

Rhythmus (1894), helped to convince later experimental psychologists, including

Meumann’s student Smith, that musical performance was a strict, chronographic process,

an experiment in metronomic musicality, an extension of labor-fatigue research. Charles

H. Sears, in his own rhythmic study, paid homage to Meumann’s objective study of

chronographic musicality:

72 It is intriguing to consider if the experimental research into musical time and action by

Meumann, Bolton, Scripture, Binet and Coutier prefigured and perhaps to some extent inspired

both Smith’s Rhythmus und Arbeit and Karl Bücher’s influential labor study Arbeit und

Rhythmus, Third Edition (Leipzig: B.G. Teubner, 1902). Bücher was the economics professor at

Leipzig University, and was undoubtedly familiar with Wundt’s and Meumann’s studies on

muscular fatigue and attention span.

Page 287: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

266

In treating rhythm from the standpoint of the player Meumann discusses the

question of how the musician is able to produce the exact fractions of time

intervals required by the notes on the written sheet. Experiments made upon

subjects skilled in piano playing to determine how accurately intervals equal to

certain fractional parts of given intervals could be produced when there were no

artificial means of assistance showed that even good musicians were liable to

considerable errors.73

In defining a new standard of musical action—in mathematical fractions of mechanically

referenced time—Meumann actually invented new problems for the “skilled musician,”

who was now charged with performing exactly to the metronome, in all notated

compositions, even when “no artificial means of assistance”—no chronographic

reference—was available in performance. Meumann continued the revolutionary

aesthetical shift in musical time by re-conceiving the very qualities of “accurate”

musicianship. Instead of considering intellectual volition, rhetorical emphasis, physical

accentuation, sensory intuition, or gestural affinity as it related to an individual’s creative

interpretation, Meumann simply and—if we heed the comments of skilled musicians

themselves—incorrectly considered a “good” performer’s inability to mimic exacting

metronomic rhythm one of “considerable error.”

Meumann found that musicians were not naturally metronomes, an observation

confirmed by many psychologists before and after him. Even Wundt admitted that his

subjects’ sense of time flow during experiments varied according to their attention span.74

The physiologist Karl von Vierordt’s seminal metronomic-action experiments from as

early as 1868—research that clearly influenced Meumann in his musical-rhythm

studies—found that a subject, when asked to reproduce the beats of a metronome after

73 Sears, 29.

74

Ribot, 273-4.

Page 288: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

267

the metronome had been stopped, would usually recall faster clicks slower, and slower

clicks faster. Metronomic time, as Vierordt’s initial research showed, was not easily

recollected or reproduced by his human subjects. This phenomenon of perception and

performance regulated in mechanical time is still recognized today as Vierordt’s law.75

As Vierordt’s contemporary Ribot reported, “individual variations” in subjects’ physical

and mental reproduction of metronomic clicks diverged considerably “on both sides of

the exact point.”76

The non-mechanical variability of music performance was a well-known fact to

experimental psychologists in the nineteenth-century. Scientific rhythm researcher Kurt

Ebhardt employed a chronographic piano apparatus that recorded performers through

electrical-contact hammers, and noted in his 1898 musical-time dissertation Zwei

Beiträge zur Psychologie des Rhythmus und des Tempo similar non-metronomic results

to Vierordt. He recognized “that feeling enters in as the determining factor of tempo” and

that even the harmony of a piece may sway instrumentalists from the strictly metronomic

actualization music notation as defined by Wundt’s science.77

By 1899, researchers Shaw

and Wrinch had acknowledged that while “the definite relation of one, a half, a fourth,

and an eight, as in written musical compositions” exists as mathematically precise

75 See “Vierordt's law, n." A Dictionary of Psychology, Andrew M. Colman, ed. (Oxford

University Press, 2006.) Oxford Reference Online, Oxford University Press. (Case Western

Reserve University, Accessed, 18 August 2008),

http://www.oxfordreference.com/views/ENTRY.html?subview=Main&entry=t87.e8789.

76

Ribot, 273.

77

Summarized in Sears, 32. For Ebhardt’s tests, conservatory musicians performed various music

selections by Mozart and Schumann, with harmonic accompaniments and without. See Kurt

Ebhardt, Zwei Beiträge zur Psychologie des Rhythmus und des Tempo (Leipzig: Johann

Ambrosius Barth, 1898), 53, for his chronographic documentation for each performance.

Ebhardt’s explanation of feeling and chronographic variation appears on page 52.

Page 289: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

268

rhythmic values under a chronographic reference, “the relative length of musical notes in

the production of a true musician is not in the exact ration of one to a half, a quarter, an

eight, and so on.”78

Fig. 4.1. Ebhardt’s chronographic piano-apparatus used to research subjects’ metronomic exactitude

during music performances. From Kurt Ebhardt, Zwei Beiträge zur Psychologie des Rhythmus und des

Tempo (1898), 30.

Despite the many empirical observations proving that the time of musical

performance was seldom, if ever, a priori metronomic, Meumann and the psychologists

he influenced, such as Ebhardt, Sears, Scripture, and Seashore, actively trained

musicians—for the exactitude and “correctness” stipulated by objective, chronographic

78 Quoted in Sears, 33. From M. A. Shaw and F. S Wrinch, “A contribution to the psychology of

time,” in University of Toronto Studies, Psychological Series I (Toronto: University Library,

1899): 101-153.

Page 290: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

269

rhythm—to perform with the metronome, in methods similar to their fatigue and attention

studies. Sears documented Meumann’s seminal chronographic research of the musician:

A second test was made in relation to the observance of tempo. In this a good

piano player was required to beat a rhythm first in accompaniment to a given

standard rhythm [from the chronographic laboratory apparatus] and then alone.

The intervals between the single strokes of the beater were 0.4 and 0.3 seconds.

After the observer [the good pianist] had beaten the rhythm thirty to forty times

the standard [metronomic] rhythm was discontinued, while the subject kept on.

As soon as the standard [metronomic] rhythm ceased the beating of the subject

began to change.79

While skilled musicians of the nineteenth century and earlier perceived and performed

music with vastly differing qualities than found in the scientific metronome, experimental

psychologists, holding to their chronographic values, assumed that musicians should

perform by absolute laboratory standards. Thus, with their misapprehension that

metronomic rhythm was a priori musical time, a host of psychologists authoritatively

judged human creativity solely through the automatical rule of chronography. Some of

the most striking musical-rhythm experiments devised by scientists, influenced by

Vierordt’s, Wundt’s, and then Meumann’s scientific rhythm studies, further highlight this

dramatic and indeed revolutionary paradigm shift in both the qualities of musical time

and the values of individual creativity for modern Western culture.

79 Sears, 29.

Page 291: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

270

Fig. 4.2. The chronographic record from a subject’s laboratory performance of various rhythmic

passages. The upper line is the metronomic temporal standard; the bottom line is the personal interpretation

of the rhythm notated below each record. From Kurt Ebhardt, Zwei Beiträge zur Psychologie des Rhythmus

und des Tempo (1898), 31.

Both in Europe and North America, the years 1894 and 1895 represent a

watershed for the publication of chronographic research on musicians and musical

rhythm. A year after Meumann’s research was published, French psychologists Binet and

Coutier, in “Recherches Graphiques sur la Musique,” reported their meticulous

laboratory experiments with pianists, in which the scientists graphically analyzed finger

strength and reaction time in relation to metronomic accuracy. They found that pianists

were not mechanically uniform in all fingers, and the longer subjects performed, their

fatigue—a common measure in other metronomic labor studies—caused further

Page 292: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

271

irregularities in time. In 1902 Sears summarized these findings and noted the extreme,

inhuman precisions Binet and Coutier held their pianist-subjects to perform under:

A renowned musician in playing five successive notes was found to retard the

interval between any two notes 0.01 of a second. In ten successive trials it was

observed that the value of the intervals always increased as they advanced up the

scale, the average increase varying from 0.01 to 0.015 of a second.80

Moreover, Binet and Coutier recognized that a “renowned musician’s” feeling of the

musical phrase and invisible accent of the metric pulse equated to chronographic

imprecision. Again in this particular study, standard performance practices became

disassociated from rhetorical expression, physical movement, or the pulse of living

musical time. When misinterpreted through the chronographic bias of scientists, the

skilled performer’s rhythmical interpretations were but errors as defined by clockwork:

In relation to the accentuation of single notes these investigations found that a

tendency exists—“1. To separate the accented note from the preceding note, 2. To

tie or slur the accented note to the following note, 3. To increase the length of the

note accented as if this increase were equivalent to an increase in intensity, 4. To

increase, especially in rapid playing the intensity of the notes which follow the

note accented.”81

Writings from the decade assert that psychologists strove their musician-subjects to be

exclusively metronomic. Ebhardt also redefined what were standard and desirable

performance techniques for skilled musicians, attesting that the “rhythmic accent exerts a

disturbing influence on the keeping of time, that it is a source of constant error, and that

in the great majority of cases the accent causes not a shortening of the preceding but a

lengthening of the subsequent member.”82

Scientists, maintaining their chronographic

80 Sears, 30-1.

81

Ibid., 31.

82

Ibid.

Page 293: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

272

bias, heard musicians’ physical sense of metric pulse, the traditional ictus metricus, to be

a mechanically imprecise act, and thus an ultimately flawed performance practice.

Scripture’s Principles

Strikingly similar to the studies by Binet and Coutier in France and Ebhart and Meumann

in Germany, E. W. Scripture’s rhythm experiments in America concerned the need for a

human’s efficient and regular response time, guided by the researcher’s coolly objective,

chronographic bias. In his 1895 texts Thinking Feeling, Doing and the article “Some

Principles of Mental Education,” derived from a lecture presented in 1894,83

Scripture

unreservedly believed that music directing ought to conform to chronographic

procedures, as he explains:

A convenient example is given by measurements on the time of rhythmic action. I

will take the case of the leader of an orchestra who must beat time with his baton.

It does not make much difference just how fast he beats, provided he beats

regularly.84

For Scripture, the conducting of musical time had little to do with the variable qualities of

pulse—or the ever-shifting emotional, structural, and rhetorical content of the specific

music conducted—but lay solely with the mechanical exactitude of the performance act.

Scripture added in his 1907 text The New Psychology, “every musician likewise must

learn to be regular in his [time] intervals, though he may to a certain extent choose their

average length.”85

Scripture, conforming to Wundt’s values, idealized the musician as a

mathematician of metronomic sound in time. In Thinking, Feeling, Doing, Scripture

83 Scripture, “Some Principles of Mental Education,” 533.

84

Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing, 263.

85

E. W. Scripture, The New Psychology (London: The Walter Scott Publishing Co., 1907), 181.

Page 294: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

273

promoted outright his chronographic bias towards human performance by answering his

own scientific problem:

What is rhythmic action? The process in the mind of the one who is acting is in

the first place an estimate of equal intervals of time; after a few strokes at equal

intervals the person knows just when to expect the next one. In other words, it is a

case of time-memory corrected by an actual stroke each time. Knowing when to

expect the next stroke, an act of will is executed so that the final action occurs in

some definite relation to the stroke, generally at the same moment of just after it.86

Given this self-perceived scientific truth, Scripture measured the quality of musical time,

first and foremost, through metronomic rhythm, since any given performance conforms

“always within a small range [of error] before and after the click, so that it [the human

action], on an average, hits the click the record is both accurate and regular. This is the

ideal of rhythmic action.”87

Scripture conceived human movement through chronographic

observation, where a subject must “correct for” physical error to achieve a mechanically

efficient “regularity.” In his 1899 Science article “Observations of Rhythmic Action,”

Scripture again verified his mechanical epistemology of rhythmic performance, stating,

“The irregularity of an act is a good expression of its difficulty.”88

Indeed, Scripture

attested to the industrial-scientific purposes of rhythmic-action studies in The New

Psychology (1907), reiterating, “natural rhythm is the one in which [each person] can

perform the greatest number of movements with the least fatigue.”89

86 Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing, 263.

87

Ibid., 257-8.

88

E. W. Scripture, “Observations of Rhythmic Action,” Science 10 (Dec. 1, 1899): 809.

89

Scripture, The New Psychology, 181.

Page 295: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

274

Fig. 4.3. Scripture’s chronographic testing of a gymnast-subject in his Yale laboratory. The study was

intended to record and regulate the personal equation of efficient rhythmic action. From E. W. Scripture,

Thinking, Feeling, Doing (1895), 262.

It is clear that Scripture’s “rhythm” originated in the labor-fatigue and attention-

fatigue experiments from Wundt’s psychology laboratory. Guided by the notion that any

divergence from automatical rhythm equated to physical problems and inefficiency,

Scripture chronographically tested gymnasts’ “accuracy and regularity” during

“rhythmical exercises with dumb bells.”90

In an identical fashion, Scripture found it

logical to connect the conductor’s baton, and by extension the conductor, to the ever-

trustworthy chronograph. He documents:

90 Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing, 262.

Page 296: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

275

To measure the regularity in a case of this kind I have arranged an electric contact

on the end of a baton, by which a spark record is made on a vibrating time-line

drawn on the smoked paper of a revolving drum. I will not go into the details of

the arrangement; it is sufficient to say that each beat leaves a record and that the

time between each record can be measured in hundredths or thousands of a sec. as

desired.91

Fig. 4.4. Scripture’s chronographic testing of a conductor-subject in his Yale laboratory. The

purposes of the study were identical to his physical-labor experiments intended to record and regulate the

personal equation for efficient rhythmic action. From E. W. Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing (1895),

260.

With precisions far greater than any Maelzel metronome could reference or regulate,

Scripture tested musicians’ performance under ever-greater measures of inhuman

rhythm—finding ever-greater “problems” of human “irregularity.” He intended these

91 Ibid. Scripture’s chronographic tests on conductors, it seems, were not isolated to his Yale

laboratory. T. O’Conor Sloane, Standard Electrical Dictionary, Second Edition (New York:

Normal W. Henley & Co., 1897), 586. The text describes Scripture’s original apparatus, and its

overriding scientific purpose:

“Baton, Electric. An orchestra conductor’s baton arranged so as to close an electric

circuit at each stroke of beating time. It is used in physiological experiments in

connection with a chronograph to determine accuracy of rhythmic action.”

Page 297: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

276

experiments, as with all chronographic procedures, to record and eventual sublimate the

personal equation, those ever-present problems of human variation, within his reductive

and replicable scientific framework. Indeed, Scripture even invented a mathematical

expression for the conductor’s personal equation, his mean variation (MV) of beating

time under chronographic testing.92

Scripture explains the value of chronographically

recording the mean variation of individual conductors:

The actual average time of a beat makes no difference within such small limits, as

music played at the rate of 1 beat in 0.40 sec is not sensibly different from that

played at 1 beat in 0.39 sec. An essential qualification, however, for the success of

an orchestra leader is his regularity in estimating intervals of time, and this can be

determined by getting his mean variation.93

Under Wundt’s methodology, Scripture and other experimental psychologists—

seemingly uneducated in the fundamentals of musical creation or performance—

redefined musicians’ actions with values and precisions that only their laboratory

machines could provide, within temporal limits only they valued. (In Scripture’s case,

accuracy to within a hundredth of a mechanical second was a sufficient and acceptable

precision for human music performances.) Using more precise apparatuses, they found

even more imprecision and imperfection with living actualizations of musical-mechanical

rhythm. With the mean variation of his conductors computed, Scripture formulated who

was the most “regular” human time-beater, and asked, “which is the better man?”94

Scripture clearly considered the “better man” to be the most automatically precise one.

92 Scripture, “Some Principles of Mental Education,” 537.

93

Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing, 261. Italics original.

94 Scripture, “Some Principles of Mental Education,” 536.

Page 298: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

277

Not only for conductors, children, and weight lifters, Scripture’s chronographic

methods, along with his scientific conceptions of musical time, applied to

instrumentalists as well:

Another example similar to the one just mentioned is that of a piano player who

must learn to strike the notes at regular intervals. The quarter notes should all be

about the same length; equal measures should be completed in equal times.95

Scripture’s interpretation of musical time—that all quarter notes were a priori

automatically equal—stemmed from his deeply ingrained chronographic bias, not his

comprehensive musical training. Scripture further explained that any divergence from

chronographic regularity represented a significant problem in musical performance—

regardless of meter, verbal indications, articulation, harmony, form, or melodic gesture,

(with continual variations in each quality) musical time at both the measure and quarter-

note levels ought to display constant metronomic rhythm. He went on to “suggest,

however, that even when the MV is so small that no one distinctly notices the irregularity,

yet the actual irregularity present may be great enough to injure the effect” of the

chronographically perfect musical performance. Chronographic precision in human

action and perception—both in his laboratory and on the concert stage—was of

paramount importance to the scientist, as Scripture subsumed individual musicality to the

strict laws of experimental psychology, stipulating, “A successful musician of any kind

should know not only that his instrument is in tune but also that he himself is in time.”96

Ibid., 537.

96 Scripture, Thinking Feeling Doing, 262. The sentence is misquoted in “Some Principles of

Mental Education,” 537, which reverses the words “tune” and “time,” thereby making the

analogy incomprehensible. Scripture goes on to calculate the regularity of ironworkers in a

similar chronographic fashion, verifying the industrial impetus for such scientific research in

efficient action in time.

Page 299: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

278

Even the most rhythmically regimented of traditional instrumental

performances—the military drummer’s cadence—was not metronomic enough for

Scripture’s tests when gauging the precision of a sergeant’s “time-marking.” Scripture’s

disdain for the chronographic errors derived from the individual musician’s personal

equation—a scientific attitude first expressed by Wundt—could not be more pronounced

when he instructs:

But before we begin an experiment we must distrust everything and everybody—

even the drummer. The drummer himself may have something the matter with

him—we will attend to that later—but at any rate we must use some arrangement

for drumming which we have proved to be exact. 97

Instead of employing a trained and skilled drummer as the reference for the sergeant’s

performance of marking time, Scripture found an automatical source of rhythm vastly

more appropriate than a living one:

The drumming we shall use will be a series of clicks at exactly equal intervals. To

produce the click we use the graphic chronometer. This is essentially a stop-watch

which makes the fine pointer beat either in seconds or in fifths of a second.98

If military drummers failed to be chronographically accurate enough for his laboratory,

Scripture’s mechanical study of pianists uncovered even more egregious human errors.99

Scripture expressed great concerned with “the problem of the two simultaneous acts”

witnessed in two-handed piano playing—a clear indicator of the musician’s flawed

97 Ibid., 255-6.

98

Ibid., 256.

99

Scripture explained his chronographic tests of drumming as follows: “Suppose we put into his

hand an electric drum-stick. Every time that the stick strikes the drum a spark is made. Since the

drummer has no watch to guide him but judges his time as he pleases, we do not use any sounder

but let him beat alone. A record can be made just as before with the chronometer, and the

regularity can be measured in tenths of a second.” Ibid., 259.

Scripture provides further graphical representations of subjects’ time-marking

performances, along with their various rhythmic irregularities when compared to his unwavering

metronomic standard. See 256-8.

Page 300: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

279

personal equation. Under the belief that all human action must display metronomic

regularity and efficiency, Scripture turned a musical keyboard into an electrified

chronograph. In Thinking Feeling Doing, he provided a photograph of his chronographic-

piano tests and described the method used to solve his entirely self-defined problem of

musical simultaneity:

We wish, now, to find out if, when we will to move the two corresponding fingers

of the two hands at the same moment, they really do move as intended or if one is

behind the other. To do this we must have two keys, two spark-coils, and two

metal points, one each side of the [chronographic] time-line…When the fingers

move, two sparks fly through the paper and two white dots are made. Do they

occur at the same moment?100

Fig. 4.5. In Scripture’s Yale laboratory, chronographically “Measuring the Simultaneity in Actions of a

Piano-player” using an electrical-contact keyboard connected to the standard recording barrel-drum. From

E. W. Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing (1895), 31.

Scripture’s answer to his own question was a resounding “no”—pianists did not use their

two hands like two synchronized laboratory metronomes:

100 Ibid., 31-32.

Page 301: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

280

Thus the will to move both hands at the same time results in moving the two at

different times. A careful investigation shows that sometimes the right precedes,

sometimes the left, in irregular order…The difference may seem small. But, for

example, the ear is very sharp and there are people in the world who, intending to

strike the keys of a piano simultaneously, generally hit one slightly behind the

other with a difference sufficient to be heard.101

Under the chronographic bias, Scripture assumed much about the skilled piano player,

and again, redefined the values of musical time within his own laboratory training and

tradition. His ear seems attuned, not to the expressive and non-notated techniques used by

the most experienced piano players, which included hand separation, chord arpeggiation,

and finger pedaling—all of which are intrinsically non-metronomic musical gestures—

but to the time and rhythm most customarily heard in barrel organs and other musical

automata.

Fig. 4.6. Scripture’s chronographic record of a pianist’s performance of a mere two notes (represented by

the two dots). The read-out shows the performer’s mechanical imprecision and lack of precise metronomic

simultaneity in two-handed playing by 0.005 sec. as referenced by the laboratory apparatus. From E. W.

Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing (1895), 32.

Precision, regularity, and simultaneity were the new values stipulated by

Scripture’s and other psychologists’ chronographic experiments in music making. All

divergence from the mechanical time apparatus constituted human error. He printed an

example of his chronographic readout, showing two notes performed by the pianist that

101 Ibid., 32.

Page 302: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

281

diverged from mechanical simultaneity by .005 seconds. Indeed, Scripture considered a

pianist’s non-synchronous practice of hand separation such a problem that he expressed

these “errors” through a three-bar passage of printed music; ironically, he exemplified the

personal equation of performance through a more “realistic” music transcription that

directly corresponded to the expressive beliefs of nineteenth-century musicians, such as

Christiani, who advocated for the positive qualities of non-metronomic musicality.

Fig. 4.7. Scripture’s interpretation of an anti-scientific, non-metronomic performance practice. Justified by

the chronographic method, Scripture transcribes a hypothetical performer’s lack of mechanical

simultaneity, exposing “irregularity” in rhythmic action during a typical music performance. From E. W.

Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing (1895), 32.

In his brief musical except, Scripture essentially verified what many skilled

musicians of the nineteenth century had been claiming all along—music notation

insufficiently signified the more complex musical practices and more nuanced rhythms

that can only be gleaned through living performances. But where skilled musicians

claimed individual expressivity and artistic meaning in living performances, Scripture

heard irregularity, imprecision, and mechanical error.

Since these rhythmic complexities were incommensurate with the objective

chronographic method, Scripture suggested that the pianist’s problems of metronomic

imprecision represented a clear “danger of over-looking small differences” which must

Page 303: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

282

be dealt with by the music teacher, who—without the benefit of scientific machines—

may be unequipped to train-out such egregious imprecision:

The error of simultaneity in piano-playing might readily be great enough to

produce a disagreeable impression on a large part of the audience and yet be so

small as to have escaped the teacher’s correction.102

Scripture assumed that music instructors—and more importantly “passive auditors”—

must hold the same chronographic biases as experimental psychologists. To facilitate

such musical-temporal perfection, Scripture’s paradigmatic pedagogue should, in a

perfect world, also have access to a chronographic apparatus:

Although such means of testing simultaneity would be desirable for every piano-

player, it is of course, impracticable to provide smoked drums, spark-coils, etc.

for general use. We must wait till some ingenious mechanic invents a hand

arrangement to place directly on the piano keys.103

Most significantly, while the perfect chronographic piano had yet to be installed and

wired into every practice room, Scripture well recognized the apparatus currently

transforming average students into his brand of “successful player”—the performer with

the lowest levels of mean variation in musical time, the most simultaneous hands, and the

most automatical rhythm. It was the machine indispensable to experimental psychologists

and their own laboratories:

By practice with the metronome successful players are able to reduce their MV till

it does not disturb the playing. It is not known just how far this may be carried, as

no one has ever taken the trouble to make accurate measurements.104

Ibid., 33.

103 Ibid., 33.

104

Scripture, “Some Principles of Mental Education,” 537.

Page 304: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

283

As Scripture relates, while the pervasive problems of chronographic imprecision

continued to plague musical performances, for now at least, Maelzel’s metronome would

succeed in solving many of the errors intrinsic to the player’s personal equation.

Bolton’s Rhythm

In 1894, the very same year Meumann’s influential thesis on rhythm was published in

Leipzig,105

and one year before Binet, Courtier, and Scripture’s publications on musical

chronography, Thaddeus L. Bolton’s American Journal of Psychology article “Rhythm”

offered the most extensive scientific discussion to date on the phenomena underlying the

rhythms of speech, music, and the body. Bolton, a Fellow at Clark University and student

of G. Stanley Hall—the founder of the first experimental-psychology laboratory in North

America106

—sought to “reduce rhythm to a more fundamental activity of mind.”107

In

keeping with Wundt’s methodologies, Bolton’s experimental process seemed to entirely

devalue the fundamental, variable rhythmic activities of the body.

Deeply familiar with physiologists’ nerve and circulation research,108

along with

psychologists’ research on fatigue and attention,109

Bolton offered the most truncated and

mythological history of music, dance, and poetic rhythm, culminating in the invention of

105 See E. Meumann, “Untersuchungen zur psychologie und aesthetik des rhythmus,” (Ph.D. diss.,

University of Leipzig, 1894).

106

Scripture, The New Psychology, vii.

107

Bolton, 146. Bolton (on page 149) considers the rhythms of walking and speech to be

voluntary, and thus “the most important and are the true types of rhythmical activity.” Pulse and

respiration are “involuntary,” and thus not bonafide rhythmic sources. Jaques-Dalcroze repeated

this concept, nearly verbatim, in Rhythm, Music, and Education, 171. Reprint from “Music, Joy,

and the School,” (1915).

108

Ibid., 150-2.

109

Ibid., 151, 155.

Page 305: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

284

“melody, though it was not disassociated from the meaning of the words. With the

discovery of the musical instrument came the discovery that a melody might be sustained

by simple tone intensities.” Skipping the history of percussion instruments entirely,

Bolton found that “although music finds its essential basis in rhythm, its distinctive

feature is the melody combined with harmony.” Concerning the main purpose of his

study, Bolton recognized, “the most important and fundamental unifying principles

underlying music is the time, without which there can be no music.” It quickly emerges,

however, that despite his broad yet specious history of rhythm, Bolton interpreted

musical time exclusively through his very modern laboratory chronograph:

Musical tones must be exactly timed, if one is to get the conception of a melody

from a series of tones. When they are exactly timed they may be farther unified by

regular changes of intensity [volume] which group the sounds into measure. The

most common measures that occur in music are 2-4, 3-4, 4-4, and 6-8 time. In

what might be termed the natural system of accents, the first note in each measure

receives a strong accent. This is really the only accent in 2-4 time. In 3-4 time the

second note also receives an accent, but it is weaker than the first. In 4-4 time

there are four grades of intensity. The first note is the strongest, the third next, the

second is weaker still, and the fourth is the weakest of all. In 6-8 time the third,

fifth and sixth are of about equal intensity, and weak. The first is strongest, the

fourth is next, and the second weaker though stronger than the third. An equal

amount of time is given to each measure—that is, the strong accent occurs at

regular intervals—but the distribution of this among the notes in a measure may

be greatly varied; the separate notes, however, always bearing constant and

simple relations to one another. The smallest fraction that may express the

relations of these notes is 1-64, and this appears only in instrumental music.110

Clearly influenced by Wundt’s initial mistranslation of musical meter from attention

experiments, Bolton defined a natural musical accent as a performer’s objective

manipulation of volume intensity along with his maintenance of mathematical-

metronomic precision at each measure. Indeed, every note in any given meter, according

to Bolton, was objectively distinguished only in degrees of volume, nothing else—a

110 Ibid., 166-7.

Page 306: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

285

highly automatical interpretation of musical performance, one directly opposed to

Christiani’s, Weber’s, and Marx’s multifaceted understanding of musical accent and

pulse. Bolton still acknowledges, at least, Christiani’s “grammatical accent,” otherwise

known as the “invisible accent” or ictus metricus: the non-notated emphasis of musical

meter considered by previous skilled performers and pedagogues to be common-practice

musicality. Yet Bolton, as with other psychologists, thought metronomic rhythm was the

a priori standard of musical time; there could be no other rhythmical epistemology when

defined by the chronographic method. Stating his chronographic bias outright, Bolton

proclaims these new values for performance culture: “Perfect time is the result of the

application of scientific methods to music.”111

For these experimental scientists with their

laboratory procedures, perfect metronomic time was the only time that mattered, or

indeed existed.

For his experiments, Bolton believed that the rhythmic stimulus should be

unrelated to any sound heard in the outside world; the “rhythmic” source must sound as

redundant and mechanically regular as possible, with no changes in pitch, duration, or

volume. Bolton explains, “the importance of an absolutely uniform series of sounds

cannot be too strongly insisted upon. A difference in sounds which would ordinarily

remain unnoticed [outside of the laboratory] is sufficient to suggest a rhythm…In the

present experiment the greatest precaution was used against any variation in the sounds

that would suggest or impose a grouping.”112

His perfect rhythmic apparatus was as

sonically sterile and devoid of human or natural sound as possible. Under these

111 Ibid., 169.

112

Ibid., 206.

Page 307: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

286

requirements, Bolton found that even Wundt marred the first rhythm-perception

experiments because the clockwork metronome implied too much of a living rhythm,

even when used as a constant artificial regulator. By 1894, Bolton had considered

Maelzel’s metronome (with Wundt’s electro-magnetic modifications) an imperfect,

unscientific machine:

The difficulty was with Wundt’s apparatus. The two sounds heard during a

complete swing of the pendulum of the metronome are not of the same intensity

or quality, and hence the impossibility of [the subject] restraining the [mental]

grouping by two113

…The Sound of the metronome which Dietze employed is full

and rich and has great carrying power [within the subject’s attention span.] Any

experiments upon the carrying power of the mind must take into consideration the

character of the sound.114

Experimental psychologists, beholden to new technologies for more precise and

reproducible measurements, continued to reference musical rhythm through new

machines emitting new artificial sounds devoid of any associations to living rhythm.

Thus, when researchers and pedagogues infused “rhythm” with the scientific values of

precision, synchronicity, and redundancy, metronomic machines redefined—and

continued to redefine—traditional human activities with increasingly automatical

qualities. To facilitate perfect “rhythm” in the laboratory, Bolton invented a special

battery-driven chronographic device, far more mechanically precise, constant, and

artificially monotonous in sound than a clockwork metronome. He explains the new

laboratory tool:

113 Ibid., 205-6.

114

Ibid., 207.

Page 308: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

287

The rate was determined by counting the clicks in the telephone by a stop-watch.

Rates between one click in two seconds and ten in one second were possible. As

the rate was a very important factor, it will be given in all cases in the

presentation of results…The telephone was placed in a different room from the

chronograph, where there was as little disturbance from other noises as possible,

especially from any noises that were in the least suggestive [for the subjects] of a

rhythm.115

For Bolton’s experiments, telephony now regulated and referenced once creative

perception because “the click of the telephone is about as simple and instantaneous a

sound as it is possible to produce.”116

“When the experiment first began,” Bolton documents, “the apparatus was set so

that about three or four clicks to the second were heard in the telephone.” Bolton

recorded his subject’s reaction to the sharp automatical sounds, which were wholly

unfamiliar, even in comparison to the metronome. The subjects, significantly, associated

this new electro-telephonic click to more familiar non-musical phenomena, and “the

sounds suggested most generally and immediately the clock. Other suggestions were:

slowly dripping water, galloping horse, pile-driver, etc.”117

Using this artificial sound

source, Bolton tested his subjects’ sense of rhythm and then carefully correlated his

findings to their respective levels of musical ability.

In discussion with his subjects, Bolton found that rhythmic imagery was an

importance reference for some musicians’ sense of pulse and meter. “Subject 1” and

“Subject 6” both with “Some musical talent and training,” and “Subject 12” with

“considerable musical talent and great interest in music” often related the external

115 Ibid., 184.

116

Ibid., 207. In another rhythm experiment, Bolton employed a rotating disk with a tuning fork

to produce an automatical series of pitches. See 228-9.

117

Ibid., 184.

Page 309: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

288

clicking sound to the musical time-reference they seemed most knowledgeable with: the

swing of the simple pendulum, a temporal aid that continued to be used in musical

education during this time.118

More than just mentally grouping the artificial sounds into

order, these musical subjects visualized a finite, silent reference of moving pulse.

Furthermore, Bolton admitted, “Almost every subject either visualized the pendulum or

spoke of the pendulum-swing movement sometime during the experiments…In my own

case and in some others there was a tendency to sway the body with the [imagined]

pendulum…It was quite visible at times.”119

They visualized and moved to a far less

precise, regulatory, or modern rhythmical reference—one Titchener considered less

effective and more “rudimentary” that pure metronomic sound.120

To make sense of the

sonic monotony, Bolton and his subjects called forth a silent image of tempo first

suggested by the scientist Galileo in the late sixteenth century as reflection of living pulse

and movement. Bolton did not stress this rediscovery: that a visual-physical epistemology

of rhythm existed prior to clockwork machines. Rather, he strictly relied on the most

artificial sound apparatus heretofore employed in modern science—a far more

progressive rhythmic machine with far greater consistency than offered by such

antiquated, seemingly anti-scientific perceptions of human pulse.

118 Ibid., 186, 190-1, 214.

119

Ibid., 186 n2. He goes on to contend that his subjects visualized a clock pendulum alone, not a

simple pendulum or a clockwork metronome: “When the rates were slow, the subject visualized

the clock pendulum and made one [telephone] tick come near the completion of each [imagined]

half swing. The clicks were then grouped by twos and were called the clock tick.” Nevertheless,

the visualization of a moving rhythm, distinct from sound clicks, provides insight to a past

epistemology of musical rhythm, as Bolton continues to mention, “by this pendulum-swing

movement groups of two, three, four, six or eight were frequently grouped into 2-groups. The

first group, then, in the 2-group was accented or more emphatic than the other, and a distinct

pause seemed to follow the second group.”

120

See Titchener, An Outline of Psychology, 182-3.

Page 310: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

289

After his meticulous chronographic testing, Bolton averaged the results—the

mathematical de-emphasis of individual variations—down to the thousands of a second.

Then, he verified Wundt’s procedures, scientifically redefining the qualities of rhythm

with ever-increasing precision:

This general principle may be stated: The conception of a rhythm demands a

perfectly regular sequence of impressions within the limits of about 1.0 sec. and

0.1 sec. A member of the [automatical] sequence may contain one or more simple

impressions. If there are a number of impressions, they may stand in any order of

arrangement, or even in a state of confusion, but each member of the sequence

must be exactly the same in the arrangement of its elements.121

Using this absolute definition, Bolton naturally transferred the rhythm of poetry to meet

his laboratory standards, commenting, “The application of this principle to poetry

demands that the accents in a line shall recur at regular intervals; it requires also that the

successive feet in a line shall be of precisely the same character.”122

If the poetic rhythm

became a chronographic value in Bolton’s study, so too did musical time, since the

notated composition represented a precise object to be performed under strict metronomic

consideration:

In a musical rhythm, however, the measures may vary with certain restrictions in

the arrangements of their [notational] elements. But it is just this variation which

constitutes the melody to a certain extent. The rhythm is varied for purposes of

melody, but it is, nevertheless, a disturbance to the [absolute metronomic]

rhythmical flow in so far that it changes the measure. The melody is a new and

higher unifying agency, which corresponds in a way to the use of rhymes in

poetry. The temporal sequence of the accents is always preserved.123

Bolton’s application of the chronographic method offers more striking examples of

scientists’ miscomprehension of traditional musical rhythm and meter, not to mention the

121 Bolton, 237.

122

Ibid., 237.

123

Ibid., 238.

Page 311: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

290

creative intentions of musicians themselves. Perhaps unbeknownst to those working

within Wundt’s methodologies, their experiments did not scientifically or irrefutably

prove how skilled performers or composers experienced musical time. These new

psychologists merely recorded the mental phenomena of “rhythmic grouping” when

subjects were faced with artificial, redundant sounds in confined laboratory settings.

Then these scientists applied those mental phenomena to their self-defined laws of

musical creation and performance. Revising rhythm strictly through objective laboratory

procedures—in opposition a performer’s self-actualization of physical pulse and

movement—Bolton even misconstrued musicians’ loudly expressed disdain of

automatical time, commenting, “although there is a feeling among musicians that rhythm

is distasteful, it is more apparent than real. It is the regular monotonous recurrence of the

same rhythm without sufficient variations that is displeasing and not the [metronomic]

rhythmic flow itself.”124

Bolton failed to realize that more meaningful definitions of

rhythm existed for skilled musicians, who often voiced their distaste for the redundant,

inhuman “rhythmic flow” heard in the clockwork metronome alone.

Sears’ Contribution

As reported in “A Contribution to the Psychology of Rhythm” (1902), the experiments of

Charles H. Sears yet again proved, to the most extensive degree to date, that musicians

did not conform to chronographic precisions. In the process, Sears takes the

chronographic bias towards human musicality to its most precise limits as well. The work

of Meumann, Ebhardt, Binet, and Courtier all greatly influenced Sears, who, at the time

of his publication, was (like Bolton nearly a decade before him) working at Clark

124 Ibid., 167. Emphasis added.

Page 312: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

291

University under the tutelage of Stanley Hall, the dean of American experimental

psychologists and founder of the American Journal of Psychology. Sears’ laboratory

research on “rhythm” again exposes the dilemma of scientific methods and technologies

when applied to fundamentally subjective acts: the more precise the chronographic

experiments, the more readily apparent are the “problems” of the musician’s personal

equation. From the very first paragraph of his American Journal of Psychology report,

Sears states his chronographic bias—prescribing how the absolute mathematical-

mechanical laws of musical performance should be actualized by all “competent

players:”

When the musician begins his studies certain statements are made to him with

regard to the relative values of notes. He is told that a half note should be given

half the time of a whole note, a quarter note half the time of a half note, an eight

note half as much time as a quarter, and so on, and that a dot placed after a note

adds one-half to its length. It is implied that all notes of the same kind should

receive equal amounts of time unless a change in tempo is indicated, that a triplet

should divide into three equal parts the time usually given to two like notes, that,

except for purposes of expression, all measures are of the same length, etc.125

Sears seems to misunderstand the deeper qualities of musical time from the outset of his

research, for with proper musical experience (something Sears may have lacked), the

performing “artist”—in contrast to the novice—did not interpret written notes with

redundant, invariable durations or mathematical-mechanical proportions, since a host of

non-notated, gestural, rhetorical, and expressive techniques informed the time of music.

Skilled musical performance transcended the limited, chronographic interpretation of

musical notation. Nevertheless, as Sears considered musicians to be passive subjects in a

laboratory process of music making, expression and personal interpretation were

exceptions to the rule of perfect performances. Sears, like preceding scientific researchers

125 Sears, 28.

Page 313: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

292

of rhythmic action, realized that to facilitate such mathematical, chronographic

performance practices, musicians required continual mechanical reference and regulation:

Toward fulfilling these requirements he strives with the metronome as an

assistant. How far the trained musician accomplishes what the notes set before

him indicate and what he sets out to do is an interesting question not only to the

psychologist, but also to the musician.126

Sears continued the psychologist’s task of redefining the problems of the modern

musician in time: how to reduce personal musical actions to the precisions of the

metronome, how to make music performance more like a chronographic experiment. In

posing these problems, Sears makes clear that many scientists increasingly believed their

reductive research methods directly transferred to the complex and nuanced performances

of “trained musicians.” Sears’ sheer ignorance of the multi-faceted qualities of living

musicality can be witnessed in his summary of past rhythmic experiments. Indeed, he

even believed that musicians’ emphasis of the metrical bar, the traditional “invisible

accent,” constituted both a performance mistake and a physiological abnormality:

Irregularity of [rhythmical] movements in playing results from peculiarities due

largely to the structure of the hand. Much of this irregularity may be overcome by

practice. Irregularity is the most marked in rapid playing which leads to the

expectation that in the production of a piece of music those measures that contain

many notes will possess greater variations than those containing only two or

three…For simple intervals without accent the greatest exactness of execution is

between 0.4-0.7 seconds. Accent, increasing the irregularity of the time relations

in the group, is the source of constant error. Usually the accented member is

lengthened.127

This new, clinical “regular rhythm” of scientific musicianship—a conceptual paradigm

shift influenced by chronographic research—one devoid of expressivity, physical and

rhetorical gesture, or a sense of pulse, becomes readily applied when Sears references

126 Ibid., 28.

127

Ibid., 33.

Page 314: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

293

“the time intervals in selections played by a music box” prior to his experiments on

musical time; for this experimental psychologist, the tones of an automaton now defined

the performance goals for the living musician-subject. And while Sears witnessed even

the barrel organ to contain mean variations (chronographic imperfection) in the

performance of both measures and notes, the automaton’s interpretation of accented beats

was far less variable or imprecise. Sears summarized that his preliminary music-box

research “does give some indication of the extent of variation in the length of measures

and notes that will ordinarily pass unobserved, and furnishes, as a very ‘mechanical’

means of producing music, an interesting basis of comparison with the performers whose

playing is next to be considered,”128

in a procedure that held human musicians up to the

unfailing standards of automatical, chronographic rhythm.

For these experiments, Sears’ musician-subjects, “all practiced musicians” (of

which his wife was one) played an organ, not a piano, electrically connected to the

chronograph so that changes of “intensity” would not factor into the interpretation of

meter or phrasing, or complicate the chronographic records. Sears chose five hymns to be

performed; yet he stipulated that the subjects play the melody alone for three of them. For

the two hymns in which the performers played all four voices, Sears again recorded only

the melody. He failed to explain why he ignored or discarded the performances of fully

voiced hymns.129

Concerned with mechanical precision in the top voice alone, Sears not

only reduced complex musical rhythm to metronomic rates but also reduced harmonic

music to simple monophony for the sake of scientific experimentation. Perhaps

128 Ibid., 34-5.

129

Ibid., 35-8.

Page 315: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

294

chronographically recording all voices uncovered too many rhythmic complexities or

nuances (such as arpeggiation and hand separation) to be dealt with through his

laboratory method;130

perhaps, too, Sears found it impossible to document music as it was

truly heard given his limited apparatus. Thus, musical time merely reflected what Sears’

technology could afford the scientist; the chronographic record of the soprano voice from

five hymns helped Sears justify the laws of rhythm for every “correct” musical

performance throughout history.

Tables 4.1 (above) and 4.2 (below). Two of Sears’ many reductive charts that quantified the

average chronographic rate of his musician-subjects’ hymn performances, devaluing human

variation and rhythmic nuance. From Charles H. Sears, “A Contribution to the Psychology of

Rhythm,” The American Journal of Psychology 13 (Jan., 1902): 53, 43.

130 Ibid., 53. He admits that, while temporal imprecision abounded in the performance of the lone

soprano voice, “it will be found that irregularities are on the whole somewhat greater when all

parts are played.”

Page 316: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

295

From the outset of his research, Sears recognized the individual problems in his

performer-subjects and their failure to conform to chronographic precisions. Even in a

single voice from these hymns, the subjects’ notes were not mathematically exact when

compared to the machine. Furthermore, Sears admits finding that “the time occupied in

playing the whole hymn or selection is of course a slightly variable one. It not only varies

for different subjects in playing the same selection, but also for the same subject in

playing the same selection at different times.”131

Sears found another troubling

performance practice not accounted for in the hymn notation—musicians would either

“overlap” (finger pedal) or put “intervals” (silent pauses) between notes, both standard

techniques that Sears seemed unprepared for, explaining that “very rarely did one tone

begin at the instant that the preceding one had come to an end, but that one tone usually

began before the preceding one had ceased.”132

In all instances, Sears counted pauses and

finger pedaling (an extension of legato articulation) as human error, and in keeping with

his reductive methods Sears states, “in the final reckoning up of the lengths of the notes it

is necessary to take these overlaps and intervals into account, and this has been done

throughout on the assumption that each note was intended by the player to last until the

following note was struck no longer.”133

Sears interpreted his subjects’ musical intentions

131 Ibid., 38.

132

Ibid.

133

Ibid. With a self-contradictory concern for musical quality, Sears later suggests that these

overlaps are “probably done in the interest of smoothness. It is nearly impossible to make the end

of the one tone and the beginning of the next exactly synchronous; and, if it were, the effect

would very likely be less flowing than with the overlaps. Intervals on the other hand would give a

staccato effect not desirable without special reason. The overlaps are by no means uniform.” See

Sears, 49. In a further reduction of individual musicality, Sears finds the average overlap time,

down to thousandths of a second, during specific passages of each hymn. See also Table XI,

“Showing number and average length of overlaps in fractions of a second,” 49.

Page 317: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

296

based upon his own chronographic bias; he assumed that every note these musicians’

played should mimic the binary “on-off” ticks of a metronome. Sears simply discounted

all perceived rhythmic abnormalities in their performances, explaining, “The overlap has

therefore been subtracted from the full time of the preceding note in every case in which

it appeared and the interval added when it occurred.”134

To prove musicians’ imprecision

in chronographic time, Sears first omitted all complex musical qualities that could not be

explained through his chronographic method, qualities that transcended the “tick-tock” of

his laboratory machine. To justify psychologists’ chronographic method of above all,

Sears conveniently erased all proof of humanity from musical rhythm.

Yet humanity lingered in living music performance, as Sears documented each

player and each note played to within hundredths of a second, finding metronomic

imprecision throughout the selected hymn tunes. Sears discovered what skilled musicians

already realized. He recorded that “the measures vary not only with the general rate but

among themselves for the same subject and the same selection,” that is, the performers’

interpretations were in constant flux when compared to the rigid time of the seconds-

clock. Beginning and concluding measures were so mechanically imprecise (owing to the

individual interpretations of initial pickup and cadencial gestures) that Sears, not

surprisingly, “excluded” them from his already reductive data, which showed only the

average lengths of measures in seconds. Again, he reduced the human realities of

performance to chronographic time and numerical quantification.135

Even with his data

reduced, averaged, and tabulated down to hundredths of a second, Sears discovered no

134 Ibid.

135

Ibid., 41.

Page 318: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

297

absolute rhythmical laws at the bar level, admitting, “There is no clearly evident order in

the variation of the measures except a retardation when nearing the end of the

selection.”136

Sears entertained the possibility, however, that the words of these familiar

hymns affected the movement of musical time—a standard epistemology that any skilled

musician could have explained to him—but he was clearly against the notion, stating,

“The evidence furnished by the tracings for such an accommodation of the music to the

words is too slight to warrant the assertion that it exists, but, nevertheless, such a relation

is not impossible.”137

Sears reports that the temporal irregularities continued at smaller denominations,

and “since the time of the measures is variable, it follows that the time of their

component parts must also vary. Such variations are found not only in the different

measures, but even in the same measure.” This scientist found conclusively that for any

given performer and any given performance (of a single line from five hymns), neither

the measures nor notes were consistent with the chronograph. Nevertheless, Sears

continued to reduce the discovered nuances and variations of musical time, neglecting to

document the temporal variations witnessed in identically notated rhythms. Instead, he

provided a chart, “Showing in hundredths of a second the average length of notes,” from

a whole note to an eighth. By averaging the note values in precise mechanical time, Sears

once again erased the actual, empirically proven variability witnessed in each of his

subjects’ actions.138

136 Ibid., 41-2.

137

Ibid., 43.

138

Ibid., 43. See Sears, Table VI. Emphasis added.

Page 319: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

298

With the reality of individual variation mathematically erased, Sears confidently

judged the absolute laws of musical performance, in which “the quarter note is taken as

the basis of comparison,”139

suggesting that the quarter note always held an objective

metronomic value in every interpretation, regardless of meter, affect, or articulation.

Thus, for certain subjects, he found quarter note realizations “sometimes too long and

sometimes too short. All subjects make the eight notes too short, sometimes very much

too short…[subject] S, also without exception, makes the notes of higher denomination

that the quarter too large.”140

If simple relationships between eight and quarter notes showed great

imperfections, Sears found even more complex and “irregular” performance of triples and

dotted figures, rhythms that never seemed to conform to his chronographic guidelines.

Under the assumption that the quarter-note triplet must fit exactly into the metronomic

time of a half note, in a vocal hymn no less, Sears reported, “if each triplet is compared

with the unaccented note of its own measure the results are conflicting, and the chances

seem about equal that the triple will be too short or too long.”141

Musical time became an

issue of objective chance, not personal volition, as Sears also discovered that not only

were triplets imprecise when compared to the larger beat, but the notes of the triplets

themselves were irregular against the chronograph. “Turing to the individual notes of the

triplet,” Sears reflects, “there is a tendency to give more time to the second note than to

the first, and more to the third than to the second…thus while the tendency is slight to

139 Ibid.

140

Ibid., 44.

141

Ibid.

Page 320: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

299

make the second note longer than the first, it is very strong to make the last longer than

either of the others.”142

More noticeable chronographic imprecision appeared when the musicians

performed the combined dotted eight and sixteenth note rhythm. Sears commented, “All

the subjects without exception made the whole measure very slightly longer than the ones

that precede and follow it, though with such slight differences that the relation may be

only accidental.”143

Since Sears viewed human performance to be imperfect and

accidental against the chronograph, he found, “A comparison of the component parts of

the last half of the measure with the average of the quarter notes in the first half of the

measure shows a great lack of uniformity in the duration of both the dotted eights and

their combination with the sixteenth notes. On the other hand the sixteenth notes with two

exceptions are all of too long duration.”144

The invisible accent of musical meter also posed some rhythmic problems for

Sears, who recognized, “It is evident from the foregoing that accented notes are often

longer than unaccented notes of the same denomination, but it is also evident that this

tendency is not present in all cases and with all players.” This standard practice of

emphasizing the metric pulse seemed less evident in one hymn, where Sears

misapprehended the accent to fall on the downbeat of each bar, when in actuality, the

musical pulse falls on the downbeat of every second full bar, as the phrasing and text

142 Ibid.

143

Ibid.

144

Ibid., 45.

Page 321: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

300

dictates.145

Sears admitted, however, that the specific expressive qualities might influence

this unwieldy, non-notated practice of rhythmic emphasis for any given composition:

One may conjecture that [the metrical accent] will appear most prominently in

pieces, e.g., marches and processionals, which require a strong marking of the

rhythm, and will be more or less completely absent in pieces of a more flowing

character.146

Perhaps without realizing it, Sears “conjectured” upon what skilled musicians had

experienced, practiced, and preached for over two centuries: musical time, as actively

performed by individuals, contained fluctuations within the bar and across the bar, a

variety of non-notated accentuations, along with “overlaps” and “intervals”—qualities

more vocal than mathematical or mechanical—which imbued living music performance

with a certain mouvement, swing, “wavy outline,” or inégalité. Yet for Sears and other

experimental psychologists, the performer’s variable qualities of musical interpretation

solely represented accident, error, or even blind chance against objective certainties of

mechanical time and rhythm.

At the end of his article, Sears printed the hymns from his experiment, only

providing the complete voices for the two hymns with the most homophonic (i.e. visually

synchronized) texture. Numbers underneath each note, representing hundredths of a

second, show the time that each performer spent playing each rhythm—of the soprano

voice alone, minus the many unquantifiable, non-metronomic techniques discussed

above. Seldom did Sears’ numbers correspond to exact rhythmic proportions. Thus,

Sears, being biased towards scientific methods, mathematically calculated and judged his

four subjects’ interpretations of musical rhythm on the reductive basis of “too long” and

145 Ibid., 46, and 58 Figure.

146

Ibid., 46.

Page 322: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

301

“too short” when compared with his infallible laboratory chronograph, which marked the

most inhuman time possible.

Fig. 4.8. A portion of Sears’ chronographic data showing the “errors” of the musical-personal equation for

the four subjects: S, B, M, and W. The numbers below the musical staff document the duration—in

hundredths of a second—that the subjects held each note of the soprano voice alone on the organ apparatus.

From Sears, “A Contribution to the Psychology of Rhythm,” AJP 13 (Jan., 1902): 57.

Musical Time Lost: Revision of Past Epistemologies through

the Chronographic Bias

Given his limited understanding of music performance, combined with a dogmatic belief

in chronography, Sears considered that all musical time throughout history must conform

to metronomic precisions. Misinterpreting an article in Music by noted Wagner scholar

Maurice Kufferath, “Rhythm, Melody, and Harmony” (1900), which describes how pulse

proportions relate to notated meter, Sears attested that the metronome logically replaced

the heartbeat as reference for every composition, justifying through a questionable and

reductionist history:

Page 323: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

302

Bach and his school tried to establish an absolute standard of rate, taking as a

typical measure a group of four quarter notes, one to every pulse beat. To this

measure the time values of all others bore a constant ratio. But such a system as

this, if strictly carried out, would be as apt to increase as to decrease such

irregularities as are here under consideration; and modern musicians having set

aside the normal pulse beat, find their absolute standard in the metronome or in

the number of quarter notes per minute.147

Sears, perhaps correctly, believed that words and pulse no longer held sway in the

understanding of musical time, since this past epistemology was palpably absent in the

procedures of experimental psychologists. But Sears also implied that even Bach, with

only his meager pulse as reference, would be rhythmically “irregular” using such

primitive, non-chronographic techniques. For Sears, the metronome clearly offered the

modern improvement on this old practice—a more exacting tool that simply reduced the

temporal irregularities of all music performance to “the number of quarter notes per

minute,” regardless of musical meter, phrase, harmony, or form. Sears recognized,

however, that Bach lived in a different musical “time” than the modern scientist or music

student.

Sears only roughly paraphrased the more significant statement from Kufferath, a

music pedagogue and theorist who did not fully conform to Sears’ chronographic bias. In

the 1900 Music article “Rhythm, Melody, and Harmony,” Kufferath stated that past

practices of musical time were indeed rapidly waning—to the detriment of music

performance—and that new metronomic methods were lacking by comparison.

According to Kufferath the heartbeat was the sufficient, standardized, and appropriate

reference of musical time for an age long since past, for performance practices long since

forgotten, and “in abandoning as a point of comparison, and as a unit of movement, the

147 Ibid., 40-1.

Page 324: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

303

normal beat of the pulse, we have abandoned a principle absolute, precise and universal,

which the metronome only imperfectly replaces.”148

Kufferath recognized that a value

shift had clearly taken place in culture: the metronome, once merely a supplement to the

general understanding of musical pulse and meter, had become the primary means of

dictating and defining musical rhythm in the modern age. Speaking for traditional

performance practices, Kufferath, perhaps ironically, found the automatical machine to

be “imperfect” against the living, variable heartbeat.

While the metronome may have been an imperfect replacement for those who

understood past performance practices, it seems that the new proponents of metronomic

action, those with a chronographic bias, became practically ignorant of the physical,

internal, and subjective qualities of time once valued by past performance culture.149

By

the turn of the century, scientists increasingly subsumed rhythms of the body to

clockwork until human pulse and movement contained little of the value that Kufferath,

and many other musicians throughout the nineteenth century, described as being essential

to music performance.

Wundt promoted this trend—the devaluation of an internalized sense of time

perception—in the 1890s, when he retracted his published theory that walking furnished

148 M. Kufferath, “Rhythm, Melody, and Harmony,” Music XVII (Nov., 1899-Apr., 1900): 38.

149

Recall that Marin Mersenne, in the early seventeenth century, reported that the heartbeat of a

healthy individual might offer a gauge for the time of day: “Some maintain that one can tell what

time it is by the movement of the pulse. The pulse quickens its movement from midnight on or

from six o'clock in the morning until noon.” See Marin Mersenne, Traite' de l'harmonie

universelle ou est conteu la Musique Theoretique et pratique (Paris, 1627); John Bernard Egan,

“Critical translation of the second book” (PhD diss., Indiana University, 1962), 43-44.

Page 325: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

304

a conceptual basis for rhythm.150

Likewise, Jaques-Dalcroze, in stark contrast to

Kufferath, had found the heartbeat to be “a matter of unconscious activity, independent of

the will, and therefore valueless for the purposes of execution and perception of rhythm”

by 1907.151

Jaques-Dalcroze, who seemed to be highly informed by the rhythmic studies

of experimental psychologists, eventually admitted in his 1915 article “Music, Joy, and

the School” that past epistemologies of rhythm and performance had effectively receded

from public knowledge, stating, “Rhythm is the basis of all vital, scientific, and artistic

phenomena…And yet we have long ceased to scan verses by means of bodily movement,

and rhythm has become a purely intellectual conception.”152

It cannot go unnoticed that

Jaques-Dalcroze considered that one, absolute “Rhythm” grounded both scientific and

artistic pursuits, just as it had in Wundt’s laboratory through the chronographic method.

As analyzed in Chapter VI, Jaques-Dalcroze, in order to solve the modern problems of

rhythm, applied Wundt’s psychological-chronographic values to train students in a new

automatical type of “bodily movement.”

Similarly revoking the physicality of human rhythm in favor of chronography,

Mary Hallock questioned whether musicians (in their capacity as composers primarily)

ever judged musical time in relation to something as ephemeral and imprecise as the

150 See Titchener, Experimental Psychology, 355: “Stumpf (Tonpsychologie, i., 340) remarks that

‘our sense of time and rhythm appears to have developed, for the most part, in walking,’ and

quotes Wundt (Phys. Psych., 2d ed., 1880, ii., 286) to the effect that the time-period which is

most accurately reproducible in idea is practically identical with the time required for a

movement of the leg in rapid walking. Wundt, however, gives up the fact, and the theory based

upon it, in his later editions (cf. ii., 1880, 287 f. ; ii., 1887, 354; ii., 1893, 416), though he still

regards bodily movement as the ultimate source of the rhythmical impression (ii., 1893, 91 ; cf.

84).”

151 Emile Jaques-Dalcroze, “The Initiation into Rhythm,” in Rhythm, Music, and Education,

translated by Harold F. Rubinstein (New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons, 1921), 81.

152

Jaques-Dalcroze, “Music, Joy, and the School,” (1915) in Rhythm, Music, and Education, 171.

Page 326: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

305

human pulse or poetic meter. Her works were cited, reviewed, and republished

throughout the decade in sources as varied as The Medical News (1905)153

and The

Metaphysical Magazine (1907).154

Hallock’s sparse article “Pulse and Rhythm” (1903) in

particular seems to have resonated with the public. In 1903, The American Monthly

Review of Reviews reflected on her strictly scientific, drastically revised interpretation of

musical time:

Are musical composers unconsciously guided by the beat of the pulse? This

question, long a matter of curious speculation, may some day be scientifically

answered by the aid of modern instruments and accumulated data. Many

interesting facts bearing on the problem are presented by Miss Mary Hallock in

the September number of the Popular Science Monthly. Commenting on the fact

that the scientific study of rhythm, so far as man is concerned, has been

approached almost wholly from the side of its conjunction with literature, this

writer says: “Looked at from that side, it is not strange that the testimony could

never be mathematically exact and emphatic.”155

The exacting mathematical-mechanical regulation of human activity stemmed exclusively

from scientists’ chronographic bias and apparatus. As Hallock recognized, this scientific

search for mechanically accurate rhythm—proven in the laboratory through

mathematically averaged and “accumulated data”—fundamentally conflicted with

traditional, experiential concepts of pulse and poetic rhythm. The reviewer continues to

quote Hallock, her exclusively scientific methodology of creativity, and what was

increasingly becoming modern civilization’s myopic view of human action in mechanical

time:

153 J. R. Baird, “Pulse in Verbal Rhythm,” The Medical News, July 29, 1905, 225. The

correspondent commented upon Hallock’s article in Poets Lore.

154

Mary Hallock Greenewalt, “The Correspondence of Pulse and Rhythm,” The Metaphysical

Magazine XXI (May-Dec., 1907): 378.

155

Albert Shaw, ed., “Pulse and Rhythm,” The American Monthly Review of Reviews XXVIII

(July-Dec., 1903): 487.

Page 327: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

306

“The only data which are of sufficient accuracy to prove that the rhythmic

phenomena of pulse first impressed on our consciousness that which can

accurately be called rhythm, are to be found in the metronomic denotations of

musical compositions. It is there, and there only, that the brain has been able

systematically to externalize the rhythm most natural to it with a sense of method

and order approximating instrumental exactitude, and capable of an exact

expression and measure in number. These furnish only a trace, but a trace

sufficient when one keeps in mind the havoc that conscious intellect can always

play with things strictly natural.”156

When those with the chronographic bias interpreted the rhythm of human action, their

assessments often reduced to “too fast or too slow,” “too long or too short” against rigid

mechanical standards. In her article “The Correspondence of Pulse and Rhythm,”

Hallock’s interpretation of rhythm confirmed this modern epistemology, in which metric

pulse and rhetorical gesture ceased to be primary qualities of musical time. The

metronome alone provided the answers to the unwavering tempo of compositions and

performances; the human pulse merely reflected an imperfect chronographic regulator.

While she admitted that musical beats could not often be strictly timed to the actual

second-ticks of a clock,157

Hallock unquestioningly believed that musical time was

strictly bound to the modern, mechanical minute—not physical pulse or movement—and

that metronomic gradations within that objective minute supplied the absolute rule of

rhythm, for hymns and popular tunes composed long before the metronome ever existed:

156 Ibid.

157

“If with the eye fixed on the second-hand of a watch or a clock the long meter doxology be

sung, every one of the equally accented notes entering simultaneously with the tick of each

consecutive second, it will become at once apparent that the melody is delivered at a rhythmic

rate of sixty beats to the minute. Should one in the same breath hum ‘Yankee-doodle,’ sounding

each of its accented notes, at the same rate, it will be found that these two melodies, standing at

the extremes of the sublime and the ridiculous, the one in character slow, the other fast, the first

combining the utmost dignity and breadth, the second ludicrously vapid and thoughtless, are both

set to precisely the same length of rhythmic time by the clock.” See Hallock, “The

Correspondence of Pulse and Rhythm,” 379. Reprint from Popular Science Monthly.

Page 328: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

307

The impression of slowness or rapidity in the music is due rather to the character

of the context and the number of notes to be played in the divisions within the

[mechanical] minute than to the actual clock time [in sixty mechanical seconds] it

takes to perform the rhythmic unit.158

Hallock, it is important to note, was a conservatory-trained pianist, teacher, and by one

Who’s Who account, a highly inventive performer.159

Her writings expose how strongly

scientific values influenced Western education, industry, and performance culture at the

turn of the century. Indeed, the epistemology of Scripture, Bolton, and Sears—originating

in Wundt’s “rhythmical” experiments—were not confined to laboratories or scientific

journals alone. By the twentieth century, their chronographic bias had invaded public

education and consequently modern society, as seen in the works of William Patterson,

Carl Emil Seashore, and most notably Jaques-Dalcroze. The scientific interpretation of

rhythm became an essential standard in the creative arts, promoted in a series of general-

interest texts and periodicals. Meanwhile past practices and epistemologies of time and

rhythm became obscured and devalued in the face of mechanical objectivity.

In another of Hallock’s articles, “Pulse in Verbal Rhythm” (1905), scientific

values clearly dominate her modern music pedagogy—following a cultural-societal trend

further explored in Chapters VI and VII. And similar to scientists such as Sears, Hallock

reconsidered a musician’s variable expression in time—a living practice that she could

not realistically deny—with absolute reference to the performance techniques of musical

automata:

158 Ibid., 379.

159

See John William Leonard, ed., Women’s Who’s Who of America, 1914-1915 (New York: The

American Commonwealth Company, 1914), 343. Sometime between 1912 and 1914, she was

“first to arrange and utilize a color lighting accompaniment shifting with every phase [phrase?] of

music in sympathy with the feeling and mood.”

Page 329: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

308

No demonstration is needed to show that at discretion each unit of time between

these accented syllables may be made faster or slower at will. In music this

shifting of the [metronomic] tempo is the spice of interpretation. One who

inclines his attention to the pianola has told me that change in the tempo lever

alone made expression; whereas change in the intensity lever alone did not.160

At

one moment we may feel like humming a few bars of a Brahms Rhapsody at the

rate of seventy-six beats to the minute, again at eighty and again at eighty-eight in

whatever motion may best fill the mood of the moment.

These time rates are mathematically gaugeable by an instrument in

universal use among musicians called the metronome.161

When once only scientists misapprehended musical time, by the turn of the century, some

trained musicians such as Hallock were making an effort to perform under the strict

guidance of metronomic impressions. Corresponding to the rise of chronographic values

in culture, the Medical News suggested that any bodily reference to musical time

represented an exception—an almost mythical performance practice—to the now

mechanical standard of modern performance, citing that, “Miss Hallock calls attention to

160 The fact cannot go unremarked that pianolas (player-pianos)—in order to sound more

lifelike—were constructed so that the human manipulator could smoothly vary volume and tempo

at any point in the piano-roll performance, accelerating and decelerating the music at his will

without the audible reference or regulation of a metronome.

