theory of learning, theory of teaching, and theory of mind · theory of teaching, and theory of...

22
© 2014 IAP All rights reserved Contemporary Perspectives on Research in Theory of Mind in Early Childhood Education, pages 269–290 Copyright © 2014 by Information Age Publishing All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. 269 CHAPTER 12 THEORY OF LEARNING, THEORY OF TEACHING, AND THEORY OF MIND How Social-Cognitive Development Influences Children’s Understanding of Learning and Teaching John Knutsen, Douglas Frye, and David M. Sobel One of the fundamental goals of research in cognitive development is to describe how children learn. Young children have powerful learning mechanisms for incorporating observed data with existing knowledge (see e.g., Griffiths, Sobel, Tenenbaum, & Gopnik, 2011). But young children also lack particular metacognitive abilities that prevent them from reflect- ing on how learning occurs or how beliefs change (e.g., Dunbar & Klahr, 1989; Klahr, 2000; Kuhn, 1989; Schauble, 1990, 1996) and possibly from introspecting in general (e.g., Flavell, Green, & Flavell, 1995). These dif- ficulties do not mean that young children cannot learn. Rather, they sug- gest that young children might engage in the process differently than older

Upload: lamnhu

Post on 06-Sep-2018

271 views

Category:

Documents


4 download

TRANSCRIPT

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

Contemporary Perspectives on Research in Theory of Mind in Early Childhood Education, pages 269–290Copyright © 2014 by Information Age PublishingAll rights of reproduction in any form reserved. 269

Chapter 12

Theory of LeArning, Theory of TeAching, And Theory of Mind

how Social-cognitive development influences children’s Understanding

of Learning and Teaching

John Knutsen, douglas frye, and david M. Sobel

One of the fundamental goals of research in cognitive development is to describe how children learn. Young children have powerful learning mechanisms for incorporating observed data with existing knowledge (see e.g., Griffiths, Sobel, Tenenbaum, & Gopnik, 2011). But young children also lack particular metacognitive abilities that prevent them from reflect-ing on how learning occurs or how beliefs change (e.g., Dunbar & Klahr, 1989; Klahr, 2000; Kuhn, 1989; Schauble, 1990, 1996) and possibly from introspecting in general (e.g., Flavell, Green, & Flavell, 1995). These dif-ficulties do not mean that young children cannot learn. Rather, they sug-gest that young children might engage in the process differently than older

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

270 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

children, reflect on their knowledge differently, and for the purposes of the present chapter, conceptualize the process of learning differently than older children.

At the same time, children’s teaching has been described as “natural cog-nition” (Strauss, 2002). Teaching is a common behavior children engage in from early on and is clearly related to children’s developing knowledge of their own and others’ mental states (e.g., Astington, & Pelletier, 1996; Strauss, Ziv, & Stein, 2002; Wellman & Lagattuta, 2004; Ziv & Frye, 2004; Ziv, Solomon, & Frye, 2008; although see Davis-Unger & Carlson, 2008, for an alternative view). Many of these investigations suggest that how children engage in teaching others and how they understand the process of teach-ing (i.e., who needs to be taught or how that teaching will take place) is a function of what children know about how their own and others’ beliefs are formed and change.

These two literatures for the most part have developed independently, and while both have seen significant advances in what we know about chil-dren’s learning and teaching, there is much to be gained from integrating these two approaches. This is the goal of this chapter. We will argue that what a child understands about the ways learning and teaching are accomplished are deeply dependent upon the child’s developing metacognitive knowledge.

We divide our review into three sections. First, we examine what chil-dren might know about learning as a mental process and how their under-standing of learning concepts potentially develops. Second, we investigate children’s developing understanding of teaching—when and how to com-municate information to others to provide them with information they did not previously possess. Finally, we consider how these concepts potentially develop in high-functioning children with Autism Spectrum Disorder, who are known to lack particular skills in social cognition.

chiLdren’S deveLoping concepTS of LeArning

What do young children know about the process of learning—the circum-stances under which ignorance is replaced by knowledge or knowledge changes based on information available in the environment? A good start-ing point for answering this question is what children know about their own knowledge, and there is evidence that such awareness is present at early ages. For instance, children use the word know in their spontaneous conversational utterances by age 3 (Shatz, Wellman, & Silber, 1983). Very young children also understand the difference between knowing and not knowing (e.g., Pillow, 1989; Pratt & Bryant, 1990). Between the ages of 3 and 5, children learn to recognize that beliefs can be false (e.g., Perner, Leekham, & Wimmer, 1987; Wimmer & Perner, 1983), the difference

[QA: There is no refer-ence for this citation.]

dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
[QA: There is no reference for this citation.] Correction- Please change reference to: "Strauss, Ziv, & Stein, 2002" Please add following to References: Strauss, S., Ziv, M., & Stein, A. (2002). Teaching as a natural cognition and its relations to preschoolers’ developing theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 17, 1473–1487.

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

theory of Learning, theory of teaching, and theory of Mind 271

between knowledge and ignorance (e.g., Hogrefe, Wimmer, & Perner, 1986), when their own beliefs have changed (e.g., Gopnik & Astington, 1988), and the difference between their own knowledge and that of an-other person (Wimmer, Hogrefe, & Perner, 1988). Moreover, during this age, children recognize that others’ false beliefs can arise from misconcep-tions about specific pieces of information (e.g., being told that “cats can read”) (Flavell, Mumme, Green, & Flavell, 1992) or from naturally occur-ring thoughts (e.g., such as that others cannot hold the appearance/real-ity distinction) (see Miller, Holmes, Gitten, & Danbury, 1997). In general, during this time, children appreciate that others can hold beliefs contrary to the actual state of the world and that they themselves can hold such false beliefs (for a review, see Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2001).

Generally, however, preschoolers do not understand where their knowl-edge comes from or that they have acquired a new piece of knowledge. For instance, when taught new pieces of knowledge, young preschoolers typically claim that they knew it all along (Esbensen, Taylor, & Stoess, 1997; Taylor, Esbensen, & Bennett, 1994). Similarly, while 4-year-olds might un-derstand that their knowledge changes, it is not until later in development that they can track how they know that change occurred (e.g., Gopnik & Graf, 1988). Further, 4-year-olds’ understanding of the indeterminacy of belief is limited (Kuhn, 2002), that is, young children lack the metacog-nitive knowledge that beliefs can be variable and can change given novel data (e.g., Carpendale & Chandler, 1996; Eisbach, 2004; Kuhn, Cheney, & Weinstock, 2000).

