the impact of female sex hormones on …web.stanford.edu/~niederle/buser.stanfordpresentation.pdf1 /...

25

Upload: doankhanh

Post on 06-Apr-2019

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

1 / 25

The Impact of Female Sex Hormones on

Competitiveness

Thomas BuserUniversity of Amsterdam

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Introduction 2 / 25

Motivation

I Gender di�erences in competitiveness are a stable �nding oflab experiments:

I Niederle and Vesterlund (2007): when given a choice, 73percent of men but only 35 percent of women compete

I Datta Gupta et al. (2005): women, but not men, arein�uenced by risk aversion when deciding whether to compete

I Gneezy et al. (2003): men signi�cantly increase e�ort whenthe compensation scheme becomes more competitive whilewomen show no signi�cant reaction

I These gender di�erences are often thought to be a cause ofthe gender gap in the labour market

I What causes these gender di�erences in competitiveness is stilllargely an open question

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Introduction 3 / 25

Nature vs. Nurture: Nurture

I Gender gap in competitiveness reversed in matrilineal society(Gneezy et al., 2008)

I Girls attending single-sex schools are signi�cantly more likelyto compete (Booth and Nolen, 2009)

I Gender gap in risk aversion disappears for girls being raised insingle-sex schools (Booth and Nolen, 2009)

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Introduction 4 / 25

Nature vs. Nurture: Nature

I No study on tournament entry

I But mixed evidence on gender gap in other areas:

I Testosterone correlated with �nancial risk taking (Apicella etal., 2008) and a higher probability of rejection in theultimatum game (Burnham, 2007)

I Bidding in �rst price auctions �uctuates over the menstrualcycle (Chen et al., 2009; Pearson and Schipper, 2009); but noe�ect of 2D:4D ratio on bidding (Pearson and Schipper, 2009)

I Testosterone levels are positively correlated with risk seeking;gender gap for MBA students in seeking out �nance careerdisappears when controlling for current and prenataltestosterone levels (Sapienza et al., 2009)

I But no e�ect of testosterone and oestrogen injections given topost-menopausal women on risk aversion, altruism, reciprocalfairness, and trust (Zethraeus et al., 2009)

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Introduction 5 / 25

This Paper

I Hormonal �uctuations - naturally occurring over the menstrualcycle or induced by contraceptives - provide exogenousvariation in oestrogen and progesterone levels

I Hypothesis: competitiveness �uctuates in sync with hormonelevels

I Hypothesis: women are more competitive when levels and/orchanges in hormone concentrations are low

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Introduction 6 / 25

Summary of Results

I Likelihood of self-selecting into competition �uctuates stronglyand signi�cantly over the menstrual cycle and with the intakeof hormonal contraceptives

I Fluctuations follow the predicted pattern: subjects aresigni�cantly less competitive in times of high levels and rapidincreases of sex hormones in the body

I The results are consistent with an evolutionary hypothesisaccording to which women are more competitive during thefertile window when competition for genetically well-endowedmales occurs and less so after ovulation is over (and duringpregnancy)

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Introduction 7 / 25

Summary of Results

I I consider three possible indirect pathways for the e�ect of sexhormones on competitiveness:

I via an impact on risk aversionI via an impact on mathematical performanceI via an impact on overcon�dence

I None of these hold up to the data

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Menstrual Cycle and Hormonal Contraceptives 8 / 25

The Menstrual Cycle

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Menstrual Cycle and Hormonal Contraceptives 9 / 25

Hormonal Contraceptives

I Constant doses of oestrogen and progestins

I 21-day intake phase and 7-day break

I Oestrogen secretion by the body is markedly reduced andprogesterone production ceases completely

I Wide range of progestins used

I Comparison between di�erent contraceptives and betweencontraceptive-takers and non-takers is impossible due toendogeneity issues

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Experimental Design and Sample 10 / 25

Experimental Design

I Closely follows design by Niederle and Vesterlund (2007)

I Groups of fourI Subjects solve simple sums of �ve two-digit numbersI Round one: piece-rate; round two: tournamentI Subjects choose compensation scheme for third roundI Subjects estimate own rank

I Additionally, I conduct a simple objective probability lotterychoice experiment

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Experimental Design and Sample 11 / 25

Sample

Contraceptive Number of Subjects Percentage

Pill 47 43.9Condoms 31 29.0Other hormonal contraceptive 6 5.6Other method 1 0.9None 22 20.6Total 107 100.0

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Experimental Design and Sample 12 / 25

Sample

Menstrual Cycle or Pill Cycle Phase Number Expected Numberof Subjects of Subjects

Phase 1: Menstrual (5 days) 11 10Phase 2: Follicular (7 days) 15 13Phase 3: Peri-Ovulatory (3 days) 9 6Phase 4: Luteal (8 days) 15 15Phase 5: Premenstrual (5 days) 4 10

Pill Break (7 days) 13 13Pill Intake Phase (21 days) 40 40

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Results 13 / 25

Basic Results

I I �nd signi�cant variations of competitiveness over themenstrual cycle

I One-way ANCOVA returns P-value of 0.03 for variation overthe menstrual cycle. Subjects are signi�cantly less competitiveduring the luteal phase compared to the rest of the cycle(P-value: 0.02).

