the psychology of music || structure and interpretation of rhythm in music

36
9 Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music Henkjan Honing Cognitive Science Center Amsterdam, Universiteit van Amsterdam, The Netherlands I. Introduction The aim of this chapter is to give an overview of research relating to the temporal aspects of music perception and cognition. This topic has quite a history as a research topic, having been covered extensively by Paul Fraisse in the first edition and Eric Clarke in the second edition of this volume (Fraisse, 1982; Clarke, 1999). However, this chapter focuses primarily on the developments in the past 10 years. More precisely, it gives an overview of rhythm, meter, timing, and tempo from both music theoretical and cognitive perspectives, focusing on perceptual aspects of rhythm. For an overview of performance aspects, see Palmer (Chapter 10, this volume); for developmental aspects, see Trainor and Hannon (Chapter 11, this volume); for neuroscience aspects, see Peretz (Chapter 13, this volume) and Wan and Schlaug (Chapter 14, this volume). The production and perception of rhythm and timing is addressed only spar- ingly in music theory. Existing theories of rhythmic structure are restricted to music as notated in a score, and, as a result, tend to refrain from making state- ments about music as it is performed by musicians, and perceived and appreciated by listeners. This might explain a commonly heard complaint on the relative impoverishment of rhythmic theory (Cooper & Meyer, 1960; Kramer, 1988; London, 2001). However, the arrival of new technologies (most notably that of MIDI 1 and of the personal computer), as well as a wider use of experimental meth- ods in music research, has resulted in a considerable increase in the number of empirically oriented investigations into rhythm, encompassing research into the nature and properties of music as an acoustical, psychological, and cognitive phe- nomenon (Bregman, 1990; Clarke & Cook, 2004; Honing, 2006b; Huron, 1999; Parncutt, 2007). This has led to an important shift from studying the music 1 Commercial standard introduced in the 1980s for the exchange of information between electronic instruments and computers. The Psychology of Music. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-381460-9.00009-2 © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Upload: henkjan

Post on 24-Dec-2016

220 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

9 Structure and Interpretation ofRhythm in Music

Henkjan Honing

Cognitive Science Center Amsterdam, Universiteit van Amsterdam,The Netherlands

I. Introduction

The aim of this chapter is to give an overview of research relating to the temporal

aspects of music perception and cognition. This topic has quite a history as a

research topic, having been covered extensively by Paul Fraisse in the first edition

and Eric Clarke in the second edition of this volume (Fraisse, 1982; Clarke, 1999).

However, this chapter focuses primarily on the developments in the past 10 years.

More precisely, it gives an overview of rhythm, meter, timing, and tempo from

both music theoretical and cognitive perspectives, focusing on perceptual aspects

of rhythm. For an overview of performance aspects, see Palmer (Chapter 10, this

volume); for developmental aspects, see Trainor and Hannon (Chapter 11, this

volume); for neuroscience aspects, see Peretz (Chapter 13, this volume) and Wan

and Schlaug (Chapter 14, this volume).

The production and perception of rhythm and timing is addressed only spar-

ingly in music theory. Existing theories of rhythmic structure are restricted to

music as notated in a score, and, as a result, tend to refrain from making state-

ments about music as it is performed by musicians, and perceived and appreciated

by listeners. This might explain a commonly heard complaint on the relative

impoverishment of rhythmic theory (Cooper & Meyer, 1960; Kramer, 1988;

London, 2001). However, the arrival of new technologies (most notably that of

MIDI1 and of the personal computer), as well as a wider use of experimental meth-

ods in music research, has resulted in a considerable increase in the number of

empirically oriented investigations into rhythm, encompassing research into the

nature and properties of music as an acoustical, psychological, and cognitive phe-

nomenon (Bregman, 1990; Clarke & Cook, 2004; Honing, 2006b; Huron, 1999;

Parncutt, 2007). This has led to an important shift from studying the music

1 Commercial standard introduced in the 1980s for the exchange of information between electronic

instruments and computers.

The Psychology of Music. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-381460-9.00009-2

© 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Page 2: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

theoretical aspects of music, that is, as notated in the score (e.g., Cooper & Meyer,

1960; Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983), to studying the cognitive aspects of music,

that is, as performed and perceived (e.g., Clarke, 1999; Gabrielsson, 1999;

Longuet-Higgins, 1987). This shift is partly based on the realization that there are

important differences between what is notated in a score, what can be measured in

a sound signal, and what is perceived by the listener (Figure 1). For instance,

a meter, while often made explicit by a time signature in the score, cannot be

(a) Audio (measured )

(b) Performance (measured )

(c) Grouping (perceived )

(d)4 3 1

Rhythm (perceived )

(e) Meter (perceived )

(f) Tempo (perceived )

(g) Timing (perceived ) 0

d →

s →

s →

t →

t →

1

f →

0

a → Figure 1 Decomposition of a

rhythmic signal in directly

measurable and perceived

components (a denotes

amplitude, t denotes time,

s position in the score, f tempo

factor, and d timing

deviation).

370 Henkjan Honing

Page 3: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

directly measured in a sounding rhythm. It is actually induced in the listener: the

listener constructs a metrical interpretation while listening to music. Further

aspects of rhythm, such as timing and tempo, are also clearly of a perceptual

nature—next to being an intrinsic aspect of performance—and cannot directly,

at least not without a model, be measured in a rhythmic signal. Hence the fields of

perception and cognition play an important role in the study of musical rhythm.

In addition, in the past few decades, a change can be observed from studying

rhythm and timing from a psychophysical perspective (studying the relation

between stimulus and sensation) using relatively simple stimulus materials

(cf. Handel, 1989), to studies in which the ecological validity of the materials used,

the task, and the effect of musical context have all been taken into account

(Clarke & Cook, 2004; Honing & Ladinig, 2008; Honing & Reips, 2008). In its

entirety, this has resulted in a substantial body of work, of which this chapter can

present only a small selection focusing on the cognitive science of rhythm and

timing. An extensive bibliography on rhythm-related studies of the past 10 years

will complement this selection.

And last, a strong connection with other structural aspects of music (see e.g.,

Chapters 6, 7, and 8, this volume), as well as the intimate relation between music

perception and performance, should be borne in mind (see Chapters 10 and 14, this

volume; Sadakata, Desain, & Honing, 2006).

II. Overview: Decomposing the Rhythmic Signal

In order to give some structure to the notion of rhythm, one possible way of

decomposing rhythm will be proposed here, primarily as a way to introduce the

concepts used in rhythm perception research.

A rhythm can be considered as consisting of several components, such as

rhythmic pattern, meter, tempo, and timing. Most listeners are able to derive these

different types of information from the acoustic signal when listening to music

(see Figure 1).

A first component is the perceived rhythmic category, referred to as a rhythmic

pattern. This is a pattern of durations that can be represented on a discrete,

symbolic scale (see Figure 1d). This concept differs from the notion of a performed

or expressively timed rhythm that is measured on a continuous scale (see

Figure 1b). A rhythmic pattern is comparable to rhythm as it is notated in a musical

score. In music theory, when referring to rhythm, this rhythmic pattern is meant

(Cooper & Meyer, 1960). Rhythmic pattern is also related to the process of catego-

rization as studied in music cognition (Desain & Honing, 1991), the process of

deriving rhythmic categories from a continuous rhythmic signal.

A second component of a rhythmic signal is the metrical structure that a listener

might assign to it. A rhythm is often interpreted in a metrical framework, be it a

regular pulse (or beat) or a hierarchically organized interpretation of two or more

levels of beat (see Figure 1e). Beat is related to the notion of tactus, as it has been

3719. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 4: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

discussed in music theory (Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983), and to the cognitive pro-

cess of beat induction (how listeners arrive at a sensation of regular pulse when lis-

tening to a varying rhythm), as it is studied in music cognition (Honing, 2012;

Parncutt, 1994; Povel & Essens, 1985). Although some theories suggest that

rhythm exists solely under metric interpretation (Longuet-Higgins, 1994), one

could also consider the figural aspects of rhythm: rhythm as a sequential pattern of

durational accents that can be grouped at the surface level (Handel, 1993). These

groups can again be hierarchically ordered (see Figure 1c), commonly referred to

as grouping (Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983) or rhythmical structure.

A third component of a rhythmic signal is tempo: the impression of the speed or

rate (and changes thereof) of the sounding pattern (Michon, 1967; see Figure 1f ).

Although it is still unclear what exactly constitutes the perception of tempo,

it seems to be related—at least in metrical music—to the cognitive notion of beat

or tactus: the speed at which the pulse of the music passes at a moderate rate,

that is, between 500 and 700 ms (Fraisse, 1982; London, 2004/2012). In music the-

ory, the notion of tempo is only sparsely addressed (Epstein, 1994).

A fourth component is expressive timing that carries the nuances in a rhythm

that can make it sound, for example, “mechanical,” “laid-back,” or “rushed” (see

Figure 1g). This is apparently caused by the fact that some notes are played

somewhat earlier or later than expected. Timing is thus not relevant to music the-

ory, but of considerable interest to music cognition research. For example, why is

a rhythm with a slightly shorter note not simply perceived as a different rhythm?

And how do all these components (i.e., rhythmic pattern, meter, timing, tempo,

etc.) interact to shape our perception of a rhythm?

In the remainder of the chapter, these four rhythmic components are discussed,

along with the perceptual and cognitive processes that can be associated with

rhythm. However, to visualize rhythm and timing in a general way, first a graphic

representation is introduced that allows the abstract space of all possible rhythms,

timed in all possible ways, to be visualized.

III. Structure and Interpretation: Visualizing Rhythm Space

A common method in the study of rhythm and timing in music is to analyze a

number of typical examples from the music literature (Epstein, 1994; Kramer,

1988; Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983). This approach, however, may cause difficulties

in that the insights obtained may be dependent on the specific choice of examples,

idiosyncrasies, or musical style. For some of the topics addressed in this chapter,

we will therefore use a visualization that captures the space of all rhythms. This is

an abstract mathematical notion that captures all possible rhythms in all possible

interpretations (Desain & Honing, 2003; Honing, 2002).

To be able to visualize this abstract rhythm space, we restrict ourselves to four-

note rhythms with three interonset intervals (IOIs). These IOIs can be projected in

a three-dimensional rhythm space in which each axis represents the length of one

372 Henkjan Honing

Page 5: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

time interval (Figure 2). Every point in that space represents a rhythm of three

intervals of a certain duration (a rhythmic signal; cf. Figure 1b). The total duration

of a four-note rhythm is shorter if the point is close to the origin and longer if it is

moved farther away from the origin.

When we reduce the space to all rhythms of a certain total duration (e.g.,

all rhythms with a total duration of 1 s), this can be depicted as a triangular

cross-section of the rhythm space (indicated in gray in Figure 2). We call this

triangle a chronotopological map (“chronos”5 time, “topos”5 place) or, alter-

natively, a rhythm chart (Figure 3).

To give an example of how to read this chart, consider rhythm A (see left side

of Figure 3). This rhythm consists of four onsets (at 0.0, 0.25, 0.75 and 1.0 s),

can be represented with the IOIs 0.25, 0.50, and 0.25 s, and is a single point in the

1

1

0

1

Inte

rval

1 (s

) →

Interval 3 (s)

Interval 2 (s) →

Figure 2 Rhythm space. A cross-section (in the shape of a

triangle) indicates all rhythms of equal length, in this case,

all rhythms with a length of 1 s.

0

0.25 0.50– 0.25–

.25 .50 .75 1

0

0.25 – 0.25 0.50–

time (s) →.25 .50 .75 1

(a)

(b)

Figure 3 Two sample rhythms (left) and their location in a chronotopological map or

rhythm chart (right).

3739. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 6: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

triangle. To locate it in the rhythm chart shown on the right side of Figure 3,

first find the value of the first interval on the relevant axis, and follow the line to

the inside of the triangle, in the direction parallel to the side of the triangle.

Then do the same for the other two intervals, and the point where the three arrows

converge indicates the position of rhythm A in rhythm space.

This chronotopological map (Figure 3, right) contains not only frequently occur-

ring rhythms from music all over the world, but also very unusual ones. There are

no exceptions; all possible rhythms consisting of three IOIs, and all possible ways

of timing them, are included. We will use such a map in discussing the four com-

ponents of rhythm mentioned before (rhythm, meter, timing, and tempo) and the

interplay between them.

IV. Rhythmic Pattern: Representation

Technically, any series of sounds or events that has duration can be called a

‘‘rhythm’’; the greater the number of component durations and the variety of

their sequential organization, the more complex the rhythm (London, 2001).