As children come to understand that mental states are related and in-tertwined with others, they also become more aware of the indeterminacy of mental states. For instance, Eisbach (2004) demonstrated that between the ages of 4 and 7, children become more aware that beliefs can lead to other beliefs in a variable manner, that is, two people who initially see the same percept can have different subsequent beliefs about that percept (if you and I see a bear, I might think of a trip to the zoo while you might think about your favorite childhood toy). To use Miller’s (2000) terminology, while young children may understand the situational characteristics of how beliefs are derived from information, they potentially do not understand how different individuals might bring different pieces of knowledge to a situation and apply that knowledge differently.

To investigate some of these issues, Sobel (2012) considered the differ-ence between preschoolers’ understanding of learning as recognizing that someone’s knowledge has changed as opposed understanding someone is capable of a particular act. Learning requires a knowledge or belief change; if children’s judgments of learning focus only on the outcome, they should not distinguish learning from various other factors (e.g., luck) that could be responsible for that outcome.

[QA: There is no refer-ence for this citation.]

[QA: There is no refer-ence for this citation.]

[QA: There is no refer-ence for this citation.]

dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
There is no reference for this citation. Correction- Please add following to References: Flavell, J., Mumme, D., Green, F., & Flavell, E. (1992). Young children's understanding of different types of beliefs. Child Development, 63,960-977.
dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
There is no reference for this citation. Correction- Please add following to References: Miller, S.A., Holmes, H.A., Gitten, J., & Danbury, J. (1997). Children’s understanding of false belief that result from developmental misconceptions. Cognitive Development, 12, 21–51.
dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
There is no reference for this citation. Correction- Please add following to References: Miller, S. A. (2000). Children's understanding of preexisting differences in knowledge and belief. Developmental Review, 20(2), 227-282.

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

272 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

In one experiment, children heard stories about characters who initially did not know a piece of information (e.g., how to solve a set of puzzles) and were exposed to a teacher who worked with them to teach them how to solve the puzzles. Of particular interest were conflict stories, in which the character made a claim about whether s/he learned how to solve the puzzles after the teacher’s intervention, which conflicted with the charac-ter’s ability to demonstrate that knowledge (i.e., the character could have claimed she learned how to solve the puzzles but failed to solve them later or the character could have claimed she did not learn how to solve the puzzles but could solve them later).

Three-year-olds did not make systematic responses when asked whether the characters learned the puzzles, while 4-year-olds reliably used the char-acter’s demonstrative abilities, and overall, the usage of these demonstra-tive abilities correlated with children’s performance on a standard measure of false belief (i.e., Perner et al., 1987), in which children must recognize that another has a belief different from the actual state of the world. Criti-cally, in another experiment, Sobel (XXXX) replicated this procedure without informing children at the onset of the story that the character was ignorant. In this case, in which the stories were not about the indetermi-nacy of the character’s beliefs, 3-year-olds’ performance improved. They, like 4-year-olds, responded based on the character’s demonstrative actions and not their claims about whether they learned (and responding was no longer related to children’s false-belief capacities).

These data suggest that preschoolers are sensitive to judgments about the indeterminacy of beliefs when making judgments about whether others have learned. However, this work only focuses on children’s appreciation of the role that changing beliefs has in learning. What about other men-tal states? Sobel, Li, and Corriveau (2007) hypothesized that the theory of mind literature also provides a blueprint for how children develop their conceptualization of the learning process. Following other research in theory of mind (e.g., Bartsch & Wellman, 1995; Wellman, 1990), children start with a relatively immature concept based on desires: a learner’s desires underlie whether information is learned, regardless of their other men-tal states. After age 4, children might realize the importance of noise-level and the importance of interest in paying attention to a particular topic (e.g., Miller & Zalenski, 1982). However, preschoolers have difficulty ar-ticulating an adult-like conception of attentional focus (e.g., Flavell et al., 1995, Flavell, Green, Flavell, & Grossman, 1997). As such, children’s under-standing of the role of attention in learning might lag behind that of de-sire. Finally, there is some understanding of intentionality at very early ages (e.g., Woodward, 1998). However, understanding that intentions and de-sires are distinct and that intended actions can produce desired results by accident occurs after children come to understand that intentional actions

[QA: Please indicate a year for this citation.]

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

theory of Learning, theory of teaching, and theory of Mind 273

are epistemic (i.e., that people intentionally act because they believe their actions will have particular results; see e.g., Feinfeld, Lee, Flavell, Green, & Flavell, 1999; Phillips, Baron-Cohen, & Rutter, 1998; Schult, 2002; for a review, see also Moses, 2001).

To test these hypotheses, Sobel et al. (2007) first performed a CHILDES analysis, examining five corpora of parent-child interactions. They isolat-ed children’s spontaneous usage of the words learn and teach, and analogs based on those roots (e.g., learning). They found that as children got old-er, their utterances were more likely to indicate an understanding of how learning was taking place. Talk about the role desire played in learning de-creased with age, while talk about the role attention played remained stable (children rarely talked about intentionality). These natural language data suggest that as children grow, they begin to understand learning in terms of other mental states in addition to desire.

The utterances they analyzed were a small sample, and the fact that chil-dren did not talk about learning much does not indicate that they lack an understanding of the learning process. In follow-up procedures, Sobel et al. (2007) investigated 4- and 6-year-olds’ understanding of whether characters learned information based on conflicts between various mental states. Chil-dren were told explicitly about two mental states of a character that were either consistent or inconsistent with each other. For example, a consistent story might be that the character wanted to learn something and attended to the relevant information, while an inconsistent story might be that the character wanted to learn, but failed to attend. They found that both age groups correctly judged whether learning would occur in the consistent cases, but justified their responses differently. Four-year-olds justified re-sponses in terms of desires (even when desire was not mentioned). Six-year-olds were more likely to justify their responses in terms of attention and intentionality of the characters.

More interesting are the responses on the inconsistent stories. Four-year-olds’ responses were desire-based—they judged that learning occurred if the character wanted to learn, regardless of the character’s other mental states. Six-year-olds were more likely to judge that characters who lacked the intention to learn or who failed to attend to information would not learn, even if they wanted to learn. There was also evidence that the older children began to appreciate that learning could be incidental—6-year-olds were more likely than 4-year-olds to say that a character who attended to information would learn it, even if she did not practice.

