Verbal control of delusions

Download Verbal control of delusions

Post on 13-Sep-2016

212 views

Category:

Documents

0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • BEHAVIOR THERAPy 21, 461-479, 1990

    Verbal Control of Delusions

    C. F. LOWE

    P. D. J. CHADWICK

    Department of Psychology University College of North Wales

    Bangor, UK

    The verbal descriptions or rules people formulate to describe both themselves and their interactions with others can profoundly influence their subsequent behavior and rule formation. Delusions may be thought of as being one class of rules. Two single- case experiments, each comprising a multiple-baseline design across three behaviors (delusions), are reported. Each experiment incorporated a number of measures of delu- sional thinking and an intervention designed to challenge these delusions and to pro- vide the clients concerned with alternative rules with which to organize their behavior. In both experiments the intervention proved highly effective in reducing the degree of conviction in the delusional beliefs, and there was some generalization of the effects across beliefs. A number of secondary measures helped to identify associated clinical benefits, and there was evidence that both clients used the alternative rules to regulate their behavior during a 6-month follow-up phase.

    Recent years have seen a growing awareness within behavior analysis and other areas of psychology of the importance of language or rules in the regu- lation of behavior. The process of learning to describe our environment alters our relation to it, because in so doing we organize and structure our subse- quent interactions (Lowe, 1979; 1983; Lowe, Horne, & Higson, 1987; Skinner, 1969; Vygotsky, 1962; 1978; Zettle & Hayes, 1982). In recognition of the potency of the regulatory function of language, a number of researchers have come to understand many clinical disorders as being maintained, at least in part, by the ways in which clients describe both themselves and their interactions with others (e.g., Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Beck, 1967; Hayes, 1987; Kelly, 1955). It seems plausible that delusional beliefs may also be main- tained, to some extent, in this way. Strauss (1969) has argued that delusional behavior is on a continuum function with normal behavior, the difference be- tween them being quantitative rather than qualitative. Delusions, like other strongly held beliefs, influence the way events are interpreted, and are simi- larly resistant to change (e.g., Maher, 1988). Indeed, because of this apparent continuity with other beliefs, the definition of what actually constitutes a de- lusion is somewhat problematic (see Garety, 1985).

    461 0005-7894/90/0461-047951.00/0 Copyright 1990 by Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy

    All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

  • 462 LOWE AND CHADWICK

    This study was conducted in 1988 while P.D.J. Chadwick was in receipt of a studentship funded by Clwyd Health Authority. Thanks are due to Mabel Tannahill for her help in setting up the research programme, to Peter Higson for help and guidance throughout and to many other staff of Clwyd Health Authority, all of whom contributed to the success of this project. Finally we should like to express our gratitude to the two individuals who participated as the main subjects of this study. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to C. F. Lowe, Depart- ment of Psychology, University College of North Wales, Bangor, Gwynedd LL57 2DG, United Kingdom.

    Attempts at modifying delusions in people diagnosed as schizophrenic have been scarce, and methodological limitations abound. Basic requirements have frequently been overlooked, such as baseline data to describe any ongoing variability, and long-term follow-up data to determine the permanence of any change; a study by Alford (1986) remains one of the few to have met both criteria. Also, treatments have often combined a number of different inter- ventions with little or no attempt at component analysis. Chadwick and Lowe (1990) used a multiple-baseline design across 6 people diagnosed as schizo- phrenic according to the criteria given in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), in order to evaluate the impact of two interventions on a number of dimensions of delusional behavior. The interventions were: first, a structured verbal chal- lenge, and second, a reality-testing intervention during which the client and researcher collaborated to devise a simple test of the rules (see Beck, 1967). The theoretical rationale for the verbal challenge came from the literature on verbal self-regulation within normal and clinical populations (Lowe, Horne, & Higson, 1987). In accordance with numerous clinical approaches to changing covert and overt verbal behavior, the interventions were designed: (1) to give the client information about the ways in which rules or beliefs can govern behavior, (2) to generate alternatives to the maladaptive rules, and (3) to assist the client to monitor and evaluate these alternative rules. Of the 6 clients, 2 rejected their beliefs completely and 3 others reported significant reductions in the degree of belief conviction. These effects were maintained over a 6-month follow-up period, and there was evidence to suggest that the intervention had enabled 5 of the clients to effectively regulate their delusional thinking.