Taxonomies of rhythmic patterns go back to Aristoxenus (cf. Cooper & Meyer,

1960; for more recent attempts to quantify rhythmic complexity, see Pressing,

1999; Shmulevich & Povel, 2000; Thul & Toussaint, 2008). In Western music

notation, rhythm is often notated proportionally (Figure 4). This can be called a

proportional or relative representation, because it indicates how the durational

intervals between the notes relate (e.g., in Figure 4, the second IOI is twice as

long as the first one).

However this notation is a music theoretical concept, and above all a practical

means for musicians to write down and remember rhythmic patterns. In itself,

it doesn’t say anything about the tempo (the speed at which it’s played) or the

timing (whether a particular note is played too early or too late) of a particular

rhythm. For example, if you were to ask a percussionist to play the rhythm

depicted in Figure 4, then it might look like the following series of measure-

ments: 0.23�0.58�0.29 sec. This is called an absolute representation, “absolute”

because from this series of measurements, the rhythm can be reproduced exactly

(for instance with a computer). It turns out that, even if a musician attempts to

play exactly in time with a metronome (i.e., a “deadpan” performance), some-

thing of his or her original timing and phrasing will still remain (Clarke, 1987).

The converse is also true: even if the rhythm sounds precisely as marked in the

score, then that doesn’t mean that it has been performed exactly as notated (see

Section V and Figure 9).

Figure 4 A proportional representation of rhythm as is common in

Western music notation.

374 Henkjan Honing

Page 7: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

We can now ask ourselves: when will a rhythmic signal (cf. Figure 1b) be heard

as rhythm X, and when as rhythm Y, because, for instance, it is performed with

some expressive timing? As a listener, how does one differentiate between rhythm

and timing? To be able to answer these questions, we will now discuss categorization:

the cognitive process by which we recognize, classify, or divide objects in the

world around us (Harnad, 2005).

V. Rhythmic Pattern and Timing: Categorization

How do listeners distill a discrete, symbolic rhythmic pattern (cf. Figure 1d) from a

series of continuous intervals, that is, the rhythmic signal (cf. Figure 1b)? Fraisse

(1982) stressed the importance of low integer ratios (like 1:1 and 1:2) in the percep-

tion of rhythm, ratios toward which noninteger rhythms will migrate. Other authors

(Nakajima, 1987) suggested that categorization can be expressed as a psychophysi-

cal function, mapping continuous intervals into discrete categories, independent of

context. Still others (Clarke, 1987) investigated whether this categorization might in

fact be a result of categorical perception (Harnad, 1987). However, the implication

of true categorical perception is that expressive timing would be barely detectable,

which clearly is not the case. Timing is actually one of the most compelling aspects

of music (Ashley, 2002; Hudson, 1994). Hence, categorization is not simply a map-

ping from a continuous variable to a discrete one—losing important continuous

information in the process. In fact, both types of information, the rhythmic pattern

and expressive timing, are available at the same time, with the categorization func-

tioning as a reference relative to which timing deviations are perceived (Clarke,

2000; Desain & Honing, 2003; Sadakata et al., 2006).

As an example of this process, consider the rhythm shown in Figure 5 (the

downward arrow depicting rhythm perception). Next to the recognition of the

rhythmic categories (represented by integers in Figure 5, bottom), a listener also

perceives the expressive timing of the performed rhythm. Even untrained listeners

can appreciate the exaggerated “against the beat” quality of the triplets (Honing &

Haas, 2008) and the natural slowing down at the end of the group of sixteenth notes

(Palmer, 1997).

Categorization in rhythm perception has commonly been studied by presenting

interpolations between different rhythmic patterns to listeners in both an identifica-

tion task (probing the recognition of categories) and a discrimination task (testing

for increased sensitivity near category boundaries). As such, it applies the paradigm

developed for categorical perception (Harnad, 1987) as used, for instance, in the

domains of speech (Repp, 1984), color perception (Saunders & van Brakel, 1997),

and the perception of melodic intervals (Burns & Ward, 1978).

Clarke (1987) describes an experiment in which a short musical sequence was

presented in two different metrical contexts (a duple and a triple meter), with the

two notes at the end of the sequence systematically being varied between the ratios

1:1 and 1:2 (Figure 6). The participants performed an identification task in which

3759. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 8: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

they had to identify the rhythm as belonging to type 1:1 or type 1:2 and a discrimi-

nation task in which they judged whether a pair of rhythms was same or different.

The resulting identification function showed a strong change in slope at the

category boundary between the two rhythms, and the discrimination function has a

strong peak in the same position, which is conventionally taken as clear evidence

for categorical perception. At the same time, however, the judgments were influ-

enced by the metrical context, showing that the category boundary is not fixed and

can be shifted by metrical context.

Schulze (1989) did a follow-up study addressing some of the methodological pro-

blems of Clarke’s study (Figure 7), the main point being that the forced-choice para-

digm used in the identification task steered the participant’s responses toward the

available categories. Schulze therefore used a somewhat different experimental

setup, in which he trained two participants with a set of interpolated rhythms and

asked them to give a graded identification response (i.e., as many response catego-

ries as stimulus types). The discrimination function was derived indirectly from

these responses. He showed that categorical rhythm perception is open to perceptual

Figure 6 Stimuli used in Clarke

(1987).

Figure 5 Example of two representations of time that are present in music: (top) a

performed rhythm in continuous time, and (bottom) the perceived rhythmic categories in

discrete, symbolic time. (A sound example is available at http://www.hum.uva.nl/mmm

/categorization/.)

376 Henkjan Honing

Page 9: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

learning: the participants were able to distinguish more categories after an intensive

training period before the experimental trials.

By contrast, Desain, Honing, and their colleagues undertook a series of empiri-

cal studies (e.g., Desain & Honing, 2003) in which a considerably larger set of tem-

poral patterns were used as stimuli: 66 four-note rhythms with a fixed total

duration of 1 s, systematically sampled over the whole rhythm space (see left side

of Figure 8).2 The results of a first experiment, in which participants were asked to

notate this set of 66 rhythms (an identification task), are summarized in the right

side of Figure 8. The thick lines demarcate the observed boundaries between the

different rhythm categories.

One can see, in Figure 8, that not all categories are of the same shape and size

(as would be predicted by a straightforward rounding-off algorithm, as used by

some music notation software). Some rhythms allow for considerable timing vari-

ation, without the risk of being perceived as another rhythm (e.g., the rhythmic

categories 1:2:1 and 1:1:2). Other rhythms allow for little variation in the timing

(e.g., 2:3:1 and 3:1:4). If timing variation is applied to these rhythms, they are

quickly perceived as an altogether different rhythm. One should note, however,

that Figure 8 depicts quite a reduction of the responses (for a complete, color

.5 .25

D

BA

C

← Interval 2 (s)

Inte

rval

1 (s

) →

Inte

rval

1 (s

) →

Interval 3 (s) →

Interval 3 (s) →

.5

.75

.25.75

.5

1 0.75 0.50 0.25 01

0.75

0.500.50

0.25

0

0.25

01

0.75

.250

← Interval 2 (s)

Rhythm

A 3:1:2B 4:1:3C 2:1:3D 3:1:4

Figure 7 Stimuli and results of Schulze (1989) depicted as a chronotopological map

(zooming-in on a small part of the rhythm space, indicated by the diagram at the left). The

gray lines indicate the perceived category boundaries. The gray area is the hypothetical

shape of the rhythmic category A. The dots identify the (interpolated) rhythms, crosses mark

the mechanical ones (i.e., A, B, C and D).

2 Desain and Honing (2003) reported only on two of the experiments. The full results, including those

that show the effect of overall tempo and dynamic accent, are available online at http://www.hum.uva

.nl/mmm/categorization.

3779. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 10: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

visualization, see Desain & Honing, 2003 or the URL mentioned in footnote 2).

Each islet in Figure 8 (right) is, in fact, a mountain, the summit of which indi-

cates the rhythm notated by the majority of listeners: the “modal” or most fre-

quently identified rhythm (Figure 9).

Interestingly, this modal rhythm never overlaps with the metronomic version,

but with a rhythm that incorporates some timing variation. For instance, the major-

ity of listeners perceived the category 1:1:1, the most frequently selected notation

for a rhythm that was timed as 0.33�0.31�0.36 s; a minority of listeners notated

1:1:1 for the rhythm 0.33�0.33�0.33 (the metronomic rhythm as indicated by the

score). Apparently, if the rhythm 1:1:1 is phrased with a small ritardando, then it

sounds more like the notated rhythm than if a computer had played the rhythm

exactly as notated. This suggests that the rhythmic categories are not simply

Figure 9 Mechanical versions of

characteristic rhythms and their

most frequently identified

interpretation (modal ).

Figure 8 (Left) The sampling of the rhythm space as used in the experiments mentioned in

the text, and (right) the results with lines marking the category boundaries (Experiment 1

from Desain & Honing, 2003).

378 Henkjan Honing

Page 11: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

integer-related rhythms such as notated in a score (see Repp, London, & Keller,

2010 for a replication of this effect for two-interval rhythms).

Furthermore, as was already noted by Clarke (1987), this categorization process

remains open to top-down cognitive influences, either influenced by the preceding

musical context (veridical expectation) or influenced by expectations as a result of

earlier exposure to music (schematic expectation; Bharucha, 1994; Huron, 2006).

When the same rhythms as used in the experiment depicted in Figure 8 are presented

in a duple or a triple meter context (i.e., primed with a beat that is subdivided either

in two or in three), the perceived categories change dramatically (Figure 10). For

example, when the rhythm 0.26�0.42�0.32 (marked with an asterisk in Figure 10)

was preceded by a rhythmic context that induced a duple meter, it was heard by

most of the participants as 1:2:1 (category marked 2 in the left side of Figure 10).

However, when the same rhythm was preceded by a rhythmic context that induced a

triple meter, then the majority of participants heard that as 1:3:2 (category marked 9

in the right side of Figure 10).3

It is puzzling, however, that although meter was shown to exert a strong influence

on the recognition of rhythm (Clarke, 1987; Desain & Honing, 2003), existing

computational models of meter can explain this phenomenon only to a small extent

(Desain & Honing, 2001). This can be considered as additional empirical support for

the idea that there is more to rhythm than meter alone, as has been emphasized in

several musicological and music theoretical studies (Honing, 2002; London, 2001).

Figure 10 The effect of duple (left) and triple (right) meter on rhythmic categorization

(Experiment 2 from Desain & Honing, 2003).

3 This empirical fact is problematic for theories that are based on musical score representations (such as

Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983): It reveals a circularity in which the leaves (or notes) of an analytic tree

(e.g., a metrical analysis) are a result of a structural interpretation to which these leaves (or notes) are,

in fact, the input. The paradox can only be resolved when one makes a distinction between the continu-

ous rhythmic signal (cf. Figure 1b) and the perceived rhythmic categories (cf. Figure 1d).

3799. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 12: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Overall, this research suggests that expressive timing cannot simply be defined

as a deviation from what is notated in the score (Todd, 1992). Based on the empiri-

cal results presented earlier, a more realistic interpretation might be that it is the most

frequently heard performance of a rhythm—rather than the canonical or integer-

related version of it, as notated in a score—that might function as a reference or cate-

gory. Or, in other words, expressive timing is best defined as a deviation from the

most frequently heard version of a rhythm, which depends on memory.

Nevertheless, the research just described also has its limitations. One of these is

related to the identification task used: the participants were asked to notate care-

fully what they had heard. This is a task that assumes specialized skills and restricts

the group of potential participants considerably, and as such, the generality of the

results. One possible alternative is to use a discrimination task instead, which

makes the task doable for most listeners and can form an alternative technique for

constructing the rhythmic charts shown earlier.

What we can conclude however, is that the ability to hear nuances in the timing

of a piece of music is an outcome of a cognitive process of categorization which,

in turn, is dependent on memory (Snyder, 2001), expectation (Huron, 2006), and

the ways in which we have been exposed to music in the course of our lives

(Honing, 2011a). A listener does not perceive rhythm as an abstract unity, as is

notated in a score, nor as a continuum in the way that physicists describe time.

We actually tend to hear rhythm and timing in what one might call “clumps”: the

islets that can be seen on the chronotopological maps shown earlier.

VI. Metrical Structure

A. Syncopation

The second component of rhythm I will discuss here is metrical structure. I will use

the terms pulse (or beat) to refer to a highly salient, periodic layer of articulation in

the musical texture (normally with a period between 400 and 800 ms) and meter to

refer to the emergent (or induced) temporal structure of at least two levels of pulse.