Taken together, these data suggest that children’s explicit judgments of whether learning occurs follow a developmental pathway that parallels their understanding of belief. Children potentially start as “desire psychologists” (Gopnik & Wellman, 1994; Wellman, 1990) and appreciate the role of per-ceptual access (e.g., Wellman et al., 2001) and then intentionality later in

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

274 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

development (e.g., Schult, 2002). Children might similarly be desire-based about whether learning occurs and come to appreciate the roles these oth-er mental states play as they move into the elementary school years.

chiLdren’S deveLoping concepTS of TeAching

Learning and teaching are often complementary activities that occur in relation to each other (LeBlanc & Bearison, 2004; Strauss & Shilony, 1994). One person teaches and the other learns in a joint interaction. However, this relation does not mean that exactly the same mental states and skills are involved for both. For example, learning can either be intentional, when we set out to acquire new knowledge or skills, or incidental, when we find out something new that we did not expect to know. Teaching, arguably, can only be intentional. We have specialized terms to distinguish instances when we learn something from someone without their help (e.g., observa-tional learning, incidental learning, or imitation).

Teaching can be defined as an intentional activity to increase someone’s knowledge or skills (Frye & Ziv, 2005; Kruger & Tomasello, 1996). Given that there is a goal to teaching, it is possible to recognize an activity as teach-ing, even if it is unsuccessful. Describing teaching as intentional also makes it possible to separate it from imitation, in which someone learns from an-other but the other might not have the goal to teach (Premack & Premack, 1996). Olson and Bruner (1996) were among the first to apply a theory of mind approach to teaching (see also Strauss, 1993; Strauss & Shilony, 1994) and reasoned that instruction depends on a perceived knowledge differ-ence between teacher and student. They point out that when there is “no attribution of ignorance [then there is] no effort to teach” (p. 12). Recog-nizing the lack of knowledge or possession of mistaken information makes it possible to adopt a goal to change that state through a chosen activity.

This mental state definition of teaching suggests that young children will need theory of mind abilities before they can recognize teaching and dis-tinguish it from other means of acquiring knowledge. Specifically, children will need to be able to appreciate when people lack knowledge or are igno-rant (Hogrefe et al., 1986) and when they have mistaken beliefs (Wimmer & Perner, 1983). They will also have to begin to identify when people are acting intentionally (Astington, 1991; Baird & Moses, 2001; Feinfeld et al., 1999; Moses, 2001; Schult, 2002). These developments are accomplished during the preschool and entry-to-school period. Their timing indicates that the understanding of teaching should be accomplished near the start of formal schooling.

Perhaps the first systematic investigation of teaching (although not described as such) comes from Piaget (1926), who compared children’s

[QA: Please indicate to which reference this page number refers.]

[QA: There is no refer-ence for this citation.]

[QA: There is no refer-ence for this citation.]

dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
Please indicate to which reference this page number refers Correction- (Olson & Bruner, 1996, p.12)
dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
There is no reference for this citation. Correction- Please change "Astington, 1991" to "Astington, 2001" Please add following to References: Astington , J. W. (2001). The paradox of intention: Assessing children’s metarepresentational understanding. In B. F., Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. B Aldwin (Eds), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.
dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
There is no reference for this citation. Correction- Please add following to References: Piaget, J. (1926). The language and thought of the child. Routledge, London.

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

theory of Learning, theory of teaching, and theory of Mind 275

spontaneous language utterances with those of other children and adults. He found that the majority of the utterances generated by two 6.5-year-olds he observed were egocentric. In further observations of children interact-ing, he outlines stages of communication, ranging from purely egocentric, in which children at age 4 have roughly two distinct monologues, to a stage in which they can dialogue about collaborative action (at roughly age 5.5), and then finally dialogue about action or events not directly present (at roughly age 7). It is interesting that these latter stages are often evidence of teaching in young children. For instance (taken as evidence of children in the second stage):

Rog (5;6) asks Ez (6;4) to explain a point in an education game: “Was there one of these ones with the yellow ones?”

Jac (7;2): You musn’t show him.

Ez: There are yellow ones. He’s doing it all wrong. That one’s much easier. You can finish it now. Go along and finish it. (Piaget, 1926, p. 61)

We do not necessarily endorse Piaget’s ideas about communication at age 4 being completely egocentric, and suggest, like many others, that young-er children might have fairly advanced perspective-taking abilities both in their natural language (e.g., Bartsch & Wellman, 1995; Shatz et al., 1983) and cognition (e.g., Miller, 2000; Wellman et al., 2001). What is interesting about this exchange (and many of these examples) is that the communica-tion describes a clear act of teaching. One child (Rog) asks for information from another child. The other child (Ez) provides that information (with the third child, Jac, more metacognitively saying that Ez should not). Such spontaneous instances of teaching are potentially commonplace for these older children. An open question is when such teaching acts emerge in children’s communication, and when children recognize acts as teaching.

identifying Teaching

We will first examine when young children recognize that teaching is tak-ing place. Ziv and Frye (2004) presented preschoolers with short scenarios based on the mental-state components of teaching. They were told simple stories in which characters differed in their knowledge of how to do some-thing (e.g., read) and then were asked to predict whether one character would try to teach the other. Both 3.5- and 5.5-year-olds were able to answer these questions correctly. They were able to say that the person who has knowledge about something would teach the other who did not (i.e., “Who should be taught?” and “Who can teach?” stories). Children were even able

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

276 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

to make this judgment when the person who possessed the knowledge was a child and the person who lacked it was an adult teacher (“Can a teacher be taught?”). This pattern of results indicates that 3.5-year-olds use knowl-edge conditions to interpret teaching and do so despite the usual role of the character. It also confirms Olson and Bruner’s (1996) prediction that young children appreciate that a lack of knowledge is necessary for teach-ing to occur.

Ziv et al. (2008) used a similar approach to see if 3.5- and 5.5-year-olds realized that intention was a defining feature of teaching. They presented children with stories that contrasted teaching with imitation, thus removing the intention to teach from the story. For instance, children heard about a character (A), who learned from watching another person (B), who was unaware that A was watching. Three-and-a-half-year-olds judged that B was teaching, while 5.5-years-olds recognized that B was not teaching.

Children were also given an example of guided discovery learning in which a teacher was trying to teach but presented the activity as if it were a game (Embedded teaching). Here, the younger children thought that the teacher only wanted to play, but the older group began to acknowledge that the adult’s goal was to teach. The results of these stories indicated that only the 5.5-year-olds used intention to distinguish teaching from learning in situations like imitation and guided discovery.

