1-s2.0-s0887618509002102-main

Upload: cva1590

Post on 04-Jun-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S0887618509002102-main

    1/7

    Its not just the judgementsIts that I dont know: Intolerance of uncertaintyas a predictor of social anxiety

    R. Nicholas Carleton, Kelsey C. Collimore, Gordon J.G. Asmundson *

    Department of Psychology and the Anxiety and Illness Behaviours Laboratory, University of Regina, Regina, Saskatchewan S4S 0A2, Canada

    1. Introduction

    Social anxiety (SA) refers to anxiety or apprehension experi-

    enced in interpersonal or performance situations (Watson &

    Friend, 1969). Individuals with high SA fear being negatively

    evaluated by others (Rapee & Heimberg, 1997; Stein, Jang, &

    Livesley, 1999), making a bad impression, or acting in a way that

    might be embarrassing (Antony & Swinson, 2000). There is also

    evidence that SA may result from fearing positive evaluation,

    suggesting concern related to evaluation in general (Weeks,

    Heimberg, & Rodebaugh, 2008; Weeks, Heimberg, Rodebaugh, &

    Norton, 2008).

    Researchers have shown that social anxiety disorder (SAD) is

    related to and exacerbated by fears that other people can detectsymptoms of SA (e.g., blushing; Rector, Szacun-Shimizu, &

    Leybman, 2007). Such fears are conceptualized within the anxiety

    sensitivity (AS;Peterson & Reiss, 1992) construct, which denotes

    the propensity to appraise anxiety-related somatic sensations,

    cognitive changes, and social consequences based on expectations

    of harmful consequences (Reiss & McNally, 1985; Taylor, 1999).

    Substantial research has demonstrated a direct relationship

    between the social subscale of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index

    (ASI;Peterson & Reiss, 1992), and both SA and SAD (Anderson and

    Hope, 2009; Asmundson & Stein, 1994; Ball, Otto, Pollack, Uccello,

    & Rosenbaum, 1995) as well as indirect relationships with the ASI

    somatic and cognitive subscales (Carleton et al., 2009).

    The AS construct has been associated with intolerance of

    uncertainty (IU) the tendency for a person to consider the

    possibility of a negative event occurring as unacceptable and

    threatening irrespective of the probability of its occurrence

    (Carleton, Sharpe, & Asmundson, 2007) and may be causally

    dependent on IU (Carleton, Sharpe, et al., 2007). For persons who

    are intolerant of uncertainty, engaging in situations with uncertain

    outcomesmay induce and perpetuate a heightened level of anxiety

    (Dugas, Gosselin, & Landouceur, 2001). People with high IU aremore likely to interpret ambiguous information as threatening

    (Heydayati, Dugas, Buhr, & Francis, 2003), therein exacerbating

    their physiological arousal (Greco & Roger, 2001, 2003) which

    serves to facilitate self-perpetuating cycles of fear (Barlow, 2002).

    IU has been a useful construct in theory and research associated

    with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and obsessive compulsive

    disorder (Dugas et al., 2001; Holaway, Heimberg, & Coles, 2006);

    however, it is now also drawing attention from researchers

    investigating panic disorder (Dugas, Marchand, & Ladouceur, 2005;

    Simmons, Matthews, Paulus, & Stein, 2008). In contrast, there is a

    relative paucity of research directly investigating the relationship

    Journal o f Anxiety Disorders 24 (2010) 189195

    A R T I C L E I N F O

    Article history:

    Received 1 June 2009

    Received in revised form 8 October 2009Accepted 19 October 2009

    Keywords:

    Intolerance of uncertainty

    Social anxiety

    Social phobia

    Generalized anxiety disorder

    A B S T R A C T

    Interest in the role of intolerance of uncertainty (IU) the tendency for a person to consider the

    possibility of a negative event occurring as unacceptable and threatening irrespective of the probability

    of its occurrence in anxiety disorders has been increasing in recent research. IU has been implicated as

    an important construct associated withgeneralizedanxiety disorder (GAD); however,a growing body of

    research suggests that levels of IU are also high among individuals with other anxiety disorders. Despite

    the increasing interest, few studies have examined the relationship between IU and social anxiety (SA).

    The purpose of the present investigation was to further investigate the relationship between IU and SA.

    Participants included 286 community members (71% women) from Canada who completed measures of

    IU, SA, anxiety sensitivity, and fear of negative evaluation (FNE). Regression analyses revealed that the

    inhibitory anxiety dimension of IU, the fear of socially observable anxiety symptoms dimension of

    anxiety sensitivity, and the FNE were consistently significant predictors of SA symptoms. Unexpectedly,

    IU and FNE were often comparable predictors of SA variance. Moreover, participants with SA symptoms

    consistent with SAD exhibited levels of IU comparable to those reported by participants with worry

    symptoms consistent with GAD. Comprehensive findings, implications, and directions for future

    research are discussed.

    2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    * Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 306 347 2415; fax: +1 306 337 3275.

    E-mail address: [email protected](Gordon J.G. Asmundson).