An important distinction to be made is that between rhythmic pattern and metri-

cal structure. Although some music theorists question and deemphasize this distinc-

tion (Hasty, 1997), the general consensus is that rhythm and meter are in fact two

very separate notions (London, 2001). While rhythm can be characterized as the

varying pattern of durations that is perceived to be present in the music, meter

involves our perception and, more importantly, (embodied) anticipation of such

rhythmic patterns. Meter is, as such, a cognitive phenomenon (London, 2012;

Longuet-Higgins, 1994).

The interaction of rhythm and meter, and the role that cognition plays in its per-

ception and appreciation can be illustrated with the phenomenon of syncopation.

It is often described, rather informally, as “an accent that has been moved for-

ward,” or as “a technique often used by composers to avoid regularity in rhythm by

displacing an emphasis in that rhythm.” (Oxford Music Online, 2011). To illustrate

380 Henkjan Honing

Page 13: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

this, consider the two rhythms depicted in Figure 11. Which of these is a synco-

pated rhythm?

A formally trained musician will easily point out the left example, guided by the

slur marking a syncopation. However, as performed by a drum computer, these

notated rhythms will sound identical. The reader here is influenced by the notation.

When we listen to a rhythm (even if it is simply a series of isochronous clicks, like

a clock), we tend to interpret it in a metrical fashion (Brochard, Abecasis, Potter,

Ragot, & Drake, 2003) and hear it as syncopated, or not, depending on the metric

interpretation. A time signature in the notation is no guarantee that a listener will

perceive the meter as such. This is illustrated by the example in Figure 12.

Western listeners tend to project a duple meter while listening to a rhythm

(Drake & Bertrand, 2001) and hence perceive a syncopation (depicted on the left

of Figure 12). However, if a listener were to expect, for example, a compound

meter (as on the right of Figure 12), then the syncopation will disappear altogether.

An important insight here is that the perception of rhythm should be seen as an

interaction between the music—the sounding rhythm—and the listener—who

projects a certain meter onto it (Fitch & Rosenfeld, 2007; Honing, 2011b; Longuet-

Higgins & Lee, 1984). We can therefore use the presence (or absence) of a

perceived syncopation as evidence for the presence (or absence) of a strong metric

expectation, which provides a method to probe metrical expectation in listeners

(see Section VI,B).

B. Beat Induction as a Fundamental Cognitive Skill

Beat induction4 is the cognitive skill that allows us to hear a regular pulse in music

and enables our synchronization with it. Perceiving this regularity in music allows

us to dance and make music together. As such, it can be considered a fundamental

human trait that, arguably, played a decisive role in the origin of music (Honing &

Ploeger, in press). Furthermore, it might be considered a spontaneously developing

and domain-specific skill, as projecting or perceiving a regular pulse seems to be

absent in spoken language (e.g., Patel, 2008).

Beat induction has been a topic of many music perception studies, mostly

concerned with the theoretical and psychological aspects of this cognitive skill

(Desain & Honing, 1999; Large & Jones, 1999; Parncutt, 1994; Povel & Essens,

Figure 11 Which rhythm is syncopated?

4 The term beat induction is preferred here over beat perception (and synchronization) to emphasize that

a beat does not always need to be physically present in order to be ‘perceived’ (cf. Figure 12).

Furthermore, it stresses that beat induction is not a passive, perceptual process but an active one in

which a rhythm evokes a particular regular pattern in the listener. How far this process is open to learn-

ing, and whether there might be a cognitive and neurological difference between beat induction and

meter induction, is a topic of current research (Honing, 2012).

3819. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 14: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

1985). More recently, the phenomenon has attracted the interest of developmental

psychologists (Hannon & Trehub, 2005), cognitive biologists (Fitch, 2006), evolu-

tionary psychologists (Honing & Ploeger, in press), and neuroscientists (Grahn &

Brett, 2007; Grube, Cooper, Chinnery, & Griffiths, 2010) as a skill that is funda-

mental to music processing that can be argued to be an innate (or spontaneously

developing), domain-specific and species-specific skill.

However, with regard to the first issue, scientists are still divided whether this

ability develops spontaneously (emphasizing a biological basis) or whether it is

learned (emphasizing a cultural basis). Some authors consider a sensitivity to the

beat to be acquired during the first year of life, suggesting that the ways in which

babies are rocked and bounced in time to music by their parents is the most impor-

tant factor in developing a sense for metrical structure (cf. Trehub & Hannon,

2006). By contrast, more recent studies emphasize a biological basis, suggesting

beat induction to be specifically tuned to music; studies have shown that beat

induction is already functional in young infants (Zentner & Eerola, 2010) as well

as 2- to 3-day-old newborns (Winkler, Haden, Ladinig, Sziller, & Honing, 2009).

These recent empirical findings can be taken as support for a genetic predisposition

for beat induction, rather than it being a result of learning (Honing, Ladinig,

Winkler, & Haden, 2009).

Furthermore, developmental studies suggest that infants are not only sensitive to

a regular pulse, but also to meter (two or more levels of pulse; Hannon & Johnson,

2005). Thus it is possible that humans possess some processing predisposition to

extract hierarchically structured regularities from complex rhythmic patterns

Figure 12 Two possible notations (labeled as ‘Score’) of the same rhythm (labeled as

‘Rhythm’). In the left example a metrical tree represents a duple meter, in the right example

it represents a compound meter (labeled as ‘Listener’). The numbers at the leaves of the

metrical tree represent the theoretical metric salience (the depth of the tree at that position in

the rhythm). A negative difference between the metric salience of a certain note N and the

succeeding rest R indicates a syncopation (Longuet-Higgins & Lee, 1984). The more

negative this difference, the more syncopated the note N or ‘louder’ the rest R.

382 Henkjan Honing

Page 15: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

(Ladinig, Honing, Haden, & Winkler, 2009). Research with newborns provides the

context within which to understand more about these fundamental capacities

(Honing, 2012; Winkler et al., 2009).

VII. Tempo and Timing: Perceptual Invariance

The two components of rhythm that are studied in the domain of music cognition,

but rarely in music theory, are tempo and timing. Timing plays an important role in

the performance, perception, and appreciation of almost all types of music. It has

been studied extensively in both music perception and performance research (for

the latter, see Palmer, Chapter 10, this volume). The most important outcome of

this research is that a significant component of the timing patterns found in music

performance—commonly referred to as expressive timing—can be explained in

terms of musical structure, such as the recurrent patterns associated with the metrical

structure that are used in jazz swing (Ashley, 2002; Honing & Haas, 2008) or the

typical slowing down at the end of phrases in classical music from the Romantic

period (Honing, 2003; Palmer, 1997). These timing patterns help in communicating

the temporal structure (such as rhythm and phrase structure) to the listener.

Furthermore, timing is adapted with regard to the global tempo: at different tempi,

other structural levels of the music are emphasized and the expressive timing is

adapted accordingly (Clarke, 1995, 1999). One might wonder, however, whether tim-

ing would remain perceptually invariant over tempo, as has been found in other cog-

nitive domains, including speech (Perkell & Klatt, 1986), motor behavior (Heuer,

1991), and object motion (Shepard, 2001).

Perceptual invariance has been the topic of several studies in music perception

(Handel, 1992; Hulse, Takeuchi, & Braaten, 1992; Repp, 1995). A well-known and

relatively uncontroversial example is melody (Dowling & Harwood, 1986). When a

melody is transposed to a different register, it not only maintains its frequency

ratios in performance, but it is also perceived as the same melody (i.e., melody

remains perceptually invariant under transposition). With respect to other aspects

of music, such as rhythm, there is less agreement in the literature.

Whereas one might expect rhythm to scale proportionally with tempo in pro-

duction and to be perceptually invariant under tempo transformation, a number of

studies have shown that this is not always the case (Handel, 1992; Monahan &

Hirsh, 1990). With respect to rhythmic pattern, listeners often do not recognize

proportionally scaled rhythms as being identical (Handel, 1993). With respect to

timing, rhythms are timed differently at different tempi (Repp, Windsor, &

Desain, 2002).

With regard to expressive timing, the literature has been divided over whether it

is relationally invariant over tempo (Desain & Honing, 1994; Repp, 1994).

Although earlier perceptual studies present rather inconclusive evidence (Repp,

1994, 1995), more recently the idea that musical performances might be understood

in terms of a tempo curve—a mental representation of timing abstracted from the

rhythmic material and overall tempo, that can be represented as a continuous shape

3839. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 16: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

or function (Desain & Honing, 1993; Honing, 2003)—has been critiqued as a

musical (Honing, 2003), perceptual (Honing, 2006a) and cognitive construct

(Honing, 2001, 2005). The alternative view proposes a richer memory representa-

tion of timing than what can be captured by an unstructured tempo curve; such

curves form the basis of several models of musical time (Epstein, 1994; Feldman,

Epstein & Richards 1992; Friberg & Sundberg, 1999; Kronman & Sundberg,

1987; Longuet-Higgins & Lisle, 1989; Sundberg & Verillo, 1980; Todd, 1992;

Todd, 1985). These recent results have led to a tempo-specific timing hypothesis

that has been shown to be valid in the perception of baroque and classical music

(Honing, 2006a) as well as in jazz performance (Honing & Haas, 2008).

However, one might hypothesize that the absence of relational invariance in the

perceptual studies mentioned is merely a result of expert knowledge, as the parti-

cipants were mostly musicians who were very familiar with a particular musical

repertoire.

To test this, Honing and Ladinig (2009) studied the influence of exposure ver-

sus expertise in making expressive timing judgments. This was done using an

online listening experiment in which listeners with different musical preferences

(exposure) and music education (expertise) were asked to compare two perfor-

mances of the same composition (15 pairs, grouped in three musical genres),

one of which was tempo-transformed (manipulating the expressive timing). The

study showed that these judgments are not primarily influenced by expertise (e.g.,

years of formal training) but mainly by exposure to a certain musical idiom.

The interplay of familiarity with a particular genre (exposure) and the level of

formal musical training (expertise) had a significant effect on discriminating a

real from a manipulated performance. In short: both in perception and in produc-

tion, timing seems to be tempo-specific.

VIII. Rhythm and Movement: Embodied Cognition

The majority of the research discussed earlier, however, focuses on rhythm cogni-

tion as a perceptual phenomenon. Therefore, in this final section, some of the more

recent work on embodied cognition is discussed.

The relation between musical rhythm and motion has been studied in a large

body of theoretical and empirical work. Early examples, from the 1920s, include

work by Alexander Truslit and Gustav Becking (see Shove & Repp, 1995, for an

overview). More recently, a number of authors (Todd, Cousins, & Lee, 2007;

Trainor, Gao, Lei, Lehtovaara & Harris, 2009) have presented evidence suggesting

a direct link between musical rhythm and movement. The link is “direct” in the

sense that it is argued that rhythm perception is influenced (or even determined) by

our physiology and body metrics, from the functioning of our vestibular system to

leg length and body size. Although there is apparently no relation between body

build and walking speed (Macdougall & Moore, 2005), some theories suggest a

direct connection between body build and preferred tempo (Todd et al., 2007).

384 Henkjan Honing

Page 17: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

The latter theory makes the peculiar prediction that body length will have an effect

on rhythm perception: longer people will prefer slower musical tempi, and shorter

people will prefer faster ones (cf. Repp, 2007).

In a rather different approach, Phillips-Silver and Trainor (2005) conducted a

developmental study that showed that body movement (rather than body size) can

influence rhythm perception. They asked mothers to rock their 7-month-old chil-

dren in time to a rhythm that could be heard in two different ways: in time (as in

a march) or in time (as in a waltz). Half of the babies were swung backward and

forward on every second beat of the bar; the other half, on every third beat.

The researchers showed that the babies in the first group preferred rhythms with

accents on every second beat ( time), whereas the group that was rocked to a

three-beat tune had a preference for rhythms with an accent on every third beat

( time). Thus, in a surprisingly simple experiment, they demonstrated that move-

ment indeed influences the perception of rhythm.

In short, there have been a number of studies that have investigated the ways in

which physiological phenomena (e.g., heart rate, spontaneous tapping rate, walking

speed) might influence or even determine rhythm perception (Leman, 2007;

Phillips-Silver & Trainor, 2008). It is important to acknowledge that these embod-

ied explanations of rhythm perception, however simple they might seem, form a

significant addition to the “mentalist” or cognitive perspective that I have largely

adopted in this chapter.