Despite pianola-performers’ ability to display an artificial sense of tempo rubato and

expressive playing, a unique treatise from 1922 entitled The Art of the Player-Piano offered a

rhythmic-training exercise directly inspired by psychologists’ now-typical attention experiments.

Sydney Grew’s “text-book for student and teacher” instructs non-musical pianola manipulators:

“The clear steady ticking of a clock helps counting. A metronome is better than a

clock, because the speed of its beats can be varied.

A clock seems to tick naturally in duple-time — tick-tack, tick-tack. But by

concentration we can make the clock present its ticks in triple-time, quadruple-time, six-

time, and even in five-time. Our metrical sense develops quickly when we train it so to

accentuate the ticking of the clock. The metronome can, by means of its adjustable bell,

be made to mark off metrical groups of two, three, four, and six.”

See Sydney Grew, The Art of the Player-Piano (London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co.,

1922), 32.

161

Mary Hallock, “Pulse in Verbal Rhythm,” Poet Lore 16 (1905): 80. Regardless of Hallock’s

assertions, music publications seldom, if ever, printed metronome marks for each variable,

interpretative moment described by Hallock’s performance practice.

Page 330: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

309

the fact that from the earliest times there has been an impression abroad in many minds

that rhythm in music was somehow associated with the pulse beat of ordinary human

existence. This idea has been repudiated as visionary.”162

For a modern age in which scientific methods and chronographic biases reigned,

the idea that the pulse could somehow reference musical rhythm seemed entirely

speculative, unfathomable even. In “An Experimental Study of Musical Enjoyment”

(1912), Harry Porter Weld extensively documented his subjects’ physical reactions and

mental visualizations to a variety of music recordings, and certified:

Our negative correlation between pulse and tempo is in opposition with the views

of Riemann and of Steinitzer, both of whom assumed that the normal pulse is the

criterion by reference to which we estimate rapidity or slowness of tempo. Indeed

Hallock has carried this so far as to compute [through printed metronome

markings] the normal heart-rate of Beethoven from an analysis of his sonatas. But

we have wholly failed to find any correlation between rhythm or tempo and pulse.

In musical productions the length of the rhythmic group varies in consequence of

acceleration, retardation, rubato, pause, forced accenting of tones: the pulse does

not respond to these variations.163

In documenting the reactions of passive auditors, Weld failed to recognize not only the

differing concepts and qualities between musical tempo and rhythm, but that the “forced

accenting of tones” represented the active performer’s continual, albeit variable sense of

pulse in metered music. He (and not Riemann) mistakenly assumed that the musician’s

162 “Special Article. Byways of Medical Literature.—XVII. Pulse Beats and Musical Rhythm,”

Medical News, Sept. 26, 1903, 622. APS Online.

163

Harry Porter Weld, “An Experimental Study of Musical Enjoyment,” The American Journal of

Psychology 23 (Apr., 1912): 253. Weld did recognize, however, that the musical sense could not

be captured through mere clockwork quantification, that more complex processes of sensory

perception and physical actualization were involved which could not be defined through

traditional rhythmic-attention experiments. Weld states: “Our introspections reveal the fact that

the ‘music consciousness’ is made up of a mass of kinaesthetic and organic sensations, motor,

vocal-motor, auditory and visual imagery, numerous associations many of which are irrelevant,

affective and emotional processes, and various phenomena which have to do with intellectual

enjoyment.”

Page 331: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

310

reference to the beating pulse should equate to a precise chronographic regulator, applied

over the course of an entire performance. Not surprisingly, Weld’s modern psychological

laboratory contained the industry-standard recording equipment: “the kymograph was so

adjusted that the drum made one complete revolution in five minutes. A time line,

marking seconds, was recorded upon the drum by means of a fourth tambour which was

actuated by a metronome; the metronome stood in an adjoining room, and every care was

taken to make our apparatus as nearly noiseless as possible.”164

Even though Weld

faithfully maintained a chronographic bias, in which the subject’s heart- or breath-rate

contained no inherent referential value during the passive exposure to metronomic sound,

he nevertheless approached one inescapable conclusion of living performance:

The human body is not equipped with a physiological metronome, by means of

which tempos are estimated in any constant fashion. The only criterion of tempo

which we employ is an indefinite 'sense of fitness' whose estimate may vary from

time to time without any concomitant variation of heart-rate, of respiration or of

other physiological process. And we estimate tempo in terms of our momentary

ability to make that motor response which seems to be most fitting for the

particular composition which constitutes our stimulus.165

With the rise of scientific values based in mechanical objectivity, historical

epistemologies of time and rhythm seemed to wane from the social consciousness; as the

above reports suggest, prior conceptions of time and action receded in favor of the

“progress” that both technology and the chronographic method offered industrialized

164 Ibid., 248.

165

Ibid., 268. He also acknowledges that pre-chronographic epistemologies of musical rhythm

indeed existed: “Musicians have themselves recognized the intimate relation between movement

and music. They have stated that their art is concerned with rest and motion,—or, as Riemann

(28; I) puts it, with rest, motion and rest. Kostlin, quoted by Gross (8; 2 If.), states that music

glides, turns, hops, leaps, jumps, dances, sways, quivers, blusters, and storms, and adds that the

auditor who adequately reproduced its movements in the physical world must become

imponderable or he would be dashed to pieces.” See Weld, 270.

Page 332: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

311

civilization. First in laboratory, and then throughout Western society, the subjective pulse

of rhythm ceased beating; it was replaced by the standardized and lifeless metronomic

click.

After experimental psychologists redefined rhythm as an absolute law of attention

and action to passive metronomic exposure, chronographic actualization became their

prescription for modern society at large. No longer for laboratory experiments on mental

perception and physical response, performances in scientific “rhythm” became a new

goal of education and aesthetics in the twentieth century. Patterson’s text The Rhythm of

Prose, while offering perhaps the most lucid and extensive survey of the many

contemporaneous (and often conflicting) meanings of rhythm, clearly exposes that

scientific values of time and action directly transferred into the realm of twentieth-

century cultural thought. Indeed, Wundt’s laboratory experiments defined Patterson’s

artistic “rhythm,” in what this scientific pedagogue considered “The New Standard”166

ringing with metronomic regularity. Patterson ascertained, “The ultimate basis of all

rhythmic experience, however, is the same. To be clear-cut, it must rest upon a series of

definite temporal units.”167

As he and other new pedagogues attested, the twentieth-

century “rhythm” of both music and poetry was automatical above all. “When a melody

is played in strict, unvarying metronome time,” Patterson explains, “swing is at its

lowest, and the ‘psychological moment’ for an accent is merely a matter of remembering

166 Patterson, 1.

167

Ibid., xxii.

Page 333: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

312

that two and two make four. What is usually meant by swing is really ‘elastic’ swing,

where the simple mathematical relations are complicated for purposes of expression.”168

As performances became viewed through the exactitudes of the chronograph

method, individual expression was now an objective calculation to perform outside the

regulatory rhythm of the metronome. Affirming psychologists’ reinvention of once

physical-musical rhythm, Patterson too devalued subjective interpretation and experience

in performance arts, favoring instead objective, regulated action, in which any audible

sense of pulse and movement—what Patterson alluded to as a non-notated sense of

“swing”169

—became re-envisioned as uncommon, extreme expressions of “rubato”

against chronographic rule:

Compensation figures conspicuously. Time stolen in one place, is repaid in

another. What Riemann calls ‘agogic accent’ (the deliberate addition of length to

a note, instead of stress, in order to give it prominence) and, of course, tempo

rubato (stolen time), belong to this category; so, though it does not seem to be

generally remembered, all effects due to accelerating and retarding the standard

tempo.170

Informed and influenced by experimental psychology, Patterson too considered the

performances of both poetry and music to be scientific acts, in which the passive

response to external mechanical impressions replaced the active, willful interpretation of

artistic rhythm. He continued to promote the modern values of objective time and

efficient action for the creative arts:

168 Ibid., 51.

169

Patterson continued to look for all answers to rhythm and performance in scientific research as

he admitted: “Wundt himself has failed to clarify the final problem of swing, but his insistence on

the importance of the idea of velocity (how fast something is going) is the best beginning we

could have. Probably those persons who are deficient in motor types of mental imagery will never

find a satisfying solution.” See Patterson, 51. Emphasis added.

170

Ibid., 51.

Page 334: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

313

To test prose-rhythm, therefore, we do not merely mark the accents with the

ancient classic symbols, and admire, with an optical delight, the “paeonic” or

“dochmaic” pattern presented [in notation]. If rhythm means anything to the

average individual, it means motor response and a sense of organized time. This is

what it means in the playing of flutes, the beating of drums, the singing of songs,

and in dancing.171

Patterson’s monograph, a study of musical as much as spoken time, championed this

“new standard” of time and action informed by chronography, which “is thus established

for passing judgment upon the rhythm of a sentence or paragraph…The new standard has

for its support not so much its apparent novelty, as its subservience to the psychological

facts of individual difference.”172

In Patterson’s claim that rhythm meant “motor

response” to society at-large, the extent to which the values of time had changed in the

cultural consciousness again becomes apparent. In the modern, mechanical age, “mere”

rhythmic notation—which must be subjectively perceived and actualized by the reader—

seemed insufficient and vague, requiring guidance from chronography. In the new

scientific age of the twentieth century, as Patterson implied, the individual’s

psychological response to organized mechanical time was the new, absolute meaning of

rhythm.

Seashore’s Musical Measures

In twentieth-century performance culture, the chronographic bias found one of its greatest

champions in Carl Emil Seashore (1866-1949), psychology professor at the University of

Iowa from 1902 until his death. Seashore’s writings confirm the complete transformation

of musical time to the chronographic paradigm of rhythm and action. Seashore—

171 Ibid., 14.

172

Ibid., 13-14.

Page 335: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

314

experienced in administering both child-study tests173

and attention studies174

in the

Wundt tradition—directly promoted the metronomic rhythm of the psychological

laboratory to music educators, scholars, and performers spanning over four decades in

notable journals such as The Musical Quarterly and later the Music Educators Journal,

where his 27-part series on music psychology ran from 1936175

through 1940.176

By Seashore’s account, he and his students had been researching the phenomena

of musical talent in the University of Iowa laboratory as early as 1901,177

and by 1911,

Seashore publicly spoke of an individual singer’s ability for rhythm as a psychological

experiment.178

He reiterated these chronographic values in his early article, “The Measure

of a Singer” (1912), published in the periodical Science:

173 G. Stanley Hall, Adolescence: Its Psychology and its Relations to Physiology, Anthropology,

Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion, and Education, Vol. 1 (New York: D. Appleton and Co., 1904),

153. According to Hall, both Seashore and Meumann were educated in Vierordt’s past

metronomic training methods. By 1899, Seashore had “found that children were able to reproduce

small time intervals of about five seconds pretty accurately at all ages from six to fifteen.

Intervals of ten seconds were shortened to 8.2, those of twenty to 14.4 and those of thirty to 13.5.

From eight or nine years on the improvement in reproducing longer intervals rapidly increased.”

174

Carl E. Seashore, Elementary Experiments in Psychology (New York: Henry Holt and

Company, 1908), 168.

175

See his earliest articles for the periodical, Carl E. Seashore, “Psychology of Music,” Music

Educators Journal 22 (Mar., 1936): 24-25; and “The Psychology of Music. II. Approaches to the

Experimental Psychology of Music,” Music Educators Journal 22 (May, 1936): 22-23.

176 See his last article, Carl E. Seashore, “Psychology of Music. XXVII. How Do We Express

Specific Emotions in Song?,” Music Educators Journal 27 (Sept., 1940): 38-39+42.

177 Carl E. Seashore, “Scientific Procedure in the Discovery of Musical Talent in the Public

Schools,” Music Supervisor’s Journal 2 (Jan., 1916): 10.

178

He addressed the American Psychological Association in December 1911. See Carl E.

Seashore, “The Measure of a Singer,” Science 35 (1912): 201.

Page 336: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

315

While the perception of time is largely associational, we depend ultimately upon

the sensory capacity for the perception of short durations and rhythmic effects.

Rhythm may be expressed through both time and intensity. For a single measure

of the perception of rhythm as a time element, we may eliminate intensity and

measure the least perceptible deviation in the duration of the recurrent sound,

uniform in all respects except duration. This, taken with the measure of intensity-

discrimination, should correlate well with the measure of perception of intensity

in rhythm-accent.179

By 1915, Seashore had brought this epistemology to music educators and performers

through the new periodical The Musical Quarterly. In the first edition of the journal,

Seashore’s article “The Measurement of Musical Talent” reiterates verbatim much of his

previous Science piece, while adding specific documentation on his research methods.180

Over his career, Seashore prescribed ever-greater rhythmical precisions for his musically

inclined readership. In his 1918 Musical Quarterly article “The Sense of Rhythm as a

Musical Talent,” scientific rhythm defined a new quality of musical talent, one

exclusively based on chronographic mimicry. Seashore confirms, “First, the capacity for

rhythm rests upon certain fundamental powers which can be measured serviceably in

various forms by methods now being introduced through experimental psychology.”181

Thus, the sense of rhythm, in Seashore’s pedagogy, became the perception and reaction

to a “true” metronomic-chronographic reference. In the Musical Quarterly, he states

outright the rhythmic epistemology found identically in textbooks by Wundt and

Titchener:

179 Seashore, “The Measure of a Singer,” Science 35 (Feb. 9, 1912): 203-204.

180

Carl Emil Seashore, “The Measurement of Musical Talent,” The Musical Quarterly 1 (Jan.,

1915): 129-148.

181

Carl Emil Seashore, “The Sense of Rhythm as a Musical Talent,” The Musical Quarterly 4

(Oct., 1918): 513.

Page 337: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

316

There are two fundamental factors in the perception of rhythm: an instinctive

tendency to group impressions in hearing, and a capacity for doing this with

precision in time and stress. The subjective tendency is so deeply ingrained, on

account of its biological service, that we irresistibly group uniform successions of

sound, such as the tick of the clock, into rhythmic measure.182

Seashore continued to promote scientific traditions of rhythm in his own text The

Psychology of Musical Talent (1919), a culmination of nearly two decades of his own

laboratory research, in which the chronographic bias fully redefined human perception

and action in music performance and education:

The sense of time. The third elemental capacity is the sense of time. This is basic

for all perception of rhythm and for rhythmic action. A limitation in this capacity

for hearing time sets a corresponding limitation upon feeling, thought, and action.

The sense of rhythm. The sense of rhythm rests upon the sense of time, the sense

of intensity, and mental imagery, but it requires in addition a number of affective

and motor qualifications; thus a person may have a keen sense of time and

intensity and still not have a pronounced sense of rhythm. 183

Given that a chronographic apparatus regulated musical time in his laboratory, Seashore

infused the musician’s intentions with mechanical ideals of so many other experimental

psychologists, stating: “The ability to keep time, both vocally and instrumentally, also

rests upon a peculiar gift of precision in action.”184

He verified that inhuman, mechanical

precision became the overriding value for the new musical rhythm in the twentieth

century, and that Wundt’s attention studies now transferred to the fundamentals of music

performance:

182 Ibid., 507.

183

Carl Emil Seashore, The Psychology of Musical Talent (Boston: Silver, Burdett, and

Company, 1919), 9.

184

Ibid., 11.

Page 338: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

317

Musically, the sense of time is the natural capacity for precision in the hearing of

the duration of tones or short time intervals…The perception of musical time is

usually of a motor nature. That is, when we hear the first interval, we respond to it

by projecting ourselves into the thing heard in some form of real or imaged

action, and when the following intervals occur, we either repeat the same real or

imaged movements or match them, as it were, with the one which is being

produced.185

He reiterates the Wundt concept of rhythm as a passive reaction to an external stimulus,

while redefining the “Nature of Rhythm” that was only exposed through chronographic

procedure:

Definition. The sense of rhythm is an instinctive disposition to group recurrent

sense impressions vividly and with precision, by time, or intensity, or both, in

such a way as to derive pleasure and efficiency through the grouping.

A complex process. Rhythm is, thus, not an attribute of sensation, like time and

intensity. It is a complex process and involves literally the whole organism in the

form of responsiveness to measured intervals of time or tone. Rhythm as a whole

presents two fundamental aspects, the perception of rhythm and rhythmic

action.186

Furthering the research of previous psychologists, Seashore envisioned musical time

solely through objective, metronomic impressions, in which the passive subject simply

reacted to external stimulus, either mentally, physically, or both; the value of rhythm thus

became apparent only in a musician’s automatical ability to respond to chronographic

reference. Given these scientific justifications, in which external mechanics regulated

exacting rhythm for internal impulses, Seashore prescribed that correct performances

required the,

Need of precision. It has been said that the appreciation of good music does not

necessarily involve a keen sense of time because musical expression takes

liberties with exact time; but the response to that is that it requires a keen sense of

time to appreciate fully the graceful and artistic deviation from right time. Indeed,

it requires a higher power than that of being merely mechanically exact. Mistakes

185 Ibid.,104-5.

186

Ibid., 115.

Page 339: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

318

in time, neglect of time, or irregularity of time cannot per se be regarded as

beautiful. The sense of time is taxed particularly hard in the larger units of

complex rhythms where artistic balances are sought.187

Seashore’s comment yet again reveals the realities of musical performance outside the

laboratory: human “expression takes liberties” in the perception and practice of rhythm

and movement. And yet again, this living reality was redefined for “mistakes” and

“irregularities” against the psychologist’s rigid chronographic bias of exacting

mechanical time and action.

No longer a human sensation or an internally derived quality of movement,

Seashore’s efficient rhythm—regulated with chronographic values—underpinned his new

tests of musical ability. In his laboratory the old time and pulse of musical meter, both

sonically and visually, was entirely effaced in favor of a succession of metronomic

quarter notes. To test a student’s musical memory, Seashore created psychological

examinations in the traditions of Wundt’s attention and fatigue studies: the metronome

ruled and regulated a new, chronographic experiment in the individual’s perception to

sound, now intended for musical training.188

With no indication of meter, affect, accent,

or articulation, these studies bore no relation to compositions or performance practices of

the pre-metronomic tradition. In these new experiments, the clockwork metronome,

which Seashore advised should beat at 94 to the minute, became the new chronograph of

musical ability for twentieth-century education.

187 Ibid., 114.

188

Over the course of his career, Seashore devised and published many musical-psychological

examinations that are beyond the scope for the present study.

Page 340: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

319

Fig. 4.9. Seashore’s musical-memory test, in which the metronome served as temporal constant, a

chronographic regulator during this experimental procedure that entirely effaced the physical sensation of

pulse, meter, accent, or musical phrase. Standard time signatures, relating traditional musical pulse, were

completely discarded in Seashore’s notation as well. The subject—as in other chronographic experiments

testing memory, fatigue, or attention—passively absorbed and reacted to the sound series defined by the

monotonous clicks of the metronome. This test was one of many that Seashore used to quantify “musical

talent” in controlled laboratory settings. Printed in Carl Emil Seashore, The Psychology of Musical Talent

(Boston: Silver, Burdett, and Company, 1919), 244.

Page 341: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

320

The Conclusions in Conflicting “Rhythm”

Somewhat ironically, as Seashore promoted experimental psychologists’ methods to the

training of twentieth-century musicians, some experimental psychologists began

expressing great reservations about the Wundt methodology of rhythm, along with the

chronographic study of musicality that followed past lines of experimentation. Some of

these next-generation scientists recognized that the reductive qualities of chronographic

rhythm represented a gross misconception of living practice. Patterson, in The Rhythm of

Prose, explained these critiques of past psychologists and the absolute laws of creative

rhythm that they attempted to expose:

As far back as Meumann, theorists such as Lotze have been accused of confusing

the conventional schedules of musical notation with music itself — the real

succession, not of sounds nor even of sound sensations, but of subjective

“impressions,” with their shifting factors and subtle illusions. It seems all the

more strange, however, that Meumann himself and virtually every one else that

has followed him should ignore, after all these warnings, the full application to

the problem of speech rhythm of what music really is — not what it appears to be

when trammeled by conventional notation; and should fail to see that the

possibilities of acceleration, syncopation, and substitutional equivalence, together

with subjective illusion, quite easily cover, for the sound-organizing type of mind,

every combination of discrete sounds within the ordinary limits of human

sensibility and within the time-limits of grouping distance, experimentally

established. Accordingly, there is no haphazard series of sounds, within these

limits, that cannot be organized by certain minds, when properly attentive, upon a

temporal basis. The problem necessarily remains a matter of individual

difference.189

While Patterson still envisioned rhythmic performance as a passive mental reaction to

external sound, he reported that limited notational data and clockwork sound could never

fully express the realities of musical time—qualities that Seashore and Jaques-Dalcroze

effaced through their metronomic-rhythm exercises and experiments.

Some notable psychologists grew mistrustful of the simplified conclusions

189 Patterson, 45.

Page 342: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

321

provided by chronographic research that artificially gauged an individual’s multifaceted

experiences of rhythm. By 1929, psychologist Christian A. Ruckmick, perhaps the most

outspoken skeptic of previous rhythmic studies, had expressed reservations about his

colleagues’ chronographic methods as they mistranslated musical time on the grounds

that:

We cannot summarily remove the rhythmical pattern from the melodic and

harmonic relationships that surround it…But since [the scientific study of]

attention for the moment centers chiefly on the temporal pattern of the rhythm as

a unit we ordinarily neglect for the most part these conditioning factors. An

elaboration of this thought means also, for example, that a rhythm perceived in

any one sensory field like the auditory field is not exactly the same in all of its

relationships as the same temporal form that is perceived in some other sensory

field. Strictly speaking, an auditory rhythm announced by a musical instrument is

not the same rhythm that is visually presented in the score.190

In a footnote to his article “The Rhythmical Experience from the Systematic Point of

View,” Ruckmick voiced this obvious discrepancy between objective notation and

subjective performance—one already expressed by experienced nineteenth-century

musicians: “It therefore follows that the reading of rhythms from score can not be

systematically identified with rhythms that are heard from musical instruments.”191

In

other words, living musical rhythm did not reflect laboratory performances in which

chronographic time precisely and mathematically dictated each distinct, printed note

value—the very hallmark of Wundt’s “rhythmic” conception.

In a sense Ruckmick began to uncover the pre-metronomic underpinnings of

rhythm. All rhythmic notations must be interpreted with non-notated phenomena, yet

psychologists since Wundt had been solely relying upon the metronomic click for their

190 Christian A. Ruckmick, “The Rhythmical Experience from the Systematic Point of View,” The

American Journal of Psychology 39 (Dec., 1927): 366.

191

Ruckmick, 366, n17.

Page 343: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

322

rhythmical interpretations. Experienced musicians, as Ruckmick recognized, used

subjective references during concert performances, which were far less controlling or

consistent than the machines employed in the mechanical laboratory. Questioning the

entire purpose of most previous scientific studies on rhythmic perception, Ruckmick

conjectured, “This opens up a long avenue of experiments on what actually occurs in the

reading of rhythms from score and their concomitant production through musical

instruments.”192

As Ruckmick ascertained, the entire tradition of experimental

psychology—spanning, at the time of his publication, over 50 years—based rhythm on

wholly impractical procedures. He concluded that rhythmic “analysis anywhere along the

line of mental processes will always remain, under the category of a science, an artificial

procedure,”193

one entirely unrelated to living, creative action.

As early as 1899, Scripture had also recognized that the laboratory rhythm of the

metronome and musical rhythm in living performance were two distinct epistemologies

of time and movement. In the periodical Science, he concluded, “Two entirely different

forms of regularly repeated action are to be distinguished. In one form the subject is left

free to repeat the movement at any interval he may choose. This includes such activities

as walking, running, rowing, beating time, and so on.” 194

The second form was dictated

through external, metronomic means. (Significantly, he still described individuals with

chronographically unfettered actions as laboratory subjects “left free” to act as

unscientifically as they wished.) Scripture admitted to the complex problems of so-called

192 Ibid.

193

Ibid.

194

Scripture, “Observations on Rhythmic Action,” 807.

Page 344: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

323

free human rhythms, problems that seldom seemed to conform to the limits of mechanical

“regularity.” He reflected, “not only does every simple activity have its own natural

rhythms; combinations of activities have rhythms that are derived from the simpler

ones…The natural periods do not always correspond with the enforced periods.”195

In The Rhythm of Prose, Patterson too commented, “Rhythmic experience is so

complex, and individuals differ so largely in the enjoyment of it, that a new phrasing of

its meaning would be necessary for each person, in order to avoid errors of inaccurate

generalization.”196

Patterson admitted that experimental psychologists had no complete

answer for the subjective meanings of musical time. Indeed, inaccurate generalizations

appeared everywhere in the rhythm tests of Meumann, Bolton, and Scripture. Patterson

paraphrased Paul Verrier’s tracts on poetic rhythm, Essai sur les principles de la

metrique anglaise (1909-1919) and “Les variations temporelles du rythme,” which

confirmed, “The equality of time-intervals is an illusion. The individual rhythm of the

speaker adapts itself to the fluctuations of sentiment without giving the impression of

being unrhythmical. Rhythm is pleasing when it coincides with an individual’s inner

rhythm, which in itself is subject to variations.”197

Verrier’s work also suggested that

“natural” rhythm was the antithesis of chronography. In 1923, a reviewer for Modern

Language Notes verified scientists’ failure to define the living rhythm of spoken

195 Ibid., 811.

196

Paterson, 88.

197

Ibid., 35. See also P. Verrier, Essai sur les principles de la metrique anglaise, III (Paris, 1909-

1919), 63; and “Les variations temporelles du rythme,” Journal de Psy. Norm. et Path. I (1913):

18.

Page 345: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

324

performances solely through seemingly precise evidence culled from apparatuses and

printed notation:

Regular rhythm in versification is a useful figure to designate a succession of

equal time-units and a consequent regular recurrence of the verse-stresses; but

language has characteristics of utterance and movement that set its rhythm free

from the strict requirements of regular rhythm as described in the physical

laboratory.198

Paull Franklin Baum, a contemporary of both Patterson and Verrier and who was

influenced by their writings, also recognized that laboratory rhythm was incommensurate

with the time of poetry and music, which originated in the subjective and creative acts of

speech and movement. Highly critical of the metronomic methodology currently

employed in analyzing poetic meter and instructing students, Baum expressed the fallacy

behind the limited, reductive rhythm championed by the chronographically biased in his

text The Principles of English Versification (1922). In his first chapter, Baum

distinguishes the various concepts and sensations that fall under the single, entirely vague

term “rhythm,” which can apply to both spatial and sonic patterns.199

“Rhythm, in its

simplest sense,” Baum notes, “is measured motion” and its essential characteristic is not

a priori mechanical repetition, but a “regularity of variation. Whatever changes or

alternates according to a recognizable system is said to be rhythmic, to posses rhythm.”200

Guided by this epistemology, Baum considered that the species of “temporal

rhythms…manifest themselves to us as phenomena of sound; hence the two concepts

time-rhythm and sound-rhythm are commonly thought of as one and the same.” Oddly,

198 J. W. B., “Brief Mention,” Modern Language Notes 38 (Feb., 1923): 127.

199

Paull Franklin Baum, The Principles of English Versification, Third edition (Cambridge:

Harvard University Press, 1924), 4.

200

Ibid., 3.

Page 346: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

325

while he speaks of images, architecture, and printed points as being rhythmic, Baum

neglected silent, visual motion—such as witnessed in a simple pendulum—as an

important reference of rhythm. Yet, he fully understood that the sound-rhythm of music

was a far more complicated phenomenon than could be heard through clockwork:

The simplest form [of sound-rhythm] is the tick-tick-tick of a watch or

metronome. But such mechanical regularity is comparatively rare, and in general

the temporal rhythms are all highly complex composites of sounds and silences.

Their highest manifestations are music and language.201

In order to explain the complexities of creative temporal rhythm more fully, Baum

separated the actualization of musical performance from rigid scientific procedures—

while ironically employing a chronographic analogy:

Thus we have always at hand both a more or less efficient bodily metronome in

the pulse and in respiration, and also a “cerebral metronome” capable not only of

easy adjustment to different rates of speed but also of that subtlest of modulations

which psychologists call the ‘elastic unit,’ and which musicians, though not so

definitely or surely, recognize as tempo rubato.202

Acknowledging that rubato was an as-of-yet indefinite and unquantifiable musical

quality, Baum also recognized, like William James and countless philosophic minds

before him, that the variable phenomenon of rhythm constituted a subjective sense, and

201 Ibid., 4-5.

202 Baum, 14. In instructing poets on meter, Baum suggests that regularity of rhythm—grounded

by this “internal metronome”—must balance unordered, rhythmic chaos: “While the formal

pattern remains fixed and inflexible, over its surface may be embroidered variations of almost

illimitable subtlety and change; but always the formal pattern must be visible, audible. The poet's

skill lies largely in preserving a balance of the artistic principles of variety in uniformity and

uniformity in variety. Once he lets go the design, he loses his metrical rhythm and writes mere

prose. Once we cease to hear and feel the faint regular beating of the metronome we fail to get the

enjoyment of sound that it is the proper function of metre to give. On the other hand, if the

mechanical design stands out too plainly, if the beat of the metronome becomes for an instant

more prominent than the music of the words, then also the artistic pleasure is gone, for too much

uniformity is as deadly to art as too much variety.” Ibid., 54-55.

Similar to Paderewski’s and Marx’s statements of nineteenth-century creativity, Baum

contended: “No poet ever wrote to a metronome accompaniment.” Ibid., 66.

Page 347: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

326

that “The sense of rhythm, as has been said, differs remarkably in different individuals—

just as the sense of touch, of smell, of hearing.”203

And this non-mechanical, variable

feeling for rhythm, as most experienced musicians had expressed for decades (if not

centuries), was heard through the musical meter and phrasing of skilled solo and

ensemble performances:

Very few of us can estimate correctly the passage of five minutes: syllables are

uttered in a few hundredths of a second. We are satisfied with the accuracy shown

by an orchestra in keeping time; but if we took a metronome to the concert we

should find the orchestra very deficient in its sense of time. The fact is that the

orchestra knows better than the metronome, that perfectly accurate time intervals

become unpleasantly monotonous, that we rebel at ‘mechanical’ music. Thus the

time divisions of pleasurable rhythm are not mathematically equal, nor even

necessarily approximately equal, but are such as are felt to be equal.204

Baum’s rhythmic conception was in stark opposition to Scripture’s assessments from the

previous century on the “regular,” chronographic ideal for orchestral performances and

performers. As Baum confirmed, the living feeling of creative rhythm, in both musical

and poetic time, could not be fully explained, intuited, or expressed through the

metronome or psychologists’ chronographic bias.

P. F. Swindle was one of the first prominent skeptics of the scientific knowledge

of “rhythm” in the field of experimental psychology, and with research that prefigured

Ruckmick and Baum, he offered perhaps the most damning assessments of the

chronographic method as applied to living, rhythmic performances. Swindle tested his

subjects’ abilities to physically perform uncommon rhythmic patterns in five, seven, and

203 Ibid. Baum theorized on the experiential rhythm of verbal performances and offered a

conclusion significant for musical performances as well: “Language is therefore a compound

instrument of both sound and meaning, and speech-rhythm, in its fullest sense, is a composite

resultant of the attributes of sound (duration, intensity, and pitch) modified by the logical and

emotional content of the words and phrases which they represent.” See Baum, 10.

204

Ibid., 57.

Page 348: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

327

eleven “impressions” and concluded similarly to Verrier and later Baum about the

sensory phenomena of rhythm. As reported in his American Journal of Psychology article

“On the Inheritance of Rhythm” (1913), Swindle realized that the human capacity for

rhythm could not be relegated to mental perception and reaction time alone, nor could it

be exposed through artificial laboratory machines and methods:

All experimental evidence seems to point to the conclusion that rhythm is

acquired by each individual, and that it is not inherited. Biological conditions—

for example, the anatomical fact that we are two-footed, two-handed, and

generally two-sided, not three-cornered or star-fish like beings—are favorable for

the development of those rhythms which have usually been considered to be

instinctive, while the other rhythms can be acquired only under special, somewhat

artificial conditions. The best means for developing rhythm is that which

approaches our ordinary life activities.205

Humans acquire rhythm, Swindle asserts, through lived action and experience, not

through precise, external controls or unaccustomed chronographic training—and thus he

expressed an epistemology shared with skilled musicians of earlier centuries. While

testing his subjects for the inherent “agreeability” of performing rhythm in uncommon

seven-patterns, Swindle found:

As to the preferred tempo [of the experiment] I found that there was a wide

disagreement among the individuals, and that some individuals preferred different

tempos from day to day.206

Given his belief that rhythm stems from basic physical actions, Swindle was highly

critical of Jaques-Dalcroze’s modern pedagogy, which seemingly inculcated artificial and

extreme precisions of rhythmic action upon young students.207

Using what Swindle

205 P. F. Swindle, “On the Inheritance of Rhythm,” The American Journal of Psychology 24 (Apr.,

1913): 202.

206

Ibid., 192.

207

Ibid., 199.

Page 349: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

328

recognized as the “counting method,” Jaques-Dalcroze’s Eurhythmics promoted a new

kind of rhythm, one more allied with psychologists’ chronographic experiments, in which

regulated, mathematically gauged performance acts ingrained a new quality of

mechanized action upon his trainees.208

How little these scientific-mathematical

processes had to do with pre-chronographic musical aesthetics is prefigured by A. B.

Marx in General musical instruction (1839). Criticizing the average, amateur music

education, Marx reflects:

With every new composition, this misery of counting, beating, and stamping

begins afresh, until a mechanical habit of equality is formed, instead of a living

feeling for equal and uniform measure and its expression. It is unfortunately too

true, that most musicians are content with the sense and capacity for mechanical

equality of measure,—for the cold inanimate beat; and consider the rich and

living rhythmical feeling as superfluous.209

Seventy-four years later, Swindle posited that if an individual’s sense for rhythm usually

conflicted with mechanical-mathematical training methods, and if lived experience and

the very structure of the body “is at the basis of rhythmic perception,” then,

The time element in all rhythm should be expected to vary. General agreement as

to tempo would be truly remarkable. The time required to execute the movements,

the time between the movements, the tempo, etc., should make such fluctuations

as can be accounted for only when the nature of the environment that called forth

the rhythm is well known.210

Due to previous psychologists’ neglect of the empirical rhythm outside of the laboratory,

or the rhythms originating in human movement itself, Swindle goes so far as to suggest

that all previous psychological investigations into rhythm were founded on false

208 Ibid., 201-2.

209

Adolf Bernhard Marx, Allgemeine Musiklehre (Leipzig, 1839); English edition, General

Musical Instruction, translated by George Macirone (London: J. Alfred Novello, 1854), 120.

Italics original.

210

Swindle., 193.

Page 350: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

329

premises: “It appears at this stage more evident than ever that the more important

problems [of rhythmic perception and action] heretofore investigated may be looked

upon as pseudo-problems.”211

Swindle conjectured that if his findings were accurate, if

individual difference in interpreting and actualizing rhythm was indeed the norm and not

an error of the personal equation, then the entire endeavor of chronographic rhythm-study

was a futile attempt at fruitless knowledge. In brief, the human actualization of “true”

metronomic “rhythm” was pseudo-science.

Swindle’s assessments—along with the many other critiques of laboratory

“rhythm” presented in this study—should raise further questions: Did Meumann, Bolton,

Sears, Scripture, and Seashore conclusively reveal the once-hidden psychological laws of

rhythm? Through their efforts, did they discover an underlying science of poetic and

musical time? Or, rather, did they invent new laws, machines, and practices that served

the purposes of their experiments and beliefs alone? Did their observations and

chronographic data benefit already-experienced composers and performers, offering them

a greater understanding of their own actions and thoughts? Or, rather, did these

psychologists support an opposing value system that, since the founding of Wundt’s

laboratory, modern scientists had invested a great deal of time, energy, and money

promoting; a value system they staked their individual careers and reputations on; a value

system that upheld fellow scientists’ authority over self-defined aesthetic truths while

devaluing the creative individual’s intelligence, ability, and experience? Under the

chronographic bias, experimental psychologists mistranslated musical rhythm through a

scientific tradition that began only with chronographic testing, a tradition that justified

diminishing creative personal interpretation through methods that dictated the time of

211 Ibid., 193.

Page 351: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

330

musical performance no differently than the time of a labor-fatigue test or an

astronomical observation.

While certain scientists in the second decade of the twentieth century began

seriously questioning the Wundt methodology of rhythm, the traditions and techniques of

chronography expanded through the writings of Seashore, who aggressively promoted

Wundt’s science to new generations of twentieth-century musicians and educators

through 1945. Indeed, Seashore can be considered the culmination of the experimental-

psychology tradition—a prominent purveyor of the reconceived qualities of musical time,

rhythm, and individual musicality for Western performance culture. Just as Meumann

and Bolton disassociated human music-making from the other subjective speech or dance

practices—allying rhythmical action instead with a series of passive technical actions

unrelated to living thought, expression, or volition—so did Seashore, a self-styled and

highly published musical pedagogue, reduce the once-creative act of performing music to

mechanical-mathematical factors and efficiently reproducible actions, all under the

objective guise of a scientific method. Two years after Swindle questioned the entire use

of psychologists’ previous “rhythmic” research, Seashore expounded upon Wundt’s

original methodology in the first edition of The Musical Quarterly (1915)—where he

devalued the creative, musical mind in favor of his chronographic procedures:

[Musical] Talent, like the dream, has been thought of as peculiarly illusive and

intangible for observation. Yet the science of individual psychology to-day

virtually "dissects" the genius, analyzes and measures talents, sets out limitations,

diagnoses the possibilities, and directs the development of the individual.212

Seashore, propounding the revolutionary aesthetics first cultivated in Wundt’s Leipzig

laboratory, treated the creative musician—the individual thinker and performer,

212 Seashore, “The Measurement of Musical Talent,” 129-130.

Page 352: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

331

unencumbered by scientific tools such as the metronome—as mere, lacking in the

essential knowledge bestowed only through psychological experimentation. Seashore

summarized his scientific culture:

Scientific psychology has given us an approach, a tool, a vision…The mere artist

views talent as we view the starlit heavens on a moonlight stroll; the one who

begins to control conditions, to employ instruments, and to apply scientific

principles (inductive and deductive) and measures, views human talents as the

astronomer views the heavenly bodies.

The astronomer magnifies distances, intensifies illuminations, analyzes the

atmospheres, reviews the records of ages, trusts his instruments and gives wings

to scientific imagination; he measures, predicts, and explains; and with it all his

visible universe grows larger, more orderly, and more sublime…The expert in the

measurement of human talents has similar opportunities. The stars form a

macrocosm; the powers of the human mind are a microcosm. Both are orderly.

Astronomy is old; the science of the human mind is barely coming into existence.

The psychology of music is a new field, quite unworked, but full of promise and

fascinating possibilities.213

Seashore, one of the first self-assigned music psychologists, was not the first to envision

the “promise and possibilities” of reducing musicians’ actions through clockwork

quantification. As we have seen, the work of Meumann, Bolton, Scripture, Ebhardt,

Sears, and others set the stage for Seashore and his chronographic prescriptions for

modern music education throughout North America. Ever since Wundt transferred the

sound of the metronome into traditional music notation, experimental psychologists

indeed perceived the phenomena of musicality like astronomers’ observations of prior

decades, applying chronographic automatons to measure and efface the personal

equation. Seashore’s epistemology of time and human action, originating in Wundt’s

laboratory, was like nothing that skilled musicians of the nineteenth century exercised;

his chronographic variety of musical time flourished in laboratories decades before most

professional musicians ever embraced the aesthetics of mechanically regulated

213 Ibid., 148.

Page 353: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

332

performances on the concert stage. Only within the last two decades of the nineteenth

century, did these scientists begin applying astronomers’ chronographic time to the

rhythm of trained musicians, in research that eventually influenced the pedagogies of

music, dance, and speech with new values of time and action.

The musician’s innate experience had no place in most psychologists’ mechanical

epistemology of performance. In scientists’ culture of efficient, replicable action,

mechanical readouts and metronomic controls trumped the lived, individual perceptions

of musical rhythm. Since experimental psychologists, as a whole, had a seemingly

passive and second-rate understanding of both music theory and practice, it is perhaps not

surprising that neither Wundt nor his followers ever fully explained the laws of the truly

skilled performers, composers, or improvisers of eighteenth- and nineteenth-century

traditions—a glaring fact alluded to by skeptical scientists such as Swindle and

Ruckmick. Considering the vast body of research they produced on the science of

“rhythm,” experimental psychologists offered no definitive answers, no chronographic or

mathematical absolutes for the exceptional minds of Mozart, Beethoven, Chopin, Liszt,

Wagner, Brahms, Mahler, Hofmann, Rachmaninoff, or many others. Wundt and his

followers neglected the compositions and performances of musical “artists” spanning

their age, those creative individuals who did not conform to the reductive, mechanical

paradigms found exclusively in experimental psychologists’ laboratories.

While ignoring skilled music performers as they truly existed outside the

laboratory, experimental psychologists’ reinterpretation of rhythm represented a

revolutionary, twofold paradigm shift, a metronomic turn for modern performance

practices: the musician became the observer, a passive subject of a standardized action, as

Page 354: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

333

musical notes now represented precise, mathematically gauged objects—outside of the

mind and body, outside of the individual’s will, outside of the personal sense of pulse—to

be observed against exacting chronographic-metronomic regulation. First in the

chronographic laboratory, musical performance became a mental-physical experiment of

mechanically efficient, exacting, and reproducible action. Identical to astronomers’

record of star transits, musicians’ performance of quarter notes, eighth notes, and smaller

units reduced to their passive observation and reaction to a succession of distant, visual

points measured under the absolute rule of chronographic time, Scripture’s “great

independent variable,” the ever-present, uncontrollable “tick-tick-tick” of metronomic

rhythm.

Page 355: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

334

CHAPTER V: THE METRONOMIC INFLUENCE

Distraction, Habituation, and Other Human Changes through Metronomic Time

With experimental psychologists using the clockwork metronome as an essential tool in

the research of the mind and body since 1875, it would only be a matter of time for them

to discover the psychological implications machine’s click upon their subjects’ thoughts

and actions. Apart from studying muscular fatigue in metronomic time, memory

recollection in metronomic time, and “rhythmical” perception in metronomic time,

scientists began to methodically test a human’s mental and physical reactions when

exposed to the sound of the metronome itself. Psychological experiments from the late

1880s through the 1930s documented the very real results of mechanical monotony, the

metronome’s clockwork click, upon an individual’s nervous and musculature system. By

the 1920s, behavioral scientists such as Pavlov had confirmed these effects: Maelzel’s

metronome significantly influenced and altered human and animal behavior; it was a

highly effective tool used to stimulate, distract, or habituate the laboratory subject.

The ability of machines to stimulate (make nervous, excitable) or distract (draw

attention away, stifle a train of thought, or cause immediate forgetfulness) may be easily

understood by anyone startled by an un-muffled motorcycle engine, the shrill ring of a

cellular phone, or rapidly firing gunshots. Habituation, however, is a psychological

process that occurs over time, as Titchener explains in his Text-book of Psychology

(1896):

Page 356: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

335

“[Habituation is] a tendency, taking shape in the course of a series of similar

observations [experiments], to experience and describe perceptions of similar

character.” The processes of the habituated consciousness are meager, uniformly

indistinct, and definitely directed by determination; the generic likeness of their

description is, therefore, due rather to lack of clearness than to qualitative

resemblance. The habitual tendencies may be classified, in order of persistence

and of influence on consciousness, under five headings: weakest are those that

depend solely upon recency of occurrence; next are those due to situations of

great insistence; stronger are those arising from the professional or other routine

activities of adult life, and therefore referable both to recency and to repetition;

stronger, again, are those originating in training during childhood, and therefore

referable to insistence and repetition. 1

According to this definition, the psyche habituates through incessant and repetitive

training, as routine actions become seemingly thoughtless over time. The Macmillan

Company’s Dictionary of Philosophy and Psychology (1901) adds to the definition, “The

term drill is also used, especially in cases in which an external authority imposes the

exercise for purposes of training, etc.”2 For these reasons according to Titchener, learning

processes based in repetitive acts were most likely to habituate impressionable children

and working professionals. Through the turn of the century, psychologists employed their

scientific metronomes to guide and dictate the repetitive training of individuals, in order

to understand how the mind and body could be habituated through “rhythmic” activities.

As numerous studies by experimental psychologists show, the metronome caused

significant behavioral changes in their subjects over time. Wundt definitively

acknowledged that frequent repetitions of an external sensation, such as the metronome,

1 Edward Bradford Titchener, A Text-book of Psychology (New York: The Macmillan Company,

1916), 539.

2 James Mark Baldwin, ed. Dictionary of Philosophy and Psychology, vol. I (New York:

Macmillan Company, 1901), 436.

Page 357: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

336

habituate the mind.3 Thus, the fact that the metronome caused distractive, stimulating,

and habituating effects has clear and serious implications for those individuals who began

regular training with the machine by the end of the nineteenth century: the student,

amateur, and eventually professional performers of Western music traditions.

Scientists’ findings on the metronomic influence might not be so surprising if we

consider that—since the early nineteenth-century—critics of the clockwork metronome

noted the psychological “crutch,” the slavishness, and the “ill-at-ease” feeling imposed

by the application of artificial time to natural musical action, as Chapter II documented.

From the very first descriptions of the device, critics noted that the mechanical tick

referencing a student’s practice often produced a mechanical-sounding musician. Before

experimental psychologists quantified the phenomena of habituation through their

scientific method, experienced musicians had already intuited and heard the performer’s

transformation brought about through metronomic training.

Sir John Lubbock used a clockwork metronome in what appears to be an early

methodical attempt at dog training, which prefigured the work of Pavlov, but with less

than promising results. In his book On Senses, Instincts, and Intelligence of Animals,

Lubbock himself described that his training apparatus, the metronome, was originally

“the instrument used for marking time when practizing the pianoforte.”4 In 1886, Popular

Science Monthly reported on Lubbock’s scientific applications of the machine, in which

he “tried to train his dog not to take a piece of bread till he had counted seven; but when

3 Wilhelm Wundt, Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology, translated from the second

edition by J. E. Creighton and E. B. Titchener (London: Swan Sonnenschein & Co., Lim, 1907),

141.

4 Sir John Lubbock, On Senses, Instincts, and Intelligence of Animals, Second Edition (London:

Kegan Paul, Trench & Co., 1889), 284.

Page 358: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

337

he used a metronome the dog showed that he was lost.”5 It seems that his dog became

distracted or confused by the sound, a prominent effect of the clockwork metronome that

psychologists would soon discover. In 1899, Frank Angell, a former student of Wundt

who specialized in the distractive potential of sounds, summarized the various and

seemingly conflicting effects witnessed on some subjects when psychologists applied the

metronome in laboratory research:

In this connection it is curious to observe that metronome beats have been used

sometimes as a means of distraction, sometimes without being noticed, and

sometimes with even the effect of fixing the attention on the regular stimuli. It

illustrates the complexity of the distraction problem that the same kind of stimulus

should come to produce either fixation or distraction according to the attitude of

the reagent.6

Titchener commented that during the standard rhythm-sense experiments, two clicking

metronomes, if continued for over a minute often caused the typical subject to viscerally

react against the barrage of artificial sounds:

Rhythmically disposed O’s [human observers (i.e. subjects)] may not object to a

longer series; but for the most part a prolongation of the clicks to 70 sec., even at

fairly quick rates (.65 and .39 sec. intervals), will render O uneasy, and perhaps

evoke the exclamation “This is horrible!” or “This is unbearable!” The “listening”

to a series of [metronomic] sounds, without the least hit of what is to be listened

for, puts a severe strain upon the attention.7

By the turn of the century, professional psychologists had noted that the metronome—

now the essential temporal reference and regulator for fatigue experiments—altered the

5 M. J. Delboeuf, “What May Animals be Taught?,” The Popular Science Monthly XXIX (New

York: D Appleton and Company, 1886): 170.

6 Frank Angell and Henry Harwood, “Experiments on Discrimination of Clangs for Different

Intervals of Time,” The American Journal of Psychology 11 (Oct., 1899): 71-72.

Edward Bradford Titchener, Experimental Psychology: A Manual of Laboratory Practice,

Volume I (New York: The Macmillan Company, 1901), 340.

Page 359: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

338

subject’s attention, potentially complicating results of the chronographic procedure.

Psychologist F. M. Urban summarized these phenomena in 1908:

There are several ways in which the beats of the metronome might possibly

influence the judgments:

l.) Regular acoustical stimuli produce a feeling process which may be the

immediate cause for judgments in a certain direction.

2.) The beats of the metronome may influence attention, which is the psychical

function affected by the rhythmic motion of lifting the weights.

3.) The acoustical stimuli may interfere with the mechanical contraction of the

muscles in such a way as to reinforce or inhibit the action of the muscles.8

The sound of the metronome had the very real possibility of distracting the mind, which

could subsequently affect the laboratory subject’s muscular reactions and emotions

during any given experiment-performance. While Lubbock’s early attempts to train his

pet under repetitive metronomic clicks failed, the reports of both Urban and Angell

confirm that psychologists by the end of the 1880s seemed far more successful at training

human subjects to react to the stimulus of the metronome, their preeminent laboratory

tool for habituation, stimulation, or distraction.

Edgar James Swift, furthering Ebbinghaus’ work from the preceding years,

published his study on the “Disturbance of the Attention During Simple Mental

Processes” (1892) in the American Journal of Psychology. This researcher used many

different disturbing sounds while the subject performed some physical-mental operation.

One series of experiments traced “the simple muscular reaction in response to an

excitation of the sense of hearing and the same taken while a metronome was ticking one

hundred and twenty times each minute.”9 Swift then changed the metronome rates (the

8 F. M. Urban, The Application of Statistical Methods to the Problems of Psychophysics

(Philadelphia: The Psychological Clinic Press, 1908), 9-10.

9 Edgar James Swift, “Disturbance of the Attention during Simple Mental Processes,” The

Page 360: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

339

disturbances) to “40, 80, 120, 160 and 200 times each minute in the respective series.”10

To further complicate matters, he had his subject read a poem, an “English book,” and a

work of Kant to the disturbances of the clicking metronome.11

Swift’s results confirmed

that the sonic imposition of clockwork indeed altered the subject’s performances, as he

admitted of past research, “it would seem that the ordinary muscular-reactions are

affected by a disturbing sound” such as the metronome.12

Scientists recorded similar reactions from experienced musicians when subjecting

them to metronomic studies in the laboratory. Thaddeus Bolton, in his comprehensive

1894 report on rhythmic perception and performance, found that one subject with

“considerable musical talent” and “long and careful training in music” felt ill effects from

the exposure to artificial, monotonous rhythms. During Bolton’s experiment, this skilled

musician perceived “a slight feeling of muscle tension in the ear and back of the

scalp…there was a feeling of innervation of the muscles connected with attention” when

each rate of the clicking apparatus changed.13

Through the turn of the century, additional psychology research certified that

metronomic time—apart from its immediate distracting effects—could habituate the

human mind; repetitive exposure to the metronome facilitated what society would later

American Journal of Psychology 5 (Oct., 1892): 5.

10

Swift, 8.

11

Ibid., 18-9.

12

Ibid., 6.

13

Thaddeus L. Bolton, “Rhythm,” The American Journal of Psychology 6 (Jan., 1894): 192.

Bolton used a telephonic device to project sharp, monotonous clicks in a more precise fashion

than Maelzel’s metronome could provide.

Page 361: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

340

recognize as the Pavlovian response. In a notable example, the Yale Psychological

Laboratory devised condition-response experiments to document their subjects’

subconscious reactions to metronomic training over time. In 1896, the periodical Nature

and Science reported on sound and action studies at Yale University where,

Another experiment consisted in dropping a light pith ball on the hand of a person

so placed that he could not see what was done. Each fall of the ball was timed to

correspond with the sound regularly emitted by a metronome. After a while the

ball was no longer dropped, but the subject of the experiment continued to feel, or

imagine that he felt, the touch of the ball at every sound from the metronome.14

In studies that clearly prefigured Pavlov’s behavioral research on animals, Yale

psychologists—which at the time included both Scripture and Seashore15

—found that the

sound of the metronome became ingrained in the subjects’ mind over time through

repetitive action. The subjects were impressed with metronomic time through habitual

training; they performed to the unwavering mechanical click, seemingly without will or

forethought, and inadvertently grasped for (and indeed physically sensed) a ball that was

no longer there.

The father of experimental psychology himself, Wilhelm Wundt, acknowledged

that the metronome elicited involuntary physical effects on his subjects when introduced

in laboratory research, and that the artificial repetitions of the device actually altered

normal bodily functions and emotions. Summarizing decades of research in his Outlines

of Psychology, Wundt states:

14 “Nature and Science,” The Youth's Companion, Jun. 11, 1896, 307. APS Online.

<http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=841142492&sid=2&Fmt=2&clientId=3338&RQT=309&

VName=HNP>

15

The Yale studies are further related by E. W. Scripture, “Measuring Hallucinations,” Science 3

(May 22, 1896): 762-3. As director of the Yale laboratory, Scripture verifies that Seashore was

working under his guidance.

Page 362: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

341

It is observed in such a case that especially the respiration tends to adapt itself to

the faster or slower rate of the strokes, becoming more rapid when the rapidity of

the metronome increases. Commonly, too, certain phases of respiration coincide

with particular strokes. Furthermore, the hearing of such an indifferent rhythm is

not unattended by emotion. When the rate changes, we observe at first a quiet,

then a sthenic, and finally, when the rapidity is greatest, an asthenic emotion.16

Wundt’s subjects, similar to those at Yale, responded sympathetically to the metronome,

reacting to the machine’s artificial rate by involuntarily adjusting their breathing—and

correspondingly their feelings—to match its automatical rhythm. Bolton, too, recorded

these common reactions on musicians during his rhythm experiments.17

By the end of the

nineteenth century, as other researchers had readily noted the physiological influences of

the metronome on laboratory subjects, these effects seemed to become standard scientific

knowledge. In 1905, H. C. Stevens published a chart in the American Journal of

Psychology summarizing the attention studies of such psychologists as Delabarre (1892),

Mentz (1895), Lehmann (1899), which showed that in most cases their subjects’ pulse or

breathing rates increased when exposed to the auditory stimulus of Maelzel’s

metronome.18

Experimental psychologists continued to explore other emotional effects of

metronomic training in the twentieth century, sometimes in great detail. In a 1906

experiment based on Titchener’s methods, Samuel Perkins Hayes used two clicking

metronomes, both hidden from the subject, as disturbance agents. The two metronomes

simultaneously ticked at 14 different rates in seemingly random successions for a total of

16 Wilhelm Wundt, Outlines of Psychology, second revised edition from the fourth revised

German edition (Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann, 1902), 193-4.

17

See Bolton, 188, 192 for some instances.

18

H. C. Stevens, “A Plethysmographic Study of Attention,” The American Journal of Psychology

16 (Oct., 1905), Table III, insert between 468-9.

Page 363: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

342

546 experiments.19

Hayes faithfully documented the subjects’ feelings, using Wundt’s

standard (and reductive) emotional spectrum: “pleasantness, strain, unpleasantness,

relaxation, excitement, depression.”20

One subject reported that the “Rapid rates often

made her head throb unpleasantly, and sometimes gave her ‘an uncomfortable feeling in

one ear’” while another experienced “a succession of muscular strains ‘in the effort to

keep up with the rate,’ accompanied by more or less confusion and hurry,—a sort of

‘driven feeling,’ that there was also ‘a continuous whirr in the head.’” 21

More

significantly, one subject described an inability to think meaningfully about her situation

when fixating on the sound of the metronome. According to Hayes, the clicking caused

her a great deal of stress:

M. described the feeling of strain as a “general muscular tension, due to a rigid

attitude.” One day this strain seemed to be localized in her forehead. It was

accompanied by a breathless feeling. She was "completely absorbed in following

the stimulus." Consciousness seemed crowded, so that there was no room for

associations. On another day she thought the strain localized in her ears; it

accompanied each stroke of the metronome, but disappeared at the least relaxation

of the attention. The strain often grew less after the first few beats of a group. She

reported that the feeling of strain was generally unpleasant.22

Researchers witnessed another subject sympathetically moving to the motion of the

metronome, a mechanical behavior similarly documented by both Wundt and Yale

researchers:

19 Samuel Perkins Hayes, “A Study of the Affective Qualities. I. The Tridimensional Theory of

Feeling,” The American Journal of Psychology 17 (Jul., 1906), 380, 382.

20

Hayes, 282.

21

Ibid., 383.

22

Ibid.

Page 364: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

343

In all the experiments with the metronome, C. noted a constant tendency to “keep

time” with the beats by some sort of muscular movement, with the throat, feet,

hands, etc., and his judgments were based upon the ease or difficulty of this

procedure. C. made 556 comparisons.23

This particular subject found, “Those rates seemed most relaxing that called forth the

least muscular exertion and approximated the ‘natural rhythm.’”24

When the metronome

clicked more rapidly, the subject became increasingly aware of the machine’s artificial,

inhuman qualities as it guided the test. Hayes reports: “The most straining rates, he says,

are those ‘that require the most muscular tension in the effort to keep time, —those that

are farthest from a natural bodily rhythm.’”25

Hayes’ subject, perhaps a musician in some

capacity, realized the implications of his own metronomic performance: his physical

sense movement, pulse, and rhythm often seemed incommensurate with the inhuman

clicks of the scientific metronome.

James Burt Miner, Columbia University Fellow in psychology, too, realized that

metronomic rhythm was not within the normal course of human action. Thus, he devised

a card-dealing experiment to test the “Effect of an Objective Rhythm on Choice” for his

student-subjects’ performances. The “objective rhythm” took the form of amplified

metronomic clicks, which Miner documented in his text Motor, Visual, and Applied

Rhythms (1903):

23 Ibid., 388.

24

Ibid., 389.

25

Ibid.

Page 365: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

344

The experiments were to test the effect of the rhythmic beating of a metronome

upon the work done by the subjects in distributing the cards. A series of records

was taken when the subjects were working normally, another when the

metronome was beating at the rate of 40 per minute with the bell sounding on

every alternate beat, and a third series with the metronome beating 200 per minute

with the bell on every alternate beat.26

With the metronome guiding and gauging his subjects’ card-dealing performance, Miner

found that each individual reacted differently to the metronome, owing to personal

variation. But in every case the metronome changed the quality of their actions, as Miner

concluded, “On the whole the slow person is quite likely to profit from an independent

rhythmical stimulus, while the quick person is very much disturbed”27

by the artificial

“rhythm” of the metronome. Valuing the potential of the metronome to improve labor

efficiencies, Miner noted that while quick-thinking minds are most often disturbed by the

machine, “the subject who does mental work indifferently will be excited by the rhythmic

accompaniment and spurred to greater effort. The independent stimulus, which is

ordinarily supposed to be distracting, seems in the latter case to favor more rapid work.”28

In 1918, Cassell and Dallenbach published their research on human attention as

influenced by the metronome, in its now-typical role as “'interrupted-continuous

distractor.” As reported in The American Journal of Psychology, the psychologists set the

metronome, “beating 120 in the minute. It was electrically controlled from the

experimenter's desk, so that it could be released and checked before and after every

26 James Burt Miner, “Motor, Visual, and Applied Rhythms,” (PhD diss., Columbia University,

1903), 99.

27

Miner, 102.

28

Ibid., 102-3.

Page 366: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

345

distraction-series” on the human subjects.29

Cassell and Dallenbach’s results are

revelatory—with significant implications for performers and pedagogues of music—since

subject “R” expressed the specific effects of the lengthy training procedure, his repetitive

metronomic conditioning over time:

“At first I found that the distraction of the metronome upset my determination for

sensory reaction, and I think I tended to give muscular reactions. The metronome

is very unpleasant.” That he did give a different kind of reaction is shown, in

comparison with the normal and the other distraction-series that followed, by the

extremely low average for that day. He reports after a normal series on the second

day (ratio of 1.052): “The reaction without the metronome seems to be more

passive than with it. When the metronome is sounding, I have actively to attend

away from it. When it is not going, I just take the stimuli passively, and react to

them as they come.” After a distraction-series on the 6th day (ratio of 1.01 I):

“While there is still some effort necessary to attend away from the metronome,

this effort is getting less. In this series, my attitude was only slightly different

from that of the preceding - [a normal series], yet it was more effortful, more

active.” And again, after a normal series on the 11th day, when the distraction and

the normal series were practically equal: “I cannot notice any difference between

these reactions and those with the metronome going. I try to take them all in a

passive attitude, just waiting until the stimulus comes without straining for it”;

and after a distraction-series on the same day: “I seem to fall quite naturally into

the ‘set’ for reacting with the metronome. It ‘feels’ natural, and I cannot see that it

distracts me. On the contrary, it is rather a steadying agency. In the preparatory

interval, its sounds drop out of consciousness.”30

As the subject learned to react to the metronome over time, his beliefs and even his

feelings gradually changed; with continual conditioning, he grew to accept the

mechanical clicks both physically and mentally. The metronome, which began as a

distraction, became something else: a psychological and physical comfort. He first had

bodily (muscular) reactions against the distractive metronomic sound, yet eventually

29 Edna E. Cassel and K. M. Dallenbach, “The Effect of Auditory Distraction upon the Sensory

Reaction,” The American Journal of Psychology 29 (Apr., 1918): 132. See the researchers’ Table

V (page 135) for the averaged, accumulated data on their subjects’ altered responses due to

metronomic distraction.

30

Cassel and Dallenbach, 141.

Page 367: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

346

came to find the clicks almost soothing. The 1918 experiment discovered the metronome

to be a normalizing influence upon the subject’s attitude: mechanical monotony became a

“steadying agency,” one that gradually felt “natural.” Only after eleven days of repetitive

exposure to the machine tests, the subject felt at ease with the incessant, artificial

regularity of metronomic time.31

As many earlier psychological studies confirmed, this

phenomenon of metronomic habituation could be expected: an individual’s thoughts,

feeling, and even actions altered with repetitive exposure to the clockwork stimulus. In

the minds of certain subjects continually trained in mechanical monotony, an artificial

distraction transformed into an overwhelmingly “natural feeling.”

Other psychologists continued to study the implications of metronomic training

on memory and recollection. The American Journal of Psychology report “On the Lapse

of Verbal Meaning with Repetition” (1919) by M. F. Bassett and C. J. Warne followed

the methods of Ebbinghaus,32

in which the psychologist trained himself to repeat words,

word-series, and nonsense syllables in strict metronomic time. “It is well known,” the

researchers explain, “that if a familiar word be stared at for a time, or repeated aloud over

and over again, the meaning drops away,” thus these scientists attempted to determine

31 In a separate attention-experiment report in 1913 by Dallenbach, another subject echoes this

initial “unpleasant” reaction to the incessant sound of the metronome. When two metronomes

were set going at different rates: “Thus, F reports, after an experiment in which the task was to

add 7 continuously: ‘The sounds of the metronomes, as a series of discontinuous clicks, were

clear in consciousness only four or five times during the experiment, and they were especially

bothersome at first. They were accompanied by strain sensations and unpleasantness.’” See Karl

M. Dallenbach, “The Measurement of Attention,” The American Journal of Psychology 24 (Oct.

1913): 467.

32

Angell and Harwood, 69.

Page 368: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

347

“the number of repetitions required for monosyllabic nouns to lose their meaning” in

metronomic time.33

With knowledge of the metronome as a sonic distracter, Bassett and Warne

realized that the click of the machine could mar their subjects’ performances, and

consequently the results of the experiment. Thus, they noted, “The rate of repetition was

controlled by a soundless metronome giving seconds; and both the number of repetitions

and the rate employed were, in a fashion, checked by taking the total time of an

experiment by a stop-watch.”34

After the initial phases of the test, two weeks elapsed, in

which the psychologists recognized a “practice effect” had taken place in the subjects,

which was explained “principally as the result of a gradual stabilization of the attitude

demanded by the instruction.”35

After the two weeks, the final test phase occurred, and

the scientists concluded:

(1) Given a stably passive attitude on the part of the observer, the meaning of a

familiar monosyllabic noun repeated aloud three times per sec. [timed by the

metronome] drops away in about 3 to 3.5 sec. (2) There is a tendency, as practice

advances, for varieties and fluctuations of meaning to give place to an habitual

meaning characterized by our observers as a 'familiar feel.' (3) Meaning may lapse

suddenly or die out gradually: the course of experience in the latter case is

obscure.36

Even with a soundless metronome, their experiment found a subject’s metronomic

performance changed mental processes and sensations through repetitive training. The

automatical experiment caused a two-pronged effect: while subjects forgot the meaning

33 M. F. Bassett and C. J. Warne, “On the Lapse of Verbal Meaning with Repetition,” The

American Journal of Psychology 30 (Oct., 1919): 415.