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    0887-6185/$ see front matter 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.10.007

    mailto:[email protected]://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/08876185http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.10.007http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.10.007http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/08876185mailto:[email protected]
  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S0887618509002102-main

    2/7

    between IU and SA, including SAD. Researchers have found that IU

    can predict changes in reported levels of SA (Riskind, Tzur,

    Williams, Mann, & Shahar, 2007); however, the ability to tolerate

    the uncertainty associated with social situations may be a critical

    element in determining SA. For example, in persons with SAD,

    uncertainty is often associated with SA before a social encounter

    (e.g., catastrophizing about possible occurrences), during the social

    encounter (e.g., catastrophizing about ambiguous stimuli), and/or

    after the social encounter (e.g., catastrophizing about possible

    consequences; Antony & Rowa, 2008).

    The IU and SA relationship has also been directly demonstrated

    as independent of the established relationships between SA, FNE,

    and AS using data from a sample of Netherlands participants

    (Boelen & Reijntjes, 2009). In that study, IU accounted for variance

    in SA symptoms beyond negative affect, FNE, and AS. While novel

    and highly coherent, Boelen and Reijntjess study included (1) only

    one measure of SA, (2) used the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale

    (IUS; Freeston, Rheaume, Letarte, Dugas, & Ladouceur, 1994),

    which can be psychometrically unstable (Carleton, Norton, &

    Asmundson, 2007), (3) did not include a measure of positive

    affectivity, and (4) used only the ASI total score.

    The primary goal of the current study was to replicate the

    previous finding of a relationship between IU and SA (Boelen &

    Reijntjes, 2009) with data from a North American communitysample. A secondary goal was to extend those findings to include

    measures of the various facets of SA (i.e., social interaction and

    performance anxiety, social distress and avoidance), negative and

    positive affect, as well as psychometrically stable measures of IU

    and AS. More specifically, the dimensions of IU and AS were not

    assessed in the previous study despite evidence that the

    dimensions of each construct function in importantly distinct

    ways (Collimore, McCabe, Carleton, & Asmundson, 2008; Gosselin

    et al., 2008; Taylor, 1999). The final goal of this study was to

    compare levels of IU across participants reporting symptoms

    congruent with SAD (without co-occurring GAD), relative to GAD

    (without comorbid SAD), or both (comorbid SAD and GAD),

    thereby paralleling previous comparative analyses of GAD and

    panic disorder with agoraphobia (Dugas et al., 2005).

    2. Methods

    2.1. Participants

    Participants included community members (n= 286) from

    Canada [82 men, 1854 years (Mage= 29.9; SD = 10.8) and 204

    women, 1855 years (Mage = 29.8 SD = 10.7)], who completed

    several self-report measures as part of a larger investigation that

    was approved by the University Research Ethics Board. Partici-

    pants were solicited with web-based advertising to participate in

    research exploring fear. Web-based data collection has been

    demonstrated to be a valid approach for questionnaire-based

    research in North America that is comparable to other datacollection methods (Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava, & John, 2004) and

    has been used in related investigations of fear constructs (Carleton

    & Asmundson, 2009; Carleton, Norton, et al., 2007). Most

    participants (70%) reported having at least some postsecondary

    education, being employed or working at home (37% full-time, 19%

    part-time, and 7% as homemakers) and being either part or full

    time students (36%). Most participants identified their ethnicity as

    Caucasian (88%), First Nations (2%), or Asian (2%). Approximately

    half (52%) reported being single and 35% reported being married.

    2.2. Measures

    Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3(ASI-3;Taylor et al., 2007). The ASI-3

    is an 18-item self-report measure assessing the tendency to fear

    symptoms of anxiety based on the belief that they may have

    harmful consequences (e.g., It scares me when I blush in front of

    people). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0

    (very little) to 4 (very much). Factor analyses supports a robust 3-

    factor structure corresponding to the three dimensions of AS (fear

    of somatic sensations, somatic; fear of cognitive dyscontrol,

    cognitive, and fear of socially observable symptoms of anxiety,

    social;Taylor, Koch, Woody, & McLean, 1996; Zinbarg, Barlow, &

    Brown, 1997). The ASI-3 has demonstrated improved internal

    consistency and factorial validity relative to the original ASI

    (Peterson & Reiss,1992). The ASI-3 has also demonstrated evidence

    for convergent, discriminant, and criterion validity (Taylor et al.,

    2007). Measures of AS demonstrate unique incremental validity

    beyond trait anxiety (Rapee & Medoro, 1994) and trait-level

    negative affectivity/ neuroticism (Zvolensky, Kotov, Antipova,

    Leen-Feldner, & Schmidt, 2005). The internal consistency was

    acceptable for the total score (a = .92), the somatic subscale score

    (a = .86), the cognitive subscale score (a = .89), and the social

    subscale score (a = .84). The average inter-item correlation was

    .40.

    Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation scale, version 2 (BFNE-II;

    Carleton, Collimore, & Asmundson, 2007; Carleton, McCreary,

    Norton, & Asmundson, 2006). The BFNE-II is a 12-item revised

    version of the Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation scale (BFNE;Leary,1983) used for measuring fears of negative evaluation (e.g., I am

    afraid that others will not approve of me). The BFNE has been

    correlated with the Social Avoidance and Distress Scale (SADS;

    Watson & Friend, 1969); however, it more accurately depicts FNE

    than it does SA (Miller, 1995). Revisions to the BFNE were made in

    accordance with previously suggested changes (Taylor, 1993) to

    remove a methodological issue stemming from four reverse-

    worded items by revising those items to be straightforwardly

    worded (Carleton, Collimore, et al., 2007; Carleton et al., 2006;

    Weeks et al., 2005). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale

    ranging from 0 (not at all characteristic of me) t o 4 (extremely

    characteristic of me). The BFNE-II demonstrates excellent internal

    consistency, correlates highly with the BFNE, and factor analyses

    have supporteda unitary solution(Carleton, Collimore, et al., 2007;Carleton et al., 2006). The internal consistency was acceptable

    (a = .98) and the average inter-item correlation was .76.