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this chapter was supported by the Hendrik Muller chair designated on behalf

of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences (KNAW), and was written while I

was a member of the Research Priority Area “Brain & Cognition” at the University of

Amsterdam. I gratefully acknowledge the constructive criticisms and detailed comments of

Eric F. Clarke, Justin London, and Caroline Palmer, and the aid of Fleur Bouwer and Sandra

Wallner in preparing this manuscript.

References

Ashley, R. (2002). Do[n’t] change a hair for me: The art of jazz rubato. Music Perception,

19(3), 311�332.

Bharucha, J. J. (1994). Tonality and expectation. In R. Aiello, & J. Sloboda (Eds.),

Musical perceptions (pp. 213�239). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Brochard, R., Abecasis, D., Potter, D., Ragot, R., & Drake, C. (2003). The “ticktock” of our

internal clock: Direct brain evidence of subjective accents in isochronous sequences.

Psychological Science, 14, 362�366.

3859. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 18: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Burns, E. M., & Ward, W. D. (1978). Categorical perception—phenomenon or epiphenome-

non: Evidence from experiments in the perception of melodic musical intervals. Journal

of the Acoustical Society of America, 63, 456�468.

Clarke, E. F. (1987). Categorical rhythm perception: An ecological perspective. In A.

Gabrielsson (Ed.), Action and perception in rhythm and music (pp. 19�33). Stockholm,

Sweden: Royal Swedish Academy of Music.

Clarke, E. F. (1995). Expression in performance: Generativity, perception and semiosis.

In J. Rink (Ed.), The practice of performance: Studies in musical interpretation

(pp. 21�54). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Clarke, E. F. (1999). Rhythm and timing in music. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music

(2nd ed., pp. 473�500). New York, NY: Academic Press.

Clarke, E. F. (2000). Categorical rhythm perception and event perception. In Proceedings of

the International Music Perception and Cognition Conference (CD ROM). Keele,

England: Keele University, Department of Psychology.

Clarke, E. F., & Cook, N. (Eds.), (2004). Empirical musicology: Aims, methods, prospects

Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Cooper, G., & Meyer, L. B. (1960). The rhythmic structure of music. Chicago, IL:

University of Chicago Press.

Desain, P., & Honing, H. (1991). Quantization of musical time: A connectionist approach.

In P. M. Todd, & D. G. Loy (Eds.), Music and connectionism. Cambridge, MA: MIT

Press.

Desain, P., & Honing, H. (1993). Tempo curves considered harmful. In J. D. Kramer (Ed.),

“Time in contemporary musical thought.” Contemporary Music Review, 7(2),

123�138.

Desain, P., & Honing, H. (1994). Does expressive timing in music performance scale

proportionally with tempo? Psychological Research, 56, 285�292.

Desain, P., & Honing, H. (1999). Computational models of beat induction: The rule-based

approach. Journal of New Music Research, 28, 29�42.

Desain, P., & Honing, H. (2001). Modeling the effect of meter in rhythmic categorization:

Preliminary results. Journal of Music Perception and Cognition, 7, 145�156.

Desain, P., & Honing, H. (2003). The formation of rhythmic categories and metric priming.

Perception, 32, 341�365.

Dowling, W. J., & Harwood, D. (1986). Music cognition. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Drake, C., & Bertrand, D. (2001). The quest for universals in temporal processing in music.

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 17�27.

Epstein, D. (1994). Shaping time: Music, the brain and performance. London, England:

Schirmer.

Feldman, J., Epstein, D., & Richards, W. (1992). Force dynamics of tempo change in music.

Music Perception, 10(2), 185�204.

Fitch, W. T. (2006). The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective.

Cognition, 100, 173.

Fitch, W. T., & Rosenfeld, A. J. (2007). Perception and production of syncopated rhythms.

Music Perception, 25(1), 43�58.

Fraisse, P. (1982). Rhythm and tempo. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music

(pp. 149�180). New York, NY: Academic Press.

Friberg, A., & Sundberg, J. (1999). Does music performance allude to locomotion? A model

of final ritardandi derived from measurements of stopping runners. Journal of the

Acoustical Society of America, 105(3), 1469�1484.

386 Henkjan Honing

Page 19: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Gabrielsson, A. (1999). The performance of music. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music

(2nd ed., pp. 501�602). New York, NY: Academic Press.

Grahn, J. A., & Brett, M. (2007). Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(5), 893�906.

Grube, M., Cooper, F. E., Chinnery, P. F., & Griffiths, T. D. (2010). Dissociation of duration-

based and beat-based auditory timing in cerebellar degeneration. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 11597�11601.

Handel, S. (1989). Listening: An introduction to the perception of auditory events.

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Handel, S. (1992). The differentiation of rhythmic structure. Perception & Psychophysics,

52, 497�507.

Handel, S. (1993). The effect of tempo and tone duration on rhythm discrimination.

Perception & Psychophysics, 54(3), 370�382.

Hannon, E. E., & Trehub, S. E. (2005). Metrical categories in infancy and adulthood.

Psychological Science, 16, 48�55.

Hannon, E. E., & Johnson, S. P. (2005). Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and melo-

dies: Implications for musical structure learning. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 354�377.

Harnad, S. (Ed.) (1987). Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition. New York,

NY: Cambridge University Press.

Harnad, S. (2005). To cognize is to categorize: Cognition is categorization. In H. Cohen, &

C. Lefebvre (Eds.), Handbook on categorization. Amsterdam, The Netherlands:

Elsevier.

Hasty, C. F. (1997). Meter as rhythm. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Heuer, H. (1991). Invariant relative timing in motor-program theory. In J. Fagard, & P. H.

Wolff (Eds.), The development of timing control and temporal organisation in

coordinated action (pp. 37�68). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

Honing, H. (2001). From time to time: The representation of timing and tempo. Computer

Music Journal, 35(3), 50�61.

Honing, H. (2002). Structure and interpretation of rhythm and timing. Dutch Journal for

Music Theory, 7(3), 227�232.

Honing, H. (2003). The final ritard: On music, motion, and kinematic models. Computer

Music Journal, 27(3), 66�72.

Honing, H. (2005). Is there a perception-based alternative to kinematic models of tempo

rubato? Music Perception, 23, 79�85.

Honing, H. (2006a). Evidence for tempo-specific timing in music using a web-based

experimental setup. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and

Performance, 32(3), 780�786.

Honing, H. (2006b). On the growing role of observation, formalization and experimental

method in musicology. Empirical Musicology Review, 1(1), 2�5.

Honing, H. (2011a). Musical cognition: A science of listening. New Brunswick, NJ:

Transaction Publishers.

Honing, H. (2011b). The illiterate listener: On music cognition, musicality and methodology.

Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Amsterdam University Press.

Honing, H. (2012). Without it no music: Beat induction as a fundamental musical trait.

The Neurosciences and Music IV—Learning and Memory. Annals of the New York

Academy of Sciences, 1252, 85�91.

Honing, H., & Haas, W. B. (2008). Swing once more: Relating timing and tempo in expert

jazz drumming. Music Perception, 25(5), 471�477.

3879. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 20: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Honing, H., & Ladinig, O. (2008). The potential of the Internet for music perception

research: A comment on lab-based versus Web-based studies. Empirical Musicology

Review, 3(1), 4�7.

Honing, H., & Ladinig, O. (2009). Exposure influences expressive timing judgments in

music. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,

35(1), 281�288.

Honing, H., Ladinig, O., Winkler, I., & Haden, G. (2009). Is beat induction innate or

learned? Probing emergent meter perception in adults and newborns using event-related

brain potentials (ERP). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169:

The Neurosciences and Music III: Disorders and Plasticity, 93�96.

Honing, H., & Ploeger, A. (2012, in press). Cognition and the evolution of music: Pitfalls and

prospects. Topics in Cognitive Science (TopiCS). doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01210.x

Honing, H., & Reips, U.-D. (2008). Web-based versus lab-based studies: A response to

Kendall (2008). Empirical Musicology Review, 3(2), 73�77.

Hudson, R. (1994). Stolen time: The history of tempo rubato. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.

Hulse, S., Takeuchi, A. H., & Braaten, R. F. (1992). Perceptual invariances in the compara-

tive psychology of music. Music Perception, 10, 151�184.

Huron, D. (1999). The new empiricism: Systematic musicology in a postmodern age. Lecture 3

from the 1999 Ernest Bloch Lectures. ,http://musiccog.ohiostate.edu/Music220/Bloch

.lectures/3.Methodology.html.Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. Cambridge,

MA: MIT Press.

Kramer, J. (1988). The time of music. New York, NY: Schirmer.

Kronman, U., & Sundberg, J. (1987). Is the musical ritard an allusion to physical motion?

In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.), Action and perception in rhythm and music (pp. 57�68).

Stockholm, Sweden: Royal Swedish Academy of Music.

Ladinig, O., Honing, H., Haden, G., & Winkler, I. (2009). Probing attentive and pre-attentive

emergent meter in adult listeners with no extensive music training. Music Perception,

26, 377�386.

Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track time-

varying events. Psychological Review, 10(1), 119�159.

Leman, M. (2007). Embodied music cognition and mediation technology. Cambridge, MA:

MIT Press.

Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, MA:

MIT Press.

London, J. (2001). Entry on ‘Rhythm.’ The new Grove dictionary of music and musicians

(2nd ed.). London, England: Macmillan.

London, J. (2012). Hearing in time. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press (Original

work published 2004)

Longuet-Higgins, H. C. (1987). Mental processes: Studies in cognitive science. Cambridge,

MA: MIT Press.

Longuet-Higgins, H. C. (1994). Artificial intelligence and musical cognition. Philosophical

Transactions of the Royal Society London A, 329, 103�113.

Longuet-Higgins, H. C., & Lee, C. S. (1984). The rhythmic interpretation of monophonic

music. Music Perception, 1(4), 424�441. (Reprinted in Longuet-Higgins, 1987)

Longuet-Higgins, H. C., & Lisle, E. R. (1989). Modeling music cognition. Contemporary

Music Review, 3, 15�27.

388 Henkjan Honing

Page 21: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

MacDougall, H. G., & Moore, S. T. (2005). Marching to the beat of the same drummer:

The spontaneous tempo of human locomotion. Journal of Applied Physiology, 99,

1164�1173.

Michon, J. A. (1967). Timing in temporal tracking. Assen, The Netherlands: Van Gorcum.

Monahan, C. B., & Hirsh, I. J. (1990). Studies in auditory timing: 2. Rhythm patterns.

Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 227�242.

Nakajima, Y. (1987). A model of empty duration perception. Perception, 16, 485�520.

Oxford Music Online (2011). Entry ‘Syncopation’. See ,http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com

/subscriber/article/grove/music/27263..

Palmer, C. (1997). Music performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 115�138.

Parncutt, R. (1994). A perceptual model of pulse salience and metrical accent in musical

rhythms. Music Perception, 11, 409�464.

Parncutt, R. (2007). Systematic musicology and the history and future of Western musical

scholarship. Journal of Interdisciplinary Music Studies, 1, 1�32.

Patel, A. (2008). Music, language, and the brain. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Perkell, J. S., & Klatt, D. H. (1986). Invariance and variability in speech processes.

Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: Movement influences infants’

rhythm perception. Science, 308, 1430.

Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2008). Multisensory rhythm perception: Vestibular

influence on auditory metrical interpretation. Brain and Cognition, 67, 94�102.

Povel, D. J., & Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Perception, 2(4),

411�440.

Pressing, J. (1999). The referential dynamics of cognition and action. Psychological Review,

106, 714�747.

Repp, B. H. (1984). Categorical perception: Issues, methods, findings. In N. J. Lass (Ed.),

Speech and language, advances in basic research and practice (Vol. 10, pp. 242�335).

New York, NY: Academic Press.

Repp, B. H. (1994). Relational invariance of expressive microstructure across global tempo

changes in music performance: An exploratory study. Psychological Research, 56,

269�284.

Repp, B. H. (1995). Quantitative effects of global tempo on expressive timing in music

performance: Some perceptual evidence. Music Perception, 13, 39�57.

Repp, B. H. (2007). Embodied rhythm. Empirical Musicology Review, 2(1), 14�16.

Repp, B. H., London, J., & Keller, P. E. (2010). Perception-production relationships and

phase correction in synchronization with two-interval rhythms. Psychological Research,

75, 227�242.

Repp, B. H., Windsor, W. L., & Desain, P. (2002). Effects of tempo on the timing of simple

musical rhythms. Music Perception, 19, 565�593.

Sadakata, M, Desain, P., & Honing, H. (2006). The Bayesian way to relate rhythm percep-

tion and production. Music Perception, 23, 269�288.

Saunders, B. A. C., & van Brakel, J. (1997). Are there nontrivial constraints on colour

categorization? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 167�228.

