2822.full

Upload: duane-brooks

Post on 13-Apr-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/26/2019 2822.full

    1/9

    BRAINA JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY

    Chronic temporal lobe epilepsy:a neurodevelopmental or progressivelydementing disease?C. Helmstaedter and C. E. Elger

    Department of Epileptology, University Hospital of Bonn, Bonn, Germany

    Correspondence to: Prof. Dr Christoph Helmstaedter,

    Neuropsychology,

    Department of Epileptology,

    University Hospital of Bonn,Sigmund Freud Str. 25, 53105 Bonn,

    Germany

    E-mail: [email protected]

    To what degree does the so-called initial hit of the brain versus chronic epilepsy contribute towards the memory impairment

    observed in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) patients? We examined cross-sectional comparisons of age-related regressions

    of verbal learning and memory in 1156 patients with chronic TLE (age range 668 years, mean epilepsy onset 14 11 years)

    versus 1000 healthy control subjects (age range 680 years) and tested the hypothesis that deviations of age regressions (i.e.

    slowed rise, accelerated decline) will reveal critical phases during which epilepsy interferes with cognitive development. Patients

    were recruited over a 20-year period at the Department of Epileptology, University of Bonn. Healthy subjects were drawn from

    an updated normative population of the Verbaler Lern- und Merkfahigkeitstest, the German pendant to the Rey Auditory Verbal

    learning Test. A significant divergence of age regressions indicates that patients fail to build up adequate learning and memoryperformance during childhood and particularly during adolescence. The learning peak (i.e. crossover into decline) is seen earlier

    in patients (at about the age of 1617 years) than for controls (at about the age of 2324 years). Decline in performance with

    ageing in patients and controls runs in parallel, but due to the initial distance between the groups, patients reach very poor

    performance levels much earlier than controls. Patients with left and right TLEs performed worse in verbal memory than controls.

    In addition, patients with left TLE performed worse than those with right TLE. However, laterality differences were evident only

    in adolescent and adult patients, and not (or less so) in children and older patients. Independent of age, hippocampal sclerosis

    was associated with poorer performance than other pathologies. The results indicate developmental hindrance plus a negative

    interaction of cognitive impairment with mental ageing, rather than a progressively dementing decline in chronic TLE patients.

    During childhood, and even more so during the decade following puberty, the critical phases for establishing episodic memory

    deficits appear. This increases the risk of premature dementia later on, even in the absence of an accelerated decline. Material

    specific verbal memory impairment in left TLE is a characteristic of the mature brain and seems to disappear at an older age. The

    findings suggest that increased attention is to be paid to the time of epilepsy onset and thereafter. Early control of epilepsy isdemanded to counteract developmental hindrance and damage at a younger age.

    Keywords: temporal lobe epilepsy; verbal memory; ageing; development; lateralization

    doi:10.1093/brain/awp182 Brain 2009: 132; 28222830 | 2822

    Received February 13, 2009. Revised May 18, 2009. Accepted May 23, 2009. Advance Access publication July 27, 2009

    The Author (2009). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved.

    For Permissions, please email: [email protected]

  • 7/26/2019 2822.full

    2/9

    IntroductionTemporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is the most common form of chronic

    focal epilepsy. Due to the localization of neuropathology in

    memory-relevant brain structures, problems in episodic memory

    represent the major cognitive co-morbidity of TLE. Episodic

    memory is defined as the ability to acquire and later retrieve

    new space- and time-related information. It is, therefore, an

    essential component in establishing continuity and biographic

    identity. Memory impairment in TLE reflects loss of structural

    morphological integrity of the temporal lobe structures, as well

    as the dynamic and potentially reversible influence of seizures

    and treatment on brain function. However, we must also consider

    whether epilepsy and seizures interfere with brain maturation, and

    if chronic uncontrolled epilepsy causes a progressive mental

    decline with ageing.

    In 2002, an international multidisciplinary meeting was held to

    discuss whether seizures damage the brain. The results were pub-

    lished in Progress in Brain Research (Sutula and Pitkanen, 2002).

    The collected evidence showed that seizures represent only a part

    of the problem and that complex molecular, cellular, synaptic andsystem level dysfunctions must also be considered when question-

    ing the chronic cognitive and behavioural impairments in epilepsy.

    At first glance, it would appear almost certain that uncontrolled

    epilepsy causes a mental decline. However, the surprising

    conclusion of Tom Sutula and Asla Pitkanen was that seizures

    may damage the brain only in some individuals, in some

    conditions, . . . (Chapter 47, p. 513).

    Lifetime-spanning studies on cognition in patients with chronic

    epilepsy are not available. At best, longitudinal studies cover an

    interval of up to 10 years. Studies addressing TLE were mostly

    conducted on surgically treated patients (as opposed to pharma-

    cologically treated patients), and although they indicate some

    decline and a slight relevance of uncontrolled seizures, the results

    are inconsistent and do not particularly favour the idea that

    epilepsy is a progressively dementing disease (Elger et al., 2004).