    Generalized Anxiety Disorder Assessment (GAD-7; Spitzer,

    Kroenke, Williams, & Lowe, 2006). The GAD-7 is a 7-item self-

    report measure designed to assess GAD symptoms (e.g., Feeling

    nervous, anxious or on edge). Items are rated on a 4-point Likert

    scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day). Use of the

    total 7-item score typically provides sufficient sensitivity and

    specificity for discerning clinical and nonclinical samples (i.e., a

    cut-off score of 10 with reports of symptoms interfering with daily

    activities can be used to distinguish persons reporting clinically

    significant distress; Spitzer et al., 2006; Swinson, 2006). The

    internal consistency was acceptable (a = .89) and the average

    inter-item correlation was .54.Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale, Short Form (IUS-12;Carleton,

    Norton, et al., 2007). The IUS-12 is a 12-item short-form of the

    original 27-item Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (Freeston et al.,

    1994) that measures reactions to uncertainty, ambiguous situa-

    tions, and the future (e.g., Unforeseen events upset me greatly).

    Items are scored on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all

    characteristic of me) to 5 (entirely characteristic of me). The IUS-12

    has a strong correlation with the original scale, r= .96, and has

    been shown to have two factors, including prospective anxiety (7

    items; e.g., I cant stand being taken by surprise) and inhibitory

    anxiety (5 items; e.g., When its time to act, uncertainty paralyses

    me), both with identically high internal consistencies, a = .85

    (Carleton, Norton, et al., 2007). The internal consistency was

    acceptable for the total score (a = .92), the prospective anxiety

    R.N. Carleton et al./ Journal of Anxiety Disorders 24 (2010) 189195190

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S0887618509002102-main

    3/7

    subscale score (a = .87) and the inhibitory anxiety subscale score

    (a = .90). The average inter-item correlation was .49.

    Social Avoidance and Distress Scale (SADS; Watson & Friend,

    1969). The SADS is a 28-item self-report measure assessing the

    tendency to avoid or be distressed by social situations (e.g., I try to

    avoid situations which force me to be very sociable). The original

    scale presented respondents with true/false questions; however, in

    line with previous research (High & Caplan, 2009) we used a 5-

    point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all characteristic of me) t o 5

    (entirely characteristic of me). The factor structure is generally

    considered to be unitary (Watson & Friend, 1969). The internal

    consistency was acceptable for the total score (a = .96) and the

    average inter-item correlation was .45.

    Social Interaction Phobia Scale(SIPS;Carleton et al., 2009). The

    SIPS is a 14-item self-report measure designed to assess symptoms

    specific to SAD (e.g., I have difficulty talking with other people).

    Each item is measured on a 5-point Likertscale, ranging from 0 (not

    at all) to 4 (extremely). Respondents indicate how much each item

    bothered them during the past week. The items were derived as a

    subset of items from the Social Interaction Anxiety and Social

    Phobia Scales (Mattick & Clarke, 1998). The SIPS is designed to

    measure three symptom dimensions (i.e., social interaction

    anxiety, fear of overt evaluation, fear of attracting attention);

    however, use of the total score provides optimal sensitivity andspecificity for discerning clinical and nonclinical samples (i.e., a

    cut-off score of 21 can typically be used to distinguish persons

    reporting clinically significant social distress). The internal

    consistency was acceptable for the total score (a = .96) and the

    average inter-item correlation was .63.

    Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded Form (PANAS-X;

    Watson & Clark, 1994; Watson, Clark, & Harkness, 1994). The

    PANAS-X is a 60-item measure which assesses the extent to which

    respondents have experienced each of 60 words and phrases that

    describe differentfeelingsand emotions withinthe past few weeks

    (e.g., cheerful or nervous). There is a strong convergence

    between trait and state indices of affect when using the PANAS-X,

    suggesting that the PANAS-X reflects trait levels of affect ( Watson

    & Clark, 1994). Each item is measured on a 5-point Likert scale,ranging from 0 (very slightly/not at all) to 4 (extremely). There is

    support for the construct validity of the PANAS-X scales,

    particularly the convergence of the negative affect and positive

    affect scales with measures of neuroticism and extraversion,

    respectively (for a review, see Watson, 2000). The internal

    consistency was acceptable for both the negative affect scale

    (a = .91) and the positive affect scale (a = .89). The average scale

    inter-item correlations were .50 for negative affect and .45 for the

    positive affect.

    2.3. Analyses

    Response differences between women and men on each

    subscale were assessed with t-tests. Pearson correlations werecalculated for each of the measured variables to provide

    indications of directions for subsequent regression analyses.