    A multiple-baseline design can also be conducted across behaviors (Kazdin, 1982), and is appropriate in the cases of clients who hold more than one delu- sional belief. Our present study comprises two single-case experiments, each employing a multiple-baseline design across 3 beliefs. In both experiments, after a minimum of 5 weeks at baseline, a verbal challenge intervention was separately directed at each delusional belief in turn, with an intervention of at least four weeks between each intervention. A number of dimensions of delusional experience were measured throughout, and long-term follow-up data were collected. Because the verbal challenge intervention was applied to each belief separately, the study offered a unique opportunity to observe the rela- tionships between each client's three delusions and, in particular, how reduc- tion of conviction in one affected the other two beliefs.

  • VERBAL CONTROL OF DELUSIONS 463

    GENERAL METHOD Subject Selection

    As in our earlier study (Chadwick & Lowe, 1990) participation was on a voluntary basis; clients who had held a delusional belief for the previous 2 or more years were asked whether they were willing to meet with a researcher who wished to discuss their beliefs. Two clients took part, both of whom were outpatients on a stable drug regime. Medication was held constant during the study. Both clients were diagnosed as schizophrenic according to the Diag- nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III) criteria (Amer- ican Psychiatric Association, 1980); these diagnoses were made independently by P.D.J. Chadwick and a clinical psychologist. Each subject presented with 3 delusional beliefs; there was no other delusional thinking.

    Measures Following Brett4ones, Garety and Hemsley (1987) we measured both de-

    gree of belief conviction, and preoccupation, using a modified form of Shapiro's (1961) Personal Questionnaire (Phillips, 1977). A Personal Questionnaire (PQ) was also used to measure the degree of anxiety experienced by the client while thinking about his or her beliefs. We offered each client five statements of in- tensity of belief conviction, preoccupation, and anxiety, respectively, and both accepted these statements as valid descriptions. Table 1 details the specific wording used for each client and shows that, while the conviction measure was concerned with how certain the client was feeling at that particular point in time, the preoccupation and the anxiety statements referred to the level of

    TABLE 1

    THE WORDING USED FOR THE PERSONAL QUESTIONNAIRE MEASURES OF CONVICTION,

    PREOCCUPATION, AND ANXIETY

    Score Conv ic t ion P reoccupat ion Anx ie ty

    0

    My be l ie f is a lmost Over the last week I Th ink ing about my be l ie f

    def in i te ly fa lse thought about my I get very s l ight ly

    bel ie f once anx ious

    1 . . p robab ly fa lse . . . 3 t imes . . . s l ight ly anx ious

    2

    . . may or may not be t rue . . .onceaday . . . fa i r ly anx ious

    3

    . . p robab ly t rue . . . 4 t imes a day . . . very anx ious

    4

    . . a lmost def in i te ly t rue . . . once an hour . . . ex t remely anx ious

    5

    N .B . Persona l Quest ionna i re scores fal l between two verba l descr ip t ions ; one might respond

    more to p robab ly t rue but less to a lmost def in i te ly t rue.

  • 464 LOWE AND CHADWICK

    symptom intensity experienced over the preceding week. The five statements for each measure were written on separate cards and ranked by the client. At the administration stage each card was presented randomly, and the client was required to say whether the intensity was more or less than that stated on the card. The score on each occasion was provided by the number of cards to which the client replied that the symptom intensity was greater than that stated on the card. The scale allowed for comparisons to be made for each subject across time. Following Hole, Rush, and Beck (1979), we measured degree of belief conviction by asking the client for a percentage rating of conviction, and this was taken after the PQ conviction score These four measures were administered at the close of every session throughout the entire study.

