visual acuity and contrast sensitivity for individual ......

7
vision Res., Vol. 37, No. 6, pp. 813–819, 1997 Pergamon Q 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved PII: S0042-6989(96)00190-3 Printed in Great Britain 0042-6989/97 $17.00 + 0,00 Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity for Individual Sloan Letters KENNETH R. ALEXANDER,*~ WEI XIE,* DEBORAH J. DERLACKI* Received 23 January 1996; in revisedform 22 May 1996 Sloan letter optotypes are used frequently to evaluate visual impairment, and scoring procedures have been developed that are based on the numbers of letters that are identified correctly. However, previous studies have presented conflicting evidence regarding the relative identifiability of the individual Sloan letters. To investigate this issue further, we measured psychometric functions for the identification of each of the 10 Sloan letters, with individual letters presented in random order on the gray-scale display of a Macintosh computer-based testing system. Data were obtained from three visually normal subjects under each of three conditions: (1) as a function of log contrast at a relatively large letter size; (2) as a function of log contrast at a letter size near the acuity limit; and (3) as a function of log MAR (minimum angle of resolution) at maximum letter contrast. Estimates of threshold log contrast and threshold log MAR were derived from best-fitting Weibull functions. Threshold log contrast for small letters showed the greatest interletter variability. There was relatively little interletter variability in either threshold log contrast for large letters or threshold log MAR for high-contrast letters. However, due to the relatively steep psychometric functions under these latter two conditions, the different Sloan letters had considerably different percent correct values near threshold. The overall pattern of results suggests that the contrast sensitivity functions for individual Sloan letters are displaced laterally along a log MAR axis, while their vertical positions are essentially equivalent. @ 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. Visualacuity Contrastsensitivity Letters CSF Fovea INTRODUCTION Letter identification is used frequently in the clinical assessment of spatial vision, including the measurement of visual acuity at high contrast (Ferris et al., 1993) and low contrast (Regan & Neima, 1983), as well as the measurement of contrast sensitivity using large (Pelli et al., 1988)and small (Rabin, 1994a,b)letter sizes. Scoring procedures have been proposed that are based on the number of individual letters that are identified correctly (e.g. Bailey et al., 1991;Elliott et al., 1991).However, an unresolved question is whether all letters are equally identifiablein these tests. The identifiability of any given letter depends to a certain extent on the letter set of which it is a member. While several letter sets have been employed in clinical tests, one that is commonly used is the set of 10 Sloan letters (NAS-NRC, 1980), which are the subject of the present investigation. Sloan letters were originally *Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of Illinois at Chicago College of Medicine, 1855 W. Taylor Street, Chicago, IL 60612, U.S.A. ‘tTo whom all correspondence should be addressed IEmail [email protected]]. chosen to provide nearly equivalent percent correct values for visual acuity (Sloan et al., 1952), but there is general agreement that the Sloan letters with curved features (especially C and O) have lower legibility in acuity tests (Sloan et al., 1952; Bennett, 1965; Strong & Woo, 1985;Ferris et al., 1993;Reich et al., 1994).There is less agreementabout the relative identifiabilityof large Sloan letters. Robson et al. (1990) reported that there were only minor differences among the various Sloan letters on the Pelli–Robson Contrast Sensitivity Chart. However, Elliott et al. (1990) concluded that there were pronounced differences in Sloan letter identifiabilityon this chart, with C being the most difficultletter to identify correctly. One possible resolution for these apparently contra- dictory findings is that the relative identifiability of individual Sloan letters may depend on the specific method that is used to assess identifiability.One method has been to determine the percent correct value for each letter at a near-threshold level, an approach that has traditionally been applied to the measurement of visual acuity (e.g. Sloan et al., 1952; Bennett, 1965; Elliott et al., 1990;Ferris et al., 1993).A second method, used less frequently, is to measure the thresholds for individual letters as derived from psychometric functions (Robson 813

Upload: lydung

Post on 27-May-2018

221 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity for Individual ... 1988)andsmall(Rabin,1994a,b)lettersizes.Scoring ... letters on the Pelli–Robson Contrast Sensitivity Chart. However,Elliott

vision Res., Vol. 37, No. 6, pp. 813–819, 1997

Pergamon Q1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reservedPII: S0042-6989(96)00190-3 Printed in Great Britain

0042-6989/97 $17.00 + 0,00

Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity forIndividual Sloan LettersKENNETH R. ALEXANDER,*~ WEI XIE,* DEBORAH J. DERLACKI*