    A review of 20 longitudinal studies examined the cognitive out-

    comes in patients with epilepsy and concluded that there is a

    mild but definite relationship between seizures and mental

    decline (Dodrill, 2004). However, these studies, and particularly

    the older reports, comprise patients with very different epilepsy

    conditions and they may well have included patients with mental

    decline due to progressive aetiology. Particularly, for those patients

    who today would be considered as suffering from progressive

    mitochondrial or auto-immunological inflammatory brain diseases,

    it is very difficult to separate the negative cognitive effects ofpathology from those of the severe seizures that are an expression

    of the progressive pathology (Helmstaedter, 2007).

    In contrast to longitudinal studies, cross-sectional studies can

    span a wider age range. Interestingly, such studies clearly suggest

    a slow cognitive decline with increased duration of epilepsy (Jokeit

    and Ebner, 1999, 2002). However, there is a major methodolog-

    ical problem with studies that examine the effect of epilepsy

    duration on cognition. Since most epilepsies start early in life, a

    longer epilepsy duration is heavily confounded by ageing, which

    itself is associated with cognitive decline.

    A reasonable way out of this dilemma is to compare age regres-

    sions in patients with TLE against those in healthy subjects. The

    concept behind this comparison states that if epilepsy systemati-

    cally changes age-related cognitive development, then this should

    become evident with significant deviations of the respective

    regression curves from each other. If, for example, long-lasting

    chronic epilepsy systematically adds to a decline, then an accel-

    eration of such a decline should show a significantly steeper

    regression for the patients than for the healthy subjects. First find-

    ings in a relatively small sample indicated that, contrary to previ-

    ous assumptions, the age regressions for healthy controls and

    patients ran in parallel (Helmstaedter and Elger, 1999). At each

    age, the patients performed worse than the healthy subjects and

    the distance between groups remained stable over time. Thus, it

    did not seem that progressive mental decline was caused by

    chronic TLE. Instead, the stable distance between patients and

    controls indicated that the cognitive problems in epilepsy must

    evolve around the time of epilepsy onset or perhaps even earlier.

    In keeping with this finding, there is indeed a long history docu-

    menting the negative effects of an early onset of epilepsy on

    cognition (Fitzhugh et al., 1965; Dikmen et al., 1975; Dikmenand Matthews, 1977; OLeary et al., 1983; Strauss et al., 1995;

    Cormack et al., 2007).

    MethodsWe extended our previous findings with adults and focused on learn-

    ing and memory performance in both children and adolescents.

    We evaluated memory data from a very large sample of 1156 patients

    with pharmaco-resistant TLE covering a wide age range. Patients were

    collected over a 20-year time period at the Department of

    Epileptology, University of Bonn. From the total neuropsychological

    database with approximately 6300 first presentations, we selected

    patients based on neuropsychological assessment of verbal learningand memory and the diagnosis of TLE defined by MRI (temporal

    lobe lesions) and/or ictal/interictal EEG. Of the 789 with available

    imaging data, the MRI indicated hippocampal sclerosis/atrophy in

    62% of the patients; 19% showed developmental lesions; 9%

    tumours; 4% vascular lesions; and 6% had no findings. For the

    remaining patients, no MRI data were available. According to the

    MRI and/or EEG data, in 595 (52%) patients a left-sided TLE was

    indicated; in 538 (46%) a right-sided TLE; and uncertain bilateral find-

    ings were present in 23 (2%) patients. The mean age at the onset of

    epilepsy was 14 11 years, with 85% of the onsets before 25 years of

    age (26%, age 05; 33%, age 614; 26%, age 1524; 15%, age

    25+). The mean duration of epilepsy was 1812 years.

    All subjects underwent verbal memory testing using the Verbaler

    Lern-und Merkfahigkeitstest (VLMT) (Helmstaedter et al., 2001),a German adaptation of the well-known Rey Auditory Verbal

    Learning Test (RAVLT). Word-list learning is commonly used for

    memory assessment in epilepsy patients. When applied before and

    after surgery, it reliably reflects temporal lobe functioning, temporal

    lobe pathology and the cognitive effects of TLE (Helmstaedter et al.,

    1997a, b; Lee et al., 2002; Loring et al., 2008).