    Hierarchical regression analyses were performed with each of

    the SADS total score and the SIPS total score as dependent

    variables.Boelen and Reijntjes (2009)placed IU in the final step of

    their hierarchical regression to evaluate whether it would account

    for any additional variance beyond neuroticism/negative affectiv-

    ity, FNE, and AS (total score), and their results supported IU as

    accounting for significant variance beyond these measures;

    however, IU, AS, and FNE have been posited to represent

    fundamental underlying cognitive constructs (Reiss & McNally,

    1985; Taylor, 1999), with IU as a potentially fundamental cognitive

    construct underlying all anxiety disorders (Carleton, Sharpe, et al.,

    2007). In contrast, negative affect and positive affect have been

    posited as overarching emotional traits and states (Watson, 2000;

    Watson & Clark, 1994; Watson et al., 1994).

    Two series of regression analyses were performed. First, the IUS

    total score was entered as block 1 of the independent variables,

    with theASI-3totalscoreentered as block 2, the BFNE-II entered as

    block 3, and the negative affect and positive affect subscales of the

    PANAS-X entered as block 4. Second, the IUS-12 subscales were

    entered as block 1 of the independent variables, with the ASI-3

    subscalesentered as block 2, the BFNE-II entered as block 3, andthe

    negative affect and positive affect subscales of the PANAS-X

    entered as block 4. In this fashion, the regression model tested the

    variance accounted for by each construct overall and each of the

    construct dimensions in a linear fashion congruent with existing

    theory. To be thorough, model results from the analyses wherein

    the independent variables were entered in reverse order are also

    presented in parentheses to delineate the variance IU predicts

    beyond negative affect, positive affect, FNE, and the ASI-3

    subscales.

    The last analyses required that participants from the sample be

    selected based on whether they reported symptoms consistent

    with a probable diagnosis of SADor GAD, or both. Participantswere

    selected using very conservative cutoff scores as recommended for

    the SIPS (i.e., a cutoff score of 30; Carleton et al., 2009) and the

    GAD-7 (i.e., a cut-off score of 15; Spitzer et al., 2006). This resultedin four groups of participants who reported (1) SA symptoms

    consistent with people diagnosed with SAD (without clinically

    significant GAD symptoms), (2) GAD symptoms consistent with

    people diagnosed with GAD (without clinically significant SA

    symptoms), (3) SA and GAD symptoms consistent with people

    diagnosed with both SAD and GAD, and (4) SA and GAD symptom

    levels lower than would be consistent with people diagnosed with

    SAD or GAD. Thereafter, IU total and subscale scores were

    compared across the four groups using analysis of variance

    (ANOVA) and Scheffepost hoc comparisons to determine whether

    differences existed in IU levels between persons with clinically

    significant SAD, GAD, both disorders, or neither disorder.

    3. Results

    3.1. Descriptive statistics

    Descriptive statistics for each dependent variable are presented

    in Table 1. None of the indices of univariate skewness and kurtosis

    in the clinical sample were sufficiently out of range to preclude the

    planned analyses (Curran, West, & Finch, 1996; Tabachnick &

    Fidell, 2001). Men and women were comparable on most subscales

    (ps > .05); however, men reported higher scores on the PANAS-X

    positive affect scale, t(284) = 2.65, p < .01, r2 = .02, while women

    reported higher scores on the GAD-7, t(284) = 2.14,p < .05, r2 = .02.

    In both cases the effect sizes were quite small (Cohen, 1988);

    accordingly, men and women were not analyzed separately. The

    correlation analyses suggested small to large relationshipsbetween each of the variables (Table 1).

    3.2. Regression analyses

    The regression results indicated a robust relationship between

    the IUS-12 total score and the SIPS total score, independent of all

    other independent variables and similar to the precedent results

    from Boelen and Reijntjes (Table 2). The regression results also

    indicated a robust relationship between the inhibitory anxiety

    subscale of the IUS-12 and the SIPS total score, independent of

    all other independent variables and comparable with the ASI-3

    social subscale and the BFNE-II (Table 2). There were no

    indications of problems with multicollinearity (i.e., all toler-

    ances>

    .30 and all variance inflation factors .30 and all

    variance inflation factors< 4.00), problems with outliers, pro-

    blems with normality, or problems with homoscedasticity

    (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). In this case, the inhibitory anxiety

    subscale of the IUS-12 accounted fora third of the variance in SADS

    scores, which was less than with the SIPS, but still the largest

    portion for the SADS. For the SADS, the absence of positive affect

    (but not negative affect) was a statistically significant predictor of

    SADS scores beyond IU, AS, and FNE. When the order of entry for

    the independent variables was reversed, negative affect and

    positive affect accounted for a fifth of the variance in SADS scores

    (Table 2); however, IU, AS, and FNE each continued to contribute

    statistically significant amounts to the variance accounted for. To

    further explore the association with IU, a subsequent regressionwas performed (details not shown) wherein the BFNE-II was

    entered as block 1, with the IUS-12 subscales in block 2, the ASI-3

    subscales in block 3, and negative and positive affect in block 4.

    Table 1

    Descriptive statistics.

