toward equal opportunities: fairness, values, and affirmative action programs in the u.s

5
Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 52, No. 4, 1996, pp. 93-97 Toward Equal Opportunities: Fairness, Values, and Affirmative Action Programs in the U.S. Weining C. Chang National University of Singapore Afirmarive action has not been enjoying widespread support in the United States. This commentary addresses the issues of why the policy of afirmative action is not supported by some Americans. Equity and equality are values cherished by many Americans. Afirmative action as a means toward equality is seen as at odds with the principle of equity and meritocracy. At the center of the problem is a “sense of fairness. Two issues may be involved in the judgment of fairness of afirmative action programs: (I) heightened salience of group identity and diferences and (2) perceived scarcity of resources. Action programs can be designed to address the issue of functional integration, therefore reducing the salience of group difer- ences. It is proposed that work force diversity should be seen as a viable means toward economic expansion, which will help address the issue of scarcity. Affirmative action is perhaps one of the most controversial policies in recent U.S. history. Growing out of the 1970s’ heightened awareness of civil rights of peoples who are “different,” the policy has not been enjoying widespread support in the U.S. (Kleugel & Smith, 1986; Lerner, 1980). Americans seem to agree with an abstract principle of “equality” but found the actual practice difficult to accept (Schuman, Steeh, & Bob, 1985). Affirmative action is a general policy, the operationalization of which seems to differ from organization to organization. A common threat in all of the affirmative action programs seems to focus on giving the “others ,” peoples who are traditionally underrepresented in “privi- leged” areas of society, education, employment, and public service oppor- tunities. Both the conceptualization and the implementation of the policy are based on perceived or real group identities or differences. Correspondence regarding this article may be sent to Dr. Weining C. Chang, Department of Social Work and Rychology, National University of Singapore, 10 Kent Ridge Crescent, Singapore 119260. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to [email protected]. 93 0022-4237/%/1200ao93so).oo/1 0 1996 me Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues

Upload: dr-weining-c-chang

Post on 26-Sep-2016

216 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Toward Equal Opportunities: Fairness, Values, and Affirmative Action Programs in the U.S

Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 52, No. 4 , 1996, pp. 93-97

Toward Equal Opportunities: Fairness, Values, and Affirmative Action Programs in the U.S.

Weining C. Chang National University of Singapore

Afirmarive action has not been enjoying widespread support in the United States. This commentary addresses the issues of why the policy of afirmative action is not supported by some Americans. Equity and equality are values cherished by many Americans. Afirmative action as a means toward equality is seen as at odds with the principle of equity and meritocracy. At the center of the problem is a “sense of fairness. ” Two issues may be involved in the judgment of fairness of afirmative action programs: ( I ) heightened salience of group identity and diferences and (2) perceived scarcity of resources. Action programs can be designed to address the issue of functional integration, therefore reducing the salience of group difer- ences. It is proposed that work force diversity should be seen as a viable means toward economic expansion, which will help address the issue of scarcity.

Affirmative action is perhaps one of the most controversial policies in recent U.S. history. Growing out of the 1970s’ heightened awareness of civil rights of peoples who are “different,” the policy has not been enjoying widespread support in the U.S. (Kleugel & Smith, 1986; Lerner, 1980). Americans seem to agree with an abstract principle of “equality” but found the actual practice difficult to accept (Schuman, Steeh, & Bob, 1985). Affirmative action is a general policy, the operationalization of which seems to differ from organization to organization. A common threat in all of the affirmative action programs seems to focus on giving the “others ,” peoples who are traditionally underrepresented in “privi- leged” areas of society, education, employment, and public service oppor- tunities. Both the conceptualization and the implementation of the policy are based on perceived or real group identities or differences.

Correspondence regarding this article may be sent to Dr. Weining C. Chang, Department of Social Work and Rychology, National University of Singapore, 10 Kent Ridge Crescent, Singapore 119260. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to [email protected].

93

0022-4237/%/1200ao93so).oo/1 0 1996 me Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues

Page 2: Toward Equal Opportunities: Fairness, Values, and Affirmative Action Programs in the U.S

94 Chang

Rokeach and Ball-Rokeach (1988) observed that the priorities Americans assign to equality, the central value behind affirmative action policies, as a terminal value, a preferred state of existence (Rokeach, 1973), fell throughout the 1970s. A direct causal link is difficult to establish but it seems that since the implementation of policies to “address past inequality,” public endorsement of equality as a desirable state seems to have been eroded. It appears that Americans do value equality as a principle but that social equality, enforced through pro- grams such as affirmative action, is seen to be at odds with other principles, such as meritocracy or fair competition-values highly cherished by the average American as the core spirit of the culture (De Tocqueville, 183W1969). In other words, equality is seen to have come at the expense of equity (Adams, 1963), which is found to be a favored principle in distributing of wealth and resources in the United States. At the heart of the matter seems to be a value conflict between equality and equity and the resulting sense of “fairness.”