    Since we standardized and published the VLMT in Germany

    (Helmstaedter et al., 2001), we also have the normative data for

    this memory test with, at present, 1000 healthy subjects tested

    between the ages 6 and 80 years. This allowed us to compare the

    age-related memory performance of patients from 6 to 70 years

    Memory course in chronic TLE Brain 2009: 132; 28222830 | 2823

  • 7/26/2019 2822.full

    3/9

    (51% males, mean age 33 12 years) against the age-related memory

    performance of healthy subjects from 6 to 80 years (55% males, mean

    age 3521 years) (Table 1). The VLMT requires consecutive learning

    and recall of a list of 15 words (list A) over five trials; free recall of this

    list after each trial is requested. After the five learning trials are com-

    plete, a second word list (list B) is presented for free recall to assess the

    effects of interference, Finally, unannounced recall of list A is

    requested after a delay of half an hour, and a recognition trial is

    performed. Recognition requires identification of the words of list A

    out of an orally presented list of 50 words which comprises the words

    from lists A and B as well as new distractor words. Structural analysis

    of this test demonstrated that it reliably assesses verbal short-term or

    working memory as well as long-term memory (Muller et al., 1997;

    Helmstaedteret al., 2001). Based on the structuralfunctional correla-

    tion studies, out of all possible test parameters, the total number ofcorrectly recalled words in immediate recall over the five learning

    trials (learning: total trials 15) was selected because it is the most

    representative of the more neocortical aspects of short-term and work-

    ing memory (Elgeret al., 1997; Helmstaedteret al., 1997a, b). Of the

    delayed recall scores, loss of learned words over time was chosen

    because this measure provides less redundant information to learning

    than absolute free delayed recall (correlation of r= 0.3 instead of

    r= 0.8). This measure is the representative of the more medial

    temporal-dependent aspects of long-term consolidation and retrieval

    (memory: loss in delayed free recall) (Helmstaedter et al., 1997a, b;

    Muller et al., 1997; Helmstaedteret al., 2001, 2002).

    ResultsBy taking both linear and non-linear dependencies into account,

    we performed a combined linear and non-linear regression analysis

    by a modelling method called LOESS or LOWESS analysis (locally

    weighted scatterplot smoothing) with a smoothing parameter

    q = 0.3 (Cleveland and Devlin, 1988). This model more adequately

    described the course of memory performance over the time than

    standard regression methods. Instead of fitting a specification of a

    function to all data in the sample, this analysis fits the regression

    to localized subsets of data to build-up a function point-by-point.

    The smoothing parameter q can be flexibly chosen (typically

    between 0.25 and 0.5) and is the proportion of data used in

    each fit. The smaller the q is, the closer the regression conforms

    to the data, the larger the q is, the lesser the regression responds

    to fluctuations in the data.

    [Linear analyses for controls had indicated that the development

    of verbal learning memory can be best explained by a quadratic

    function (learning: r2 = 0.15, F= 87.1, P50.001; memory:

    r2 = 0.03, F= 16.4, P50.001); whereas in patients, learning and

    memory curves were best approximated by a linear and logarith-

    mic function, respectively (learning: r2 = 0.031,F= 37.1,P50.001;

    memory: r2 = 0.03, F= 40.1, P50.001).]

    The cross-sectional development of verbal learning in relation to

    age (i.e. the comparison of the age regressions of learning in

    healthy subjects and TLE patients) is displayed in Fig. 1A. The

    performance of both groups in 5-year increments until the age

    of 30 years and 10-year increments until the age of 80 years is

    displayed in Fig. 1B. The respective courses of verbal memory are

    displayed in Fig. 2A and B.

    Healthy controls show a course of verbal learning in relation to

    age as expected from developmental psychological research(Grady and Craik, 2000; Salthouse, 2009) (Fig. 1A). There

    clearly is a linear increase in the performance until about the

    age of 2223 years, at which point a slow, but steady, linear

    decline is present. Comparing the course of memory in healthy

    subjects and patients beyond the age of 25 years shows that the

    decline in learning capacity in patients runs largely parallel to that

    in the controls, but at a much lower level. After the age of 50

    years, the patients and controls come closer, but because of the

    decreasing number of older patients, their development is difficult

    to determine via the scatter plot. The results for the age groups

    (Fig. 1B) show that patients and controls are closer to each other

    between 60 and 70 years, mainly because of the large variance in

    patient performance.The course of verbal memory (loss of learned words over time)

    shows how verbal memory performance becomes optimized in

    healthy subjects until young adulthood, in that retrieval efficiency

    steadily improves along with the increase observed for learning

    (Fig. 2A and B). The best retrieval performance (i.e. the best learn-

    ing and least loss of what has been learned) is observed at the age

    of 2223 years, where the mean loss approaches the zero line.

    From this age onward, a decline becomes evident until age 40,

    and then plateaus until the age of 70 years. After 70 years of age,

    some further decline is indicated. Again, the memory courses of

    healthy subjects and TLE patients over the age of 25 years run

    largely parallel and the results on ageing are in keeping with our

    previous findings in a selected group of patients with left mesialTLE (Helmstaedter and Elger, 1999). The results on ageing are also

    in line with recent longitudinal findings on quantitative MRI in

    patients with TLE and healthy subjects, which again fail to provide

    evidence that chronic TLE progressively adds damage to the brain

    (Helmstaedter and Elger, 1999; Liu et al., 2003, 2005).