Schulze, H. H. (1989). Categorical perception of rhythmic patterns. Psychological Research,

51, 10�15.

Shepard, R. (2001). Perceptual-cognitive universals as reflections of the world. Behavioral

and Brain Sciences, 24, 581�601.

3899. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 22: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Shmulevich, I., & Povel, D. (2000). Complexity measures of musical rhythms. In P. Desain, &

W. L. Windsor (Eds.), Rhythm perception and production (pp. 239�244). Lisse,

The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.

Shove, P., & Repp, B. H. (1995). Musical motion and performance: Theoretical and empirical

perspectives. In J. Rink (Ed.), The practice of performance (pp. 55�83). Cambridge,

UK: Cambridge University Press.

Snyder, B. (2001). Music and memory: An introduction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Sundberg, J., & Verillo, V. (1980). On the anatomy of the ritard: A study of timing in music.

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 68(3), 772�779.

Thul, E., & Toussaint, G. T. (2008). Analysis of musical rhythm complexity measures in a

cultural context (pp. 1�9). Proceedings of the Canadian Conference on Computer

Science and Software Engineering. Montreal, Canada: Concordia University.

Todd, N. P. M. (1985). A model of expressive timing in tonal music. Music Perception, 3(1),

33�58.

Todd, N. P. M. (1992). The dynamics of dynamics: A model of musical expression. Journal

of the Acoustical Society of America, 91(6), 3540.

Todd, N. P. M, Cousins, R., & Lee, C. S. (2007). The contribution of anthropometric

factors to individual differences in the perception of rhythm. Empirical Musicology

Review, 2, 1�13.

Trainor, L. J., Gao, X., Lei, J., Lehtovaara, K., & Harris, L. R. (2009). The primal role of the

vestibular system in determining musical rhythm. Cortex, 45, 35�43.

Trehub, S. E., & Hannon, E. E. (2006). Infant music perception: Domain-general or domain-

specific mechanisms? Cognition, 100(1), 73�99.

Winkler, I., Haden, G., Ladinig, O., Sziller, I., & Honing, H. (2009). Newborn infants detect

the beat in music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 2468�2471.

Zentner, M., & Eerola, T. (2010). Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 5768�5773.

Bibliography

Abe, J.-I., & Okada, A. (2004). Integration of metrical and tonal organization in melody per-

ception. Japanese Psychological Research, 46(4), 298�307. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5584

.2004.00262.x

Abecasis, D., Brochard, R., Granot, R. Y., & Drake, C. (2005). Differential brain response to

metrical accents in isochronous auditory sequences. Music Perception, 22(3), 549�562.

doi:10.1525/mp.2005.22.3.549

Agawu, V. K. (2006). Structural analysis or cultural analysis? Competing perspectives on the

“standard pattern” of West African rhythm. Journal of the American Musicological

Society, 59(1), 1�46. doi:10.1525/jams.2006.59.1.1

Ashley, R. (2002). Do[n’t] change a hair for me: The art of jazz rubato. Music Perception,

19(3), 311�332.

Bailes, F., & Dean, R. (2007). Listener detection of segmentation in computer-generated

sound: An exploratory experimental study. Journal of New Music Research, 36(2),

83�93. doi:10.1080/09298210701755123

Bailey, J. A., & Penhune, V. B. (2010). Rhythm synchronization performance and

auditory working memory in early- and late-trained musicians. Experimental Brain

Research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Experimentation Cerebrale, 204(1), 91�101.

doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2299-y

390 Henkjan Honing

Page 23: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Bartolo, R., & Merchant, H. (2009). Learning and generalization of time production in

humans: Rules of transfer across modalities and interval durations. Experimental Brain

Research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Experimentation Cerebrale, 197(1), 91�100.

doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1895-1

Bengtsson, S. L., Ullen, F., Ehrsson, H. H., Hashimoto, T., Kito, T., & Naito, E., et al.

(2009). Listening to rhythms activates motor and premotor cortices. Cortex: A Journal

Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 45(1), 62�71. doi:10.1016

/j.cortex.2008.07.002

Berens, M. S., & Pastore, R. E. (2005). Contextual relative temporal duration judgment:

An investigation of sequence interruptions. Perception & Psychophysics, 67(1),

102�119. Retrieved from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15912875.Bergeson, T. R., & Trehub, S. E. (2006). Infant’s perception of rhythmic patterns. Music

Perception, 23(4), 345�360.

Bettermann, H., Amponsah, D., Cysarz, D., & van Leeuwen, P. (1999). Musical rhythms in

heart period dynamics: A cross-cultural and interdisciplinary approach to cardiac

rhythms. The American Journal of Physiology, 277(5 Pt 2), H1762�H1770. Retrieved

from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10564129.Bispham, J. (2006). Rhythm in music: What is it? Who has it? And why? Music Perception,

24(2), 125�134.

Bobin-Begue, A., Provasi, J., Marks, A., & Pouthas, V. (2006). Influence of auditory tempo

on the endogenous rhythm of non-nutritive sucking. Revue Europeenne de Psychologie

Appliquee/European Review of Applied Psychology, 56(4), 239�245. doi:10.1016/j

.erap.2005.09.006

Boltz, M. G. (1998). Tempo discrimination of musical patterns: Effects due to pitch and

rhythmic structure. Perception & Psychophysics, 60(8), 1357�1373. Retrieved from

,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9865077.Boltz, M. G. (1999). The processing of melodic and temporal information: Independent or

unified dimensions? Journal of New Music Research, 28(1), 67�79. doi:10.1076

/jnmr.28.1.67.3121

Boltz, M. G. (2010). Rate and duration memory of naturalistic sounds. Acta Psychologica,

135(2), 168�181. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.06.004

Brochard, R., Touzalin, P., Despres, O., & Dufour, A. (2008). Evidence of beat perception

via purely tactile stimulation. Brain Research, 1223, 59�64. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.

2008.05.050

Brodsky, W., Henik, A., Rubinstein, B.-S., & Zorman, M. (2003). Auditory imagery from

musical notation in expert musicians. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(4), 602�612.

Retrieved from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12812282.Broughton, M., & Stevens, C. (2009). Music, movement and marimba: An investiga-

tion of the role of movement and gesture in communicating musical expression

to an audience. Psychology of Music, 37(2), 137�153. doi:10.1177

/0305735608094511

Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechan-

isms of interval timing. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 6(10), 755�765. doi:10.1038/

nrn1764

Butterfield, M. (2010). Participatory discrepancies and the perception of beats in jazz. Music

Perception, 27(3), 157�175.

Cariani, P. (2001). Temporal codes, timing nets, and music perception. Journal of New

Music Research, 30(2), 107�135. doi:10.1076/jnmr.30.2.107.7115

3919. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 24: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Carson, B. (2007). Perceiving and distinguishing simple timespan ratios without metric

einforcement. Journal of New Music Research, 36(4), 313�336. doi:10.1080

/09298210802015898

Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., & Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Listening to musical rhythms recruits

motor regions of the brain. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY: 1991), 18(12), 2844�2854.

doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn042

Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., & Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Moving on time: Brain network for

auditory-motor synchronization is modulated by rhythm complexity and musical train-

ing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(2), 226�239. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20018

Chen, J. L., Zatorre, R. J., & Penhune, V. B. (2006). Interactions between auditory and

dorsal premotor cortex during synchronization to musical rhythms. NeuroImage, 32(4),

1771�1781. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.207

Cohen, D., & Katz, R. (2008). Rhythmic patterns reflecting cognitive constraints and

aesthetic ideals. Journal of New Music Research, 37(1), 15�35. doi:10.1080/

09298210802095635

Correa, A., & Nobre, A. C. (2008). Neural modulation by regularity and passage of time.

Journal of Neurophysiology, 100(3), 1649�1655. doi:10.1152/jn.90656.2008

Coull, J., & Nobre, A. C. (2008). Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with

fMRI. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 137�144. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.011

Cuddy, L. L., & Hebert, S. (2002). Detection of metric structure in auditory figural patterns.

Perception & Psychophysics, 64(6), 909�918. Retrieved from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm

.nih.gov/pubmed/12269298.

Dixon, S. (2001). Automatic extraction of tempo and beat from expressive performances.

Journal of New Music Research, 30(1), 39�58. doi:10.1076/jnmr.30.1.39.7119

Dixon, S., Goebl, W., & Cambouropoulos, E. (2006). Perceptual smoothness of tempo in

expressively performed music. Music Perception, 23(3), 195�214. doi:10.1525/

mp.2006.23.3.195

Drake, C., & Bertrand, D. (2001). The quest for universals in temporal processing in music.

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 17�27. Retrieved from ,http://www

.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11458828.Drake, C., Jones, M. R., & Baruch, C. (2000). The development of rhythmic attending in

auditory sequences: Attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition, 77(3),

251�288. Retrieved from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11018511.Drake, C., Penel, A., & Bigand, E. (2000). Tapping in time with mechanically and

expressively performed music. Music Perception, 18(1), 1�23. JSTOR. Retrieved from

,http://www.jstor.org/stable/40285899.Eck, D. (2001). A positive-evidence model for rhythmical beat induction. Journal of New

Music Research, 30(2), 187�200. doi:10.1076/jnmr.30.2.187.7110

Eck, D. (2006). Identifying metrical and temporal structure with an autocorrelation phase matrix.

Music Perception, 24(2), 167�176. doi:10.1525/mp.2006.24.2.167

Ehrle, N., & Samson, S. (2005). Auditory discrimination of anisochrony: Influence of the

tempo and musical backgrounds of listeners. Brain and Cognition, 58(1), 133�147.

doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.014

Eitan, Z., & Granot, R. Y. (2006). How music moves: Musical parameters and listener’s

images of motion. Music Perception, 23(3), 221�247. doi:10.1038/149354c0

Ellis, R. J., & Jones, M. R. (2009). The role of accent salience and joint accent structure in

meter perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and

Performance, 35(1), 264�280. doi:10.1037/a0013482

392 Henkjan Honing

Page 25: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Eronen, A. J., & Klapuri, A. P. (2010). Music tempo estimation with k-NN regression. IEEE

Transactions on Audio, Speech, and Language Processing, 18(1), 50�57. IEEE Press.

Retrieved from ,http://portal.acm.org/citation.cfm?id51820662.1820667.Escoffier, N., Sheng, D. Y. J., & Schirmer, A. (2010). Unattended musical beats enhance

visual processing. Acta Psychologica, 135(1), 12�16. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.005

Firmino, E. A., Bueno, O., & Bigand, E. (2009). Travelling through pitch space speeds up

musical time. Music Perception, 26(3), 205�209. doi:10.1525/mp.2009.26.3.205

Fitch, W. T., & Rosenfeld, A. J. (2007). Perception and production of syncopated rhythms.

Music Perception, 25(1), 43�58. Annual Reviews 4139 El Camino Way, PO Box

10139, Palo Alto, CA 94303-0139, USA. Retrieved from ,http://caliber.ucpress.net

/doi/abs/10.1525/mp.2007.25.1.43.Flach, R., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2004). Recognizing one’s own clapping: The role of

temporal cues. Psychological Research, 69(1�2), 147�156. doi:10.1007/s00426-003

-0165-2

Friberg, A., & Sundstrom, A. (2002). Swing ratios and ensemble timing in jazz performance:

Evidence for a common rhythmic pattern. Music Perception, 19, 333�349.

Fujioka, T., Trainor, L. J., Large, E. W., & Ross, B. (2009). Beta and gamma rhythms in

human auditory cortex during musical beat processing. Annals of the New York

Academy of Sciences, 1169, 89�92. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04779.x

Fujioka, T., Zendel, B. R., & Ross, B. (2010). Endogenous neuromagnetic activity for

mental hierarchy of timing. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of

the Society for Neuroscience, 30(9), 3458�3466. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086

-09.2010

Geiser, E., Sandmann, P., Jancke, L., & Meyer, M. (2010). Refinement of metre perception:

Training increases hierarchical metre processing. The European Journal of

Neuroscience, 32(11), 1979�1985. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07462.x

Geiser, E., Ziegler, E., Jancke, L., & Meyer, M. (2009). Early electrophysiological correlates

of meter and rhythm processing in music perception. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the

Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 45(1), 93�102. doi:10.1016/

j.cortex.2007.09.010

Geringer, J. M. (2010). Musician’s preferences for tempo and pitch levels in recorded

orchestral music. Journal of Research in Music Education, 58(3), 294�308.

doi:10.1177/0022429410380464

Geringer, J. M., Madsen, C. K., & Macleod, R. B. (2007). Effects of articulation styles

on perception of modulated tempos in violin excerpts. International Journal of

Music Education, 25(2), 165�176. doi:10.1177/0255761407079954

Geringer, J. M., Madsen, C. K., Macleod, R. B., & Droe, K. (2006). The effect of articula-

tion style on perception of modulated tempo. Journal of Research in Music Education,

54(4), 324. SAGE Publications. Retrieved from ,http://jrm.sagepub.com/content/54/4

/324.short.Gerry, D. W., Faux, A. L., & Trainor, L. J. (2010). Effects of Kindermusik training on

infants’ rhythmic enculturation. Developmental Science, 13(3), 545�551. doi:10.1111/j

.1467-7687.2009.00912.x

Gouyon, F., & Meudic, B. (2003). Towards rhythmic content processing of musical signals:

Fostering complementary approaches. Journal of New Music Research, 32(1), 41�64.

doi:10.1076/jnmr.32.1.41.16804

Gouyon, F., Klapuri, A. P., Dixon, S., Alonso, M., Tzanetakis, G., & Uhle, C., et al. (2006).