Finally, Ziv et al. (2008) investigated whether children appreciate the role belief has in guiding instructors’ actions. The actual knowledge states of the learners do not determine teaching, but the perception of those knowledge states do. For example, what if a teacher were to have a false belief about a student’s knowledge, as might occur when someone under-estimates the level of understanding of a new class or particular student? If this situation is judged on the actual knowledge states involved, no teach-ing would be predicted, but if the teacher’s perspective is relevant, there will be an attempt to teach. Ziv et al. presented 3.5- and 5.5-year-olds with stories in which teachers had a variety of inaccurate beliefs. In these stories, the teacher either assumed the student did not know, when he actually did (Underestimate learner); or assumed he knew, when he did not (Overesti-mate learner); or assumed that his own knowledge was correct, when it was not (Overestimate self). Only the 5.5-year-olds were able to predict teach-ing on the basis of the teacher’s point of view, and this understanding was correlated with success on a standard unexpected-transfer false-belief task (Wimmer & Perner, 1983). Thus, the more complicated knowledge condi-tions again reveal a developmental advantage for 5.5-year-olds in being able to understand what teaching is and when it will occur.

The difference between younger and older preschoolers’ understand-ing of the mental state components of teaching has direct implications for their concept of teaching and may affect how they recognize it in everyday

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

theory of Learning, theory of teaching, and theory of Mind 277

situations. For example, teaching and imitation are both specific means of knowledge acquisition with unique characteristics. The current results, however, indicate that only older preschoolers distinguish between the two. The distinction is important because understanding the difference is neces-sary for having different expectations in the two situations. In real instances that correspond to the stories presented here, young preschoolers might expect an adult’s level of involvement in a student’s learning to be the same whether the adult was teaching or just being observed doing something.

Moreover, some of the data on children’s understanding of the learn-ing process integrate well with these results. In both cases, children have to separate beliefs from reality, suggesting that what children know about how beliefs are formed, represented, and change informs their understand-ing of both learning and teaching. That said, the inferences children have to make are slightly different. In the cases in which children have to dis-tinguish between claims or false beliefs about learning and the reality of evidence for learning, children have to ignore the claim and focus on the reality. In the cases of judging teaching, children have to use the false belief of the teacher while ignoring the reality of the knowledge states.

children’s Teaching

One activity in which it must be expected that children’s conception of teaching would make a difference is in their own teaching. It is not un-usual for school-aged children to be given the opportunity to engage in peer teaching (e.g., Flynn, 2010; Rogoff, 1990) or reciprocal instruction (e.g., Brown & Palincsar, 1989). If young children are just developing a con-ception of teaching, it is likely to make an observable difference in how they approach the activity. Arguably, intention links the understanding and conduct of teaching. Because teaching is an intentional activity, you have to understand what it is in order to be able to do it.

Consequently, placing young children in a situation in which they have an opportunity to teach could be a good test of their understanding of teaching. In order to recognize whom they should teach, children must realize that a potential student lacks skill or knowledge, and they must in-tentionally choose a strategy to do something about it. Wood, Wood, Ain-sworth, and O’Malley (1995) examined children’s teaching by studying pairs of 3-, 5-, and 7-year-olds in which one child knew how to construct a block pattern because they had previously been taught by an adult and the other did not. The oldest children displayed the most success in teaching by using verbal instructions and contingent instruction in which they adjusted the amount of help they gave depending on the learner’s current success.

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

278 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

Similarly, Ashley and Tomasello (1998) tested pairs of toddlers from 2 to 3.5 years of age in which they had to act cooperatively to operate a simple device. Then each child was given the opportunity to teach a new child what to do. Only the 3.5-year-olds appeared to teach in this situation. They coordinated their actions more with the new child, gave specific directives for what the other child should do, and produced some demonstrations to illustrate what should be done.

Children’s teaching also appears related to their emerging theory of mind abilities (Astington & Pelletier, 1996; Strauss et al., 2002). For in-stance, Strauss et al. (2002) asked 3.5- and 5.5-year-olds to teach a peer a simple board game. They found that the younger children were more likely to demonstrate the moves in the game and the older children were more likely to explain them, and the number of explanations was related to children’s success on standard false-belief measures. Davis-Unger and Carlson (2008) performed a more detailed investigation of the task and introduced instances in which an adult learner made (deliberate) errors. They found that over the preschool ages, children increased the time, rules presented, and strategies used in teaching and that all of these differences were related to theory of mind performance. Five-and-a-half-year-olds were also much better at noticing a learner’s errors and responding with a variety of strategies. Recently, Howe, Recchia, Della Porta, and Funamoto (2012) found similar patterns for sibling pairs, with older siblings being better at adjusting their strategies to teaching two different types of tasks.

The pattern of results across these studies is generally supportive of the idea that children develop an understanding of teaching during the pre-school period. Their efforts to engage in teaching seem to begin at 3.5 years and continue to change thereafter. There is evidence that this un-derstanding is related to standard theory of mind developments. It will be interesting if more specific relations can be established between children’s understanding of what is needed in a teaching situation and what strategies they start to employ. For example, when children are trying to bring about a particular outcome in their teaching, do they become more aware of de-viations from that outcome and more motivated to correct them? Similarly, as we suggested above regarding learning, children who develop the ability to integrate mental states together to recognize that beliefs are not fixed should also be able to make deeper inferences about the roles different mental states play in their own teaching.

children’s Learning from Teaching

Several studies have recently suggested that, at very early ages, children make pedagogical inferences from others’ teaching (e.g., Bonawitz et al.,

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

theory of Learning, theory of teaching, and theory of Mind 279

2011; Gweon, Pelton, & Schulz, 2011) based on their natural ability to read the intentions of others during communication (what Csibra & Gergeley, 2009 call “natural pedagogy”). These studies, however, categorize teaching as an act of communication, subject to children reading the intentionality of their interlocutor, that is, whether their actions are goal-directed. We have suggested that children’s understanding of the epistemic aspect of in-tentionality is also critical to their understanding of learning and teaching. Studies on pedagogy do not examine how the child’s own knowledge of teaching affects whether they understand why another would teach. That is, does understanding what teaching is—an activity to change what others know—help children to participate in attending to it?

There is surprisingly little research on this question. Woodburn (2008) made a first step in considering this question in a 1-year longitudinal study of the transition from Head Start to kindergarten. Some 120 Head Start and kindergarten participants (ages 3.3 to 6.8) were assessed on (a) an understanding-of-teaching scale constructed from the previously described teaching stories (Ziv & Frye, 2004; Ziv et al., 2008), (b) the Wellman and Liu (2004) theory-of-mind scale, (c) the Learning Behav-ior Scales (McDermott, 1999; McDermott, Green, Francis, & Stott, 1999), (d) Adjustment Scales for Preschool Intervention (Noone-Lutz, Fantuzzo, & McDermott, 2002), and (e) standardized measures of literacy (Test of Early Reading Ability—TERA) (Reid, Hresko, & Hamil, 2001) and early mathematics (a short form of Ginsburg & Baroody’s [2003] Test of Early Math Ability—TEMA).