    The major finding of this study becomes evident when the

    course of learning and memory development in childhood and

    adolescence is compared among the groups. Here, the slopes

    diverge significantly, in that a steep increase of the learning per-

    formance in healthy subjects contrasts with a flattened increase in

    Table 1 Distribution of patients and control subjects overage groups between 6 and 80 years

    Age categories Groups Total

    Controls TLE

    610 134 28 162

    1115 104 68 172

    1620 83 107 190

    2125 104 139 243

    2630 92 152 244

    3140 94 355 449

    4150 103 208 311

    5160 98 82 180

    6170 136 17 153

    7180 52 0 52

    Total 1000 1156 2156

    Until the age of 30 years, 5-year increments and in the time after 30 years,

    10-year increments were choosen.

    2824 | Brain 2009: 132; 28222830 C. Helmstaedter and C. E. Elger

  • 7/26/2019 2822.full

    4/9

    patients (Fig. 1A and B). When compared with healthy subjects

    who increased their learning and memory performance until their

    early mid-twenties, the TLE patients completely failed to build-up

    their respective performance. They demonstrated only a very

    moderate increase in learning capacity which came to a halt at

    the age of 1617 years. Instead of further improving their perfor-

    mance, the patients had already reached the point at which verbal

    learning turns over into a steady decline. When the groups are

    Figure 1 (A) Age regressions of verbal learning (total learning over five learning trials) in patients with chronic TLE and healthy controlsubjects (CONTR) indicate developmental hindrance rather than progressive accelerated cognitive decline in the patient group.

    The figure displays a scatter plot representing all individual subjects. The two vertical lines indicate the turn over when learning starts

    to decline. (B) The results on verbal learning with subjects grouped by age increments of 5 and later of 10 years. Bars represent 90% CI

    for the group means. Note how the gap between patients and controls increases with the age of 1620 years and how it reaches

    a maximum difference with the age of 2125 years where patients already turned into decline.

    Memory course in chronic TLE Brain 2009: 132; 28222830 | 2825

  • 7/26/2019 2822.full

    5/9

    broken down into 5- and 10-year increments, the patients and

    control subjects are closest to each other at a young age

    (Fig. 1B). Thereafter, the gap opens at 1620 years of age, and

    then reaches the maximum difference for the 2125 years of age

    groups. At that stage, healthy subjects are still improving their

    performance, whereas the patients are already in significant decline.

    Unlike their performance in learning, the patients performance

    in memory appears to decline linearly from the very beginning

    until the age of 2627 years. Memory in healthy subjects increases

    linearly and peaks at age 2122 years (Fig. 2A and B). As with

    learning, the age group comparison shows that the gap opens at

    age 1620 years and continues until 2125 years. Accordingly, a

    Figure 2 (A) Age regressions of verbal memory (loss of learned words in half hour delayed free recall) in patients with chronic TLE andhealthy control subjects (CONTR) indicate that this performance decreases in patients from the very beginning (positive values mean

    greater losses). The two vertical lines indicate the time from which on memory goes over into a plateau. (B) The results on verbal

    memory with subjects grouped by age increments of 5 and later of 10 years. Bars represent 90% CI for the group means. Note the

    steady decline in patients, how the gap between patients and controls increases with the age of 1620 years and how it reaches a

    maximum difference with the age of 2125 years.

    2826 | Brain 2009: 132; 28222830 C. Helmstaedter and C. E. Elger

  • 7/26/2019 2822.full

    6/9

    slow development of verbal learning and deteriorating verbal

    memory is implied, but there is no hard evidence suggesting an

    accelerated decline with a longer duration of epilepsy at an older

    age. Finally, it should be noted that at an older age, learning and

    memory performances show a different age dependence

    (i.e. learning is more significantly modulated by ageing than

    memory).

    With regard to the findings on learning, it might be interesting

    to note that separate analyses performed for memory span at the

    first learning trial on list A and the single learning trial on list B

    showed very similar results as observed with total learning. The

    courses of memory span performance across the ages largely

    mirrored those seen with learning (figures are not shown). The

    rationale for adding this analysis was that memory span depends

    on frontally mediated processes of working memory rather than

    on medialtemporal, long-term consolidation. However, the com-

    parable results are not surprising since total learning and memory

    spans are highly correlated with each other (list A, r= 0.76; list B,

    r= 0.56) (Muller et al., 1997).

    The underlying pathology and the hemispheric lateralization ofTLE are major determinants of the verbal memory impairment in

    TLE (Wieser, 2004). Since the respective subgroups were suffi-

    ciently large, we also calculated subsequent analyses by taking

    the presence versus absence of the hippocampal sclerosis and

    the lateralizaton of the epilepsy in the left versus right hemisphere

    into account.

    As for the pathology, 491 (62%) of 789 patients with MRI were

    diagnosed with a hippocampal sclerosis/atrophy (AHS), 298 (38%)

    patients had another pathology. Comparison of the groups

    indicated that verbal learning and memory performances were

    worse in patients with hippocampal sclerosis than for those

    patients with another pathology (learning F= 16.1, P50.000

    memory F=6.9, P50.01). However, this difference was seen

    only when the total groups were taken into consideration. When

    the different age groups were calculated separately, the differ-

    ences between patients with AHS and other pathologies were

    not significant.