Received 23 January 1996; in revisedform 22 May 1996

Sloan letter optotypes are used frequently to evaluate visual impairment, and scoring procedureshave been developed that are based on the numbers of letters that are identified correctly. However,previous studies have presented conflicting evidence regarding the relative identifiability of theindividual Sloan letters. To investigate this issue further, we measured psychometric functions forthe identification of each of the 10 Sloan letters, with individual letters presented in random orderon the gray-scale display of a Macintosh computer-based testing system. Data were obtained fromthree visually normal subjects under each of three conditions: (1) as a function of log contrast at arelatively large letter size; (2) as a function of log contrast at a letter size near the acuity limit; and(3) as a function of log MAR (minimum angle of resolution) at maximum letter contrast. Estimatesof threshold log contrast and threshold log MAR were derived from best-fitting Weibull functions.Threshold log contrast for small letters showed the greatest interletter variability. There wasrelatively little interletter variability in either threshold log contrast for large letters or thresholdlog MAR for high-contrast letters. However, due to the relatively steep psychometric functionsunder these latter two conditions, the different Sloan letters had considerably different percentcorrect values near threshold. The overall pattern of results suggests that the contrast sensitivityfunctions for individual Sloan letters are displaced laterally along a log MAR axis, while theirvertical positions are essentially equivalent. @ 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Visualacuity Contrastsensitivity Letters CSF Fovea

INTRODUCTION

Letter identification is used frequently in the clinicalassessmentof spatial vision, including the measurementof visual acuity at high contrast (Ferris et al., 1993) andlow contrast (Regan & Neima, 1983), as well as themeasurement of contrast sensitivityusing large (Pelli etal., 1988)and small (Rabin, 1994a,b)letter sizes.Scoringprocedures have been proposed that are based on thenumber of individual letters that are identifiedcorrectly(e.g. Bailey et al., 1991;Elliott et al., 1991).However,anunresolved question is whether all letters are equallyidentifiablein these tests.

The identifiability of any given letter depends to acertain extent on the letter set of which it is a member.While several letter sets have been employed in clinicaltests, one that is commonly used is the set of 10 Sloanletters (NAS-NRC, 1980), which are the subject of thepresent investigation. Sloan letters were originally

*Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University ofIllinois at Chicago College of Medicine, 1855 W. Taylor Street,Chicago, IL 60612, U.S.A.

‘tTo whom all correspondence should be addressed [email protected]].

chosen to provide nearly equivalent percent correctvalues for visual acuity (Sloan et al., 1952),but there isgeneral agreement that the Sloan letters with curvedfeatures (especially C and O) have lower legibility inacuity tests (Sloan et al., 1952;Bennett, 1965; Strong &Woo, 1985;Ferris et al., 1993;Reich et al., 1994).Thereis less agreementabout the relative identifiabilityof largeSloan letters. Robson et al. (1990) reported that therewere only minor differences among the various Sloanletters on the Pelli–Robson Contrast Sensitivity Chart.However, Elliott et al. (1990) concluded that there werepronounced differences in Sloan letter identifiabilityonthis chart,with C being the most difficultletter to identifycorrectly.

One possible resolution for these apparently contra-dictory findings is that the relative identifiability ofindividual Sloan letters may depend on the specificmethod that is used to assess identifiability.One methodhas been to determine the percent correct value for eachletter at a near-threshold level, an approach that hastraditionally been applied to the measurement of visualacuity (e.g. Sloan et al., 1952; Bennett, 1965; Elliott etal., 1990;Ferris et al., 1993).A second method,used lessfrequently, is to measure the thresholds for individualletters as derived from psychometric functions (Robson

813

Page 2: Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity for Individual ... 1988)andsmall(Rabin,1994a,b)lettersizes.Scoring ... letters on the Pelli–Robson Contrast Sensitivity Chart. However,Elliott

814 K. R. ALEXANDER et al.

et al., 1990). These two methods may lead to differentconclusions, depending on the nature of the underlyingpsychometric functions for letter identification. Forexample, if the psychometric functions for individualletters are all relatively steep but are closely spaced, thenthe different Sloan letters may have nearly identicalthresholdsbut quite different percent correct values forstimuli that are near threshold.The data of Homer et al.(1985) suggest that, at least for visual acuity, psycho-metric functions for letter identificationdo indeed haverelativelysteep slopes, althoughtheir study examinedtheslope of psychometric functions for sets of letters ratherthan for individual letters. A primary purpose of thepresent study was to determine whether this type ofmethodological difference accounts for the apparentdisagreements among previous studies as to the relativeidentifiability of Sloan letters. To investigate thisquestion, we measured psychometric functions for theidentificationof the individualSloan letters.