Peterson ( 1994) explored the intricate relationship between perceived “fair- ness” and the value paradox between equality and meritocracy. Supported by empirical data and LISREL modeling, Peterson suggested that the perceived fairness would be the crucial factor in determining whether individuals would support or oppose affirmative action as a policy. Perceived fairness, in turn, is influenced by evaluation on the basis of a “fairness heuristic,” a general subjec- tive evaluation criterion used by the individual to make evaluative judgments (Lind, 1992). This heuristic may come from many sources; one of them is the subjective value systems of the individual. The subjective values of the individu- al are organized in a hierarchy of value preferences (Rokeach, 1973). Mer- itocracy and equality for all are two values of the Americans but, at different times in history and for different individuals, these two values are not necessarily of equal preference (Rokeach, 1973; Rokeach & Ball-Rokeach, 1988). Peter- son’s work, using paired comparison of different values, found that these values are somewhat negatively correlated-that is, for many Americans, equality and meritocracy are not seen as compatible to each other. Using affirmative action as a procedure would imply the redistribution of resources according to group or categorical identity. If resources are perceived to be scarce and limited, this redistribution is seen to be at the expense of some deserving members of society; therefore it is not “fair.” This commentary will attempt to address the potential reasons why affirmative action policies are sometimes not seen as fair.

Heightened Salience of Group Differences

Affirmative action, as a general policy, often involves a set of policies that share a common concern: the inclusion of categorical membership such as race and gender. Under this general policy, a diversity of action procedures have been designed and implemented by various educational and employment agencies (Nacoste, 1990). These affirmative action plans, though intending to provide

Page 3: Toward Equal Opportunities: Fairness, Values, and Affirmative Action Programs in the U.S

Qual Opportunities 95

equal opportunity, inadvertently call attention to ethnic categorization and differ- ences.

Social psychological literature abounds with evidence suggesting that sa- lience of group identity facilitates stereotyping and differential treatment (see, for instance, Tajfel, 1982). As Allport and current-day cognitive psychologists have observed, the human mind thinks in categories, and once formed, categories are the basis of normal judgment (Allport, 1954, p. 20). The weight of evidence seems to suggest that the most effective means of reducing prejudice is to eradi- cate group boundaries or at least reduce the salience of categories (Devine, 1995). Methods suggested include creating functional groups composed of mem- bers from previously different groups (see, for instance, Brewer, Ho, Lee, & Miller, 1987). When members of different groups are able to forge a superordi- nate group identity, the “them-ness” (outgroup) and “we-ness” (ingroup) concep- tualization will be reduced; it follows that intergroup discrimination and conflicts will correspondingly decrease.

In reality, however, an affirmative action appointee is labeled as a “special” employee and treated differently, either positively or negatively. This “otherness” perception will not be eradicated. Labeling the candidate will bring heightened salience of group identity to the candidate. This salience often interferes with processing of information of the individual by bringing forth the “illusory cor- relation’’ (Hamilton & Rose, 1980) between the group identity and potential merit or demerit of the candidate. Evaluation of performance and contribution made by the appointee has been more likely tinted by the highly salient eth- nidgender classification and the related political consideration. Affirmative ac- tion appointees are often seen as less qualified and less competent, and they are not seen to have “paid their dues” to justify the reward that comes with their appointments. Affirmative action is thus seen as unfair as it deprives the other candidates of a fair chance to compete. This would offend the sense of fairness of those who subscribe to the equity principle (Adams, 1963). Resentment can be incurred by the perceived special treatment of the affirmative action appointee.

Perceived Scarcity of Resources

Resentment on the part of other candidates is particularly intense when opportunities are scarce and the gain of the affirmative action appointee is seen as at the expense of other more competent candidates. In other words, the perceived resources and the perception of resources as a “zero-sum game” or as an “ex- panding pie” is of critical relevance here. Affirmative action is often seen not as a means to help expand the pie, but as a redistribution of existing-and in econom- ically hard times-limited resources. The appointee is often seen as a recipient of some kind of charity rather than deserving an opportunity to serve as a viable, contributing member to the economy. In other words, diversity of the work force is not seen as a means of economic expansion but as a social charity-that is, an

Page 4: Toward Equal Opportunities: Fairness, Values, and Affirmative Action Programs in the U.S

96 Chang

expendable luxury. If it has to come at the expense of others’ opportunities, then it is doubly unfair.