34

Ibid., 415.

35

Ibid., 417.

36

Ibid., 418.

Page 369: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

348

of words, they gained comfort, a “familiar feel,” in the habit of metronomic repetition, a

sensation similarly recorded in previous metronomic experiments. These researchers

confirmed the process of metronomic habituation in their subject’s performance: natural

(common or previous) mental associations disappeared with the routine of monotonous,

mechanical practice. The subjects, who were “passively” trained to utter sounds without

reflection, came to parrot the words they spoke. In the late nineteenth century,

Ebbinghaus was perhaps the first to recognize the effect caused by his seminal word-

repetition experiments, which seemingly transformed subjects into “sheer mechanical

associators” that lost verbal understanding through repetitive training defined by the

clockwork metronome.

The Metronome and Pavlovian Response

By the 1920s, Wundt’s experimental methods and apparatuses had expanded into the

fledgling science of behavioral psychology, which also researched the phenomena of

habituation and conditioned response, now in less willing subjects. Here, as in other

scientific venues, the metronome continued its essential chronographic role during the

testing and training of animals, most notably sheep and dogs. In addition, the metronome

provided the standard means of distraction, stimulation, and habituation for behavioral

psychologists’ animal research.

In 1923, behavioral psychologist G. V. Anrep described the metronome as one

common apparatus in providing a “strong ‘extra stimulus’” for conditioned-response

training of dogs.37

Likewise, a 1926 American Journal of Psychology article, “A

37 G. V. Anrep, “The Irradiation of Conditioned Reflexes,” Proceedings of the Royal Society of

London. Series B, Containing Papers of a Biological Character 94 (April 3, 1923): 416.

Page 370: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

349

Laboratory for the Study of Conditioned Motor Reflexes,” by Cornell University

Psychologist H. S. Liddell illustrated an elaborate (and soundproof) “animal room” for

electro-shock experiments gauged by scientific metronome. Liddell describes that,

besides its typical use in chronographic research and training,

The metronome provides a convenient conditioned stimulus…the metronome is

placed behind the animal and can be started and stopped by the experimenter from

the adjoining room by means of a rod passing through the wall, bearing at one end

a short bar and at the other end the knife of a single pole switch. When the switch

is closed the bar releases the metronome pendulum and at the same time an

electric circuit is closed through a signal magnet which traces the metronome

beats on the smoked paper of the kymograph.38

Fig. 5.1. Liddell’s soundproof “Animal Room” for “the Study of Conditioned Motor Reflexes.” The

metronome, labeled as “C”, appears to the right of the bound sheep and is electrically connected to the

chronographic recording device in the adjoining room. From H. S. Liddell, “A Laboratory for the Study of

Conditioned Motor Reflexes,” The American Journal of Psychology 37 (Jul., 1926): insert.

38 H. S. Liddell, “A Laboratory for the Study of Conditioned Motor Reflexes,” The American

Journal of Psychology 37 (Jul., 1926): 419.

Page 371: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

350

According to this researcher, Pavlov had already devised animal rooms containing similar

apparatuses and methods. On May 10, 1928, Pavlov lectured at the Royal Society of

London, describing the tools and techniques he had been using for years in conditioned-

response experiments. During his talk, he considered the permanent effects of external

stimuli on the cerebral cortex of dogs, commenting:

In my recent published lectures an observation was mentioned showing that it is

possible to derange a point pertaining to a separate conditioned stimulus, namely,

the sound of a metronome, leaving points corresponding to other auditory stimuli

undamaged.39

Pavlov noted that, even when he conditioned the dog to negatively react against the

metronome, other sounds were unaffected: “Reflexes to other auditory stimuli, such as

buzzing, hissing or bubbling sounds, remained normal.” 40

Through these experiments, he

realized that such metronomic conditioning could cause permanent psychological effects,

and “by mere repetition of a conditioned stimulus for a prolonged period it is possible to

render the cortical point more or less permanently inhibited. For instance, on repeating an

auditory conditioned stimulus day after day many times in each experiment, it finally

became null and void, a condition which lasted for some time.”41

As Pavlov found, the

metronome had the ability to significantly damage or alter the animal psyche.

Pavlov’s work influenced other behavioral psychologists in following decades, as

researchers continued to employ the metronome in conditioned-response experiments.

39 I. P. Pavlov, “Croonian Lecture: Certain Problems in the Psychology of the Cerebral

Hemispheres,” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Containing Papers of a

Biological Character 103 (June 1, 1928): 102.

40

Ibid.

41

Ibid., 103.

Page 372: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

351

Roger Brown Loucks’ American Journal of Psychology article “An Automatic Technique

for Establishing Conditioned Reflexes” (1932) verified that, “It is frequently desirable in

conditioned reflex investigations to utilize rhythmical stimuli, e.g. a flashing light, an

intermittent tactual prick, or a metronome beat.”42

He summarized Pavlov’s methodology

with the standard scientific knowledge that continual, repetitive exposure to one type of

stimulus—especially the metronome—caused mental and physical detriment to the

subject (the dog):

Furthermore, considerable flexibility is exercised in the routine employed with

one and the same dog. For example, on one day 4 stimuli or trials may be given,

the next day, 12; on one day a flashing light, a bell, and a metronome may be

administered…Pavlov has offered considerable evidence to show that by the

constant repetition of one stimulus a dog may be ruined for further work.43

As Loucks suggested, Pavlov’s research methodology accounted for the relatively

humane treatment of his dogs, which the scientist considered as individuals who

displayed variable character traits.44

These behavioral psychologists seemed to realize

that overexposure to metronome—which initially functioned either to stimulate or

distract—eventually caused psychological damage to his animal subjects. Ironically it

seems, as Pavlov and other behavioralists recognized the metronome’s potential for

psychological damage, most music teachers championing metronomic training in their

42 Roger Brown Loucks, “An Automatic Technique for Establishing Conditioned Reflexes,” The

American Journal of Psychology 44 (Apr., 1932): 338.

43

Ibid., 342.

44

Ibid. “General Procedure. Mechanization of the training involves certain basic principles

which may be briefly considered. In Russian studies of conditioned reflexes, there has been a

marked tendency to emphasize the individuality of each dog. Pavlov looks upon his animals as

exhibiting definite ‘temperaments.’ Accordingly, the training procedure to be used with one dog

differs from that most adequate for another.”

Page 373: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

352

modern pedagogies did not readily consider the implications of such mechanical stimulus

upon their young students, or upon themselves.

Metronomic Influences Known in Modern Society

By the second decade of the twentieth century, the general public had certainly gained

greater awareness of the metronomic effect on the human psyche. On February 18, 1914

a whimsical New York Times article “The Dormant Waker” described the ability of an

“unnamed” Harvard professor to enter into a “twilight state” of hypnosis. The writer

reported on one medically recognized way the professor might have achieved such a

trance:

The patient or subject is asked to fixate his attention on some object, while at the

same time listening to the beats of a metronome; the patient’s eyes are then

closed, he is to keep very quiet, while the metronome or some other monotonous

stimulus is continued. After some time…the patient is in a hypnoidal state

favorable for the emergence of subconscious experiences.

In this state of hypnosis, the article continues, the patient is “characterized by a trait of

suggestibility.” 45

The subject, therefore, could then be brainwashed when exposed to the

sound of the metronome. Yet, in a similar experimental procedure, psychologist James

Burt Miner found the metronome to be a disturbing influence upon his hypnotized

subject, recounting in his text, Motor, Visual and Applied Rhythms (1903), “When J was

sound asleep she showed the usual hypnotic condition by accepting simple commands

like, 'you cannot bend your arm,' etc. I then started the metronome and merely told her to

listen to it. Her hand, resting on the table, now seemed to be giving spasmodic jerks.

These were, however, quite irregular.”46

45 “The Dormant Waker,” New York Times, Feb. 18, 1913, 12.

46

Miner, 28.

Page 374: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

353

While metronomic hypnosis may not have been a common practice outside of

university professors’ offices, laboratories, or magic shows, writers by the last quarter of

the nineteenth century—an age many historians consider the second wave of the

Industrial Revolution—had intuited the ill-effects of automatical machines on the average

mind in daily life. The American neurologist George Beard (1839-1883) realized that

mechanical precisions created a new tension in urbanized society. His oft-quoted book

American Nervousness Its Causes and Consequences (1881) portrayed the mechanically

effected man, who—well outside of the laboratory—now conformed daily to increasingly

precise and prevalent time-tellers, along with the timetables that invariably corresponded

to the industrial workday and transportation. Beard pinpointed the specific technologies

and technological values that seemed to distract, stimulate, and habituate the modern

citizen’s psyche:

Clocks and Watches. — Necessity of Punctuality.

The perfection of clocks and the invention of watches have something to do with

modern nervousness, since they compel us to be on time, and excite the habit of

looking to see the exact moment, so as not to be late for trains or

appointments…A nervous man cannot take out his watch and look at it when the

time for an appointment or train is near, without affecting his pulse, and the effect

on that pulse, if we could but measure and weigh it, would be found to be

correlated to a loss to the nervous system. Punctuality is a greater thief of nervous

force than is procrastination of time. We are under constant strain, mostly

unconscious, oftentimes in sleeping as well as in waking hours, to get somewhere

or do something at some definite moment.47

Beard did not discuss the effects of the clockwork metronome on the human mind or

pulse for obvious reasons: While American-made pocket watch sales boomed during the

time of his publication, the foreign-made Maelzel metronome had yet to become a

standard device in American students’ music education. The mechanical time keeping of

47

George M. Beard, American Nervousness its Causes and Consequences (New York: G. P.

Putnam’s Sons, 1881), 103-4.

Page 375: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

354

music lagged far behind the time keeping of urban life and industrial travel. Nevertheless,

by the turn of the century—once the metronome became a standard apparatus in both the

musical and psychological training of precise “rhythmic” action—musicians and

researches had noted that the machine elicited the very same effects that Beard described

decades prior in the use of pocket watches, effects including the rise of pulse and

breathing rates and other unconscious tendencies. Alongside trains and factory

machinery, the metronome came to exemplify the rapid, artificial pace of a modern

world; as critics suggested—and psychologists confirmed— the metronome was yet

another invention that, through its incessant artificiality, either altered or damaged the

abilities of individuals to think, feel, and move.

For experimental psychologist E. W. Scripture, the faster pace of human life, as

promoted through his scientific methods, represented a significant benefit for the

industrial world. As he intended many of his “rhythmic” studies to show, faster machines

facilitated people to think and act with greater rapidity and efficiency—while reducing

the “mean variation” of individual action. By 1895, Scripture had described the

successful scientific methods that created the modern mind—citing effects that Beard,

more than a decade prior, found to be neurologically destructive:

To-day the mental processes of the mass of the people go at a much more rapid

rate than they did a few centuries ago. The mind has been educated by our whole

civilization to act more rapidly. The difference between the sluggish Englishman

of mediaeval times and the quick Yankee of to-day is delightfully told in Mark

Twain’s “King Arthur.” If it were possible to take a man of two centuries ago and

bring him into the laboratory, the results obtained from experiments upon him

would be entirely different from those obtained from one of the students of to-

day. The reactions of the student would be much more rapid, especially the

complicated ones.48

48 E. W. Scripture, “Some Principles of Mental Education,” The School Review 3 (Nov., 1895):

544.

Page 376: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

355

As Scripture suggests, modern man has benefited greatly from rapid “rhythmic” rates,

automatic machines, and chronographic training practices, such as those found in his

Yale laboratory.

Not everyone was as enthused by the increasing rapidity of human thought and

existence in the early twentieth century, a Machine Age fueled by scientific values and

industrial progress. Two generations after American Nervousness first appeared, Walter

Lippmann expressed nearly identical concerns as George Beard over the modern,

mechanically affected mind in his book Public Opinion (1922). Realizing the essential

role the metronome played in laboratory research, Lipmann invoked Maelzel’s machine

to expose greater problems in the industrial world and the increasingly mechanized

reality of modern living:

If the comparatively simple conditions of a laboratory can so readily flatten out

discrimination, what must be the effect of city life? In the laboratory the fatigue is

slight enough, the distraction rather trivial. Both are balanced in measure by the

subject's interest and self-consciousness. Yet if the beat of a metronome will

depress intelligence, what do eight or twelve hours of noise, odor, and heat in a

factory, or day upon day among chattering typewriters and telephone bells and

slamming doors, do to the political judgments formed on the basis of newspapers

read in street-cars and subways? 49

Scientists found that the clockwork metronome influenced subjects in many ways, and

Lippmann, while acknowledging their reductive laboratory methods, realized the

extended implications of psychological research: metronomic distractions now appeared

everywhere in the modern cityscape, thus it seemed only logical that these omnipresent,

artificial sounds altered the thoughts and feelings of the entire industrialized populous.

By 1910, Edward Baxter Perry, too, had recognized that modern machines influenced

49 Walter Lippmann, Public Opinion (New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company, 1922), 72.

Page 377: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

356

even the musical mind. In Stories of Standard Teaching Pieces he expressed his disdain

for the nerve-racking pace of many piano performances, suggesting that musicians’

repetitive day-to-day exposure to automatical machines—either as stimulators or

distracters—effaced the more leisurely, lyrical, and personal interpretations heard in past

performance practices. Perry essentially confirms Scripture’s 1895 assertion of the

increasing mechanical speed of human response and action, as he chides:

Have we no time or taste for anything but hurry up music, because we travel by

express train and do business by wire? Must we also have our music ground out,

machine-like, on high-speed gearing and served against time, like hash at a depot

lunch counter? What wonder that our people have musical indigestion!50

As we read both Lippmann’s and Perry’s very similar line of questioning, as well as the

findings of experimental psychologists that spanned over three decades, further inquiries

regarding the clockwork metronome’s effect upon musicality should logically surface,

including the following: Was Maelzel’s device a culprit in a new musical nervousness, a

new “machine-like” performance practice? Was the metronome a habitual influence upon

the minds of modern musicians, driving them to subconsciously “hurry up” without

forethought or reflection, informing their performances with the rapid “rhythm” of

mechanical clicks, rather than the subjective rhythm of poetic meter, the variable

movement of rhetorical speech, or the emotional sway of their own internal pulse?

According to many skilled musicians, educators, and perceptive critics over the course of

the nineteenth and early twentieth centuries, the resounding answer to each of these

questions was a definitive “yes.”

By 1924, poet Ezra Pound (1885-1972), writing under the pseudonym William

50 Edward Baxter Perry, Stories of Standard Teaching Pieces (Philadelphia: Theodore Presser

Co., 1910), 43-44.

Page 378: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

357

Atheling, a prominent Parisian music critic, had also heard the metronome actively

changing the nature of musical time in living performances. Indeed, he believed

Maelzel’s machine had noticeably effaced the once variable and expressive qualities of

musical time itself. In his guise as the musical traditionalist Atheling, Pound perceived

that,

After a century of trained orchestral performers, and of the present system of

training, we find “musicians” who are solely sensitive to size. Their ability to

count, their metronomic ability, has engulphed them, and they have become

insensitive to shape.51

Pound presumed that all professional musicians embraced the metronome as early as the

famed automata showman Maelzel had introduced it, and that skilled performers

habituated to its click for nearly a century. He argued that those performing

“musicians”—his quotes distinguish them from creative musical “artists”—who slavishly

trained in metronomic time became desensitized to nuanced and expressive techniques in

the process. Pound seemed particularly aware of the metronome’s ability to habituate the

modern musician to mechanized “rhythm.” He heard “musicians” performing as if trained

to utter sounds without any meaning; without reflection or personal choice, Pound’s

metronomic “musicians” recall the “sheer mechanical associators” of Ebbinghaus’ word-

repetition experiments.

Similar to Beard, Scripture, and Lippman before him, Pound also perceived that

modern life, increasingly inundated with automatical machines, continued to alter the

mind towards greater values of artificial, metronomic accuracy. In The New Criterion

(1926), Pound intuited that these scientific values, alongside the chronographic bias,

51 Ezra Pound, Antheil and the Treatise on Harmony (Reprint, New York: Da Capo Press, 1968),

132.

Page 379: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

358

informed the performances and compositions of his age; that the ever-increasing,

precision-oriented machines of the twentieth century seemingly ingrained performers’

with new standards of rhythm and movement:

The mind, even the musician’s mind, is conditioned by contemporary things, our

minimum, in a time when the old atom is “bombarded” by electricity, when

chemical atoms and elements are more strictly considered, is no longer the

minimum of sixteenth century pre-chemists. Both this composer and this

executant [George Antheil and Olga Rudge]…have acquired—perhaps only half

consciously—a new precision.52

As Pound keenly recognized, “exactitude” and “precision” are values that develop over

the ages through the culturally specific machines and methods employed to define those

vales. Perhaps aware of the numerous critical and scientific sources documenting the

effects of automatical machines upon the individual, the modernist poet’s recognition was

fundamentally correct: mechanically regulated action—first actualized in the nineteenth-

century sciences—had continued to alter human thought, action, and “even the

musician’s mind” itself.53

Yet, unbeknownst to Pound, the habitual, metronomic

influence he heard in contemporary orchestra and chamber performances was a relatively

recent phenomenon, a distinct performance practice of modernity established only within

52 Pound, 148-9.

53

By 1927 music critic Paul Rosenfeld had similarly described this habitual shift in modern,

musical mentality: “It can almost be said that the ideal unconsciously animating the best

musicians of to-day is the expression of positive, decided feeling in a very impersonal, reserved,

and stone-like form…Just what the causes of these almost universal changes of feeling so

decisive for the forms of art are, remains, it must again be confessed, pretty definitely hidden

from us.” See Paul Rosenfeld, Modern Tendencies in Music (New York: The Caxton Institute,

1927), 37.

Perhaps confirming Pound’s assessment, George Antheil explained his practice regimen

during the 1920s as a habitual process, calculated to achieve musical-mechanical precision: “You

keep on. You must never stop. And so technique comes to you. It begins to fit like a suit. You

play concert pieces so many times that you could hardly play a wrong note in them if you wanted

to. The hind part of the brain takes over.” See George Antheil, Bad Boy of Music (Reprint,

Hollywood: Samuel French, 1990), 68.

Page 380: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

359

his lifetime. Indeed, sources suggest that musicians did not take a century to habituate to

metronomic action, but just over a generation.

As the next chapter explores, roughly three decades prior to Pound’s 1924

assertion that musicians habituated to the metronome en masse, young students first

began comprehensively training under the values of precise mechanical rhythm and the

constant regulation of clockwork time—in direct correspondence with the rise of the new,

scientific music education. Alongside a host of other pedagogies influenced by

experimental psychology and its chronographic bias, modern music training employed

the clockwork metronome to engender seemingly automatical qualities of action in the

student, which paralleled other performance-procedures found in the progressive fields of

science and industry. By the mid-1890s, in what can be considered a dramatic paradigm

shift in education, these scientific teachers had considered automatical responses and

habitual actions—invariably dictated by metronomic “rhythm” and chronographic

methods—as prized qualities in the study and training of the modern child. By the turn of

the century, while experimental psychologists conclusively documented the mind-altering

effects of the metronome on laboratory subjects, metronomic habituation became a

predominant technique in teaching a new generation of students throughout Western

society in the modern values of “precise” and objective rhythmic action.

Page 381: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

360

CHAPTER VI: THE METRONOMIC EDUCATION

In order for consumers to embrace recently emergent technologies—such as simple

pendulums, clockwork metronomes, or quartz wristwatches—inventors, educators, and

other proponents must develop instructional methods for these inventions. Despite the

potential value and convenience behind new inventions, machines have no inherent

meaning or purpose until a community of users imparts meaning and importance into

them in a standardized fashion. People must become educated in the utility and

importance of new tools for these devices to form a part of practical life. We are taught—

either directly through instruction or indirectly through long-standing social

conventions—that hammers are useful for building chairs, not opening cans; that

vacuums clear floors, not cut grass. We are taught how to write with a pen, to play the

violin, to ride a bike, to use a mobile phone. Indeed, the child (not to mention the

untrained adult) does not enter the world with a priori understandings of the values of

tools or how they should ideally function. Machines are only important if and when

society instills importance in them through formal or informal educational training, which

over time instills an assumed and communally accepted tradition of use.

A tradition of use for any given invention can even transcend the creator’s

original conceptions. While inventors often introduce machines for specific and limited

purposes, over time, a community’s ever-changing needs and desires might significantly

alter or adapt these technologies in ways not originally envisioned by their inventors.1

Such adaptations are clearly witnessed in the use of the clockwork metronome. The

1 For example, the Internet—originally intended for specialized scientists to exchange research

data quickly and efficiently, and which has become a primary avenue of global commerce,

communication, and entertainment—is perhaps the best example today of a technological

divergence from an original purpose.

Page 382: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

361

preceding chapters revealed that different communities placed different values on an

identical invention. Throughout the nineteenth century, so many skilled performers and

composers found the artificial clockwork metronome—regardless of its mechanical

“accuracy”—entirely meaningless as it related to their still-living traditions of musicality,

while, in the very same century, experimental scientists seemed to make the machine

their own with no qualms whatsoever, even improving its precision for their own

purposes. The metronome, with its invariable clockwork rhythm, practically suggested

itself as the most useful and necessary apparatus in the mechanical laboratory for

“experimental” scientists throughout the Western world. While skilled musicians did not

find the metronome a priori important and purposeful as a temporal dictator—since a

simple pendulum could always suffice for an initial pulse reference—nineteenth-century

physiologists and psychologists were truly the first to collectively discover and champion

the lasting meaning and utility hidden within Maelzel’s clockwork metronome as the

constant chronographic reference and regulator for their clinical experiments.

As past chapters have documented, the clockwork metronome and metronomic

indications were neither intuitive nor sufficient when applied to music notation and living

performances. Maelzel, keenly aware that systematic education in his new machine was

required for its adoption, found it essential to provide publishers and composers,

including Beethoven, with detailed instructions on metronomic tempo transcription,

which many musicians ignored, misinterpreted, or reinterpreted with their own intentions.

In a letter dated April 19, 1818, Maelzel even described to Beethoven his seemingly

unsuccessful attempts to teach something new to those handfuls of professional

Page 383: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

362

musicians, amateurs, and publishers first introduced to the clockwork metronome and its

cryptic numerical-temporal scale. Maelzel railed:

There are stupid and lazy people who must be fed the truth with a cooking ladle,

and who do not want to take any, not even the least trouble to learn something—

and there are only too many of these in Paris.2

Over a year earlier, while in London, Maelzel similarly attempted to instruct consumers

at large in the values—the “truth”—of the clockwork metronome and its beats-per-

minute scale through his obscure “Metronic Tutor” (c1817).3 Maelzel’s final attempt to

teach the “stupid and lazy” in the superiority of metronomic tempo dictation and practice

occurred through his “Kurze Abhandlung” (1836).4 As the inventor-promoter recognized,

no potential consumer would buy the metronome if the purposes of the machine remained

vague and trifling. But Maelzel, by himself, did not succeed in his entrepreneurial

pursuits; he failed to inculcate musicians and consumers of his generation with the value

of the clockwork metronome as an aid for music publications and practice. It would take

many more decades before pedagogical communities accepted the machine as the

temporal standard of music training, publication, and performance in the Western

2 Theodore Albrecht, ed., Letters to Beethoven and Other Correspondence, Volume 2 (Lincoln &

London: University of Nebraska Press, 1996), 137, Letter 248. Albrecht, in footnotes to this

letter, provides useful source information on Maelzel’s various metronome-chart iterations that

the inventor distributed during this time.

3 [Classified Section,] Times, Feb. 19, 1817, 1. In a self-aggrandizing—and highly fictitious—

manner perhaps expected from the celebrated automata entertainer, Maelzel’s London metronome

advertisement read: “METRONOME, or Musical Time-keeper.—Mr. MAELZEL respectfully

informs the Musical Public, that he is returned from the Continent, where his endeavors to

establish universally the standard measure for musical time, indicated by the Metronome have

met with complete success. Metronomes in any quantity, and at various prices, may now be had

at his manufactory, 50, Berwick-street, Soho, and at the principal music shops: likewise his

Metronic Tutor, being a book of rudiments for the pianoforte, calculated, with the assistance of

the Metronome, to render the pupil a steady timeist, price 7s, 6d.”

4 Johann Nepomuk Maelzel, Kurze Abhandlung uber den Metronomen von Maezel und dessen

Anwendung als Tempobezeichnung, sowohl als bei dem Unterricht in der Musik (Mainz, 1836).

Page 384: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

363

tradition. Indeed, the utility of the metronome and the importance of metronomic

“rhythm” had never come to fruition in nineteenth-century musical culture or aesthetics

as it had in the experimental sciences; past creative “artists” never trained under the belief

that the automatical swing or click of the metronome overrode their internal sense and

personal expression of musical time. As strongly suggested by compositions,

performances, and the musical discourse spanning the nineteenth century, musical

“artists’” living epistemology of time was completely distanced from the external clicks

of mere clockwork.

The gradual establishment of a tradition of use for technologies depends upon

developing social, historical, and educational contexts, and this cultural phenomenon is

especially apparent in the history of automatic timekeepers and time tellers, Maelzel’s

metronome included. Consider that most individuals in modern civilization, those who

learned to read precise (or even unmarked) watch-faces, multi-function clock hands, and

synchronized digital displays down to the millisecond, find sundials—the most prevalent

and long-standing time-telling technologies in history—valueless or even indecipherable.

Sundials are not “bad” time tellers when compared to wristwatches or mobile phones;

they are vastly different than wristwatches or mobile phones, displaying a different value

system—a past epistemology of time not accounted for in the modern world. People

seldom look at sundials, even when available, because most find these objects of no

importance in light of more modern and “precise” mechanical technologies. Indeed,

while most moderns know how to set a digital watch or program a cell phone, few are

educated in reading or installing a sundial, a device that acts more like a trite sculpture

today than a time teller. The tradition of use for sundials—along with other non-

Page 385: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

364

automatical time-telling tools and techniques—has definitively passed. One New York

Times reviewer in 1920 summarized his contemporary culture’s very recent embrace of

automatic (metronomic), precise, and globally synchronous time telling, acknowledging:

Modern life is run on a timetable. The stop watch is perhaps the most typical

modern invention, because it symbolizes the infinite subdivision of time that has

come in since the industrial revolution…Really precise time telling has not been

needed in the world until within a few years…It would be hard to run the

Twentieth Century on a sun dial.5

It would be equally preposterous to suggest that the medieval world could run on

stopwatches.6 Just as many learned to operate stopwatches, take photographic portraits, or

ride bicycles in the late nineteenth century7—an age when “really precise” mechanical

time-telling came to fruition for industrialized society—new generations in Western

civilization became trained to use, value, and believe in the “truth” of Maelzel’s

clockwork metronome, now outside of high-technology science laboratories, as the

prevailing and eventually sole temporal reference for musical education and subsequent

performances. This chapter shows how the emerging applications of the clockwork

5

“Time and Clocks,” New York Times, June 27, 1920, 60, ProQuest Historical Newspapers. As I

argue, the metronome perhaps more than the stopwatch was the modern, industrial timekeeper

par excellence. Indeed, the same book reviewer described pocket watches as “solar metronomes.”

6

To further exemplify the contextual values of machines in society, I could employ what Einstein

would have considered a “thought experiment:” a twenty-first-century laptop computer would

certainly seem pointless to an isolated nineteenth-century agrarian township that spent the day

tilling or irrigating land without electricity. For technologically underdeveloped societies, a

laptop computer would not be a priori useful, important, or even interesting in the way our

contemporary Western society finds the machine to be; I could conjecture that, when put in this

anachronistic context, the laptop computer would be best employed somehow as a farming

implement. Indeed, the personal computers of today had no place—conceptually or physically—

in civilizations of the distant past, just as the untold inventions of the next century are both

unavailable and unfathomable to us in the present.

7 See Stephen Kern, The Culture of Time and Space, 1880-1918, Second Edition (Cambridge:

Harvard University Press, 2003) for a history detailing the influence of emergent technologies,

notably the bicycle and the photographic camera, within late nineteenth-century culture.

Page 386: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

365

metronome in public education had little to do with the past “artistic” traditions of music

creation or interpretation alluded to by so many musicians and critics over the nineteenth

century.

The metronome, by the late nineteenth-century, had some profoundly invasive

applications in public education when incorporated in novel experimental pedagogies,

which promoted the new paradigm of “rhythm” first employed by nineteenth-century

laboratory scientists. And, while the chronographic method effaced personal rhythm in

the laboratory, pedagogues’ application of continual and precise metronomic time

eventually eroded and replaced the subjective values of pulse, meter, rhetoric, and

interpretive movement in modern music training. Thus, while the invention of the

metronome occurred early in the nineteenth century, the systematic use and socio-cultural

importance of Maelzel’s metronome only coalesced through new pedagogies in the late

nineteenth century that prescribed mechanically “rhythmic” timekeeping for the efficient,

standardized, orderly, replicable—and ultimately “scientific”—training of human action.

This chapter charts a now-familiar technology—the ticking, clockwork metronome—as it

drastically transitioned in importance and utility for Western society; through new

“scientific” values of “rhythmic” training, the clockwork metronome—once an anathema

within the past traditions of artistic performance—transformed into the unquestioned rule

of time and action, creating both a new cultural tradition of use and a new performance

practice for modern industrial civilization.

Page 387: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

366

Prior to the Scientific Pedagogies, Military and Gymnastic Methods

By the third quarter of the nineteenth century, while both physiologists and the new

experimental psychologists employed the often-modified metronome for chronographic

research in their laboratories, certain educators had begun using the clockwork device in

new ways for more efficient, regimented education. Prior to experimental psychologists’

direct influence on modern pedagogies, teachers often associated the rigid techniques of

metronomic training with military paradigms. One unique example published in 1876, A

Trip to Music-Land, A Fairy Tale by Emma L. Shedlock, offered a creative means of

music instruction for young British schoolchildren, in which the clockwork metronome

played a prominent role as drillmaster of musical time. The author makes clear that her

fairy tale should not be taken to explain the complexities of music to “Artists and

Professionals.” Rather, her intention was to “obviate this ‘drudgery’” of teaching “the

rudiments of Music” in standard music classes through the use of an imaginative (albeit

lengthy and intricate) story, explanatory images, and entertaining objects.8 She concluded

her preface with her hope that, “If by these means I have rendered teaching the Elements

of Music any pleasanter at once to teacher and to pupil, the sole object of a Trip to Music-

Land will have been fully attained.”9

For restless young schoolchildren, her musical novel may have seemed an

entertaining multi-media spectacle. The teacher would perform a show-and-tell, reading

the fairy tale and explaining each element of music notation as it appeared through the

course of the narrative, all while displaying high-quality cartoons by J. King James

8 Emma L. Shedlock, A Trip to Music-land (London: Blackie & Son, Paternoster Buildings,

1876), vii.

9 Shedlock, viii.

Page 388: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

367

within the book (in addition to the music paraphernalia, which included the clockwork

metronome, purchased separately through the publisher or another source).10

Shedlock’s

tale either objectified or personified each detail of music composition and notation: King

Harmony had the body of a Cello, Time (the Prime Minister) was a hybrid creature, a

man “that nature (Music-Land nature of course) had endowed…with a large [audibly

ticking] clock in the very center of his chest.”11

Other characters included Dr. Rest,

Queen Melody, Rhythm “(Time’s Father),”12

and so on. All printed notes, regardless of

their value, were soldiers in the Music-Land Army, and each was depicted with a human

frame underneath a printed note value as their head. In Chapter VII of the fairy tale, the

Metronome character appears as a pivotal figure in the story; he is an Austrian general

who arrived in Music-Land (an island nation more reminiscent of Venice than England)

to drill the troops, creating order in Time, thus bringing new prosperity and success to the

kingdom.

10

The back-page advertisement for A. N. Myers & Co. products include the “Music-Land Bricks”

and “Dr. Rest’s Cannons” needed to tell Shedlock’s story. While Myers did not produce Maelzel

metronomes, in a final statement, their ad suggests how teachers might obtain a clockwork

device: “Lists of the Separate Kindergarten Amusements and Occupations may be obtained on

application.” See advertising supplement to Shedlock, A Trip to Music-Land, [4].

11

Shedlock, 8. Shedlock immediately identifies, or at least allegorizes, the foundations of musical

time through the most available and precise societal time-teller of her day, the two-handed clock.

Moreover, in her children’s tale, she associates the values of a minim, crotchet, and quaver with

proportional fractions of Time’s clock-face (in seconds). Through the two-handed clock and

Maelzel’s intentions, she helped redefine musical notation with more precise, chronographic

intentions. See Shedlock, 11.

12

Rhythm first appears in Shedlock, 20.

Page 389: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

368

Fig. 6.1. Detail of the “Landing of Metronome and Sub-division” in Music-land. Included in the

landing party are General Metronome’s attentive note-soldiers. This is the first of many appearances of the

metronome character in the storyline drawings by J. King James. From Emma L. Shedlock, A Trip to

Music-land (London: Blackie & Son, Paternoster Buildings, 1876), insert after 36.

To facilitate the new musical-military organization of Music-Land, Metronome’s

assistant Subdivision, a rather stout military drum with a sword, whom Shedlock

describes as a “dwarf,” violently chopped the notes of Music-Land into smaller rhythmic

soldiers.13

These note-soldiers formed the improved, orderly Music-Land Army.

The numerous depictions of General Metronome expose an automaton in keeping

with Maelzel’s culture. Metronome’s appendages and head seemed human, yet his heart

housed only clockwork. In Chapter VIII of the tale, General Metronome displays the

innards of his torso to the court and King Harmony:

Then at a touch of his finger, a pendulum inside began to rock steadily backwards

and forwards, with sound indeed loud enough to be heard by the Music-Land

army, while an invisible bell ringing clearly to every first note, distinctly marked

two, three, and four time.14

13

See Shedlock, 36-37 for the first appearance of Subdivision and General Metronome.

14

Shedlock, 44.

Page 390: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

369

Shedlock’s metronome-as-military general is an appropriate allegory for the uses of the

machine in nineteenth-century children’s pedagogy prior to experimental psychologists’

wide-scale influence on middle-class teaching methods in the 1890s. (Her publication

appeared only six years after Great Britain established a compulsory, national system of

elementary public education.)15

Indeed, Shedlock’s battlefield metronome had little in

common with artists’ conceptions of living performance—individual expression,

spontaneous creation, or nuanced interpretation formed no part of the mechanical drilling

of Music-Land troops. Shedlock’s General Metronome even exclaims his true hegemonic

intention to the King:

“I do no teaching, not I” said Metronome, again laughing, “I stand on the battle-

field and give my men the rate at which they are to go. Semibreves, Minims,

Crotchets, and Quavers soon learn to march to my one, two and three!”16

15

While the British 1870 education act was intended to be compulsory, historians such as Eric

Hobsbawm note, “It did not become effectively compulsory until 1891.” See Eric Hobsbawm,

Industry and Empire (Reprint, New York: The New Press, 1999), 134.

16

The King in her tale alludes to the fact that before Metronome’s arrival, each composition—

“each battle”—required new instructions as to the time the army of notes needed to move. See

Shedlock, 44.

Page 391: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

370

Fig. 6.2. Detail of General Metronome showing himself, and his clockwork functions, to King

Harmony and the Court of Music-land. Outside, the Music-land note-army wages a battle. From

Shedlock, A Trip to Music-land (1876), insert after 46.

Considering that her highly creative fable merely exemplified the basics of music

notation for kindergarteners, the children listening to her entertaining text did not learn to

physically perform music to the click of the metronome—the machine did not actively

guide their living actions as it did for subjects in Wundt’s laboratory (or the fictional

Music-Land army). Instead, Shedlock employed the imaginary vision of the clockwork

man-machine as an innocuous and superficial entertainment, a supplement to her fable-

lesson, and a way to make the complex and ineffable qualities of musical time substantial

to kindergarteners through General Metronome, the gregarious cartoon-automaton.17

17

While all of the intricate details of her tale cannot be explored in the present survey, it is

significant to note that the narrator, before arriving in the imaginary Music-Land, was practicing

“five-finger exercises” when a fairy appeared by her metronome—which she does not specify

was moving at the time. The fairy reinterpreted the music of the “Instruction Book” on the

Page 392: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

371

The metronome’s place in culture, however, began to uncannily mirror

Shedlock’s tale outside of the kindergarten classroom. Only three years before this

kindergarten publication, an English article “‘Company’ Playing” relates how the new

machine could help define and dictate musical-performance goals for an older set of

amateur Victorian music makers. The author was highly critical of the “inferiority of the

average piano-forte heard in society and in the family circle, and the poor return obtained,

for the time and money spent in the teaching girls music, or rather what is called such”

and offers a metronomic method intended to improve performance: “Supposing that you

practise with a metronome, which is a capital thing if you can get one; try to catch up the

time you lost in your rallentando by an accelerando in the second bar so as again to start

your third bar with the first beat or bell of the metronome.” 18

While the above article strongly implies that the metronome was by no means

universally used by musical amateurs in England well through the 1870’s—and was

never to prevent a necessary, a priori temporal flexibility—this new machine indeed

arrived during the nineteenth century to teach a host of students how to “march to the

one, two, three” of a clockwork click for activities completely unrelated to “artistic”

music performance. According to the first edition of Grove’s Dictionary (1880), military

bands (and conceivably military troops) could purchase “a very large and loud

Metronome…by Messrs. Rudall & Carte of London” intended for mass, regimented

narrator’s piano as a regimented army, proclaiming, “Why here are pictures of Music-Land, and

the soldiers marching along to battle in their different regiments, and you call then ‘pieces of

music,’ ‘notes,’ and ‘bars.’” Thus from the very outset of the fairy tale, Shedlock allegorizes all

of music notation, for the sake of her elementary educational methods, through a fundamentally

militaristic, not “artistic” paradigm. See Shedlock, 1-2.

18

See S. R., “‘Company’ Playing,” The Monthly Packet of Evening Readings for Members of the

English Church XVI (July-December, 1873): 200, 204.

Page 393: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

372

actions in mechanical time.19

Proof that the military species of the metronome existed

well through the twentieth century can be seen in a 1900 report by The United States War

Department, describing the training of troops with a drummer invariably guided by such a

device.20

In 1901, a Canadian government document listed essential equipment for

military troops, including a “military metronome” at a cost of $10.22.21

The highly regimented physical pedagogy of gymnastics, a field that closely allied

with both physiological (often dubbed “hygienic”) and military training methods, seemed

to be one of the first non-“artistic” performance activities to embrace the constant use of

metronomic time.22

(We recall French physiologist Trousseau using the metronome-

gymnastics treatment on his patients in the 1830s). And as early as the 1820s, Colonel

Amoros’ Paris gymnastic school—perhaps the first and most prominent government-

19

George Grove, A Dictionary of Music and Musicians, Volume II (London: Macmillan and Co.,

1880), 320. The reference speculates that such a loud, over-sized machine could be helpful for

ensemble rehearsals of non-military musicians, stating, “an instrument of this kind may often be

used, with great advantage, when a number of vocal or instrumental performers practice

together.”

It cannot be overlooked that Macmillan and Co., the publishers of this music dictionary,

were prominent publishers of physiology texts, both in London and New York, during the

nineteenth century.

20

U.S. War Department, Annual Reports (Washington: Government Printing Office, 1900), 276.

21

Canada Parliament, “Sessional Paper No. 1,” Sessional Papers XXXV (1901): Q-65.

22

None other than Richard Wagner noted the vast distinction between the sensitive, nuanced

actualizations of music tempos and the popular gymnastics regimens of the mid-nineteenth

century. Contending that “our conductors so frequently fail to find the true tempo because they

are ignorant of singing,” Wagner considered such anti-expressive, misguided musicians to be

people who “look upon music as a singularly abstract sort of thing, an amalgam of grammar,

arithmetic, and digital gymnastics;--to be an adept in which may fit a man for a mastership at a

conservatory or musical gymnasium; but it does not follow from this that he will be able to put

life an soul into a musical performance.” See Richard Wagner, On Conducting (Über das

Dirigiren), trans. Edward Dannreuther (London: William Reeves, 1897), 19.

Page 394: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

373

sponsored institution of its kind in modern Europe23

—employed metronomic training

methods for a variety of bodily activities. As reported in the American Journal of

Education (1826), Amoros’ seemingly automatical exercises,

Consist in chanting different pieces, the rhythm of each of which corresponds

with the various movements of the legs, arms, and body, which the pupils execute

on the spot. A metronome regulates these movements. The pupil thus learns to

measure time and space, to regulate with precision the common step, the

accelerated step, and the leaps of the gymnastic course.24

The Colonel’s training regimen for students seems to closely mirror the fictional General

Metronome’s intentions for Music-Land’s army. While nineteenth-century gymnastic

students readily learned to move their limbs by the machine’s clockwork, their training

was not limited to regimented walking and jumping alone. James Madison Watson’s

Hand-Book of Calisthenics and Gymnastics: A Complete Drill-Book for Schools,

Families, and Gymnasiums (1864) is an early American source that suggests the

possibility of metronomic regulation for “gymnastic reading:”

Rhythm may be applied to reading or recitation, in connection with Calisthenics

or Gymnastics, by making the unit of measuring time the same as in music, a beat

of the hand, foot, or metronome, or a motion from any assignable position.25

For Watson, the metronome was one of four methods—and the only externalized,

mechanical method—of performing rhythmically, and his comment suggests that the

23

According to Edouard Foucaud, “Colonel Amoros, a Spanish refugee, was the principal, the

most active, and the most intelligent promoter of gymnastics as connected with the physical

education of children. He founded a fine establishment in Paris (1819)…in which he endeavored

to develop the physical strength and agility of children.” See Edouard Foucaud, The Book of

Illustrious Mechanics of Europe and America, translated from the French by John Frost (New

York: Appelton & Co, 1847), 313.

24 From the London Scientific Gazette, “Intelligence: Col. Amoros’ Gymnastic school Paris,” in

American Journal of Education I (Boston: Wiat, Green, and Co., 1826), 689.

25

J. Madison Watson, Hand-book of Calisthenics and Gymnastics: A Complete Drill-Book for

Schools, Families, and Gymnasiums (New York: Schermerhorn, Bancroft & Co., 1864), 70.

Page 395: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

374

metronome was not always available or desirable for “measuring time” in such gymnastic

or music instruction, where the hand or foot usually sufficed as a time reference, as it had

for centuries. Yet, it is distinctly possible that Watson implied the use of a simple

pendulum and not the clockwork “metronome” in his exercises. Consider that, for “Vocal

Exercises with Calisthenics,” Watson suggests, “the members of the class count

continuously in concert, from one to eight inclusive, at an average rate of ninety in a

minute, which rate may be most readily determined by the use of a metronome. The

instructor gives the words of command, and the students take the required positions and

execute all the movements in exact time marked by the counting.”26

In his instructions,

the metronome predetermined the rate, possibly by its pendulum swing alone, but the

“exact time” was maintained by the students’ counting, not by the metronome clicking.

Many contemporaneous musical and scientific sources suggested that the simple

pendulum was an exceedingly appropriate temporal reference, and Watson, too, may

have considered the visual swing of the pendulum bob a sufficient indicator to start his

gymnastic performances. Indeed, Watson may not have considered the metronome useful

for traditional music performances at all: he provides no metronome marks for the many

musical examples by Verdi, Meyerbeer, Donizetti, and others that accompany his

callisthenic exercises.27

(The physically perceived dance meters of this music, in two-,

three-, or four-time, required no strict clockwork guidance). Moreover, the drill-book

supplies a highly detailed rendering of Watson’s gymnastic studio, where a musician

26

Ibid., 124.

27 See Watson, 149-153; 158-162; 187-190; and 206-209 for music examples.

Page 396: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

375

actively assists during a relay game, known as “The Indian Club Race,” on the spinet

piano, which seems bare of any clockwork metronome.28

Fig. 6.3. Detail of Watson’s gymnastics studio where students prepared to play “The Indian Club

Race.” While Watson states that the pianist merely plays three chords to start the activity, this pianist

clearly has sheet music in front of her and is in the process of performing a work—seemingly without the

regulated assistance of a metronome, an increasingly popular tool for gymnastics exercises. From J.

Madison Watson, Hand-book of Calisthenics and Gymnastics: A Complete Drill-Book for Schools,

Families, and Gymnasiums (New York: Schermerhorn, Bancroft & Co., 1864), insert before 251.

Nevertheless, the pedagogical regimentation offered through the clockwork

metronome, exploited by military bands and soldiers in training, was significantly

valuable in other non-“artistic” fields of education by the next decade. Metronomic

regulation seemed particularly appropriate for some educators, perhaps inspired by earlier

gymnastic methods, in the teaching of handwriting. The Manual of Free-hand

Penmanship (1877) suggested the novel incorporation of a metronome (which cannot be

certified as the clockwork variety), an “instrument, owned by some schools for beating

time in music, [which] can be very pleasantly used for keeping the concert time in

28

For instructions on the gymnastics race, see Watson, 384.

Page 397: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

376

writing, setting it faster or slower as required.”29

The instructors clearly articulate the

controlling purposes of the device in mass, student training:

If we wish to teach a class, they must all write the same thing at the same time:

otherwise the teaching becomes individual, and, with a large class, necessarily

ineffective. Fix this in your mind, then, that the class must keep together. The

rapid must be restrained, the slow urged on, to their mutual advantage. The class,

especially in its earlier stages, must be treated as a machine, and brought into a

unity of working movement. To teach writing successfully, as any thing else,

absolute obedience and exact execution are imperatively needed.30

As these educators maintained, the metronome—which only “some schools” employed

for music training—was both a normalizing and regulative tool that diminished

individuality for the sake of efficient, simplified group instruction. These were the very

same values astronomers and experimental psychologists applied to the chronograph and

metronome when diminishing the personal equation of their “observer”-subjects. The

metronome’s place in the elementary classroom, as in training camps and university

laboratories of the same decade, thus reduced individual expression-in-action—

normalizing children in a mechanical “unity of working movement”—through systematic

exercise.

Adult students, too, relied on the clockwork metronome for non-musical training

prior to the 1890s. In the learning of takigraphy, a rapid note-dictation technique with

condensed spelling rules, the metronome became a coveted tool. In 1886, the periodical

The Shorthand Writer responded to inquiries about the metronome—what it was and

where it could be purchased—confirming that, in North America at least, the still-obscure

clockwork metronome was primarily intended for children’s musical instruction. To learn

29

Alvin R. Dutton, B. Harrison, et al., Manual of Free-hand Penmanship (Boston: J.W.C. Gilman

& Co., 1877), 9.

30

Ibid., 8.

Page 398: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

377

takigraphy, however, a metronome “without the bel is just as good for gauging the

dictation in teaching shorthand…The instrument can be obtaind of almost any large

dealer in musical merchandise, or The Shorthand Writer can furnish them.”31

(The

peculiarities of takigraphic spelling are retained). The metronome’s automatical bell

reflected the once-human sensation of metric pulse in music, and as this comment

suggests, it was unnecessary for writing exercises; the efficient regulation of handwriting

needed no such metrical reference. Mechanically aided writing, alongside quasi-military

educational drilling, continued in public schools throughout the early twentieth century.

In 1892, the American Teacher printed a small article “Writing in the Lower Grades” that

advocates for using the metronome in penmanship exercises. It instructs the student to

“Glide across your paper, forward and back, keep time with the metronome. Ready,

write.”32

Albert Ross Parsons, one of the first and most noted American music pedagogues

promoting a new scientific school of piano instruction, championed the clockwork

metronome as well for his systematic and regulated drilling methods in the mid-1880s. In

utilizing Maelzel’s metronome, Parsons seems directly inspired by the educational

models previously employed in military and gymnastics drilling, and he seemed to be one

of the first piano teachers in North America to specifically prescribe the clockwork

machine—not the simple pendulum—for his novel, normalizing training techniques. In

the aptly titled The Science of Pianoforte Practice (1886), Parsons offered his critique of

a few emerging machines, inspired by physiological apparatuses, intended for “scientific”

31

[“Notices,”] The Shorthand Writer V (Jan., 1886): 14.

32

Lyman D. Smith, “Writing in the Lower Grades,” The American Teacher IX (June, 1892): 393.

Page 399: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

378

or technique-based piano training including, “First, the technicon, as fundamental to

clavier work; secondly, the techniphone, as a special means of clavier work; [and thirdly,]

the metronome, as regulating and governing clavier work.”33

Parsons’ essay suggests that there was no scientific method yet associated with

Maelzel’s machine in music education, and that current pedagogues disagreed

considerably as to its proper employment. Similar to the other apparatuses rapidly

emerging in the expanding middle-class musical marketplace, the clockwork

metronome—often a scientific reference when tuning organ pipes—was still an

unfamiliar and experimental tool in the field of music education.34

The clockwork

metronome had not yet achieved the tradition of use familiar to students and

professionals in the twentieth century.

Of the three machines he discussed, Parsons was most favorable toward Maelzel’s

metronome. In The Science of Pianoforte Practice he introduced the machine to a

readership still unfamiliar with its purpose, and more importantly, he explained how it

should never be employed:

33

Albert Ross Parsons, The Science of Pianoforte Practice (New York: G. Schirmer, 1886), 15.

34

One year after Parsons’ text appeared, G.-A. Hirn’s Construction et Emploi du Métronome en

Musique verified as much in France. Commenting on Saint-Saëns’ essay “The Metronome,” Hirn

recognized that because of the imprecision from one metronome to the next, there currently was

no single correct use of the machine for music publications or practicing, in contrast to more

standardized tuning methods that now used metronomic indications. Even if a standardized

metronome could be made, Hirn argued, extreme precision was not an issue since the machine

should never be used to regulate music performance in any case. See G.-A. Hirn, Construction et

Emploi du Métronome en Musique (Paris: Gauthier-Villars, 1887), 5-6; and Camille Saint-Saëns,

“The Metronome,” in Outspoken Essays on Music, trans. Fred Rothwell (London: Kegan Paul,

Trench, Trubner & Co., 1922): 122-4.

Page 400: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

379

If the metronome suggests only a glance at a certain conventional sign at the

beginning of a piece, and then a mad race through the notes, heedless of

everything save the inexorable tick-tack of the conscienceless machine, then,

confessedly, it is no aid to practice.35

He realized that anyone slavishly attended to automatical “tick-tack” often ignored the

“conventional sign,” (that is, the musical meter) and neglected the sense of metric pulse

vital for meaningful and expressive performance. Moreover, Parsons reiterated the

commonly held belief that musical time was under the lone authority of the skilled

performer, who in no way ought to be influenced by such mindless mechanical action. He

remarks: “A word as to metronomic designations of tempo in pieces…the musician, if he

consults absolute metronomic signs at all, does so chiefly with a view to comparing them

with his own impressions on the subject.”36

While Parsons admitted that the metronome should have no place in the skilled

actualizations of music compositions—and that initial tempo numbers printed for a work

should not be misinterpreted as regulating musical time—he nevertheless prescribed the

machine as a normalizing agent of musical individuality, used to drill typical (non-

“artistic”) students in repetitive technical exercises:

Started at a judiciously moderate tempo, and then set faster and faster by regular

degrees as practice progresses, it enables one to apply himself systematically to

the working out of a given problem, for days or weeks, independent of varying

moods. Without its aid, the tempo of practice varies incredibly from day to day,

nay, even hour to hour, according to the state of the weather, of one's nerves, etc.

Yesterday, perhaps, everything moved on quietly. To-day cloudy skies and a

heavy air cause everything to drag stupidly. To-morrow one's spirits are above

par, and everything fairly spins. But the day after, nervous restlessness induces

injurious hurrying, and an indigestion in the fingers follows, unfitting the hand for

smooth playing for a day or two.

35

Parsons, 21-22. Emphasis added.

36

Ibid., 24.

Page 401: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

380

In contrast to this, judicious practice with the metronome means steadiness

and repose of mind and muscles in work. In relieving the mind of responsibility

for steadiness of tempo, and supplying a graded scale for safely increasing the

speed, the mental strain of prolonged practice is surprisingly lightened.37

Parsons admitted to the living realities of time perception: the sense of time varies with

the subject, with the moment, and with the environment. (Roughly six decades prior,

professional astronomers recorded the ephemeral, non-mechanical nature of time

perception, a concept still voiced by philosopher-psychologists such as William James in

Parsons’ day). Moreover, Parsons considered that tempo variability—during practicing

only—manifested as a personal problem of physiology: nerves, moods, and the

musculature system all contributed to unpredictable human-temporal fluctuations. As he

implied, the metronome could cure these aliments. Indeed, Parsons described a process of

“lightening mental strain” that seems to suggest the effects of habituation; as he related,

the mind is “relived of the responsibility” of thinking about (and perhaps even of feeling)

the musical time through repetitive training under “the inexorable tick-tack of the

conscienceless machine.”

Parsons probably did not consider the extent to which his method of mindless,

practice-room drill-work eventually and subconsciously transferred to performance, or

that metronomic training instilled the subject with mechanical qualities, which critics

intuited and psychologists documented through the remainder of the century. A year after

the appearance of Parson’s text, G.-A. Hirn, in his pedagogical essay Construction et

Emploi du Métronome en Musique (1887), reiterated: “ The metronome is destined, not to

beat the measure [time] (this would be exceptional and serve to mechanize Art, as Weber

feared), but to indicate from the outset, to a director or performer, the average movement

37

Ibid., 22.

Page 402: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

381

desired by the composer,” and that the skilled musician had the authority and

responsibility to vary the time with nuances essential to the “musical sentiment.”38

Parsons, well aware of such contemporaneous criticisms of the machine, did attempt to

quell fears of his new scientific pedagogy, asking, “Does any one still hold the use of the

metronome to be dangerous to musical sensibility?”39

Parsons, as with following technically minded pedagogues, seemed less

concerned with “artistic” musical traditions, and more with novel pedagogical methods

and tools applied with “scientific” values in order to train a new generation of amateur

musicians. This is not to say that Parsons was uninfluenced by more typical nineteenth-

century instruction techniques as well; he described a student’s practice as a military

battle, where the metronome formed the infantry configurations essential in achieving

victory in practice.40

Significantly, Parsons most rigorously employed the metronome,

not for music training, but for gymnastic exercises of his own invention, which he offered

in the appendix to The Science of Pianoforte Practice. Besides being the dean of

scientific piano pedagogues in late nineteenth century America, Parsons was the author of

38

See Hirn, 4. “Le métronome est destiné, non à batter la measure (ceci ne peut etre

qu’exceptionnel et reviendrait à mécaniser l’Art, comme le craignait Weber), mais à indiquer des

l’abord, à un chef d’orchestra ou à un exécutant, le mouvement moyen que désire le compositeur,

movement auquel on doit se tenir tant que l’artiste n’indique pas de changement, et auquel on

doit sans cesse revenir à travers les petits écarts que commande le sentiment musical.”

39 Parsons, 24.

40

Ibid., 30. Parsons writes: “Therefore, it is well in attacking a new piece not to count too

confidently on achieving it in one unbroken series of conflicts, renewed day by day until it has

succumbed. Rather, let the student throw his whole energies into the work daily as long as ground

is visibly gained, and until the chief difficulties are at least hemmed in. If then its surrender does

not follow within a reasonable time, let it be formally invested, and metronome parallels be run

zigzagging toward its ultimate tempo…This mode of procedure somewhat resembles a military

campaign, in that one does not foresee precisely which objective points will be achieved first, but

only determines the line of effort to be pursued.”

Page 403: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

382

gymnastic training books, which were also published by G. Schirmer.41

Indeed, Parsons

envisioned his new scientific piano regimen to be a logical extension of gymnastics and

its mechanized drilling methods:

The use of metronomically-regulated rithms [sic.], both in gymnastic work and in

practice, not only prevents excitement of mood and allays nervous restlessness,

but also, besides awakening and training the rithmic sense which is artistically so

important, it enables the student to get an amount of exercise at a single practice

which would otherwise be impossible without muscular exhaustion. For it is a fact

of consequence to the student that, even when the point of fatigue has been

reached in exercise, a simple change of rithm will enable him to take a fresh start

without the loss of a minute's time, and proceed with a new series of movements,

as though no fatigue had been felt.

The exercises here given occupy less than fifteen minutes in performance

when executed the prescribed number of times, with metronomic beating as

directed.42

The metronome aided in a comprehensive “scientific” education of the body and mind

that Parsons was uniquely qualified to instruct. Parsons again suggested that the machine

quelled the variable emotions of the student over time. The metronome trained-out

fatigue and nervousness, instilling instead a sense of “rithm” keeping with the gymnastic

traditions of repetitive “muscular exercise.”

For the many “Rithmical Exercises” Parsons included in The Science of

Pianoforte Practice, he instructs: “Set the metronome for all of the exercises at ‘100’ for

a movement and a count to each stroke.” How little this training had to do with artistic

performance—and how much it resembled a military drill—is readily apparent as Parson

dictates:

41

Ibid., 44.

42

Ibid., 45.

Page 404: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

383

At stroke “1” of the metronome throw the shoulders promptly DOWN. Remain

perfectly QUIET, with the shoulders DOWN until the time of the first note of the

rithm expires. With the second note, let the shoulders return, as with an elastic

recoil, to the original position, UP, here again to remain QUIET until the next

"1.”43

Parsons perhaps realized that his metronomic exercises had nothing to do with the

“artistic” performance of music compositions. While he indeed documented the rhythms

of these physical exercises in standard music notation, Parsons significantly omitted any

time signatures. His rhythmic exercises were non-metrical in appearance and thus suggest

non-metrical qualities in performance. No consistent “strong beat” or “invisible (agogic)

accent” runs throughout the notated excerpts; for the gymnast-pianist, no sense of pulse

or bar emphasis was possible since no bars existed—visually or aurally—in these

repetitive, four-“click” physical exercises. Instead the students actualized mechanically

precise “up and down, back and forward” movements (with “quiet” holds) all while

counting with the clockwork metronome for the greatest possible consistency and

regimentation as defined by this particular militaristic-gymnastic pedagogy.

43

Ibid., 46.

Page 405: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

384

Fig. 6.4. One of Parsons’ many musical-gymnastic exercises for modern pianists. Probably derived

from physiological and psychological methods, Parsons’ repetitive drills equalized human motion through

the clicks of the metronome, while revising musical notation to express metronomic rhythm exclusively.

Time signatures, denoting the physical sensation of metric pulse, are thus effaced. Contradictorily, Parsons

stipulated that such metronomic regulation should never transfer to music compositions or “artistic”

performances. From Albert Ross Parsons, The Science of Pianoforte Practice (New York: G. Schirmer,

1886), 49.

Parsons’ treatise represented an emerging trend in arts education, a new tradition

informed by new mechanical apparatuses and scientific methods. His novel educational

approach necessitated the clockwork machine for reasons entirely divorced from past

performance practices and understandings of musical time and action. Yet, even during

Parsons’ lifetime, many musicians and educators across the Western world, such as Hirn,

still understood that the metronome did not provide the a priori time of creative music,

speech, or dance performances, and thus ought not to be a primary aspect of educational

training.

Similar to Hirn, critic Frank Ritchie, in his article “Music and Words” (1899)

from The Living Age magazine, reflected upon the mechanical and “gymnastic” methods

Page 406: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

385

of teaching poetry that had also emerged over the previous decades: “As regards the

practice of extending the metrical value of syllables, it may be admitted that considerable

license is permissible and even desirable: to read poetry to the beat of a metronome

would be an atrocity.”44

Lyman’s Normal Music Course (1896) also recounted the

individual student’s rhythmic freedom to be paramount, as his source was one of the last

to advocate for a simple pendulum, a non-automatical time reference for the teaching of

musical time.45

Julia E. Crane in her Music Teacher’s Manual, first published in 1889,

stated a similar belief, realizing that traditional artists and progressive scientists infused

the meaning of “rhythm” with two opposing values:

By accurate rhythm is not meant metronome accuracy. Rote singing should

always be [musically] rhythmical, with no sound of dragging or hurrying. There

can be no absolute rule in this matter, but all teachers must consider that while

dragging and hurrying are musical sins, proper pauses at the close of the phrases,

and musical ritardandos and accelerandos are the signs of artistic excellence. The

meaning of the song and the capacity of the class singing it, must determine the

length of the phrases, and the rate of singing.46

As the metronome became more available, educators such as Crane, Lyman, and Ritchie,

who valued the intrinsic humanity of musical time and rhythm, rejected the machine in

the education of impressionable youth. Even if Crane used a simple-pendulum

“metronome,” a device more commonly used to teach young singers and instrumentalists

44

Frank Ritchie, “Music and Words,” The Living Age, Aug. 26, 1899, 561. APS Online.

http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=511051102&sid=7&Fmt=2&clientId=3338&RQT=309&V

Name=HNP.

45

See, for examples, Frederic A. Lyman, The Normal Music in the Schoolroom (New York:

Silver, Burdett and Co., 1896), 28-29, 40-44, 66.

46 Julia E. Crane, Music Teachers Manual, Seventh Edition (Potsdam, NY: Elliot Fay & Sons,

1915), 70.

Page 407: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

386

during her age,47

she suggested that any external source could never replace the

importance of interpretation. For these pedagogues, and to some extent even the

scientifically influenced Parsons, musical “meaning” in “artistic” time—which had no

absolute, scientific rules—trumped clockwork. Nevertheless, as modern culture

developed, this value system drastically reversed in general education: what once was

clearly an atrocity for some—the application of the precise clockwork rhythm to

subjective culture—rapidly became a virtue, a goal to strive for, in new psychology-

influenced pedagogies.

The Modern Chronographic Education

The continual, redundant “rhythm” of the clockwork metronome, as first applied in

science laboratories, provided a temporal control for reproducible experiments on

inanimate and living creatures. As we will see, the machine had provided the very same

uses for the general public by the turn of the century. Employed with scientific and

experimental methodologies—founded on laboratory chronography—the metronome

became a common apparatus for the modern, standardized education of the masses. As

first discovered in the psychological laboratory, a subject must train in metronomic time

in order to precisely act in metronomic time. Yet, while experimental scientists most

47

Other sources published in the same year clearly indicate the exclusive use of the simple

pendulum. See for instance L. T. Wade, The Quincy Course of Study in Music (Boston: Silver,

Burdett, & Co., 1889), 24. It recommends that the teacher:

“Affix the metronome supporter to the top of the Chart in such a manner that the

metronome will swing across the page, and adjust length of string to measure forty-eight

inches from supporter to ball.

Set the metronome in motion, and ask the pupils to sing the exercise guided by it,

singing each quarter-note (pointing to each) long enough for the metronome to swing

once across the page, and singing the half-note (pointing to it) long enough for the

metronome to swing twice across the page.”

Page 408: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

387

prominently valued strict, mechanical mimicry above individual variation, a new wave of

pedagogues surfaced in the 1890s—years after Parsons’ musical-gymnastics exercises—

who applied the clockwork metronome with intentions once only expressed by these

psychologists. Their stipulation: in order for children to consistently play in precise

“rhythm,” they must be guided by the constant “rhythmic” sound of the metronome.

Due in large part to experimental psychologists’ prescriptions, the meanings of

time and rhythm were rapidly changing towards greater chronographic precisions

throughout Western society. In 1895, E. W. Scripture—the prominent psychologist who

tested musicians in mechanical performance in his Yale laboratory—publicly expressed

the social-industrial need for his new training methods, with their corresponding

chronographic values of rhythmic action, to expand throughout modern education. First

in his text Thinking, Feeling, Doing and later in the periodical The School Review,

Scripture proclaimed:

A great object in the education of children is to reduce their reaction times…A

person should be trained to rapid movements. The arm should be trained to move

quickly as in boxing or in fencing. All things being equal, the quickest man “gets

there.”48

Scripture explained that these values of temporal efficiency, facilitated through

chronographic methods, now applied to all actions of a child’s education, including

sensory perception and artistic creation:

Children should be trained to move all their limbs quickly. Special attention

should be paid to the arm movements which are trained in drawing, writing,

putting things in place, as in sorting colors or grains of corn as rapidly as possible,

etc. Other simple exercises might be added, e.g., the number of dots or the

number of lines that can be made in a quarter of a minute.49

48

E. W. Scripture, “Some Principles of Mental Education,” The School Review 3 (Nov., 1895):

544.

49

Scripture, 544-5.