    We compared patients with left (N =595) and right TLE

    (N = 538). Both groups were impaired in verbal learning and

    memory in relation to controls and, in addition, the patients

    with left TLE performed worse than those with right TLE

    (F=140 and 141 with P50.000 for learning and memory).

    However, as indicated in Table 2, the leftright difference in

    verbal learning and memory was confined to adolescence and

    adulthood and the occurrence of leftright differences across the

    ages was different for verbal learning and memory. In verbal

    learning (total: trials 15), significant leftright differences were

    observed between the age of 3150 years. In verbal memory

    (loss in delayed free recall), the leftright differences were seen

    between the ages of 1150 years. With an older age (450 years),the leftright differences became less pronounced and failed to

    reach statistical significance.

    DiscussionAge regressions of verbal learning and memory in patients with

    TLE and healthy subjects were compared with the test hypothesis

    that significant deviances of the age regressions would reveal

    critical phases whereby epilepsy interferes with cognitive

    Table 2 Differences in verbal learning (total learning) and memory (loss of learned words after delay) across the ages asdependent on the lateralization of temporal lobe epilepsy in the left versus right hemisphere

    Agecategories(years)

    N mean (SD) F/sig nif . Agecategories(years)

    N mean (SD) F/signif.

    510 VLMT learning (trials 15) R-TLE 12 40.33 (11.98) 0.001 NS 3140 R-TLE 154 46.18 (10.06) 4.45

    L-TLE 16 40.19 (11.16) L-TLE 196 44.04 (8.93)

    VLMT memory(loss over time) R-TLE 12 1.08 (1.56) 0.06 NS R-TLE 154 2.35 (2.19) 26.22

    L-TLE 16 1.25 (1.77) L-TLE 196 3.56 (2.19)

    1115 VLMT learning(trials 15) R-TLE 36 48.33 (9.36) 0.44 NS 4150 R-TLE 101 45.22 (9.37) 13.01

    L-TLE 31 46.58 (12.19) L-TLE 100 40.13 (10.59)

    VLMT memory (loss over t ime) R-TLE 36 1.47 (1.64) 5.40 R-TLE 101 2.39 (2.42) 10.41

    L-TLE 31 2.35 (1.42) L-TLE 100 3.45 (2.20)

    1620 VLMT learning(trials 15) R-TLE 47 49.06 (8.42) 0.039 5160 R-TLE 41 42.85 (9.18) 1.84 NS

    L-TLE 58 49.41 (9.47) L-TLE 38 40.11 (8.73)

    VLMT memory (loss over t ime) R-TLE 47 1.55 (2.09) 4.50 R-TLE 41 2.92 (2.25) 0.36 NS

    L-TLE 58 2.43 (2.12) L-TLE 38 3.23 (2.30)

    2125 VLMT learning (t rials 15) R-TLE 71 48.18 (9.22) 1.81 NS 6170 R-TLE 12 42.75 (9.59) 0.21 NS

    L-TLE 67 46.06 (9.27) L-TLE 3 40.00 (6.55)

    VLMT memory (loss over t ime) R-TLE 71 2.21 (1.97) 5.125 R-TLE 12 2.33 (2.70) 3.93 NS

    L-TLE 67 3.01 (2.19) L-TLE 3 6.00 (3.60)

    2630 VLMT learning (trials 15) R-TLE 64 46.30 (10.1) 0.273 NS

    L-TLE 86 45.48 (9.016)VLMT Memory [loss over time] R-TLE 64 2.09 (2.39) 13.74

    L-TLE 12 40.33 (11.980)

    Note that left-right differences mainly appear as a characteristic of the mature and adult brain.P50.05; P50.01; P50.001; NS= not significant.

    Memory course in chronic TLE Brain 2009: 132; 28222830 | 2827

  • 7/26/2019 2822.full

    7/9

    development. Since most epilepsies start early in life, with

    increasing age of patients at the time of evaluation, the impact

    of the onset of epilepsy will have a decreasing contribution to

    the resulting performance, whereas the contribution of the

    duration of epilepsy will increase. Therefore, if deviances exist

    at a young age, then one should be able to confirm either

    developmental hindrance or retardation. If there are deviances at

    an older age, this should be indicative of an accelerated mental

    decline.

    If we use this as our basis, the present results provide strong

    evidence of a neurodevelopmental hindrance in chronic TLE

    patients, which becomes most evident in the decade after puberty.

    While the gap between patients and controls increasingly widens

    at a younger age, the course of learning and memory at an older

    age runs largely in parallel. However, patients at every age score

    are at a much lower level than healthy subjects. Thus, no progres-

    sive degenerative decline can be confirmed from these findings.

    So far, this is in keeping with the results that we published in 1999

    involving a selected group of patients with pure left mesial TLE

    (Helmstaedter and Elger, 1999).