It is also possible that the identifiabilityof the variousSloan letters depends on the angular subtense of theletters. It has often been assumedthat letter identificationis based on a constantrange of object spatialfrequencies,ca 1–3 cycles per letter, regardless of letter size (e.g.Regan et al., 1981;Legge et al., 1985;Solomon & Pelli,1994). Based on this assumption,the properties of letteridentification should be the same across letter angularsubtense, since the same band of object spatial frequen-cies would be involved at all letter sizes. However, wehave demonstrated recently that the object spatialfrequencies that are used for letter identificationtend tovary with letter angular subtense,shiftingto lower objectspatial frequencies as the angular subtense of the lettersdecreases toward the acuity limit (Alexander et al.,1994).Because the informationcontentof lettersvariesatdifferent object spatial frequencies (Parish & Sperling,1991; Nasanen et al., 1993; Hall et al., 1996), a shift inthe critical band of object spatial frequencieswith letterangularsubtenseimpliesthat the relativeidentifiabilityofthe various Sloan letters may be different at small andlarge letter sizes. This would be consistentwith previousreports that confusions among block letters (Gervais etal., 1984) and lower-case letters (Bouma, 1971) tend tovary with the angular subtense of the letters. A secondpurpose of our study, then, was to determinewhether therelative identifiability of the individual Sloan lettersvaries with the angular subtense of the letters.

METHODS

Subjects

Three subjects with normal vision participated in thestudy. Two subjects (S1 and S2), female, ages 23 and28 yr, respectively, had minimal experience in psycho-physicalexperimentsand were naive as to the purposeofthe study. The third subject (S3), male, age 47 yr, hadconsiderable experience in psychophysical experimentsand was familiar with the purpose of the study. The logminimumangleof resolution(MAR)valuesof S1,S2,and

S3were –0.03, –0.05, –0.20, respectively,as measuredwith a LighthouseDistance Visual Acuity Test, and theirrefractive errors were –1.00 + 0.25 x 95, –9.00 + 3.25x 85, and –4.75, respectively.

Stimuli

Test stimuli consisted of the 10 Sloan letters,constructed according to published guidelines (NAS-NRC, 1980) and identical to those used in our previousstudies (e.g. Alexander et al., 1994).The smallest letterswere constructedfrom a 15x 15 pixel array, with a letterstroke width of 3 pixels. Larger letters were constructedfrom proportionally larger pixel arrays that varied insteps of 0.1 log unit, with the largest letters derived froma 400x 400 pixel array. Letters were generated by anApple MacintoshIIfx microcomputerand were presentedindividually on an Apple high-resolution gray-scalemonitor that had a P4 phosphor, a vertical scan rate of66.67Hz, and a resolution of 640x 480 pixels.

Three test conditionswere used:

1. Threshold log contrast for Sloan letters with a logMAR value of 1.3 (20/400 Snellen equivalent),presented at a test distance of 2.7 m;

2. Threshold log contrast for Sloan letters with logMAR valuesof either 0.2 [20/32Snellen equivalent,(Sl and S2)]or 0.1 [20/25 Snellen equivalent, (S3)],presented at a test distance of 7.2 m; and

3. Threshold log MAR (i.e. visual acuity) for high-contrast Sloan letters presented at a test distance of7.2 m.

Each letter was presented in the center of a rectangularadapting field that was displayed continuously through-out the session and that subtended 1.7 deg horizontallyand 1.3 deg vertically at a viewing distance of 7.2 m (or4.5 deg by 3.4 deg at a viewing distanceof 2.7 m), with aluminance of 1.9 log cd/m2 as calibrated with a SpectraSpotmeter.

The stimulusdisplaywas viewed monocularlythrougha photometer with a best refractive correction, and a 2-mm artificial pupil was used to control the retinalilluminance of the stimuli. The display monitor, whichwas the only source of illuminationin the test area, wasplaced behind the subjects, with stray light shielded byblack cloth, and the letterswere viewed in a front-surfacemirror. Stimulus Iuminanceswere controlled by an ISRVideo Attenuator and Video Toolbox software, asdescribed by Pelli and Zhang (1991). Linearized colorlookup tables that were loaded during the video retraceperiods defined the pixel luminance for each videoframe.

The stimulus contrast was modulated by a D6 (sixthderivative of a Gaussian) temporal waveform at atemporal frequency of 2 Hz, in order to limit the targetduration as well as to restrict its temporal frequencycontent.Accordingto ourpreviousdata (Alexanderet al.,1994), results for letter identification obtained at thistemporal frequency should not differ substaritiallyfromthose obtained at lower temporal frequencies (longer

Page 3: Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity for Individual ... 1988)andsmall(Rabin,1994a,b)lettersizes.Scoring ... letters on the Pelli–Robson Contrast Sensitivity Chart. However,Elliott

ACUITY AND CONTRAST SENSITIVITY FOR LE’fTERS 815

.-

90 -

80 -

70 -

60 -

50 -

40 -

30

20 -2

u=0ARASOv

10 ~.A*

o -s Om; m

–1 .8 – 1.2 –0.6 0.0 -0.6 0.0

Log Contrast Log MAR

FIGURE 1. Psychometric functions for letter identification for S3 for the 10 Sloan letters, as indicated in the legend. Theleftmost data set represents percent correct vs log contrast for letters with a log MAR value of 1.3 (Snellen equivalent of20/400). The middle data set represents percent correct vs log contrast for letters with a log MAR value of 0.1 (20/25 Snellenequivalent). The rightmost data set represents percent correct vs log MAR for letters of maximum contrast. Solid curves

represent least-squares best fits of Weibull functions [Eq. (3)].