As would be expected with children of these ages, there was improved performance on each of the measures over the course of the year. Rasch modeling indicated that a 5-item subset of the understanding-of-teaching stories could be treated as a scale. Simultaneous multiple regression analy-ses using the literacy scores at time two as outcomes indicated that all of the independent variables at time two (age, understanding of teaching, theory of mind, learning behavior scale) were related to the literacy outcome. When the predictive relation between the independent variables at time one and the literacy outcome at time two was tested, only initial literacy and understanding of teaching scores remained in the equation. These find-ings indicate that the children who started with better literacy skills and the ones who had the better understanding of teaching were the ones who benefitted the most from the literacy instruction that was presented during the first year of school. This finding suggests that knowing what it means to teach does benefit children’s ability to learn from teachers.

[QA: Year does not agree with reference.]

dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
Year does not agree with reference. Correction- Please change "1999" to "1996"

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

280 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

Summary

We have suggested that children’s developing social cognition influences how they teach, how they understand teaching, and how they learn from teachers. Our general conclusion is that two basic aspects of social-cognitive development greatly influence these processes: how children understand the ways in which beliefs are formed and change, and their ability to in-tegrate mental states together to recognize that beliefs are not fixed and depend on other mental states. Children’s understanding of teaching, like their understanding of learning, relies on their developing knowledge of others’ mental states.

LeArning And TeAching in chiLdren WiTh AUTiSM SpecTrUM diSorder

Given that children’s developing understanding of belief and its relation and integration with other mental states (such as intention) plays a primary role in their understanding of the concepts of learning and teaching, what happens to children’s understanding of learning and teaching when their understanding of beliefs and intention is impaired? Autism Spectrum Dis-order (ASD) is a prominent developmental disorder that is found in about 1% of the US population (CDC, 2006). It is known to affect the ontogenesis of metacognitive abilities, such as the understanding of belief and inten-tion in others. ASD manifests in the first 3 years of life and is characterized by impairments in social skills, verbal and nonverbal communication, and by the presence of restricted interests, behaviors, and activities (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). One dominant psychological hypothesis that explains these diminished social and communicative characteristics is that children with ASD have an impaired theory of mind (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 1989; Frith, 1989; Leslie, 1987). Does a diminished capacity for mental-state understanding in children with ASD affect these children’s understanding of the processes of learning and teaching?

We have seen that typically developing (TD) children’s understanding of the processes of learning and teaching relates to their developing theory of mind. For instance, an understanding of others’ intentions is critical for understanding aspects of the learning process (Sobel et al., 2007), and rec-ognizing that teaching is an activity that is performed on purpose (Ziv et al., 2008) is critical for understanding aspects of the teaching process. Chil-dren who have difficulty understanding differences in knowledge states be-tween two agents may be limited in their ability to appreciate the role that changing beliefs might have in learning, or to see the reason for someone to teach another. Similarly, if children have trouble understanding when

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

theory of Learning, theory of teaching, and theory of Mind 281

others act intentionally, then they are likely to struggle with understanding the intentional components that underlie the processes of learning and teaching. Investigating the developing understanding of others’ beliefs and intentions in children with ASD may reveal what children who are autistic know about the processes of learning and teaching. Moreover, such inves-tigations could inform us about the relation between children’s developing theory of mind and the understanding of learning and teaching.

Research investigating aspects of social-cognitive development in ASD has provided repeated demonstrations that “high functioning” children with ASD (HFA) have delays or deficits on mental-state attribution tasks compared with TD children matched for verbal mental, and chronologi-cal age (see Baron-Cohen, 1989). In particular, HFA children exhibit fun-damental impairments or significant delays in understanding inaccurate knowledge or false belief in others (e.g., Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985; Peterson, Wellman, & Liu, 2005; Senju et al., 2010). A meta-analysis of studies employing a variety of false-belief paradigms showed that chil-dren with ASD on average are not successful on false-belief tasks until they have an estimated verbal mental age (VMA) of at least 11 years (Happé, 1995). To date, there have been no reported cases of a group of children with ASD whose mental age is equivalent to typically developing preschool-ers who pass standard false-belief tasks (Baron-Cohen, 1989; Lombardo & Baron-Cohen, 2010). Based on the extensive literature documenting an attenuated theory of mind and, in particular, impairments in false-belief understanding in HFA children, there is good reason to suspect that delays in the understanding of belief for children who are autistic might obstruct their ability to determine what actions constitute learning and teaching.

Belief understanding, however, is not the only element involved in un-derstanding learning and teaching. Understanding others’ intentions is crucial to understanding core components of learning and teaching. As demonstrated in the previous sections, judgments of intentionality relate to both the understanding of learning and the understanding of teaching. For example, Ziv et al. (2008) asked preschoolers to judge the intent of an instructor’s action in an embedded teaching task. In this story, children had to determine whether a given action is brought about by one of two inten-tions (e.g., to play a game or to teach). Four-year-olds performed signifi-cantly worse than 5-year-olds, a finding consistent with Baird and Moses’s (2001, Study 1) “same action/different intention” task, in which children were required to judge whether the same action could be motivated by different intentions. Further, understanding the “successful imitation” task (Ziv et al., 2008) involves appreciating that a desired outcome (e.g., learn-ing how to tie a knot) resulted from an unintentional action on the part of the teacher. Schult (2002) demonstrated that TD 4-year-olds struggled to understand cases in which a character’s desires were satisfied by an

[QA: There is no refer-ence for this citation.]

dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
There is no reference for this citation. Correction- Please add following to References: Happé, F. G. E. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843-855.

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

282 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

unintended action (i.e., they interpreted such events by saying that the ac-tions were intentional all along). Five-year-olds were more likely to appreci-ate that desires could be satisfied by unintentional actions.

What about HFA children? Are HFA children delayed in their under-standing of intention and thus possibly delayed in understanding the relevance of intention to learning and teaching? Unlike false-belief un-derstanding, relatively little research has examined the understanding of others’ intentionality in HFA children. Moreover, the work that has been done has resulted in discordant findings showing HFA children sometimes do and sometimes do not perform worse than matched controls on tasks that examine judgments of others’ intention-in-action (Russell & Hill, 2001; but see also Phillips et al., 1998; Williams & Happé, 2010). Stud-ies that test judgments of nonverbal intention-in-action have also yielded mixed results (see Aldridge, Stone, Sweeney, & Bower, 2000; Carpenter, Pennington, & Rogers, 2001; Hornbeck, 2001; but also see Huang, Heyes, & Charman, 2002, 2006).