    The age at which development comes to a halt indicates thatthe problems with learning arise in later brain maturation, which

    is no longer intrinsically and ontogenetically determined, but rather

    mainly extrinsically driven by experience (Segalowitz and Hiscock,

    2002). Learning (i.e. the short-term and working memory aspects

    of memory) highly depends on attention, executive functions and

    language. Healthy subjects are able to improve their learning

    capacity by increasing these more extra-temporally and mostly

    frontally-associated functions. Learning, as well as memory in

    terms of the access to learned materials, improves with better

    behavioural organization and the extension of semantic networks

    and knowledge systems. One may hypothesize that patients fail in

    this particular respect.

    It is remarkable that the age at which patients becomeincreasingly impaired marks the approximate endpoint of func-

    tional cerebral plasticity, which helps to preserve language and

    language-related memory aspects in early onset epilepsy

    (Helmstaedter et al., 1997a, b, Helmstaedter, 1999). As pointed

    out elsewhere in more detail (Helmstaedter and Elger, 1998;

    Helmstaedter, 1999), the neocortical and mesio-limbic aspects of

    verbal learning and memory seem to have different functional

    plasticity. Mesial encoding functions are more bilaterally disposed,

    and unilateral damage can, in part, be compensated by homolo-

    gous contralateral structures quite independent of age. Unilateral

    selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy demonstrates that surgical

    damage often causes additional memory impairment, but will not

    lead to global amnesia as long as no contralateral damage exists.Plasticity for material-specific neocortical functions is, in contrast,

    time-limited, in that neocortical functions become increasingly

    dependent on a single hemisphere. The critical period for contra-

    lateral restitution and compensation in the presence of unilateral

    damage extends to puberty (Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997;

    Grunwald et al., 1998; Helmstaedter and Elger, 1998;

    Helmstaedter, 1999; Liegeois et al., 2008). Accordingly, the time

    course of the more medial-temporal-dependent memory aspect

    among the age groups is comparably stable at an older age,

    whereas learning experiences a significant modulation with

    neocortical development. In the cross-sectional perspective, brain

    maturation and development also seem to be important for what

    is referred to as lateralization-dependent material-specific memory

    impairment in TLE (Saling, 2009). First, verbal learning and

    memory impairment are not confined to left TLE, i.e. patients

    with right TLE perform poorly as well, although to a lesser

    degree than left TLE patients. Of major interest, with regard to

    the lateralization of TLE is the finding that the leftright differ-

    ences in verbal learning and memory are observed in adolescent

    and adult patients, but not (or less so) in children and older

    patients. Thus, the impairment of verbal memory and its

    material-specific affection in left TLE emerge in the mature brain

    when hemispheric specialization becomes complete. At an older

    age, the material specificity of the memory impairment in lateral-

    ized TLE interestingly seems to disappear. Whether this reflects a

    converging memory impairment pattern independent on laterali-

    zation because of generally decreasing memory resources and

    mental reserve capacities at an older age is an interesting

    hypothesis which deserves further attention.

    Hippocampal sclerosis, which was the most frequent pathology

    in the evaluated group of TLE patients, had a differential impacton verbal learning and memory. The patients with hippocampal

    sclerosis performed worse than those with another pathology

    independent of age. Due to the cross-sectional study design, we

    cannot provide answers regarding the aetiology of the observed

    developmental hindrance. Do the data show additive damage,

    particularly in childhood and adolescence? Do they indicate early

    damage which subsequently grows into the impairment when the

    affected areas should have their developmental spurts? Does

    spreading epileptic activity have a distant effect on the develop-

    ment of brain areas not directly involved in epilepsy? Learning and

    memory are closely associated with temporal lobe structures and

    are thus most critical for the study of the effects of chronic TLE on

    cognition. Indeed, it is likely that the time-related changes inlearning and memory are linked to fluctuations in IQ as a function

    of age. As already mentioned in the introduction, there is ample

    evidence from the literature to suggest that an earlier onset of epi-

    lepsy is associated with a poorer intellectual and educational out-

    come (Strauss et al., 1995; Dikmen et al., 1977; Helmstaedter,

    2005; Cormack et al., 2007). Evidence for the neurodevelopmen-

    tal hypothesis originates from studies using quantitative brain vol-

    umetric measures. These studies show that an early onset TLE, in

    contrast to a late onset TLE, has substantial negative effects on

    extratemporal cortical development (Hermann et al., 2002, 2003;

    Weberet al., 2007; Kaaden et al., 2008).

    As for the concerns about dementia in TLE, it is important to

    note that although the data indicate a neurodevelopmental originof memory problems, patients are nevertheless at an increased risk

    of having dementiathat is, if we use the term dementia in its

    broadest sense. Even without an accelerated decline, the negative

    interaction of the initially established damage with normal ageing

    significantly reduces the patients reserve capacities at an older age

    when processes of normal or even pathological ageing take place.