Page 409: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

388

The precise durations of a child’s attention span and his physical responsiveness were

great concerns for Scripture, as for many experimental psychologists referencing

metronomic rhythm. Maintaining his chronographic bias, Scripture proclaimed,

“Children should be trained in rapid association,”50

which his science was uniquely

qualified to offer the whole of general education: “Here, too, almost every exercise at

present used in the schools could be so taught as to contribute to education of this

principle and that, too, at a saving, not an increase, of time and labor. The only new thing

required is a thorough knowledge of the principles of the new psychology on the part of

the teacher.”51

The new scientific paradigms of education that ascended by the turn of the

century, championed by Scripture and many others, reflected systematic, automatic, and

habitual training methods: instructional procedures endorsed by experimental

psychologists through the application of metronomic rhythm and chronographic

procedures. The highly influential educator G. Stanley Hall—an early student of Wundt,

founding editor of the American Journal of Psychology, and President of Clark

University—codified this new “experimental” wave of children’s education in Youth: Its

Education, Regimen, and Hygiene, first published in 1904. Like many psychologists

before him, Hall—who oversaw the musical-chronographic “rhythmic” experiments of

his students Bolton (1894) and Sears (1902)—advocated for the automatical training of

children, who seemed merely subjects in a more public laboratory:

50

Scripture, 544.

51

Scripture, 546.

Page 410: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

389

The ideas of space, time, and physical causation, and of many a moral and social

licit and non-licit, are rapidly unfolding. Never again [in a child’s formative

years] will there be such susceptibility to drill and discipline, such plasticity to

habituation, or such ready adjustment to new conditions. It is the age of external

and mechanical training. Reading, writing, drawing, manual training, musical

technic, foreign tounges and their pronunciation, the manipulation of numbers and

of geometrical elements, and many kinds of skill have now their golden

hour…This is not teaching in its true sense so much as it is drill, inculcation, and

regimentation. The method should be mechanical, repetitive, authoritative,

dogmatic. The automatic powers are at their very apex.52

Hall, Scripture, and others called for their laboratory procedures to inform all aspects of a

child’s learning—from music performance to writing, reading, and speaking. As Hall

confirmed, the new pedagogy valued automatism above individualistic thought and

action, which was by nature inefficient, variable, nuanced, and subjective. Scripture, in

1895, uncannily prophesized that these laboratory values would drastically influence

general education, and music education especially, in what he considered:

A new pedagogy as founded on the new (or experimental) psychology. The new

psychology determines by careful observation and experiment the laws of mental

life…The new psychology is founded on carefully obtained facts. It is sincerely to

be hoped that a new pedagogy will rapidly develop on the basis of the accurately

determined methods and carefully established principles of experimental

psychology. The anatomical rooms and the physiological laboratory are the

foundations of the science and art of medicine; the physical laboratory is the

foundation of civil and electrical engineering; before long I hope to see the

psychological laboratory and the psychological clinic at the foundation of all

education.53

Scripture did not have long to wait to see his wish become a reality, to witness the rise of

a chronographic classroom. By 1900, the scientist had corresponded and consulted with

music educators across the world, including Oscar Raif from the Berlin Hochschule,

52

G. Stanley Hall, Youth: Its Education, Regimen, and Hygiene (New York: D. Appleton and

Company, 1907), 4-5. This text is a reduction of Hall’s 1904 book Adolescence.

53

Scripture, 547-8.

Page 411: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

390

regarding new, experimental teaching methods using the metronome.54

In the following

years, scientific pedagogies, accompanied by scientific rhythm, strongly influenced

general education, invariably defining a student’s actions and perceptions through

normalizing chronographic values, just as they had in the psychology laboratory. And

while these scientifically inspired pedagogies both modernized and streamlined teaching

methods through drilling, habituation, and discipline—under the “external and

mechanical training” of chronography—they consequently diminished the variable and

subjective meanings of living rhythm and movement with aid of the scientific

metronome.

The New Sciences of Music Education and Child-Study with the Metronome

As Parsons’ 1886 text already showed, music pedagogy was not immune to the rising

influence of scientific values, methods, and technologies, and over the decades,

instruction methods increasingly advocated for students to train with ever-more precise

references to objective, metronomic rhythm. Edward Baxter Perry had recognized by the

early 1890s that machines were becoming trusted aids for training middle-class music

students in technical precision, and that this trend was entirely supported by popular

music periodicals and publishers, those with the greatest potential to gain financially

from selling new technologies. Perry believed that some of the very same machines

Parsons described in The Science of Pianoforte Practice actively effaced creativity and

expression in music education and performance, contending that machines, employed

54

See E. W. Scripture, “Cross-Education,” Appletons’ Popular Science Monthly LVI (March,

1900): 590. According to his letter, Raif began metronomic training experiments on his

conservatory students in 1898.

Page 412: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

391

with scientific values, stunted the student’s musical sense.55

Voicing his protest in an

aptly entitled article “The Other Side” (1894), Perry alluded to Parsons’ methods and

even more novel pedagogies emerging in the scientifically driven decade:

For some years past our musical periodicals have been the arena in which the

numerous champions of diverse modern methods and modern improvements in

the study of the pianoforte have striven gallantly for pre-eminence…Surgery,

calisthenics and psychology have all been drafted into the service of pianism, as

well as mechanical ingenuity, Yankee shrewdness in surmounting or evading

difficulties, and the habit of systematizing and concentrating effort, engendered

by our practical age.56

Despite Perry’s warnings, the chronographic bias of experimental psychologists

decisively shaped modern drilling techniques at the core of general education during the

1890s. In 1900 aesthetician and psychologist Henry Davies57

penned the introductory

essay, “A Plea for a Science of Art,” in volume XVIII of the widely read periodical

Music, in which he stated an ever-growing allegiance to the new culture defined by

experimental psychology:

One by one the great intellectual interests of men have come under the influence

of the scientific spirit. The so-called individual knowledges are now regarded as

constituting so many illustrations of this fact. The last to stand out, as it were, is

art. But even this interest is destined to yield as, I believe, a foregone conclusion,

55 Perry was consistent in his derision of mechanical apparatuses applied to music education.

Writing in Music VI, Perry exclaims: “I have deplored their introduction somewhat as having a

tendency to emphasize our national error of giving undue weight to the mechanical and technical

side of art work; but when they show us players who are the equals or superiors as artists of those

who have always ignored such mechanical devices, I shall be very ready to acknowledge and

welcome them.” See Edward Baxter Perry, et al., “The Pedagogical Aspects of the Practice

Clavier,” Music VI (May-Oct., 1894): 325.

56 Edward Baxter Perry, “The Other Side,” Music V (November, 1893-April, 1894): 207. Perry

describes many machines currently on the market that Parsons described—in including the

technicon, an artificial harness for the student’s hands—that supposedly improved physical

technique.

57

Henry Davies, “A Plea for a Science of Art,” Music XVIII (May, 1900): 15.

Page 413: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

392

especially when respect is had to the achievements of psychological investigation

in recent years.58

Davies believed that the creative arts and experimental sciences were converging ever

closer, for the good of both fields. “The aesthetic of our time,” he reiterates, “seeks to rest

on carefully guarded observation, measurement, statistics, experiment, in a word, on a

method.”59

By the turn of the century, music education had enmeshed with the scientific

methods and values of experimental psychology, as music pedagogues—in the pursuit of

objective, scientific norms—increasingly viewed the metronome as an infallible constant,

a chronograph against subjective musical perception. The metronome was no longer a

reference for subjective, internal movement; it became the objective rule of human

action, redefining once living rhythm with scientific precision in seemingly all aspects of

public education.

The work of experimental psychologists, inventors, and modern pedagogues all

attest to these shifting cultural values: greater mechanical precisions in all aspects of

human performance were appropriate, desirable, and necessary for the new generation of

students—thus a new cultural tradition of use for the clockwork metronome ascended. An

1893 United States patent filing by British inventor John Treadway Hanson provides a

telling analogy of the new metronomic qualities in musical instruction. While living,

professional conductors of the age were often chastised for being metronomic, Hanson’s

new mechanized “metronome”—specifically intended for the amateur, student

musician—replicated the beat patterns of a conductor’s baton with the appearance of a

58

Ibid., 1.

59

Ibid., 8.

Page 414: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

393

mindless automaton60

along with a mechanized sound production “much the same as that

of a musical box.”61

Waning from the consciousness of these modern, scientific educators and

inventors was the application of the metronome as a non-regulatory, initial pulse

reference—one that posed less detriment to individual creation, interpretation, and

expression in time. In this scientifically informed age, in which chronographic

automatism and habitual training reigned, a simple pendulum was insufficient compared

to clockwork. Indeed, pedagogues of this new systematic and experimental training—

informed through the chronographic bias—found contextual “meaning” and subjective

interpretation in time too imprecise for a host of performance acts. William Patterson, in

The Rhythm of Prose, voiced the great “problem” in understanding and actualizing

rhythm in twentieth-century culture. Up to the present day, Patterson reported, “Rhythm

has been treated as an art form instead of as a form of motor expression…‘Meaning’

interferes with estimates of duration and intensity,”62

two quantifiable values of the

experimental psychologist’s chronographic understanding of rhythm. Indeed, Patterson’s

assessment was accurate when compared to past beliefs in creative, subjective time:

60

John Treadway Hanson, “Metronome,” U.S. Patent #512,470 (Filed Mar. 13, 1893; Patented

Jan. 9, 1894). On December 12, 1893, Hanson described his invention to the Royal Music

Association and highlighted the glaring omissions from his American patent filing. He admitted,

“It is but mechanical and has the limitations inseparable from mechanism, and can only be of use

for the practice of students when no conductor is present. Still you may possibly think there may

be some advantage in students becoming accustomed to the various beats of a bâton, and learning

to attach a meaning to each of them.”

See John Treadway Hanson, “A New Metronome,” in Proceedings of the Musical

Association, 20th Sess. 1893–1894 (London: Novello, Ewer and Co., 1894): 23.

61 Hanson, “A New Metronome,” 24.

62 William Morrison Patterson, The Rhythm of Prose, An Experimental Investigation of Individual

Difference in the Sense of Rhythm (New York: Columbia University Press, 1916), 30. Patterson

cites the theories of W. Brown in Time in English verse rhythm (1908).

Page 415: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

394

rhythm was once considered a living, nuanced quality—a creative “art form”—not a

scientific control. Before experimental psychologists applied the scientific metronome to

human action and perception, “artistic” rhythm was interpretive before it was

metronomic—musical and spoken rhythm was complex and meaningful, a rhythm

irreducible to mere mechanics. As Patterson’s comment implied, the messy problems of

“art” and interpretation interfered with the quantifiable (and ultimately reductive)

knowledge of chronographic time and action.

As early as 1894—corresponding to the release of scientific “rhythm” studies by

Meumann, Bolton, and others—American music periodicals espoused ever-greater

mechanical methods of instruction to young and inexperienced students, in which

exacting metronomic regulation was now paramount to the understanding of musical time

and performance practices. As its title exposes, the turn-of–the-century periodical Music:

A Monthly Magazine Devoted to the Art, Science, Technic and Literature of Music

consistently discussed music and education through mechanical reference and technical

knowledge. For instance, W. S. B. Mathews’ Music article “Schumann, the Poet of the

Pianoforte”—appearing in the same issue as Perry’s diatribe against mechanical music

education—deemed with scientific certainty that the theme of the Schumann’s “‘Etudes

Symphoniques’…moves in quarters at the rate of 54 of the metronome.”63

With no

discussion of the importance of the performer’s expressive interpretation, pulse-sense, or

63

W. S. B. Mathews, “Schumann, the Poet of the Pianoforte,” Music V (Nov. 1893-Apr. 1894):

713. Reprinted in Music Its Ideals and Methods (Philadelphia: Theodore Presser, 1897), 87.

From the pre-chronographic past, the caveat of John Alexander Fuller-Maitland provides

the directly opposite instruction: “We must warn the student of Schumann's pianoforte works

against placing any reliance in the metronome marks, which are quite incorrect in all the existing

editions, the composer's own metronome being altogether out of order.” See John Alexander

Fuller-Maitland, Schumann (London: Sampson Low, Marston, Searle & Rivington, 1884), 61.

Page 416: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

395

individuality, Mathews (the editor of the periodical) thereby reduced the poetry of

Schumann’s music to replicable clockwork. As many periodicals suggest, musical

pedagogues under the influence of scientific “rhythm” applied the metronome not as a

mere reference; the machine began to actively regulate time and action, as it had done in

the laboratory for nearly two decades prior. Thus, for Music’s readership along with those

musical amateurs failing to heed the warnings of experienced musicians such as Edward

Baxter Perry, the chronographic bias began influencing their foundational understandings

of music education and performance.64

By 1895 middle-class music students had gained even greater access to new

methods and machines for instruction, which required the purchase of a clockwork

metronome for their technical-musical training. The article “A Lesson from the

Metronome” by an anonymous writer “H. L. B.” from the Christian Science Journal

attests to this trend, along with the new applications of the Maelzel metronome as a

chronographic regulator of musical action. The narrator, a middle-class mother of a

typical pupil, relates the impact of the new clockwork metronome upon her daughter’s

daily practice. The writer was not an experienced musician and was totally unaware of

the metronome until her daughter brought one home. Indeed, it seems that the metronome

was still not common knowledge in the standard middle-class “musical” household.65

64

Nevertheless, the word “metronome,” even through the 1890s, could alternatively indicate a

simple pendulum, and thus it is difficult to ascertain pedagogical applications of the clockwork

device with absolute certainty during the last years of the nineteenth century. In 1893, for

instance, Music IV reported upon Zeckwer’s patent #360,550 filed in 1886 as a “species of

metronome” in the form of a simple pendulum. See Music IV (1893): 47.

65

It must be stated that, as late as the 1910 edition, The Christian Science Hymnal, which

includes five hymns written by Rev. Mary Baker Eddy, the Founder of Christian Science and The

Christian Science Journal, does not include one metronome mark in the entire book. See The

Christian Science Hymnal (Boston: The Christian Science Publishing Society), 1910.

Page 417: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

396

“For the benefit of some who may be as ignorant as I was,” she admits, “let me say the

metronome is a small machine for indicating the correct time and speed at which a

musical composition should be played. It ticks by clock work and rings a tiny bell at

regular intervals thus ‘marking time’ for the musical student.”66

But for this musical

layman, the standard bell metronome reflected far more than marking the time: it was the

truth of time itself. She depicts what must have been a customary scene for the era, as the

novice music student and the well meaning but musically clueless mother wrestled to

conform within artificial, metronomic accuracy:

After our young woman had been at the piano a few moments I heard vexed

exclamations. As I went to her assistance she cried “Oh, Mamma, I can't play with

this thing! It won't come out right!”

Conscious of superior knowledge, I attempted to show her how it should

be done, but speedily found myself in her dilemma. While I counted one, two,

three, four; one, two, three, four; my monitor had marked one, two, three, four;

one, two, three, four; one, two. Again, and again, I tried, and presently began the

movement in proper time. I was jubilant! But a few more bars found me far ahead

of the recording bell and I too, began to say, “Oh, pshaw!” The little machine had

a quiet way of ticking on quite unconscious of my blunders. It was intensely silly

to be provoked with it.67

Despite the young daughter’s and her own inability to perform in strict metronomic

time—as the laboratory subject would be forced to do “passively”—the mother believed

the metronomic reference was the unerring one. She found herself to “blunder” in the

perception of musical time as compared to the metronome, with its mechanical bell

dictating the bar with automatical consistency. Unknowing of the anti-metronomic

aesthetic of musicians such as Hofmann, Saint-Saëns, Berlioz, Sternberg, Brahms, Perry

66

H.L.B., “A Lesson from the Metronome,” Christian Science Journal XIII (April, 1895): 375.

67

H.L.B., 375-6.

Page 418: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

397

and countless others, mother and child continued to struggle with automatical time. The

writer continues:

I had often gone to my daughter saying positively, “Child, you are not counting

right”—and had received the answer “it is right.” Now there could be no question

whether her music were right or wrong. If it agreed with the monitor it was right:

if it disagreed it was wrong and there could be also no offense.

Her daughter’s innate rhythmic feeling conflicted with an automaton, and the young lady

adamantly felt that she perceived musical time correctly compared with a machine. Yet

the narrator rationalized with scientific certainty that the truth must lay with the

metronome, and that any diversion from mechanical time only reflected human error—It

was their unique personal equations that were the “problem” when compared to the

clockwork standard. She confesses, “My blunders were absolutely nothing to the

metronome, neither thing nor person was to be condemned, but my own proud self.”68

Against its chronographic precision, only humans could be found wanting. For this

mother, a staunch Christian Scientist, the metronome was not simply the representation of

musical time; her essay was a modern morality tale informed through the chronographic

bias, in which the metronome—a tool the writer was totally ignorant of her entire life—

became the very analogy of God’s rule.69

She states, “Brought face to face with divine

law mortal mind stands abashed,” concluding from the modern lesson in clockwork

performance that “Science is immutably right.”70

Similar to the laboratory chronograph

68

H.L.B., 376.

69

She verifies: “There is and can be no mistake here. God makes the rule and everything short of

strict agreement with his unerring law is discordant. Until work is compared with God's way,

mortals can never be sure it is well done. Brought face to face with divine law mortal mind stands

abashed.” H.L.B., 376.

70

H.L.B., 376-7.

Page 419: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

398

and the God of Christian Science, the metronome offered the disinterested, objective, and

infallible truth, one the mere human could only aspire to mimic, albeit imperfectly.

This 1895 Christian Science Journal article allegorizes the significant value shift

in time for middle-class culture, as music teachers and their students began accepting the

metronome as the unerring reference for modern education. Indeed, while few sources

considered the metronome to be godly, by the 1890s, music periodicals, references, and

performing editions nevertheless had attested that the metronome was the scientific (and

thus irrefutable) truth of time, a machine that presided above and beyond the variable

“problems” of human perception. For instance, A Dictionary of Musical Terms (1895),

published by G. Schirmer (a company with an undoubted commercial interest in

clockwork metronome sales ever since Parsons’ music and gymnastic treatises),71

espoused the new chronographic bias of education and performance, stating that the

definitive metronome mark is placed “at the head of a composition for exactly indicating

its tempo” and is “much used by beginners and students, for learning to play strictly in

time, and for timing their practice.”72

Unlike the critical Encyclopedia Britannica entry

71

The obvious fact exists that published music with metronome numbers tacitly requires and

encourages consumers to purchase and use metronomes. Music publishers’ economic relationship

to metronome sales, sheet music, and metronome-advertisement income is not the primary focus

of this study, but it is important to mention that some of the very first music publishers to print

metronome numbers—notably Breitkopf und Härtel and Tobias Haslinger—also sold

metronomes in their shops.

Even Grove’s first edition Dictionary of Music (1880) promoted the emerging economic

interests of national music publishers and metronome manufacturers (who now supplied

machines for new pedagogical texts such as Shedlock’s Trip to Music-Land). Within its

“metronome” definition, the British reference states: “By far the best Metronomes now attainable

are those manufactured in England for Messrs. Cocks, Chappell, Ashdown & Perry, and other

well-known Music Publishers. French Metronomes are far less durable than these, and, as a

general rule, far less accurate time-keepers; though it is sometimes possible to meet with one

which will beat evenly enough, as long as it lasts.” Grove, vol. II (1880): 320.

72

Theodore Baker, A Dictionary of Musical Terms, Third Edition (New York: G. Schirmer,

1897), 121.

Page 420: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

399

seen in Chapter II, the Schirmer reference failed to relate the reductive, mechanical

effects that the metronome imbued in the performer, or that living music performance

seldom corresponded—or should correspond—to precise clockwork rhythm.

By the mid-1890s, science and music periodicals had converged in endorsing the

metronome as a premiere external guide of rhythmic action. Strict metronomic regulation

in human training, once reserved exclusively for the laboratory, was rapidly becoming the

standard rule of middle-class music pedagogy. Volume VIII of Music: A Monthly

Magazine Devoted to the Art, Science, Technic and Literature of Music (1895) called for

the amateur student to perform music “rhythmically,” that is, with incessant, metronomic

consistency. In the article “Rubato” by William Smythe Babcock Mathews, the editor of

the periodical, personal interpretation ceased to be a primary factor in an amateur’s

habitual musical training, as he asserts:

Rubato is not something to be sought…What almost every piano student needs to

learn more than any other one thing is first to play in measure. When you have a

rhythmical feeling, so that you can play with the metronome without any

particular hampering of freedom, then the next thing is to learn to feel the

expression…This varying of rhythm [rubato] must be done in such a way that the

hearer is not conscious of it as accelerando or retard, but simply as expression:

and when it is well done the metronome will come right with the playing when

you take the whole eight or sixteen measures in succession.73

Mathews, perhaps unaware of the machine’s habituating effects, recognized that the

metronome defined a “rhythmical feeling” for the student—but it was a “rhythm” of a

mechanical species. For Mathews, the expressive, linguistic, and physical qualities of

time—now reconceived as an atypical and individual variation (in rubato)—must be

regulated first and foremost by the metronome. Indeed, he urged students to use the

73

William Smythe Babcock Mathews, “The Practical Teacher. Rubato,” Music VIII (May-

October 1895): 325.

Page 421: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

400

machine when gauging the mechanical time-accuracy of symmetric musical phrases,

even before they “learn to feel expression”—an exactly opposite instruction from Julia E.

Crane’s 1889 assessment of artistic rhythm. Mathews’ advice even contrasts with

Parsons’ metronome-caveat from 1886. Thus Mathews imbued music instruction with an

increasing scientific precision, one that devalued the personal equation of the performer-

student and contradicted the attitudes and practices of skilled musicians and

knowledgeable critics of his day (and of the past century). No longer was the metronome

for simple technical drilling or pulse reference; no longer were the movements of speech

and rhetoric primary aspects of musical phrasing and expression. Mathews implied that

the clockwork metronome actively structured and grounded musical tunes, in both

practice and performance, regardless of a piece’s unique melodic, harmonic, metric, or

formal qualities. Such clockwork conceptions of musical time, along with the rigid and

reductive views of musical rhythm and movement, would become even more precise in

the following decades as scientific pedagogies continued to gain popularity.

By 1897, as the clockwork metronome finally began to be manufactured within

North America, the periodical Music had stressed to music students the importance of the

tool for practicing the latest sheet music and instruction books (with all-important

metronome markings). Conveniently, Music sold clockwork metronomes at a discount

with subscriptions to the periodical. (North American music periodicals used the

metronome-subscription incentive as a common sales tactic in the late nineteenth

century.) Employing redundant, metronomic language, Music’s full-page announcement

exclaims, imperative after imperative, the essential value of the clockwork machine for

the modern student’s precisely regimented musical education:

Page 422: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

401

A METRONOME SHOULD BE ON EVERY PIANO.

We have placed a large order for metronomes with the celebrated

American house of the John Church Company, and are now prepared to send

them out as premiums, as given below…

EVERY PIANO STUDENT OUGHT TO HAVE A METRONOME.

EVERY SINGER OUGHT TO HAVE A METRONOME.

EVERY TEACHER OUGHT TO HAVE A METRONOME.

The Metronome tells you the rate of movement at which you ought to play or sing

a piece.

The Metronome tells you whether you can go through the piece in unbroken

measure.

The Metronome tells you whether you have doubled the speed exactly in Mason's

exercises.

The Metronome tells you whether you are playing your Virgil exercises at the

proper speed.

The Metronome is your rhythmic guide, philosopher and friend, without which at

hand for consultation you will be all at sea.

The Metronome tells you whether you are playing your slow movements without

hurrying on the one hand or without hanging back.74

Such blatantly cajoling advertisements for the apparently still unfamiliar Maelzel

metronome were nothing uncommon. As early as 1888, Etude magazine, published by

Theodore Presser, promoted the technological novelty of Maelzel’s machine (in contrast

to the simple pendulum) in an advertisement that read: “What shall I buy for Christmas

presents?…A metronome, with clockwork, is a fine present for the progressive

student.”75

Nevertheless, Etude continued to offer “teacher’s pocket metronomes”—

graduated simple pendulums—in their ad section (and as subscription prizes) through the

1890s. Not willing to miss any opportunity for profit, the Theodore Presser shop directly

sold both kinds of metronome to consumers eager for music instruction.76

Perhaps it is

74

[advertisement], Music XI (Apr., 1897), inside cover.

75

Etude VI (1888): 193. ML e85 v6 1888 no. 12, Sibley Music Library, Eastman School of

Music.

76

See, for example, Etude VI: 181.

Page 423: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

402

telling that Etude did not consistently print metronome numbers for the many music

selections offered within the periodical until 1895. And by 1898, new advertisements for

the Maelzel metronome—with an enticing image of the clockwork machine—had begun

to supersede advertisements and images of simple tempo-pendulums in Presser’s Etude

magazine.77

Figure 6.5. A typical advertisement in Theodore Presser’s Etude, circa 1890s, for the “Teacher’s Pocket

Metronome,” an adjustable simple pendulum with a graduated scale intended for general music education.

Such devices, commonly used throughout nineteenth-century pedagogy, were solely intended for an initial

or basic tempo-pulse reference.

Finding a home both in the laboratory, classroom, and music studio, the

clockwork metronome was a tool for many purposes by the turn-of-the-century. While

music periodicals stressed the metronome in musical education, other periodicals equally

championed the clockwork metronome for the modern science education. The magazine

Science (1897), for instance, considered Maelzel’s machine an essential apparatus for

public schools in order to “enable the properly trained teacher to give a very thorough

course in laboratory instruction.”78

The textbook A First Course on Physics (1906),

77

See Etude XVI (1898): 83. ML e85 v16 no.3, Sibley Music Library, Eastman School of Music.

78

Franklin W. Barrows, “The New York State Science Teachers' Association,” Science 5

(Mar.19, 1897): 466.

Page 424: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

403

suggests that the metronome was a standard schoolroom device, not only for music

training, but also for a variety of physics experiments including tests on Newton’s Second

Law of Motion.79

In the last years of the nineteenth century, educators under the chronographic bias

prominently voiced the importance of this new metronomic education for the typical

student. In the 1896 Musical Visitor article “The metronome; its use,” M. L. Karr

proclaimed the benefits of this still novel tool of musical instruction, stating, “There is a

new school of teaching most successfully in operation and growing in popularity, one of

whose distinctive features is the use of this little instrument.”80

It cannot go unnoticed

that the publisher of the Musical Visitor was the John Church Company, and just over a

year after Karr’s article appeared, the firm would become the first major producer of

clockwork metronomes in North America.81

Karr confirms that clockwork metronomes and metronomic rhythm were indeed

forming the centerpiece of the new scientific method of music education espoused by

eager music publishers and modern pedagogues. For Karr, the clockwork metronome,

like the observatory chronograph or the laboratory kymograph, “encourages precision,

promptness, and accuracy—nay, insists, with its strenuous monotony, on all these.”82

Under scientific prescriptions, monotony and mechanical precision were new cultural

79

Robert Andrews Millikan and Henry Gordon Gale, A First Course on Physics (Boston: Ginn &

Company, 1906), 26, 27, 34.

80

M. L. Karr, “The metronome—its use,” The Musical Visitor 25 (Apr., 1896): 98.

81

See [Advertisement], The Musical Visitor, a Magazine of Musical Literature and Music 26

(Dec., 1897): 335.

82

Karr, 98.

Page 425: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

404

values for performance pedagogy. Through this non-living temporal reference, the

metronome vanquishes the child’s volatile emotional state—his personal equation—in

practice and performance, as Karr explains:

It is self that must be conquered, spasmodic, nervous, untutored self, before

anything can be done in public; and this calm, unemotional, logical little ticker is

a support beyond words in the battle.83

Karr, echoing Parsons’ gymnastic-hygienic pedagogy roughly a decade prior, points out

that the variables of the child’s personal equation must be regulated—“conquered”

even—through habitual use of the monotonous, lifeless metronome. As many suggested,

the metronome reflected inhumanity, but for Karr, this was a positive, scientific value;

the metronome reduced all subjective, unpredictable elements of musical interpretation

and action to mechanical certainty, especially for the impressionable young student. In

this modern pedagogy, “logical,” “unemotional” mechanics trumped personal expression.

As Karr describes, the metronome trained both the mind and body of the student through

artificial precision, and,

Is most helpful, too, in the assigning of technical work…Even the driest detail

work will be done by a child if he knows you expect it, and if he knows just how

to do it. Then, too, early practice with the metronome gives fidelity to the whole

time sense…The playing of scales, for instance, first in quarter notes, then

sixteenths, then thirty-seconds, to the metronome, at from sixty to two hundred

beats in the minute, can not fail to give accuracy to the time-thought as well as

facility to the fingers.84

Karr’s novel pedagogy, even more precisely than Parsons’ or Mathews’ instructions,

imbued the building blocks of all tonal music with metronomic regularity. Through

habitual training the child musician learned that mechanical clicks informed all notions of

83

Ibid.

84

Ibid.

Page 426: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

405

“rhythm,” ingraining the student with a redundant, accurate “time-thought” for tonal

scales, arpeggios, and the like. Significantly, Karr provides no discussion of the simple

pendulum—it seems that in his increasingly scientific culture, time and action could be

most precisely and systematically trained through clockwork sound. Thus, as Karr

describes the importance of developing the “time sense” in practice, his new brand of

temporal “accuracy” reflected the student’s ability to mimic artificial, metronomic

rhythm.

Similar scientific values appear in volume XIII (1898) of Music where Bertram C.

Henry’s article “The Development of Rhythmic Conception” instructed both the modern

teacher and the impressionable student to act solely in the clockwork rhythm of the

metronome:

In order to feel the rhythm in an uneven succession of tones, the listener must be

able to measure off equal spaces of time. The next step should be directed toward

the cultivation of this power. A metronome is all but indispensable. Set the index

at 100, and let the pupil count in unison with the strokes…Practice of this sort

should be continued until the pupil can preserve the tempo perfectly some twenty

counts after the metronome is silenced, at all rates of speed from 72 to 144. The

slow tempo is apt to offer more difficulty than the fast.85

As reflected in contemporaneous scientific research, new pedagogical methods and

machines created a new precision-oriented goal: it was essential for the student to think

and perform within metronomic time. In Henry’s view, the music pupil must habituate to

every metronomic rate with unflinching mechanical regularity—even when the artificial

time-source had ceased. This training imparted the student with “perfect” mechanical

time before a single composition was ever performed. Henry seemingly failed to consider

85

Bertram C. Henry, “Development of Rhythmic Conception,” Music XIII (Nov., 1897-Apr.,

1898): 469-470.

Page 427: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

406

the student’s intuitive or individual interpretation of rhythm to be a primary, vital quality

of musical time in practice or performance.

These pedagogical methods were not isolated examples of the new importance of

metronomic training that emerged in Western civilization during the end of the nineteenth

century. Indeed, the metronome guided a variety of learning-acts in the modern

classroom, as all aspects of education seemed to move towards the scientific method of

chronography. Since the 1880s, as we have seen, experimental psychologists trained

subjects to mimic the rate of the metronome as precisely as possible, employing

procedures nearly identical to Henry’s “Development of Rhythmic Conception.” At

Johns Hopkins University, for instance, experiments “On the Time-sense” ingrained

subjects with a sense of time and action totally unlike anything encountered in their

normal lives. The premiere American psychology journal of the decade, Mind, related the

Hopkins experiments in 1886:

The method of experimenting was somewhat similar to that of Vierordt, and

consisted in a general way, in impressing upon the mind intervals of time by

means of a metronome, and in reproducing the same after the metronome had

been stopped.86

Subjects in the Hopkins study were “impressed” to react in consistent and unwavering

metronomic time for a scientific practice conceived by Karl Vierordt in the 1860s.

Identically to Henry’s music students, the John Hopkins’s subjects performed against the

very same artificial time reference, which they had to “rhythmically” reproduce even

when the machine had stopped. While these experiments seemed logical and worthwhile

for experimental psychologists, skilled musicians of the age—such as those encountered

in Chapters I and II—would likely have found the procedure to be a strange human

86

Lewis T. Stevens, “On The Time-Sense,” Mind 11 (Jul., 1886): 393.

Page 428: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

407

pantomime in Maelzelian automatism. The report explains the chronographic process,

devised over a decade before Henry’s Music article:

The individual under experiment tapped the lever synchronously with the beats of

a metronome. When he had become perfectly familiar with the given interval, the

drum was set in motion and the first round of the tracing was taken, with the

metronome still beating; the latter was then stopped, while the person kept on

tapping the lever at the same rate. The average of the intervals recorded in the first

line of the tracing was the standard time; the rest of the tracings gave successive

reproductions of this standard.87

This “rhythmic” training through chronography continued well into the twentieth century.

An Elementary Laboratory Course in Psychology (1916) by Harvard psychologists

Herbert Sidney Langfeld and Floyd Henry Allport codified a practically identical time-

training procedure to the Hopkins’ “time-sense” tests and Henry’s 1898 musical lesson

for “The Development of Rhythmic Conception.” The psychologists report:

Method: The metronome is started at its slowest rate, and the subjects of the class

listen passively for a few minutes until they become adjusted to the rhythm of the

beats. The instructor then measures a time interval of between 60 and 90 seconds

by tapping at its beginning and end. The subject (in each pair of students) listens

passively during this interval to the ticking of the metronome. He should not,

however, see the instrument nor count its beats. The experimenter (in each pair)

carefully times the interval given by the instructor. As soon as the second tap is

given the metronome is silenced, and the subject reproduces the interval as

accurately as possible, not by tapping, but by a movement of the index finger at

the beginning and end of his estimated interval. The error of reproduction (plus or

minus) is noted by the experimenter. Ten such trials are made.

The entire procedure is repeated four times, using four other speeds of the

metronome, including its highest speed, but keeping the same standard time

interval.88

At the turn of the century, the metronome was essential to a host of similar pedagogical

procedures, the purposes of which were to improve classroom performance and quantify

87

Ibid., 394.

88

Herbert Sidney Langfeld and Floyd Henry Allport, An Elementary Laboratory Course in

Psychology (Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1916), 76.

Page 429: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

408

students’ “innate” abilities through chronographic experimentation. To promote these

aims, experimental psychologists, including Wundt,89

introduced their mental and

physical experiments to the populous under the heading of “child study” in the last years

of the nineteenth century. As a prominent tool of child study, the metronome informed,

defined, and regulated the abilities and actions of children; with the scientific metronome,

psychologists sought to provide infallible and absolute knowledge regarding the mental

and physical capacities of the young mind and body.90

Psychological experiments that tested the normal “musical sense” of children

certainly began before 1888, the year Wildermuth published a study of the auditory

capacities of both typical children and the mentally deficient. According to an American

Journal of Psychology reviewer, “with the still more defective portion of the idiots (30

cases) simpler tests had to be used (noises, a metronome, music-box, etc.) and the effect

judged from the aspect of gesture.”91

Scripture, in his university laboratory, tested

children’s ability to physically mark time with metronomic precision before 1895.92

In

Thinking, Feeling, Doing, he also charted the results of tapping-fatigue tests administered

to New Haven school children, aged six to seventeen years.93

By 1897, Ebbinghaus had

89

See Wundt, “Psychical Development of the Child,” in Outlines of Psychology (Leipzig:

Wilhelm Engelmann, 1902), 316. He devotes considerable attention to a child’s developing

“rhythmic” abilities that clearly prefigures Jaques-Dalcroze’s concerns and teachings.

90

While the topic cannot be approached in depth here, it is important to point out that the

utilitarian purposes of these following experimental-psychology studies served as measures for

child labor, an economically sanctioned practice rampant in the industrialized world during this

time.

91

“Psychological Literature,” American Journal of Psychology 2 (May, 1889): 507.

92

Scripture, “Some Principles of Mental Education,” 541-2.

93

Scripture, Thinking, Feeling, Doing, 36.

Page 430: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

409

created intelligence tests for children to be administered in the laboratory using the

typical chronographic apparatus.94

In the last years of the nineteenth century, child-study experiments rapidly

expanded to public classrooms, especially across North America, where many middle-

class children became passive subjects, quantified and regulated through metronomic

controls for scientific ends. For instance, in 1898, experimental psychologists employed

both a bell metronome and a “metronome, with electrical connection” to test students as

reported in Experimental Study of Children, Including Anthropometrical and Psycho-

Physical Measurements of Washington School Children.95

Child-study psychologists

applied similar laboratory methods and apparatus in Chicago classrooms only years later.

A Frank Leslie’s Popular Monthly article entitled “Scientific Child Study” (1901) by

Edward Marshall describes: “Several thousand school children have been examined and

experimented with, as carefully and as scientifically as any student is taught to

experiment with chemicals in a school laboratory.” Marshall explained that the purpose

of child study was to measure a prized value for the modern psychologist: human

normality. “Perfection, to the mind of the scientist, means absolute normality,” Marshall

confirms, and “it is the normal child who makes the best student on average.”96

In search

of this perfectly “normal” child, experimental psychologists measured the size, weight,

94

Edwin G. Boring, A History of Experimental Psychology (New York: Appleton-Century

Company, 1929), 383.

95

Arthur MacDonald, Experimental Study of Children, Including Anthropometrical and Psycho-

Physical Measurements of Washington School Children, a Bibliography (Washington:

Government Printing Office, 1899), 1199-1200.

96

Edward Marshall, “Scientific Child Study,” Frank Leslie's Popular Monthly 51 (Mar., 1901): 3.

APS Online.

Page 431: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

410

head shape, breathing capacities, auditory limits, and fatigue tolerances of these young

subjects. To test physical endurance, scientists applied their standard chronographic

machine, an ergograph,

To this table the child’s arm is clamped and strapped so as to prevent, so far as

possible, any motion whatever of any part except the middle finger. Over the

finger a clamp is fastened, connecting with a string, to the end of which is

fastened a weight. The weight, after reference to the previous records made of the

child, is arranged so as to represent exactly seven per cent of the child’s weight.

After the apparatus had been perfectly adjusted, a metronome, the instrument used

to beat time when children are practicing music, was started. The child under

investigation was instructed to raise the middle finger of the locked hand every

time the metronome beat. After the finger was raised it was to be lowered as

steadily as possible. It was arranged that in ninety seconds the finger should be

raised forty-five times, or once every two seconds. A part of the apparatus is a

traveling pencil, which makes, in a waving line, a record on paper, showing the

varying heights to which the weight is raised with every effort of the child. The

usefulness of these ergographic records is easily apparent. They supplied the

missing link in the chain of tests making up the Chicago scheme of physical child-

study. They indicated endurance.97

Frank Leslie’s Popular Monthly prints the image of various mechanical apparatuses,

including the scientific metronome that sat atop the ergograph. As depicted, the child-

subject was strapped, regulated, and recorded to study his “normal” metronomic

endurances. The periodical also codified the schema of head shapes of children in order

to determine a student’s levels of mental normality or deviance.98

Head calipers, the

metronome, and other machines, as these experiments confirm, were tools in researching

the “best,” most regular, most normal student—now precisely defined through the

modern experimental science of child study.

97

Ibid., 8.

98

Ibid., 7-8.

Page 432: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

411

Fig. 6.6. Detail from “Tests and Measurements in Child Study.” The child depicted to the right is

connected to an ergograph, as he continually lifts his finger to the beating of a metronome to test his

endurance. From Edward Marshall, “Scientific Child Study,” Frank Leslie's Popular Monthly 51 (Mar.,

1901): 3.

Metronomic testing with the ergograph continued well into the twentieth century for all

age levels, both for educational and industrial purposes. A 1903 report in the American

Journal of Psychology describes the standard chronographic test to quantify muscular

fatigue, in which university students repetitively raise their finger, again attached to a

small weight, for as long as possible at various points during the day. The report states,

“The subjects all made uniformly rapid contractions, keeping time to the beat of a

metronome…the subject was required to continue his contractions till he could no longer

bring his finger up” to the appropriate ruler-height.99

During the turn of the century, the metronome clearly performed a similar

function in both child study and scientifically inspired music pedagogy: the machine

became a constant temporal control over the child’s movement, a measure of the personal

equation against a scientifically “normal,” mechanical rhythm. Music: Devoted to the Art,

99

A. Caswell Ellis and Maud Margaret Shipe, “A Study of the Accuracy of the Present Methods

of Testing Fatigue,” The American Journal of Psychology 14 (Jul.-Oct., 1903): 233.

Page 433: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

412

Science, Technic and Literature of Music continued to be an outspoken vehicle for the

chronographic bias, metronomic pedagogy, and the new science of music education. Thus

we find that T. Carl Whitmer’s Music article, “The Time-Marking System in Music”

(1900) reads like a laboratory experiment where the metronome offered the undisputed

rule of action, placed above and beyond the variable qualities of human performance.

Indeed, the very definition of “natural” musical rhythm becomes mechanical as Whitmer

states:

There are the natural and the artificial accents to deal with when an attempt is

made. The difference must be settled by the metronome. A natural accent is that

accent which the metronome describes, so to speak, by a complete single

oscillation. Measure, by hearing, the impression made from the click to the end of

its swing one way only. That is natural accent. The artificial accent is more

violent, and is in ordinary use in conservatories.100

According to Whitmer, the click of the clockwork metronome was “natural” time in

music: a stunningly reversed epistemology from skilled performers of the age, yet one

highly familiar to experimental psychologists. Whitmer, in the tradition of scientists and

astronomers before him, believed metronomic time offered the single, true rhythmic

reference for human action. Thus, for this scientific pedagogue, chronographic methods

logically presided over the personal equation of the student musician as well. In

Whitmer’s pedagogy, the temporal paradigm shift had occurred in full: the externalized

“rhythm” of the metronome replaced the “imprecise” (once natural) human rhythm of

musical time. (By 1917, in his book The Way of My Mind, Whitmer had encouraged

100

T. Carl Whitmer, “The Time-Marking System in Music,” Music XVIII (June, 1900): 172.

Page 434: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

413

music teachers to “read new books…on psychology to bring your teaching methods up to

date”101

and to “expect absolute accuracy in mechanical matters from your students.”)102

Infused with scientific goals, the clockwork metronome was rapidly displacing

old epistemologies and teaching techniques for all aspects of education, while the values

of rhythmic automatism increasingly became part of a youth’s standard music education.

This trend can be witnessed in new patents filings for improved clockwork metronomes,

which rapidly appear from 1897 to 1902 in no less than six independent designs.103

Two

inventions filed in 1902 represent the increasingly precision-oriented use of the

metronome in music training, in which the primary goal of the machine was not to

artificially reference the initial human pulse of musical meter, but rather to project the

objective, redundant sound of clockwork. United States Patent #738,093 by J. Brady is a

metronome strikingly fashioned as a gothic church, one that forgoes a bell—the inventor

101

T. Carl Whitmer, Indicating the Whitmer Methods for the Development of Individuality in

Piano, Musical Composition and Appreciation (Pittsburgh: Siviter & Co., 1917), 37.

Of the new instructional methods of composition, Whitmer states: “It is, of course, a

truism to say that an intimate knowledge of mental processes is essential to every instructor of

composition or anything else. Indeed, keeping abreast of all contemporaneous movements in

psychology is one of our first emphatic needs.” See Whitmer, The Way of My Mind (Pittsburgh:

Pierpont, Siviter & Co., 1917), 85.

In Whitmer’s 1917 text, however, the discussion of the metronome is palpably absent.

Whitmer may have recanted his radical metronome championing from the turn of the century

perhaps due to the musical effects heard through overusing the machine. He now calls for the

moderation of purely mechanical-technical music pedagogy. Commenting on the instructors of

his age, Whitmer conveniently distances himself from any particular school of thought,

proclaiming: “The sentimentalists in music have produced players who were all gush and little

technic; the materialists have given birth to ‘inferior pianolas.’” See Whitmer, The Way of My

Mind, 11.

102

Ibid., 30.

103

Also see U.S. Patents #618,336; #637,624; #619,857; #662,568. These inventions do not

include the improved metronome regulators for player pianos or pianolas that appear from 1893

through 1901.

Page 435: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

414

considered the metrical reference “a distraction” from the “regular sound of the beats.”104

Inventor Ernest Adams—patenting what seems to be one of the first electric metronomes

designed specifically for music instruction (#734,032)—defined the metronome as an

apparatus recently accepted in teaching, one that now ruled over a student’s daily practice

and rhythmic sense, stating:

In the study of music the metronome has come to be an important element not

only in determining the tempo that a certain composition shall be played at, but in

aiding the student in technical exercises to maintain a precision of rhythm in any

tempo from largo to presto. There is, however, a disadvantage in the present

marketable types, that consists in the necessity of stopping frequently and often in

the middle of a difficult passage to wind up the barrel-spring of the device.105

For Adams and the student musicians he exemplifies, the overriding purpose of the

metronome was now to provide automatical consistency throughout musical practice:

When the machine stopped, so did the child, until he wound-up the ticker again. With

Adams’ motorized metronome, the young student could now faithfully hear the redundant

sound of clockwork for every note of his daily practice routine and music

performances—all without dropping the “precision of rhythm” so valued in contemporary

laboratory and classroom chronography.

Yet, traditional techniques and tools had not completely disappeared in the face of

technical progress and scientific pursuits. As already explained, pedagogues of earlier

nineteenth-century traditions found silent, simple pendulums to reflect a less artificial

time—one that still afforded human variation, expression, and interpretive choice in both

practice and performance. Thus, music periodicals continued to run advertisements for

104

James Brady, “Metronome,” U.S. Patent #738,093 (Filed Apr. 5, 1902; Patented Sept. 1,

1903).

105

Ernest Adams, “Electric Metronome,” U.S. Patent #734,032 (Filed July 30, 1902; Patented

July 21, 1903): 5.

Page 436: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

415

simple pendulums into the twentieth century. These “pocket metronomes” offered an

easy pulse reference using Maelzel’s standard beats-per-minute scale, which dictated

through a finite swing the publisher’s initial tempo mark, while still affording the

musician his own artistic interpretations of pulse, rhythm, gesture, and movement. Music

periodicals sold and offered these pendulum metronomes as subscription prizes to their

readership. United States patents suggest a continued but waning interest for tempo

pendulums—with more precise and adjustable scales—into the twentieth century, as

witnessed in sporadic filings from 1886,106

1890, 1911, 1916, and 1922.107

The 1886

tempo-pendulum patent #360,550 was marketed to the American public for a time,

advertised in Etude magazine and other periodicals by 1888 as the “Zeckwer

Metronome.”108

106

Richard Zeckwer, “Metronome,” U.S. Patent #360,550 (Filed Dec. 22, 1886; Patented Apr. 5,

1887).

107

See U.S. Patents #449,635 by A. G. Pinfold (1890); #1,157,042 by C. Richmond (1911);

#1,191,285 by E. Cushing (1916); #1,548,738 by M. Pappas (1922).

108

See Zeckwer, 1. Also Etude VI (1888): 173, 193. Sibley Library ML e85 v6 1888 no12.

Page 437: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

416

Fig. 6.7. Front and side detail of U.S. Patent #360,550, filed on December, 22, 1886, known as the

“Zeckwer Metronome,” one of the more popular simple-pendulum metronomes marketed in late-

nineteenth-century America. Zeckwer’s silent tempo-pendulum incorporated Maelzel’s beats-per-minute

scale for use with music publications and etude books. It provided tempo numbers when referencing both

the half-swing and a complete oscillation.

As late as 1916, Clarence G. Hamilton, in his treatise How to Use the Metronome

Correctly, reported that antiquated simple pendulums—which now displayed “grave

disadvantages” compared to clockwork metronomes—were still used in very limited

settings, admitting: “even to the present day, the chronometer style of metronome is

sometimes employed for chorus work, especially in connection with the public

schools.”109

109 Clarence G. Hamilton, How to Use the Metronome Correctly (Philadelphia: Theodore Presser

Co., 1916), 4.

Page 438: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

417

Despite these remaining simple tempo-pendulum references, the majority of

metronome patent filings and new music periodicals attest that a host of instructors,

inventors, and publishers—including Hamilton, Whitmer, and Adams—found the

clockwork metronome to be the only temporal standard for a truly scientific music

education. While some teachers continued to cling to the old educational practices and

pendulums, Whitmer realized that even those modern instructors who first embraced the

clockwork metronome did not use the machine as consistently or as methodically as he

believed necessary. Indeed, with the recent wide-scale embrace of this clockwork

technology, personal discrepancies abounded in the application of the metronome; a

standard tradition of use had not fully surfaced by the turn of the century. In 1900,

Whitmer urged his readership to become more scientific (more precise) in the application

of the clockwork metronome as the temporal control for music education. He proclaims:

The metronome is, then, an all important factor in our plan. Its use has become

more and more general, but there seems to be no uniform treatment of it. Or,

rather, does its use have no vital principle involved. As our own "values" must be

made more vital so must the metronome be used more and more consistently and

uniformly in order to render absolute those values.110

For his part, Whitmer “uniformly” applied the metronome to the music student much like

a psychologist experimented on a subject. Whitmer instructs his musical audience:

In the first place calculate all note-values when used as units by the

auricular measuring of metronomic or natural accent.

In the second place make the metronomic unit correspond to the time-unit

and not, as is so often at present, with a unit greater or less than the time-unit.

The difficulty of measuring those impulses by the metronomic oscillation

is great, but then it is accurate. Unless this necessity be recognized as a first step

the vitality of our values will never be developed. If we desire to become sensitive

we must have manual, ocular and auricular problems to solve.111

110

Whitmer, “The Time-Marking System in Music,” 172-3.

111

Ibid., 173.

Page 439: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

418

Whitmer, writing in this “scientific” music periodical, makes clear that laboratory

methods ought to apply to all creative acts: rhythmical accuracy now meant metronomic

mimicry. Whitmer called for a new musical education, in which the “problems to solve”

involved normalizing the personal equation. The metronome, then, offered a “vital”

reference for a subject’s problematic rhythmic sense, which must be made scientifically

“consistent,” “accurate,” and “uniform.” (It seems no coincidence that Whitmer himself

was a subject of musical-psychological experiments in the University of Missouri

laboratory, as reported in Max Meyer’s “Experimental Studies in the Psychology of

Music” (1903) in the American Journal of Psychology).112

Indeed, Whitmer, a pedagogue

completely swayed by the chronographic bias, suggested that if the metronome could also

provide “accurate” emotions, the human musician would be well advised to abide by the

machine’s unerring reference:

We have no moodal metronome and so a word becomes of use to indicate a

certain spirit or mood — when once the grammatical accents are decided by our

metronome. But away with all those useless French, German, Italian and English

names. A piece of music is too polyglot.113

To perform with a semblance of spirit, Whitmer contends, one must first use the

clockwork metronome to decide the rhythm. For Whitmer, these imprecise (now

“useless”) verbal signifiers—which, as Hummel noted as early as 1828, held clues to

112

Max Meyer, “Experimental Studies in the Psychology of Music,” The American Journal of

Psychology 14 (Jul.-Oct., 1903): 192-214. Whitmer’s comments appear 210-12.

113 Whitmer, “The Time Marking System in Music,” Music XIX (Apr., 1901): 123.

Page 440: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

419

more complex, nuanced, and emotional interpretations114

—could not compare to the strict

rule of mechanical guidance, for either musical time or expression.

James Matthew McLaughlin, Director of Music for Boston Public Schools, in his

Elements & Notation of Music (1902), further uncovers the conflicting relationship

between past rhythmical values and present applications of the clockwork metronome,

made apparent through Whitmer’s scientific pedagogy. McLaughlin seems to straddle

two epistemologies of musical time: one subjective and internalized, the other mechanical

and externalized. He recounts the importance of the simple pendulum115

along with the

physical sense of meter116

taught by A.B. Marx, Riemann, Lussy, and others from the

nineteenth century. He paraphrases these past pedagogues’ realizations that the

metronome only provides “the average rate of speed at which the composition should be

taken; for music, in the course of its expression of the emotions and its revelation of the

free action of the mind, would at once become spiritless and uninspiring if fettered by

arbitrary mathematical distinctions in time-measurement.”117

Nevertheless, McLaughlin

incorporates new teaching methods of musical time that contradict these pre-metronomic

educators and their traditional values of musical performance; McLaughlin’s new

instructions are nearly identical to psychological memory experiments of the age:

114

For his discussion of Italian affect, movement, and tempo words, see J. N. Hummel,

Ausführlich theoretisch-practische Anweisung zum Piano-forte Spiel, I (Wien: T. Haslinger,

1828), 5th

Kapitel, 66-68.

115

James M. McLaughlin, Elements & Notation of Music (Boston: Ginn & Company, 1902), 78-

79.

116

Ibid., 21-24.

117

Ibid., 78.

Page 441: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

420

304. How to memorize q=60. Guided by the second-hand of a watch, count the seconds as follows:

One-thousand one, one-thousand two, one-thousand three, etc. With a little practice the speed of this rhythm can be readily recalled when the accented syllables, one, two, three, etc., will indicate the value of each quarter-note in the standard tempo q=60. From this, other tempi may be at least approximated.

305. To discover the rate of speed of a composition during its performance count

the number of beats in a quarter of a minute and multiply by four.118

As Vierordt, Wundt, Ebbinghaus and other experimental psychologists well understood,

one must learn to react, memorize, and repeat in the rhythm of precision clockwork in

order to perform like clockwork. The writings of McLaughlin, Whitmer, Henry, and Karr

suggest that this peculiar training within metronomic “rhythm” became the normal course

of a student’s education, one that focused on habitual and mechanical repetition above

personal insight or perception. Even in athletics—most conspicuously in the quasi-

militaristic field of gymnastics—metronomic regulation continued to be a highly prized

aspect of instruction, which further extended to the “rhythmic-gymnastics” of Jaques-

Dalcroze. In The Training of the Body for Games, Athletics, Gymnastics, and Other

Forms of Exercise and for Health, Growth, and Development (1901), medical doctor F.

A. Schmidt prescribes that,

You should always practise the expected and regular before the unexpected and

sudden. That is to say, you should let a movement become a habit, a regular habit,

before you use it in an emergency. Therefore at the outset Rhythm is very

important. Later on, I suggest a Metronome, or perhaps a Phonograph, as a great

help for the maintenance of Rhythm. For, if you can see or hear anything keeping

time, your motions will be far more rhythmical.119

118

Ibid., 80.

119

F. A. Schmidt and Eustace Hamilton Miles, The Training of the Body for Games, Athletics,

Gymnastics, and Other Forms of Exercise and for Health, Growth, and Development (New York:

E. P. Dutton & Co., 1901), 7.

Page 442: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

421

Guided by this rapidly emerging culture where automatical machines defined the absolute

“Rhythm” of human movement, schoolroom and private teachers embraced mechanized

teaching methods, in part because they inculcated seemingly efficient habits upon their

child-students. And habitual metronomic training—not skilled “artistic” performance—

was the prevailing, positive value in a youth’s modern education according to

experimental psychologists of the age.

In the years corresponding to the rise of metronomic music training, child-study

experiments continued to quantify a range of performance abilities in students. Ellis and

Shipe tested children’s reading fatigue in average words per second, as dictated by a

metronome, measured down to the thousands of a second.120

As related in The American

Journal of Psychology (1904), Binet tested a subject’s intelligence through the “Accuracy

in Counting Metronome Taps and in Counting Dots.”121

The Outlines of Educational

Psychology: An Introduction to the Science of Education (1911) by William Henry Pyle

offered an additional intelligence experiment that tested memory retention and attention

span while reading in the classroom. “While one paragraph is being read,” Pyle instructs,

“distractions may be furnished by a metronome and an electric bell.”122

Patent filings for new clockwork metronomes continued to appear in the first

decade of the new century. No less than two inventions from 1908 strikingly conflate the

metronome with those now-popular timekeepers of the urban-industrial world; patents by

120

Ellis and Shipe, 240-1.

121

C. Spearman, “’General Intelligence’ Objectively Determined and Measured,” The American

Journal of Psychology 15 (Apr., 1904): 217.

122

William Henry Pyle, The Outlines of Educational Psychology: An Introduction to the Science

of Education (Baltimore: Warkick & York, Inc., 1911), 203.

Page 443: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

422

the American Charles White (#923,094) and the Swiss inventor Henri Coullery

(#1,058,659) depict metronomes in the form of the ubiquitous pocket watch. Each patent

suggests that Maelzel’s (rather Winkel’s) old design, with its seemingly superfluous

pendulum, was inefficient when compared with the sleek portability of these new,

automatical pocket-watch metronomes.123

While these particular designs found little

acceptance with manufacturers, their patent texts clearly endorse the new instructional

values for the modern youth. Metronome “improvements” such as these continued to

offer greater and more precise automatism, while the sound of clockwork “rhythm”

completely replaced the vision of a pendulum swing. Indeed, modern technology

continued to efface the simple pendulum, not to mention the human body itself, as the

predominant reference for musical time of previous centuries.

Fig. 6.8. Detail of U.S. metronome patent #923,094 by C. A. White & E. R. Hunter. Filed on October

20, 1908, this modern metronome—which now completely lacks the visual pulse reference of a swinging

pendulum—has an outward appearance and internal movement that is virtually indistinguishable from a

contemporaneous pocket watch or stopwatch. This and other “improved” clockwork metronomes attest to

the rising influence of the chronographic method—and the increasing neglect of silent, visual, and finite

tempo references—in twentieth-century music education and performance.

123

See Henri Coullery, “Metronome,” U.S. Patent #1,058,659 (Filed Feb. 24, 1908; Patented Apr.

8, 1913): 4; and Charles White, “Metronome,” U.S. Patent #923,094 (Filed Oct. 20, 1908;

Patented May 25, 1909): 4.

Page 444: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

423

Despite some inefficiencies and inaccuracies when compared to more popular

time-telling machines like the pocket watch, Maelzel’s clockwork metronome continued

to thrive in the modern musical marketplace. A slew of music publications increasingly

promoted the machine to the novice musician as a necessary companion for newly

published etudes and method books that stressed technical achievement above all. For the

sake of musical precision, proponents of the metronome unreservedly urged the young

music student to use the machine regularly and consistently, while the silent “non-

scientific” simple pendulum seemed to disappear from pedagogical discourse entirely.

For example, under the definition of “string pendulum,” the editor of the Musical

Dictionary of the American History and Encyclopedia of Music (1910) simply prints,

“see metronome” (the clockwork variety), while completely bypassing his own printed

citation for the nineteenth-century “Weber chronometer” (an actual string pendulum).124

The clockwork metronome, as opposed to the simple pendulum, became essential

in the piano-training methods of J. Alfred Johnstone, who published a slew of texts on

music technique and interpretation between 1908 and 1927.125

Yet Johnstone, similar to

many technically oriented pedagogues, seemed to conflate the concepts of creative

musical interpretation with chronographic training, as once-expressive gestures became

solely ruled by metronomic time. Louis Charles Elson’s piano-centric Modern Music and

Musicians (1918) recounts Johnstone’s modern, mechanized pedagogy:

124

W. L. Hubbard, ed. Musical Dictionary, in series The American History and Encyclopedia

(New York: Irving Squire, 1910): 501, 512, 575.

125

See for instance, J. Alfred Johnstone, Touch, Phrasing, and Interpretation (London: W.

Reeves, 1900); J. Alfred Johnstone, Essentials in piano-playing and other musical studies

(London: W. Reeves, 1914).

Page 445: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

424

The slow trill of two notes is repeated thirty to forty times with each pair of

fingers, at a metronome rate of 40 to 60 for each note. The trill is taken at its

slowest at first, and it is even practicable to let the student rest a beat between

each note for a time…The slow trill is to be practiced continually, with the

metronome mark raised in later lessons until 96 is reached…as the pupil grows

more and more proficient, he may take double notes or triplets, with each beat,

then with each half-beat, and so on. But speed should never be increased unless

the movements are kept correct.126

Since scale passages [in actual compositions] often occur in varying

rhythms, Johnston advocates the use of a metronome, and the playing of many

rhythmic figures, such as an eight-note followed by two sixteenths, and eighth-

note followed by a triplet of sixteenths, and so on.127

Broken chords are emphasized as being a good preparation for arpeggios

[in actual compositions]…Each should be repeated from ten to twenty times, with

the metronome.128

As Johnstone’s exercises show, the clockwork machine offered modern methods of

training musical action, now anachronistically transferred to pre-metronomic musical

techniques of trilling, arpeggiation, and scale runs. Johnstone’s redundant, precise, and

automatical training techniques could not be actualized with the simple-pendulum

“metronome” of the previous century, not to mention the subjective pulse-sense.

But, as their own pedagogies expose, modern music educators seemed

unconcerned with past practices or the questionable aesthetic effects arising from their

progressive, mechanically aided teaching methods. In another example from the

American History and Encyclopedia of Music, the article “Letters from a Musician” in

the volume The Essentials of Music (1910) depicts a seemingly experienced pedagogue

who advised his young nephew (a common middle-class music student) on the superior

benefits of metronomic-musical training. “I wish to start you out on your musical studies

126

Arthur Elson, “Modern Piano Methods,” in Modern Music and Musicians, Louis Charles

Elson, ed., vol. I (New York: The University Society, Inc., 1918), 70.

127

Ibid., 72.

128

Ibid., 72-3.

Page 446: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

425

with a wholesome idea about the metronome,” the writer Edward Morris Bowman

confides, while acknowledging the prominent caveats against the machine that he has

obviously heard in the past:

Possibly Miss Proctor will say to you: “O, don't practise with that thing; it will

make your playing mechanical.” Or some one else will exclaim: “My! No artist

ever plays with a metronome, or like one, either!”

Another will say: “I would not have one in the house; makes me so

nervous to hear it!” “I tried it once, and I could not keep with it at all. That thing

is of no use whatever!”

Now, just listen to your uncle. A good metronome is one of the greatest

helps that you can use. Get one immediately, use it for certain purposes right

along, until it is worn out and then — buy another. Never be without one. They

are made with and without a bell.129

With the unbridled zeal of a metronome salesman, Bowman summarizes and then

discounts all of the valid invectives against the clockwork metronome that had appeared

over the preceding decades: Maelzel’s machine reduces musical meaning in time; skilled,

creative professionals (i.e. “artists”) did not accept it; the metronome creates mechanical

qualities of performance never intended by composers; one must be trained to act with

such artificial qualities; furthermore, the machine creates a mental distraction, altering the

user’s time-sense through habitual repetition. Despite these well-worn rejections, this

writer found the clockwork metronome—a machine that he suggests is still uncommon in

129

Edward Morris Bowman, “Letters from a Musician to His Nephew,” in The Essentials of

Music, Volume 1, Emil Liebling, editor; in series The American History and Encyclopedia, ed.

W. L. Hubbard (New York: Irving Squire, 1910), 154.

As the text states, Bowman, among many accomplishments, “is Dr. Mason’s successor as

a teacher of piano.” Mason’s novel technical exercises, as related by Music metronome ads of the

late 1890s, were some of the first pedagogical etudes specifically paired with the clockwork

metronome.

Page 447: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

426

the “provinces”130

—to be the most important tool of a child’s instruction for instilling a

“normal” sense of rhythm.

Bowman’s essay continued to promote the metronome solely for its mechanical

precision, as he drastically revised past conceptions of musical time in favor of greater

chronographic methods. Indeed, he divorced the metronome ever further from the

reflection of human rhythm, stating that the “bell is not of much value.” The bell, as

many recognized, was the only way of distinguishing the once-physical phenomenon of

metric pulse within the redundant clockwork clicks. (Indeed, only fifteen years prior, a

Christian Science Journal writer considered the metronome’s bell divinely infallible).

Yet “Uncle” Bowman seemed to exclusively value incessant clockwork monotony,

stating, “The chief value of the metronome is in the regular tick of the machine, rather

than in the tap of the bell.”131

By disavowing both the swing of the pendulum and the

sound of the meter-bell, this novel music educator—similar to some inventors of the

age—regarded musical time as a mere series of automatical sounds with no remnant of

physical rhythm or visual movement whatsoever.

It seems that throughout his career E. M. Bowman was one of the most vocal

champions of the clockwork metronome for the average American student’s musical

education. Published two decades prior to his “metronome letter,” the Bowman’s-

Weitzman’s Manual of Music Theory (1879) pleaded for greater regularity in tempo

interpretation, stating: “The relative values of notes always remain the same, but the

130

He states: “The quick exercise will require the use of a metronome. I suppose now that you do

not have metronomes up at Barton. No, you could not plow with them or cut hay or slide down a

hill on them.” See Bowman, 153.

131

Ibid., 154.

Page 448: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

427

absolute values change with every change in tempo. The Metronome, invented by

Maelzel, indicates the tempo with mathematical exactness, and it is to be regretted that it

is not more generally used.”132

Through each successive decade in the twentieth century, it seems, the

metronome became applied in music education with ever-increasing chronographic

values. By 1910, Bowman had valued the machine as a purely objective, scientific

reference and regulator of musical time. Among the many reasons that the writer placed

“so much value on this appliance,” the industrial and scientific values of efficiency, cost,

and habitual regulation stand out:

The metronome will give you an exact standard of meter and rhythm, and help to

train your sense of time… Unless there is a strong natural talent in time-keeping,

it is a long and, of course, expensive task to train a pupil to correct habits in this

respect.