target durations). Temporal frequencyas:

up= <3/(7rat)

(COP)was defined

(1)

where at is a time constant(Swansonet al., 1984).Lettercontrast (C) was defined according to the Weberdefinition:

c = (LT– L~)/L~ (2)

where LT and LB were the luminance of the target andbackground, respectively. At the point of maximumcontrast during stimulus presentation, the stimuli hadnegative contrast (i.e. LT < LB). The temporal character-istics of the stimuli were confirmed by a photocell andoscilloscope.

Procedure

Subjects were first given a brief practice series inwhich they were required to identify individual Sloanletters presented at a variety of sizes and contrasts, inorder to familiarize them with the Sloan letter set. Initialestimates of the threshold for log contrast and log MARwere then determined with 10-alternativeforced-choicestaircases, using a two-down, one-up decision rule. Thisrule provides an estimate of the 71% correct point on apsychometricfunction(Levitt, 1970).Next, a log contrastor log MAR value near the initial thresholdestimatewaschosen, and 200 trials (20 trials per letter)were presentedat that log contrast or log MAR value, with letterspresented in random order. A brief warning tonepreceded each stimulus presentation. Subjects wereinstructed to identify verbally which letter had beenpresented,and to guess if they were uncertain.They werealso informed that all letters would be presented equallyoften and were instructed to try to equalize theirresponses across letters. Only responses from the Sloanletter set were accepted. The interstimulus interval wastypically 2–3 see, during which time the subject’s

responsewas entered into the computer by the examinerand the next stimuluswas generated.

Additional log contrast and log MAR values werepresented in separate sessions,with log contrast and logMAR varying in 0.1 log unit steps, until completepsychometric functions were obtained for each letter.Threshold estimates for each letter were obtained fromleast-squaresbest fits of a Weibull function:

P = 1 – (1 – g) exp[–lOb(x-’)] (3)

where P is the percent correct at a given log contrast (x),g = UN (where N is the number of Sloan letters), b is aparameter that describes the slope of the Weibullfunction, and tis the threshold log contrast (Pelli et al.,1988).

RESULTS

Psychometric functions for the identification of thevarious Sloan letters are illustrated for subject S3 in Fig.1. The psychometric functions were shallowest in thecase of percent correct vs log contrast for small letters(middle data set), and the data showed considerabledispersionunder this condition.The functionswere muchsteeperfor percent correct vs log contrastfor large letters(left data set) and for percent correct vs log MAR (rightdata set), and the data were more tightly clustered underthese latter two conditions. The mean values of thederived slope parameter b for subject S3 were 2.5, 6.8,and 6.9 for small letters, large letters, and log MAR,respectively.A similarpattern of results,with shallowestslopes for small letters, was obtained from the othertwo subjects (mean values of b for small letters, largeletters, and log MAR were 3.5, 5.1, and 6.8 for subjectSl, and 2.7, 6.0, and 10.0 for subject S2). The letterconfusion matrices for each condition were examinedfor evidence of response bias using X2 tests, but no

Page 4: Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity for Individual ... 1988)andsmall(Rabin,1994a,b)lettersizes.Scoring ... letters on the Pelli–Robson Contrast Sensitivity Chart. However,Elliott

816 K. R. ALEXANDER et al.

CD HKNORSVZ-1.2 ~ I

t

AA

–1 .5AAA AA ‘AA

-1.8 Is,

–2.1 i

–1 .2+m0b –1 .5 -Ac AA AA AAA AA

z– 1.8 -w 52

3–2. 1

I–1.5 .AA

AAA

A AAA

– I.8 1t ‘3 I

.2,1 ~CD HKNORSVZ

FIGURE 2. Plots of the best-fit parameter log t(threshold log contrast)for S1, S2, and S3 for the Sloan letters indicated on the abscissa; all

letters had a log MAR value of 1.3.

discernible patterns of response bias were apparentamong the three subjects.

Values of log t (representing thresholdsfor either logcontrast or log MAR) were then derived for each letterfrom the fits of the Weibull functions.Figures 2–4 showthe derived values of threshold log contrast for largeletters, threshold log contrast for small letters, andthreshold log MAR, respectively. Each graph in eachfigurerepresentsthe resultsfor an individualsubject.The

CD HKNORSVZ0.0

s,I

0.0*(n S20s –0.3 -0 ●c ●

●●

: ● *-0.6 -

m b

3–0.9

0.0S3

–0.3 -** ●

● ●● **

–0.6 -●

–o.9-CD HKNORSVZ

FIGURE 3. Plots of the best-fit parameter log t(threshold log contrast)for S1, S2, and S3 for the Sloan letters indicated on the abscissa; letters

had log MAR values of either 0.2 (Sl, S2) or 0.1 (S3).