    In this regard, the present data parallel those from the extraordi-

    nary longitudinal Scottish Mental Survey, which indicated that

    childhood IQ is a significant predictor for later dementia and mor-

    tality (Whalley et al., 2000; Starret al., 2008). How later acquired

    2828 | Brain 2009: 132; 28222830 C. Helmstaedter and C. E. Elger

  • 7/26/2019 2822.full

    8/9

    brain damage can change the patients cognitive perspective with

    ageing has been demonstrated with the effects of epilepsy surgery

    on the age regression of verbal learning and memory performance

    (Helmstaedteret al., 2002).

    At this stage of the evaluation, the data cannot provide final

    answers to the questions concerning the basic mechanisms behind

    the developmental hindrance and the detailed impact of different

    aetiologies, pathologies, seizure activity or medication on memory

    in the lifetime cycle. The main message of this study was to pro-

    vide a macro view of verbal learning and memory in TLE patients

    of various ages, and to reveal that our perspective towards

    patients with chronic epilepsy should change; it must shift away

    from viewing epilepsy as a progressively dementing disease and

    back towards showing how epilepsy, at its origin, interferes with

    brain maturation and cognitive development. In the long run, this

    increases the risk of premature dementia. Additional efforts are

    thus needed to identify patients who are at risk and to counteract

    negative cognitive development in TLE at the very beginning.

    ReferencesCleveland WS, Devlin SJ. Locally weighted regression: an approach to

    regression analysis by local fitting. J. Am. Stat. Assoc 1988; 83:

    596610.

    Cormack F, Cross JH, Isaacs E, Harkness W, Wright I, Vargha-Khadem F,

    et al. The development of intellectual abilities in pediatric temporal

    lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 2007; 48: 2014.

    Dikmen S, Matthews CG. Effect of major motor seizure frequency upon

    cognitive-intellectual functions in adults. Epilepsia 1977; 18: 219.

    Dikmen S, Matthews CG, Harley JP. The effect of early versus late onset

    of major motor epilepsy upon cognitive-intellectual performance.

    Epilepsia 1975; 16: 7381.

    Dikmen S, Matthews CG, Harley JP. Effect of early versus late onset of

    major motor epilepsy on cognitive-intellectual performance: further

    considerations. Epilepsia 1977; 18: 316.

    Dodrill CB. Neuropsychological effects of seizures. Epilepsy Behav 2004;5 (Suppl 1): 214.

    Elger CE, Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M. Chronic epilepsy and cognition.

    Lancet Neurol 2004; 3: 66372.

    Elger CE, Grunwald T, Lehnertz K, Kutas M, Helmstaedter C,

    Brockhaus A, et al. Human temporal lobe potentials in verbal learning

    and memory processes. Neuropsychologia 1997; 35: 65767.

    Fitzhugh LC, Fitzhugh KB, Reitan RM. Adaptive abilities and intellectual

    functioning of hospitalized alcoholics: further considerations. Q J Stud

    Alcohol 1965; 26: 40211.

    Grady CL, Craik FI. Changes in memory processing with age. Curr Opin

    Neurobiol 2000; 10: 22431.

    Grunwald T, Lehnertz K, Helmstaedter C, Kutas M, Pezer N, Kurthen M,

    et al. Limbic ERPs predict verbal memory after left-sided hippocam-

    pectomy. Neuroreport 1998; 9: 33758.

    Helmstaedter CA. Prediction of memory reserve capacity. Adv Neurol1999; 81: 2719.

    Helmstaedter C. Effects of chronic epilepsy on declarative memory

    systems. Prog Brain Res 2002; 135: 43953.

    Helmstaedter C. Effects of chronic temporal lobe epilepsy on memory

    function. In: Arzimanoglou A, Aldenkamp AP, Cross H, Lassonde M,

    Moshe SL, Schmitz B, editors.. Cognitive dysfunction in children with

    temporal lobe epilepsy. France: John Libbey; 2005. p. 1330.

    Helmstaedter C. Cognitive outcome of status epilepticus in adults.

    Epilepsia 2007; 48 (Suppl 8): 8590.

    Helmstaedter C, Elger CE. Functional plasticity after left anterior temporal

    lobectomy: reconstitution and compensation of verbal memory func-

    tions. Epilepsia 1998; 39: 399406.

    Helmstaedter C, Elger CE. The phantom of progressive dementia in

    epilepsy. Lancet 1999; 354: 21334.

    Helmstaedter C, Grunwald T, Lehnertz K, Gleissner U, Elger CE.

    Differential involvement of left temporolateral and temporomesial

    structures in verbal declarative learning and memory: evidence from

    temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Cogn 1997a; 35: 11031.

    Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M, Linke DB, Elger CE. Patterns of language

    dominance in focal left and right hemisphere epilepsies: Relation to

    MRI findings, EEG, sex, and age at onset of epilepsy. Brain Cognit

    1997b; 33: 13550.Helmstaedter C, Lendt M, Lux S. VMLT Verbaler Lern- und

    Merkfahigkeitstest. Gottingen: Beltz Test GmbH; 2001.