Despite these inconsistent observations about intention-in-action, it is clear that HFA children are delayed in understanding intentions-in-others. Two recent studies suggest that HFA children struggle with developing an appreciation of intentional versus unintentional behaviors in others (D’Entremont & Yazbek, 2007; Williams & Happé, 2010). However, to de-termine more fully whether HFA children understand intentions (in oth-ers), which then provides critical information about their understanding of the intentional aspects of learning and teaching, we need to investigate whether HFA children have an appreciation for the mental state itself, as has been done in studies with TD children.

Knutsen (2012) investigated HFA children’s (Mean CA: 7;9) understand-ing of the concept of teaching and intentions in others, as well as the rela-tion between these two concepts. To examine the knowledge and inten-tional aspects of teaching, HFA children were presented with tasks similar to those used in Ziv and Frye (2004) and Ziv et al., (2008). To examine the distinction between intentions and desires and to examine the under-standing that different intentions may be used to carry out the same action, HFA children were presented with tasks used by Schult (2002) and Baird and Moses (2001), respectively. The teaching measure was composed of (a) knowledge stories that included a simple knowledge task (e.g., Who should be taught?), a teacher’s misestimation of the learner’s knowledge tasks, and a teacher’s misestimation of own knowledge tasks; and (b) intention-in-teaching stories that included a successful (intentional) teaching task, a failed (intentional) teaching task, a successful (nonintentional by the teacher) imitation task, and two teaching-embedded-in-a-game (guided dis-covery learning and its control) tasks (Frye & Ziv, 2005). In the intention-desire distinction measure, children were presented with stories in which

dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
Please change "(2012)" to "(2013)" Please add following to References: Knutsen, J. D. (2013). The understanding of teaching in children with autism spectrum disorder. Published Doctoral Dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

theory of Learning, theory of teaching, and theory of Mind 283

the character’s intention or desire either was or was not satisfied (Schult, 2002); whereas the “same action/different intentions” measure had two characters performing an identical action motivated by substantially differ-ent desires and intentions (Baird & Moses, 2001).

In the teaching stories, Knutsen (2012) found HFA children to be im-paired in understanding the knowledge-state component that underlies teaching. The HFA children responded above chance, although were sig-nificantly worse than the TD 3- and 4-year-olds in the original sample (Ziv & Frye, 2004) on the simple knowledge task (“Who should be taught?”). They were also significantly worse on the two intention stories in which the teacher’s attempt to teach either resulted in the student learning or not (the successful teaching; failed teaching condition from Ziv et al., 2008). HFA participants also performed at or below chance and were significantly worse than the original sample of TD 5- and 6-year-olds (Ziv & Frye, 2004) on all of the knowledge teaching stories in which a character misestimated the knowledge state either of the other or of him/herself. Finally, HFA children struggled at tasks that involved a teacher having a false belief about either their own knowledge state or the knowledge state of the learner. For instance, when a teacher thought that their student already knows how to spell his/her name, when in fact the student does not, HFA children claimed that the teacher would still try to teach the student how to spell his/her name.

These preliminary findings suggest that HFA children’s understanding of teaching and learning is constrained by their impaired understanding of teaching as an intentional activity that requires appreciating that there is a knowledge difference between two people. HFA children’s metacognitive delays in the understanding of beliefs and intentions in others impedes their ability to understand these two mental-state components that underlie the teaching process. HFA children’s understanding of the teaching aspects of the learning process (e.g., when learning occurs or not, based on wheth-er the attempt to teach occurred) is also potentially compromised by their delays in mental-state understanding.

These findings provide a roadmap for conducting further research on young typically and atypically developing children’s understanding of the processes of teaching and learning as well as how such abilities relate to their mental-state understanding. For instance, a next step might be to in-vestigate HFA children’s understanding of their own intention and its rela-tion to their understanding of the processes of learning and teaching. The results also imply that HFA children can be expected to struggle with activi-ties in the classroom that require understanding beliefs and intention in others, such as following a teacher’s presentation that is being performed on purpose to impart knowledge to the student. Additional research should be conducted to explore HFA children’s reactions in the classroom to

dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
Please change: Knutsen "(2012)" to Knutsen "(2013)"

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

284 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

teaching and learning situations by manipulating additional mental-state attributes used in particular teaching situations, such as the epistemic com-ponent of teaching and learning.

concLUding ThoUghTS

The question of when and how young children understand the processes of learning and teaching has more than theoretical consequences. School readiness is often evaluated in terms of young children’s first grasp of litera-cy, numeracy, and social skills (e.g., Boyer, 1991; Mashburn & Pianta, 2006). Besides this focus on the different content areas of instruction, however, another important factor might be the understanding of instruction itself. Are children likely to show the most benefit from school if they do not yet grasp what teaching is? The same issue is important in preschool. The general question of whether students should be exposed to direct instruc-tion (e.g., Klahr & Nigam, 2004) or guided discovery learning (e.g., May-er, 2004) is long-standing (e.g., Anastasiow, Sibley, Leonhardt, & Borich, 1970), but it may have the most purchase in preschool. Because guided discovery learning does not require the understanding that instruction is taking place, there may be good grounds for employing it in preschool if young children do not yet know what teaching is. The answers to these questions about appropriate practice with children at the beginning of school might be advanced if it were possible to determine what they under-stood about teaching.

Further, what young children know about learning and teaching poten-tially relates to how they are engaged by and feel about the process of learn-ing and being taught. These questions are particularly important given re-search on elementary school children, which suggests that how children understand the learning process potentially influences their later concep-tions of knowledge, school, or themselves (Dweck, 1999; Dweck & Leggitt, 1988; Eccles, Wigfield, & Schiefele, 1998; Li, 2004; Skinner, 1995; Stipek & Mac Iver, 1989). What young children know about learning might ini-tially shape how engaged they are by learning as well as their developing concepts of their own or others’ intellectual ability or drive. If children’s academic engagement is influenced by their understanding of learning and teaching, then one might also conceptualize early interventions that foster understanding of these processes.