I P-value of 0.04 for the null of equality between the 7-daypill-break and the rest of the 28-day pill-cycle

I Con�rmed by non-parametric tests and regression results

I Variations in competitiveness are signi�cantly correlated withvariations in both progesterone and oestrogen

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Results 14 / 25

Regression Results

Competitiveness (1) (2) (3)

Cycle Phase 1 reference

Cycle Phase 2 -0.0121 -0.128 -0.147(0.208) (0.224) (0.231)

Cycle Phase 3 0.121 0.0658 0.0257(0.228) (0.241) (0.247)

Cycle Phase 4 -0.279 -0.432** -0.439**(0.198) (0.188) (0.194)

Cycle Phase 5 0.455*** 0.323* 0.305(0.158) (0.186) (0.192)

Controls for demographics no yes yes

Controls for risk aversion no no yesand maths scores (rounds 1 and 2)

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Results 15 / 25

Regression Results

Competitiveness (1) (2) (3)

Oestrogen (level) 0.0041 0.0055 0.0045(0.0092) (0.0095) (0.0097)

Progesterone (level) -0.0116** -0.0145*** -0.0140***(0.0045) (0.0036) (0.0040)

Controls for demographics no yes yes

Controls for risk aversion no no yesand maths scores (rounds 1 and 2)

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Results 16 / 25

Regression Results

Competitiveness (4) (5) (6) (7)

Oestrogen (level) 0.0035(0.0085)

Progesterone (level) -0.0090*(0.0048)

Oestrogen (change) -0.0173* -0.0213** -0.0204** -0.0177(0.0103) (0.0100) (0.0100) (0.0107)

Progesterone (change) -0.0410** -0.0519*** -0.0529*** -0.0333**(0.0177) (0.0117) (0.0116) (0.0162)

Controls for demographics no yes yes yes

Controls for risk aversion no no yes noand maths scores

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Results 17 / 25

Regression Results

Competitiveness (1) (2) (3)

Hormonal Contraceptives -0.315** -0.321* -0.258*(0.156) (0.166) (0.143)

Controls for demographics no yes yes

Controls for risk aversion no no yesand maths scores (rounds 1 and 2)

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Results 18 / 25

Evolutionary Background

I Progesterone has sedative and anti-anxiety e�ects and acts asa modulator of neurotransmitter receptors

I Evolutionary hypothesis: women are competitive during thefertile phase of their cycle when competition for geneticallystrong mates occurs and not competitive after ovulation isover (and during pregnancy) when commitment a long-termpartner is more important

I Women, who are in charge of taking care of the o�spring, facehigher costs of competitive behaviour compared to men, whospread their semen and run, because their death generallymeans the loss of their current o�spring (Campbell, 2002).This is especially true during pregnancy when hormonalconditions are similar to those in the luteal phase (Jones et al.,2005).

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Results 19 / 25

Evolutionary Background

I Women's commitment to their romantic relationship andattraction to femininity in male faces are positively andsigni�cantly correlated with progesterone levels (Jones et al.,2005)

I During fertile phase women are more likely to engage inextra-pair copulations (Bellis and Baker, 1990) and moreattracted to testosterone-related masculine facial features(Penton-Voak and Perrett, 2000)

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Pathways 20 / 25

Risk Aversion

I Is impact of sex hormones on competitiveness mediatedthrough impact on risk aversion?

I Chen et al. (2005) hypothesise that the impact of themenstrual cycle on auction bids is mediated via an impact onrisk aversion

I Datta Gupta et al. (2005) show that women are in�uenced bytheir degree of risk aversion when deciding whether to competeor not

I Majority of lab experiments show that women are more riskaverse than men (Croson and Gneezy, 2009)

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Pathways 21 / 25

Risk Aversion

I Competitiveness signi�cantly varies with risk aversion

I Kruskal-Wallis P-value: 0.01I Linear probability model: the most risk seeking subjects are

around forty percent more likely to compete than the most riskaverse

I But sex hormones have no impact on risk aversion

I Kruskal-Wallis P-values: 0.79 for variation across themenstrual cycle and 0.18 for contraceptive-induced variation

I Regression results show no impact either

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Pathways 22 / 25

Maths Scores

I Psychologists have found evidence of �uctuations of cognitiveabilities over the menstrual cycle

I Does not hold

I There is no signi�cant impact of sex hormones on averagemaths scores (Kruskal-Wallis P-value for variation over themenstrual cycle: 0.8281)

I Maths scores do not in�uence decision of whether or not tocompete

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Pathways 23 / 25

Maths Scores

Competitiveness (1) (2) (3)

Mean Score (Rounds 1 and 2) 0.00365 0.0113 0.00671(0.0125) (0.0125) (0.0120)

Controls no yes yes

Controls for cycle and contraceptives no no yes

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Pathways 24 / 25

Overcon�dence

I I �nd some evidence that (over)con�dence in�uencestournament entry

I Subjects who belief that they are amongst the two best in theirgroup are 16 percent more likely to enter the tournament(P-value: 0.0638; one-sided t-test)

I Subjects who overestimate their performance in round two are13 percent more likely to compete (P-value: 0.1006)

I But beliefs of individuals are not a�ected by sex hormones:Kruskal-Wallis P-values are 0.5931 for variation over themenstrual cycle and 0.3447 for contraceptive-induced variation

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser

Pathways 25 / 25

Conclusions

I Female sex hormones are important for explaining genderdi�erences in competitiveness and potentially other economicbehaviours

I Part of gender di�erences in labour market decisions could becaused by biological factors

The Impact of female sex hormones on competitiveness Thomas Buser