The use of a metronome establishes in a short time, even in otherwise

discouraging cases, a standard of time-sense. The metronome is a cold-blooded

machine. It works the same way every day. It never gets excited or discouraged. It

does not balk or run away.133

Being a good “timeist” in the nineteenth century was always considered an innate,

subjective musical talent, yet in this emerging pedagogy where scientific methods

trumped individuality, metronomic time now regulated expressive techniques, “rhythm,”

“time-sense,” along with musical movements and fluctuations once intuited by skilled

performers alone. As with child study, the metronome’s purpose in music, according to

Bowman and other proponents, was to habitually drill an artificial and normalizing time-

sense into children. An education in thoughtful, individual, or insightful interpretation

132

Carl Friederich Weitzman and E. M. Bowman, Bowman’s-Weitzman’s Manual of Music

Theory a Concise, Comprehensive and Practical Text-Book on the Science of Music (New York:

Wm. A. Pond & Co., 1897), 17.

133 Bowman, “Letters from a Musician to His Nephew,” 155.

Page 449: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

428

seemed less of a consideration.134

Indeed, the benefits of the metronome were evident: it

provided efficient and cost-effective instruction, two predominant values of modern,

“scientifically managed” labor training. And while “uncle” refrained from considering

the cold-blooded metronome to be godly, as The Christian Science Journal correspondent

did fifteen years prior, Bowman’s words imply that the metronome’s inhuman,

controlling rhythm was in fact infallible, especially when teaching unmusical children.

Indeed, he later instructed his nephew to habitually practice once-subjective techniques of

accelerando and ritardando under perfect clockwork guidance, realizing, “Mechanically

safe and systematic work of this character cannot be done without a metronome.”135

Even

while championing the device, Bowman unwittingly concurred with skilled musician-

“artists” of the age: the metronome begets mechanical behavior in musical performance.

The Recollection of Musical Time Past

As these modern metronome advocates suggested, the machine generalized or normalized

musical time with objective, reproducible certainties. But in the process, the clockwork

metronome re-standardized the meaning of rhythm within chronographic goals and

scientific values. Thus, the personal equation of the musician, which reflected subjective

134

After relating the importance of this metronomic regulation in pedagogy, the writer realizes

that printed metronome marks themselves are often impractical, since there are many

discrepancies between editions of the same composition. He suggests taking the average of

several different publications to derive the “exact” speed intend by composers. Ibid., 156.

135

Bowman realized, however, that the metronomic yoke must eventually be broken in order to

achieve expressive playing, but only after the student has been thoroughly trained in the values of

mechanical perfection: “If the time is found to be correct and the music can be played up to the

proper speed, the metronome should not be used further with that selection, as the piece is now

ready to be played in that flexible style which is demanded by the laws of expression and

emotional delivery. To practise with the metronome while studying expression is, of course, a

misuse of the little machine. Expressive playing constantly varies, more or less, in speed. In such

playing, the metronome is not to be used.” Ibid., 155-6.

Page 450: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

429

expression itself, became a mere relationship between mechanical speeds, rates, and

reactions. By the twentieth century, the scientific discourse of musical time had revolved

around a simplified, metronomic standard of action, as the new music educators had

defined children’s technical training and rhythmical abilities solely through clockwork

movement.

Those musicians trained in pre-metronomic performance practices and teaching

methods heard and criticized the effect: the continual clockwork click erased a student’s

subjective perception of musical time. Speaking at the Music Teachers National

Association Conference in 1907, Kate S. Chittenden, from the American Institute of

Applied Music in New York, remembered the onset of this new scientific pedagogy,

proselytized by periodicals such as Music and the Musical Visitor and accepted by music

instructors across the continent. Chittenden confirmed that this habitual metronomic-

musical training began, as the present survey suggests, only at the end of the nineteenth

century:

Piano-teaching has changed so much in the last fifteen years that grade work in

one sense is not so hard to arrange as formerly; that is, if one be governed by

technic exercises played with the click of the metronome — so many notes in a

trill, in scale, in arpeggio, octaves, chords, etc. (which implies a hand-position and

good fingers). It was a great boon to piano-teachers and students when this minute

system was condensed and presented to the public, simplifying as well as making

thorough this one branch of pianistic teaching.136

Chittenden too recognized that the metronome was an efficient aid for mass education;

the metronome simplified pedagogy—reducing, regulating time and movement to

136

Kate S. Chittenden, “Report of Piano Conference,” in Studies in Musical Education History

and Aesthetics, Papers and Proceedings of the Music Teachers’ National Association (December

27-31, 1907): 144-145.

Page 451: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

430

reproducible certainties. The metronome made teaching easy.137

Yet she also realized that

the machine reduced the subjective perceptions of musical expression to mechanized

clicks. Like so many skilled performers, she pined for the nuanced, individualistic, and

subjective music education of the pre-metronomic past, stating, “I would myself rather go

back and take Christiani's order of requisites: talent first; then emotion, the aesthetic

element; intelligence, the scientific element; and last, technic, the mechanical, in its

proper place.”138

In modern piano pedagogy—increasingly distanced from the culture and

aesthetics of Christiani— she found some new teaching methods to be “built rather too

much on the dangerous metronomic basis of mere exercises which develop machines.”139

For Chittenden, a musical pedagogue of former traditions, the metronome clearly created

automatical students, reducing performance values to scientific precisions and

techniques, reducing musical meaning to clockwork. She recognized, like many

musicians of the past century, that the efficiency and ease afforded by this new

metronomic pedagogy did not equate to living musicality. Yet systematized performing

methods, technique-based etude books, and clockwork metronomes had become the

pedagogical controls for a normalizing education that reduced creativity and

137

This reductive pedagogy appears in the contemporaneous Half-hour Lessons in Music, in

which all external, objective time-timekeeping is conflated for simplified explanations of musical

tempo and rhythm. It instructs the young novice: “’Now time in music, children, is expressed by

steady, even counts, or beats…We reckon time by the clock in seconds and minutes, and the

metronome ticks in exactly the same way as the clock…Each tick of the clock means one

second—each beat of the metronome means one quarter.”

Without further discussion on the sensation or perception of internal pulse and

movement, the pedagogue finally states: “Remember, when you learn to play, use the metronome

only a moment, to show you what time was in the composer’s mind. Never depend on it wholly to

keep steady time.” See Mary Ann Torrey Kotzschmar, Half-hour Lessons in Music (Boston:

Oliver Ditson Co., 1907), 13-14.

138 Chittenden, 145.

139

Ibid., 146.

Page 452: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

431

individuality, seemingly for an entire generation of musical amateurs. Chittenden

admitted outright that, “This is the hardest task of the teacher, to round up into some

scheme a proper selection [of pedagogical pieces] which shall admit of a chance of

individualism in the pupil (for, thankfully we can say, they are not all automatoms.

[sic.])”140

The most renowned performers, the musical “artists” of Western music continued

to express an equal resentment of the effects of mechanical regulation on human

creativity, even through the first decade of the twentieth century. In his book Success in

Music and How it is Won (1909), Henry Theophius Finck wrote of Ignacy Jan

Paderewski (1860-1941), one of the last performers of pre-metronomic traditions, that his

musicality reflected an individual spirit, one in opposition to the redundant, normalizing

rhythm that currently informed every action of the young piano student:

140

Ibid., 145. As early as 1854, Adolf Bernhard Marx, too, chastised the growing wave of ill-

equipped music teachers in their attempts to train German middle-class musical amateurs through

routine and regulative methods, in which ultimately, “Art is made mechanical, and as a

mechanism transmitted to the people.” In The Music of the Nineteenth Century and Its Culture,

Marx—prefiguring both Christiani and Chittenden’s realizations—continued his diatribe against

the consumer-driven culture of music production with its anti-artistic, standardized methods of

musical instruction:

“Compare the never-ending exercises of our myriads of amateurs, virtuosi and would-be

virtuosi on the piano, with the number of real works of art with which the learner is made

acquainted, putting artistic comprehension and performance altogether out of the

question. And, finally, inquire how few are rewarded with adequate success for all their

toils and sacrifices; and how many, on the other hand, after years of laborious study,

either give up music altogether, or never advance a step beyond their last lesson. After

such an examination, every one will concede that unless these far-extended studies and

exercises can be made to yield greater and nobler results, or the labour and time of

preparation reduced to a fair proportion, with the attainable success, the practice of

music, instead of benefiting mankind, is only a means of culpably squandering away our

time and money, and our nervous energy.

So far, however, are most people from seeing this, that they absolutely mistake

the means for the end. One might show this in every sphere of musical practice, but no

where so plainly as in the wide-spreading branch of pianoforte-playing.” See Marx, 75.

Page 453: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

432

No matter what Paderewski plays, he usually seems to be improvising, to follow

the inspiration of the moment, to create the music while he performs it. His

playing is the negation of the mechanical in music, the assassination of the

metronome.141

In Finck’s text, Paderewski himself defined the vast distinction between subjective

musical time of the past and the objective, scientific time of the present. He employed a

technical analogy to prove his point: the metronome was intended for a world of science,

not the creative performance arts. Speaking from experience, Paderewski explains:

Our human metronome, the heart, under the influence of emotion, ceases to beat

regularly—physiology calls it arythmia…To be emotional in musical

interpretation, yet obedient to the initial tempo and true to the metronome, means

about as much as being sentimental in engineering. Mechanical execution and

emotion are incompatible.142

Paderewski too acknowledged that the metronome could be useful for unmusical

children, but he made clear that no nineteenth-century composer conceived of musical

time with scientific regulation, stating that “a composer’s imagination and an interpreter’s

emotion are not bound to be the humble slaves of either metronome or tempo.”143

Indeed

the renowned pianist hurled a special insult upon those who slavishly perceived music to

141

Henry T. Finck, Success in Music and How it is Won (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons,

1909), 321.

In a telling comparison from the pro-scientific periodical Music, Emil Liebling criticizes

Paderewski for his non-mechanical musical qualities. In the process, Liebling presents his own

modern, metronomic aesthetic: “Perhaps the most reprehensible liberties are taken with rhythms

and tempos. In this regard Paderewski has much to answer for. Without desiring to apply the

metronome to his performances, it yet is undeniable that he hardly ever preserves the

[metronomic] rhythm of a movement long enough to give the listener an absolute idea of

[metronomic] time.” See Emil Liebling, “A Pianistic Retrospect,” Music I (Nov., 1891-Apr.,

1892): 585.

142

Finck, 455.

143

Ibid., 456.

Page 454: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

433

move in mechanical time, especially in Beethoven’s works. Such misguided pedagogues

and performers were “the tempo-sticklers and metronome-believers.”144

The Expansion of Metronome Believing and Metronomic Training

in Twentieth-Century Society

As Paderewski’s comments imply, the modern world, now structured in precise,

synchronized mechanical time, and the musical culture of the past were in conflict. Yet

mechanical execution was everywhere in the new century: the metronome now imbued

youth education, child study, experimental psychology, and industry with the same

incessant clockwork rhythm. In the second decade of the twentieth century, many

experimental psychologists “believed in” the machine more than ever, and they continued

to test public school children guided by the values and rhythm of metronomic time. The

Conservation of a Child (1912) by psychologist Arthur Holmes described and

photographed a variety of child studies that incorporated the metronome and ergograph,

“an instrument for measuring muscular co-ordination, attention, and power of

endurance.”145

In the 1914 publication Mental and Physical Tests: A book of directions

compiled with special reference to the experimental study of school children in the

laboratory or classroom by Guy Montrose Whipple, the metronome quantified and

regulated “physical and motor capacity” and “endurance of grip”146

in children.

Whipple’s text provides a highly detailed image of the arm-clamping machine with the

144

Ibid., 456.

145

Arthur Holmes, The Conservation of the Child (Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott Company,

1912), insert after 230.

146

Guy Montrose Whipple, Manual of Mental and Physical Tests (Baltimore: Warwick & York,

Inc., 1914), 116-7.

Page 455: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

434

ticking metronome-reference. Further aiming experiments consisted of children striking

“in time with the beat of the metronome, following a two-beat rhythm, so that the pencil

hits the target at the one beat and is drawn back at the next”147

at 138 beats-per-minute.

Fig. 6.9. A standard ergograph for endurance tests on laborers and students. The Maelzel metronome,

regulating the tempo of the performance, appears at the far right corner of the chronographic apparatus.

From Guy Montrose Whipple, Manual of Mental and Physical Tests (Baltimore: Warwick & York, Inc.,

1914), 121.

In 1914 the Bucknel Foundation in California sponsored tests to quantify “Mental

Deviation Among Children” using the now familiar “apparatus and materials necessary

for physical and mental tests, including scales, stadiometer, dynamometer, kymograph,

Marey tambour, spirometer, tachistoscope, metronome, head calipers, anthropometric

tape, target test, vision tests, and an assortment of printed forms for use in intelligence

testing. The value of the equipment is approximately $800.”148

By 1920 the New York

City school system had devised child-studies for many psychological measures, including

the memorization of numbers in metronomic time. Using a series of cards with digits, the

147

Ibid., 149-150.

148

Lewis M. Terman, Research in Mental Deviation Among Children (Stanford: Stanford

University, 1915), 14.

Page 456: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

435

experimenter described the process to the student-subject, gently saying, “I’ll start this

little ticker going to show us how fast to read the numbers. (Start the metronome at rate

of 60.) See, we say a number every time it ticks. (Illustrate.)”149

It seems the industrial world was not concerned with Paderewski’s criticisms

either. The training systems devised by noted Harvard psychologist Hugo Münsterberg

also relied on the metronome to measure and regulate the activities school children,

students, factory laborers, and office workers in the twentieth-century. Münsterberg’s

work seemingly upheld a mantra that labor-mastermind Frederick Winslow Taylor first

proclaimed in his preface to The Principles of Scientific Management (1911): “In the past

the man has been first; in the future the system must be first.”150

In 1913 the New York Times article “New Science Suggested to Solve Social

Problems” summarized Münsterberg’s labor methods, reiterating the now-prevalent

belief: “There is already far-reaching agreement that the problems of artistic creation, of

scientific observation, of social reform, and many similar endeavors must be

acknowledged as organic parts of applied psychology.”151

For psychologists the

“problems” of human activity, which included “artistic creation,” were actually

“problems” of individual variation. These problems required, according to psychologists,

chronographic methods and machines to both register and reduce the variations of

149

Evelyn Dewey, Emily Child, and Beardsley Ruml, Methods and Results of Testing School

Children (New York: E. P. Dutton & Company, 1920), 58-9.

150

Frederick Winslow Taylor, The Principles of Scientific Management (New York: Harper &

Brothers, 1911; Reprint, Mineola: Dover, 1998), iv.

151

“New Science Suggested to Solve Social Problems,” New York Times, Feb 9, 1913, SM10.

Further details of this labor study are found in Margarete Anna Adelheid Münsterberg, Hugo

Münsterberg (New York: D. Appleton and Company, 1922), 213-214.

Page 457: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

436

personal equations to mechanical certainty, for all activities of industrialized society.

Thus, outside of the Harvard laboratory, Münsterberg applied the standard aiming tests to

the work of telephone operators, where the metronome controlled the subjects’ individual

hand-eye movements when operating a pencil. Such tests found an employee’s “psychical

rhythm,” their ability to perform efficiently in the prescribed time of the metronome.152

While chronographic methods became infused into modern culture and industry,

astronomy too did not forget the scientific origins of the metronome as objective

regulator of the observer’s stellar perceptions. Sarah Frances Whiting’s book Daytime

and Evening Exercises in Astronomy, for Schools and Colleges (1912) instructed the

student astronomer on how to act and react with metronomic accuracy, stating, “Practice

with the transit, clock, and chronograph may be gained by using a chronograph to find

the time of the beat of a metronome to the tenth of a second.”153

The same precisions

sought with difficulty by professional astronomers in the early nineteenth century through

the eye-and-ear method eventually became standardized practices, easily acquired by

university students using the objective rhythm of the now ubiquitous clockwork

metronome.

Among their seemingly infinite uses for the scientific metronome, psychologists

devised a learning experiment in 1913 where a subject repeatedly ordered a deck of cards

152

“New Science Suggested to Solve Social Problems,” New York Times, Feb. 9, 1913, SM10.

Systematic typing in “words per minute” likely owes something to the metronomic

training practices used to increase efficiency and productivity in office employment.

153 Sarah Frances Whiting, Daytime and Evening Exercises in Astronomy, for Schools and

Colleges (Boston: Ginn and Company, 1912), 51 n1.

Page 458: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

437

strictly guided by metronomic time.154

In 1918, The United States Military found the

metronome to be the most effective temporal regulator for certain motion studies on

aviator pilots.155

By 1913 some surgeons had used the metronome at the rate of 100

beats-per-minute to “rhythmically apply” electric shocks in the treatment of obesity.156

A

1927 patent filing documents a physiology apparatus similar to those used in nineteenth-

century operations on frogs; it seems that the metronome continued to provide a temporal

control for artificial respiration, but as Robert Swope’s invention suggests, an improved

metronome-breather could be successfully applied to the human subject.157

Metronomic reading, writing, and speaking continued well through the first half

of the twentieth century as now-standardized practices in the modern, scientific

education. In 1910 the New York Times advocated for writing with metronome to

strengthen penmanship and attain scientific precision in action. To execute these newly

devised writing drills,

154

Mildred Browning, Dorothy E. Brown, and M. F. Washburn, “The Effect of the Interval

between Repetitions on the Speed of Learning a Series of Movements,” The American Journal of

Psychology 24 (Oct., 1913): 580-583, 580.

The researchers explain: “The cards were numbered on the back. The observer, always

with eyes closed, sat holding the pack, her hands above a mark on the table against which the

center of the diagram rested. The experimenter took hold of her right wrist, and moved her right

hand, to the time of a metronome, in such a way as to sort out the pack of cards correctly on the

diagram, putting each card in the compartment bearing the letter which corresponded to the letter

on the card.”

155

War Department, Medical Research Laboratory (Washington D.C.: Government Printing

Office, 1918), 189

156

Sinclair Tousey, Medical Electricity, Röntgen Rays, and Radium (Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders

Company, 1921), 428.

157

Robert Swope, “Resuscitation and Artificial Respiration Apparatus,” U.S. Patent #1,848,232

(Filed Sept. 29, 1927; Patented Mar. 8, 1932).

Page 459: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

438

It is required that these should be made in one minute. This is about regular

practice speed. The regularity of movement is as carefully watched as in music. A

jerky, spasmodic motion may be found reflected in the letters. In some cases a

metronome or musical instrument is employed, while others merely beating time

with a ruler or chalk box.158

The 1922 University of Chicago publication, The Elementary School Journal, defines a

far more precise regimen in “A Course in Handwriting:”

The metronome is introduced for the first time at the beginning of the second

month. Up to this time the children have not sufficient control to enable them to

follow the beat of a mechanical instrument. The teacher must indicate the count

with her voice and vary it to suit the capacities of the children. Even now the

children will have to be accustomed somewhat gradually to the regular beat. The

rate of 50 beats per minute is suggested to begin with.159

When performing the dramas of Shakespeare, too, the metronome became a new

rhythmic standard. Clayton Meeker Hamilton, in Studies in Stagecraft (1914), suggests

that playwrights and directors “take a scene from Hamlet and another from The

Thunderbolt and ask themselves precisely how rapidly or slowly these passages should be

played in order to achieve their best effect upon the stage. Let them, if necessary,

experiment with a metronome until they get the rhythm right.”160

Metronomic training in

precise “rhythmic” gradations continued to inform the modern cultural education well

into the twentieth century. As late as 1938, a Vice President of the University of Chicago

had proclaimed in The Scientific Monthly, “Rhythmic Reading is good reading. A

158

“For Muscular Writing, A New Method of Penmanship and Why It Is Popular as Well as

Strenuous,” New York Times, Mar. 6, 1910, SM12.

159

Frank N. Freeman, “A Course in Handwriting: II,” Elementary School Journal XXII (Sept.,

1921-June, 1922): 445.

160

Clayton Meeker Hamilton, Studies in Stagecraft (New York: Henry Hold and Company,

1914), 193.

Page 460: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

439

metronome, or even beating time with the hand, helps at first in acquiring smooth, even

eye jumps.”161

Yet, as many over the last century recognized, a machine that ingrains “precision”

also restricts and reduces personal variation. Thus Robert Robertson Rusk, in

Experimental Education (1919), reflected upon the drawbacks of instructing students to

write though these modern, mechanical techniques,162

stating, “Children display fewer

independent impulses than when writing without the [metronomic] beat. The compulsion

to [mechanically] rhythmical writing thereby induced causes the child's writing

artificially to approximate to that of the adult. Writing to a beat has on the whole a

marked influence on handwriting…It is consequently important that the tempo of writing

to the beat should not be made too quick…Into the exact psychological and pathological

analysis of writing we cannot here enter, but as writing, like speech, is a form of

expression.”163

Rusk implied, as Paderewski and Chittenden before him, that individual

expression—the personal equation so derided by psychologists—was thwarted by

objective metronomic regulation. The metronome imposed an artificial “rhythmical”

161

William Burnett Benton, “Speeding Up Our Reading,” The Scientific Monthly 47 (Sept.,

1938): 263.

162

In 1914 text, The Teaching of Handwriting (Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1914),

Frank N. Freeman was less dogmatic about the external time source for classroom instruction,

stating, “The time is usually marked by counting, making a series of raps with a ruler, handclaps,

or metronome beats. A still better method of indicating tempo, which has long been used for

marching, dancing, gymnastic exercises, etc., is music.” See Freeman, The Teaching of

Handwriting, 105.

This statement contrasts with his more precise indications found in “A Course in

Handwriting: II” from the 1922 Elementary School Journal XXII.

163

Robert Robertson Rusk, Experimental Education (London: Longmans, Green and Co., 1919),

287.

Page 461: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

440

value upon a child’s performance, merely “approximating” a natural and nuanced rhythm

learned only through personal experience.

Comprehensive, Automatical Pedagogies

Despite Rusk’s realization, the new aesthetic of habituation and automatism, described in

1904 by psychologist and educator G. Stanley Hall, took hold in general education, as

students increasingly learned to speak, write, and perform by chronographic standards.

The most conspicuous and influential proponent of this scientific trend of “experimental

education” was Eugene Jaques-Dalcroze. His new pedagogy, Eurhythmics or “rhythmic-

gymnastics,” seemed to impart the child-student with a precise, externalized rhythm

exploited by physiologists and experimental psychologists decades before him.164

In 1905

164

Jaques-Dalcroze readily acknowledged that Eurhythmics was necessary, in large part, due to

the increasing demand in modern society for amateur middle-class children to learn, seemingly to

the exclusion of all else, the technical fundamentals of music. By 1909, Jaques-Dalcroze had

attested, “Schools of Music, formerly frequented only by born musicians, gifted from birth with

unusual powers of perception for sound and rhythm, to-day receive all who are fond of music,

however little Nature may have endowed them with the necessary capacity for musical expression

and realization. The number of solo players, both pianists and violinists, is constantly increasing,

instrumental technique is being developed to an extraordinary degree, but everywhere, too, the

question is being asked whether the quality of instrumental players is equal to their quantity, and

whether the acquirement of extraordinary technique is likely to help musical progress when this

technique is not joined to musical powers, if not of the first rank, at least normal.”

As Jaques-Dalcroze contested, the culture of educating creative musical “artists” was on

the wane: “Of ten certificated pianists of to-day, at the most one, if indeed one, is capable of

recognizing one key from another, of improvising four bars with character or so as to give

pleasure to the listener, of giving expression to a composition without the help of the more or less

numerous annotations with which present day composers have to burden their work, of

experiencing any feeling whatever when they listen to, or perform, the composition of another.

The solo players of older days were without exception complete musicians, able to improvise and

compose, artists driven irresistibly towards art by a noble thirst for aesthetic expression, whereas

most young people who devote themselves nowadays to solo playing have the gifts neither of

hearing nor of expression, are content to imitate the composer's expression without the power of

feeling it, and have no other sensibility than that of the fingers, no other motor faculty than an

automatism painfully acquired. Solo playing of the present day has specialized in a finger

technique which takes no account of the faculty of mental expression. It is no longer a means, it

has become an end.”

See Emile Jaques-Dalcroze, “Rhythm as a Factor of Education,” reprinted in The

Eurhythmics of Jaques-Dalcroze (Boston: Small Maynard and Company, 1913), 15-16.

Page 462: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

441

Jaques-Dalcroze presented Eurhythmics performances to the public, and around 1906 he

started to comprehensively instruct teachers in his methods.165

By 1907, Jaques-Dalcroze

had described his seemingly habitual training techniques in the article “Initiation into

Rhythm,” stating:

By means of movements of the whole body, we may equip ourselves to realize

and perceive rhythms.

Consciousness of rhythm is the faculty of "placing" every succession and

combination of fractions of time in all their gradations of rapidity and strength.

This consciousness is acquired by means of muscular contractions and relaxations

in every degree of strength and rapidity.166

In 1909, Jaques-Dalcroze succinctly confirmed that his educational methods were yet

another outcropping of the typical psychological child-studies in time and action:

Before teaching the relation which exists between sound and movement, it is wise

to undertake the independent study of each of these two elements. Tone is

evidently secondary, since it has not its origin and model in ourselves, whereas

movement is instinctive in man and therefore primary. Therefore I begin the study

of music by careful and experimental teaching of movement. This is based in

earliest childhood on the automatic exercise of marching, for marching is the

natural model of time measure…This method is entirely based upon experiments

many times repeated, and not one of the exercises has been adopted until it has

been applied under different forms and under different conditions and its

usefulness definitely proved.167

With a rhythmic epistemology strikingly opposite of Paderewski’s and identical to both

Wundt’s and Scripture’s, Jaques-Dalcroze even discounted the heartbeat as a sufficient

measure for musical time in the modern age.168

In 1912 a British correspondent to The

165 Percy B. Ingham, “The Jaques-Dalcroze Method,” in The Eurhythmics of Jaques-Dalcroze

(Boston: Small Maynard and Company, 1913), 34, 35.

166

Emile Jaques-Dalcroze, “The Initiation into Rhythm,” in Rhythm, Music, and Education,

translated by Harold F. Rubinstein (New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons, 1921), 79-80.

167 Jaques-Dalcroze, “Rhythm as a Factor of Education,” 17, 18.

168 Jaques-Dalcroze, “The Initiation into Rhythm,” 82.

Page 463: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

442

New York Times described Jaques-Dalcroze’s methods as yet another form of psychology

experiment, one performed outside of the laboratory on child-subjects: “All the exercises,

it is said, develop the sense of hearing, the power of accurate time and rhythm memory,

and, above all, demand a high degree of attention.”169

To a great extent, Jaques-

Dalcroze’s Eurhythmics was not concerned with the performance traditions of creative

artists such as Paderewski, Rachmaninoff, Hofmann, Brahms, or countess others of past

centuries. In 1915 Jaques-Dalcroze was quoted as saying, “My pupils themselves began

to realize that they were not merely being taught music, but were being generally

educated. They were undergoing psycho-physical transformations.”170

The Times

reported on one of Jaques-Dalcroze’s child-training demonstrations in London on

November 12, 1913 (he would produce many over the next decade), witnessing that,

His aim is twofold. First he seeks to produce in his pupils the most intimate

possible realization of the rhythms of music by training them to execute a variety

of steps and gestures in exact correspondence with the time of an accompanying

piece. Secondly, he endeavors, by this elaborate training in calculated and

coordinated movements, to teach nerve-control, and establish on the part of his

students a perfect command over all their limbs.171

Jaques-Dalcroze’s pedagogy, however, was not entirely new, as he conflated many past

and contemporaneous techniques of non-musical, metronomic youth-training already

encountered in this survey: military drill, calisthenics and gymnastics, along with child-

study experimentation. Jaques-Dalcroze was clearly the most prominent in a wave of

educators attempting to train humans to move and act with greater, seemingly

169

“Rhythm in Education,” New York Times, Dec. 1, 1912, C4.

170

Quoted in Grace Smith, “The Eurhythmics of Jaques-Dalcroze,” Journal of Proceedings and

Addresses, National Education Association (August 16-27, 1915): 871.

171

“M. Dalcroze’s ‘Eurhythmics,’” Times, November 13, 1913, 10.

Page 464: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

443

automatical regularity. Nevertheless, according to Arnold Bennett’s text Mental

Efficiency (1911), numerous Eurhythmics-like pedagogies were readily available to

modern American and British citizens. Bennett states, “In all the American newspapers

and all the American monthlies are innumerable illustrated announcements of ‘physical-

culture specialists,’ who guarantee to make all the organs of the body perform their duties

with the mighty precision of a 60 h.p. motor-car that never breaks down…vast numbers

of people must be worried about the non-efficiency of their bodies, and on the way to

achieve efficiency. In our more modest British fashion, we have the same phenomenon in

England. And it is growing.”172

Harkening to Bennett’s assessment of these modern, regimented physical

pedagogies, the New York Times (1914) reported on one of the earliest Eurhythmics

schools in North America, directed by former Jaques-Dalcroze student Ida

Lenggenhagen:

The visitor to Miss Lenggenhagen’s studio will be amazed to see a class

“realizing” one of these complicated rhythms and at the same time preparing for a

fresh and still more complicated one. For while arms are beating a certain rhythm

and feet are expressing eights and sixteenths, the music will change to a different

rhythm, and, while still continuing, the dancers will listen and mentally register

the new rhythm being played. At the signal “Hopp!” they are ready to change,

while the music may have slipped into still another intricacy.173

As the New York Times reporter expressed, Eurhythmics was a very new and unusual

performance practice to behold, remarking, “It all seems absurd enough until you get the

172

Arnold Bennett, Mental Efficiency and other Hints to Men and Women (New York: George H.

Doran Company, 1911), 7-8. Bennett makes no specific mention of Jaques-Dalcroze or his

Eurhythmics.

173 “Dalcroze Eurhythmics Have Come to New York,” New York Times, Jan. 25, 1914, SM5.

Page 465: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

444

idea.”174

The reporter’s initial reaction to the sight of these performances is perhaps

understandable, since Eurhythmics exercises were grounded in a chronographic time, in

which a small, automatical note-unit presided over every perception and actualization of

human motion. Regardless of meter, phase, harmony, or affect, “the quarter note is

translated into a single movement and is the unit of the rhythm.”175

In another exposé of Lenggenhagen’s young studio, this time during a New York

performance, Margaret Naumburg’s 1914 report “The Dalcroze Idea, What Eurhythmics

is and What it Means” in Outlook magazine confirmed Jaques-Dalcroze’s new method of

chronographic education, in which the student’s individual will for movement became

subsumed through a scientific system of response training:

The Dalcroze method makes bodily movements depend on the exact musical

values, so that the music and the bodily expression are so closely related that

together they formed a whole…the Dalcroze method depends on exact

interpretation of note and measure duration.176

Jaques-Dalcroze’s musical-time exercises published in Rhythm, Music, and Education

(1921) confirm this “rhythmic” practice: a constant, metronomic time-unit—usually a

quarter or eight note—grounded every bar in each of his novel etudes. Traditional metric

notation seldom applied; standard meters are replaced with new signatures, dictating the

number of invariable note-units per bar. Indeed, traditional meters are not applicable to

the actualization of Jaques-Dalcroze’s music studies: the student must subdivide in

mechanically equal “clicks” to perform his etudes accurately. One cannot glean the ictus

174

Ibid.

175

Ibid.

176 Margaret Naumburg, “The Dalcroze Idea, What Eurhythmics is and What it Means,” Outlook,

Jan. 17, 1914, 130. APS Online.

Page 466: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

445

metricus, Riemann’s agogik, the invisible accent—and certainly not Christiani’s “wavy

outline”—through Jaques-Dalcroze’s revised time signatures; while Jaques-Dalcroze

provides the necessary translation from traditional time signatures to his new constant-

beat “measure denominator,” the sensory hierarchy of metrical rhythm, pulse, and

rhetoric, no longer underlies his objectivist Eurhythmics notation.

Figure 6.10. Jaques-Dalcroze’s reinvented time-signature notation for Eurhythmics that stresses the

mathematical-automatical equality and constancy of small internal beats—regardless of bar length.

Correspondingly, the notational system effaced the physical sense of pulse, swing, and movement once

represented by traditional time signatures. From Jaques-Dalcroze, Rhythm, Music, and Education (New

York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons, 1921), “Musical Supplement,” 1.

As with Wundt’s, Titchener’s, and Seashore’s exercises in the perception of rhythm,

physically perceived meter, movement, or pulse no longer held sway, either in print or in

practice. A comparison of these scientific rhythmic training examples shows their

Page 467: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

446

uncanny similarity: Jaques-Dalcroze’s Eurhythmics studies are strongly reminiscent of

experimental psychologists’ testing methods from the last decade of the nineteenth

century.

Figure 6.11. Some of Jaques-Dalcroze’s many Eurhythmics exercises for the training of precise rhythmic

performance, where the mathematical-automatical consistency of small note-unit “divisions” was of

paramount importance. As with similar rhythmic procedures from gymnastics regimens and physiological

experiments, this now-objectified “rhythm” was de-contextualized from musical phrase, harmony, the

traditional, physical sense of meter, alongside rhetorical gesture and affect. From Jaques-Dalcroze, Rhythm,

Music, and Education (New York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons, 1921), “Musical Supplement,” 3.

Naumburg’s 1914 Outlook article further explained that the “rhythm” imparted through

Eurhythmics correspondingly effaced individual interpretation, and diminished the

prevalent “problems” of personal rhythmic action for the sake of normalized, mass

regulation. She quotes Jaques-Dalcroze:

Page 468: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

447

“Most children have no instinct for time, for time values, for accentuation, for

physical balance, because the motor faculties are not the same in all individuals

and because a number of obstacles impede the exact and rapid realization of

mental conceptions. One child is always behind the beat when marching, another

always ahead, another takes unequal steps, another, on the contrary, lacks balance.

All these faults, if not corrected in the first years, will reappear later in the

musical technic of the individual. Unsteady time when singing or playing,

confusion in playing, inability to follow when accompanying, accentuating too

roughly or with lack of precision, all these faults have their origin in the child’s

muscular and nervous control, in lack of coordination between the mind which

conceives, the brain which orders, the nerve which transmits, and the muscle

which executes.”177

As Jaques-Dalcroze himself explained, Eurhythmics was an extension of physiology,

experimental psychology, and child-study; Eurhythmics was a new chronographic

science training “creative” human movements under mechanically informed, precision

time. Eventually, Jaques-Dalcroze’s music and dance pedagogy helped infuse

automatism—the metronomic rhythm first espoused by experimental psychologists—into

a modern, industrial-inspired performance arts, a mechanical-mathematical aesthetic first

recognized by many in the music of Igor Stravinsky, as Chapter VII relates in greater

detail.

Jaques-Dalcroze, the pedagogue of modern, twentieth-century “Rhythm,” had

wide-ranging influence in modern culture, in the fields of music, language, dance, and

177

Naumburg, 130-131. Also printed in Jaques-Dalcroze, “Rhythm as a Factor of Education,” 18.

In a lecture given to the Dresden Teachers’ Association on May 28, 1912, Jaques-

Dalcroze further explained the rhythmic “problems” of unmusical children consist of muscular

and neurological disorders—problems commonly perceived by experimental psychologists

through chronographic child-study: “There are always children who are not able to sing in time,

or even to beat time, to walk in time, or to graduate the strength and rapidity of their movements.

Such children are unrythmic, and it will generally be noticed that these children are stiff and

awkward, often also over-excitable. This lack of rhythm is almost like a disease. It is caused by

the lack of balance between the mental and physical powers…To repeat, the causes of this lack of

rhythm all lie in the important but insufficiently recognized psycho-physiological sphere of the

co-ordination of brain, never-paths and muscles.” See “From the Lectures of Jaques-Dalcroze,” in

The Eurhythmics of Jaques-Dalcroze, 27-28.

Page 469: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

448

even psychology. Perhaps affected by Jaques-Dalcroze’s rhythmic pedagogy, William

Patterson prescribed in The Rhythm of Prose (1916) that,

If the student wishes to develop his time-sense systematically, he should make

himself expert in the automatic performance of syncopating tasks, such as tapping

two's against three's or even five's against seven's…let him tap off the drum-beat

rhythm of his most impelling experience with a sentence till he learns it.178

With this habitual training in absolute, objective rhythm, the student of both music and

literature aspired to be a living automaton—a quality valued by experimental

psychologists, not traditional performance “artists.” Correspondingly, the chronographic-

metronomic time that predominated educational methods by the second decade of the

twentieth century effaced the values of individual movement and experience in music,

speech, and dance. William Patterson’s influences and intentions, too, were abundantly

evident as he writes: “Meumann has already made clear the fact that all regular or

rhythmical movements tend to become automatic and that this automatism is of great

service to a performer in the matter of keeping time.”179

Trained under these comprehensive, automatical pedagogies, the individual’s

perception was no longer part of the performance equation. The student-subject’s new

challenge—even in prose speech—was to conform to metronomic rhythm. As Rusk

reports in Experimental Education, influential psychologist Ernst Meumann challenged

elementary-school children to beat-time in music as mechanically precise as possible

during various experimental exercises. He found that,

178

Patterson, 99.

179

Ibid., 23.

Page 470: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

449

The highest rate at which the seven-year-old child can perform this task is when

the notes succeed one another at 4/10 second interval; the power thus to

accompany fails with many twelve-year-old children when the interval is greater

than two seconds. With the shortest times the tapping is too slow, with the longer

too quick, and it is observed that children frequently do not notice their mistakes.

These children consequently cannot apprehend and reproduce musical measures

involving intervals of less than .4 seconds or greater than about 1.8 seconds, and

the duration of notes or divisions of notes beyond these limits cannot be

appreciated or reproduced by many children.180

While children naturally failed to perform musical rhythms with the exacting automatical

qualities defined by the chronographic method, Rusk recognized that Meumann’s work

was not an attempt at futility. Rusk reflected on how the première twentieth-century

pedagogue of modern rhythmic-training continued to habituate youth in mechanically

precise performances, stating that a child’s automatical-rhythm “ability can be greatly

improved by training, such systems as that of Emile Jaques-Dalcroze prove beyond

doubt.”181

Indeed, Jaques-Dalcroze described his own pedagogy of Eurhythmics as a

chronographic experiment in habituated mechanical action:

The object of the method is, in the first instance, to create by the help of rhythm a

rapid and regular current of communication between brain and body…It is a

question of eliminating in every muscular movement, by the help of will, the

untimely intervention of muscles useless for the movement in question, and thus

developing attention, consciousness and will-power. Next must be created an

automatic technique for all those muscular movements which do not need the help

of the consciousness…Thanks to the co-ordination of the nerve-centers, to the

formation and development of the greatest possible number of motor habits, my

method assures the freest possible play to subconscious expression.182

Jaques-Dalcroze defined “rhythm” identically to experimental psychologists such as

Meumann, Myers, Sears, Scripture, Wundt, and a host of psychologists currently training

180

Rusk, 69.

181

Ibid.

182 Jaques-Dalcroze, “Rhythm as a Factor of Education,” 18-19.

Page 471: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

450

an industrialized society in efficient, mechanized actions and responses. Recalling the

methods of Vierordt and in his seminal metronomic-action studies from the 1860s, Percy

B. Ingham in 1913 confirms the scientific-rhythmic traditions of Jaques-Dalcroze’s

educational theories and practices:

A rhythm in music consists of a regularly recurring series of accented sounds,

unaccented sounds, and rests, expressed in rhythmic gymnastics by movements

and inhibitions of movements. Individuals who are rhythmically uncertain

generally have a muscular system which is irregularly responsive to mental

stimuli; the response many be too rapid or too slow; in either case impulse or

inhibition falls at the wrong moment, the change of movement is not made to

time, and the physical expression of the rhythm is blurred.183

As the educational values of Jaques-Dalcroze attest—and as they directly reflect the

theories of Scripture and Hall—twentieth-century training methods in the fields of

industry, art, and science all seemed to be converging through the identical applications

of objective, efficient, and externalized rhythmic regulation. Correspondingly, these

pedagogies tacitly contained the now-prevalent chronographic bias against individual,

subjective action.

Factories around the world continued to bring laborers “up to date” in the

chronographic methods of experimental psychology as well. It can be no coincidence that

two German businessmen, Harald and Wolf Dohrn, built, funded, and administrated

Jaques-Dalcroze’s College for Rhythmic Training, otherwise known as the “Temple of

Rhythm” in Hellerau, their fabricated garden city near Dresden.184

In the 1921 book Mind

and Work: The Psychological Factors in Industry and Commerce, psychologist (and

183 Percy B. Ingham, 42-43. The writer also notes, “The Jaques-Dalcroze method aims at nothing

more or less than the training of rhythmic innervations.” See The Eurhythmics of Jaques-

Dalcroze, 39.

184 Ibid., 36. See also Isa Partsch-Bergsohn, Modern Dance in Germany and the United States

(Switzerland: Harwood Academic Publishers, 1994), 7.

Page 472: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

451

Member of the Industrial Fatigue Research Board) Charles S. Myers codified the

standard fatigue test where the laborer-subject’s “finger is voluntarily bent and extended

to the rhythm of a metronome, the cylinder winds the wire round it and the weight is

raised.”185

Jules Amar, the Director of the Laboratory of Physiological Research in the

Conservatoire des Arts et Métiers, described similar labor fatigue experiments in his

book Physiology of Industrial Organization (1918), in which a subject “voluntarily”

responded while “The rhythm is measured by a metronome, and the weight to be moved

by the muscular contractions does not vary.”186

Amar also reported on re-training efforts

for disabled factory workers, during strength exercises in which “The pace will be set by

a metronome, marking from 30 to 300 oscillations.”187

185

Charles S. Myers, Mind and Work: The Psychological Factors in Industry and Commerce,

(New York: Putnam's Sons, 1921), 39. As skilled nineteenth-century musicians could argue, if the

finger moved to the externalized, artificial beat of the metronome, the subject’s actions were not

really “voluntary.”

186

Jules Amar, The Physiology of Industrial Organization and the Re-employment of the

Disabled, translated by Bernard Miall (London: The Library Press Limited, 1918), 92.

187

Ibid., 148.

Page 473: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

452

Figure 6.12. One of Jules Amar’s subjects, an 18-year-old athlete, during a typical work-fatigue

experiment based on chronographic methods. Apparatuses employed include an “ergonomic cycle,”

“respiration gauge,” a “Tonograph,” “Pneumograph,” and the metronome, which appears at the left corner

of the table. From Jules Amar, The Physiology of Industrial Organization and the Re-employment of the

Disabled, translated by Bernard Miall (London: The Library Press Limited, 1918), 84.

Scientific “rhythm” training continued to gain ever-greater precisions in all aspects of

general education, music pedagogy, and labor training. In Measure Your Mind: The

Mentimeter and how to Use It (1920), Frank Parker Stockbridge and M. R. Trabue

recounted the now industry-standard aiming tests that measured performance accuracy

through metronomic regulation. While the subject placed a brass pencil in various small

holes, “The speed of the subject’s movements was controlled by a metronome, so as to

allow thirty trials per minute.”188

This standard chronographic test eventually applied to

serious music students as well. Psychologist Carl Emil Seashore promoted identical

188

M. R. Trabue and Frank Parker Stockbridge, Measure Your Mind: The Mentimeter and how to

Use it (New York: Doubleday, Page & Company, 1920), 81-82.

Page 474: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

453

methods and apparatuses when training a musician’s “Precision in Movement,” as he

related in The Psychology of Musical Talent (1919):

The precision target. To test for direction of movement in its simplest form, we

use what is known as the precision target… The test consists of determining how

many holes the subject can put the needle into without touching the target plate.

The metronome is beating, and he is required to put the pointer into each

successive hole at the rate of one in two seconds.189

As Seashore implies, the student musician was now the “subject” of the standard

precision-target training. Horace B. McChesney’s 1926 patent filing develops upon a

similar labor-training machine for “keyboard operation” in telegraphy; it is an invention

that clearly recalls not only the traditions of other laboratory apparatuses but also the

modern trends in scientific piano-pedagogy, in which the clockwork metronome presided

over the precise time and exact rhythm for the efficient human’s every key-stroke.190

Figure 6.13. Details from U.S. Patent #1,688,935, an “Apparatus for Testing the Aptitude of Keyboard

Operators,” filed on September 27, 1926 by H. B. McChesney. The modified, electrical-contact Maelzel

metronome appears to the right.

189

Carl Emil Seashore, The Psychology of Musical Talent (Boston: Silver, Burdett, and

Company, 1919), 183.

190

Horace B. McChesney, “Apparatus for Testing the Aptitude of Keyboard Operators,” U.S.

Patent #1,688,935 (Filed Sept. 27, 1926; Patented Oct. 23, 1928).

Page 475: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

454

Similar to other scientific pedagogues of his age, Carl Emil Seashore transferred

every value of experimental psychology and labor training to the mechanical study of

human music making. In the 1918 Musical Quarterly article “The Sense of Rhythm as a

Musical Talent,” Seashore applied the same “rhythm” of Wundt’s laboratory to his

musician-subject’s talent for “musical rhythm,” based on experimental methods and

clockwork apparatuses.191

Once again this experimental psychologist neglected a skilled

musical artist’s intuition, intelligence, or performing experience in these laboratory

procedures; the subject’s internal or expressive understanding in time had little

relationship to the prevailing techniques of psychology. Rather, Seashore’s goal was

identical to other child-study psychologists of the new century: the quantification of

normality through standardized laboratory methods and machines. He proclaims in the

Psychology of Musical Talent, “The musical mind is first of all a normal mind. Indeed,

the normal mind is musical to the extent that it is normal.”192

With the purposes of both

defining and indeed creating a normal student, Seashore confirmed that even “music may

use science for its own benefit in the understanding and mastery of technique. By definite

tests scientifically determined, it is possible to determine which children possess musical

ability of a high order and may therefore be given the opportunity to become artists.”193

With his professional experience, not as a performing musician, but as a child-

study psychologist, Seashore became the self-appointed arbiter of correct musical time

191

Carl Emil Seashore, “The Sense of Rhythm as a Musical Talent,” The Musical Quarterly 4

(Oct., 1918): 507.

192 Seashore, The Psychology of Musical Talent, 6.

193

Ibid.

Page 476: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

455

and by extension the personal equation of the modern musical “artist.” He reiterated

psychologists’ self-defined problems with human performance:

That enormous individual differences in this [“sense of rhythm”] endowment

obtain is a noted, if not notorious, fact. There are three ways of approaching the

problem of concrete analysis or measurement for the purpose of securing reliable

information about the relative presence or absence of this talent.194

According to a host of experienced experimental psychologists, traditional music

instructors were incapable of recognizing or solving these problems in their individual

students. Indeed, Seashore expressed some disdain for traditional and individual teaching

methods, since with such anti-scientific instruction “only a small fraction of this baffling

field [of traditional music pedagogy] gets into print, because, of all the subjects of

instruction, none has more individual ‘systems’ of instruction and in none is there so

much of the atmosphere of trade secrecy. Master teachers develop methods which are

more or less original and the students of the masters come to form groups between whom

there is the keenest rivalry.”195

To counteract these highly subjective teaching methods, Seashore devised

musical-talent experiments for both young children and professional musicians in-

training, in which “The merit of the test lies in the fact that it is stripped down to its

elements, which can be accurately defined, controlled, and measured.”196

In his Manual

194

Ibid., 124.

195

Ibid., v. He admits, however, “The cause of this does not, of course, lie entirely within the

personal idiosyncrasies of teachers of music, vocal and instrumental; rather is it due in great part

to the imponderability of the aesthetic element in music. Interpretation and expression are not

easily measured in any exact way: taste and individual differences are constituent factors in any

verdict about the relative superiority of rival methods, and these have not yet been, and in all

probability can never completely be, subjected to definite measurement.”

196

Seashore, The Psychology of Musical Talent, 202.

Page 477: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

456

of Instructions and Interpretations for Measures of Musical Talent (1919), Seashore

reduced the quantifiable aspects of “Musical Action”—defined as the “natural capacity

for skill in accurate and musically expressive production of tones”—to these criteria: “1.

Control of pitch / 2. Control of intensity / 3. Control of time / 4. Control of timbre / 5.

Control of rhythm / 6. Control of volume.”197

As Seashore rightly acknowledged, such chronographic research and training

methods were well outside the reach of traditional music teachers. Due to the lack of

laboratory apparatus in the music studio, Seashore realized that only experimental

psychologists could define “true musicality” with the tools and techniques they had been

using for decades. Thus Seashore envisioned the extension of experimental psychology

even further into the realms of performance practices, since his process was,

Unfortunately not adapted for general use by musicians themselves. It

presupposes a technique, an equipment, and a skill in psychological analysis

which the musician does not possess. It requires a specialist trained in music and

psychology and will tend to open a new profession—that of a consulting

psychologist in music. Since the elaborate measurements will be made only on

those who have serious aspirations for a professional career in music, many will

not be needed; but laboratories might well be maintained in a few of the principal

music centers.198

Keeping with the chronographic bias of his field, Seashore supposed that every human

impulse reduced to mere mechanical processes, that the individual’s interpretive will was

quantifiable in clockwork. The processes used in child study and labor training, in

Seashore’s view, logically transferred to all aspiring professionals working within the

197

Carl Emil Seashore, Manual of Instructions and Interpretations for Measures of Musical

Talent (New York: Columbia Phonograph Company, [1919]), 16. Seashore devised this manual

based on The Psychology of Musical Talent as a companion to phonographic records produced by

Columbia, which were employed as the testing apparatus for musicians-in-training.

198

Carl Emil Seashore, “The Measurement of Musical Talent,” The Musical Quarterly 1 (Jan.,

1915): 146.

Page 478: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

457

confines of “rhythmic action.” In the Psychology of Musical Talent (1919), Seashore

explained how his scientific training methods drastically redefined the performance goals

of the modern musician:

Precision in the time of rhythmic action may be measured by varying the timed

action tests so as to produce long and short intervals, the duration of which may

be controlled as in actual playing…We, therefore, use the telegraph key with the

chronograph, as before, and required the subject to mark a given musical

rhythm…To this measure of time precision, we might easily add the measure of

the precision of stress or accent, as described for intensity. To record this, the

telegraph key is mounted on a spring which is attached to a similar spring on the

phonograph, carrying a tracing point which marks the degree of pressure by a

tracing parallel to the time line and similar to it, except that the measure is in

terms of the degrees of pressure…Such apparatus might be available and effective

for specific training in precision of time and touch in the essentials of musical

rhythm and might well furnish a series of exercises in a conservatory.199

The “essentials of musical rhythm” that Seashore valued so highly were those not of

Beethoven but of the astronomical observatory and psychological laboratory, where

“precise” mechanical accuracy was placed far above the individual’s creative, subjective

perceptions. Seashore’s essential rhythm was defined in unremitting clockwork, a time

that completely replaced the movement and pulse of living music performance. Seashore

even stated that his “timed action test may be regarded as an imitation of keeping time, as

in the playing on the piano, marching, dancing, and all other forms of rhythmic

movement.”200

His widely published articles and books, spanning the first half of the

twentieth century, suggest that these values of experimental psychology became accepted

norm in music education. Indeed, his article “The Measurement of Musical Talent”

199

Seashore, The Psychology of Musical Talent, 203-4.

200

Ibid., 202. Seashore did not intend his methods for mere scientific observation, but for a new

scientific education, which “will train both the ear and hand because the graphic record is

quantitatively exact and records smaller differences than the [chronographically] untrained ear

can hear or hand execute.”

Page 479: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

458

appeared in the very first volume of The Musical Quarterly in 1915. Seashore’s work

exposes the extent to which scientific methods had irrevocably revised the fundamentals

of musical time in education: the scientific metronome controlled rhythm—constant,

automatical rhythm—for every movement and perception of the twentieth-century

musician. With assistance from Seashore, the metronome-as-chronograph had finally

arrived to dictate and define the personal equation of “musical talent” in the twentieth

century;201

the methods and machines of Wundt’s experimental laboratory finally seemed

appropriate and desirable for this new incarnation of chronographic child-study. Thus, a

new tradition of use for the metronome had ascended: Chronographic regulation became

a pedagogical standard for the comprehensive technical training of both amateur and

conservatory musicians in the now accepted and sought-after precisions of scientific

rhythm.202

Seashore was one of the most prominent teacher-technicians to apply scientific

machines and methods to once complex and subjective performance-acts, but he was not

alone. A host of scientific pedagogues continued to apply the metronome as an apparatus

specifically intended for chronographic music training. In 1920, for example, inventor

and pedagogue James Rigg devised a breathing apparatus for student singers and wind-

201

Seashore wholeheartedly considered that a child’s musical-physical ability could reduce to a

series of experiments, thus exposing: “The personal equation. The permanent traits of an

individual, as conceived in terms of capacities, are sometimes spoken of as his personal equation.

One person is slow and sure; another is quick and sure; one lives in the world of feeling, another

in the world of reflection; one has a remarkable sense of rhythm; another is devoid of this

capacity. The knowledge of such traits or capacities can be built up into a personal equation

which will enable us to predict with reasonable certainty the aptitude and probable promise of

achievement of such a person.” See Seashore, The Psychology of Musical Talent, 15.

202

While the topic is not in the scope of the present survey, it is important to note that Seashore

had a long-standing relationship with the Eastman School of Music, where he actively applied his

scientific methods under the approval of the school’s director Howard Hanson.

Page 480: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

459

players where a metronome regulated lung-training exercises.203

Dutch inventor Maurits

Jonas Goudsmit likewise treated the music student as a subject in mechanical

performance. In his 1923 patent filing, the inventor reiterated the now-commonly held

belief in the modern metronome, a belief in which human interpretations, sensations, and

perceptions were of little consequence:

As the indications given in pieces of music, such as allegro, andante, and other

indications, are too indefinite to indicate to the pupil or the practicing artist the

exact time to keep when studying a piece of music, for this purpose use is made of

the metronome, as is known.204

Mechanical exactitude, not sensory interpretation, was the goal of chronographic research

and education in twentieth-century culture. Under this epistemology, “indefinite” verbal

descriptions no longer held qualitative value in musical interpretation and performance;

only a definitive metronomic rhythm would suffice “to keep” musical time in the

student—and, as Goudsmit suggests, eventually the “practicing artist.” As these inventors

realized, chronographic precisions could always be improved upon. Thus some

technologically progressive scientists, teachers, and inventors no longer considered the

clockwork metronome precise enough to consistently regulate musical practice in the

twentieth century. Goudsmit, like the inventors of pocket-watch, electric, and

phonograph205

metronomes, believed that to ingrain precise mechanical consistency in

the child, Maelzel’s old design with its now-superfluous pendulum should be discounted

203

James Burlington Rigg, “Means for and Method of Regulating Breathing…” U.S. Patent

#1,354,212 (Filed Feb. 9, 1920; Patented Sept. 28, 1920).

204

Maurits Goudsmit, “Music Time Indicator.” U.S. Patent #1,783,537 (Filed Aug. 2, 1923;

Patented Dec. 2, 1930): 1. The inventor also filed this device for a Germany patent on April 10,

1928.

205

Adelaide Ewing, “Device for Counting Time,” U.S. Patent #1,496,258 (Filed Nov. 1922;

Patented June 3, 1924).

Page 481: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

460

altogether. He states:

The disadvantages connected with studying with the aid of the metronome are of

many kinds…It is troublesome for the player to have to pay attention by ear to the

rhythm by the ticking of the metronome, while he must at the same time

accurately observe the pitch…A further disadvantage consists in that when the

metronome is in front of the player, the swing of the pendulum has a disturbing

effect on the eye itself, as it must read the notes at the same time.206

Again, for those with a chronographic bias, the modern student’s dilemma—his personal-

equation problem to solve—was in being completely automatical: One could seldom stay

slavishly faithful to the swing or click of the metronome while simultaneously reading

the music notation. Clarence G. Hamilton, in his music-education treatise How to Use the

Metronome Correctly, concurred that the rhythm of the clockwork click now

predominated over the pendulum movement, attesting that the ear “should be taught to

apprehend the beats, since it is frequently inconvenient to watch the pendulum and often

confusing to attempt to do so.”207

The solution to this unwieldy performance practice, which distracted the student

from the most perfect automatical-rhythmical accuracy, was obvious to Goudsmit, since

child-study psychologists had already practiced a better method of inducing mechanical

action: connect the student directly to the chronographic device. His “invention consists

of an apparatus, which imparts the time of the music to be played mechanically to the

foot of the player.” Patent filing #1,783,537 shows the striking similarity of Goudsmit’s

new metronome-apparatus to the contemporaneous ergograph.

206

Goudsmit, 1.

207 C. G. Hamilton, 11.

Page 482: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

461

Figure 6.14. Detail of U.S. Patent #1,496,258, filed on August 2, 1923 by Maurits Goudsmit, showing a

“Music Time Indicator” that connects to the performer’s foot and physically dictates the “exact”

metronomic rhythm for “the pupil and the practicing artist.” Forgoing both the visual and sonic rhythmic

reference of Maelzel’s old clockwork design, this modern metronomic harness could additionally dictate a

redundant, artificial metrical accent to the performer’s appendage.

Thus, a music student, like the subject of a child study, would physically conform to the

metronomic source in full. By the time of Goudsmit’s work in musical chronography,

subjective and creative values had largely given way to scientific-mechanical values. The

intentions of scientific pedagogues, such as Jaques-Dalcroze, Patterson, and Seashore,

were transparent: habitually infuse the child with the most precise “rhythmic” values

possible as defined by a scientific apparatus—a coolly objective (once considered

mindless) automaton. In 1916 music professor Clarence G. Hamilton, summarizing the

now prevalent values of the scientific-musical culture, likewise allegorized the

metronome as the “balance wheel” for the “temperamental extravagances” the music

Page 483: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

462

student—the metronome became the essential mechanical regulator for a child-

performer’s internal clockwork.208

Through the values imposed by scientists, scientific

pedagogues, and their intertwined methodologies, a drastic and lasting temporal shift had

clearly occurred within Western society: a machine now controlled and defined the once-

subjective meanings and practices of musical time. For these scientific-music educators,

the metronomic hegemony was not only acknowledged but also completely welcomed; a

hegemony in which constant clockwork motion effaced nuanced rhythmical feeling, and

automatical sound quashed the comparatively imperfect, imprecise pulse-sense.

208

See C. G. Hamilton, 10: “For the student, however, the metronome’s most important office is

to give command over absolute time-values. Tempo is so dependent upon the mood of the

moment that students, especially with those of highly emotional natures, should practice technical

exercises and parts of pieces daily with the metronome as a balance wheel to their temperamental

extravagances.”

Page 484: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

463

CHAPTER VII: THE ORIGINS OF A CHRONOGRAPHIC

MUSICAL CULTURE

Objective regulation, control, and efficiency through standardized, precision

measurement: these were the essential values of the scientific metronome—values

diametrically opposed to the beliefs and practices of creative nineteenth-century

musicians. Only in the new scientifically organized society of the twentieth century did

the metronome become a fundamental cultural reference for human endeavors, in which

music making was one metronomic act among many. A Dictionary of Military Terms

(1918) by West Point instructor Edward S. Farrow confirms the primary usefulness of

Maelzel’s machine as infallible temporal regulator for standardized training. Not

surprisingly, while individualistic human musicianship was not an issue in Farrow’s

definition of the military metronome, he recounts another cold-blooded, anti-musical

automaton of identical origins:

Metronome.—A machine for indicating the correct time or cadence. It was

invented in 1815 by the inventor of the automatic trumpeter. The test of a correct

metronome is that when set at 60 it shall beat seconds.1

“Correct time” for the marching of troops, the testing of children, and the training of

workers in the new century was invariably mechanical; in a culture where Yoga initiates

were even instructed to “get a metronome or a watch or a clock…set it going, so that you

have an exact measurement” for meditative breathing,2 the “correct time” for the modern

1 Edward S. Farrow, A Dictionary of Military Terms, (Revised Edition, New York: Thomas Y.

Crowell Company, 1918), 379. Farrow’s definition is slightly truncated from stock descriptions

appearing since the nineteenth century, including Chambers’ Encyclopedia, Revised Edition, Vol.

VI (London: W. and R. Chambers, 1874), 429.

2 Hereward Carrington, Higher Psychical Development (Yoga Philosophy), an Outline of the

Secret Hindu Teachings (New York: Dodd, Mead and Company, 1920), 61.

Page 485: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

464

Western world reduced to mere clockwork. As Hereward Carrington’s modern brand of

“Yoga Philosophy” verified, “there must be perfect rhythm established,” a perfect

metronomic rhythm totally anachronistic, non-traditional, and artificial when compared

to organic human actions and past physical-mental practices. Thus, in all aspects of

modern Western living, the clockwork metronome redefined the nature of time as it

actively regulated people—whether in meditation, marching, or music making—along

with their personal equations through scientific methods of training, education, and

experiment.

In the twentieth century, metronomic time became the primary “rhythm”

throughout much of Western society. Jaques-Dalcroze, Münsterberg, Seashore, Patterson,

and others promoted automatical pedagogies grounded by a “time” and “rhythm” entirely

separated from individual experience, subjectivity, or volition. Amar’s 1914 labor

training reference The Human Motor, or, The Scientific Foundations of Labor and

Industry, too, championed distinctly artificial time as applied to living action (well

beyond the book’s striking title). In The Human Motor—a virtual mirror for modern

society’s prevailing values of industrialization—automatical machines predominated, as

Amar defined the “musical pendulum” no longer as the silent, simple pendulum but

exclusively as the clockwork metronome: the premiere chronographic regulator of the

new century.3 Indeed, by the publication of Amar’s text, it seems that the definition of

time and rhythm was found in the clockwork metronome alone:

3 Jules Amar, The Human Motor, or, The Scientific Foundations of Labor and Industry (London:

George Routledge & Sons, Ltd., 1920), 35.

Page 486: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

465

197. Measurement of Time.—The unit of time is the second. It is rarely

necessary, in the phenomena under consideration, to measure small fractions of a

second. However, in walking at 100 or 150 steps a minute, or in operating the

keys of a piano, or a typewriter, etc. the duration of each movement may be from,

say, 1/2 to 1/5 of a second. It suffices in most cases to use a metronome, which

will give the cadence of the work, will regulate it invariably, and will indicate the

duration of any isolated, mechanical act. 4

Amar confirmed that the metronome was one of the most valued time sources in the

scientific-industrial century for the precise temporal control of human activities—

typewriting and playing the piano among many “mechanical acts.” Even modern music

periodicals espoused the metronome for the most efficient, objective, and unfeeling

measurer of musical training and education. In 1854, only astronomers required such

exacting rhythmic regulation, which was made possible through the observatory

chronograph first installed at Greenwich. Yet in the twentieth century the metronome

became the ubiquitous chronograph of an industrialized society. Just as in astronomical

observation, metronomic rhythm redefined and reduced the personal equation from all

“operations” of science, industry, and education. Meanwhile the human’s individual

perception, as astronomers realized in previous decades, became subservient to this

objective mechanical rule and reference. Modern educators such as Jaques-Dalcroze

furthered this chronographic belief—that even musical time was primarily based in

mechanical precision. Indeed, in 1919, the premier “Rhythm” pedagogue described the

measurement of metered music with striking similarity to Amar’s “time-measurement”

definition, as an objective attempt “to shade sounds in all degrees of speed…(by a

4 Ibid., 266.

Page 487: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

466

mathematic division of each sound into fractions of a half, third, quarter, eighth, etc., of

its time-value).”5

Maelzel’s metronome never represented the nuanced and variable nineteenth-

century practices of creativity or expression in music, dance, or spoken performance; its

often misinterpreted and mis-transcribed numbers never represented anything more than

slight, initial references to human movement and pulse. During much of the nineteenth

century, Maelzel’s clockwork metronome only reflected the initial swing of the simple

pendulum; its bell an artificial facsimile of a physically derived sense of meter—Just as

the simple pendulum once reflected a fundamental human movement, tactus, or pulse.

Indeed, the metronome was an overly precise tool for artistic musicians of the nineteenth

century, and it did not factor into the actions or thoughts of those instilled with former

temporal values based on physicality, language, and intuition. As Paderewski

experientially understood, “Mechanical execution and emotion are incompatible.”6

Before mass education fell under the influence of experimental psychologists, scientific

rhythm—distinguished solely by automatism—and “artistic” human rhythm were

considered two opposing qualities.

Nevertheless, humans readily learned to act to the click of a metronome during

laboratory experiments, private practicing, and classroom instruction. In both the arts and

sciences, old temporal perceptions gave way to new time-telling technologies, and the

5 Emile Jaques-Dalcroze, “Eurhythmics and Moving Plastic,” in Rhythm, Music and Education,

translated by Harold F. Rubinstein (New York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons, 1921), 274.