    Correlations

    M(SD) S(.14) K(.29) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

    1. IUS-12 total score 30.69 (11.07) .50 .46 1.00

    2. IUS-12 prospective

    anxiety subscale

    1 9.45 (6.5 8) .25 .71 .94

    3. IUS-12 inhibitory anxiety subscale 11.24 (5.32) .73 .41 .91 .73

    4. ASI-3 total score 19.14 (14.38) .94 .26 .71 .60 .725. ASI-3 somatic subscale 5.39 (5.33) 1.04 .31 .50 .43 .52 .83

    6. ASI-3 cognitive subscale 4.72 (5.41) 1.30 .87 .59 .50 .61 .86 .62

    7. ASI-3 social subscale 9.03 (6.20) .57 .66 .69 .59 .71 .85 .53 .59

    8. BFNE-II 23.77 (14.98) .03 1.29 .59 .50 .61 .65 .43 .49 .71

    9. PANAS-X negative affect 23.60 (8.93) .60 .32 .60 .52 .61 .58 .41 .50 .55 .49

    1 0. PANAS-X positive affe ct 2 7.77 (8.2 2) .14 .69 .29 .26 .29 .26 .20 .22 .23 .28 .25

    11. SIPS total score 18.00 (14.56) .82 .17 .69 .59 .71 .68 .45 .54 .72 .68 .54 .28

    12. SADS total score 52.84 (24.76) .14 .77 .54 .48 .54 .44 .23 .32 .54 .49 .35 .36 .78

    NoteS: Skew (standard error); K: kurtosis (standard error). All correlations were statistically significant (p< .01); IUS-12: intolerance of uncertainy-12; ASI-3: anxiety

    sensitivity index-3; BFNE-II: brief fear of negative evaluation scale-II; PANAS-X: positive and negative affect scale; SIPS: social interaction phobia scale; SADS: social

    avoidance and distress scale.

    Table 2

    Regression results for SIPS.

    Model step Constant Coefficient statistics Correlations Model step statistics (IVs in reversed order)

    b t p r Part r DR2 DF p

    Total scores

    2.23

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S0887618509002102-main

    5/7

    Results indicated that the BFNE-II accounted for 24% of the

    variance in SADS score, whereas in the previous analysis the

    inhibitory anxiety subscale of the IUS-12 accounted for 30% of the

    variance (seeTable 3).

    3.3. Group comparisons

    The results of the ANOVA indicated statistically significant

    differences based on symptomgroupsfor the IUS-12 total score, F(3,

    282) = 61.46, p < .001, eta2 = .40, the IUS-12 prospective anxiety

    subscale score, F(3, 282) = 34.81,p < .001, eta2 = .27, andthe IUS-12

    inhibitory anxiety subscale score F(3, 282) = 75.55, p < .001,

    eta2 = .45. The substantially different sample sizes within each ofthe symptoms groups, while not prohibitive for ANOVA, does make

    meeting theassumption of homogeneityof variance moreimportant

    (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001); however, even if violated, a correction

    can be made using appropriately discriminating post hoc tests and

    an increasingly stringent alpha (Judd, McClelland, & Culhane, 1995;

    Tabachnick & Fidell,2001). Theassumption of homogeneity wasmet

    forthe IUS-12 totalscore (p > .10) and prospective anxiety subscale

    (p > .10). In contrast, there was a slight variance for the IUS-12

    inhibitory anxiety subscale (p= .03). Nevertheless, use of Scheffe

    post hoc tests and the large effect sizes suggest that the differences

    are likely to be robust (Table 4). Overall, people with symptoms

    consistent with both GAD and SAD reported higher IU levels than

    people with symptoms consistent with either SAD or GAD alone

    (who reported comparableIU levels),who inturn,reported higher IUlevels than people who reported symptoms that were consistent

    with neither SAD nor GAD.

    4. Discussion

    The current study had three goals. The first was to replicate the

    previously demonstrated relationship between IU and SA (Boelen

    & Reijntjes, 2009) with data from a North American community

    sample. The second goal was to extend those findings to include

    measures of social interaction and performance anxiety, social

    avoidance and distress, negative and positive affect, as well as

    psychometrically stable measures and subscales of IU and AS. The

    third and final goal paralleled previous research comparing IU

    levels across GAD and panic disorder with agoraphobia samples

    (Dugas et al., 2005) by comparing levels of IU across participants

    reporting symptoms consistent with probable diagnoses of SAD,GAD, both disorders, or neither disorder.

    The results of the correlation analyses demonstrated significant

    interrelationships between all of the variables of interest and in

    theoretically congruent directions (e.g., positive affect was

    negatively correlated with SA,whereas FNE andSA were positively

    correlated). These results are in accordance with growing research

    indicating a relationship between SA, FNE (Weeks et al., 2005), AS

    (Orsillo, Lilienfeld, & Heimberg, 1994), negative affect/ neuroticism

    (Barlow, 2002), and IU (Boelen & Reijntjes, 2009). The current and

    more specific analysis demonstrated that within AS and IU, the

    subscales are differentially associated with SA, whether measured

    as a function of social interaction and performance anxiety or

    social avoidance and distress.

    The first series of regression results supported a robustrelationship between IU and SA as measured by the SIPS and the

    SADS, independent of all other variables andsimilar to theprecedent

    Table 3

    Regression results for SADS.

    Model step Constant Coefficient statistics Correlations Model step statistics (IVs in reversed order)

    b t p r Part r DR2 DF p

    Total scores

    5.54

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S0887618509002102-main

    6/7

    results from Boelen and Reijntjes (2009). The second series of

    hierarchical regression analyses predicting either social interaction

    and performance anxiety, or social avoidance and distress, provided

    separate quantifications of the differential associations demon-

    strated in the correlation analysis. IU inhibitory anxiety accounted

    for more than half (51%) of the variance in social interaction and

    performance anxiety, withthe ASI-3 social subscale and the BFNE-II

    accounting for statistically significant, but smaller portions of

    variance (i.e., 9% and 3%, respectively). In this case, negative affect

    and positive affect were not statistically significant predictors of

    social interaction and performance anxiety, together accounting for

    less than 1% of the variance. When the order was reversed and

    negative affect and positive affect were entered first, the constructs

    accountedfor a statistically significantamount of thevariance(32%),

    whichwas much lessthanIU accountedfor when it wasentered first.