CD HKNORSVZ0.6, 7

Is,

0.3

0.0 ■ ■ ■ . . ■ ■ ■ .■

\–0.3 I I

0.6 , 1

–0.3 I J

0.6S5

0.3

0.0 -~mmmwwmw

m~–0.3

CD HKNORSVZ

FIGURE 4. Plots of the best-fit parameter log t(threshold log MAR)for S,, S2, and S3 for the Sloan letters indicated on the abscissa.

ordinates are scaled equivalently in all three figures inorder to facilitate comparisonsacross conditions.

For large letters (Fig. 2), the interletter variability inthresholdlog contrastwas relatively low for all subjects.The range of threshold values was 0.12, 0.08, and0.15 log units for subjects Sl, S2 and S3, respectively.Interlettervariabilityin thresholdlog contrastwas greaterfor small letters (Fig. 3). Under this condition, the rangeof thresholdvalues was 0.25, 0.38, and 0.33 log units forsubjectsS1,S2,and S3,respectively.Interlettervariabilityin the values of log MAR (Fig. 4) was similar to that forthreshold log contrast for large letters. The range ofthreshold values for log MAR was 0.17, 0.12, and0.14 log units for subjects S1, S2,and S3, respectively.

For threshold log MAR (Fig. 4), the pattern of resultsfor the various Sloan letters was similar among the threesubjects. That is, there were statistically significantcorrelations among the subjects (S1 vs S2, r = 0.70,P < 0.05;S~vs S~,r = 0.90,P < 0.01; Szvs S~,r = 0.70,P < 0.05). The Sloan letters with angular features(particularly K, N, V, and Z) tended to have the lowestthreshold log MAR values, while letters with curvedfeatures (especially O and C) tended to have the highestthreshold log MAR values. In the case of threshold logcontrast for small letters (Fig. 3), the results werecorrelated significantly in two comparisons (S1 vs S3,r = 0.74, P < 0.05; S2 VS S3,r = 0.91, P < 0.01) but notin the third comparison (S1vs S2, r = 0.60, P = n.s.). Inthe case of thresholdlog contrastfor large letters (Fig. 2),there were no significantcorrelationsbetween any of thesubjects (S1vs S2, r = 0.02, P = n.s.; SJ vs S3, r = 0.54,P = n.s.; Sa vs S3, r = 0.12, P = n.s.).

An alternative way of viewing the interrelationshipsamong the thresholddata is to replot the thresholdvaluesin the format of letter contrast sensitivityfunctions.Thisis illustrated for subject S2 in Fig. 5. The data points in

Page 5: Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity for Individual ... 1988)andsmall(Rabin,1994a,b)lettersizes.Scoring ... letters on the Pelli–Robson Contrast Sensitivity Chart. However,Elliott

ACUITY AND CONTRAST SENSITIVITY FOR LE’ITERS 817

1,8

1,5

% 1,201..z: 0.9

m~ 0,6

I

(J,~

‘“”~– 1.5 – 1.2 –0.9 –0.6 –0.3 0.0 0.3

– Log MAR

FIGURE 5. Threshold log contrast and threshold log MAR values forSloan letters O and Z for S2, replotted from Figs 2+ in the format ofcontrast sensitivity functions. Solid curves represent the least-squaresbest fits of a negative exponential [Eq. (4)] fit to the data for each letter.

Fig. 5 representthe values of log t for Sloan letters O andZ, replotted from Figs 2-4. The curves represent least-squares best fits of a negative exponential equation thatwe have used previously to describe letter contrastsensitivityfunctions (Alexander et al., 1992):

~ = ~e(–P(W’W) (4)

where .s represents sensitivity; and a and p are scalingparametersrepresentingvertical and horizontalpositions,respectively, on log–log coordinates.These curves werefit to the three data points for each letter. The curves forSloan letters O and Z differed primarily in the values oflogp (differenceof 0.09 log unit),while the valuesof loga were nearly equivalent (difference of 0.01 log unit).That is, the pattern of results for letters O and Z can berepresented as primarily horizontal displacements of acontrast sensitivityfunction.