6

Henry T. Finck, Success in Music and How it is Won (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons,

1909), 455.

Page 488: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

467

values of time and action correspondingly grew more mechanically precise with habitual

training. Eventually, mechanical precisions trumped the understandings of physical

movement, meter, and pulse, as scientific methods and technologies continued to

modernize mass education. Scientists and educators alike forgot the physical basis of

musical tempo and motion in favor of the metronome’s automatical, redundant click—a

reference constantly to be improved upon for the sake of scientific endeavors—to be

made more accurate, efficient, and exacting over time. Thus, inventors and scientists

continued to modernize even Maelzel’s metronome through more precise gearing,

motors, electrical current, and eventually quartz technology. Indeed, the goals of

automatical technology know no end. First in the laboratory and then the practice room,

external metronomic timekeepers continued to increase in artificial precision and

regularity—and they continue to do so through our day. Over time, performer’s practices

and perceptions of musical movement and rhythm seem to parallel these technological

trends; in the twentieth century, the standardized performance-arts education seems to

move in lockstep with these increasingly precise, and wholly artificial, time-keeping

apparatuses.

In the laboratory of the 1920s behavioral psychologists such as Pavlov continued

to show that the clockwork metronome affected and altered the mammalian mind through

repetitive training.7 Sometimes the monotonous clicks sonically distracted a subject’s

7 For instance, see C. J. Warden and L. H. Warner, “The Sensory Capacities and Intelligence of

Dogs, with a Report on the Ability of the Noted Dog ‘Fellow’ to Respond to Verbal Stimuli,” The

Quarterly Review of Biology 3 (Mar., 1928): 13. The researchers note, “Other work by Pavlov’s

students indicates remarkable discriminatory ability for notes alike in pitch but differing in timbre

or tone color, and also extreme sensitivity to differences in the periodicity of the beating of a

metronome.”

Page 489: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

468

ability for thought and action, at other times the mechanical stimulus influenced a

recurring, habitual behavior. The conditioned responses caused by the machine’s click

were not a guarded secret, and experimental psychologists readily acknowledged that

metronomic sound habituated people, especially impressionable youths, in a host of

activities. Florence Edna Mateer in Child Behavior; a Critical and Experimental Study of

Young Children (1918) noted that even the most fundamental of human sensations, such

as hunger, could be controlled and re-trained through the metronome. She described

psychological metronome-experiments on children that mirrored Pavlov’s tests, which

induced dogs to salivate. Mateer reports:

An auditory stimulus, the ringing of a metronome, was used for ten seconds and

then ten seconds after the ringing stopped the child was fed. As early as the

second trial the reaction occurred, but it occurred during the ringing of the

metronome. Gradually it came later and later after the initiation of the auditory

stimulus and finally after 15 trials was delayed until the metronome has stopped.

The results of the work upon these eight children left no doubt in the mind

of the experimenter as to the feasibility of applying the method of conditioned

reflexes in the study of young children.8

Ironically, while Pavlov’s new branch of experimental psychology limited metronomic-

training experiments to animals, the metronome grew all the more essential in the

habitual training of young, amateur musicians. Henry Granger Hanchett, in his 1905 text

The Art of the Musician, acknowledged these habitual metronomic methods that rapidly

emerged during his time devalued the student’s musicality and creativity for the sake of

mechanical proficiency. Hanchett likened these scientific-musical pedagogies to the

severe, inhuman labor methods of the industrial factory, and argued:

8 Florence Edna Mateer, Child Behavior; a Critical and Experimental Study of Young Children by

the Method of Conditioned Reflexes (Boston: Richard G. Badger, 1920), 134-5.

Page 490: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

469

Great music stimulates great technic; but when we shall have gained a true

conception of what music study means we shall not concern ourselves to spend

hours upon scales, arpeggios and etudes, and only brief moments upon tone-

poems…does not he resemble the man with the muck-rake, who while ignorant of

the wealth of tonal imaginings strives day after day under the lash of the

metronome to attain a velocity of a thousand notes a minute?9

For roughly a century, skilled musicians instinctually understood that incessant practice

with the clockwork metronome reflected a tyrannical, artificial imposition upon the

student’s creative sense. Even Rachmaninoff noticed the dangerous effects that scientific

training methods had upon youth, stating by 1913:

I do not approve of continual practice with the metronome. The metronome is

designed to set the time, and if not abused is a very faithful servant. However, it

should only be used for this purpose. The most mechanical playing imaginable

can proceed from those who make themselves slaves to this little musical clock,

which was never intended to stand like a ruler over every minute of the student's

practice time.10

Could it be that twentieth-century music students and teachers became habituated en

masse to the metronome through modern pedagogies, of which Eurhythmics was both the

most scientific and the most severe? Were not Paderewski’s “metronome-believers” in

conservatories and concert halls, Hanchett’s “muck-rakers” and Rachmaninoff’s “slaves”

in practice rooms—in addition to Pavlov’s laboratory dogs—trained under the very same

external stimulus? Many non-musicians and critics in an industrializing society had

grown to recognize that automatic machines influenced and altered the human psyche.

9 Henry G. Hanchett, The Art of the Musician, a Guide to the Intelligent Appreciation of Music

(New York: Grosset & Dunlap, 1905), 314.

10 James Francis Cooke, Great Pianists on Piano Playing: Study Talks with Foremost Virtuosos

(Reprint, Philadelphia: Theodore Presser Co., 1917), 213. Rachmaninoff comments, “The

metronome itself must not be used ‘with closed eyes,’ as we should say it in Russia. The player

must use discretion.”

Page 491: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

470

Indeed, cultural critic Walter Lipmann recalled what seemed to be common knowledge

by 1922, that “the beat of a metronome will depress intelligence.”11

Yet modern rhythmical education was grounded in automatical behavior. Jaques-

Dalcroze justified his pedagogical intentions through these scientific values, which were

directly based in the regulative, drilling experiments of psychologists such as Hall,

Meumann, and others. In his 1914 article “Rhythmic Movement, Solfège, and

Improvisation,” Jaques-Dalcroze prescribed that his “special gymnastic system,

habituating muscles to contract and relax, and corporal lines to widen and shrink in time

and space, should supplement metrical feeling and instinct for rhythm.”12

Valuing the

new role of experimental psychology in societal education, Jaques-Dalcroze proclaimed:

“The better our lives are regulated, the freer we become in every way…The more

automatism possessed by our body, the more our soul will rise above material things.”13

Nearly a decade earlier, educator and psychologist G. Stanley Hall also expressed this

paradoxical view of automatical humanity, stating “the ascent from drudgery to freedom”

in education “comes through automatism.”14

Other scientific-musical pedagogues including Harriette Moore Brower also

admitted to the mechanized, habitual effects of their rhythmic-training:

11 Walter Lippman, Public Opinion (New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company, 1922), 72.

12

Jaques-Dalcroze, “Rhythmic Movement, Solfège, and Improvisation,” in Rhythm, Music, and

Education, 115.

13

Ibid., 116.

14

Hall, 46-7.

Page 492: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

471

I cannot urge too strongly the constant effort to play in time…we must have a just

sense of the mathematical values of notes. This is only acquired by constant

timekeeping and counting.15

Time-beating exercises may be made by using short

pieces of moderate difficulty and tapping the time value of the notes to the beat of

the metronome. Easy movements from the Mozart sonatas will furnish abundant

material.16

The only way to have a metronome in one’s head, that is, to have a

thoroughly grounded sense of rhythm, is to make good use of this valuable little

monitor. Let us stand up for this tireless little “policeman.”17

For the modern pedagogues of rhythm, Jaques-Dalcroze most conspicuously, habituation

to standardized and precise mechanical action—with the corresponding loss of inefficient

mental reflection and subjective judgment—was a virtuous influence on the modern

child, whose “physical movements repeatedly performed create corresponding images in

the brain…The more the pupil concentrates while making that movement, the clearer will

be the corresponding mental image, and the more fully will the sense for meter and

rhythm be developed. We might say that these movement images store up the

innervations which bring about the actual movement” in Eurhythmics training.18

15 Harriette Moore Brower, Self-Help in Piano Study (New York: Frederick A. Stokes Co., 1920),

16-17.

16

Ibid., 18.

17

Ibid., 120-121.

18

Ingham, 43.

Page 493: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

472

Figure 7.1. Jaques-Dalcroze’s young Eurhythmics students perform an exercise in his neo-Hellenic school

at Hellerau, Germany—otherwise known as the Temple of Rhythm. From Ethel Ingham, “Lessons at

Hellerau,” in The Eurhythmics of Jaques-Dalcroze (Boston: Small, Maynard, and Company, 1913).

Beyond the activities devised for many isolated laboratory experiments, it is

certain that metronomic rhythm aided, informed, and influenced children and adults in a

myriad of performance acts only beginning at the turn of the century. And as education

with the clockwork metronome grew, the belief in metronomic action grew. The

metronomic research that originated in the astronomical observatory moved rapidly to

many modern laboratories by the 1870s, where the metronome’s precise clockwork sound

guided a variety of chronographic tests on human physicality, perception, and

performance through the scientific paradigm of “rhythm.” By the last decade of the

nineteenth century, experimental psychologists moved out of their university-funded

laboratories and imbued their mechanical methods upon the mass education of an

industrializing, middle-class culture. In 1904 Stanley Hall stated in Youth: Its Education,

Page 494: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

473

Regime, and Hygiene that children’s “conduct should be mechanized as early and

completely as possible. The child’s notion of what is right is what is habitual, and the

simple, to which all else is reduced in thought, is identified with the familiar.”19

Thus

evidence suggests that in the thirty years from 1890 to 1920, the metronome became

“identified with the familiar” in public education; the simple clicks of the metronome

became “what is right” in musical time. The clockwork metronome and its rhythm

became, for many teachers and students, what behavioral psychologists might have

recognized as an “expectation” to standard musical thought and action; for those trained

under metronomic reference, the combined practice of reading precisely typeset notation

while listening to a continuous, unremitting succession of clockwork clicks became a

new rhythmic reality, the new unquestioned underpinning of modern music performance.

Starting in the mid-1890s, both teachers and scientists applied the metronome

with the same intentions: to measure and inculcate the standards of clockwork movement

upon their subjects. As a new wave of “scientific” teachers began drilling young amateurs

to efficiently perform scales, trills, accelerando, and ritardando within artificial

metronomic constrictions, it seems probable that the nature of music performance

fundamentally and irrevocably changed in the minds of a new generation of twentieth-

century performers subscribing to these methods. With the individual sensation of pulse

and movement effectively devalued in education, the modern student trained with the

goal of scientifically synchronizing melodic phrases, gestures, ornaments, and harmonies

to objective, precise metronomic rates alone. The Eurhythmics of Jaques-Dalcroze (1913)

confirmed the regimented values of mass, rhythmic youth training:

19 Hall, 336.

Page 495: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

474

Although feeling for rhythm is more or less latent in us all and can be developed,

few have it naturally perfect…By means of these [exercises] the pupil is taught

how to arrest movement suddenly or slowly, to move alternately forwards or

backwards, to spring at a given signal, to lie down or stand up in the exact time of

a bar of music—in each case with a minimum of muscular effort and without for a

moment losing the feeling for each time-unit of the music.20

Thus, musical time—once predominantly conceived through the variable qualities of

human pulse, rhetoric, movement, and gesture—converged ever closer to the temporal

invariability heard in the precise clockwork “time-unit” and metronomic “impression.”

Through their neglect of the silent, simple pendulum, a new generation of

twentieth-century performers, pedagogues, critics, and researchers considered that a

finite, swinging pulse was too vague and imperfect a reference for modern times—during

an age where chronographic methods and metronomic precisions practically dominated

over all facets of mass education—an age that “would be hard to run on a sun dial.”21

As

the simple pendulum became an arcane tool in both the arts and the sciences, even the

swing of Maelzel’s metronome became the remnant of a past epistemology, a hopelessly

“imprecise” and inefficient signifier of musical movement. The metronome’s clockwork

click alone now defined the true time, a new precision-oriented rhythm: the

homogeneous, chronographic constant for a normalizing and standardized education.

As a last generation of skilled musicians warned of the anti-creative, anti-

expressive, and anti-human qualities of metronomic action, automatical values rapidly

became the rule of movement in time; despite the final caveats of those such as

Constantin von Sternberg, the rhythm of the machine began to efficiently train and

20 Ingham, 43.

21“Time and Clocks,” New York Times, June 27, 1920, 60.

Page 496: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

475

educate the whole of an industrialized civilization in a new performance culture. Indeed,

Carl Emil Seashore, in 1919, verified the acceptance of the modern, chronographic

procedure of musical performance with little regard for—or even knowledge of—past

practices, values, and traditions, as he definitively claimed: “The capacity for rhythm

rests upon certain fundamental powers which can be measured serviceably in various

forms by methods now being introduced through experimental psychology.”22

The Automatical Musician Actualized, Circa 1920 and Beyond

All skilled musicians—seasoned scholars and educators, too—begin their intellectual and

creative lives first as students. The effects of elementary pedagogies upon advanced

practices have yet to be explored by musicologists in greater detail. Yet given the

scientific knowledge that metronomic habituation on the mind and body can indeed occur

over time, a strong possibility exists that musicians inculcated with metronomic-musical

time—the modern, chronographic “capacity for rhythm”—during early ages may

maintain mechanical ideals of performance practices and aesthetics in adulthood. If a

metronomic effect on the mind and body exists, then a metronomic “musicality” taught

during youth may not diminish with age; those roots, when unchecked, possibly grow

stronger.

The continuing intellectual and cultural history presented in this final chapter

further shows that many twentieth-century music researchers, performers, and

composers—either educated in or favorable towards metronomic values—continued to

redefine musical time and action through scientific, mechanically oriented aesthetics. As

22 Seashore, “The Sense of Rhythm as a Musical Talent,” 513.

Page 497: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

476

the musical practices and pedagogies of our time confirm, “metronome believing”

eventually became an accepted and standard practice of music making. While Hofmann,

Paderewski, Sternberg, and Rachmaninoff represented the waning tradition of pre-

metronomic musicians, more scientifically influenced researchers, composers, and

performers—many educated through music-conservatory regimens at the turn of the

century—clearly promoted the modern clockwork metronome in ways that seemingly

effaced prior notions and qualities of musical temporality. When playing like a

metronome once seemed “odd,” “uncanny,” “abhorrent,” or even “impossible,” the third

decade of the twentieth century witnessed the practice eventually become expected and

“commonplace”—and a completely positive trait of musicianship. No longer limited to

children’s practice routines, metronomic-musical time became a new fundamental for

skilled musicians, musical scholars, and modern composers alike. The remainder of this

study shows how a new wave of professional researchers, educators, and creative

musicians in the twentieth century espoused a new “musicality,” in which mechanically

objective rhythms and tempos became a guiding a priori aesthetic for compositions and

performance practices both past and present. It is a chronographic “musicality” that, as I

suggest in this conclusion, still prevails in many of the research methodologies,

performances, and compositions of our age.

Although musicians such as Jaques-Dalcroze and Mary Hallock seem to

foreshadow this larger, century-long trend, Béla Bartók (1881-1945) perhaps most

conspicuously exemplifies how modern scientifically oriented musicians in the twentieth

century began to consider music notation, performance practices, and musicological

research through greater chronographic precisions. Bartók’s ever-growing reliance on

Page 498: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

477

precise mechanical time controls—including the metronome and the stopwatch—to

define initial tempos, tempo fluctuations, and performance durations is well documented

in recent years by László Somfai.23

As a more scientifically oriented musician, concerned

with more precise ways of documenting musical time and performance practices for folk,

classical, and modern musical cultures, Bartók’s use of the metronome was far more

rigorous and consistent than Beethoven’s, or any composer-performer’s application of the

machine from previous centuries. It should be noted that Bartók’s penchant for musical-

mechanical time study more faithfully mirrors contemporaneous the laboratory research

of Seashore and the labor-training methods of Frederick Taylor and Hugo Münsterberg.

Around 1906, Bartók began transcribing Hungarian folk tunes with highly precise

metronomic markings, in ethnographic musical research also similar to experimental

psychologist Charles Samuel Myers’s studies, which occurred less than a decade prior.24

23 László Somfai, Béla Bartók: Composition, Concepts, and Autograph Sources, Volume 9 of

Ernest Bloch lectures (Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press, 1996), 252-262.

24

See C. S. Myers, et al., “Anthropological Reviews and Miscellanea,” The Journal of the

Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 29 (1899).

Page 499: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

478

Figure 7.2. Two of Bartók’s folk-music transcriptions using high-precision metronomic data to help define the tempi and rhythms of performances that did not originally use or necessitate such mechanical time controls. These studies were in keeping with contemporaneous ethnographic research done by experimental psychologists such as C. S. Myers. The tempo markings “Parlando x =320” and “Tempo giuso q =94” attest to Bartók’s penchant for detailed, scientific-metronomic calculations of musical time. See http://www.merian.de/fotostrecke/fotostrecke-47840-5.html. Accessed September 20, 2009.

While Bartók regularly took the clockwork metronome with him to record folk

tunes as far away as Turkey,25

evidence also suggests that the metronome was a near-

permanent fixture atop Bartók’s piano to more precisely convey musical-temporal

intentions for both his creations and the compositions of others. Encouraged to be a

technically proficient pianist in his youth, Bartók once described his common routine-

based practice sessions, perhaps under the lash of the metronome, with his mother, his

first piano teacher, keeping close watch. In 1931, he admitted to a writer for The Living

Age, “practicing scales bored me, as it does every child, bored me to death.”26

Yet, the

25 A. Adnan Saygun, “Bartók in Turkey,” The Musical Quarterly 37 (Jan., 1951): 6.

26 Desider Kosztolanyi, “Béla Bartók, Hungarian Composer,” The Living Age, Aug. 1931, 566.

Page 500: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

479

metronome and mechanical time controls seem to have been a close part of Bartók’s

musical education, scholarship, and certainly his compositional processes his entire life.

Indeed, Bartók provided meticulous metronomic information for many of his pedagogical

piano compositions, a fact which one music educator commented upon in 1961 as it

related to the modern values of “musicality:”

As regards specifics of musicianship in the Mikrokosmos [1926, 1932-9], the

observer probably will note the exactitude in musical terms how the pieces are to

be played…almost all of the pieces contain three kinds of tempo indicators:

traditional tempo marks in the Italian language, metronome marks, and time of

performance expressed in minutes and seconds. The performer is left with no

doubt as to Bartók’s intentions and, what may be more important, he is provided

with the opportunity to acquire a concept of tempo in tangible [i.e. mechanical

and objective] terms.27

Bartók, through his research methods and compositional processes, exemplifies this

cultural paradigm shift in the values of musical time, which necessitated a newly

“skilled” professional musician to work alongside the clockwork metronome for a now

desirable mechanical accuracy in practice, performance, and musical creation. For many

modern performer-composers, the clockwork metronome—once primarily intended to

gently assist novices, amateurs, and the perpetually un-musical—became a primary tool

in the promotion of more exacting beats-per-minute “rhythms” and “tempos” for

increasingly precise performance practices. While musicologist Robert Philip rightly

notes that Bartók’s own recordings confirm his musicality to be far less “rigid” that those

of today, the contemporaneous reception of Bartók’s piano playing was, at times, telling

of the new metronomic aesthetic in professional music making. In 1922, Percy Scholes,

perhaps most notably described Bartók’s style in the London Observer as “a hard, cold

27 Benjamin Suchoff, “Béla Bartók's Contributions to Music Education,” Journal of Research in

Music Education 9 (Spring, 1961): 6

Page 501: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

480

rattle of a keyboard, violently attacked in chance combinations of keys and notes, with

the stiffened metal muscle of a jerkily rhythmic automaton.”28

Figure 7.3. An historical reconstruction of Bartók’s somewhat machine-oriented music room, circa 1940,

containing a phonograph, a typewriter, and a clockwork metronome, which sits directly atop the piano. The

importance Bartók placed upon the clockwork metronome and metronomic indications was far greater than

that of composers from previous centuries, Beethoven especially. See

http://www.merian.de/fotostrecke/fotostrecke-47840-2.html. Accessed September 20, 2009.

Bartók was neither the first nor the last performer-composer in the modern

century to be considered a musical robot due to his new conceptualizations and

28 Robert Philip, Performing Music in the Age of Recording (New Haven: Yale University Press,

2004), 174.

Page 502: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

481

actualizations of the metronomic pulse. Some contemporaneous critics recognized that it

was Stravinsky who most prominently refashioned modern musicality and musical time,

seemingly bringing a century-old Maelzelian world to full fruition. Indeed, the

mechanical culture Maelzel first exhibited on isolated stages seemed for many to be the

hallmark of Stravinsky’s creativity—in which an unfeeling, automatical rhythm

predominated over variable, rhetorical expressions of musical time. Critic Paul

Rosenfeld, for example, heard The Rite of Spring in 1920—seven years after its

premiere—moving with the inhuman rhythmic precisions of the modern mechanical

world:

The new steel organs of man have begotten their music in “Le Sacre du

printemps.” For with Stravinsky, the rhythms of machinery enter musical art.

With this his magistral work a new chapter of music commences, the

spiritualization of the new body of man is manifest…Through him, music has

become again cubical, lapidary, massive, mechanistic…

Above all, there is rhythm that lunges and beats and reiterates and dances

with all the steely perfect tirelessness of the machine, shoots out and draws back,

shoots upward and shoots down, with the inhuman motion of titanic arms of steel.

Indeed, the change is as radical, as complete, as though in the midst of moonlit

noble gardens a giant machine had arisen swiftly from the ground and inundated

the night with electrical glare and set its metal thews and organs and joints

relentlessly whirring, relentlessly functioning.29

Stravinsky’s The Rite of Spring seemed for many, including Rosenfeld, Pound, and later

Adorno, to project an inhuman “time” founded on incessant, mechanical movement.

Rosenfeld, in 1927, continued this assessment by painting a verbal portrait of Stravinsky

as a peculiar brand of modern musician, seemingly habituated to the automatical rhythm

of his age:

29 Paul Rosenfeld, Musical Portraits (New York: Harcourt, Brace and Howe, 1920), 191-2.

Page 503: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

482

No doubt, the mechanistic view of life, the vision of a world almost emptied of

divine and human energy and moving with the relentless precision of clocks and

machinery, was to a great extent in Stravinsky a purely sensuous reflex from the

prodigies of the age of steel, from the feelings of the huge modern cities so far

removed from the irregular, unpredictable rhythms of Nature, and from

industrialism’s mechanical application of the preferred individual function.30

Similar to Scholes’ description of Bartók, Rosenfeld suggested that Stravinsky was on

some level a musical automaton—but for Rosenfeld, by 1920 at least, this had become a

positive value in support of a modernist-mechanical aesthetic. For Rosenfeld, Stravinsky

reflected a precision-based musician devoid of all problems inherent the personal

equation, a perfect reproducer of metronomic motions:

It is as though the infection of the dancing, lunging, pumping piston-rods, walking

beams, drills, has awakened out of Stravinsky a response and given him his power

to beat out rhythm. The machine has always fascinated him.31

Many became fascinated with the prospect of a “musical-mechanical rhythm” during the

age, and for some, the machine now projected the only “rhythm” for which to aspire. Not

limiting himself to children’s education, Jaques-Dalcroze—a significant influence on

Ballets Russes dancer and Rite of Spring choreographer Vaslav Nijinsky32

—was similarly

enthralled with the aesthetics of the automatical. Beyond his Temple of Rhythm, which

had closed due to World War I, Jaques-Dalcroze aspired for a modern musical culture

seemingly regulated through a continuous, industrialized “rhythm” of the factory. In his

essay “Eurhythmics and Musical Composition” (1915), Jaques-Dalcroze expresses

30 Paul Rosenfeld, Modern Tendencies in Music (New York: The Caxton Institute, 1927), 49.

31

Rosenfeld, Musical Portraits, 199.

32

See Margaret Naumburg, “The Dalcroze Idea,” Outlook, Jan. 17, 1914, 131. As the source

relates, Nijinksy visited The Temple of Rhythm prior to 1914.

Page 504: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

483

fascination for a modern Maelzelian stage, where the very heart of “rhythm” could be

found in the movements of modern automata:

No true musician, entering a hall of machinery in full movement, could fail to be

captivated by the whirr of the fabulous symphony produced by the magic of

combined and dissociated rhythms, and to be tempted inevitable to extract the

secrets of this moving and quivering life that animates nature, man, and his work

alike.33

For many, including Rosenfeld, Pound, and Jaques-Dalcroze, the actualization of the

automatical—stimulated in part by scientific-industrial training methods appearing earlier

in the century—was indeed occurring throughout Western musical culture well into the

1920s. And for many, this audible standard of continuous, externalized, mechanical

rhythm—which the clockwork metronome prominently provided in compositional,

pedagogical, and performance practices—was a necessary and positive aesthetic of

“musical time” to embrace.

While numerous Eurhythmics schools were in existence throughout the world by

1924,34

Stravinsky, a decade after the première of le Sacre, seemed to be at the vanguard

of an aesthetical trend first promoted by Jaques-Dalcroze—a desire for objective,

calculable rhythms and continuous, standardized tempos uninfluenced by personal

interpretation. While this historical reception has been somewhat downplayed by Richard

Taruskin in his meticulous and comprehensive Stravinsky and the Russian Traditions,

33 The essay is translated and reprinted in Dalcroze, Rhythm, Music, and Education (1921), 146.

34

In the 1925 publication The Importance of Being Rhythmic, author Jo Pennington documents

the rapid spread of Eurhythmics training; she lists no less than 28 independent schools that

trained Jaques-Dalcroze’s methods to musicians, actors, and dancers in major metropolitan areas

throughout the United States. See Jo Pennington, The Importance of Being Rhythmic, Appendix

“List of Private and Special Schools in the United States Where Dalcroze Eurhythmics Is

Taught,” 141-142. The list was complied in the summer of 1924.

Page 505: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

484

some contemporaneous perceptions in the 1920s of Stravinsky’s rhythmical-mechanical

qualities are significant to mention here. In 1924, New York Times critic Olin Downes

likened Stravinsky to a scientist, who constructed the rhythms of le Sacre with atomic

energy.35

A New York Times article from 1925, expounding on the composer’s affinity

for the pianola, quoted Stravinsky proclaiming: “I began mechanical music.”36

Of course, the Italian Futurists could be considered the first truly mechanical

composers of the twentieth century, as they took musical time and performance practices

outside of the realm of living pulse and human movement entirely. For Russolo, the

personal equation of musicality and the pulse-sense held no relevance within a culture

that “possesses today more than a thousand different machines, each with a distinctive

noise. With the incessant multiplication of new machines we shall some day be able to

distinguish ten, twenty, or thirty thousand different noises.”37

Yet, less radically than the Futurists, Stravinsky had seemed at times to be

modernity’s greatest living embodiment of Johann Maelzel’s automaton culture by the

1920s. Mitzi Kolisch, writing for the Independent in 1925, further pointed to Stravinsky’s

Dalcrozian sensibilities when she commented, “Stravinsky had turned

mathematician…his mind has become too involved in the geometric problems of splitting

rhythms.”38

Identically to Wundt, Jaques-Dalcroze, and Seashore, Stravinsky strongly

35 Olin Downes, “Music,” New York Times, Mar. 16, 1924, S5. ProQuest Historical Newspapers.

36

M. B. Levick, “Stravinsky Sees Vision of a New Music,” New York Times, Jan. 18, 1925,

SM12. ProQuest Historical Newspapers.

37

From Russolo, “The Art of Noise,” reprinted in Ernest Newman, A Musical Motley (New York:

John Lane Company, 1919), 298-299.

38 Mitzi Kolisch, “Stravinsky--Russian of the Russians,” The Independent, May 16, 1925, 559.

Page 506: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

485

believed that “musical time” was an objective, scientific process of action and reaction

outside of individual control. In his ironically titled collection of published lectures,

Poetics of Music, Stravinsky explained the science of musical time:

The laws that regulate the movement of sounds require the presence of a

measureable and constant value: meter, a purely material element, through which

rhythm, a purely formal element is realized. In other words, meter answers the

question of how many equal parts the musical unit which we call a measure is to

be divided into, and rhythm answers the question of how these equal parts will be

grouped within a given measure.39

He described that musical time had at its basis “an obsession with regularity” formed

exclusively through “isochronous beats.”40

It can be no wonder that even late in life,

Stravinsky, once-hailed a musical machine, kept close by a pocket-watch metronome

with “hands [that swing] back and forth,” as his student Earnest Andersson noted in

1940.41

Perhaps the most conspicuous promoter of the radical metronomic-aesthetic of

musicality in the 1920s was also its most unlikely champion—he was a once-promising

student of Constantin von Sternberg, that last of the traditional pedagogues to howl down

the clockwork metronome as destroyer of musicality for students and professionals alike.

Sternberg’s former pupil, the young musician who mechanized living tempo and rhythm

to its greatest extent to date: George Antheil. According to Antheil himself, the impetus

for creating his “infamous” Ballet mécanique (1924-5), was on some level, a brash,

39 Igor Stravinsky, Poetics of Music in the Form of Six Lessons, translated by Arthur Knodel &

Ingolf Dahl (New York: Vintage Books, 1956), 28.

40

Ibid., 29.

41 H. Colin Slim, “Lessons with Stravinsky: The Notebook of Earnest Andersson (1879-1940),”

JAMS 62 (Summer, 2009): 389.

Page 507: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

486

“youthful” reaction against the pre-metronomic aesthetics and culture of his elders

Sternberg and Joseph Hofmann. While critics including Pound and Rosenfeld certainly

heard the metronomic imprint of Stravinsky in the Ballet mécanique—most notably from

the stage-works le Sacre (1913), Renard (premiered 1922), and the heavily Eurhythmics-

charged Les Noces (premiered 1923)—Antheil’s work represented for many the most

blatant attempt to mechanize musicality through an industrial-scientific ideal: an aesthetic

which continued to sublimate the personal equation, the living sensation and actualization

of rhythm, to objective mechanical time and motion, as Robert Forrest Wilson noted in

1925:

When first he heard an automatic piano play a scale he know that the human hand

and the human equation in the rendition of music were doomed. Virtuosity could

never hope to equal the flawless even beauty of that performance; and so George

Antheil turned to the composition of the precisian, machine made music for

electric driven pianos.42

According to Bartók, Stravinsky considered the benefit of the mechanical player piano

for related reasons. Bartók recalled, “some famous composers (Stravinsky for instance)

wrote compositions specifically for pianola…The intent, however, was not to achieve

superior performance but to restrict the an absolute minimum the intervention of the

performer’s personality,” that is, the performer’s personal-equation of rhythm.43

As with

Stravinsky before him, Antheil found the automaton to now provide the inspiration and

actualization of a musical “virtuosity” in which individualistic musicianship expressed

through rhetorical rhythmic gestures all but died.

42 Robert Forrest Wilson, “Paris for Young Art,” The Bookman; a Review of Books and Life (Jun.

1925): 408.

43 Quoted in Vera Stravinsky and Robert Craft, Stravinsky in Pictures and Documents (New

York: Simon and Schuster, 1978), 164.

Page 508: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

487

Yet even before Antheil composed the Ballet mécanique, in which musicians

played a role identical to the chronographically pounding pianolas on stage, Antheil’s

performance practices as a famed recitalist garnered considerable recognition for its

remarkable mechanical “virtuosity”—perhaps matching that “flawless even beauty” he

reportedly heard in mechanical pianos. It was Erik Satie who, in recognition of this

uncanny musicianship, reportedly shouted at the riot ensuing Antheil’s 1923 solo recital

in Paris: “What precision!”44

Pound likewise proclaimed Antheil to be the new musician

who performed and composed under “a new precision” particularly suited to the

mechanical age. Indeed, Wilson’s 1925 portrait of the young composer suggests that

Antheil was imprinted by the incessant, mechanical rhythms emanating from the

industrial environs of his childhood, explaining, “the sounds of Trenton—its machine

shops, its potteries, the Pennsylvania trains rushing through the station—are the sounds of

America that he remembers.”45

Beyond these mechanical influences, Antheil’s seemingly uncommon musical-

mechanical performance quality, as the pianist-composer implied decades later, came

about through a near-automatical practice procedure:

You keep on. You must never stop. And so technique comes to you. It begins to

fit like a suit. You play concert pieces so many times that you could hardly play a

wrong note in them if you wanted to. The hind part of the brain takes over.46

44 George Antheil, Bad Boy of Music (Reprint, Hollywood: Samuel French, 1990), 133. Satie

seems to have taken an interest in mechanical pianos and music making since Stravinsky’s

pianola compositions. Writing to Stravinsky in October 1922, Satie, in considering the new

objectivist aesthetic, recognized “the stupid prejudice that artists (?) have against all mechanical

interpretation, and I point out [in an article] all that we owe to you in this respect” of mechanizing

music. See Stravinsky: Selected Correspondence, ed. Robert Craft, volume I (New York: Knopf,

1982), 12.

45

Wilson, 409.

46

See George Antheil, Bad Boy of Music, 68.

Page 509: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

488

Looking back on his controversial career, Antheil depicted himself as something of a

Wundtian or Pavlovian laboratory subject, who was exposed daily to chronographic

repetition and reaction. Admitting to a habitual training regimen, Antheil seemingly

sought to become musically automatical, a living pianola, or what psychologist

Ebbinghaus once called a “sheer mechanical associator.” Stravinsky similarly considered

composing as habituation to a mechanically objective sounds and actions, stating in 1929,

“every musical work comes by impression which, crystallizing in the brain, in the ear,

little by little but mathematically, become finally concrete in notes and rhythms.”47

As

Antheil and Stravinsky suggest—and the pedagogies of Hall and Jaques-Dalcroze

verify—musical automatism held little stigma in the modern century for modern culture.

Even disregarding the anecdotal evidence suggesting that Antheil habituated to

metronomic rhythm early in life, Antheil’s practice routine—which resulted in the

musical-mechanical precision that many contemporaries thought essential to mention—

was undoubtedly informed by a modern chronographic pedagogy and aesthetics well

distanced from the compositional teachings of Sternberg. Indeed, Antheil did not keep his

Dalcrozian-Stravinkian concepts of musical time a secret. After achieving notoriety from

the Ballet mécanique première, he occasionally attempted to promote and market a new

system of “Antheilicized Notation, Or Music for Everybody Who Can Tell One from

Two and Two from Three.” In his manuscript treatise, Antheil offers an increasingly

common scientific aesthetic of musical time and rhythmical notation—espoused by

47

Boris de Schloezer, “A Classic Art,” The Dial; a Semi-monthly Journal of Literary Criticism,

Discussion, and Information, Jul. 1929, 597. American Periodicals Series Online.

Page 510: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

489

Scripture and Seashore before him—that correspondingly discounted the subjective,

verbal, and sensory epistemologies of the past:

There is no literature or fantasy about this notation. It is absolute, mathematical,

and pictorial…Any system denoting the movement of time in space in an

unmathematical, sentimental, and literary way is to be condemned as

incomprehensible.

One can only measure spaces with mathematics!48

[The new system] will be a universal musical housecleaning. [It is] the greatest

thing that musical art [can do] to flourish in an age that is forever [becoming] less

sentimental, and more practical, and [of] greater speed.49

The mechanical efficiency of time and musical action espoused by experimental

psychologists in laboratory settings seemingly grounded Antheil’s musicality in the

practice room and on the concert stage. Due to Antheil’s distinctly mechanistic and

mathematical concepts of tempo, rhythm, and speed, some critics heard not only

precision in his European piano recitals, but also a performance practice devoid of

humanity—a quality applying both to his own mechanically inspired compositions and

his interpretations of historical music. The Times noted on June 23, 1922 that a

corresponding devaluation of rhetorical gesture and individual rhythmical pulsation

resulted from Antheil’s precision-based performance practices:

Nowadays the style and method have become commonplace, and one soon finds it

difficult to keep one’s attention unless maybe, some very strong personal factor

comes into play in the performance. Mr. Antheil’s playing is too superficial for

that, and though he can be noisy and vehement and has plenty of facility and

agility, it all sounded very dry and unconvincing.

48 George Antheil, “Antheilicized Notation, Or Music for Everybody who Can Tell One from

Two and Two From Three, Without previous experience anyone can now read music at sight.

Instantaneous System of Reading Music,” (Manuscript), Box 10, p. 3-4, Antheil Collection,

Library of Congress, Washington, D.C.

49

Ibid., 5.

Page 511: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

490

This critic recognized that Antheil’s modern aesthetics of musical time—which for some

“have become commonplace” in the early 1920s—also transferred to the pre-metronomic

past. Similarly unconvincing musical results were heard “with his treatment of some

Chopin; technically often not without point—for he has a certain crispness of touch and

rhythm which is attractive—but of poetry not a trace.”50

Many reporters, hearkening to

the pre-metronomic aesthetics of Antheil’s former teacher Sternberg, Paderewski, and

Hofmann, suggested that musical poetics—that rhetorical-musical time gleaned through

traditional meter—was incompatible with the “new precision” heard in mathematically

metronomic performance practices.

Consistent metronomic motion seemingly applied to most of Antheil’s

pedagogical, interpretive, and compositional practices, as Richard L. Stokes intuited after

Antheil’s inglorious 1927 concert in New York City. The critic ascertained that the Ballet

mécanique was musically elementary, expressively stilted, and just as engaging as a

metronomic practice routine since “its material was composed of melodic figures as

rudimentary as finger exercises and of rhythmic noise. And these rhythms were so

monotonous, so iterative and so dull that they grew a weariness to the flesh.”51

The New

York Herald Tribune critic heard a similarly odd, chronographic musical quality, since

the piece was “written in three parts—Allegro—Allegro—allegro. The composition

50 “‘Modern’ Piano Pieces,” Times, June 23, 1922, 7F.

51

Richard L. Stokes, “Realm of Music,” The Evening World, Monday April 11, 1927. Clipping

from George Antheil Collection, Library of Congress, Washington D.C.

Page 512: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

491

begins in one tempo and ends in one tempo; it starts forte and ends forte.”52

Even of

Antheil’s more-tempered, “neo-classical” opera Transatlantic, Alfred Einstein concluded

in 1930: “This music intrinsically possesses no organ of expression. It has, further, no

tempo, and this despite the inclusion within its pages of every sort of rhythm in [notated]

outline.”53

It seemed that in the modern musicality of Antheil, more so than for any

performer-composer preceding him, a metronomic education informed a decidedly

metronomic aesthetic.

Chronographic Aesthetics, Modern Musicality,

and Historical Performance Practices

Ironically, as Bartók and Antheil were being criticized for their automatism, their lack of

musical poetry and living pulse in the recital hall, a prominent promoter of inhuman,

chronographic aesthetics emerged in academia: Carl Emil Seashore. In leaving antiquated

and morally questionable child-study research behind him, Seashore became de facto one

of Western civilization’s first performance-practice musicologists in the early 1920s. In

this capacity, Seashore again charged himself with solving the ever-present problems of

the personal equation, which he clothed in an uncanny new term: “Artistic Deviation.”

Seashore clearly considered that the search for “deviation” was no longer limited to

laboratory subjects or classroom children, but experienced concertizing musicians as

well. Seashore’s “artistic deviation” was, as to be expected by the chronographic

methodology, a human “deviation” from the automatical. Guided by values of

52 “Boos Greet Antheil Ballet of Machines,” New York Herald Tribute, April 11, 1927. Clipping

from George Antheil Collection, Library of Congress, Washington D.C.

53

Alfred Einstein, “‘Transatlantic’ in Frankfort,” New York Times, Jun 22, 1930, X5. ProQuest

Historical Newspapers.

Page 513: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

492

normalization and standardization, mechanical measurements and efficient, reproducible

actions, Seashore, in 1923, described how his chronographic science applied to the

professional musician of the twentieth century: “Everything in the nature of musical

emotion that the musician conveys to the listener can be recorded, measured, repeated,

and controlled for experimental purposes; and that thus we have at hand an approach

which is extraordinarily promising for the scientific study of the expression of musical

emotion.”54

But empirical research using modern methods and machines was not his only aim;

Seashore did not merely wish to chronographically record the musician’s feelings and

actions for posterity. As with the work of other experimental psychologists, Seashore’s

isolated research was to be employed in standardized pedagogies, through modern

conservatory training and beyond. His new chronographic pedagogy touted, implicitly

and explicitly, new modes of performance, a new standard of musical time, and

correspondingly, a new aesthetics of musicality for modern culture en masse. With his

musical-chronographic studies, Seashore vindicated a modern epistemology in which

mechanical action was the first principle to musical time, and indeed, all “time.” In the

1925 article “Deviation from the Regular as an Art Principle,” Seashore expounds upon

the new scientific musicality, in which the mechanically regular, the chronographically

consistent, was the fundamental state of rhythm and tempo, even in advanced practices of

the most skilled performers:

54 C. E. Seashore, “Measurements on the Expression of Emotion in Music,” Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 9, No. 9 (Sept. 15, 1923): 325.

Page 514: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

493

The unlimited re-sources for vocal and instrumental art lie in artistic deviation

from the pure, the true, the exact, the perfect, the rigid, the even and the

precise…The variation from the exact which is due to incapacity for rendering the

exact is, on the whole, ugly. The artist who is to vary effectively from the exact

must know the exact and must have mastered its attainment before his emotion

can express itself adequately through a sort of flirtation with it.55

In Seashore’s nascent brand of musicology, an “exacting” chronographic culture, a

“pure” scientific aesthetics came to full fruition, which explained away volatile human

actions as inconsistencies—considered nothing more than “deviant” behavior from the

strictly mechanical. The chronographic-laboratory ideal of precise automatical action,

espoused decades earlier by Wundt, Hall, Scripture, and others, now became the

objective musical ideal for scientists and singers alike: The “rigid” is “perfect;” the

mechanically “precise” is “beautiful.” “Ugliness,” in contrast, is variation from the

mechanical norm, an aesthetical fault in the now a priori exacting metronomic standard

of rhythm—a standard now applicable to music of all ages.

Seashore’s laboratory aesthetics of musical attractiveness dictated that expression

was no longer innate to musicality, but a logistical deviation from a mechanical absolute.

His epistemology of musical time, in keeping with many proponents of the new scientific

culture, became ever distanced from the time of rhetoric, pulse-sense, or physical

movement. Devaluing the humanity of musical time to greater extents, Seashore

considered that the necessary capacity in being musical was in first being metronomic—

the very foundations of musicality were to be decided in terms of now a priori

chronographic procedure. In his 1942 article “Artistic Deviation as an Esthetic Principle

55 C. E. Seashore and Milton Metfessel, “Deviation from the Regular as an Art Principle,”

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 11 (Sept. 15,

1925): 538.

Page 515: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

494

in Music,” Seashore explained how the individualism inherent in music making—the

personal equation—continued to pose problems for all performers of the modern century

to identify and solve. Citing a myriad of human inconsistencies in musical time and

action, he clearly defined “artistic deviation” from “regular musical time” as an

individual variation from chronographic beats-per-minute precision:

Assuming that the [professional] artist is competent so that the constellation of

variations is significant, the student of esthetics must ask for every cross section

of the score: Why did he augment or diminish the interval to this degree at this

point? Why did he increase or decrease the loudness in this direction and to this

degree at this point? Why did he take liberties with the metronomic time by

lengthening or shortening, by anticipating or by over-holding the note at this

point?56

Seashore recast a complex of musical performance practices in time and tone, along with

human expression, musicality, and aesthetics, into a laboratory procedure established in

Bessel’s nineteenth-century astronomical observatory. He portrays a new musicologist—

the new cultural-scientific aesthetician—as “musical astronomer,” who views the skilled

musician as producer of an objective “constellation of variations.” And like the

astronomer, this performance-practice researcher looked through “every cross section” to

find individual “points” gauged by the ever-present absolute of “metronomic time.” For

Seashore, scientific performance practices became musical performance practices.

Seashore’s “why” dealt with anachronistic mechanical precisions rather than performers’

active, variable processes of rhetorical expression and pulse-sensation. His “why” failed

to consider if a beats-per-minute epistemology factored into the musician’s temporal

“liberties” in the first place. Yet, for those with the scientific-aesthetician’s

chronographic bias—in which all rhythmic values from all ages implied a metronomic-

56 Carl E. Seashore, “Artistic Deviation as an Esthetic Principle in Music,” The Scientific

Monthly 54 (Feb., 1942), 104.

Page 516: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

495

mathematical absolute—performance-practice research sought a mechanistic ideal that

never existed; it espoused precisions and values of precise action that, prior to the

twentieth century, never existed.

Seashore’s musical aesthetics—in which musicians of all centuries and abilities

were indeed recast as secondary, largely passive agents in the phenomena of musical

temporality—foreshadowed and to some extent promoted values which remain prevalent

in performance-practice research to this day. Seashore’s science informed musicological

subtexts such as these: Innate sensations and feelings for musical pulse and meter are

largely discounted; musical time is ultimately mechanical in description, transcription,

and origin; rhythm has little to do with human agency, and it merely reflects the

performer’s secondary reaction to the objective or mechanical standard. Modern

researchers and musicians subscribing to these subtexts seemed no longer content with

qualitative verbal descriptions founded upon an imprecise sensory knowledge, which

implied an a priori variable and individual nature to musical time and performance. Their

scholarly tradition was increasingly distanced from the experiential performance-practice

pedagogies of Rousseau, A. B. Marx, G. Weber, Christiani, and others. Rather, they

tended towards the scientific, the quantifiable, and the objective—albeit reductive—

temporal “proof” found in machines, mathematical calculations, and metronomic devices.

Directly informed by these scientific subtexts, Béla Bartók explicitly devalued

more individualistic folk musicians during his ethnographic-musicological research, in

favor of often-unattainable norms referenced through ever-increasing mechanical

measures. Bartók in his 1936 article “Why and How Do We Collect Folk Music?”

answered the questions of pre-mechanical music making through modern scientific

Page 517: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

496

methodologies and apparatuses. As experimental psychologists such as Seashore and

Scripture before him, Bartók quickly devalued the subjectivity of musical interpretations;

he discounted real performance variability in the speculative search for an objective,

modern musical norm. Bartók explains:

Sometimes the melody is good but the performance is affected…it is important to

avoid recording persons who perform in an inadequate way. A certain bold or

haughty performance, glissades, sentimental slackening of the dance rhythm is

met, fashionable for decades—especially with men and boys—which in my

opinion is a regrettable influence of the city gipsy music or, rather, the

performance mode of the upper classes. However, we must investigate this

transformed type of performance from the scientific viewpoint, no matter how

mediocre we may feel it to be from the aesthetic one. Since we are aquatinted

with it to the point of satiety, it is advisable not to record performances of this

kind.

During the notation of the melodies on the spot, use of the metronome will

enable us to determine the tempo as exactly as possible. If a phonograph is used,

it may also be necessary to time the length of the strophes by means of a

stopwatch.57

For Bartók, those performing outside of the musical-mechanical standard—which

striking included both the lower-class Gypsies and upper-class esthetes, but not “regular”

middle-class musicians such as himself—could easily be disregarded in his scientific

studies. While Bartók probably believed his tools and methods would best document

unfamiliar music for posterity through modern notation and metronomic transcriptions,

he admitted that on occasion his techniques were incommensurate with performance

realities. In documenting Bulgarian performance practices, for instance, Bartók mis-

transcribed certain dance tunes with “faulty rhythms,” thus creating far more simplified

compositions that upheld the modern values of metronomic consistency and

57 Reprinted in Béla Bartók, Essays, edited by Benjamin Suchoff (New York: St. Martins Press,

1976), 18.

Page 518: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

497

impersonalized rhythm.58

Bartók also admitted that his more mechanized understanding

of tempo and meter did not always relate to a pre-metronomic musicality, commenting in

1938, “When I first saw these unfamiliar [Bulgarian] rhythms, in which such fine

differences are decisive, I could hardly imagine that they really existed. But then I

seemed to remember that in my own collection of Rumanian material I had come across

similar phenomena, but at the time had not dared—if I might put it that way—to take

note of them.”59

This typical scientific methodology that rejected the unquantifiable

seamlessly transferred to a new aesthetic of musicological and performance practice

research; it favored the reductive laboratory tradition of chronography above a more

complex, individualistic, and pre-metronomic musicality, which could not often be

“precisely” documented through traditional Western notation or beats-per-minute

indications.

Elements of this methodology are likewise found in musicologist Curt Sachs’

Rhythm and Tempo (1953), which occasionally diminished the individual performer’s

movement and pulse sense—the rhythmical-personal equation—in favor of anachronistic

temporal epistemologies. Sachs took pains to assert, “the [human] tactus was wholly

unconcerned with the actual rhythm, with grouping or accent…the tactus, does not, and

cannot, reflect the rhythm of a piece.”60

Unquantifiable historical-rhythmical movement,

for Sachs, often seemed an unfounded and indefinite guide of tempo and rhythm. The

58 Béla Bartók, “The So Called Bulgarian Rhythm” (1938), reprinted in Essays, 42-43.

59

Ibid., 44-45. Perhaps pointing to his epistemological discrepancies, Bartók first “heard” these

rhythms, he did not “see” them. Since these performances were born of an aural culture, the

“Bulgarian rhythm” was not originally notated.

60 Curt Sachs, Rhythm and Tempo (New York: W.W. Norton & Co., 1953), 242-3.

Page 519: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

498

human tactus, that is real physical motion, seemed at times to be a mere and imperfect

metronomic substitute—perhaps one reason why Sachs translated nearly all historical

repertories with metronomic tempo numbers and consistently described “rhythm” under

mathematically “additive” or “divisive” formulations. And despite evidence to the

contrary, he discounted that any significant feeling for musical time existed in past, pre-

metronomic practices when he surmised: “The strength of good beats…varied

considerably according to the nature of a piece. But performers and listeners were hardly

aware of these minute shades and had no reason for mentioning them.”61

The evidence

presented in Chapter I of this study suggests the need to rethink Sachs’ assumptions and

assertions.

Sachs also considered the performer’s pulse-sense, the once weighted “accent” of

metered music, to be a psychological process of imagining order out of monotony—and

not active or intentional rhythmic expression. Like Seashore and Bartók before him,

Sachs redefines musical time in terms of modern performance practices and

chronographic aesthetics, while neglecting the subjective epistemologies of the past:

The present author thinks that the best antonym of “metrical” would be

“accentual”… Unfortunately, this word is not quite adequate either. Actually, we

accent very little unless there is a sforzato mark on a note; and considerable part

of our allegedly accentual literature is played on organ, and another part was in

earlier centuries performed on the harpsichord, where [volume] accents are

outright impossible…The term “accentual” is, however, acceptable as long as we

keep in mind the basic fact that, as pointed out before, accent, albeit regulative, is

not necessarily perceptible, provided that we ourselves project into music our

awareness of an accentual pattern.62

61 See Sachs, 256-7.

62 Ibid., 28.

Page 520: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

499

Sachs points to a marked turn in the modern musical-temporal epistemology, since meter

and accent were, well through the nineteenth century, often considered a symbiosis. His

modern devaluation of an “alleged” subjective feeling for musical “accent” in favor of an

implied, temporal absolute continues in more recent scholarly studies, as witnessed in

George Houle’s Meter in Music (1987) in which musical “accent” is also akin to a

passive mechanical adjustment of dynamics, not a variable sensation of sonic weight.

Houle, again without supporting evidence, but with a metronomic bias present throughout

his book, states that “accent, defined as dynamic stress by seventeenth- and eighteenth-

century writers, was one of the means of enhancing the perception of meter, but it

became predominant only in the last half of the eighteenth century.”63

Both Sachs and

Houle covertly discounted the subjective sense, the “weight” or “lingering” of musical

accent, since it cannot be definitively quantified through a metronomic indication or

chronographic epistemology.

Without the individual’s “invisible,” “weighted” pulse-sense as guide, the

historical word “tempo” then became for Houle, as it did for many in the twentieth

century, an implicit beats-per-minute value with louder “good” notes distinguishing

themselves from weaker “bad” notes. “Tempo,” Houle believes is “a word that evolved in

the seventeenth century from a mensural term to one meaning the speed of notes. It was

subsequently accepted into English, as designating the [objective] speed of he musical

beat.”64

Under Houle’s modern definitions of a seemingly motor-driven musical time—

63 George Houle, Meter in Music 1600-1800 (Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1987), viii.

64

Ibid., 21. Ironically perhaps, Houle recognized that French term for tempo may have more

subjective meanings when he stated, “temps can be translated ‘beat,’ ‘pulse,’ or ‘time’ [thus]

Page 521: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

500

which transferred to historical musical time—the human pulse-sense, movement, and

rhetorical expressivity played little role. Rather, musical meter was a scientific problem

for the professional musician to solve, even when no precise scientific method existed.

Houle places the modern scientific bias upon historical musical interpretation:

It seems to be the belief of most seventeenth- and eighteenth-century theorists that

musical meter is naturally and adequately perceived by the listener and only

secondarily heightened through performance techniques.

Ever since Wundt’s time studies, it has been the listener who primarily hears—and not

the performer who primarily expresses—the chronographic process of “rhythm.” While

Houle, throughout Meter in Music, urged musicians to learn these historical

“performance techniques” through historical treatises in order to better meet the needs of

long-deceased composer’s exacting wishes,65

he perhaps hypocritically offered

justifications through modern performance practices, which often demand today’s

musician to passively react to an largely uncontrollable “time:”

Most performers today are aware of how crude it is to suggest that the measure is

defined by a regular accent or dynamic stress based on bar lines and time

signatures. This confidence in the listener’s basic metrical perception as defined

by quantitas intrinseca gives rise to a remarkable variety of subtle articulation

techniques that delicately enhance and shape the perception.66

For Houle, this “quantitas intrinsica”—by no means a universally understood, described,

or documented term—became akin to the universal-historical chronographic science of

musical time: it formed an objective succession of “good” and “bad” notes, a tick-tock

English equivalents must be chosen with caution.” He did not explain what cautionary measures

to take, or why they were necessary. See Houle, 37.

65

See for instance, Houle, viii.

66

Ibid., 84.

Page 522: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

501

binary, precisely defining historical performance practices in a continual “tempo giusto”

through a passive process of “metrical perception.” Under this performance science, a

musician ought not to “click” a metrical accent too loudly, for a seemingly individualistic

“crudity” (Seashore’s irregular musical “ugliness”) would result—Houle gives this

prescription again with the mistaken assumption that “accent” only equals an objective

and calculable volume change. Yet, this imagined and absolutist rule of historical musical

time—one hearkening to the rule of metronomic-musical regulation in his century—did

not hold for historical treatises spanning 1600 to 1900 such as those seen in Chapter I.

Indeed, Houle hints at incompatibility between his modern aesthetics and pre-

chronographic musical-temporal practices when, in critiquing Rameau’s time-beating

instructions, he states: “The assignment of a je-ne-sçai-quoi de gracieux to the unequal

beats, while charming, is not precise enough to help the performer. It is not easy to

describe the subtleties of measure organization.”67

Even in 1987, it was modern

mechanical “precision,” an ever-increasing variable, not the imperfect whimsical

“charms” of personal temporal agency (the true historical constant) that seemed to shape

scholarly beliefs in an absolutely quantifiable, objective “organization” for past musical

time.

A Continuing, Chronographic Refashioning of Historical Musical Time

through Neo-Classicism, Modern Performances,

and Scientific-Musicological Scholarship

The move away from subjective rhythm and tempo—which previously occurred in

metronomic pedagogies, furthered in Seashore’s methodologies, and heard in concert

performances by Bartók, Antheil, and Stravinsky—also manifested in the 1920s through

67 Ibid., 52.

Page 523: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

502

compositions that some might consider neo-classical in style. In certain “historically

inspired” works, the mechanical-musical time of modernity further subsumed the

subjective, rhetorical “time” of the past—an outcome not often realized by performers,

critics, or even composers of the age. While such “barbaric” (sometimes “folk”-inspired)

compositions such as Bartók’s “Allegro Barbaro” (1911) and The Miraculous Mandarin

(1924); Stravinsky’s le Sacre; Prokofiev’s Scythian Suite (1914-15) and Le pas d’acier

(“The Steel Step,” 1925); and Antheil’s Ballet mécanique and A Jazz Symphony (1925)

made explicit to many a rough and violent rhythm driven by metronomic motion, a

contrasting neo-classical aesthetic—such as heard in Ravel’s Le tombeau de Couperin

(1914, 1917); Prokofiev’s Classical Symphony (1916-17); Stravinsky’s Pulcinella (1919-

20) and Oedipus rex (1926-7); and Antheil’s Transatlantic (1927-8)—masked this very

same automatic-chronographic time and “beats-per-minute mentality” through a

seemingly dispassionate use of traditional forms and compositional techniques. In

compositions such as these, the mechanical brutality of early modernists’ works

seemingly “evolved” into a more tempered metronomic pulse—but it was a mechanical

pulse nonetheless, and most significantly for this performance-practice history, it was

often mistakenly received as the musical pulse of the “classical” past. During this age,

many past compositional forms and rhythms were revamped with mechanical-temporal

precisions. Even a deeply traditional Spanish dance took on modern automatical qualities

for Ravel, who admitted: “I gained much of my inspiration from machinery…I love

going over factories and seeing vast machinery at work. It is awe-inspiring and great. It

was a factory which inspired my Bolero (1927).”68

Once some composers fused modern

68 Reprinted in Daniel Albright, ed., Modernism and Music (Chicago: University of Chicago

Page 524: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

503

automatical rhythm to pre-metronomic musical forms and meters, even past compositions

became reconsidered as cold, calculable, and objective—thus, some so-called neo-

classical compositions often shared with historically “classical” compositions the modern

metronomic pulse, which was clearly becoming the homogeneous trait of musical time in

the twentieth century.

While a comprehensive analysis of the significant modern and “historically

inspired” compositions that project or promote an increasingly objective metronomic

time must wait for a future study, it is essential to chart presently how some viewed so-

called “neo-classical” aesthetics without distinguishing between the precise metronomic

times of modernity and the musical rhythm of a pre-chronographic past, and how this

lack of distinction influenced “musicality” for performers, composers, and scholars alike

throughout the century. In his 1926 article “The Younger Composers of France; Esthetic

Tendencies of Generation,” early-music scholar Henry Prunières voiced skepticism

regarding the aesthetics of so-called neo-classicism, which seemed to him shallow

mimicry, a trend which would yield nothing truly new or culturally relevant. Prunières

warned that modern composers “have much to learn from a Josquin or Palestrina,

Montevere, Lully, Purcell, Carissimi, Vivaldi, Scarlatti—but while cultivating these

masters they may conceive other musical forms than those they were taught at school;

they must beware of becoming imitators and losing the imprint of their age.”69

Yet as

many other critics recognized, those seemingly “automatical” modernists, some of whom

Press, 2004), 308.

69

Henry Prunieres, “The Younger Composers of France; Esthetic Tendencies of Generation,”

New York Times, Aug. 29, 1926, X7. ProQuest Historical Newspapers.

Page 525: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

504

have been labeled neo-classicists—Stravinsky, Hindemith, Ravel, Prokofiev, and Antheil

among them—strongly imprinted the compositional forms and techniques of the past

with modern metronomic rhythm and regulation, less blatantly conflating pre-mechanical

compositional techniques and forms with a “Ballet mécanique” temporal aesthetic.

In contrast to Prunières’ skepticism of the compositional trend, the New York

Times in 1928 recognized modern rhythmic tendencies in the neo-classicism of

“Stravinsky, [who] has attempted to install his 1924 twelve-cylinder motor in a post-

chaise built when Bach was cantor at Leipsic.”70

The anonymous critic, in other words,

believed that Stravinsky’s modern, automatic engine of rhythm revamped the very

structure of baroque form—now strikingly considered a historical music-machine of

lesser power and precision. Regarding the specious Bach-Stravinsky relationship voiced

by many during the decade, one perceptive contemporary well understood the distinction

between Stravinsky’s claims to Baroque music and a modern-music reality that some

cultural critics seemingly neglected. In a 1928 Musical Times article, “The Stravinsky

Legends,” Leonid Savaneev considered that it was Bach who became mechanized

through modern, temporally objective aesthetics, and not Stravinsky who was becoming

Baroque:

[Stravinsky’s] Sonata and Concerto have nothing in common with Bach as

regards rhythm, harmony, melody, polyphony, or figuration. Whence, then, the

legend? Are the commentators too little acquainted with Bach, and know him

from hearsay as a 'dry' and 'mechanical' composer, such as he was at one time

regarded by the less cultured elements of the public? In that case they have

probably accepted the dryness and the deliberate in-expressiveness of Stravinsky's

latest works as the qualities which liken him to Bach.71

70 “Looking Ahead In Composing,” New York Times, Apr. 15, 1928, 122. ProQuest Historical

Newspapers.

71 Leonid Sabaneev and S. W. Pring, “The Stravinsky Legends,” The Musical Times 69 (Sept. 1,

Page 526: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

505

When listening to other composers inspired by Baroque-music traditions, critic Paul

Rosenfeld too occasionally envisioned historical musical meters and rhythms through the

mechanical precisions—and values—of the present, especially in the music of Paul

Hindemith, a composer who would become a great motivator for the early-music

movement in American academia during the 1940s. In 1927, Rosenfeld already heard the

“obsessional beat” regulating Hindemith’s “Bachian” concerti grossi Kammermusik

(1921), and that the composer, like Stravinsky, was “confronted with the problem of

uniting mechanical ‘objective’ rhythms with ‘subjective,’ personal, and strongly

sentimental elements.” Rosenfeld found that Hindemith’s music exhibited “the pace and

rhythm of contemporary life,” yet somewhat paradoxically, it also contained “archaic

hardness” which moved “to classically dry, precise, snappy rhythm.”72

Telling of the

new metronomic “early-music” aesthetic that continues in some scholarship and

performances to this day, Rosenfeld heard the “archaic” rhythm in Hindemith as a

scientific rhythm manifest through a standardized and exacting time-source. And

significantly, the critic misconstrued this modern quality as a being intrinsic to past

musicality:

In the characteristic rhythms of the early eighteenth century, so spry, robust, and

precise, there lies the suggestion of a kind of movement objective, external,

removed form the empirically personal, and still not incompatible with human

feeling.73

1928): 786.

72 Paul Rosenfeld, “Musical Chronicle,” The Dial; a Semi-monthly Journal of Literary Criticism

(May 1927), 445. American Periodicals Series Online.

73

Ibid.

Page 527: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

506

Rosenfeld clearly expressed the rhythmical tendencies of his age, yet he transferred to

them an imaginary “classical” musical past, in which Hindemith’s anachronistically

“objective” and “precise” rhythm—a seemingly “neo-classical” chronographic

aesthetic—reigned beyond the historical values of musical rhythm once grounded on

more immediate, internal expressions for pulse, movement, and rhetoric. Like other

composers of his age, Hindemith seemed to be trained in and accepting of a new

mechanically oriented rhythm. Indeed, Hindemith served as a military drummer for his

German regimental band during WWI74

and took pride in his uncommon ability at this

musical role. In a letter from the field dated February 6, 1918, he made sure to report: “In

the band I bang the big drum with skill and enthusiasm. I am assured that this instrument

has never before been handled with such precise rhythm.”75

Stravinsky, Rosenfeld, and Hindemith were not alone in an imagining of past

musical time through a more mechanically “precise” and seemingly “obsessional” beat of

the modern age. Indeed, it appeared decades earlier in the militaristic-gymnastic arts

pedagogy of Jaques-Dalcroze’s neo-Hellenistic Temple of [Modern] Rhythm. It could be

argued that, in the previous century, Wilhelm Wundt and his followers were the first

proponents to desensitize and objectify musical time through mathematical-metronomic

proportions. Regardless of the origins of this cultural trend, what composers of the 1920s

74 Paul Hindemith, Selected letters of Paul Hindemith, edited by Geoffrey Skelton (New Haven:

Yale University Press, 1995), 17.

75 Hindemith, 18. He also occasionally expressed his desire—similarly to Stravinsky, Bartók,

Ravel, and Antheil—to take personal interpretations, along with individual temporal agency, out

of the performance of his compositions. On May 8, 1930 Hindemith explained his creative

intentions to Elizabeth Coolidge: “I have turned by back almost completely on [professional]

concert music and have been writing almost exclusively for amateurs, for children, for radio, for

mechanical instruments, etc.” See Hindemith, 58-59.