    Moreover, in thereverseorder analysis IU continuedto account for a

    statistically significant amount of the variance (4%) above and

    beyond negative affect, positive affect, FNE, and the AS fear of

    socially observable anxiety symptoms.

    IU inhibitory anxiety accounted for a third (30%) of the variance

    in social avoidance and distress; however, this was notably less

    than the variance accounted for in social interaction and

    performance anxiety. The ASI-3 social subscale and BFNE-II again

    accounted for statistically significant, but smaller, portions ofvariance (6% and 1%, respectively). In this case reduced positive

    affect but not negative affect accounted for an additional

    statistically significant portion of variance (4%). This suggests that

    the absence of positive affect (Watson, 2000; Watson & Clark,

    1994; Watson et al., 1994) may be related to social avoidance and

    distress. When the order was reversed and negative affect and

    positive affect were entered first, the constructs accounted for a

    statistically significant amount of the variance (20%) that was less

    than IU accounted forwhen it was entered first. Moreover, IU again

    accounted for a statistically significant amount of the variance (4%)

    above and beyond negative affect, positive affect, FNE, and the AS

    fear of socially observable anxiety symptoms.

    Overall, the results of the regression analyses support a robust,

    independent relationship between SA and inhibitory anxietyassociated with IU. There was also evidence of a robust relationship

    betweenSA andthe fearof socially observable anxietysymptoms,as

    well as SA and fears of being negatively evaluated. There was no

    evidence of a relationship between SA and worrying about future

    uncertainty (i.e., prospective anxiety), fears of somaticsensations, or

    fears of cognitive dyscontrol. In contrast to previous research

    (Boelen & Reijntjes, 2009), the relationship between negative affect

    and SA was equivocal at best because, although SA may result in

    negative affect or the absence of positive affect, neither negative

    affect nor positive affect predicted SA beyond cognitive constructs

    such as IU,or ASI-3 social, or FNEin this sample. The results suggest

    that the initially posited, tested, and theoretically congruent

    (Carleton, Sharpe, et al., 2007; Taylor, 1999; Watson, 2000)

    hierarchical ordering withIU as underlying andnegative affectandpositive affect as overarching constructs better reflects the social

    interaction and performance anxiety data from this sample;

    however, the directional influence of negative and positive affect

    on social avoidance and distress remains unknown. Positive affect

    mayserve a protectivefunctionagainst SA;alternatively,lowerfears

    of the uncertainty of social situations, socially observable anxiety

    symptoms, and fears of negative evaluation may facilitate the

    development of positive affect.

    The comparisons across the symptom groups suggest that there

    are differences in IU levels between persons with a probable

    diagnosis of SAD, GAD, neither, or both disorders. People reporting

    SAD and GAD symptoms well below levels reported by people

    meeting diagnostic criteria for either reported IU levels statistically

    significantly lower than persons reporting symptoms congruent

    with diagnoses of either or both SAD and GAD. People reporting

    symptoms comparable to those reported by people diagnosed with

    either SAD or GAD reported comparable levels of IU. This suggests

    that differences wherein people with GAD reported higher IU

    levels than people with panic disorder with agoraphobia (Dugas et

    al., 2005) may not be pervasive across all anxiety disorders. People

    reporting symptoms consistent with both SAD and GAD reported

    statistically significantly higher levels of IU than all other groups.

    These results contribute to a growing body of literature suggesting

    that IU may be a fundamental component of several anxiety

    disorders, and further evidence that IU may vary across anxiety

    disorders, being more pertinent for some anxiety disorders relative

    to others.

    There are several limitations to the current study that provide

    directions for future research. First, there was no diagnostic

    information available for the current sample. There may be

    important differences associated with people who have been

    formally diagnosed with SAD or GAD and their responses to the

    variables measured in the current study. Future researchers should

    explore these variables across diagnosed clinical samples, particu-

    larly persons with SAD. Moreover, future research should consider

    including a measure of IU as a treatment outcome measure among

    persons treated for SAD to determine whether reductions in SA

    symptoms correspond with reductions in IU levels. Alternatively,researchers might explore whether targeted reductions in IU levels

    result in reductions in SA symptoms, even in the absence of

    treatments targeting SA. Second, the structure of the current

    regression analyses suggests a linear hierarchical relationship

    between IU, AS, FNE, negative and positive affect, and SA that

    remains untested with longitudinal data. Although current theory

    supports cognitive constructs as determinant bases for affective

    constructs (Barlow, 2002), future researchers might attempt to

    complete prospective studies to better determine causal relation-

    ships, even if those relationships are reciprocal. Third, the Social

    PhobiaInventory(SPIN; Connoret al.,2000) usedpreviously(Boelen

    & Reijntjes, 2009) was notavailable in the current data. Accordingly,

    it is possible that neuroticism is a significant component of a

    dimensionofSAmeasuredbytheSPINbutnottheSIPSortheSADS.Itis also possible that negative affect and neuroticism are sufficiently

    differentor that the measure of neuroticism used in the previous

    study (i.e., the Shortened Eysenck Personality Questionnaire

    Neuroticism subscale;Eysenck, Eysenck, & Barrett, 1985) and the

    PANAS-X are sufficiently different that the relationship was not

    replicablewith thecurrentdata. Based onprevious research withthe

    PANAS-X (Watson, 2000), this possibility is unlikely; however,

    future researchers might test this possibility before ruling out a

    robust independent relationship between negative affect, neuroti-

    cism, and SA.