If this is the case in general, then there shouldbe a highcorrelationbetween thresholdlog MAR for high-contrastletters and threshold log contrast for small letters, sincethese are nearly orthogonal measures of performance.Conversely,thresholdlog contrastfor large letters shouldnot be correlated to any substantialdegree with either ofthese two measures. In agreement with this expectation,threshold log MAR for high-contrast letters was corre-lated significantlywith threshold log contrast for smallletters for all three subjects (r= 0.83, 0.87, and 0.69, forsubjects Sl, S2, and S3, respectively; P < 0.05) whilethreshold log contrast for large letters was not correlatedsignificantly with threshold log MAR for any subject(r= 0.02, 0.50, and 0.25, for subjects S,, S,, and S,,respectively;P = n.s.). Furthermore, threshold log con-trast for large letterswas not correlatedwith thresholdlogcontrast for small letters for either subject S1 or S2(r= 0.12 and 0.21, respectively; P = n.s.). However,these two measures were correlated significantly forsubject S3 (r = 0.64; P < 0.05), which indicates that, forS3,the contrast sensitivityfunctionsfor individualSloan

letters were displaced somewhat vertically as well ashorizontally.

DISCUSSION

The primary aim of this study was to reconcileapparent discrepanciesamong previous reports as to therelative identifiabilityof the various Sloan letters. Ourresults confirmour initial supposition:conclusionsaboutthe relative identifiability of Sloan letters depend onwhether the assessmentis based on percentcorrectvaluesfor stimuli near threshold or on thresholdsper se. In thecase of contrast sensitivityfor large letters (Fig. 1, left),the psychometric functions for the various Sloan letterswere relatively steep and closely spaced. As a result,there could be fairly large differences in the percentcorrect values for stimuli near threshold, in agreementwith the findings of Elliott et al. (1990), but littlevariation in the threshold log contrast values for theindividual letters (Fig. 2), which is consistent with thereport of Robson et al. (1990). The psychometricfunctions for log MAR at high contrast were similarlysteep and closely spaced (Fig. 1, right), so a similarconsiderationapplies in this case. That is, there could befairly large differencesin the valuesof percent correct forthe different Sloan letters near threshold, consistentwithprevious studies (e.g. Sloan et al., 1952; Bennett, 1965;Strong & Woo, 1985; Ferris et al., 1993), whiledifferences in the values of threshold log MAR for thevarious Sloan letters were relatively small (Fig. 4).

A second aim of this study was to determine whetherthe pattern of Sloan letter identifiabilityvaried with lettersize. Our results indicate that interletter variability inthreshold log contrastwas substantiallygreater for smallletters than for large letters, but the pattern of thresholdsfor the individualletters was similar at the various lettersizes. For example, Sloan letters with curved features,particularly O and C, tended to produce the highestvalues of threshold log MAR and threshold log contrast,while letterswith angularfeatures,particularlyZ, had thelowest threshold values. This agrees well with previousreports (e.g. Sloan et al., 1952;Bennett, 1965; Strong &WOO,1985; Elliott et al., 1990; Ferris et al., 1993), inwhich estimates of relative interletter variability werebased on percent correct values for near-thresholdlettersrather on thresholdsper se.

The psychometricfunctions for threshold log contrastfor small letters (Fig. 1, middle) were shallower than foreither threshold log contrast for large letters or forthreshold log MAR for high-contrast letters. Blommaertand Timmers (1987)also observeda relative flatteningofletter psychometric functions for contrast at small lettersizes, although their psychometric functions were basedon the use of an alphabetrather than on individualletters.From indirect evidence, they proposed that the psycho-metric functions for individual letters had a constantslope across letter sizes, but that there was a greaterinterlettervariability in the position of the psychometricfunctionsat small letter sizes, so that the group functionhad a shallowerslope. However, our results indicate that

Page 6: Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity for Individual ... 1988)andsmall(Rabin,1994a,b)lettersizes.Scoring ... letters on the Pelli–Robson Contrast Sensitivity Chart. However,Elliott

818 K. R. ALEXANDER et al.

the psychometric functions for the individual letters infact have shallower slopes at small sizes.

The overall pattern of findings in our study isconsistentwith the hypothesisthat the contrastsensitivityfunctions for the individual Sloan letters are shiftedlaterally along a log MAR axis, while their verticalpositions are approximatelythe same, as is illustrated inFig. 5. According to this model, small changes in the logMAR values of the letters can produce relatively largechanges in contrast sensitivityat small letter sizes, owingto the relatively steep slope of the contrast sensitivityfunction.This is likely to contributeto the shallowslopesand relatively broad separation of the psychometricfunctions for small letters (Fig. 1, middle). This modelcan also accountfor the greater variability in small-lettercontrastsensitivitythan in visual acuity, as observedbothin our study (Fig. 3 vs Fig. 4) and in clinical testing(Rabin, 1994a,b).

These lateral shifts in the contrast sensitivityfunctionsfor individualSloan letters are likely due to two factors.The first factor is that, as noted in the Introduction,letteridentification is based on different object spatialfrequencies at different letter sizes (Alexander et al.,1994).For example, as the angular subtenseof the lettersdecreases toward the acuity limit, letter identificationtends to be based on lower object spatialfrequencies.Thesecond factor is that the spectral power of the differentSloan letters varies differentially across object spatialfrequency bands (Parish & Sperling, 1991; Nasanen etal., 1993; Hall et al., 1996). As a consequence, theinformation content of letters varies with letter angularsubtense,leading to differencesin the contrast sensitivityfunctions for the various Sloan letters.