Page 528: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

507

considered “objective music”76

—perhaps more accurately described by current science

historians as “mechanically objective” music—often represented a scientific

“rediscovery” of historical compositions, forms, and techniques through the modern

perspective of mechanical-musical time. None other than George Antheil, reflecting upon

the musical culture of his youth, summarized elements of what some recognized as the

neo-classical trend—which, seemingly unbeknownst to Antheil, lingered in the

pedagogies and practices of the 1940s:

The truth of the matter is that we have just emerged from a great stylist period,

where style was everything and content nothing—insofar as human meaning is

concerned. Let us take the “Symphony in C” of Stravinsky. I love this music very

much; it is fine and great music of its period; yet, compared to the great later

symphonies of Beethoven, or even those of Mozart, with which it pretends to be

spiritually aligned, it is cold veal, unemotional as a well-made clock mechanism.77

It is telling that Antheil, by the publication of his 1945 autobiography, had attempted on

numerous occasions to distance himself from his so-called “non-sentimental period” of

the past. And while he chided Stravinsky’s automatical neo-classical music of the 1920s,

Antheil reconsidered his own heartless and metronomic Ballet mécanique with the

admonition: “I am a classist at heart, and the Ballet mécanique is essentially a Romantic

work, breaking all the barriers, the rules and thriving upon it.”78

In recalling his musical-

mechanical indiscretions later in life, Antheil recast himself—as Stravinsky did

likewise—as a “classicist at heart,” who followed classicist compositional ideals, albeit

with often-unacknowledged values of modern mechanical precision. Antheil took this

76 See for instance Bartók, “Mechanical Music” (1937) in Béla Bartók, Essays (New York: St.

Martins Press, 1976), 291.

77

Antheil, Bad Boy of Music, 356.

78

Ibid., 140.

Page 529: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

508

stance as early as 1927 when he wrote to the traditionalist Joseph Hofmann, claiming the

Ballet mécanique represented the composer’s sturm und drang period of creativity.79

Yet,

as Antheil seems have discounted, his past and present work continued to uphold and

promote a “mathematical-metronomic” musicality, vehemently breaking from the

subjective temporality of both the “Classical” and “Romantic” eras. Despite claims of

being within a classical or romantic tradition, Antheil, similar to other “traditionally

inspired” composers of his age, continued to compose and perform with a strong

chronographic belief in the modern clockwork metronome.80

As chronographic pedagogies and aesthetics were increasingly popularized

through the 1920s, the musical-modernists’ penchant to imbue the values of “mechanical

objectivity” upon past compositions and performance practices—from the so-called

Baroque, Classical, and Romantic ages—seemed to expand correspondingly. While in

1924, the Parisian music critic William Atheling claimed that musicians’ “ability to

count, their metronomic ability, has engulfed them, and they have become insensitive to

shape,”81

Ezra Pound, Atheling’s iconoclastic alter ego, further promoted George

Antheil’s musical-mechanical aesthetics as being connected, by mere degrees, to past

compositional and performance practices. Pound claimed in 1927 that Antheil’s “new

quasi-sculptural solidity is something different from the magnificent stiffness or rigidity

79 See George Antheil, “An Explanation of My Evolution since 1924,” (Manuscript to Joseph

Hofmann, Signed Dec. 1927), p. 3, Box 10, Antheil Collection, Library of Congress, Washington

D.C.

80 One can hear Antheil’s performance practices from the time of his autobiography in the sound

recording Antheil Plays Antheil (San Francisco: Other Minds, 2000).

81

Ezra Pound, Antheil and the Treatise on Harmony (Reprint, New York: Da Capo Press, 1968),

132.

Page 530: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

509

of Bach's multilinear mechanism.”82

What may have seemed hyperbole or literary

flourish in Pound’s description—that J. S. Bach composed stiff, rigid musical

machines—was becoming the prevailing belief for many: Bach’s music moved to the

metronome. Indeed, the British Times prescribed this Pound-Antheil aesthetic to a larger

Western culture only seven years later, on September 1934, when an anonymous critic

instructs: “Two things are essential to the right interpretation of Bach: a sense of line and

rhythm. The rhythm should be metronomic since any rigidity that might arise is corrected

by the flow of the melodic line.”83

As the musical-temporal culture of Bach became more

distant—and as scientific aesthetics of objectivity came to the fore—the twentieth-

century view of baroque music seemed coolly inhuman, strictly calculable, rigid, “pure,”

“absolute,” and fundamentally chronographic.

It was Béla Bartók who perhaps best articulated this aesthetical trend, the modern

mechanization of historical music, around 1930. Bartók, who measured folk, classical,

and his own music under ever-more precise metronomic indications, and who was heard

as a living musical automaton in 1922, similarly refashioned Bach with metronomic

attributes—with the temporal qualities of Stravinsky, Antheil, or Hindemith more than

with the subjective, rhetorical expressivity of pre-industrial musicians. The birth of the

early-music movement, Bartók seems to relate, was founded on this modern metronomic

aesthetic, which all but ignored past epistemologies of living musical time. By

exemplifying a composer-performer of two centuries past, Bartók defines a prevailing

mechanistic value of his modern culture:

82 Ibid., 49.

83

[“Music Review,”] The Times, Thursday, Sept. 6, 1934, 10.

Page 531: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

510

The whole world is calling a halt, returning to Bach and to still earlier composers.

It is demanding an objective, anti-literary kind of music that represents no

subjective feeling but is absolute music. 84

Unlike musicians of the previous century, many by the third decade of the twentieth

century had often reconceived an “early music” in which the personal equation, the

individual’s “subjective feeling” for pulse and movement, ceased to be an essential

factor. The historical music of Bach and others now “demanded” modern mechanical

objectivity in performance. Subjective feeling for musical time, as Bartók relates, was no

longer accessed, “represented,” or warranted beyond the rhythmic notation of the page;

personal choice or expression for tempo and rhythm could not be actualized through the

“unfeeling” music from an imaginary chronographic past. As Bartók would be called an

automaton his own day, the very same composer recast Bach as a mechanically perfect

musician who composed absolute music, in which subjective feeling for musical time

was no longer an essential or desirable quality for auditors or performers.85

These were starkly opposing notions from those of pre-metronomic musicians

such as Marx, Christiani, Sternberg, and Saint-Seäns, who recognized that music notation

traditionally failed to relate the sensory, subjective feelings for living rhythm, and that

“artistic” performers must supply this want. Bach’s music, increasingly distanced from

human movement or sensation as a first principle, was now “absolute” and “objective”—

a performance practice more suited to the ideals of Bartók and Stravinsky the any

84 Desider Kosztolanyi, “Béla Bartok, Hungarian Composer,” The Living Age, Aug. 1931, 565.

American Periodicals Series Online.

85 Bartók admits however—even while he defined musical time through automatical machinery:

“Every art is necessarily human…If it were not so, music would become much too mechanical.

Even Bach expressed something, certain moments in life…” Bartók alluded to Bach’s

compositional technique of text painting, not the potential for rhythmical or rhetorical expression

in baroque performance practices.

Page 532: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

511

musician preceding the modern age. Modernist conductor Herman Scherchen made this

mechanization of historic music—and the sublimation of historical rhythmic

expression—obvious in 1946 when he provided his interpretation of Bach’s supposed

“last composition” from the Art of the Fugue. In The Nature of Music, Scherchen goes so

far as to consider the “natural” human pulse and pulse sense completely under

chronographic rule—as the now prevailing rhythmical epistemology throughout musical

history—when he states:

The metronome marking which suites both forms of [Bach’s chorale melody] is: q/e=72 (Maelzel’s metronome). This allows listeners to think in terms of two quavers together…this tempo, which has been arrived at by [modern] theoretical and practical experience, corresponds to the Integer Valor of the Middle Ages, and is the human normal value, by which seventy two pulse beats and eighteen full respirations are produced a minute…[The physical result] corresponds also with the physical fatigue of the blind and dying Bach.

86

For Scherchen, even Bach’s last musical thought, breath, and pulse clocked-in at 72 beats

to the modern mechanical minute: the trusted chronographic norm for “listeners” to

passively perceive. As the “essential” rhythm for modern, early-modern, and “Middle”

ages could now be found in the positive quality of being “metronomic,” a new modern-

musical champion emerged from distant centuries: the “automatical Bach.” This

paradigmatic, yet imagined Baroque composer bridged the past and the present through

modern metronomic rhythm. In his own chronographic study of historical performance

practices, musicologist Sachs similarly “tried to metronomize Bach’s B minor Mass, each

86 Herman Scherchen, The Nature of Music (Vom Wesen der Musik, 1946), translated by William

Mann (London: Denis Dobson Ltd., 1950), 87-88. Scherchen did not consider the fact that a

dying man might not have a steady, continual, or equal pulse that could be so accurately

represented by a modern metronomic tempo.

Paradoxically also, Scherchen concludes his analysis of Bach’s work by stating, “it can

only be understood with reference to the special circumstances under which it was composed.”

Yet Scherchen failed to admit that Bach’s “final composition” was not composed with Maelzel’s

metronome in the Baroque composer’s mind, ear, or vicinity. See Scherchen, 108

Page 533: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

512

movement separately and on various days, and found that his beat was consistently near

M.M. 80, covering now a quarter note, now an eighth, now even a half” and so on.87

Given these contemporaneous mechanizations of baroque dance-meters and pulses, the

imagined musical automaton J. S. Bach was perhaps most fully exhumed through the

interpretations of Glen Gould, who in death himself is still praised and promoted as a

premiere icon of a precise mechanical-mathematical “musicality,” a quality once

abhorred by pre-scientific educators, performers, and aestheticians.

Increasingly Modern, Metronomic Regulations for a “Classical Music” Culture

As this final chapter only begins to explain, the distinctions between modern aesthetics,

education, and musicology are by no means “rigidly” defined amongst themselves, and it

will take another study to find the many connections that these twentieth-century musical

disciplines share in their values of past performance practices, musical temporality, and

”metronome believing.” Nevertheless, for modernists valuing the Wundtian and

Dalcrozian goals of precise action informed by a chronographic standard, rhythm became

a notational problem to solve, an objective calculation outside of expressive human

intervention for the music of all ages. Thus, modern music scholars continued to revise

music history correspondingly. Like modernist aestheticians before him, Sachs—who

expressed great interest in the work of Seashore88

and who could not bring himself to

question the historical relevance behind his own metronomic methodology—implied

throughout Rhythm and Tempo that musical form, now far from the primacy of human

87 Sachs, 33.

88

Ibid., 18-19.

Page 534: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

513

agency, consisted of static musical measures, calculated building blocks, in which

“rhythm” was the lifeless constructive material within those building blocks. And

although Sachs eschewed any discussion of Eurhythmics, his “rhythm” corresponded

nearly identically with the ideals of Jaques-Dalcroze and George Antheil. Indeed, while

Sachs occasionally acknowledged the importance of personal, “non-metronomic” leeway

in interpretations, any subjective sensory expression for meter and rhythm—once an a

priori rule of musical time—had little to do with Sachs’ own modernist, seemingly

“Antheilicized” summation: “Ours is a mathematically counting notation” 89

and “the

quarter note [is] our motor unit.”90

This implied “motor unit” of musical time—which calls forth alternate visions of

Maelzel’s automata, Wundt’s chronographic laboratory, Jaques-Dalcroze’s rhythmical

prodigies, and the “ballet” of modern industrial machinery—applied to other pre-

metronomic compositions performed since the Mechanical Age as well. For one Times

critic who attended the 1933 Salzburg Festival, an anachronistically “classical,”

metronomic rhythm pervaded a concert conducted by Richard Strauss—a composer

straddling a neo-classical aesthetic of temporal objectivity. The critic described that when

Strauss directed a certain Mozart symphony:

89 Ibid., 168.

90

Ibid., 173. A similar statement appears on page 201. In his text, Sachs eschews any substantial

discussion of Jaques-Dalcroze or Eurhythmics, a pedagogy founded two generations before

Sachs’ study appeared.

Page 535: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

514

His style was rigourously, perfectly classical—a metronome beat, a set balanced

colour-scheme. But the effect was dull. Perhaps this was because one was unable

to follow a great man’s vision of finer quintessential outlines; perhaps, however,

because in concentrating so fixedly upon the ultimate details of which a design, if

it is to be significant and vital and not a mere super-imposed mould, must be

composed. 91

As this writer suggests beyond the performance itself, the “classical” became allied with

the objectively “perfect:” a rigid architectural “mould” and metronomic “time” control

outside of the interpretive musician’s will to change or even feel. Not limited to a

Baroque ideal, modernist performance practices seemed to present Mozart through ever-

greater standards of contemporaneous, metronomic action: a continually important and

positive aesthetic to achieve when performing “classical” music. Video documentation of

an elder Richard Strauss conducting the Vienna Philharmonic in 1944 exists, and for

some, he might exhibit movements of a lifeless conducting automaton during even his

own classically formulated rondeaux Till Eulenspiegel; his arms slightly waving to a

decided “metronome beat.” Indeed, conductor George Szell attested that Strauss, in some

performances, would be so emotionally disengaged from the musical time—or maybe

“objectively minded” towards the mechanical time—as to glance at his pocket watch

while the orchestra played before him.92

Strauss was not the only iconic conductor to treat late eighteenth- and nineteenth-

century music with greater chronographic precisions. In 1930, a Times critic heard the

modern metronomic world exhibited in a performance of Schubert’s “Unfinished

91 “Salzburg Festival. Mozart and Strauss,” The Times, Aug. 15, 1933, 8, col D.

92 See George Szell interview in The Art of Conducting, Chapter 7 (Video), IMG Artists/BBC

Coproduction in association with Teldec Classics International & Sender Freies Berlin (UK:

International Management Group, Inc., 1993).

Page 536: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

515

Symphony” conducted by Felix Weingartner, whose interpretation was “following the

fashion for speed.” The critic reports:

D. Weingartner’s reading of [the finale], consistent though it was in conception,

did not commend itself to us as much as the more obvious interpretation, which

regards it as a kind of translation into music of mechanical speed with the four

minims as the fly-wheel of the engine.93

Under the guise of “absolute composer intention,” other contemporaneous interpreters

took published metronomic numbers as precise guides—for gauging the modern musical

“motor unit”—in their ever-stricter chronographic treatments of nineteenth-century

compositions. This modern interpretive aesthetic of Werktreue, which continues to this

day, often misconstrued historical faithfulness to the composer’s printed indications with

the modern, scientific faith in a near-constant metronomic adherence in performance.94

This pseudo-scientific Werktreue aesthetic has often made as an implicit value a

chronographically objective “truth,” a metronomic “truth,” conversely finding “falsity” in

the more variable personal equation of living interpretive action, which, beyond the

printed page, thrived centuries prior to twentieth-century chronographic aesthetics or

pedagogies.

Thus, some modern writers, pedagogues, and conductors who devalued or ignored

the once-common understanding of rhetorical movement and rhythmic pulse as indicators

of musical time conversely considered the clockwork metronome the most appropriate

93 “London Symphony Orchestra: A Schubert Evening,” The Times, Tuesday, Mar. 25, 1930, 14.

94 While they seem to eschew the mechanical-scientific basis for the twentieth-century Werktreue

aesthetic, Richard Taruskin and Lydia Goehr provide important recent discussions of this cultural

philosophy. See Richard Taruskin, Text and Act: Essays on Music and Performance (Oxford:

Oxford University Press, 1995); and Lydia Goehr, The Imaginary Museum of Musical Works

(Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1992).

Page 537: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

516

interpretive tool for getting at their imagined mechanical “truth” driving nineteenth-

century tempi, especially for the music of Beethoven. In 1932, a Times correspondent

focused on the metronome as essential to finding “authoritative” musical answers for

compositions nearly a century old. Citing Malcolm Seargent’s interpretation of

Mendelssohn’s Elijah, the writer considers the long-deceased, yet absolutely “definite”

composer’s intention intricately intertwined with the modern clockwork metronome:

Behind Mendelssohn’s metronomic markings there is a definite interpretive idea,

and it is incumbent on every conductor to bring out that idea…The metronomic

marks are frequently disregarded because in so many older works they are not

authoritative but merely the suggestions of an editor. Mendelssohn’s are

authoritative, and the conductor who would give a perfect performance of Elijah

must begin by reading them intelligently.95

As this critic implied, the clockwork metronome seemed to offer a greater truth than it

had during the previous century, supporting positivistic interpretive practices and

objectivist readings of rhythmical notation guided by limited beats-per-minute data. Thus

it seems “intelligent” interpretation and metronomic calculation converged closer in

modern performance practices, as sensory and rhetorical qualities of rhythm and tempo

continued to wane in importance. This greater value placed upon metronomic musical

interpretations could be expected, given that modern educational models of music

direction, such as found in Oberlin Professor Karl Wilson Gehrkens’ Essentials in

Conducting (1919), instructed the aspiring professional conductor that “hours of practice

in beating time will be necessary…it should also be done with the metronome clicking or

with some one playing the piano much of the time, in order that the habit of maintaining

95 “Elijah Restudied: Interpreting The Metronome,” The Times, Saturday, Oct. 29, 1932, 13.

Page 538: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

517

an absolutely steady, even tempo may evolve.”96

Entering the third decade of the

twentieth century, even some conservatory-trained conductors, it seems, were habitually

acquiring a new “skill” by practicing, interpreting, and leading a new musical-

automatical “tempo,” one now desirable for Werktreue interpretations and “essential” for

modern performance practices.

In this cultural milieu in which positivistic interpretations, compositional

aesthetics of “objectivity,” industrialism, and music education would all converge

through greater chronographic beliefs in action, Eugène Borrel was perhaps the first

conservatory-based musicologist and instrumentalist to revise past musical practices with

modernist precisions. In the article “Les indications metronomiques…du XVIIIe siecle”

(1928), Loulié’s chronométre, a silent, simple pendulum, took on chronographic qualities

for Borrel, who by the very act of “metronoming” French baroque dances infused,

consciously or not, an archaic and speculative machine—originally intended to reflect the

motion of the human tactus—with more regulative, automatical actions common to the

educational, compositional, and performance practices of Borrel’s culture.97

In 1938,

Rosamund Harding, following Borrel’s published time studies, would strikingly consider

in these scattered, mechanical markings “the origins of musical time and expression”

while offering a revisionist history in which Loulié’s chronométre became the “most

celebrated” attempt to quantify musical time before Maelzel’s metronome.98

Some

96 Karl Wilson Gehrkens, Essentials in Conducting (Philadelphia: Oliver Ditson Co., 1919), 35.

97 See Eugène Borrel, “Les indications metronomiques…du XVIIIe siècle,” Revue de musicology

T.9c (1928): 149-153.

98

See R. E. M. Harding, Origins of Musical Time and Expression (London: Oxford University

Press, 1938).

Page 539: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

518

musicologists and “historically informed” musicians, well after Seashore, Borrel,

Harding, and Sachs, continued to further their beats-per-minute methodology for early-

music performance practices, perhaps guided by scientific aesthetics and modern

Werktreue philosophies. Yet, by indicating that minuets, gigues, marches, and even vocal

airs—alongside markings such as allegro, tendrement, adagio, vite, or vivace—fell under

a speculative range of absolute beats-per-minute possibilities, they perhaps unwittingly

moved ever closer to the experimental psychologists’ ideal of clockwork action,

devaluing the human agency intrinsic to past musical-temporal epistemologies while

promoting a more regulative and homogeneous aesthetic exclusive to the modern,

mechanical age.

Scientific musicologists’ metronomic studies of historical-musical time did not

remain limited to academic spheres. In 1946, modern-music champion and conductor

Scherchen took Borrel’s “findings” alongside the writings of early-modern pendulum

scientist Joseph Sauveur to justify a chronographic-musical past. As a prominent

upholder of modern aesthetics and Werktreue musicality, Scherchen clearly imposed his

values of time, rhythm, and action on all of Western music history, in what can be

appropriately termed the fallacy of historical precision—a fallacy also forwarded by the

contemporaneous psychologist Seashore, musicologists Sachs and later Erich Schwandt,

and anyone assuming continuous metronomic clicks relate to the tempi and rhythms of

pre-metronomic compositions. In The Nature of Music, Scherchen—not conceptually far

from many twentieth-century performance-practice musicologists—modernizes the

precision of past musical time for modern culture thusly:

Page 540: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

519

That Sauveur was correct in his attempt to create a means of measuring music and

acoustics as precisely as possible, is proved by modern practice…his [simple-

pendulum] metronome might have served Bartók, Stravinsky, and others for a

minutely precise indication of tempi and duration of movements, had it only been

currently available [to these modern composers...]

Loulié’s chronometer of 1686 and Sauveur’s more precise echometre of

1701 were greeted with equal enthusiasm, just as a hundred years later Maelzel’s

metronome was applauded by Beethoven and his contemporaries.99

It seems that as the beliefs and actions in musical time moved towards greater, more

modern chronographic precisions, so too did music history and historical performance

practices in the minds of many during the twentieth century. With a historiography

similar to Scherchen’s, Sachs speciously and with no supporting evidence imagined even

Quantz to have considered tempi using a clockwork metronome.100

Marin Mersenne too

became a modern chronographic scientist, as Sachs believed the early-modern theorist—

in a time-culture before precise “seconds hand” clockwork technology—“equated the

[‘un-rhythmical’] tactus beat with a pulse beat…but as this was not precise enough from

person to person and from hour to hour, many contemporaries, he said, preferred the

duration of a second on the dial for the semibreve or motor unit, which amounts to M.M.

60 for the quarter note.”101

In the minds of “metronome believing” musicians and

scholars, a musical culture that thrived well before Maelzel’s “invention” became just as

consistently chronographic as their twentieth-century musical culture that wholeheartedly

embraced the clockwork metronome.

99 Scherchen, trans. by Mann, 32-33.

100

Sachs, 34.

101

Ibid., 273.

Page 541: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

520

It then comes as no surprise that other esteemed musicians and pedagogues

including violinist Rudolf Kolisch would completely disregard Beethoven’s comments,

his life, and the history of technology in favor of a modern mechanical aesthetic of

musicality and musical time when performing Beethoven’s masterworks. Kolisch scoffed

in 1942, “opponents of the [clockwork] metronome will even go so far as to make the

unbelievably nonsensical assertion that our modern metronome differs considerably from

Beethoven’s. (My answer: That could be true only if the speed of the earth’s rotation had

changed in the meantime.)”102

Kolsich seemed not to consider that beyond technological

developments, it was Western musical culture—with its scientific faith in chronographic

rules and regulations—that had changed in the meantime. Perhaps implicitly guided by

this disciplinary-wide devaluing of the personal equation in favor of mechanical

objectivity—promoted throughout the twentieth century by such seemingly disparate

figures such as Seashore, Borrel, Harding, Antheil, Scherchen, and Schwandt—

musicologist George Houle even offered historical automata from Engramelle’s machine-

building treatise La tonotechnie as “uniquely precise” evidence of “the effect of metrical

articulation” as it showed “quantitative articulation with absolute clarity.”103

In 1987,

lifeless musical machines, including both historical automata and modern metronomes—

technologies once abhorred for their lack of living rhythm and expression—continued to

prominently inform modern scholarship with a now “precise,” “quantitative,” and

“absolute” source of “musical time” for modern purposes.

102 Rudolf Kolisch, “Tempo and Character in Beethoven's Music,” The Musical Quarterly 77

(Spring, 1993), 90. This article is a reprint from his 1942 work.

103

Houle, 110.

Page 542: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

521

During the 1930s, evidence suggests that other modern interpreters, including

Arturo Toscanini, first began rigorously applying metronomic measures in order to

objectively and “perfectly” reproduce musical tempi even for later nineteenth-century

music. While Toscanini’s 1932 concert rendition of Bach’s “Concerto for Four Pianos”

with the New York Philharmonic was for one Wall Street Journal critic, “devoid of the

dread metronomic effects invariably to be looked for in undertakings of the sort,”104

contemporaneous historian David Ewen suggested that the Italian maestro took great

pains to adhere to the metronome for more “Romantic” genres, including nineteenth-

century Wagnerian opera. In his book The Man with the Baton (1936), Ewen supports

Toscanini’s modern, mechanically oriented interpretations:

In Bayreuth in 1930…the “perfect Wagnerites” felt that in the prelude to

Tannhäuser, Toscanini took an altogether unorthodox tempo. Toscanini quietly

summoned his critics to a piano and there, with the aide of a metronome, proved

that his tempo was meticulously perfect, precisely the way Wagner had so

carefully designated in the score.105

Yet Toscanini, as Ewen’s anecdote suggests, might have paid little heed to Wagner’s

more detailed verbal indications, and the composer’s ultimate rejection of metronomic

action as it relates to meaningful interpretations of his operas—and that no single

metronomic “tempo” was consistently applicable to any of his music. As Wagner

explained in Über das Dirigiren, metronomic indications, wherever they sporadically

104 “The Theatre,” Wall Street Journal, Oct. 18, 1932, 3. ProQuest Historical Newspapers.

105

David Ewen, The Man with the Baton - The Story of Conductors and Their Orchestras

(Thomas Y. Crowell Co., 1936), 194. In keeping with the mechanically objective aesthetics of his

age while rejecting the personal equation, Ewen downplayed Wagner’s own statements on the

flexibility of musical time, even discounting them when he surmised that in the conducting of

Berlioz, Liszt, and Wagner, “too many exaggerations and dramatics distorted their performances.

However, with all their faults, they definitely pointed the way” to modern conducting techniques

and practices. See Ewen, 85.

Page 543: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

522

appeared in his earlier operas, were always secondary to rhetorical-rhythmical qualities,

those necessary variations and nuances that the composer never “designated in the score,”

and which belie any “meticulously perfect” or absolutely “precise” tempo replication, a

chronographic value sought in Western musical culture only since the twentieth century.

In a later trend that can only be slightly cited here, some including Theodore

Adorno and Curt Sachs106

clearly recognized that new musical media and broadcasts

elicited even more exacting mechanical performance practices. Unlike previous

automatical creations such as Maelzel’s automaton trumpeter or the household pianola,

later “mechanical” forms of music presentation—heard notably through wax cylinders,

phonographs, radio transmissions, and moving pictures—further manifested metronomic

attributes in living musicians, alongside listeners’ expectations for the exactly replicable

performance. Accounts suggest that recorded media often required performers to work

with quicker overall tempi, so music could fit to into the fixed time frames allotted by the

recording surfaces or broadcast schedules. These imposed limitations often resulted in

performances with more regulated musical movements and less interpretive variations.

Movie conductors, as reported in a 1934 New York Times article, often beat time “with a

stop-watch…because it is essential that the symphonic accompaniment coincide perfectly

with the action.”107

Fewer subjective rhythmic gestures or rhetorical effects could be

106 Sachs, only in the very last page of his text, describes that “the length of a recording groove

and the inexorable, often tragicomic timing impositions of broadcasts, movies, or television force

the performers and even composers into an often unmusical, antimusical straightjacket.” He

speaks more of the new limitations imposed by recording durations rather than tick-tock

chronographic regulation heard in the modern metronome—a machine he seemed largely familiar

with throughout his lifetime. See Sachs, 380.

107

Frank S. Nugent, “Sound and Fury in Remote Astoria,” New York Times, Dec. 30, 1934, X5;

ProQuest Historical Newspapers.

Page 544: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

523

conveyed when mechanical media constricted the overall duration of any given musical

performance—and composition—to a “start and stop” time.

None other than George Antheil experienced this mechanical imposition in his

role as film-score composer—and the New York Times reporter Frank Nugent did not let

the irony escape his readers:

Mr. Antheil, whose “Ballet Mécanique”…used such “instruments” as anvils,

whirling airplane propellers, electric bells and automobile horns, admitted it was

something of a problem to compose music to split seconds.108

Antheil—once hailed as the most “precise” pianist of his age, a composer who sought to

mechanize music by charging living performers to play identically to automatical

pianolas on the concert stage, a theorist who attested to the mathematical essentiality of

rhythmic notation—eventually found difficulty with the increasing chronographic

precision demanded by new modes of mechanical musicality, specifically, “the matter of

music [timed] by the stop watch.”109

By 1934, Antheil had experienced first hand that

even his “musicality” became ever constricted through mechanical-temporal controls—

away from individual, variable expressions—as the methods and machines of

composition and performance continued to actively and rapidly redefine “musical

precision” during his age.

Thus by the 1930s, with the rise of radio broadcasts, recordings, and movies, it

seems that many professional musical performances had moved ever closer to the

chronographic procedure of the nineteenth-century laboratory. It is likely that through

recordings, listeners’ notions of musical “performance” became detached from the true

108 Ibid.

109

Ibid.

Page 545: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

524

agents of musical performance—the living performers themselves—and thus the personal

equation of musicality, alongside the intrinsic humanity of musical time, further receded

in importance for many consumers of twentieth-century media. Recording technologies

of the age often reflect this continued sublimation of individualistic “time”—and the

increasing objectification of an absolutely “correct” time—in music performances of the

Western tradition. In an early example from 1931, Walt Disney filed a patent for a film-

score recording system that better regulated living musicians through constant

metronome clicks—a precursor to the ubiquitous “click-track” recording technologies

employed presently. Disney’s metronomic machine attests to the close similarity that

many modern musical-performance practices now shared with passive, psychological

experiments in “rhythm” from the previous century.

Figure 7.4. Walt Disney’s 1931 metronomic apparatus for recording movie soundtracks. The

clockwork click of the metronome, shown at the lower left (#40), was electronically transmitted from an

adjacent room directly into the headphones of musicians. The process mimics nineteenth-century

chronographic methods in observatories and laboratories, but now it applied to skilled musicians in the

active metronomic regulation of their performances. “Method and apparatus for Synchronizing

Photoplays,” (Filed Apr. 2, 1931; Patented Dec. 26, 1933).

Page 546: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

525

Perhaps due to the convergence of modern aesthetics, growing metronomic

pedagogies, recorded media, Werktreue interpretations, and the scientific-musicological

research informing “historical” performance practices, many came to assume that

“classical music” was intrinsically linked to mechanical time controls, that “classical

music” necessitated the temporal tyranny of a modern metronomic pulse. Ironically, two

years after patenting their new chronographic “Method and Apparatus for Synchronizing

Photoplays,” Walt Disney Productions released the short film Music Land on October 3,

1935, humorously confirming that modern mechanical culture imposed a very real

hegemony upon past musical-temporal traditions. When caught by guards from “The

Land of Symphony,” the young protagonist, a love-stricken jazz saxophone, was thrown

into an appropriate “Classical Music Prison” for modern times—he landed inside a large,

clicking clockwork metronome. Trapped within, the saxophone paced back and forth

synchronously to the incessant tick-tock of his stodgy Maelzelian holding cell.110

110 Thanks to Dr. Daniel Goldmark for directing me to a copy of the short film.

Page 547: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

526

Figure 7.5. Two consecutive scenes from Walt Disney’s 1935 “Silly Symphony” entitled Music Land.

Guards from The Land of Symphony, left, hurled the saxophone from the Isle of Jazz into their physical-

temporal metronome-prison, where inside, right, the hero sympathetically moved in accordance with its

clockwork clicks.

Conclusion: a “Rhythm” Narrowed, a “Time” Deceased

There can be no doubt that twentieth-century Western culture, following the growth of

metronomic pedagogies, witnessed the ascendance of a chronographic aesthetic of

musical objectivity that stipulated more normative and regulative standards of time,

rhythm, and action for compositions both past and present. For some during this age, a

mechanical hegemony was decidedly coming to fruition through “metronome believing,”

and modern culture seemed to be fulfilling the qualities of Johann Maelzel’s automaton

shows of a century prior: Replicating once subjective living actions with metronomic,

beats-per-minute consistencies and precisions was becoming a commonplace occurrence

on the music stage, in the practice room and recording studio.

Perhaps in recognition of the mechanical evolution that Samuel Butler

prophesized “will hold the real supremacy over the world and its inhabitants” only 60

years prior, artist Man Ray created an emblem of his Machine Age in the 1920s; it was a

clockwork metronome with a single eye attached to the swinging pendulum. The original

Page 548: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

527

title of Man Ray’s uncanny Metronome-Cyclops: Object to be Destroyed. Indeed, in the

first third of the modern century, Maelzel’s “mere automaton,” which a century later

became Brower’s “tireless little policeman,” seemed more like Man Ray’s monstrous

tyrant, continually redefining musicality so that the serious student and professional

performer, even the conducting “maestro,” often seemed a hapless subject against now

“authoritative” mechanical tempi, “definitive” metronomic pulses, and “precisely”

calculable rhythms—all while neglecting or devaluing the physical sensations and

gestures that once informed more subjective musical-temporal epistemologies. New York

Times critic John Martin suggested that this metronomic turn—which hearkened back to

Wundt’s experimental laboratory—actively redefined the creative time of music and

dance on the concert stage and in the broadcasting studio during his age, claiming in

1929:

Rhythm is becoming constricted into ever-narrower meanings; it is already used

almost as a synonymous term for the regularity of the audible beat.111

Martin’s assertion cannot be taken lightly, since evidence suggests that scientific

“rhythm”—objectified chronographic time—increasingly defined and normalized

musicality for a host of students, professional performers, and scholars in the twentieth

century. By 1920, even the early-music pedagogue and composer Vincent D’Indy had

directly paraphrased experimental psychologists when defining “rhythm,” in which

metronomic clicks were the absolute source and measure of a largely reactionary mental

phenomenon.112

Given this growing scientific epistemology of time and action—

111 John Martin, “The Dance: Listening In,” New York Times, Mar. 31, 1929, 108.

112 “When we hear successive sounds of equal duration like those of the metronome, one of the

two has more intensity that the other; we can at will, M. D’Indy tells us, attribute to the more

Page 549: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

528

appearing in the musical academy, the concert hall, and the classroom—William

Atheling’s 1924 claim that musicians were “engulfed” by their seemingly habituated

mathematic and “metronomic ability” likewise cannot be taken as mere hyperbole. For

those attentive to the incompatibilities between the rhetorical and the mechanical,

qualities of musical time were audibly and perhaps irrevocably changed. Indeed, it seems

that D’Indy, Jaques-Dalcroze, Seashore, Pound, Antheil, Stravinsky, Sachs, and many

others, had long since discounted the pre-metronomic “rhythm” expressed by A. B.

Marx’s profoundly contrasting, anti-scientific summation from a century prior: “Rhythm

is the expression of the will and pleasure of him who formed it; and we recognize in it

either the determined purpose of the artist, or his sensible delight in a well-arranged and

pleasing or significant succession of tonal quantities.”113

The narrowing of “rhythm” into a limited mechanical epistemology, the “engulfment” of

musicians to a de-sensitized musical time, their increasing “metronomic ability” to

synchronize to ever-more precise clicking impressions—these are not matters of a

modernist or neo-classical past. In keeping with a twentieth-century scientific culture, the

mechanization of musicality remains present in many pedagogies, research

methodologies, and performance practices to this very day. Indeed, evidence suggests

that chronographic, or automatical, performance practices grow more precise and

intense sound the odd numbers…or the even numbers. ‘The possibility we have of choosing by a

mere effort of will one or the other of these inequalities, clearly proves that rhythm proceeds not

from the [mechanical] sounds themselves but from a necessity of our own mind’” [in choosing a

pattern from those mechanical sounds.] See Camille Saint-Saëns, trans. Fred Rothwell, “The

Ideas of M. Vincent D’Indy,” The Musical Times 61 (Apr. 1, 1920), 244.

113 Marx, The Music of the Nineteenth Century and Its Culture, 33.

Page 550: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

529

regulative as successive generations of musicians and researchers continue to employ

metronomic machines with a scientific desire to standardize “tempo” and “rhythm,”

alongside the very notions of “musicality.”

Thus, by way of a conclusion, we must acknowledge a historical shift in the

cultural values and actions that define musical time; it is a turn which prominently

emerged in twentieth-century culture, which pitted the desire for objectivity and precision

in a mechanically homogeneous and regulative “time” against those contrasting,

subjective epistemologies that once upheld the variability of rhetorical delivery, physical

movement, and personal-temporal sensation. For a culture increasingly reliant on

machines and mechanical data for the teaching, composing, and performing of music, this

metronomic turn is barely recognized as having occurred at all. Yet, as inheritors of

modernity, how can we claim to understand those historical “musical times” so long as

we cling to pedagogies and research methodologies that uphold the machines and

mechanical-temporal values of modernity? So long as scholars and performers place

greatest importance on those tried and true metronomic stipulations—that one should

play or conduct faster or slower to the clockwork beat, that one should keep the

metronomic “tempo” consistent throughout a piece, that one should first play all notes

“precisely” to the implied tick-tock grounding each musical measure—then we will never

get nearer to the expressive “intention” of a Beethoven, Mozart, Bach, or Monteverdi, or

more realistically, the profoundly creative rhythmical and rhetorical interpretations that

their musical works both warrant and deserve. When academics, pedagogues, and

performers place primary value on metronomes and metronomic data in order to

approach “composer intention and historical knowledge,” they are not solving the

Page 551: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

530

musical problems of past ages, they are providing limited and normative answers for our

age—for a time-culture that desires efficient musical reproduction and replication before

and beyond individualistic, variable, and anti-mechanical rhythmical qualities.

Pedagogues, performers, and scholars must then account for two truisms

seemingly disregarded by many in our current musical culture. Paderewski expressed the

first: mechanical execution and pre-modern musical expression are incompatible.

Conductor Walter Damrosch, who was ironically labeled a living metronome early in his

career, recounted the second: the possibility exists for musicians to play too precisely to

be good.114

Despite these pre-modern truisms, the standards of musical-temporal

precision and consistency—which increase over the decades as metronome technology

does—obscure and efface the subjective, sensory qualities of musical time by

normalizing and regulating the “precise beat” for young and old, amateur and

professional, performer and scholar. And while evidence suggests that musical-temporal

values and actions were fundamentally anti-chronographic throughout the majority of

Western musical history, modern scientific pedagogies and performance practices

continue to run in opposition to those historical beliefs and actions—again reflecting the

profound metronomic turn in “musical time” that perhaps incited Debussy to exclaim

upon experiencing Nijinsky’s “odd mathematical twist” as a dancer-choreographer, “I

hold Monsieur Dalcroze to be one of the worst enemies of music!”115

114 See Walter Damrosch, My Musical Life (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, 1923), 320. At

the turn of the century, a European newspaper threw the criticism at Damrosch in reaction to his

New York Philharmonic touring concert.

115 Reprinted in Albright, 85. Despite Boulez’s claims to Debussy’s modernism, Debussy himself

seems to have taken a decidedly non-chronographic, nineteenth-century view towards metronome

indications, stating, “You know my opinion of these metronome markings: They are good for just

Page 552: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

531

How can we ever fully describe, conceive, or even experience pre-metronomic

music and musicality when a prevailing Dalcrozian culture continues to equate “good

rhythm” with precise chronographic action in both education and performance? In

learning past performance concepts and practices, can players and scholars fully

disregard their own metronomic education and aesthetics? Can modern musicians fully

un-tether this seemingly ingrained “truth” of mechanical-musical time? Perhaps by first

acknowledging the practical and conceptual distinctions between innate human

sensations and constant mechanical regulations, we may begin to answer these questions

in greater detail, while also recognizing the extent to which metronomic references have

redefined the living pulse-sense for both contemporary and historical music

performances. Furthermore, we ought to acknowledge that the pre-mechanical “timeist”

seems long-since discounted today by a seldom-questioned fundamental of musical time,

one that places tempo, rhythm, and the clicking metronome under a unified epistemology.

And we are unlikely to understand or regain the expressive rhythmical qualities of the

historical “timeist” if scholars, performers, and pedagogues continue to endorse

precision-based methods that unhesitatingly claim the intrinsic and necessary fusion

between the living musician and the modern automaton—such as this one published in

2007:

one measure, as ‘roses last the length of a morning.’” This quote from Debussy’s correspondence

is reprinted numerous times in James R. Briscoe, ed., Debussy in Performance (New Haven: Yale

University Press, 1999), 83-4, 99, 276, etc.

Page 553: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

532

By setting the metronome at a speed you feel is comfortable for your successful

execution of the passage, you have eliminated the necessity for [self-]governing

the speed of your playing—the metronome will do it for you…When using the

metronome to improve your sense of timing and rhythm…listen to the metronome

tick for a while. Move your body to the rhythm of the ticks. Once you’ve

internalized the rhythm, you’re ready to start playing.

Some students complain that they have a hard time playing while the

metronome is ticking—that’s precisely why they need to use it. It’s paramount

that they get their body and the metronome moving together to develop their

sense of rhythm…Once you have learned a new piece of music, prove to yourself

that you know it well by playing it with the metronome ticking.116

In the eighteenth century, Quantz once considered playing continuously to the living

pulse impossible; Rousseau once considered playing to the silent swing of a simple

pendulum unnecessary, impossible, and impractical. In the nineteenth century, Berlioz

recognized that playing to Maelzel’s clockwork metronome was not only impossible, but

would result in a severely “cold” and “inexpressive” musicality if attempted. Just over a

century ago, Joseph Hofmann warned students never to play with the clockwork

metronome, for a metamorphosis would undoubtedly result in their sense of “musical

rhythm.” Yet beginning in the twentieth century, and proceeding into the twenty-first,

many amateurs and professionals are not only instructed, encouraged, and required to

play and practice with metronomes of much greater automatical precision, but can play

automatically in both rehearsals and concerts—click by click, in quarters, sixteenths,

thirty-seconds, and beyond—with the ease, consistency, and accuracy of cold, calculating

machines. Practicing and performing with mechanical precision is no longer an anomaly

or abhorrence it once was to musical cultures of past centuries. For the majority of

musicians trained in a chronographically biased “classical” and “early-music” tradition,

116 Neil Miller, The Piano Lessons Book: The Piano Student's Guide for Getting the Most Out of

Practicing, Lessons, Your Teacher and Yourself ([n.p.]: CreateSpace, 2007), 214-215.

Page 554: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

533

playing to the modern metronome is not simply an acceptable standard practice—it is a

necessary and “paramount” ability to acquire.

It is only since the modern age—which for some constitutes a daily “chronarchy”

of precise mechanical “time” constraints, moving to ever increasing mechanical

“rhythms”—that performing like a clockwork metronome is commonplace. An implicit

metronomic hegemony over musical time has indeed taken place during the modern

century, and it has seemingly overturned the primacy of more subjective, variable senses

of musical temporality. Consider comments from some of the most renowned and

accomplished performers in our age, such as Emanuel Ax—who endorsed and practiced

the very chronographic “rhythm” taught to twentieth-century novices with little

understanding of the foundations, feelings, or actions that defined the pre-metronomic

musical “times.” Speaking to Thomas Robert May in March 2005, Ax said:

[In learning John Adams’ pianola-like concerto Century Rolls] it was an

incredible discovery of having good rhythm. Most classical pianists—myself at

the head of the list, don’t necessarily have such good rhythm. We’re always

thinking about line and harmony but we don’t necessarily have a good feel for the

inexorable pulse. This is why with pieces like the Pulcinella reduction [for piano

and cello] of Stravinsky—which I worked on with Yo-Yo [Ma] this week—I had

a hard time just being accurate, not rushing, not slowing down. It’s incredible

discipline that really helps you with everything. There’s so much to think about

for a pianist: line, harmony, the way that one note relates to the next, because the

sound decays, so we have to think about connecting things; sometimes rhythm

takes a backseat. So I practiced with a metronome, which I’ve been doing ever

since. I’m realizing that you need to put rhythm into the hierarchy that you’re

working out. 117

Since the scientific twentieth century, playing like a metronome means to play correctly,

with “rhythmical precision” and “accurate tempi” for music from all ages—for the

117 Thomas Robert May, The John Adams Reader: Essential Writings on an American Composer

(Pompton Plains, NJ: Amadeus Press, 2006), 273.

Page 555: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

534

compositions of Bach, Beethoven, Stravinsky, and Adams included. As Ax implies, the

“inexorable pulse” of the constant metronomic click remains the assumed standard of

“good rhythm,” musical “accuracy,” and even musical “feeling” for many professional

performers—for whom “invisible,” pre-metronomic pulse-senses and musical-rhetorical

gestures seem of negligible importance from the outset.

Ax is not alone in lacking musical-temporal perspective; for many musicians

today, the scientific aesthetic that stipulates an objective metronomic “rhythm” and

“tempo” remains the exclusive value of “musical time” for compositions throughout

history. Under the increasingly metronomic epistemology of tempo in our age,

musicologist Robert Philip found that “it is now difficult to imagine what Scholes was

hearing” in 1922 when the London critic described Bartók’s playing as automatical,118

since Philip himself heard Bartók, in recordings, play “groups of semiquavers more

impressionistically than modern demands for clarity allow,” while “chords are

arpeggiated to a startling extent” in one Beethoven sonata. For Philip, Bartók’s playing,

which “could not be deduced from his scores…sounds surprisingly ‘Romantic’ in its

freedoms.”119

It is telling that for contemporary musicians and musicologists such as

Philip, who are increasingly attuned to ever-more metronomic rhythms and aesthetics, the

musically automatical performances of the 1920s and beyond now sound surprisingly

free, individualist (in that modern pejorative “Romantic”), and anti-scientific by

comparison. Thus individualistic musical “freedoms” have largely been recast as an

affected sense of romanticized “rubato” breaking from an assumed and expected

118 Philip, 174.

119

Ibid., 173.

Page 556: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

535

metronomic control. For many decades, the printed New Grove Dictionary noted through

2001, “the popularity of [Joseph Hofmann’s]…free, Romantic style of performance

waned considerably” after the first third of the twentieth century.120

Grove Music Online

continues to cite as an individualistic oddity that the “Romantic pianist” Josef Hofmann

played with “sudden, improvisatory eruptions [i.e. undocumented “invisible” accents

that] served to heighten the tension and emotional content of each piece he performed,”

yet this authoritative reference still fails to ascribe Hofmann’s performance practices to

his self-acknowledged sense of a pre- or anti-metronomic “rhythmicality.”121

In many modern interpretations that now “demand clarity” with little of those

“surprisingly Romantic freedoms”—in which any “rushing” or “slowing down” from an

implicit and continual tick-tock “time” is an audible anomaly—the musical “tempo” of

Bach often sounds like the “tempo” of Bartók; the “rhythms” of Vivaldi often are

indistinguishable from those of Steve Reich; the subjective and variable pulse-sense

underling Beethoven’s concertos and symphonies often approaches the pianola-like,

Dalcrozian “rigidity” of Stravinsky’s Les Noces or Antheil’s Ballet mécanique; in

orchestra concerts today, Debussy’s La Mer often rolls synchronously to John Adams’

ever-more chronographic Short Ride in a Fast Machine. The expansion of a metronomic

education and aesthetic has made this musical-temporal homogeneity possible, expected,

and nearly unquestioned in our age. Thus, it is for our Western musical culture—which

120 Gregor Benko, "Hofmann, Josef," in The New Grove Dictionary, Second edition, Vol. 11: 603-

4.

121 Gregor Benko, "Hofmann, Josef," in Grove Music Online. Oxford Music Online,

http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/13172 (accessed December 3,

2009).

Page 557: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

536

continues to endorse metronomes in scientific performance-practice methodologies and

mass chronographic pedagogies such as Eurhythmics and the Suzuki Method, with their

anachronistic refashionings of historical “tempo” and “rhythm”—that Adorno’s 1948

realization grows more urgent: “Today there is no music showing any trace of the power

of the historical hour that has remained totally unaffected by the decline of [individual]

experience…The dying out of subjective time in music seems totally unavoidable in the

midst of a humanity which has made itself into a thing—into an object of its own

organization.”122

Man Ray’s original artwork-metronome “Object to be Destroyed” was, as its

name foretold, not long for this world. Yet in the continually expanding mechanical

environment in which he lived—where chronographic measurement and regulation

became a prevailing practice for many cultural, industrial, and educational performance-

practices—Man Ray understood that the destruction of one solitary metronome could not

stave off the seemingly unstoppable expansion of metronomic values and actions in the

modern age. Perhaps in reaction to a century informed and influenced by mechanical

objectivity, he created many following iterations of the clockwork metronome from 1923

to 1975, directly paralleling the growing use and importance of the machine in Western

society. These gazing clockwork replications, housed in museums around the world,

would subversively attest to the dominance that metronomic technologies still hold over

modern pedagogies and aesthetics in our day. The title of Man Ray’s widespread series of

resurrected twentieth-century Maelzel metronomes: Object Indestructible.

122 From Theodore Adorno, Philosophy of Modern Music (1948), reprinted in Daniel Albright, ed.

Modernism and Music (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2004), 77.

Page 558: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

537

Figure 7.6. One of Man Ray’s series of nearly identical clockwork metronomes entitled

“Object Indestructible,” 1923-1975.

Long after Beethoven and Bessel, the Western world grew to embrace Maelzel’s

machine as the regulator of objective time for once personal, subjective performance.

Considering his brief, fleeting, and inconsistent interest in mechanically aided tempo-

description, Beethoven could never have imagined the machine’s complete “tyranny”

over all facets of education, culture, and society nearly a century after his passing.

Metronomic performance practices thrived only since the twentieth century. Indeed, by

1920—the year that Wilhelm Wundt, the father of experimental psychology, died—it

seems that the metronome was the common temporal denominator of the modern world.

Page 559: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

538

Only in this self-dubbed Machine Age did typewriting, weight lifting, eye blinking,123

card playing, speaking, writing, walking, breathing, and even piano playing come to be

measured, regulated, and judged against the same artificial control, the same clockwork

automaton, the same scientific metronome.

123 See for instance C. E. Ferree, “An Experimental Examination of the Phenomena Usually

Attributed to Fluctuation of Attention,” The American Journal of Psychology 17 (Jan., 1906): 81-

120; and Sidney M. Newhall, “The Control of Eyelid Movements in Visual Experiments,” The

American Journal of Psychology 44 (Jul., 1932): 555-570.

Page 560: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

539

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Sources before 1815

L'Académie française. Dictionnaire de L'Académie française. 1st Edition. Paris:

L'Académie française, 1694. 4th Edition. Paris: L'Académie française, 1762.

Bayle, Pierre. The dictionary historical and critical of Mr. Peter Bayle. The second

edition. London: printed for J. J. and P. Knapton, etc., 1734-38.

Brossard, Sébastien de. Dictionaire de Musique. Sixieme Edition. Amsterdam: Pierre

Mortier, 1710.

______. A musical dictionary: containing a full explanation of all the terms made use of

in the historical, theoretical, and practical parts of music. London: J. Robson,

1769.

Burney, Charles. The Present State of Music in France and Italy. Second Edition,

Corrected. London: T. Becket and Co., 1773.

______. The Present State of Music in Germany, the Netherlands. and United Provinces.

In two Volumes. Second Edition, Corrected. London: Becket and Co., 1775.

Caccini, Guilio. Le nuove musiche, 1602. Selections translated in Strunk’s Source

Readings in Music History, ed. Leo Treitler, 608-617. New York: Norton, 1998.

Callcott, John Wall. A Musical Grammar in Four Parts. First American Edition. Boston:

West & Blake and Manning & Loring, [1810]. Third Edition. London: Robert

Birchall, 1817.

Chambers, Ephraim. Cyclopædia: or, a universal dictionary of the arts and sciences. 2

vols. The fifth edition. London, 1743.

Choragus (c1630). Selections translated in Strunk’s Source Readings in Music History,

ed. Leo Treitler, 629-634. New York: Norton, 1998.

Couperin, François. L’art de toucher le clavecin. Paris: Author, 1717. Translated by

Margery Halford. Van Nuys, CA: Alfred Pub. Co., 1995.

Decremps, Henri. The conjurer unmasked; or, la magie blanche dévoilée: being a clear

and full explanation of all the surprizing performances exhibited ...London:

Printed for C. Stalker, 1785.

Démotz de la Salle [Demoz]. Méthode de musique selon un nouveau système. Paris:

Pierre Simon, 1728.

Page 561: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

540

Derham, W. The artificial clock-maker. A treatise of watch and clock-work, shewing to

the meanest capacities the art of calculating numbers to all sorts of movement.

London: printed for James Knapton, 1714.

Descartes, René. Compendium Musicae (Ms.,1618). Utrecht: Jansson, 1650. Translation

by Walter Robert. [Rome]: American Institute of Musicology, 1961.

______. Discourse on the method of rightly conducting the reason, and seeking the Truth

in the Sciences (1637). Translated from the French. Edinburgh: Sutherland and

Knox, 1850.

Diderot, Denis. Encyclopédie ou Dictionnaire raisonné des sciences, des arts et des

métiers… Paris: Briasson, 1751-1772.

Ellicott, John. “An Account of the Influence Which Two Pendulum Clocks Were

Observed to Have upon Each Other.” Philosophical Transactions (1683-1775) 41

(1739 - 1741): 126-128.

______. “A Description of Two Methods, by Which the Irregularity of the Motion of a

Clock, Arising from the Influence of Heat and Cold upon the Rod of the

Pendulum, May be Prevented.” Philosophical Transactions (1683-1775) 47 (1751

- 1752): 479-494.

Encyclopædia Britannica; or, a dictionary of arts, sciences, &c. The second edition;

greatly improved and enlarged. 10 Volumes. Edinburgh: printed for J. Balfour and

Co. W. Gordon, J. Bell, J. Dickson, C. Elliot, etc., 1778-83.

Frere, Alexandre. Transpositions de musique, reduites au naturel, par le secours de la

modulation. Amsterdam: Roger, 1710.

Gardiner, J. S. A definition of minuet-dancing, rules for behaviour in company &c. A

dialogue between a lady, and a dancing-master. Translated by John Peyton.

Madeley: J. Edmund, 1786.

Grassineau, James. A musical dictionary: A new edition, to which is added an appendix.

London: J. Robson, 1769.

An historical miscellany of the curiosities and rarities in nature and art. Comprising new

and entertaining descriptions of the most surprising volcanos, caverns, cataracts,

whirlpools, ... in every part of the habitable world. Volume III. London, [1794-

1800]. Eighteenth Century Collections Online. Gale.

http://find.galegroup.com/ecco/infomark.do?&contentSet=ECCOArticles&type=

multipage&tabID=T001&prodId=ECCO&docId=CW101876105&source=gale&

userGroupName=cwru_main&version=1.0&docLevel=FASCIMILE.

Page 562: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

541

Holcroft, Thomas. Deaf and Dumb, or the Abbe de l'Epee. London, New York: Samuel

French & Son, 1801. English Prose Drama Full-Text Database.

http://ebooks.ohiolink.edu.

______. Knave; or Not? London: Printed for G. G. and J. Robinson [etc.], 1798. English

Prose Drama Full-Text Database. http://ebooks.ohiolink.edu.

Huygens, Christiaan. Horologium oscillatorium (1673). Translated as The Pendulum

Clock or Geometrical Demonstrations Concerning the Motion of Pendula as

Applied to Clocks by Richard J. Blackwell. Ames: Iowa State University Press,

1986.

Imison, John. The school of arts; or, an introduction to useful knowledge, being a

compilation of real experiments and improvements, in several pleasing branches

of science…London: printed for the author, 1785.

“Instructions Concerning the Use of Pendulum-Watches, for Finding the Longitude at

Sea.” Philosophical Transactions (1665-1678) 4 (1669): 937-976.

Keatinge, H. S. Musical preceptor, or, A key to harmony. Baltimore: H. S. Keatinge,

1808.

La Fontaine, Augustus; The village pastor and his children. Translated from the German.

New-York: D. Longworth, 1810.

Lecointe, John. An Apology for Dancing. Translated by John Peyton. London: J. Kippax,

1752.

Loulié, Etienne. Eléments ou principes de musique. Paris: Ballard, 1696; Amsterdam:

Roger, 1698.

L’Affilard, Michel. Principes très-faciles pour bein apprendre la musique. First edition.

Paris: Ballard, 1694.

______. Principes très-faciles pour bein apprendre la musique. Fifth edition. Paris:

Ballard, 1705.

Malcolm, Alexander. A treatise of musick, speculative, practical, and historical.

Edinburgh: Printed by the Author, 1721.

Masson, Charles. Nouveau traité des règles pour la composition de la musique. Paris:

Ballard, 1699.

Mattheson, Johann. Kern melodischer Wissenschafft. Hamburg: C. Herold, 1735.

Page 563: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

542

Mersenne, Marin. Harmonie universelle. Paris: Cramoisy, 1636. Translated by Carol

MacClintock in Readings in the History of Music Performance. Bloomington:

Indiana University Press, 1979.

______. Les préludes de l'harmonie universelle: ou Questions curieuses, utiles aux

prédicateurs, aux théologiens, aux astrologues, aux médecins & aux philosophes.

Paris: H. Guenon, 1634.

______. Traite' de l'harmonie universelle ou est conteu la Musique Theoretique et

pratique. Paris, 1627. Translated in John Bernard Egan as “Critical translation of

the second book.” PhD diss., Indiana University, 1962.

Minshull, John. A Comedy, Entitled the Sprightly Widow. New-York: Printed for the

Author, 1803. American Drama Full-Text Database. http://ebooks.ohiolink.edu.

O'Hara, Charles P. F. The Gentleman's musical repository. New York: Printed for the

author, 1813.

Ons-em-Bray [D'Onsembray]. Catalogue des Livre et Estampes de la Bibliotheque de feu

Monsieur Pajot, Comte D'Onsembray, Honoraire de L'Academie Royale des

Sciences. Paris: Martin et Damonneville, 1756.

Perrine. Pieces de luth en musique avec des regles pour les toucher parfaitement sur le

luth et sur le clavessin. Paris: Author, 1680.

Pilkington, H. W. A Musical Dictionary. Boston: Watson & Bangs, 1812.

Pinchbeck, William Frederick. Witchcraft, or the Art of Fortune-Telling Unveiled.

Boston: printed for the Author, 1805.

Quantz, Johann Joachim. Essai d'une méthode pour apprendre à jouer de la flute

traversière. Berlin: Voss, 1752.

______. Versuch einer Anweisung die Flöte traversiere zu spielen. Berlin: J. F. Voss,

1752. Translated by Edward R. Reilly as On playing the flute. Boston:

Northeastern University Press, 2001.

Rameau, Jean-Philippe. Traite de L'Harmonie. Paris: Ballard, 1722. Translated by Philip

Gossett. New York: Dover Publications [1971].

Rees, Abraham. The cyclopædia; or, Universal dictionary of arts, sciences, and

literature. In forty-one volumes. Philadelphia: Samuel F. Bradford, and Murray,

Fairman and Co., 1805.

Rittenhouse, David. “On the Improvement of Time-Keepers.” Transactions of the

American Philosophical Society 4 (1799): 26-28.

Page 564: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

543

Saint Lambert, Michel de. Les principes du clavecin. Paris: Ballard, 1702. Translated and

edited by Rebecca Harris-Warrick. London: Cambridge University Press 1984.

Sauveur, Joseph. Principes D'Acoustic et de Musique ou Systeme General des Intervalles

des sons… Paris, 1701.

Simpson, Christopher. A compendium: or, introduction to practical musick in five parts.

Fourth edition with additions. London: W. Pearson, for John Cullen, 1706.

Steele, Joshua. Prosodia Rationalis, an Essay Towards Establishing the Melody and

Measure of Speech. London: T. Payne, 1779.

Sully, Henri. Règle artificielle du tems. Paris: Gregoire Dupuis, 1717.

Tans'ur, William. A new musical grammar, and dictionary: or, a general introduction to

the whole art of musick. In four books. The third edition with large additions.

London: Robert Brown, for James Hodges, 1756.

Tomlinson, Kellom. The Art of Dancing. London: Printed by the author, 1735.

Türk, Daniel Gottlob. Klavierschule, oder, Anweisung zum Klavierspielen für Lehrer und

Lernende: mit kritischen Anmerkungen (1789). Leipsig und Halle: Auf Kosten des

Verfassers, in Kommission bey Schwickert, 1802.

Turner, William. Sound anatomiz’d, in a philosophical essay on musick. Wherein is

explained the nature of sound. London: William Pearson, 1724.

Walther, Johann Gottfried. Musicalisches lexicon. Leipzig: W. Deer, 1732

Ward, John. A Practical Method To Discover the Longitude at Sea By a New Contrived

Automaton. London: James Woodward, 1714.

Winstanley, William. The Hypocrite Unmask'd. New York: Geo. F. Hopkins, 1801.

American Drama Full-Text Database. http://ebooks.ohiolink.edu.

Sources after 1815

Albrecht, Theodore, ed. Letters to Beethoven and Other Correspondence. Three

Volumes. Lincoln & London: University of Nebraska Press, 1996.

Albright, Daniel, ed., Modernism and Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press,

2004.

Page 565: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

544

______. Quantum poetics: Yeats, Pound, Eliot, and the Science of Modernism.

Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press, 1997

Adams, Ernest. “Electric Metronome.” U.S. Patent #734,032. Filed July 30, 1902.

Patented July 21, 1903.

Adams, John. The New Teaching. London: Hodder and Stoughton, 1918.

Albright, Daniel, ed. Modernism and Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2004.

Alvin, H. and R. Prieur. Métronomie experimentale. Paris: Librairie Fischbacher, 1895.

Amar, Jules. The Human Motor, or, The Scientific Foundations of Labor and Industry.

London: George Routledge & Sons, Ltd., 1920.

______. The Physiology of Industrial Organization and the Re-employment of the

Disabled. Translated by Bernard Miall. London: The Library Press Limited, 1918.

American Society of Dental Surgeons. “French Medical Societies.” American Journal of

Dental Science IV (Dec. 1843): 126-127.

Angell, Frank and Henry Harwood. “Experiments on Discrimination of Clangs for

Different Intervals of Time.” The American Journal of Psychology 11 (Oct.,

1899): 67-79.

Anrep, G. V. “The Irradiation of Conditioned Reflexes.” In Proceedings of the Royal

Society of London. Series B, Containing Papers of a Biological Character 94

(April 3, 1923): 404-426.

Antheil, George. “Antheilicized Notation, Or Music for Everybody who Can Tell One

from Two and Two From Three, Without previous experience anyone can now

read music at sight. Instantaneous System of Reading Music.” Manuscript. Box

10, Antheil Collection, Library of Congress, Washington D.C.

______. Bad Boy of Music. Reprint. Hollywood: Samuel French, 1990.

______. “An Explanation of My Evolution since 1924.” Manuscript to Joseph Hofmann,

Signed Dec. 1927. Box 10, Antheil Collection, Library of Congress, Washington

D.C.

Antisell, Thomas. Hand-Book of the Useful Arts. New York: George P. Putnam, 1852.

Ariotti, Piero. "Galileo on the Isochrony of the Pendulum." Isis 59 (Winter, 1968): 414-

426.

Page 566: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

545

Armonville, J. R. Le guide des artistes or Répertoire des arts et manufanctures. Paris:

Chaingnieau, 1818.

B., H. L. “A Lesson from the Metronome.” Christian Science Journal XIII (April, 1895):

375-377.

B., J. W. “Brief Mention.” Modern Language Notes 38 (Feb., 1923): 127.

Baker, Theodore. A Dictionary of Musical Terms. Third Edition. New York: G. Schirmer,

1897.

Baldwin, James Mark, ed. Dictionary of Philosophy and Psychology. Volume I. New

York: Macmillan Company, 1901.

Barnett, Jo Ellen. Time's Pendulum: the Quest to Capture Time--from Sundials to Atomic

Clocks. New York: Plenum Trade, 1998.

Barnum, P. T. The Autobiography of P. T. Barnum. Second edition. London: Ward and

Lock, 1855.

Barrows, Franklin W. “The New York State Science Teachers' Association.” Science 5

(Mar. 19, 1897): 457-468.

Bartky, Ian. Selling the True Time, Nineteenth Century Timekeeping in America.

Stanford: Stanford University Press, 2000.

Bartók, Béla. Essays. Edited by Benjamin Suchoff. New York: St. Martins Press, 1976.

Bassett, M. F., and C. J. Warne. “On the Lapse of Verbal Meaning with Repetition.” The

American Journal of Psychology 30 (Oct., 1919): 415-418.

Baum, Paull Franklin. The Principles of English Versification. Third edition. Cambridge:

Harvard University Press, 1924.

Beard, George M. American Nervousness its Causes and Consequences. New York: G. P.

Putnam’s Sons, 1881.

Beethoven, Ludwig van. Beethoven; letters, journals and conversations. Edited,

translated, and introduced by Michael Hamburger. New York: Pantheon, [1952].

Reprint, New York: Thames and Hudson, 1984.

______. Beethoven's letters. A critical edition with explanatory notes by Dr. A.C.

Kalischer, translated with preface by J.S. Shedlock. London: J. M. Dent; New

York: E. P. Dutton, 1909. Reprint, New York: Dover Editions, 1972.

Page 567: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

546

______. Beethoven's letters, 1790-1826, from the collection of L. Nohl. Also his letters to

the archduke Rudolph. Tr. by Lady Wallace. In two volumes. London: Longmans,

Green, and Co., 1866.