    Theresults of this study support a robustrelationship betweenIU

    and SA that is independent of AS, FNE, negative affect, and positive

    affect. Indeed, the inability to tolerate the uncertainty associated

    with social situations may be a critical element in the developmentand maintenance of SAD. Although yet untested, treatments which

    focus on increasing tolerance for the uncertainty inherent in social

    situations may provide help in relieving SAD symptoms.

    Acknowledgements

    K.C. Collimore is supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health

    Research doctoral grant (FRN # 85321).

    References

    Anderson, E. R., & Hope, D. A. (2009). The relationship among social phobia, objectiveand perceived physiological reactivity, and anxiety sensitivity in an adolescent

    population. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 1826.

    R.N. Carleton et al./ Journal of Anxiety Disorders 24 (2010) 189195194

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S0887618509002102-main

    7/7

    Antony, M. M, & Rowa, K. (2008). Social anxiety disorder. Ashland, OH:Hogrefe & HuberPublishers.

    Antony, M. M., & Swinson, R. P. (2000).The shyness and social anxiety workbook: proventechniques for overcoming your fears. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications.

    Asmundson, G. J. G., & Stein, M. B. (1994). Selective processing of social threat inpatients with generalized social phobia: evaluation using a dot-probe paradigm.

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 8, 107117.Ball, S. G., Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., Uccello, R., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (1995). Differ-

    entiating social phobia and panic disorder: a test of core beliefs. Cognitive Therapyand Research, 19, 473481.

    Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Boelen, P. A., & Reijntjes, A. (2009). Intolerance of uncertainty and social anxiety.Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 130135.Carleton, R. N., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2009). The multidimensionality of fear of pain:

    construct independence for the fear of pain questionnaire-short form and the painanxiety symptoms scale-20. The Journal of Pain, 10, 2937.

    Carleton, R. N., Collimore, K. C., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2007). Social anxiety and fear ofnegative evaluation: construct validity of the BFNE-II. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,

    21, 131141.Carleton, R. N., Collimore, K. C., Asmundson, G. J., McCabe, R. E., Rowa, K., & Antony, M.

    M. (2009). Refining and validating the social interaction anxiety scale and thesocial phobia scale. Depression and Anxiety, 26, E71E81.

    Carleton, R. N., McCreary, D. R., Norton, P. J., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2006). Brief fear ofnegative evaluation scale-revised. Depression and Anxiety, 23, 297303.

    Carleton, R. N., Norton, M. A., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2007b). Fearing the unknown: ashortversion of the intoleranceof uncertaintyscale.Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21,105117.

    Carleton, R. N., Sharpe, D., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2007c). Anxiety sensitivity andintoleranceof uncertainty:requisites of the fundamentalfears? Behaviour Researchand Therapy, 45, 23072316.

    Cohen, J. (1988).Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences(2nd ed.). Mawah,NJ: Erlbaum.

    Collimore, K. C., McCabe, R. E., Carleton, R. N., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2008). Mediaexposure and dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: differential associations withPTSD symptom clusters. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 10211028.

    Connor, K. M., Davidson, J. R. T., Churchill, L. E., Sherwood, A., Foa, E., & Weisler, R. H.(2000). Psychometric properties of the social phobia inventory (SPIN): new self-rating scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 379386.

    Curran, P. J., West, S. G., & Finch, J. F. (1996). The robustness of test statistics tononnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. PsychologicalMethods, 1, 1629.

    Dugas, M. J.,Gosselin,P., & Landouceur,R. (2001). Intoleranceof uncertaintyand worry:investigating specificityin a nonclinicalsample. CognitiveTherapyand Research,25,551558.

    Dugas, M. J., Marchand, A., & Ladouceur, R. (2005). Further validation of a cognitive-behavioral model of generalized anxiety disorder: diagnostic and symptom spe-cificity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19, 329343.

    Eysenck, S. B., Eysenck, H. J., & Barrett, P. (1985). A revised version of the psychoticismscale. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 2129.

    Freeston, M., Rheaume, J., Letarte, H., Dugas, M. J., & Ladouceur, R. (1994). Why dopeople worry? Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 791802.

    Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004). Should we trust web-basedstudies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about internet question-naires.American Psychologist, 59, 93104.

    Gosselin, P., Ladouceur, R., Evers, A., Laverdiere, A., Routhier, S., & Tremblay-Picard, M.(2008). Evaluation of intolerance of uncertainty: development and validation of anew self-report measure. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 14271439.

    Greco, V., & Roger, D. (2001). Coping with uncertainty: the construction and validationof a new measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 519534.

    Greco, V., & Roger, D. (2003). Uncertainty, stress and health.Personality and IndividualDifferences, 34, 10571068.

    Heydayati, M., Dugas, M. J., Buhr, K., & Francis, K. (2003). The relationship betweenintolerance of uncertainty and the interpretation of ambiguous and unambiguousinformation. Paper presented at the annual convention of the association foradvancement of behaviour therapy.