Of necessity,the data for the differenttest conditionsinour study were collected across several sessions,whichlikely contributeda certain amountof noise to the results.In addition, although we did not find any markedresponse biases among the subjects, any slight tendencytoward a response bias for certain letters could haveaffected threshold estimates. However, these sources ofvariability are likely to be relatively minor, as indicatedby the overall agreement in the pattern of results amongthe three subjects, as well as by the consistencybetweenour results and those of previous studies.

Althoughthe resultsof the present studywere obtainedunder controlled conditions, using subjects trained withthe Sloan letter set, nevertheless our results haveimplications for clinical testing. Performance on lettercharts depends to a large extent on the probability ofidentifyingindividual letters correctly.As shown by ourdata, as well as by those of previousstudies (Sloan et al.,1952;Bennett, 1965;Strong & Woo, 1985;Elliott et al.,1990; Fen-is et al., 1993), the probability of correctidentificationcan vary widely among the different Sloanletters. In clinical testing,however, a relevant questionishow much these differences in the probabilityof correctidentification will actually affect measures of contrastsensitivity and visual acuity. In the case of small-lettercontrast sensitivity, we observed that the range of

threshold differences among Sloan letters was ca0.3 log unit (Fig. 3). This interletter variability inthreshold log contrast is likely to contribute to the highdegree of variability that has been observed clinically insmall-lettercontrast sensitivity(Rabin, 1994a,b).

However, we found lower variability in the thresholdsfor individual letters in the case of large-letter contrastsensitivityand high-contrastlog MAR. For example, therange of differences in threshold log contrast for largeletters was ca 0.12 log unit (Fig. 2), and the range ofdifferences in threshold log MAR was ctz 0.14 log unit(Fig. 4). These ranges correspond approximately to oneline on clinical charts (0.15 log unit for the Pelli–Robsonchart and 0.1 log unit for log MAR charts). Furthermore,these rangesare similar in magnitudeto estimatesof test–retest reliabilityfor contrast sensitivityand visual acuity.For example, the 95% confidence limits for a change incontrast sensitivity on the Pelli–Robson chart wasestimated as 0.2 log unit (Elliott et al., 1991). Thecoefficient of repeatability on the Ferris visual acuitychart has been estimated as 0.12 log MAR (Elliott &Sheridan, 1988), and as 0.14 log MAR on a computer-based simulationof this chart (Arditi & Cagenello,1993).It is likely that differences in the thresholds among theSloan letters contributed to this intertest variability.However, this is unlikely to be the entire explanation,sinceexactlythe same chartswere used in both initialandrepeat testing in two of these studies (Elliott & Sheridan,1988; Elliott et al., 1991). Therefore, it is not clear atpresent whether choosinga letter set with less variabilityin thresholds will improve test–retest reliability in theclinical setting.

In conclusion, we have found that variability amongthe variousSloan letterswas greatest in the case of small-letter contrast sensitivity. For large-letter contrastsensitivityand visual acuity, there could be considerabledifferences in the probability of correct identificationofletters at near-thresholdvalues, but the actual thresholdvalues were quite similar across letters. The pattern offindings suggests that the contrast sensitivity functionsfor the various Sloan letters are displaced laterally alonga log MAR axis, while overall contrast sensitivity isnearly equivalent among Sloan letters.

REFERENCES

Alexander, K. R., Derlacki, D. J. & Fishman, G. A. (1992). Contrastthresholds for letter identification in retinitis pigmentosa. Investiga-tive Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 33, 18461852.

Alexander, K. R., Xie, W. & Derlacki, D. J. (1994). Spatial-frequencycharacteristics of letter identification. Journal of tire Optical Societyof America A, 11, 2375–2382.

Arditi, A. & Cagenello, R. (1993). On the statistical reliability of letter-chart visual acuity measurements. Investigative Ophthalmology andVisual Science, 34, 120-129.

Bailey, I. L., Bullimore, M. A., Raasch, T. W. & Taylor, H. R. (1991).Clinical grading and the effects of scaling. Investigative Ophthal-mology and Visual Science, 32, 422432.

Bennett, A. G. (1965). Ophthalmic test types. British Journal ofPhysiological Optics, 22, 23%271.

Blommaert, F. J. J. & Timmers, H. (1987). Letter recognition at lowcontrast levels: effects of letter size. Perception, 16, 421432.

Page 7: Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity for Individual ... 1988)andsmall(Rabin,1994a,b)lettersizes.Scoring ... letters on the Pelli–Robson Contrast Sensitivity Chart. However,Elliott

ACUITY AND CONTRAST SENSITIVITY FOR LETTERS 819

Bouma, H. (1971). Visual recognition of isolated lower-case letters.Vision Research, 11, 459-474.