______. Letters. Collected, translated and edited with an introduction, appendixes, notes

and indexes by Emily Anderson. 3 volumes. London: Macmillan; New York: St.

Martin's Press, 1961.

______. New Beethoven letters. Translated and annotated by Donald W. MacArdle and

Ludwig Misch. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press, [1957].

______. Piano Sonata no. 32 in C minor, Op. 111. Reproduction of the autograph

manuscript together with a reprint of the Breitkoph & Härtel complete works

edition. The facsimile series of music manuscripts. Eric Simon, editor. New York:

Dover Publications, Inc. 1968.

______. The Symphony of Life: Letters by Ludwig van Beethoven. Translated and

prefaced by Ulrich L. Steindorff. Los Angeles: U.S. Library Association, Inc.,

c1933.

Bennett, Arnold. Mental Efficiency and other Hints to Men and Women. New York:

George H. Doran Company, 1911.

Benton, William Burnett. “Speeding Up Our Reading.” The Scientific Monthly 47 (Sept.,

1938): 261-263.

Berhard, Karl. Travels through North America, During the Years 1825 and 1826. Vol. 1.

Philadelphia: Carey, Lea &Carey, 1828.

Berlioz, Hector. Grand traité d’instrumentation et d’orchestration modernes, op.10.

Paris: Schonenberger, 1855.

______. A Treatise on Modern Instrumentation and Orchestration. Trans. Mary Cowden

Clarke. London: Novello, Ewer, and Co., 1856.

Blitz, Antonio. Life and Adventures of Signor Blitz. Hartford: T. Belknap, 1872.

Bolton, Thaddeus L. “Rhythm.” The American Journal of Psychology 6 (Jan., 1894):

145-238.

Boorstin, Daniel. The Discoverers. New York: Random House, 1983.

Boring, Edwin G. A History of Experimental Psychology. New York: Appleton-Century

Company, 1929.

Borrel, Eugène. “Les indications metronomiques…du XVIIIe siecle.” Revue de

musicology T.9c (1928): 149-153.

Page 568: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

547

______. L'interprétation de la musique française (de Lully à la Révolution). Paris: Alcan,

1934.

Bowman, Edward Morris. “Letters from a Musician to His Nephew.” In The Essentials of

Music, Volume 1, Emil Liebling, editor, 125-221. The American History and

Encyclopedia, edited by W. L. Hubbard. New York: Irving Squire, 1910.

Brady, James. “Metronome.” U.S. Patent #738,093. Filed Apr. 5, 1902. Patented Sept. 1,

1903.

Brambach, Wilhelm. Rhythmische und metrische Untersuchungen. Leipzig: B. G.

Teubner, 1871.

Brearley, Harry C. Time Telling Through the Ages. New York: Doubleday, Page & Co.,

1919.

Briscoe, James R., ed. Debussy in Performance. New Haven: Yale University Press,

1999.

Brower, Harriette Moore. Piano Mastery, Second Series. New York: Frederick A. Stokes

Co., 1917.

______. Self-Help in Piano Study. New York: Frederick A. Stokes Co., 1920.

Browning, Mildred, Dorothy E. Brown, and M. F. Washburn. “The Effect of the Interval

between Repetitions on the Speed of Learning a Series of Movements.” The

American Journal of Psychology 24 (Oct., 1913): 580-583.

Bryan, B. T. Stenotypy, The Machine Way in Shorthand. Revised Edition. Indianapolis:

The Stenotype Company, 1915.

Bücher, Karl. Arbeit und Rhythmus. Third Edition. Leipzig: B. G. Teubner, 1902.

Buchner, Alexander. Mechanical Musical Instruments. London: Batchworth Press [n.d.].

Burdon-Sanderson, John Scott. Syllabus of a course of lectures on physiology. Second

edition. London: K. Lewis, 1879.

Burney, Sarah Harriet. Geraldine Fauconberg. Philadelphia: M. Carey and Son, 1817.

Burnley, James. The Romance of Invention. London: Cassel & Company, Ltd., 1886.

Butler, Samuel. A First Year in Canterbury Settlement. Edited by R. A. Streatfield.

London. A. C. Fifield, 1914.

Page 569: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

548

Cadett, James. “ On the Establishment of a Standard Measurement of Sensitiveness, no

II.” The British Journal of Photography XXVII (Oct. 15, 1880): 496-7.

The Cambridge Scientific Instrument Company. Physiological instruments manufactured

by the Cambridge Scientific Instrument Company. Cambridge: Printed at the

University Press, 1899.

Cameron, Andrew C. “The Problems and Prospects of Labor.” The Address of the

National Labor Congress to the Workingmen of the United States (Chicago, 1867)

[Commons, IX, 141–168]. In Annals of American History.

http://america.eb.com/america/article?articleId=386309&query=automaton.

Canada Parliament. “Sessional Paper No. 1.” Sessional Papers XXXV (1901): Q-65.

Carpenter, William. Principles of Mental Physiology. Fourth edition. New York: D.

Appleton and Company, 1883.

Carrington, Hereward. Higher Psychical Development (Yoga Philosophy), an Outline of

the Secret Hindu Teachings. New York: Dodd, Mead and Company, 1920.

Cassel, Edna E. and K. M. Dallenbach. “The Effect of Auditory Distraction upon the

Sensory Reaction.” The American Journal of Psychology 29 (Apr., 1918): 129-

143.

Cattell, James McKeen. “The Psychological Laboratory at Leipsic.” Mind 13 (Jan.,

1888): 37-51.

Channel, David. The Vital Machine, a Study of Technology and Organic Life. New York:

Oxford University Press, 1991.

Chase, Mary Wood. Natural laws in piano technic. Boston: Oliver Ditson Co., 1910.

Chaulieu, Charles. Chaulieu on the metronome. London: R. Cocks & Co., 1843.

Chittenden, Kate S. “Report of Piano Conference.” In Studies in Musical Education

History and Aesthetics, Papers and Proceedings of the Music Teachers’ National

Association (December 27-31, 1907): 144-151.

Christensen, Thomas, ed. The Cambridge History of Western Music Theory. Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press, 2006.

Christiani, Adolph Friedrich. The Principles of Expression in Pianoforte Playing. New

York: Harper & Brothers, 1885.

Church of Christ. The Christian Science Hymnal. Boston: The Christian Science

Publishing Society, 1910.

Page 570: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

549

Cohen, Albert. "Etienne Loulié as a Music Theorist." JAMS XVIII (1965): 70–72.

Colby, Frank Moore, editor. The International Year Book, A Compendium of the World’s

Progress for the Year 1907. New York: Dodd, Mead, and Company, 1908.

Combarieu, Jules. Théorie du Rythme dans la Composition Moderne. Paris: Alphonse

Picard et Fils, 1897.

Comstock, Andrew. A System of Elocution with Special Reference to Gesture, to the

Treatment of Stammering and Defective Articulation. Philadelphia: E. H. Butler &

Co., 1855.

Constant, Premier Valet de Chambre. Recollections of the Private Life of Napoleon. Vol.

II. Translated by Walter Clark. Akron: Saalfield Publishing Co., 1907.

“A Contribution to Our Knowledge of Phytolacca Decandra, and of Grindelia Rocusta.”

In Atlanta Medical and Surgical Journal, 477-482. Atlanta: H. H. Dickson, 1878.

Cooke, James Francis. Great Pianists on Piano Playing: Study Talks with Foremost

Virtuosos. Reprint. Philadelphia: Theodore Presser Co., 1917.

Coullery, Henri. “Metronome.” U.S. Patent #1,058,659. Filed Feb. 24, 1908. Patented

Apr. 8, 1913.

Craft, Robert. “Saint Lambert.” In Small Craft Advisories, 146-147. New York: Thames

and Hudson, 1989.

Crane, Julia E. Music Teachers Manual. Seventh Edition. Potsdam, NY: Elliot Fay &

Sons, 1915.

Cunningham, Allan. Roorkee Hydraulic Experiments. [N.P.]: Thomason College Press,

1881.

Dahlhaus, Carl. Nineteenth-Century Music. Berkeley: University of California Press,

1991.

“Dalcroze Eurhythmics Have Come to New York.” New York Times, Jan 25, 1914, SM5.

Dallenbach, Karl M. “The Measurement of Attention.” The American Journal of

Psychology 24 (Oct. 1913): 465-507.

Damrosch, Walter. My Musical Life. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, 1923.

Daniels, Arthur H. “The Memory and After-Image and Attentions.” The American

Journal of Psychology 6 (Jan., 1895): 558-564.

Page 571: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

550

Datson, Lorraine and Peter Galison. Objectivity. New York: Zone Books, 2007.

Dauprat, Louis François. Le professeur de musique. Paris: A. Quinzard, 1857.

Davies, Henry. “A Plea for a Science of Art.” Music XVIII (May, 1900): 1-15.

Dechevrens, Antoine. Du rythme dans l'hymnographie latine. Paris: Delhomme et

Briguet, 1895.

Delafield, John. Mysticism and its Results; Being an Inquiry into the Uses and Abuses of

Secrecy. Saint Louis: Edwards & Bushnell, 1857. Gutenberg eBook #22314.

http://www.gutenberg.org.

Delboeuf, M. J. “What May Animals be Taught?” The Popular Science Monthly XXIX

(New York: D Appleton and Company, 1886): 168-179.

Dewar, James. “On the Physiological Action of Light.” In Royal Institution of Great

Britain, Proceedings VIII (1875-1878): 137-149.

Dewey, Evelyn, Emily Child, and Beardsley Ruml. Methods and Results of Testing

School Children. New York: E. P. Dutton & Company, 1920.

Dickinson, Edward. The Education of a Music Lover. New York: Charles Scribner’s

Sons, 1911.

Dierig, Sven. “Engines for Experiment: Laboratory Revolution and Industrial Labor in

the Nineteenth-Century City.” Osiris 18 (2003): 116-134.

Disney, Walt, et al. “Method and apparatus for Synchronizing Photoplays.” U.S. Patent

#1,941,341. Applied Apr. 2, 1931. Patented Dec. 26, 1933.

Donington, Robert. The Interpretation of Early Music. New York: Norton, 1992.

“The Dormant Waker.” New York Times, Feb. 18, 1913, 12.

Duglison, Robley, A Dictionary of Medical Science. Twenty-first edition. Philadelphia:

Lea Brothers & Co., 1895.

Dutton, Alvin R., B. Harrison, et al. Manual of Free-hand Penmanship. Boston: J. W. C.

Gilman & Co., 1877.

Dyson, W. F. “Total Eclipse of the Sun, 1901 May 18.” Monthly Notices of the Royal

Astronomical Society LXII (Nov. 1901-Nov. 1902): 235-247.

Ebel, Otto. The Metronome and Its Use. New York: Chandler & Held music co. [1903].

Page 572: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

551

Ebhardt, Kurt. Zwei Beiträge zur Psychologie des Rhythmus und des Tempo. Leipzig:

Johann Ambrosius Barth, 1898.

Edmunds, W. Sound and Rhythm. London: Baillière, Tindall and Cox, 1906.

“Elijah Restudied: Interpreting The Metronome.” The Times, Saturday, Oct. 29, 1932, 13.

Ellis, A. Caswell and Maud Margaret Shipe. “A Study of the Accuracy of the Present

Methods of Testing Fatigue.” The American Journal of Psychology 14 (Jul.-Oct.,

1903): 232-245.

Elson, Arthur. “Modern Piano Methods.” In Modern Music and Musicians, Volume 1,

69-82. Louis Charles Elson, ed. New York: The University Society, Inc., 1918.

Elson, Louis C. Mistakes and Disputed Points in Music and Music Teaching.

Philadelphia: Theodore Presser Co., 1910.

Engel, Carl. The Pianist’s Hand-book. London: Hope and Co., 1853.

Evans, Henry Ridgely. “The Romance of Automata.” The Open Court, Mar. 1, 1905,

131.

Ewen, David. The Man with the Baton; the Story of Conductors and Their Orchestras.

New York: Thomas Y. Crowell company, 1937.

Ewing, Adelaide. “Device for Counting Time.” U.S. Patent #1,496,258. Filed Nov. 1922.

Patented Jun. 3, 1924.

Farrow, Edward S. A Dictionary of Military Terms. Revised Edition. New York: Thomas

Y. Crowell Company, 1918.

Ferree, C. E. “An Experimental Examination of the Phenomena Usually Attributed to

Fluctuation of Attention.” The American Journal of Psychology 17 (Jan., 1906):

81-120.

Fétis, François Joseph. Biographie universelle des musiciens et bibliographie générale.

Buxelles: Meline, Cans et Companie, 1837-1844.

______. Histoire générale de la musique. Two volumes. Paris: Librairie de Firmin Didot

Frères, Fils et cie., 1869.

______. Manuel des principes de musique: à l'usage des professeurs et des élèves de

toutes les écoles de musique, particulièrement des écoles primaires. Paris: M.

Schlesinger, 1837.

Page 573: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

552

______. La musique mise à la portée de tout le monde. Paris, A. Mesnier, 1830.

Translated as Music explained to the world: or, How to understand music and

enjoy its performance. Translated for the Boston Academy of Music. Boston:

Benjamin Perkins, 1842.

Finck, Henry T. Success in Music and How it is Won. New York: Charles Scribner’s

Sons, 1909.

______. Wagner and His Works. Volume I. Fifth edition. New York: Charles Scribner’s

Sons, 1898.

Fink, Robert. “‘Rigoroso ( = 126)’: ‘The Rite of Spring’ and the Forging of a Modernist

Performing Style.” JAMS 52 (1999): 299-362.

Fitzhugh, George. “The Failure of Free Society.” From Sociology for the South, or the

Failure of Free Society (Richmond, Va., 1854): iii, 7–12, 34–48, 83–95, 161–163,

177–186. In Annals of American History.

http://america.eb.com/america/article?articleId=386012&query=automaton.

Foucaud, Edward. Les artisans illustres. Paris: Béthune et Plon, 1841.

______. The Book of Illustrious Mechanics of Europe and America. Translated from the

French by John Frost. New York: Appelton & Co, 1847.

Frangsmyr, Tore, and J. L. Heilbron, ed. The Quantifying Spirit in the 18th Century.

Berkeley: University of California Press, 1990.

Fraser, Thomas R. “On the Physiological Action of the Calabar Bean.” Journal of

Anatomy and Physiology I (Nov., 1886): 323-332.

Freeman, Frank N. “A Course in Handwriting: II.” Elementary School Journal XXII

(Sept. 1921-June, 1922): 441-450.

______. The Teaching of Handwriting. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1914.

Fuller-Maitland, John Alexander. Schumann. London: Sampson Low, Marston, Searle &

Rivington, 1884.

Galison, Peter. Einstein’s Clocks, Poincaré’s Maps. New York: W.W. Norton and Co.,

2003.

Gamble, Eleanor A. McC. “A Study of Three Variables in Memorizing.” The American

Journal of Psychology 39 (Dec., 1927): 223-234.

“Gardiner’s Music of Nature” [Book Review]. In The New York Review V (July, 1838):

44-67.

Page 574: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

553

Geary, Eleanor. Musical education: with practical observations on the art of piano-forte

playing. New and revised edition. London: D’Almaine and Co., 1851.

Gehrkens, Karl Wilson. Essentials in Conducting. Philadelphia: Oliver Ditson Co., 1919.

Glyn, Margaret. The Rhythmic Conception of Music. London: Longmans, Green, and Co.,

1907.

Goehr, Lydia. The Imaginary Museum of Musical Works. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1992.

Goldstein, Joanna. A Beethoven Enigma: Performance Practice and the Piano Sonata,

Opus 111. American university studies. Series XX. New York: Peter Lang, 1988.

Goodrich, Helen and Bertha Payne. “Music.” The Course of Study…devoted to the work

of the Chicago Institute I (July, 1900): 417-420.

Goudsmit, Maurits. “Music Time Indicator.” U.S. Patent #1,783,537. Filed Aug. 2, 1923.

Patented Dec. 2, 1930.

Gould, Hanna F. “To the Automaton Chess Player.” In Landmark Anthologies:

Specimens of American poetry. Boston: S. G. Goodrich & Co, [1829]. American

Poetry Full-Text Database. http://ebooks.ohiolink.edu.

Grew, Sydney. The Art of the Player-Piano. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner &

Co., 1922.

Grey, William. “Time.” The English Mechanic and World of Science 39 (May 9, 1884):

218.

Grove, George, ed. A Dictionary of Music and Musicians. In Three Volumes. London:

Macmillan and Co., 1880-1889.

Grund, Francis Joseph. The Americans in their Moral, Social, and Political Relations.

Boston: Marsh, Capen, and Lyon, 1837.

Hall, G. Stanley. Adolescence: Its Psychology and its Relations to Physiology,

Anthropology, Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion, and Education. Vol. 1. New

York: D. Appleton and Co., 1904.

______. Youth: Its Education, Regimen, and Hygiene. New York: D. Appleton and

Company, 1907.

Hall, Winfred S. A Laboratory Guide in Physiology. Chicago: Chicago Medical Book

Co., 1897.

Page 575: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

554

Hallock (Greenewalt), Mary. “The Correspondence of Pulse and Rhythm.” The

Metaphysical Magazine XXI (May-Dec., 1907): 378-380.

______. “Pulse in Verbal Rhythm.” Poet Lore 16 (1905): 79-85.

Hamilton, Clarence G. How to Use the Metronome Correctly. Philadelphia: Theodore

Presser Co., 1916.

______. Piano Teaching, Its Principles and Problems. Boston: Oliver Ditson Company,

1910.

Hamilton, Clayton Meeker. Studies in Stagecraft. New York: Henry Hold and Company,

1914.

Hanchett, Henry G. The Art of the Musician, a Guide to the Intelligent Appreciation of

Music. New York: Grosset & Dunlap, 1905.

Hanson, John Treadway. “Metronome.” U.S. Patent #512,470. Filed Mar. 13, 1893.

Patented Jan. 9, 1894.

______. “A New Metronome.” In Proceedings of the Musical Association, 20th Sess.

(1893 – 1894), 23-24. London: Novello, Ewer and Co., 1894.

Harding, R. E. M. Origins of Musical Time and Expression. London: Oxford University

Press, 1938.

Harris-Warrick, Rebecca. "Interpreting Pendulum Markings for French Baroque Dances.”

Historical performance 6 (1993): 9-22.

Hauptmann, Moritz. The Letters of a Leipzig Cantor. Edited by Alfred Schöne and

Ferdinand Hiller. Translated by A.D. Coleridge. Volume II. London: Novello,

Ewer and Co., and Richard Bentley and Son, 1892.

______. The Nature of Harmony and Metre. Translated and edited by W. E. Heathcote.

London: Swan Sonnenschein & Co., 1888.

Hayes, Samuel Perkins. “A Study of the Affective Qualities. I. The Tridimensional

Theory of Feeling.” The American Journal of Psychology 17 (Jul., 1906): 358-

393.

Hazard, Willis P. Annals of Philadelphia and Pennsylvania in the Olden Time.

Philadelphia: J. M. Stoddart & Co., 1879.

Hefling, Stephen E. Rhythmic Alteration in Seventeenth- and Eighteenth-Century Music.

New York: Schirmer, 1993.

Page 576: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

555

Henderson, W. J. The Orchestra and Orchestral Music. New York: Charles Scribner’s

Sons, 1899.

Henry, Bertram C. “Development of Rhythmic Conception.” Music XIII (Nov., 1897-

Apr., 1898): 469-471.

Hickok, Laurens Perseus. Rational Psychology or The Subjective Idea and Objective Law

of All Intelligence. Auburn: Derby, Miller & Co., 1849.

Hindemith, Paul. Selected Letters of Paul Hindemith. Edited by Geoffrey Skelton. New

Haven: Yale University Press, 1995.

Hirn, G.-A. Construction et Emploi du Métronome en Musique. Paris: Gauthier-Villars,

1887.

Hobsbawm, Eric. Industry and Empire. Reprint. New York: The New Press, 1999.

Hofmann, Joseph. Piano Playing. New York: The McClure Company, 1908.

______. “Playing the Piano Correctly.” The Ladies’ Home Journal, Oct. 1, 1901, 18.

______. “Playing the Piano Successfully.” The Ladies’ Home Journal, Mar. 1905, 9.

Holland, Bernhard. “When Histrionics Undermine the Music and the Pianist.” New York

Times, February 6, 2008.

<http://www.nytimes.com/2008/02/06/arts/music/06look.html>

Holmes, Arthur. The Conservation of the Child. Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott Company,

1912.

Holmes, Oliver Wendell. “The Human Wheel, its Spokes and Felloes.” Originally in

Atlantic Monthly 11 (May, 1863). Reprinted in Soundings from the Atlantic, 282-

327. Boston: Tickner and Fields, 1866.

______. Over the Teacups. Cambridge: Riverside Press, 1891.

Hopkins, G. The Metronome, and How to Use It. London: Weekes & Co., 1925.

Hosmer, George L. “The Eclipse Expedition of the Massachusetts Institute of

Technology to Washington: Part II. Report of Observations.” Technology

Quarterly and Proceedings of the Society of Arts XIII (Sept., 1900): 165-171.

Horkheimer, Max, and Theodore Adorno. Dialectic of Enlightenment (1944). Edited by

Gunzelin Schmid Noerr. Translated by Edmund Jephcott. Stanford: Stanford

University Press, 2002.

Page 577: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

556

Houle, George. Meter in Music 1600-1800. Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1987.

Howe, Henry. Memoirs of the Most Eminent American Mechanics, also Lives of

Distinguished European Mechanics. New York: J. C. Derby, 1856.

Hoyer, Egbert von and Franz Kreuter. Technologisches Wörterbuch, Deutsch-englisch-

französisch: Gewerbe & Industrie. Wiesbaden: J. F. Bergman, 1902.

Hubbard, W. L., ed. Musical Dictionary. In series The American History and

Encyclopedia. New York: Irving Squire, 1910.

Hudson, Richard. Stolen Time: The History of Tempo Rubato. Oxford: Clarendon Press,

1994.

Hummel, Johann Nepomuck. Ausführlich theoretisch-practische Anweisung zum Piano-

forte Spiel. Three volumes. Wein: Tobias Haslinger, 1828.

Hutton, Charles. A Philosophical and Mathematical Dictionary. A New Edition. In Two

Volumes. London: Printed for the author, 1815.

Ingham, Percy B. “The Jaques-Dalcroze Method.” In The Eurhythmics of Jaques-

Dalcroze, 31-45. Boston: Small Maynard and Company, 1913.

“Intelligence: Col. Amoros’ Gymnastic school Paris.” In American Journal of Education

I (Boston: Wiat, Green, and Co., 1826): 689-690.

Jacques, Edgar F. “The Composer’s Intention.” In Proceedings of the Musical

Association, 18th Sess. (1891-1892): 35-54.

Jaques-Dalcroze, Emile. “Eurhythmics and Moving Plastic.” In Rhythm, Music and

Education, 257-286. Translated by Harold F. Rubinstein. New York: G.P.

Putnam’s Sons, 1921.

______. “The Initiation into Rhythm.” In Rhythm, Music, and Education, 79-92.

Translated by Harold F. Rubinstein. New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons, 1921.

______. “Rhythm as a Factor of Education.” In The Eurhythmics of Jaques-Dalcroze, 15-

25. Boston: Small Maynard and Company, 1913.

______. “Rhythmic Movement, Solfège, and Improvisation,” In Rhythm, Music, and

Education, 115-142. Translated by Harold F. Rubinstein. New York: G.P.

Putnam’s Sons, 1921.

James, William. The Principles of Psychology. Volume 1. New York: Henry Holt and

Co., 1890.

Page 578: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

557

Jastrow, Joseph; H. K. Wolfe; W. L. Hervey; Nicholas Murray Butler; William James;

George Trumbull Ladd; James McKeen Cattell; W. L. Bryan; E. C. Sanford; J.

Mark Baldwin. “Psychology in American Colleges and Universities.” The

American Journal of Psychology 3 (Apr., 1890): 275-286.

Jefferson, Thomas. Memoirs, Correspondence, and Private Papers of Thomas Jefferson.

Volume IV. London: Colburn and Bentley, 1829.

Johns Hopkins University. “List of the More Important Physiological Apparatus in the

Biological Laboratory.” In Report of the President, 62-63. Baltimore: William K.

Boyle & Son, 1876.

______. Studies from the Biological Laboratory, Session 1877-78. Edited by H. Newell

Martin. Baltimore: John Murphy & Co., 1879.

Johnstone, J. Alfred. Essentials in Piano-Playing and Other Musical Studies. London: W.

Reeves, 1914.

______. Touch, Phrasing, and Interpretation. London: W. Reeves, 1900.

Karr, M. L. “The metronome—its use.” The Musical Visitor 25 (Apr., 1896): 98.

Kennedy, John P. Memoirs of the Life of William Wirt. Volume II. Philadelphia: Lea and

Blanchard, 1850.

Kern, Stephen. The Culture of Time and Space, 1880-1918. Second Edition. Cambridge:

Harvard University Press, 2003.

Ketchum, Joseph. “Apparatus for Producing Artificial Respiration.” U.S. Patent

#320,070. Filed Apr. 1, 1885. Patented June 16, 1885.

Knight, Edward H. Knight’s American Mechanical Dictionary. Volume II. New York: J.

B. Ford and Co., 1875.

______. Knight’s New Mechanical Dictionary. Boston: Houghton, Mifflin and Company,

1884.

Köhler, Louis. Die Melodie der Sprache in ihrer Anwendung besonders auf das Lied und

die Oper. Leipzig: J. J. Weber, 1853.

Kolisch, Mitzi. “Stravinsky--Russian of the Russians.” The Independent, May 16, 1925,

559.

Kolisch, Rudolf. “Tempo and Character in Beethoven's Music” (1942). Reprint in The

Musical Quarterly 77 (Spring, 1993): 90-131.

Page 579: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

558

Kosztolanyi, Desider. “Béla Bartok, Hungarian Composer.” The Living Age, Aug. 1931,

565.

Kotzschmar, Mary Ann Torrey. Half-hour Lessons in Music. Boston: Oliver Ditson Co.,

1907.

Krehbiel, Henry Edward. Music and Manners in the Classical Period: Essays. Second

Edition. New York: Scribner & Sons, 1898.

Kufferath, M. “Rhythm, Melody, and Harmony.” Music XVII (Nov. 1899-Apr. 1900):

31-39.

Kuhn, Thomas S. “The Function of Measurement.” Isis 52 (1961): 161-193.

______. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Third Edition. Chicago: University of

Chicago Press, 1996.

Kullak, Franz. Beethoven’s Piano-Playing. Translated by Theodore Baker. New York: G.

Schirmer, 1901.

Landes, David S. Revolution in Time: Clocks and the Making of the Modern World.

Second Edition. Cambridge: Harvard U. Press, 2000.

______. “The Wilkins Lecture, 1988: Hand and Mind in Time Measurement: The

Contributions of Art and Science.” In Notes and Records of the Royal Society of

London 43 (January 1989): 57-69.

Langfeld, Herbert Sidney, and Floyd Henry Allport. An Elementary Laboratory Course

in Psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1916.

Lardner, Dionysius. Handbook of Astronomy. Third edition, revised and edited by Edwin

Dunkin. London: James Walton, 1867.

Lardner, Dionysius, ed. The Museum of Science and Art. Vol. III. London: Walton and

Maberly, 1859.

Lawrence, Vera Brodsky. Strong on Music. Volume I. Oxford: Oxford University Press,

1988.

Leonard, John William ed. Women’s Who’s Who of America, 1914-1915. New York: The

American Commonwealth Company, 1914.

Levick, M. B. “Stravinsky Sees Vision of a New Music.” New York Times, Jan. 18, 1925,

SM12.

Lichtner, William. Time Study and Job Analysis as Applied to Standardization of

Methods and Operations. New York: The Roland Press Company, 1921.

Page 580: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

559

Liddell, H. S. “A Laboratory for the Study of Conditioned Motor Reflexes.” The

American Journal of Psychology 37 (Jul., 1926): 418-419.

Liebling, Emil. “A Pianistic Retrospect.” Music I (Nov 1891-Apr. 1892): 583-586.

Lindberg, David C., and Robert S. Westman, ed. Reappraisals of the Scientific

Revolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990.

Lippman, Walter. Public Opinion. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company, 1922.

Liszt, Franz. Letters of Franz Liszt. Collected and edited by La Mara. Translated by

Constance Bache. Volume II. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, 1894.

Lloyd, H. Alan. "Mechanical Timekeepers." In A History of Technology, Chapter 24.

Edited by Charles Singer. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1954.

Lockyer, J. Norman. Stargazing Past and Present. London: Macmillan and Co., 1878.

Logier, Johann Bernhard and John Green. Logier's system of the science of music,

harmony, and practical composition. New edition. [London]: Boosey and Co.,

[182?].

“London Symphony Orchestra: A Schubert Evening.” The Times, Tuesday, Mar. 25,

1930, 14.

Loucks, Roger Brown. “An Automatic Technique for Establishing Conditioned

Reflexes.” The American Journal of Psychology 44 (Apr., 1932): 338-343.

Lubbock, Sir John. On Senses, Instincts, and Intelligence of Animals. Second Edition.

London: Kegan Paul, Trench & Co.,1889.

Lussy, Mathis. Grammaire de l'exécution musicale: l'anacrouse dans la musique

moderne. Paris: Librairie Fischbacher, 1903.

______. Traité de l'expression musicale. Deuxième Édition. Paris: Heugel et Cie., 1874.

______. Le rythme musical: son origine, sa fonction et son accentuation. Paris: Librairie

Fischbacher, 1883.

Lyman, Frederic A. The Normal Music in the Schoolroom. New York: Silver, Burdett

and Co., 1896.

“M. Dalcroze’s ‘Eurhythmics.’” Times, November 13, 1913, 10.

Page 581: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

560

MacClintock, Carol. Readings in the History of Music Performance. Bloomington:

Indiana University Press, 1979.

MacDonald, Arthur. Experimental Study of Children, Including Anthropometrical and

Psycho-Physical Measurements of Washington School Children, a Bibliography.

Washington: Government Printing Office, 1899.

Macey, Samuel L., ed. Encyclopedia of time. New York: Garland, 1994.

Maelzel, Johann Nepomuk. Kurze Abhandlung uber den Metronomen von Maezel und

dessen Anwendung als Tempobezeichnung, sowohl als bei dem Unterricht in der

Musik. Mainz, 1836.

Marshall, Edward. “Scientific Child Study.” Frank Leslie's Popular Monthly 51 (Mar.

1901): 2-13. APS Online database.

http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=220098461&sid=5&Fmt=2&clientId=3338

&RQT=309&VName=HNP.

Martin, H. Newell. “Respiration of the Frog.” The Journal of Physiology 1 (1878-9): 131-

170.

______. “Respiration of the Frog.” In Studies from the Biological Laboratory, Session

1877-1888. Volume I, 131-170. Baltimore: Johns Murphy & Co. University,

1879.

Martin, John. “The Dance: Listening In.” New York Times, Mar. 31, 1929, 108.

Marx, Adolf Bernhard. General Musical Instruction (Allgemeine Musiklehre). Translated

by George Macirone. London: J. Alfred Novello, 1854.

______. Introduction to the Interpretation of Beethoven Piano Works. Translated by

Fannie Louise Gwinner. Chicago: Clayton F. Summy Co., 1895.

______. The Music of the Nineteenth Century and Its Culture. London: Robert Cocks,

and Co., 1854.

______. Universal School of Music. Translated from the Fifth Edition of the Original

German by A. H. Wehrhan. London: Robert Cocks and Co., 1853.

Mateer, Florence Edna. Child Behavior; a Critical and Experimental Study of Young

Children by the Method of Conditioned Reflexes. Boston: Richard G. Badger,

1920.

Mathews, William Smythe Babcock. How to Understand Music: A Concise Course in

Musical Intelligence and Taste. Chicago: Lyon & Healy, 1881.

Page 582: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

561

______. Music Its Ideals and Methods. Philadelphia: Theodore Presser, 1897.

______. “The Practical Teacher. Rubato.” Music VIII (May-October 1895): 324-5.

______. “Schumann, the Poet of the Pianoforte.” Music V (November 1893- April 1894):

698-721.

Matthew, James E. The Literature of Music. London: Elliot Stock, 1896.

Maunder, E. Walter. The Royal Observatory, Greenwich. London: The Religious Tract

Society, 1900.

Maurice, Klaus. Die Franzoesische Pendule Des 18.Jahrhunderts…Berlin: Gruyter &

Co., 1967.

Maxham, Robert Eugene. "The Contributions of Joseph Sauveur (1653-1716) to

Acoustics." PhD diss., Eastman School of Music, 1976.

May, Thomas Robert. The John Adams Reader: Essential Writings on an American

Composer. Pompton Plains, NJ: Amadeus Press, 2006.

McChesney, Horace B. “Apparatus for Testing the Aptitude of Keyboard Operators.”

U.S. Patent #1,688,935. Filed Sept. 27, 1926. Patented Oct. 23, 1928.

McEwen, John. The Thought in Music: An Enquiry into the Principles of Musical

Rhythm, Phrasing, and Expression. London: MacMillan and Co., 1912.

McKendrick, John Gray. A Text Book of Physiology. Glasgow: James Maclehose & Sons,

1888.

McLaughlin, James M. Elements & Notation of Music. Boston: Ginn & Company, 1902.

“Metronomes.” The Musical Visitor 26, Dec. 1, 1897, 335.

Metzner, Paul. Crescendo of the Virtuoso. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1998.

Meumann, Ernst. The Psychology of Learning. Translated by John Wallace Baird. New

York: D. Appleton and Co., 1913.

______. “Untersuchungen zur psychologie und aesthetik des rhythmus.” PhD diss.,

University of Leipzig, 1894.

Meyer, Max. “Experimental Studies in the Psychology of Music.” The American Journal

of Psychology 14 (Jul.-Oct., 1903): 192-214.

Page 583: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

562

M’Gregor-Robertson, J. The Elements of Physiological Physics: An Outline of the

Elementary Facts. London: Cassell & Company, 1884.

Miehling, Klaus. Das Tempo in der Musik von Barock und Vorklassik. Wilhemshaven:

Floian Noetzel Verlag, 1993.

Miller, Neil. The Piano Lessons Book: The Piano Student's Guide for Getting the Most

Out of Practicing, Lessons, Your Teacher and Yourself. [N.P.]: CreateSpace,

2007.

Millikan, Robert Andrews and Henry Gordon Gale. A First Course on Physics. Boston:

Ginn & Company, 1906.

Miner, James Burt. “Motor, Visual, and Applied Rhythms.” PhD diss., Columbia

University, 1903.

Mocquereau, André. Le Nombre Musical Grégorien. Tome 1. Rome: Société de Saint

Jean L’Évangéliste, 1908.

“‘Modern’ Piano Pieces.” Times, June 23, 1922, 7F.

Moeller, Joseph, and Hermann Thoms. Real-Enzyklopädie der Gesamten Pharmazie.

Volume VIII. Leipsig: Urban & Schwarzenberg, 1907.

Moore, Aubertine Woodward. For My Musical Friend. New York: Dodge Publishing

Co., 1900.

Mumford, Lewis. Technics and Civilization. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1934.

Münsterberg, Margarete Anna Adelheid. Hugo Münsterberg. New York: D. Appleton

and Company, 1922.

“Music.” The Albion, a Journal of News, Politics, and Literature, Mar. 8, 1856, 115.

“Musical Customs and Practices.” Saturday Evening Post, Sept. 28, 1861, 4.

“Musical Review.” Harbinger, Devoted to Social and Political Progress, Feb. 27, 1847,

185.

Myers, Charles S. “Anthropological Reviews and Miscellanea.” The Journal of the

Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 29 (1899): 222-223.

______. Mind and Work: The Psychological Factors in Industry and Commerce. New

York: Putnam's Sons, 1921.

Page 584: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

563

______. A Text-book of Experimental Psychology. New York: Longmans, Green & Co.,

1909.

“Nature and Science.” The Youth's Companion, Jun. 11, 1896, 307. APS Online.

http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=841142492&sid=2&Fmt=2&clientId=3338

&RQT=309&VName=HNP.

Naumburg, Margaret. “The Dalcroze Idea, What Eurhythmics is and What it Means.”

Outlook, Jan. 17, 1914, 127-131.

Newall, H. F. “Total Solar Eclipse of 1901 May 17-18.” Monthly Notices of the Royal

Astronomical Society LXII (London: Royal Astronomical Society, 1902): 209-

234.

Newhall, Sidney M. “The Control of Eyelid Movements in Visual Experiments.” The

American Journal of Psychology 44 (Jul., 1932): 555-570.

Newman, Ernest. A Musical Motley. New York: John Lane Company, 1919.

Newman, William. Beethoven on Beethoven: Playing His Piano Music His Way. New

York:

W.W. Norton & Co., 1988.

[“Notices.”] The Shorthand Writer V (Jan., 1886): 14.

Nugent, Frank S. “Sound and Fury in Remote Astoria.” New York Times, Dec. 30, 1934,

X5.

Ott, Isaac. The Action of Medicines. Philadelphia: Lindsay & Blakiston, 1878.

Ott, J. “Original Communications. Physiological Action of Gelsemia.” Philadelphia

Medical Times, Jul. 31, 1875, 689-693. American Periodical Series Online.

<http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=726640772&sid=1&Fmt=2&clientId=333

8&RQT=309&VName=HNP>

Parker, Annie. “Automata.” Ladies’ Wreath, a Magazine devoted to Literature Industry

and Religion, Oct. 5, 1855, 360.

Parker, John R. A Musical Biography. Boston: Stone & Fovell, 1825.

Parsons, Albert Ross. The Science of Pianoforte Practice. New York: G. Schirmer, 1886.

Partsch-Bergsohn, Isa. Modern Dance in Germany and the United States. Switzerland:

Harwood Academic Publishers, 1994.

Page 585: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

564

Patterson, William Morrison. The Rhythm of Prose, An Experimental Investigation of

Individual Difference in the Sense of Rhythm. New York: Columbia University

Press, 1916.

Pavlov, I. P. “Croonian Lecture: Certain Problems in the Psychology of the Cerebral

Hemispheres.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Containing

Papers of a Biological Character 103 (June 1, 1928): 97-110.

Pennington, Jo. The Importance of Being Rhythmic; a Study of the Principles of Dalcroze

Eurhythmics Applied to General Education and to the Arts of Music, Dancing and

Acting. Based on and adapted from "Rhythm, music and education," by Émile

Jaques-Dalcroze. With an introduction by Walter Damrosch. New York and

London: G.P. Putnam, 1925.

Pepper, John Henry. Cycolpaedic Science Simplified. London: Frederick Warne and Co.,

1869.

Perry, Edward Baxter. Descriptive Analyses of Piano Works. Philadelphia: Theo. Presser

Co., 1902.

______. “The Other Side.” Music V (November 1893- April 1894): 207-214.

______. “Rubato Playing,” The Musical Visitor, Aug. 1897, 202.

______. Stories of Standard Teaching Pieces. Philadelphia: Theo. Presser Co., 1910.

Perry, Edward Baxter, et al. “The Pedagogical Aspects of the Practice Clavier.” Music VI

(May-Oct., 1894): 323-336.

Philharmonicus. “An Explanation of the Notation Employed in the Scale of Maelzel’s

Metronome.” The Edinburgh Magazine and Literary Miscellany I (October,

1817): 223-4.

Philip, Robert. Performing Music in the Age of Recording. New Haven: Yale University

Press, 2004.

Pickering, Edward C. Elements of Physical Manipulation. New York: Hurd and

Houghton, 1873.

Poe, Edgar Allen. “Maelzel’s Chess Player.” Southern Literary Messenger II (1836):

318-326.

Porter, Theodore. Trust in Numbers: the Pursuit of Objectivity in Science and Public Life.

Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1995.

Page 586: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

565

Pound, Ezra. Antheil and the Treatise on Harmony. Reprint. New York: Da Capo Press,

1968.

Prunieres, Henry. “The Younger Composers of France; Esthetic Tendencies of

Generation.” New York Times, Aug. 29, 1926, X7.

“Psychological Literature” [Book Review]. American Journal of Psychology 2 (May,

1889): 507-508.

Pyle, William Henry. The Outlines of Educational Psychology: An Introduction to the

Science of Education. Baltimore: Warkick & York, Inc 1911.

R., S. “‘Company’ Playing.” The Monthly Packet of Evening Readings for Members of

the English Church XVI (July-December, 1873): 200-205.

Ranking, W. H., and C. B. Radcliffe, eds. Half-Yearly Abstract of the Medical Sciences

XXVII (Jan.-June, 1858): 73-74.

Raymond, George Lansing. Rhythm and harmony in poetry and music. Second edition

revised. New York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons, 1909.

The Repertory of Arts, Manufacturers, and Agriculture. Vol. XXVIII, Second Series.

London: J. Wyatt, 1816.

“Report on Physiology.” British and Foreign Medico-chirurgical Review LXV (Jan.,

1864): 232-237.

“Rhythm and tempo rubato.” The Times, April 16, 1910, 13.

“Rhythm in Education.” New York Times, Dec 1, 1912, C4.

Ribot, Théodule. German Psychology of To-day: The Empirical School. Translated by

James Mark Baldwin. New York: Charles Scribner’s and Sons, 1886.

Richards, E. G. Mapping Time. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1998.

Richardson, Benjamin W. “Researches on the Treatment of Suspended Animation.” The

British and Foreign Medico-chirurgical Review XXXI (January-April, 1863):

478-505.

Riemann, Hugo. Musikalische Dynamik und Agogik. Hamburg: D. Rahter, 1884.

Rigg, James Burlington. “Means for and Method of Regulating Breathing…” U.S. Patent

#1,354,212. Filed Feb. 9, 1920. Patented Sept. 28, 1920.

Page 587: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

566

Ritchie, Frank. “Music and Words.” The Living Age, Aug. 26, 1899, 558-563. APS

Online.

http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=511051102&sid=7&Fmt=2&clientId=3338

&RQT=309&VName=HNP.

Robbins, Arthur G. “The Eclipse Expedition of the MIT to Washington, Georgia.”

Technology Quarterly and Proceedings of the Society of Arts XIII (Sept., 1900):

171-173.

Roe, Richard. The Principles of Rhythm both in Speech and Music. Dublin: R.

Graisberry, 1823.

Rosenfeld, Paul. Modern Tendencies in Music. New York: The Caxton Institute, 1927.

______. “Musical Chronicle.” The Dial; a Semi-monthly Journal of Literary Criticism,

May 1927, 445. American Periodicals Series Online.

______. Musical Portraits. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Howe, 1920.

Rossum, Gerhard Dohrn-van. History of the Hour: Clocks and Modern Orders. Chicago:

The University of Chicago Press, 1992.

Rougnon, Paul. Principes de la musique (entretiens théoriques développés). In

Encyclopédie de la musique et dictionnaire du Conservatoire, [183]-363. Paris: C.

Delagrave, 1925.

______. Le rythme & la mesure: Traité complet théorique, analytique & practique. Paris:

Enoch, [1920-1929?]

Ruckmick, Christian A. “A Bibliography of Rhythm.” The American Journal of

Psychology 26 (Jul., 1915): 457-459. ______. “A Bibliography of ‘Rhythm’ (Second Supplementary List).” The American

Journal of Psychology 29 (Apr., 1918): 214-218.

______. “A Bibliography of ‘Rhythm’ (Third Supplementary List).” The American

Journal of Psychology 35 (Jul., 1924): 407-413.

______. “The Rhythmical Experience from the Systematic Point of View.” The American

Journal of Psychology 39 (Dec., 1927): 356-366. ______. “The Role of Kinaesthesis in the Perception of Rhythm.” The American Journal

of Psychology 24 (Jul., 1913): 305-359.

Rusk, Robert Robertson. Experimental Education. London: Longmans, Green and Co.,

1919.

Page 588: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

567

Sabaneev, Leonid, and S. W. Pring. “The Stravinsky Legends.” The Musical Times 69

(Sep. 1, 1928): 785-787.

Sachs, Curt. Rhythm and Tempo. New York: W.W. Norton & Co., 1953.

______. “Rhythm and Tempo: An Introduction.” The Musical Quarterly 38 (Jul., 1952):

384-398.

Saint-Saëns, Camille. “The Ideas of M. Vincent D’Indy.” The Musical Times 61 (Apr. 1,

1920): 244-246.

______. “Letter 4—No Title.” Century Illustrated Magazine, Nov. 1891, 156.

______. “The Metronome.” In Outspoken Essays on Music, 122-4. Trans. Fred Rothwell.

London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co., 1922.

______. “A Note on Rameau.” In Outspoken Essays on Music, 89-96. Trans. Fred

Rothwell. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co., 1922.

Sanford, E. C. “Personal Equation.” The American Journal of Psychology 2 (November,

1889): 3-38.

Saygun, A. Adnan. “Bartok in Turkey.” The Musical Quarterly 37 (Jan., 1951): 5-9.

Schauffler, Robert Haven. The Musical Amateur, A Book on the Human Side of Music.

Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1911.

Scherchen, Hermann. The Nature of Music. Translated by William Mann. London: Denis

Dobson Ltd., 1950.

Schindler, Anton. The Life of Beethoven. Edited by Ignace Moscheles. London: Henry

Colburn, 1841.

“Schindler’s life of Beethoven, edited by Moscheles.” The Times, April 10, 1841, 6.

de Schloezer, Boris. “A Classic Art.” The Dial; a Semi-monthly Journal of Literary

Criticism, Discussion, and Information, Jul. 1929, 597. American Periodicals

Series Online.

Schmidt, F. A. and Eustace Hamilton Miles. The Training of the Body for Games,

Athletics, Gymnastics, and Other Forms of Exercise and for Health, Growth, and

Development. New York: E. P. Dutton & Co., 1901.

Schwandt, Erich. “L'Affilard on the French Court Dances.” The Musical Quarterly 60

(1974): 389-400.

Page 589: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

568

______. “L'Affilard's Published ‘Sketchbooks.’” The Musical Quarterly 63 (January

1977): 99-113.

______. “The principles of L'Affilard.” Early music VIII/1 (1980): 77-81.

Scripture, E. W. “Cross-Education.” Appletons’ Popular Science Monthly LVI (March,

1900): 589-596.

______. “Measuring Hallucinations.” Science 3 (May 22, 1896): 762-3.

______. The New Psychology. London: The Walter Scott Publishing Co., 1907.

______. “Observations of Rhythmic Action,” Science 10 (Dec. 1, 1899): 801-811.

______. “Some Principles of Mental Education.” The School Review 3 (Nov., 1895): 533-

548.

______. Thinking, Feeling, Doing. Meadville: The Chautauqua-Century Press, 1895.

Sears, Charles H. “A Contribution to the Psychology of Rhythm.” The American Journal

of Psychology 13 (Jan., 1902): 28-61.

______. “Studies in Rhythm.” PhD diss., Clark University, 1902.

Sears, Robert. The Wonders of the World. Second Series. New York: Robert Sears, 1856.

Seashore, Carl Emil. “Artistic Deviation as an Esthetic Principle in Music.” The Scientific

Monthly 54 (Feb., 1942): 99-109.

______. Elementary Experiments in Psychology. New York: Henry Holt and Company,

1908.

______. Manual of Instructions and Interpretations for Measures of Musical Talent. New

York: Columbia Phonograph Company, [1919].

______. “The Measure of a Singer.” Science 35 (Feb. 9, 1912): 201-212.

______. “The Measurement of Musical Talent.” The Musical Quarterly 1 (Jan., 1915):

129-148.

______. “Measurements on the Expression of Emotion in Music.” Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 9 (Sep. 15, 1923):

323-325

______. “Psychology of Music.” Music Educators Journal 22 (Mar., 1936): 24-25.

Page 590: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

569

______. “The Psychology of Music. II. Approaches to the Experimental Psychology of

Music.” Music Educators Journal 22 (May, 1936): 22-23.

______. “Psychology of Music. XXVII. How Do We Express Specific Emotions in

Song?” Music Educators Journal 27 (Sep., 1940): 38-39, 42.

______. The Psychology of Musical Talent. Boston: Silver, Burdett, and Company, 1919.

______. “Scientific Procedure in the Discovery of Musical Talent in the Public Schools.”

Music Supervisor’s Journal 2 (Jan, 1916): 10-11.

______. “The Sense of Rhythm as a Musical Talent.” The Musical Quarterly 4 (Oct.,

1918): 507-515.

Seashore, Carl Emil, and Milton Metfessel. “Deviation from the Regular as an Art

Principle.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States

of America 11 (Sep. 15, 1925): 538-542.

Segsworth, A. E. “On the Difference Sensibility for the Valuation of Space Distances

with the Help of Arm Movements.” The American Journal of Psychology 6 (Jun.,

1894): 369-407.

Semmens, Richard. Joseph Sauveur's Treatise on the theory of music. London, Ontario:

U. Western Ontario, 1987.

Senner, Wayne M., ed. The Critical Reception of Beethoven’s Compositions by His

German Contemporaries. Volume 1. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press,

1999.

Sharp, Stella Emily. “Individual Psychology: A Study in Psychological Method.” The

American Journal of Psychology 10 (Apr., 1899): 329-391.

Shaw, Albert, ed. “Pulse and Rhythm.” The American Monthly Review of Reviews

XXVIII (July-Dec., 1903): 487-488.

Shaw, M. A., and F. S Wrinch. “A contribution to the psychology of time.” In University

of Toronto Studies, Psychological Series I, 101-153. Toronto: University Library,

1899.

Shedlock, Emma L. A Trip to Music-land. London: Blackie & Son, 1876.

Sherman, Stuart. Telling Time: Clocks, Diaries, and English Diurnal Form 1660-1785.

Chicago: Universiry of Chicago Press, 1996.

Sherover, Charles M. ed. The Human Experience of Time, the Development of its

Philosophic Meaning. Reprint. Evanston: Northwestern University Press, 2001.

Page 591: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

570

Slim, H. Colin. “Lessons with Stravinsky: The Notebook of Earnest Andersson (1879-

1940).” Journal of the American Musicological Society 62 (Summer, 2009): 323-

412.

Sloane, T. O’Conor. Standard Electrical Dictionary. Second Edition. New York: Normal

W. Henley & Co., 1897.

Smedley, Menella Bute. “The Maiden Aunt—No. V.” Sharpe’s London Magazine, Dec.

4, 1847, 92.

Smith, Grace. “The Eurhythmics of Jaques-Dalcroze.” Journal of Proceedings and

Addresses, National Education Association (August 16-27, 1915): 869-872.

Smith, Lyman D. “Writing in the Lower Grades.” The American Teacher IX (June,

1892): 393-394.

Smith, Margret Keiver. Rhythmus und Arbeit. Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann, 1900.

Smith, Theodate L. “On Muscular Memory.” The American Journal of Psychology 7

(July, 1896): 453-490.

Smith, W. G. “The Relation of Attention to Memory.” Mind 4 (Jan., 1895): 47-73.

Somfai, László. Béla Bartók: Composition, Concepts, and Autograph Sources. Volume 9

of Ernest Bloch lectures. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California

Press, 1996.

Sonnenschein, E[dward] A[dolf]. What is Rhythm? An Essay: Accompanied by an

appendix on experimental syllable-measurement in which Stephen Jones and

Eileen Macleod have co-operated. Oxford: B. Blackwell, 1925.

Spearman, C. “‘General Intelligence’ Objectively Determined and Measured.” The

American Journal of Psychology 15 (Apr., 1904): 201-292.

“Special Article. Byways of Medical Literature.—XVII. Pulse Beats and Musical

Rhythm.” Medical News, Sept. 26, 1903, 622.

Squire, Carrie Ransom. “A Genetic Study of Rhythm.” PhD diss., Cornell University,

1901.

Staley, S. C., and D. M. Lowery. Manual of Gymnastic Dancing. New York: Association

Press, 1920.

Stephens, Carlene. On Time. Boston: Bullfinch Press, 2002.

Page 592: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

571

Sternberg, Constantin von. Ethics and Esthetics of Piano-Playing. New York: G.

Schirmer, 1917.

Stevens, H. C. “A Plethysmographic Study of Attention.” The American Journal of

Psychology 16 (Oct., 1905): 409-483.

Stevens, Lewis T. “On The Time-Sense.” Mind 11 (Jul., 1886): 393-404.

Stoddart, J. M. Stoddart’s Encyclopedia Americana. Volume II. New York: J. M

Stoddart, 1884.

Stravinsky, Igor. Poetics of Music in the Form of Six Lessons. Translated by Arthur

Knodel & Ingolf Dahl. New York: Vintage Books, 1956.

______. Selected Correspondence. Edited by Robert Craft. Three Volumes.

Stravinsky, Vera, and Robert Craft. Stravinsky in Pictures and Documents. New York:

Simon and Schuster, 1978.

Stumpf, Carl. Tonpsychologie. Erster Band. Leipzig: Verlag von S. Hirzel, 1883.

“Substitute for Maelzel’s Metronome.” The Musical Magazine; or, Repository of Musical

Science, Literature...Feb. 29, 1840, 67.

Suchoff, Benjamin. “Béla Bartók's Contributions to Music Education.” Journal of

Research in Music Education 9 (Spring, 1961): 3-9.

Swift, Edgar James. “Disturbance of the Attention during Simple Mental Processes,” The

American Journal of Psychology 5 (Oct., 1892): 1-19.

Swindle, P. F. “On the Inheritance of Rhythm.” The American Journal of Psychology 24

(Apr., 1913): 180-203.

Swope, Robert. “Resuscitation and Artificial Respiration Apparatus.” U.S. Patent

#1848232. Filed Sept. 29, 1927. Patented Mar. 8, 1932.

Taruskin, Richard. Text and Act: Essays on Music and Performance. Oxford: Oxford

University Press, 1995.

Taylor, Frederick Winslow. The Principles of Scientific Management. New York: Harper

& Brothers, 1911. Reprint. Mineola: Dover, 1998.

______. Scientific Management. Reprint. Westport: Greenwood Press, 1972.

______. Shop Management. New York: Harper & Brothers, 1912.

Page 593: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

572

Taylor, John. The Student’s Text-book of the Science of Music. London: George Philip

and Son, 1876.

Tebbutt, John. “Equatorial Comparison of Uranus…” Monthly Notices of the Royal

Astronomical Society LVIII (Nov.1897-Nov.1898): 21-24.

Terman, Lewis M. Research in Mental Deviation Among Children. Stanford: Stanford

University, 1915.

Thayer, Alexander Wheelock. The Life of Ludwig van Beethoven. Edited, revised and

amended from the original English manuscript and the German editions of

Hermann Deiters and Hugo Riemann, concluded, and all the documents newly

translated, by Henry Edward Krehbiel. Vol. 2. New York: The Beethoven

Association [1921].

“The Theory of Practicing.” The Youth's Companion, Feb. 1, 1912, 66

Thomson, Thomas, Daniel Keyte Sandford, and Allan Cunningham. The Popular

Encyclopedia. Volume 1. Glasgow: Blackie & Son, 1836.

Thompson, E. P. “Time, Work-Discipline, and Industrial Capitalism.” Past and Present

38 (Dec., 1967): 56-97. “Three Impressions of Theodore Thomas.” Outlook, Feb. 4, 1905, 316.

Tiersch, Otto. Rhythmik, Dynamik und Phrasierungslehre der homophonen Musik.

Berlin: Robert Oppenheim, 1886.

Timbs, John. A Year-book of Facts in Science and Art. London: David Bogue, 1856.

______. Stories of Inventors and Discoverers in Science and the Useful Arts. London:

Kent and Co., 1860.

“Time and Clocks.” New York Times, June 27, 1920, 60.

Titchener, Edward Bradford. A Beginner’s Psychology. New York: The Macmillan

Company, 1916.

______. “The Equipment of a Psychological Laboratory.” The American Journal of

Psychology 11 (Jan., 1900): 251-265.

______. Experimental Psychology: A Manual of Laboratory Practice. Volume 1. New

York: The Macmillan Company, 1901.

______. Lectures on the Experimental Psychology of the Thought-Processes. New York:

The Macmillan Company, 1909.

Page 594: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

573

______. An Outline of Psychology. New York: The Macmillan Company, 1902.

______. A Text-book of Psychology. New York: The Macmillan Company, 1916.

Tousey, Sinclair. Medical Electricity, Röntgen Rays, and Radium. Philadelphia: W. B.

Saunders Company, 1921.

Tozer, Samuel. “Cheap Metronomes or Time-Beaters.” Mechanics’ Magazine XX no.

544 (Jan. 11, 1834): 255.

Trabue, M. R. and Frank Parker Stockbridge. Measure Your Mind: The Mentimeter and

how to Use it. New York: Doubleday, Page & Company, 1920.

Trousseau, Armande. Lectures on Clinical Medicine. Volume I. Translated by P. V.

Bazire. London: Robert Hardwicke, 1867.

Tufts, John. The Cecilian Series of Study and Song. Book IV. New York: Silver, Burdett

and Co., 1892.

______. A Handbook of Vocal Music. New York: Silver, Burdett and Co., 1896.

U.S. Congress. Register of Debates in Congress. Volume XIV. Washington: Gales and

Seaton, 1837.

U.S. War Department. Annual Reports. Washington: Government Printing Office, 1900.

______. Medical Research Laboratory. Washington D. C.: Government Printing Office,

1918.

University of Calcutta. Calendar. Calcutta: Thacker, Sprink & Co., 1908.

Ufer, Christian, editor. Die Anfänge des kindlichen Seelenlebens. Langensalza: Hermann

Beyer & Söhne, 1894.

Urban, F. M. The Application of Statistical Methods to the Problems of Psychophysics.

Philadelphia: The Psychological Clinic Press, 1908.

Urban, Sylvanus. “Review of New Musical Publications.” The Gentleman’s Magazine,

Mar. 1816, 251.

Ure, Andrew. The Philosophy of Manufactures: or An Exposition of the Scientific, Moral,

and Commercial Economy of the Factory System of Great Britain. London:

Charles Knight, 1835.

Vechten, Carl Van. In the Garret. New York: Alfred A Knopf, 1920.

Page 595: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

574

Verrier, Paul. Essai sur les principles de la metrique anglaise. Three Volumes. Paris: H.

Welter, 1909-1919.

______. “Les variations temporelles du rythme.” Journal de Psy. Norm. et Path. I (1913):

18-[?].

Vierordt, Karl. Der Zeitsinn nach versuchen. Tübingen: H. Laupp’schen Buchhandlung,

1868.

Wade, L. T. The Quincy Course of Study in Music. Boston: Silver, Burdett, & Co., 1889.

Wagner, Richard. On Conducting (Über das Dirigiren). Trans. by Edward Dannreuther.

London: William Reeves, 1897.

Wagner, Richard, and Franz Liszt. Correspondence of Wagner and Liszt. Translated by

Francis Hueffer. New Edition Revised by W. Ashton Ellis. Volumes I and II.

London: H. Grevel and Co., 1897.

Walker, Ernest. Free Thought and the Musician. London: Oxford University Press, 1946.

Wallin, J. E. Wallace. Researches on the rhythm of speech. In series Studies from the Yale

Psychological Laboratory. Volume IX. New Haven: Yale University, 1901.

Warden, C. J. and L. H. Warner. “The Sensory Capacities and Intelligence of Dogs, with

a Report on the Ability of the Noted Dog ‘Fellow’ to Respond to Verbal Stimuli.”

The Quarterly Review of Biology 3 (Mar., 1928): 1-28.

Warner, James F., and Gottfried Weber. A Universal Dictionary of Musical Terms.

Boston: J. H. Wilkins & R. B. Carter, 1842.

Warner, Susan (aka Elizabeth Wetherell). Queechy (c1840). Gutenberg eBook #8874.

http://www.gutenberg.org.

Watson, J. Madison. Hand-book of Calisthenics and Gymnastics: A Complete Drill-Book

for Schools, Families, and Gymnasiums. New York: Schermerhorn, Bancroft &

Co., 1864.

Watts, Frank. An Introduction to the Psychological Problems of Industry. New York: The

Macmillan Company, 1921.

Webb, Hanor A. “Starting the Small Physics Laboratory.” Peabody Journal of Education

1 (Sept., 1923): 86-94.

Weber, C. M. von. “Letter By C. M. Von Weber on the Performance of Dramatic Song.”

The Harmonicon V, part 1 (1827): 219-220.

Page 596: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

575

Weber, Gottfried. General Music Teacher: Adapted to Self-instruction, Both for Teachers

and Learners; Embracing Also an Extensive Dictionary of Musical Terms. Trans.

by James Franklin Warner. Boston: J. H. Wilkins & R.B. Carter, 1841.

______. Theory of Musical Composition. Translated from the third German edition by

James F. Warner. Edited by John Bishop. Volume 1. London: Robert Cocks and

Co., 1851.

Weber, Johannès. Les illusions musicales. Paris: Librairie Fischbacher, 1883.

Webster, Daniel. “Technical Progress and Prosperity.” In The Writings and Speeches of

Daniel Webster Uncollected I, 63–78. Boston, 1903. Annals of American History.

http://america.eb.com/america/article?articleId=385768&query=maelzel.

Accessed August 3, 2007.

Weimar, Gottfried. Über Choralrhythmus. Giessen: J. Ricker’sche Verlagsbuchhandlung,

1899.

Weitzman, Carl Friederich, and E. M. Bowman. Bowman’s-Weitzman’s Manual of Music

Theory a Concise, Comprehensive and Practical Text-Book on the Science of

Music. New York: Wm A. Pond & Co., 1897.

Weld, Harry Porter. “An Experimental Study of Musical Enjoyment.” The American

Journal of Psychology 23 (Apr., 1912): 245-308.

Westphal, Rudolf Georg Hermann. Allgemeine theorie der musikalischen rhythmik seit J.

S. Bach. Leipzig: Breitkoph & Härtel, 1880.

Whipple, Guy Montrose. Manual of Mental and Physical Tests. Baltimore: Warwick &

York, Inc., 1914.

White, Charles. “Metronome.” U.S. Patent #923,094. Filed Oct. 20, 1908. Patented May

25, 1909.

Whiting, Sarah Frances. Daytime and Evening Exercises in Astronomy, for Schools and

Colleges. Boston: Ginn and Company, 1912.

Whitmer, T. Carl. Indicating the Whitmer Methods for the Development of Individuality

in Piano, Musical Composition and Appreciation. Pittsburgh: Siviter & Co., 1917.

______. “The Time-Marking System in Music.” Music XVIII (June, 1900): 164-173.

______. The Way of My Mind. Pittsburgh: Pierpont, Siviter & Co., 1917.

Whitrow, G. J. Time in History. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1988.

Page 597: Bonus, Alexander Evan__The Metronomic Performance Practice - A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, And the Mechanization of Musicality

576

Wiehmayer, Theodor. Musikalische Rhythmik und Metrik. Magdeburg: Heinrichshofen’s

Verlag, 1917.

Willis, Richard Storrs. “On Musical Tempo.” Musical World and New York Musical

Times, June 11, 1853, 82.

Willis, Robert. An attempt to analyse the automaton chess player, of Mr. de Kempelen.

London: Booth, 1821.

Wilson, Robert Forrest. “Paris for Young Art.” The Bookman; a Review of Books and

Life (Jun. 1925): 403-412.

Wise, M. Norton, ed. The Values of Precision. Princeton: Princeton University Press,

1995.

Wodell, F. W. Choir and Chorus Conducting. Philadelphia: Theodore Presser, 1909.

Woodrow, Herbert. A Quantitative Study of Rhythm, The Effect of Variations in Intensity,

Rate, and Duration. New York: The Science Press, 1909.

Wright, Lawrence. Clockwork Man, the Story of Time, its Origins, its Uses, its Tyranny.

Reprint. New York: Marboro Books Corp., 1992.

Wundt, Wilhelm Max. Ethics: An Investigation of the Facts and Laws of the Moral Life.

Translated from the Second German Edition (1892) by Titchener, Gulliver, and

Washburn. Vol. 1. London: Swan Sonnenschein & Co., Lim., 1902.

______. Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology. Translated from the second

German edition by J. E. Creighton and E. B. Titchener. London: Swan

Sonnenschein & Co., 1907.

______. Outlines of Psychology. Second revised edition from the fourth revised German

edition. Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann, 1902.

Young, Bicknell. “Rhythm in Singing.” Music XIX, no. 6 (April 1901): 567- 571.

Zeckwer, Richard. “Metronome.” U.S. Patent #360,550. Filed Dec. 22, 1886. Patented

Apr. 5, 1887.

Zorn, Friedrich Albert. Grammar of the Art of Dancing. Edited by Alfonso Josephs

Sheafe. Boston: Heintzemann Press, 1905.