    High, A. C., & Caplan, S. E. (2009). Social anxiety and computer-mediated communica-tion during initial interactions: implications for the hyperpersonal perspective.

    Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 475482.Holaway, R. M., Heimberg, R. G., & Coles, M. E. (2006). A comparison of intolerance ofuncertainty in analogue obsessive-compulsive disorder and generalized anxietydisorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 158174.

    Judd, C. M., McClelland, G. H., & Culhane, S. E. (1995). Data analysis: continuing issues inthe everyday analysis of psychological data.Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 433465.

    Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Personalityand Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 371375.

    Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of measures of socialphobiascrutinyfear and socialinteraction anxiety. Behaviour Researchand Therapy,

    36, 455470.Miller, M. B. (1995). Coefficient alpha: a basic introduction from the perspectives of

    classical test theory and structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Model-ing, 2, 255273.

    Orsillo, S. M., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Heimberg, R. G. (1994). Social phobia and response tochallenge procedures: examining the interaction between anxiety sensitivity andtrait anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 8, 247258.

    Peterson, R. A., & Reiss, S. (1992).Anxiety sensitivity index manual(2nd ed.). Worthing-ton, OH: International Diagnostic Systems.Rapee,R. M.,& Heimberg,R. G.(1997). A cognitive-behavioralmodel ofanxiety insocial

    phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 741756.Rapee, R. M., & Medoro, L. (1994). Fear of physical sensations and trait anxiety as

    mediators of the response to hyperventilation in nonclinical subjects. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 103 , 693699.

    Rector, N. A., Szacun-Shimizu, K., & Leybman, M. (2007). Anxiety sensitivity within theanxiety disorders: disorder-specific sensitivities and depression comorbidity.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 19671975.

    Reiss,S.,& McNally,R. J.(1985). Theexpectancy modelof fear.In: S.Reiss& R.R. Bootzin(Eds.),Theoretical issues in behaviour therapy (pp. 107121). New York, NY: Aca-demic Press.

    Riskind, J. H., Tzur, D., Williams, N. L., Mann, B., & Shahar, G. (2007). Short-termpredictive effects of the looming cognitive style on anxiety disorder symptomsunder restrictive methodological conditions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45,17651777.

    Simmons, A., Matthews, S. C., Paulus, M. P., & Stein, M. B. (2008). Intolerance ofuncertainty correlates with insula activation during affective ambiguity. Neu-

    roscience Letters, 430, 9297.Spitzer,R. L.,Kroenke,K., Williams,J. B.,& Lowe, B.(2006).A briefmeasure forassessing

    generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Archives of Internal Medicine, 166, 10921097.

    Stein,M. B.,Jang,K. L.,& Livesley,W. J.(1999). Heritabilityof anxiety sensitivity:a twinstudy.American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 246251.

    Swinson, R.P. (2006). TheGAD-7scalewas accurate fordiagnosing generalised anxietydisorder.Evidence-Based Medicine, 11, 184.

    Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). New York,NY: Harper and Row.

    Taylor, S. (1993). The structure of fundamental fears. Journal of Behavior Therapy andExperimental Psychiatry, 24, 289299.

    Taylor, S. (Ed.). (1999). Anxiety sensitivity: theory, research, and treatment of the fear ofanxiety.Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Taylor, S., Koch, W. J., Woody, S., & McLean, P. (1996). Anxiety sensitivity and depres-sion: how are they related?Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 474479.

    Taylor, S., Zvolensky, M. J., Cox, B. J., Deacon, B., Heimberg, R. G., Ledley, D. R., et al.(2007). Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initialvalidation of the anxiety sensitivity index-3. Psychological Assessment, 19,

    176188.Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Watson, D.,& Clark,L. A.(1994). The PANAS-X:manual for the positiveand negative affect

    schedule-expanded form. Iowa City, IA: Watson and Clark.Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Harkness, A. R. (1994). Structures of personality and their

    relevance to psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 1831.Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of

    Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 448457.Weeks, J. W., Heimberg, R. G., Fresco, D. M., Hart, T. A., Turk, C. L., Schneier, F. R., et al.

    (2005). Empirical validation and psychometric evaluation of the brief fear ofnegative evaluation scale in patients with social anxiety disorder. Psychological

    Assessment, 17, 179190.Weeks,J. W.,Heimberg, R.G., & Rodebaugh,T. L.(2008a).The fear of positive evaluation

    scale: assessing a proposed cognitive component of social anxiety. Journal ofAnxiety Disorders, 22, 4455.

    Weeks, J. W., Heimberg, R. G., Rodebaugh, T. L., & Norton, P. J. (2008b). Exploring therelationship between fear of positive evaluation and social anxiety. Journal of

    Anxiety Disorders, 22, 386400.

    Zinbarg, R. E., Barlow, D. H., & Brown, T. A. (1997). Hierarchical structure and generalfactor saturation of the anxiety sensitivity index. Psychological Assessment, 9, 277284.

    Zvolensky, M. J., Kotov, R., Antipova, A. V., Leen-Feldner, E. W., & Schmidt, N. B. (2005).Evaluating anxiety sensitivity, exposure to aversive life conditions, and proble-matic drinking in Russia: a test using an epidemiological sample. Addictive Beha-viors, 30, 567570.

    R.N. Carleton et al./ Journal of Anxiety Disorders 24 (2010) 189195 195