An experimental comparison of audio tempo induction algorithms. IEEE Transactions

3939. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 26: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

on Audio, Speech and Language Processing, 14(5), 1832�1844. doi:10.1109/

TSA.2005.858509

Gouyon, F., Widmer, G., Serra, X., & Flexer, A. (2006). Acoustic cues to beat induction: A

machine learning perspective. Music Perception, 24(2), 177�188. doi:10.1525/

mp.2006.24.2.177

Grahn, J. A. (2009). The role of the basal ganglia in beat perception: Neuroimaging and

neuropsychological investigations. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169,

35�45. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04553.x

Grahn, J. A. (2009). Neuroscientific investigations of musical rhythm: Recent advances and

future challenges. Contemporary Music Review, 28(3), 251�277. doi:10.1080/

07494460903404360

Grahn, J. A., & Brett, M. (2007). Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(5), 893�906. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.893

Grahn, J. A., & Brett, M. (2009). Impairment of beat-based rhythm discrimination in

Parkinson’s disease. Cortex, 45(1), 54�61. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.01.005

Grahn, J. A., & Mcauley, J. D. (2009). Neural bases of individual differences in beat

perception. NeuroImage, 47(4), 1894�1903. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.039

Grahn, J. A., & Rowe, J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: Premotor and striatal interactions in

musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. The Journal of Neuroscience:

The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(23), 7540�7548. doi:10.1523/

JNEUROSCI.2018-08.2009

Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neurosci-

ence findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(3),

561�582. doi:10.3758/APP.72.3.561

Grondin, S., & Killeen, P. R. (2009). Tracking time with song and count: Different Weber

functions for musicians and nonmusicians. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71

(7), 1649�1654. doi:10.3758/APP.71.7.1649

Grube, M., & Griffiths, T. D. (2009). Metricality-enhanced temporal encoding and the sub-

jective perception of rhythmic sequences. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the

Nervous System and Behavior, 45(1), 72�79. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.01.006

Grube, M., Cooper, F. E., Chinnery, P. F., & Griffiths, T. D. (2010). Dissociation of duration-

based and beat-based auditory timing in cerebellar degeneration. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(25), 11597�11601.

doi:10.1073/pnas.0910473107

Guastavino, C., Gomez, F., Toussaint, G., Marandola, F., & Gomez, E. (2009). Measuring

similarity between flamenco rhythmic patterns. Journal of New Music Research, 38(2),

129�138. doi:10.1080/09298210903229968

Habegger, L. (2010). Number concept and rhythmic response in early childhood. Music

Education Research, 12(3), 269�280. doi:10.1080/14613808.2010.504810

Hagmann, C. E., & Cook, R. G. (2010). Testing meter, rhythm, and tempo discriminations in

pigeons. Behavioural Processes, 85(2), 99�110. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.06.015

Handel, S., Todd, S. K., & Zoidis, A. M. (2009). Rhythmic structure in humpback whale

(Megaptera novaeangliae) songs: Preliminary implications for song production and per-

ception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(6), EL225�EL230.

doi:10.1121/1.3124712

Hannon, E. E. (2009). Perceiving speech rhythm in music: Listeners classify instrumental

songs according to language of origin. Cognition, 111(3), 403�409. doi:10.1016/

j.cognition.2009.03.003

394 Henkjan Honing

Page 27: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Hannon, E. E., & Johnson, S. P. (2005). Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and melo-

dies: Implications for musical structure learning. Cognitive Psychology, 50(4),

354�377. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.09.003

Hannon, E. E., & Trehub, S. E. (2005). Tuning in to musical rhythms: Infants learn more

readily than adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United

States of America, 102(35), 12639�12643. doi:10.1073/pnas.0504254102

Hannon, E. E., & Trehub, S. E. (2005). Metrical categories in infancy and adulthood.

Psychological Science: A Journal of the American Psychological Society/APS, 16(1),

48�55. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00779.x

Hannon, E. E., Snyder, J. S., Eerola, T., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2004). The role of melodic and

temporal cues in perceiving musical meter. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human

Perception and Performance, 30(5), 956�974. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.5.956

Hay, J. S. F., & Diehl, R. L. (2007). Perception of rhythmic grouping: Testing the iambic/

trochaic law. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(1), 113�122. Retrieved from ,http://

www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17515221.

Hopyan, T., Schellenberg, E. G., & Dennis, M. (2009). Perception of strong-meter and

weak-meter rhythms in children with spina bifida meningomyelocele. Journal of the

International Neuropsychological Society, 15(4), 521�528. doi:10.1017/

S1355617709090845

Hove, M. J., Sutherland, M. E., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2010, June). Ethnicity effects in rela-

tive pitch. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.3.310

Husain, G. (2002). Effects of musical tempo and mode on arousal, mood, and spatial abilities.

Music Perception, 24(1), 171�329. doi:10.1525/mp.2002.20.2.151

Ilie, G., & Thompson, W. F. (2006). A comparison of acoustic cues in music and speech for

three dimensions of affect. Music Perception, 23(4), 319�329.

Iversen, J. R., & Patel, A. D. (2008). The Beat Alignment Test (BAT): Surveying beat

processing abilities in the general population. Proceedings of the 10th International

Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, 2�5.

Iversen, J. R., Patel, A. D., & Ohgushi, K. (2008). Perception of rhythmic grouping depends

on auditory experience. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(4),

2263�2271. doi:10.1121/1.2973189

Iversen, J. R., Repp, B. H., & Patel, A. D. (2009). Top-down control of rhythm perception

modulates early auditory responses. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,

1169, 58�73. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04579.x

Iyer, V. (2002). Embodied mind, situated cognition, and expressive microtiming in African-

American music. Music Perception, 19(3), 387�414. doi:10.1525/mp.2002.19.3.387

Jackendoff, R., & Lerdahl, F. (2006). The capacity for music: What is it, and what’s special

about it? Cognition, 100(1), 33�72. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.005

Jongsma, M. L. A., Meeuwissen, E., Vos, P. G., & Maes, R. (2007). Rhythm perception:

Speeding up or slowing down affects different subcomponents of the ERP P3 com-

plex. Biological Psychology, 75(3), 219�228. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.02.003

Keller, P. E., & Burnham, D. K. (2005). Musical meter in attention to multipart rhythm.

Music Perception, 22(4), 629�661. doi:10.1525/mp.2005.22.4.629

Klapuri, A. P., Eronen, A. J., & Astola, J. T. (2006). Analysis of the meter of acoustic

musical signals. IEEE Transactions on Audio, Speech and Language Processing, 14(1),

342�355. doi:10.1109/TSA.2005.854090

Koelsch, S., & Siebel, W. A. (2005). Towards a neural basis of music perception. Trends in

Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 578�584. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.001

3959. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 28: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Kong, Y.-Y., Cruz, R., Jones, J. A., & Zeng, F. -G. (2004). Music perception with temporal

cues in acoustic and electric hearing. Ear and Hearing, 25(2), 173�185. doi:10.1097

/01.AUD.0000120365.97792.2F

Kornysheva, K., von Cramon, D. Y., Jacobsen, T., & Schubotz, R. I. (2010). Tuning-in to

the beat: Aesthetic appreciation of musical rhythms correlates with a premotor activity

boost. Human Brain Mapping, 31(1), 48�64. doi:10.1002/hbm.20844

Krampe, R. T., Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Timing, sequencing, and executive control in

repetitive movement production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human

Perception and Performance, 31(3), 379�397. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.3.379

Krumhansl, C. L. (2000). Rhythm and pitch in music cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 126

(1), 159�179. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.126.1.159

Kuck, H., Grossbach, M., Bangert, M., & Altenmuller, E. (2003). Brain processing of meter

and rhythm in music: Electrophysiological evidence of a common network. Annals of

the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 244�253.

Lalitte, P., Bigand, E., Poulin-Charronnat, B., McAdams, S., Delbee, C., & D’Adamo,

D. (2004). The perceptual structure of thematic materials in The Angel of Death.

Music Perception, 22(2), 265�296. doi:10.1525/mp.2004.22.2.265

Large, E. W. (2000). On synchronizing movements to music. Human Movement Science, 19

(4), 527�566. doi:10.1016/S0167-9457(00)00026-9

Large, E. W. (2001). Periodicity, pattern formation, and metric structure. Journal of New

Music Research, 30(2), 173�185. doi:10.1076/jnmr.30.2.173.7113

Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track time-

varying events. Psychological Review, 106(1), 119�159.

Large, E. W., & Palmer, C. (2002). Perceiving temporal regularity in music. Cognitive

Science, 26(1), 1�37. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2601_1

Large, E. W., & Snyder, J. S. (2009). Pulse and meter as neural resonance. Annals of the New

York Academy of Sciences, 1169(1), 46�57. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04550.x

Large, E. W., Fink, P. W., & Kelso, S. J. A. (2002). Tracking simple and complex

sequences. Psychological Research, 66(1), 3�17. doi:10.1007/s004260100069

Lebrun-Guillaud, G., & Tillmann, B. (2007). Influence of a tone’s tonal function on temporal

change detection. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(8), 1450�1459.

Lebrun-Guillaud, G., Tillmann, B., & Justus, T. (2008). Perception of tonal and temporal

structures in chord sequences by patients with cerebellar damage. Music Perception,

25(4), 271�283.

Levitin, D. J., & Tirovolas, A. K. (2009). Current advances in the cognitive neuroscience of

music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 211�231. doi:10.1111

/j.1749-6632.2009.04417.x

Lewis, P. A., Wing, A. M., Pope, P. A., Praamstra, P., & Miall, R. C. (2004). Brain activity

correlates differentially with increasing temporal complexity of rhythms during initiali-

sation, synchronisation, and continuation phases of paced finger tapping.

Neuropsychologia, 42(10), 1301�1312. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.03.001

Lewkowicz, D. J. (2000). The development of intersensory temporal perception: An epige-

netic systems/limitations view. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 281�308. Retrieved

from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10748644.Lewkowicz, D. J. (2003). Learning and discrimination of audiovisual events in human

infants: The hierarchical relation between intersensory temporal synchrony and rhyth-

mic pattern cues. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 795�804. doi:10.1037/

0012-1649.39.5.795

396 Henkjan Honing

Page 29: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Lewkowicz, D. J., & Marcovitch, S. (2006). Perception of audiovisual rhythm and its invari-

ance in 4- to 10-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(4), 288�300.

The University of North Carolina at Greensboro. doi:10.1002/dev

Loehr, J. D., & Palmer, C. (2011). Subdividing the beat: Auditory and motor contributions to

synchronization. Music Perception, 26(5), 415�425.

London, J. (2002). Cognitive constraints on metric systems: Some observations and

hypotheses. Music Perception, 19(4), 529�550. doi:10.1525/mp.2002.19.4.529

London, J. (2002). Cognitive constraints on metric systems: Some observations and hypothe-

ses. Music Perception, 19(4), 529�550.

London, J., Himberg, T., & Cross, I. (2009). The effect of structural and performance factors

in the perception of anacruses. Music Perception, 27(2), 103�120. doi:10.1525/

mp.2009.27.2.103

London, J., Keller, P. E., & Repp, B. H. (2005). Production and synchronization of uneven

rhythms at fast tempi. Music Perception, 23(1), 61�78. Retrieved from ,http://www

.icmpc8.umn.edu/proceedings/ICMPC8/PDF/AUTHOR/MP040233.PDF.

Longhi, E. (2009). ‘Songese’: Maternal structuring of musical interaction with infants.