Given these ideas, and given that children’s understanding of learning and teaching appears connected with their social-cognitive development, a critical question is how children’s understanding of the learning and teach-ing processes relate to their broader beliefs about school and their school readiness. For instance, understanding the extent to which the process of

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

theory of Learning, theory of teaching, and theory of Mind 285

learning involves integrating various mental states might relate to Dweck and Leggett’s (1988) “implicit theories” of intelligence as fixed or mallea-ble constructs. The more the child recognizes that learning involves inte-grating various mental states, the more the child could conceptualize one’s learning ability as malleable (particularly if the child recognizes those men-tal states can change). Similarly, while children clearly learn much from guided discovery, the more children understand what they can learn from teaching, the more value they get from approaches to teaching that poten-tially emphasize direct instruction. Finally, interventions for HFA children that focus on social-cognitive development might affect how these children conceptualize learning and teaching, and particularly if those interven-tions focus on applying what children have learned to their daily lives, and more generally may improve how they learn from others. Such interven-tions could have broad application in the classroom, where now ~1% of the population is diagnosed with ASD. All of these hypotheses require future study and would have great relevance to and importance for how to trans-late basic research in social-cognitive development to the classroom.

referenceS

Aldridge, M. A., Stone, K. R., Sweeney, M. H., & Bower, T. G. R. (2000). Preverbal children with autism understand intentions of others. Developmental Science, 3, 294–301.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men-tal disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Anastasiow, N. J., Sibley, S. A., Leonhardt, T. M., & Borich, G. D. (1970). A compari-son of guided discovery, discovery and didactic teaching of math to kindergar-ten poverty children. American Educational Research Journal, 7, 493–510.

Ashley, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Cooperative problem-solving and teaching in preschoolers. Social Development, 7, 143–163.

Astington, J. W. (2001). The paradox of intention: Assessing children’s metarepresenta-tional understanding. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [QA: There is no citation for this reference.]

Astington, J. W., & Pelletier, J. (1996). The language of mind: Its role in teaching and learning. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of education and human development (pp. 593–620). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Baird, J. A., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Young children’s understanding that identical actions may be motivated by different intentions. Journal of Cognition and De-velopment, 2, 413–448.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific devel-opmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285–298.

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have the-ory of mind? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
Correction- Please see sticky note on pg. 274

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

286 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Bonawitz, E. B., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Goodman, N., Spelke, E., & Schulz, L. E. (2011). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Teaching limits children’s spontaneous exploration and discovery. Cognition, 120, 322–330.

Boyer, E. (1991). Ready to learn: A mandate for the nation. Princeton, NJ: Carnegie Foundation for the Advancement of Teaching.

Brown, A. L., & Palincsar, A. S. (1989). Guided, cooperative learning and individual knowledge acquisition. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruc-tion: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 393–451). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Carpendale, J., & Chandler, M. (1996). On the distinction between false belief un-derstanding and subscribing to an interpretive theory of mind. Child Develop-ment, 67, 1686–1706.

Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2001). Understanding of others’ intentions in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-ders, 31, 589–599.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2006). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, United States. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 58, 1–20.

Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148–153.

Davis-Unger, A. C., & Carlson, S. M. (2008). Development of teaching skills and rela-tions to theory of mind in preschoolers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9, 26–45.

D’Entremont, B., & Yazbek, A. (2007). Imitation of intentional and accidental ac-tions by children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1665–1678.

Dunbar, K., & Klahr, D. (1989). Developmental differences in scientific discovery processes. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 109–143). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology.Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and

personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In N. Eisen-

berg (Ed.), Social, emotional, and personality development in handbook of child psy-chology (Vol. 3, pp. 1017–1096). New York, NY: Wiley.

Eisbach, A. O. (2004). Children’s developing awareness of diversity in people’s trains of thought. Child Development, 75, 1694–1707.

Esbensen, B. M., Taylor, M., & Stoess, C. (1997). Children’s behavioral understand-ing of knowledge acquisition. Cognitive Development, 12, 53–84.

Feinfield, K. A., Lee, P. P., Flavell, E. R., Green, F. L., & Flavell, J. H. (1999). Young children’s understanding of intention. Cognitive Development, 14, 463–486.

Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1995). Young children’s knowledge about thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60(1, Series No. 243).

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

theory of Learning, theory of teaching, and theory of Mind 287

Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., Flavell, E. R., & Grossman, J. B. (1997). The development of children’s knowledge about inner speech. Child Development, 68, 39–47.

Flynn, E. (2010). Underpinning collaborative learning. In B. Sokol, U. Muller, J. Carpendale, A. Young, & G. Iarocci (Eds.), Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions (pp. 312–336). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Frye, D., & Ziv, M. (2005). Teaching and learning as intentional activities. In B. D.

Homer & C. Tamis-Lemonda (Eds.), The development of social cognition and com-munication (pp. 231–258). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Ginsburg, H. P., & Baroody, A. J. (2003). Test of early mathematics ability (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro Ed.

Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appear-ance-reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 26–37.

Gopnik, A., & Graf, P. (1988). Knowing how you know: Young children’s ability to identify and remember the sources of their beliefs. Child Development, 59, 1366–1371.

Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (1994). The theory theory. In L. Hirschfield & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 257–293). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Griffiths, T. L., Sobel, D. M., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Gopnik, A. (2011). Bayes and blickets: Effects of knowledge on causal induction in children and adults. Cognitive Science, 35, 1407–1455.

Gweon, H., Pelton, H., & Schulz, L. E. (2011, July 20–23). Adults and school-aged children accurately evaluate sins of omission in pedagogical contexts. Proceed-ings of the 33rd annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Boston, MA.

Hogrefe, G. J., Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1986). Ignorance vs. false belief: A devel-opmental lag in attribution of epistemic states. Child Development, 57, 567–582.

Hornbeck, V. S. (2001). Understanding of intention and imitation in young chil-dren with autism, children with other developmental disabilities and children with typical development. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sci-ences and Engineering, 62(3-B), 1579. (UMI No. AAT 3006974)

Howe, N., Recchia, H., Della Porta, S., & Funamoto, A. (2012). “The driver doesn’t sit, he stands up like the Flintstones!”: Sibling teaching during teacher-direct-ed and self-guided tasks. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 208–231.

Huang, C., Heyes, C., & Charman, T. (2002). Infants’ behavioral reenactment of “failed attempts”: Exploring the roles of emulation learning, stimulus en-hancement, and understanding of intentions. Developmental Psychology, 38, 840–855.

Huang, C., Heyes, C., & Charman, T. (2006). Preschoolers’ behavioural reenact-ment of “failed attempts”: The roles of intention-reading, emulation and mimicry. Cognitive Development, 21, 36–45.