    Again, in keeping with Brett-Jones et al. (1987), accommodation and reac- tion to hypothetical contradiction (RTHC) were assessed. The accommoda- tion measure was concerned with the awareness demonstrated by the client of actual occurrences that were contradictory to his or her belief, and with how these had affected the belief. Accommodation was measured at the start of every session by asking the client whether anything had happened over the past week to alter his or her belief in any way. RTHC was measured following accommodation at Weeks 2 and 4 of baseline to evaluate the client's potential for accommodation of evidence at odds with his or her belief. A plausible but contradictory occurrence was posed and the client was asked how, if at all, this would change the belief. In each experiment RTHC was conducted in the case of only one of the three beliefs.

    Two measures were included to cover at least some of the possible clinical ramifications of the loss or partial loss of a delusion. These were the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, 1967), and a short symptom checklist com- prising those items from Wing's Present State Examination (Wing, Cooper & Sartorius, 1974) describing the various forms of schizophrenic delusions and hallucinations. It should be stressed that the symptom checklist was em- ployed not in any diagnostic capacity, but solely for descriptive purposes. The BDI and symptom checklist were administered in the session prior to chal- lenging each of the three beliefs and in the final session of the intervention phase (i.e., in the last baseline session and on three occasions during the inter- vention phase) and at each follow-up.

    Procedure Sessions lasting approximately 90 min each were conducted once a week

    throughout the study. All interviews were conducted by the second author. Phase 1: Preliminary Interviewing. Two interviews were conducted with each

    client and these satisfied the dual purpose of ascertaining the belief to be modified and establishing a rapport.

    Phase 2: Baseline. Throughout the baseline we accumulated as much data as possible about the beliefs, and in particular the evidence, both past and present, that had helped to establish and maintain the beliefs. In the case of each belief, during the final baseline session the client was presented with every piece of evidence he or she had cited and was asked to rank them in order

  • VERBAL CONTROL OF DELUSIONS 465

    of importance to the belief system. At no point was any aspect of a belief challenged until it was subjected to the verbal challenge.

    Phase 3: Verbal challenge. Throughout the intervention the client was en- couraged to view a deluded belief as being only one possible interpretation of events. The discussion was conducted within an atmosphere of "collabora- tive empiricism" (Beck, 1967): the clients were not told that their beliefs were wrong, but were asked to suspend their conviction that the beliefs were un- deniably true and to consider alternatives. A non-confrontational approach of this kind has been supported by Milton et al. (1978). Initially, following Watts, Powell, and Austin (1973), we challenged the evidence for the belief in inverse order of importance. In each case the client's interpretation of events was countered by a non-deluded interpretation put forward by the researcher. An integral part of this discussion involved the researcher making clear to the client the way beliefs can exert a profound influence over the interpreta- tions placed upon events-that is, the client was made aware of the regulatory function of language (see Vygotsky, 1962).

    After discussing all the evidence for the belief, we challenged the belief it- self in three stages, which in practice overlapped. First, we drew attention to any inconsistency and irrationality within the client's belief system; this was tantamount to posing the question, "Would it make sense for things to be as you say they are?" We then presented an alternative explanation for what had been happening to the client. In accordance with the continuum view of delusions, and in deference to the growing dissatisfaction with the concept of schizophrenia (see Bentall, Jackson, & Pilgrim, 1988), a feature of the present research was that clients were not told that their beliefs were a sign of illness. Rather the beliefs were interpreted as having developed in reaction to, and as a way of making sense of, particular experiences that the clients had en- countered (see Maher & Ross, 1984; Maher, 1988). Finally, we argued that the available evidence offered strong support for the alternative interpretations.

    Phase 4" Follow-up. To assess for maintenance of behavior change, 1-month, 3-month, and 6-month follow-up meetings were conducted. At these sessions all the measures were administered (with the exception of RTHC) in the order and manner described earlier.

    Phase 5: Independent Assessment. An independent clinical psychologist who was kept blind with respect to the introduction of the invention interviewed the clients to assess their degree of conviction in each of their delusional be- liefs and to obtain their observations on the study. The principal procedural difference between Experiments 1 and 2 was the number and timing of these external assessments and, accordingly, this aspect of the procedure is specified separately for each experiment below.

    EXPERIMENT 1 Method

    Subject CP, a 29-year-old white British male, had a p...

Recommended

View more >