    Helmstaedter C, Reuber M, Elger CC. Interaction of cognitive aging and

    memory deficits related to epilepsy surgery. Ann Neurol 2002; 52:

    8994.

    Hermann B, Hansen R, Seidenberg M, Magnotta V, OLeary D.

    Neurodevelopmental vulnerability of the corpus callosum to childhood

    onset localization-related epilepsy. Neuroimage 2003; 18: 28492.

    Hermann B, Seidenberg M, Bell B, Rutecki P, Sheth R, Ruggles K, et al.

    The neurodevelopmental impact of childhood-onset temporal lobe

    epilepsy on brain structure and function. Epilepsia 2002; 43:

    106271.

    Jokeit H, Ebner A. Long term effects of refractory temporal lobe epilepsy

    on cognitive abilities: a cross sectional study. J Neurol Neurosurg

    Psychiat 1999; 67: 4450.Jokeit H, Ebner A. Effects of chronic epilepsy on intellectual functions.

    Prog Brain Res 2002; 135: 45563.

    Kaaden S, Quesada CM, Schramm J, Urbach H, Helmstaedter C. Voxel-

    based morphometry (VBM) supports neurodevelopmental hypothesis

    regarding early onset epilepsy in temporal lobe epilepsy.

    In: Proceedings of the 8th European Congress of Epileptology.

    8th ECE: 234, September 2125, 2008. Berlin. p. 234.

    Lee TM, Yip JT, Jones-Gotman M. Memory deficits after resection from

    left or right anterior temporal lobe in humans: a meta-analytic review.

    Epilepsia 2002; 43: 28391.

    Liegeois F, Cross JH, Polkey C, Harkness W, Vargha-Khadem F.

    Language after hemispherectomy in childhood: contributions from

    memory and intelligence. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46: 31017.

    Liu RS, Lemieux L, Bell GS, Sisodiya SM, Shorvon SD, Sander JW, et al.

    A longitudinal study of brain morphometrics using quantitative mag-

    netic resonance imaging and difference image analysis. Neuroimage

    2003; 20: 2233.

    Liu RS, Lemieux L, Bell GS, Sisodiya SM, Bartlett PA, Shorvon SD, et al.

    Cerebral damage in epilepsy: a population-based longitudinal quanti-

    tative MRI study. Epilepsia 2005; 46: 148294.

    Loring DW, Strauss E, Hermann BP, Barr WB, Perrine K, Trenerry MR,

    et al. Differential neuropsychological test sensitivity to left temporal

    lobe epilepsy. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008; 14: 394400.

    Muller H, Hasse-Sander I, Horn R, Helmstaedter C, Elger CE. Rey

    Auditory-Verbal Learning Test: Structure of a modified German ver-

    sion. J Clin Psychol 1997; 53: 66371.

    OLeary DS, Lovell MR, Sackellares JC, Berent S, Giordani B,

    Seidenberg M, et al. Effects of age of onset of partial and generalized

    seizures on neuropsychological performance in children. J Nerv Ment

    Dis 1983; 171: 6249.

    Saling MM. Verbal memory in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: beyondmaterial specificity. Brain 2009; 132: 57082.

    Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol

    Aging 2009; 30: 50714.

    Segalowitz S, Hiscock M. The neuropsychology of normal develop-

    ment: developmental neuroscience and a new constructivism.

    In: Segalowitz SJ, Rapin I, editors. Child neuropsychology. Part 1

    2nd edn., Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2002. p. 727.

    Starr JM, Deary IJ, Whalley LJ. All-cause mortality in the Aberdeen 1921

    birth cohort: effects of socio-demographic, physical and cognitive

    factors. BMC Public Health 2008; 8: 307.

    Strauss E, Loring D, Chelune G, Hunter M, Hermann B, Perrine K, et al.

    Predicting cognitive impairment in epilepsy: findings from the

    Memory course in chronic TLE Brain 2009: 132; 28222830 | 2829

  • 7/26/2019 2822.full

    9/9

    Bozeman epilepsy consortium. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1995; 17:

    90917.

    Sutula T, Pitkanen A, editors. Do seizures damage the brain. Amsterdam:

    Elsevier; 2002.

    Vargha-Khadem F, Carr LJ, Isaacs E, Brett E, Adams C, Mishkin M. Onset

    of speech after left hemispherectomy in a nine-year-old boy. Brain

    1997; 120 (Pt 1): 15982.

    Weber B, Luders E, Faber J, Richter S, Quesada CM, Urbach H, et al.

    Distinct regional atrophy in the corpus callosum of patients with tem-

    poral lobe epilepsy. Brain 2007; 130 (Pt 12): 314954.

    Whalley LJ, Starr JM, Athawes R, Hunter D, Pattie A, Deary IJ.

    Childhood mental ability and dementia. Neurology 2000; 55: 14559.

    Wieser HG. Mesial temporal lobe epilepsy with hippocampal sclerosis.

    Epilepsia 2004; 45: 695714.

    2830 | Brain 2009: 132; 28222830 C. Helmstaedter and C. E. Elger