Klahr, D. (2000). Exploring science: The cognition and development of discovery processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

288 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychological Science, 15, 661–667.

Knutsen, J. D. (2012). The understanding of teaching in autism spectrum disorders. Dis-sertation thesis in preparation, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.

Kruger, A. C., & Tomasello, M. (1996). Cultural learning and learning culture. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of education and human develop-ment (pp. 369–387). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96, 674–689.

Kuhn, D. (2002). What is scientific thinking and how does it develop? In U. Gos-wami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 371–393). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Kuhn, D., Cheney, R., & Weinstock, M. (2000). The development of epistemological understanding. Cognitive Development, 15, 309–328.

LeBlanc, G., & Bearison, D. J. (2004). Teaching and learning as a bi-directional activity: Investigating dyadic interactions between child teachers and child learners. Cognitive Development, 19, 499–515.

Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

Li, J. (2004). Learning as a task and a virtue: US and Chinese preschoolers explain learning. Developmental Psychology, 40, 595–605.

Lombardo, M. V., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2010). Unraveling the paradox of the autistic self. WIREs Cognitive Science, 1, 393–403.

Mashburn, A. J., & Pianta, R. C. (2006). Social relationships and school readiness. Early Education and Development, 17, 151–176.

Mayer, R. E. (2004). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? The case for guided methods of instruction. American Psychologist, 59, 14–19.

McDermott, P. A. (1999). National scales of differential learning behaviors among American children and adolescents. School Psychology Review, 28, 280–291.

McDermott, P. A., Green, L. F., Francis, J. M., & Stott, D. H. (1996). Preschool Learn-ing Behaviors scale. Philadelphia, PA: Edumetric and Clinical Science.

Miller, P. H., & Zalenski, R. (1982). Preschoolers’ knowledge about attention. Devel-opmental Psychology, 18, 871–875.

Moses, L. J. (2001). Some thoughts on ascribing complex intentional concepts to young children. In B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 69–84). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Noone-Lutz, M., Fantuzzo, J. F., & McDermott, P. A., (2002). Multidimensional as-sessment of emotional and behavioral adjustment of low-income preschool children: Development and initial validation. Early Childhood Research Quar-terly, 17, 338–355.

Olson, D. R., & Bruner, J. S. (1996). Folk psychology and folk pedagogy. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of education and human development (pp. 9–27). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

dknutsen-adm
Sticky Note
Correction- Please remove "Knutsen, J. D. (2012) The understanding of teaching in autism spectrum disorders. Dissertation thesis in preparation, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia"

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

theory of Learning, theory of teaching, and theory of Mind 289

Perner, J., Leekham, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137.

Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory-of-mind develop-ment for children with deafness or autism. Child Development, 76, 502–517.

Phillips, A. T., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1998). Understanding intention in normal development and in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 337–348.

Pillow, B. H. (1989). Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 116–129.

Pratt, C., & Bryant, P. (1990). Young children understand that looking leads to knowing (so long as they are looking into a single barrel). Child Development, 61, 973–982.

Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1996). Why animals lack pedagogy and some cultures have more of it than others. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), The hand-book of human development and education (pp. 302–344). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Reid, D., Hresko, W., & Hamill, D. (1989). Test of early reading ability. Austin TX: Pro-Ed.

Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. Oxford, UK; New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Russell, J., & Hill, E. L. (2001). Action-monitoring and intention reporting in chil-dren with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 317–328.

Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 31–57.

Schauble, L. (1996). The development of scientific reasoning in knowledge-rich contexts. Developmental Psychology, 32, 102–119.

Schult, C. A. (2002). Children’s understanding of the distinction between inten-tions and desires. Child Development, 73, 1727–1747.

Senju, A., Southgate, V., Miura, Y., Matsui, T., Hasegawa, T., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Csibra, G. (2010). Absence of spontaneous action anticipation by false belief attribution in children with autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psycho-pathology, 22, 353–360.

Shatz, M., Wellman, H. M., & Silber, S. (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state. Cognition, 14, 301–321.

Skinner, E. (1995). Perceived control, motivation, and coping. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Sobel, D. M. (2012). Can you do it?: How preschoolers judge whether others have learned. Manuscript submitted for publication, Brown University, Providence, RI.

Sobel, D. M., Li, J., & Corriveau, K. C. (2007). “They danced around in my head and then I learned them”: Children’s developing conceptions of learning events. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 345–369.

Stipek, D., & Mac Iver, D. (1989). Developmental change in children’s assessment of intellectual competence. Child Development, 60, 521–538.

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

© 201

4 IA

P

All rig

hts

rese

rved

290 J. KNUtSeN, D. FrYe, and D. M. SOBeL

Strauss, S. (1993). Teachers’ pedagogical content knowledge about children’s minds and learning: Implications for teacher education. Educational Psycholo-gist, 28, 279–290.

Strauss, S., & Shilony, T. (1994). Teachers’ models of children’s minds and learn-ing. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain-specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 455-473). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Strauss, S., Ziv, M., & Stein, A. (2002). Teaching as a natural cognition and its rela-tions to preschoolers’ developing theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 17, 1473–1787.

Taylor, M., Esbensen, B. M., & Bennett, R. T. (1994). Children’s understanding of knowledge acquisition: The tendency for children to report that they have always known what they have just learned. Child Development, 65, 1581–1604.

Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. K. (2001). A meta-analysis of theory of mind:

The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.Wellman, H. M., & Lagattuta, K. H. (2004). Theory of mind for learning and teach-

ing: The nature and role of explanation. Cognitive Development, 19, 479–497.Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Develop-

ment, 75, 523–541.Williams, D. M., & Happé, F. (2010). Representing intentions in self and others:

Studies of autism and typical development. Developmental Science, 13, 307–319.Wimmer, H., Hogrefe, G. J., & Perner, J. (1988). Children’s understanding of infor-

mational access as source of knowledge. Child Development, 59, 386–396.Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and con-

straining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of de-ception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.

Wood, D., Wood, H., Ainsworth, S., & O’Malley, C. (1995). On becoming a tutor: Toward an ontogenetic model. Cognition and Instruction, 13, 565–581.

Woodburn, E. M. (2008). The social aspects of learning: The role of theory of mind, chil-dren’s understanding of teaching and social-behavioral competence in school readiness. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.

Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.

Ziv, M., & Frye, D. (2004). Children’s understanding of teaching: The role of knowl-edge and belief. Cognitive Development, 19, 457–477.

Ziv, M., Solomon, A., & Frye, D. (2008). Young children’s recognition of the inten-tionality of teaching. Child Development, 79, 1237–1256.