Affirmative action programs in the U.S., from this author’s point of view, can only serve as temporary measures to address past inequity. Our immediate attention in the U.S., then, should be focused on a fair and more acceptable practice of affirmative action.

Based on the prevailing values of the Americans, democracy and mer- itocracy, equity and equality, one can surmise that “fairness” means an equal opportunity to compete, an equal opportunity where individuals can exercise their efforts in realizing their potential and contribute to the community. Affirma- tive action programs, then, should be designed to provide equal opportunities to learn, work, and contribute.

The following recommendations may be helpful to ease the sense of “unfair- ness” in affirmative action programs: (1) Open and merit-based selection on affirmative action appointment. Affirmative action programs are designed to address past inequities; they are attempts to balance the ethnic proportions in the work force. As such, affirmative action programs should, at least in the short run, be focused on filling positions with qualified members of underrepresented popu- lations. To avoid criticism of filling positions with inferior, and therefore “unde- served” personnel, selection should be open and merit based. Here the emphasis is on the openness or the public and objective nature of the selection procedure. (2) Equity in evaluation: Once appointed, the organization should make sure the appointee is given the same challenges and evaluation as any other appointee. (3) Functional integration: More importantly, a mechanism for integrated social- ization should be provided so that the appointee is working alongside the others and suffers or enjoys “the same fate” (Triandis, 1989) as the others in order to be incorporated into the ingroup rather than remaining as an outsider. In other words, making the workforce diverse in its ethnic makeup. By making all able-bodied citizens contributing members is a viable and perhaps vital means to economic expansion whereby more opportunities will be created for the entire population.

References

Adams, J . S. (1963). Toward an understanding of inequity. Journal ofAbnorma1 and Social Psycholo-

Allpoa. G . W. (1954). The nature ofprejudice. Reading, M A Addison-Wiley. Brewer, M. B. , Ho, H., Lee, J., &Miller, N. (1987). Social identity and social distance among Hong

Kong school children. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 13, 156- 165. De Tbcqueville, A. (1969). Of individualism in democracies (G. Lawrence, Trans.). In J. P. Mayer

(Ed.), Democracy in America (Vol. 2, pp. 506-508). New York: Doubleday, Anchor Books. (Original work published 1835)

Devine, P. G. (1995). Prejudice and out-group perception. In A. Tesser (Ed.), Advanced social psychology (pp. 467-524). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Hamilton, D. L., & Rose, T. L. (1980). Illusory correlation and the maintenance of stereotype beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46,44-56.

gy, 67, 422-436.

Page 5: Toward Equal Opportunities: Fairness, Values, and Affirmative Action Programs in the U.S

Equal Opportunities 97

Kleugel, J. R., & Smith, E. R. (1986). Beliefs about inequity. New York: Aldine De Gruyter. Lerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world. New York: Plenum. Lind, E. A. (1992, March 28). The fairness heuristic: Rationality and “relationaliry” in procedural

evaluation. Paper presented at the Forth International Conference of the Society for the Advancement of Socio-Economics, Irvine, CA.

Nacoste, R. (1990). Sources of stigma. Law and Policy, 12, 175-195. Peterson, R. S. (1994). The role of values in predicting fairness judgments and support of affirmative

Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New Y o k Free Press. Rokeach, M., & Ball-Rokeach, S. J. (1988). Stability and change in American value priorities,

Schuman, H., Steeh, C., & Bob, L. (1985). Racial attitudes in America: Trends and interpreta-

Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup attitudes. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 1-39. Triandis, H. C. (1989). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and collectivism. In J. J. Bremen

(Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 37. Cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 41- 133). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

action. Journal of Social Issues, 50(4), 95-1 15.

1968-1981. American Psychologist, 44. 775-784.

tions. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

WEINING C. CHANG, born in Nanjing, China, was subsequently educated in Taiwan where she earned a law degree from National Taiwan University and in the U.S. where she got an M.A. and a Ph.D. in psychology from the University of Houston. Her interest in culture and ethnic minorities in the U.S. led her to do postdoctoral research at Harvard University’s Graduate School of Education. She is presently Associate Professor at the National University of Singapore. Her current interests are studies of cultures, values, and achievement motivation in Asian countries.