Psychology of Music, 37(2), 195�213. doi:10.1177/0305735608097042

Lucas, B. J., Schubert, E., & Halpern, A. R. (2010). Perception of emotion in sounded and

imagined music. Music Perception, 27(5), 399�412.

Luck, G., Toiviainen, P., Erkkila, J., Lartillot, O., Riikkila, K., & Makela, A., et al. (2008).

Modelling the relationships between emotional responses to, and musical content of,

music therapy improvisations. Psychology of Music, 36(1), 25�45. doi:10.1177

/0305735607079714

Madison, G. S. (2009). An auditory illusion of infinite tempo change based on multiple temporal

levels. PloS One, 4(12), e8151. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0008151

Makris, I., & Mullet, E. (2003). Judging the pleasantness of contour-rhythm-pitch-timbre

musical combinations. The American Journal of Psychology, 116(4), 581�611. JSTOR.

Retrieved from ,http://www.jstor.org/stable/1423661.Margulis, E. H. (2007). Silences in music are musical not silent: An exploratory study of

context effects on the experience of musical pauses. Music Perception, 24(5), 485�506.

Marmel, F., Tillmann, B., & Dowling, W. J. (2008). Tonal expectations influence pitch

perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(5), 841�852. doi:10.3758/PP.70.5.841

McAdams, S., Vieillard, S., Houix, O., & Reynolds, R. (2004). Perception of musical

similarity among contemporary thematic materials in two instrumentations. Music

Perception, 22(2), 207�237. doi:10.1525/mp.2004.22.2.207

McAdams, S., Vines, B. W., Vieillard, S., Smith, B. K., & Reynolds, R. (2004). Influences

of large-scale form on continuous ratings in response to a contemporary piece in a live

concert setting. Music Perception, 22(2), 297�350. doi:10.1525/mp.2004.22.2.297

McAngus Todd, N. P., O’Boyle, D. J., & Lee, C. S. (1999). A sensory-motor theory of

rhythm, time perception and beat induction. Journal of New Music Research, 28(1),

5�28. doi:10.1076/jnmr.28.1.5.3124

McAuley, J. D., & Henry, M. J. (2010). Modality effects in rhythm processing: Auditory

encoding of visual rhythms is neither obligatory nor automatic. Attention, Perception &

Psychophysics, 72(5), 1377�1389. doi:10.3758/APP.72.5.1377

McAuley, J. D., & Jones, M. R. (2003). Modeling effects of rhythmic context on perceived

duration: A comparison of interval and entrainment approaches to short-interval timing.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(6),

1102�1125. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.29.6.1102

3979. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 30: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

McAuley, J. D., Jones, M. R., Holub, S., Johnston, H. M., & Miller, N. S. (2006). The time of

our lives: Life span development of timing and event tracking. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: General, 135(3), 348�367. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.348

McAuley, J. D., & Miller, N. S. (2007). Picking up the pace: Effects of global temporal

context on sensitivity to the tempo of auditory sequences. Perception &

Psychophysics, 69(5), 709�718. Retrieved from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov

/pubmed/17929694.McDermott, J. H., & Oxenham, A. J. (2008). Music perception, pitch, and the auditory

system. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(4), 452�463. doi:10.1016/j.conb.

2008.09.005

McGraw, A. C. (2008). The perception and cognition of time in Balinese music.

Empirical Musicology Review, 3(2), 38�54. Retrieved from ,http://scholar.google

.com/scholar?hl5en&btnG5Search&q5intitle:The1Perception1and1Cognition1of

1Time1in1Balinese1Music#0.McKinney, M. F., & Moelants, D. (2006). Ambiguity in tempo perception: What draws

listeners to different metrical levels? Music Perception, 24(2), 155�166.

McKinney, M. F., Moelants, D., Davies, M. E. P., & Klapuri, A. P. (2007). Evaluation of

audio beat tracking and music tempo extraction algorithms. Journal of New Music

Research, 36(1), 1�16. doi:10.1080/09298210701653252

Merker, B. H., Madison, G. S., & Eckerdal, P. (2009). On the role and origin of isochrony in

human rhythmic entrainment. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous

System and Behavior, 45(1), 4�17. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.06.011

Miyauchi, R., & Nakajima, Y. (2005). Bilateral assimilation of two neighboring empty

time intervals. Music Perception, 22(3), 411�424. doi:10.1525/mp.2005.22.3.411

Moelants, D., & van Noorden, L. (2005). The influence of pitch interval on the perception

of polyrhythms. Music Perception, 22(3), 425�440. doi:10.1525/mp.2005.22.3.425

Morrison, S. J., & Demorest, S. M. (2009). Cultural constraints on music perception and

cognition. Cultural Neuroscience: Cultural Influences on Brain Function, 178, 67�77.

doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17805-6

Mullensiefen, D., Pfleiderer, M., & Frieler, K. (2009). The perception of accents in pop

music melodies. Journal of New Music Research, 38(1), 19�44. doi:10.1080/

09298210903085857

Nakata, T., & Mitani, C. (2005). Influences of temporal fluctuation on infant attention.

Music Perception, 22(3), 401�409. doi:10.1525/mp.2005.22.3.401

Nakata, T., Trehub, S. E., Mitani, C., & Kanda, Y. (2006). Pitch and timing in the songs of

deaf children with cochlear implants. Music Perception, 24(2), 147�154. doi:10.1525/

mp.2006.24.2.147

Nan, Y., Knosche, T. R., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). The perception of musical phrase

structure: A cross-cultural ERP study. Brain Research, 1094(1), 179�191. doi:10.1016/

j.brainres.2006.03.115

Nardo, D., Brunetti, R., Cupellini, E., & Belardinelli, M. O. (2009). The influence of melodic and

rhythmic redundancies on recognition memory for unknown musical themes. Musicae

Scientiae, 13(2), 337�355. ESCOM, European Society for Cognitive Sciences and Music.

Retrieved from ,http://apps.isiknowledge.com/full_record.do?product5UA&search_

mode5GeneralSearch&qid51&SID5U2HbMc56OgoKfkIbKf1&page51&doc51&

colname5WOS.Naveda, L., & Leman, M. (2009). A cross-modal heuristic for periodic pattern analysis of

samba music and dance. Journal of New Music Research, 38(3), 255�283. doi:10.1080/

09298210903105432

398 Henkjan Honing

Page 31: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Noorden, V., Eck, D., & Scott, S. K. (2005). Editorial: New research in rhythm perception

and production. Music Perception, 22(3), 365�369. doi:10.1525/mp.2005.22.3.365

Oberfeld, D. (2008). Does a rhythmic context have an effect on perceptual weights in auditory

intensity processing? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology5Revue Canadienne

de Psychologie Experimentale, 62(1), 24�32. doi:10.1037/1196-1961.62.1.24

Overy, K., Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Clarke, E. F. (2003). Dyslexia and music:

Measuring musical timing skills. Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 9(1), 18�36.

doi:10.1002/dys.233

Overy, K., Norton, A. C., Cronin, K. T., Gaab, N., Alsop, D. C., Winner, E., & Schlaug, G.

(2004). Imaging melody and rhythm processing in young children. NeuroReport, 15

(11), 1723�1726. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000136055.77095.f1

Overy, K., Norton, A. C., Cronin, K. T., Winner, E., & Schlaug (2005). Examining

rhythm and melody processing in young children using FMRI. Annals of the New

York Academy of Sciences, 1060, 210�218. doi:10.1196/annals.1360.014

Paraskevopoulos, E., Tsapkini, K., & Peretz, I. (2010). Cultural aspects of music perception:

Validation of a Greek version of the Montreal battery of evaluation of amusias. Journal

of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 16(4), 695�704. doi:10.1017

/S1355617710000494

Pashler, H. (2001). Perception and production of brief durations: Beat-based versus interval-

based timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and

Performance, 27(2), 485�493. Elsevier. Retrieved from ,http://linkinghub.elsevier

.com/retrieve/pii/S0096152302000901.

Patel, A. D. (2006). Musical rhythm, linguistic rhythm, and human evolution. Music

Perception, 24, 99�104.

Patel, A. D., & Daniele, J. R. (2003). An empirical comparison of rhythm in language and

music. Cognition, 87(1), B35�B45. doi:10.1016/S0

Patel, A. D., Iversen, J. R., & Rosenberg, J. C. (2006). Comparing the rhythm and melody of

speech and music: The case of British English and French. Journal of the Acoustical

Society of America, 119(5), 3034�3047. doi:10.1121/1.2179657

Patel, A. D., Iversen, J. R., Bregman, M. R., & Schulz, I. (2009). Studying synchronization

to a musical beat in nonhuman animals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,

1169, 459�469. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04581.x

Patel, A. D., Iversen, J. R., Chen, Y., & Repp, B. H. (2005). The influence of metricality and

modality on synchronization with a beat. Experimental Brain Research. Experimentelle

Hirnforschung. Experimentation Cerebrale, 163(2), 226�238. doi:10.1007/s00221-004-

2159-8

Pfordresher, P. Q. (2003). The role of melodic and rhythmic accents in musical structure.

Music Perception, 20(4), 431�464. doi:10.1525/mp.2003.20.4.431

Phillips-Silver, J. (2009). On the meaning of movement in music, development and the brain.

Contemporary Music Review, 28(3), 293�314. doi:10.1080/07494460903404394

Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Hearing what the body feels: Auditory encoding

of rhythmic movement. Cognition, 105(3), 533�546. doi:10.1016/j.cognition

.2006.11.006

Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2008). Vestibular influence on auditory metrical interpre-

tation. Brain and Cognition, 67(1), 94�102. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.11.007

Phillips-Silver, J., Aktipis, C. A., & Bryant, G. A. (2010). The ecology of entrainment:

Foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music Perception, 28(1), 3�14.

Potter, D. D., Fenwick, M., Abecasis, D., & Brochard, R. (2009). Perceiving rhythm

where none exists: Event-related potential (ERP) correlates of subjective accenting.

3999. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 32: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 45(1),

103�109. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.01.004

Pouliot, M., & Grondin, S. (2005). A response-time approach for estimating sensitivity to

auditory tempo changes. Music Perception, 22(3), 389�399. doi:10.1525/

mp.2005.22.3.389

Pressing, J. (2002). Black Atlantic rhythm: Its computational and transcultural foundations.

Music Perception, 19(3), 285�310. doi:10.1525/mp.2002.19.3.285

Prince, J. B., Schmuckler, M. A., & Thompson, W. F. (2009). The effect of task and pitch

structure on pitch-time interactions in music. Memory & Cognition, 37(3), 368�381.

doi:10.3758/MC.37.3.368

Prince, J. B., Thompson, W. F., & Schmuckler, M. A. (2009). Pitch and time, tonality and

meter: How do musical dimensions combine? Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1598�1617. doi:10.1037/a0016456

Purwins, H., Herrera, P., Grachten, M., Hazan, A., Marxer, R., & Serra, X. (2008).

Computational models of music perception and cognition I: The perceptual and cogni-

tive processing chain. Physics of Life Reviews, 5(3), 151�168. doi:10.1016/

j.plrev.2008.03.004

Quinn, S., & Watt, R. (2006). The perception of tempo in music. Perception (London), 35

(2), 267�280. doi:10.1068/p5353

Rammsayer, T. H., & Brandler, S. (2004). Aspects of temporal information processing:

A dimensional analysis. Psychological Research, 69(1�2), 115�123. doi:10.1007/

s00426-003-0164-3

Rankin, S. K., Large, E. W., & Fink, P. W. (2009). Fractal tempo fluxtuation and pulse pre-

diction. Music Perception, 26(5), 401�413.

Reed, R. (2003). Tempo change and interpretation preference. In Proceedings of the

European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (pp. 558�561). Hannover,

Germany: University of Hannover.

Remijn, G. B., & Kojima, H. (2010). Active versus passive listening to auditory streaming

stimuli: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 15(3), 1�9.

doi:10.1117/1.3431104

Repp, B. H. (2001). Processes underlying adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor

synchronization. Human Movement Science, 20(3), 277�312. Retrieved from ,http://www

.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11517673.Repp, B. H. (2002). Perception of timing is more context sensitive than sensorimotor syn-

chronization. Perception & Psychophysics, 64(5), 703�716. Retrieved from ,http://www

.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12201330.Repp, B. H. (2002). Effects of tempo on the timing of simple musical rhythms. Music

Perception, 13(4), 455�593. doi:10.1525/mp.2002.19.4.565

Repp, B. H. (2003). Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual

sequences: The synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivi-

sion. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35(4), 355�370.