Elliott, D. B., Bullimore, M. A. & Bailey, I. L. (1991). Improving thereliability of the Pelli–Robson contrast sensitivity chart. ClinicalVision Sciences, 6, 471-475.

Elliott, D. B. & Sheridan, M. (1988). The use of accurate visual acuitymeasurements in clinical anti-cataract formulation trials. O@thal-mic and Physiological Optics, 8, 397401.

Elliott, D. B., Whitaker, D. & Bonette, L. (1990). Differences in thelegibility of letters at contrast threshold using the Pelli–Robsonchart. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 10, 323–326.

Ferris III, F. L., Freidlin, V., Kassoff, A., Green, S. B. & Milton, R. C.(1993). Relative letter and position difficulty on visual acuity chartsfrom the Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study. AmericanJoarnal of Ophthalmology, 116, 735–740.

Gervais, M. J., Harvey, L. O. & Roberts, J. O. (1984). Identificationconfusions among letters of the alphabet. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology (Human Perceptual Performance), 10, 655–666.

Hall, H. L., Miller, J. M., Greivenkamp, J. E. & Schwiegerling, J. T.(1996). Prediction of identification difficulty of Sloan letters basedon spatial frequency content. Investigative Ophthalmology andVisaal Science (Suppl.), 37, S1076.

Homer, D. G., Paul, A. D., Katz, B. & Bedell, H. E. (1985). Variationsin the slope of the psychometric acuity function with acuitythreshold and scale. Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics,62, 895–900.

Legge, G. E., Pelli, D. G., Rubin, G. S. & Schleske, M. M. (1985).Psychophysics of reading—I. Normal vision. Vision Research, 25,239–252.

Levitt, H. (1970). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics.Journal of the Acoustical Socie~ of America, 49, 467-477.

Nasanen, R., Kukkonen, H. & Rovamo, J. (1993). Spatial integration ofband-pass filtered patterns in noise. Vision Research, 33, 903–911.

NAS-NRC (1980). Recommended standard procedures for the clinicalmeasurement and specification of visual acuity: Report of WorkingGroup 39. Advances in Ophthalmology, 41, 103–148.

Parish, D. H. & Sperling, G. (1991). Object spatial frequencies, retinalspatial frequencies, noise, and the efficiency of letter discrimination.Vision Research, 31, 1399–1415.

Pelli, D. G., Robson, J. G. & Wilkins, A. J. (1988). The design of a new

letter chart for measuring contrast sensitivity. Clinical VisionSciences, 2, 187–199.

Pelli, D. G. & Zhang, L. (1991). Accurate control of contrast onmicrocomputer displays. Vision Research, 31, 1337–1350.

Rabin, J. (1994a) Optical defocus: differential effects on size andcontrast letter recognition thresholds. Investigative Ophthalmologyand Visual Science, 35, 646&648.

Rabin, J. (1994b) Luminance effects on visual acuity and small lettercontrast sensitivity. Optometry and Vision Science, 71, 685–688.

Regan, D. & Neima, D. (1983). Low-contrast letter charts as a test ofvisual function. Ophthalmology, 90, 1192–1200.

Regan, D., Raymond, J., Ginsburg, A. & Murray, T. J. (1981). Contrastsensitivity, visual acuity and the discrimination of Snellen letters inmultiple sclerosis. Brain, 104, 333–350.

Reich, L. N., Bedell, H. E. & Feaster, J. M. (1994). Relative legibilityand confusions of Sloan letters in the fovea and peripheral retina.Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 35(Suppl.), 1951.

Robson, J. G., Pelli, D. G. & Zhang, L. (1990). Contrast sensitivity forletters: relative visibility of different letters. Investigative Ophthal-mology and Visual Science (Suppl.), 31, 186.

Sloan, L. L., Rowland, W. M. & Altman, A. (1952). Comparison ofthree types of test target for measurement of visual acuity. QuarterlyReview of Ophthalmology and Otorhinolaryngology, 8, 4–17.

Solomon, J. A. & Pelli, D. G. (1994). The visual filter mediating letteridentification. Nature, 369, 395–397.

Strong, G. & Woo, G. C. (1985). A distance visual acuity chartincorporating some new design features. Archives of Ophthal-mology, 103, 4446.

Swanson, W. H., Wilson, H. R. & Giese, S. C. (1984). Contrastmatching data predicted from contrast increment thresholds. VisionResearchj 24, 63–75.

Acknowledgements—This research was supported by Research GrantEY08301 from the National Eye Institute, National Institutes ofHealth, Bethesda, MD, by a grant from the Illinois Eye Fund,University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, and by a center grantfrom The Foundation Fighting Blindness, Baltimore, MD. We thankMarlos A. G. Viana for statistical advice.