Repp, B. H. (2005). Rate limits of on-beat and off-beat tapping with simple auditory

rhythms: 1. Qualitative observations. Music Perception, 22(3), 479�496. doi:10.1525/

mp.2005.22.3.479

Repp, B. H. (2005). Rate limits of on-beat and off-beat tapping with simple auditory

rhythms: 2. The roles of different kinds of accent. Music Perception, 23(2), 165�187.

Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature.

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 969�992. Retrieved from ,http://www.ncbi

.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16615317.

400 Henkjan Honing

Page 33: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Repp, B. H. (2007). Hearing a melody in different ways: Multistability of metrical interpreta-

tion, reflected in rate limits of sensorimotor synchronization. Cognition, 102(3),

434�454. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.02.003

Repp, B. H. (2008). Multiple temporal references in sensorimotor synchronization with

metrical auditory sequences. Psychological Research, 72(1), 79�98. doi:10.1007

/s00426-006-0067-1

Repp, B. H. (2010). Sensorimotor synchronization and perception of timing: Effects of music

training and task experience. Human Movement Science, 29(2), 200�213. doi:10.1016

/j.humov.2009.08.002

Repp, B. H. (2010). Do metrical accents create illusory phenomenal accents? Attention,

Perception & Psychophysics, 72(5), 1390�1403. doi:10.3758/APP.72.5.1390

Repp, B. H., & Bruttomesso, M. (2009). A filled duration illusion in music: Effects of metri-

cal subdivision on the perception and production of beat tempo. Advances in Cognitive

Psychology, 5, 114�134.

Repp, B. H., & Doggett, R. (2007). Tapping to a very slow beat: A comparison of musicians

and nonmusicians. Music Perception, 24(4), 367�376. doi:10.1525/mp.2007.24.4.367

Repp, B. H., & Jendoubi, H. (2009). Flexibility of temporal expectations for triple subdivision

of a beat. Advances in Cognitive Psychology / University of Finance and Management in

Warsaw, 5(203), 27�41. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0063-7

Repp, B. H., & Keller, P. E. (2004). Adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchroni-

zation: Effects of intention, attention, and awareness. The Quarterly Journal of

Experimental Psychology: A, Human Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 499�521.

doi:10.1080/02724980343000369

Repp, B. H., & Keller, P. E. (2008). Sensorimotor synchronization with adaptively timed

sequences. Human Movement Science, 27(3), 423�456. doi:10.1016/j.humov

.2008.02.016

Repp, B. H., & Penel, A. (2004). Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory

than to visual rhythms. Psychological Research, 68(4), 252�270. doi:10.1007

/s00426-003-0143-8

Repp, B. H., Iversen, J. R., & Patel, A. D. (2008). Tracking an imposed beat within a metri-

cal grid. Music Perception, 26(1), 1�18.

Repp, B. H., London, J., & Keller, P. E. (2010). Perception-production relationships and

phase correction in synchronization with two-interval rhythms. Psychological Research,

Online. doi:10.1007/s00426-010-0301-8

Riggle, M. (2009). A simpler explanation for vestibular influence on beat perception: No

specialized unit needed. Empirical Musicology Review, 4(1), 19�22. Retrieved from

,http://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl5en&btnG5Search&q5intitle:A1Simpler1Explanation1for1Vestibular1Influence1on1Beat1Perception:1No1Specialized1

Unit1Needed#0.Ross, B. (2009). Challenges facing theories of music and language co-evolution. Journal of

Musical Arts in Africa, 6(1), 61�76. doi:10.2989/JMAA.2009.6.1.4.1056

Sadakata, M., & Ohgushi, K. (2004). A cross-cultural comparison study of the production

of simple rhythmic patterns. Psychology of Music, 32(4), 389�403. Retrieved from

,http://pom.sagepub.com/content/32/4/389.abstract.Sakai, K., Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S., Takino, R., Tamada, T., & Iwata, N. K., et al. (1999).

Neural representation of a rhythm depends on its interval ratio. The Journal of

Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 19(22), 10074�10081.

Retrieved from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10559415.

4019. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 34: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Schimmel, O., & Kohlrausch, A. (2008). On the influence of interaural differences on tempo-

ral perception of noise bursts of different durations. Journal of the Acoustical Society of

America, 123(2), 986�997. doi:10.1121/1.2821979

Schulze, H. -H., & Braunschweig, T. U. (2005). Keeping synchrony while tempo changes:

Accelerando and ritardando. Music Perception, 22(3), 461�477. doi:10.1525/

mp.2005.22.3.461

Schwartze, M., Keller, P. E., Patel, A. D., & Kotz, S. A. (2010). The impact of basal ganglia

lesions on sensorimotor synchronization, spontaneous motor tempo, and the detection of

tempo changes. Behavioural Brain Research. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.09.015

Serrien, D. J., & Spape, M. M. (2010). Coupling between perception and action timing dur-

ing sensorimotor synchronization. Neuroscience letters, 486(3), 215�219. doi:10.1016/

j.neulet.2010.09.056

Slobodian, L. N. (2008). Perception and production of linguistic and musical rhythm

by Korean and English middle school students. Empirical Musicology Review, 3(4),

187�204. Retrieved from ,http://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl5en&btnG5Search&q5

intitle:Perception1and1production1of1linguistic1and1musical1rhythm1by1Korean1and1English1middle1school1students#0.

Snyder, J. S., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2001). Tapping to ragtime: Cues to pulse finding. Music

Perception, 18(4), 455�489. University of California Press. Retrieved from ,http://apps

.isiknowledge.com/CitedFullRecord.do?product5UA&db_id5WOS&SID5N1kea

j3j11cakI5aIIi&search_mode5CitedFullRecord&isickref5120772261.Snyder, J. S., & Large, E. W. (2005). Gamma-band activity reflects the metric structure of

rhythmic tone sequences. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 117�126.

doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.12.014

Snyder, J. S., Hannon, E. E., Large, E. W., & Christiansen, M. H. (2006). Synchronization

and continuation tapping to complex meters. Music Perception, 24(2), 135�146.

doi:10.1525/mp.2006.24.2.135

Soley, G., & Hannon, E. E. (2010). Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture:

A cross-cultural comparison. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 286�292. doi:10.1037

/a0017555

Tanaka, S., Tsuzaki, M., Aiba, E., & Kato, H. (2008). Auditory sensitivity to temporal devia-

tions from perceptual isochrony: Comparison of the starting point and ending point of

acoustic change. Japanese Psychological Research, 50(4), 223�231. John Wiley & Sons.

Retrieved from ,http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/121537937/abstract.Tekman, H. G. (2001). Accenting and detection of timing variations in tone sequences:

Different kinds of accents have different effects. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(3),

514�523. Retrieved from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11414138.Tekman, H. G. (2002). Perceptual integration of timing and intensity variations in the

perception of musical accents. The Journal of General Psychology, 129(2), 181�191.

Retrieved from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12153134.Tekman, H. G. (2003). Effects of accenting and regularity on the detection of temporal

deviations: Does regularity facilitate performance? The Journal of General

Psychology, 130(3), 247�258. Retrieved from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed

/12926511.Temperley, D. (2000). Meter and grouping in African music: A view from music theory.

Ethnomusicology, 44(1), 65. doi:10.2307/852655

Temperley, D. (2004). Communicative pressure and the evolution of musical styles. Music

Perception, 21(3), 313�337. doi:10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.313

402 Henkjan Honing

Page 35: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Temperley, D. (2004). An evaluation system for metrical models. Computer Musical

Journal, 28(3), 28�44. doi:10.1162/0148926041790621

Temperley, D. (2010). Modeling common-practice rhythm. Music Perception, 27(5),

355�376.

Temperley, D., & Bartlette, C. (2002). Parallelism as a factor in metrical analysis. Music

Perception, 20(2), 117�149. doi:10.1525/mp.2002.20.2.117

Ten Hoopen, G., Sasaki, T., Nakajima, Y., Remijn, G. B., Massier, B., & Rhebergen, K. S.,

et al. (2006). Time-shrinking and categorical temporal ratio perception: Evidence for a

1:1 temporal category. Music Perception, 24(1), 1�22.

Todd, N. P. M. A., Cousins, R., & Lee, C. S. (2007). The contribution of anthropometric

factors to individual differences in the perception of rhythm. Empirical Musicology

Review, 2(1), 1�13. Retrieved from ,http://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl5en&btnG5

Search&q5intitle:The1Contribution1of1Anthropometric1Factors1to1Individual1Differences1in1the1Perception1of1Rhythm#0.

Todd, N. P. M. A., Lee, C. S., & O’Boyle, D. J. (2002). A sensorimotor theory of temporal

tracking and beat induction. Psychological Research, 66(1), 26�39. doi:10.1007

/s004260100071

Toiviainen, P. (2003). Tapping to Bach: Resonance-based modeling of pulse. Music

Perception, 14(1), 43�80. doi:10.1525/mp.2003.21.1.43

Toiviainen, P., & Eerola, T. (2006). Autocorrelation in meter induction: The role of accent

structure. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(2), 1164�1170.

doi:10.1121/1.2146084

Toiviainen, P., Luck, G., & Thompson, M. R. (2009). Embodied metre: Hierarchical eigen-

modes in spontaneous movement to music. Cognitive Processing, 10(Suppl 2),

S325�S327. doi:10.1007/s10339-009-0304-9

Toiviainen, P., Luck, G., & Thompson, M. R. (2010). Embodied meter: Hierarchical eigen-

modes in music-induced movement. Music Perception, 28(1), 59�70.

Tomic, S. T., & Janata, P. (2008). Beyond the beat: Modeling metric structure in music and

performance. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(6), 4024�4041.

doi:10.1121/1.3006382

Trainor, L. J. (2007). Do preferred beat rate and entrainment to the beat have a common ori-

gin in movement? Empirical Musicology Review, 2(1), 17�20. Retrieved from ,http://

scholar.google.com/scholar?hl5en&btnG5Search&q5intitle:Do1Preferred1Beat1Rate1and1Entrainment1to1the1Beat1Have1a1Common1Origin1in1Movement1?#0.

Trainor, L. J., & Unrau, A. (2009). Extracting the beat: An experience-dependent complex

integration of multisensory information involving multiple levels of the nervous system.

Empirical Musicology Review, 4(1), 32�36. Retrieved from ,http://scholar.google.

com/scholar?hl5en&btnG5Search&q5intitle:Extracting1the1beat:1An1experience

-dependent1complex1integration1of1multisensory1information1involving1multiple1levels1of1the1nervous1system#0.

Trainor, L. J., Gao, X., Lei, J.-J., Lehtovaara, K., & Harris, L. R. (2009). The primal role of

the vestibular system in determining musical rhythm. Cortex, 45(1), 35�43.

doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.10.014

Trehub, S. E., & Hannon, E. E. (2006). Infant music perception: Domain-general or domain-

specific mechanisms? Cognition, 100(1), 73�99. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.006

Trehub, S. E., & Hannon, E. E. (2009). Conventional rhythms enhance infants’ and adults’

perception of musical patterns. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous

System nd Behavior, 45(1), 110�118. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.012

4039. Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Page 36: The Psychology of Music || Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm in Music

Vines, B. W., Nuzzo, R. L., & Levitin, D. J. (2005). Analyzing temporal dynamics in music:

Differential calculus, physics, and functional data analysis techniques. Music

Perception, 23(3), 137�152.

Volk, A. (2008). The study of syncopation using inner metric analysis: Linking theoretical

and experimental analysis of metre in music. Journal of New Music Research, 37(4),

259�273. doi:10.1080/09298210802680758

Volman, M. J., & Geuze, R. H. (2000). Temporal stability of rhythmic tapping “on” and “off

the beat”: A developmental study. Psychological Research, 63(1), 62�69. Retrieved

from ,http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10743387.

Vongpaisal, T., Trehub, S. E., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2009). Identification of TV tunes by

children with cochlear implants. Music Perception, 27(1), 17�24.

Vuust, P., Roepstorff, A., Wallentin, M., Mouridsen, K., & Østergaard, L. (2006). It don’t

mean a thing. . . Keeping the rhythm during polyrhythmic tension, activates language

areas (BA47). NeuroImage, 31(2), 832�841. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.037

Zarco, W., Merchant, H., Prado, L., & Mendez, J. C. (2009). Subsecond timing in

primates: Comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus mon-

keys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102(6), 3191�3202. doi:10.1152/jn.00066.2009

Zentner, M., & Eerola, T. (2010). Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proceedings

of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(13),

5768�5773. doi:10.1073/pnas.1000121107

404 Henkjan Honing