specificity of emotional processing in … · specificity of emotional processing in posttraumatic...

6
1 SPECIAL ARTICLES SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER 1 1, 2 Dana Cristina Her?a , Doina Cosman Some authors regard basic emotions as the building blocks of emotional life, leading to more elaborate emotional processes through recombination (1). A range of basic emotions, universally true and with a socioculturally modulated expression, were identified – anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness and surprise (2), generated by stimuli with positive or negative valence. However, there is no consensus yet regarding the number and type of basic emotions, due to the diversity of distinctive definition criteria (1). Thus, basic emotions have been identified by a unique neural pattern (3), unique appraisal pattern (4), unique response tendency (5), unique pattern of physiological response (6), unique facial expression (7), or unique experiential trait (8). Other studies dwell on the hypothesis that emotional life is based on a limited set of sub-emotional variables. Thus, six appraisal variables have been postulated (novelty, valence, relevance, congruence of the purpose, coping potential, instrumental traits) (9), structured as dimensional (10) or discrete variables (11, 12), hence the infinity of complex specific emotions generated by their combinations. Russell (13) emphasizes in this respect the role of the dimensional variables valence (positive, negative) and arousal, specific to emotional experience and neurophysiological activity. Affect, as a complex of phyisiological and motivational factors (1), refers to mood (lasting affective state, flexible and adaptive reflection of inner feelings to environmental context) and feelings (intense, transient, positively or negatively polarized affective states generated by specific experiences), and additionally to appraisals, attitudes and preferences (2). Emotions differ from basal mood through duration (brief for emotions, sustained for mood), intensity (high – emotions, and low – mood), and presence (in emotions) or absence (in mood) of a specific target, respectively (14). It is noteworthy that emotional generation and/or regulation based on individual stimulus expectancies is The plasticity of emotional processing throughout development, and the origin of its individual and inter-individual variations can be partly explained through the involvement of distinctive neural mechanisms in different aspects of emotional expression (15). 1 Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Medicine and Pharmacy “Iuliu Ha?ieganu” Cluj-Napoca. 2 rd 3 Psychiatric Clinic, Cluj County Emergency Hospital Received October 03, 2011, Revised January 09, 2012, Accepted February 02, 2012. rd Contact data: 3 Psychiatric Clinic Cluj- Napoca, Str. V. Babe? 43, Pavilion III, 400012, Romania; Tel/fax 0264590576; email: [email protected] Abstract: Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) represents, from a clinical perspective, an entity characterized by a complex psychopathological pattern of symptoms triggered by the exposure to a traumatic event, while from a phenomenological perspective it entails a specific, dysfunctional pattern of maladjustive affective, cognitive and behavioral reactions to a specific type of events. If an interpretation of real or potential life threat, paired with helplessness and intense negative affect of fear or horror, is provided to negative or even neutral events, trauma – related stimuli may elicit an operant conditioning through which the complex clinical features pf PTSD develop. The current study is an overview of specific isuues of emotional processing in PTSD emerged from current research, with an emphasis on the phenomenology of this disorder, due to the fact that PTSD dynamic is conditioned by specific life events, a specific family, sociocultural and developmental context and, mostly, by the presence of individual vulnerability expressed by rigid, dysfunctional, maladjustive and context – inappropriate affective, behavioral and cognitive reaction patterns. Keywords: posttraumatic stress disorder, emotional processing, phenomenology, vulnerability. Rezumat: Tulburarea de stres posttraumatic (PTSD) reprezintã, din perspectivã clinicã, o entitate caracterizatã printr-un tablou simptomatologic complex, declan?at de expunerea la un eveniment traumatizant, iar din perspectivã fenomenologicã, un tipar specific, disfunc?ional, de reac?ie dezadaptativã afectivã, cognitivã ?i comportamentalã la o anumitã categorie de evenimente. Dacã evenimentelor negative sau chiar neutre li se furnizeazã o interpretare implicând risc vital real sau poten?ial asociat cu neputin?ã ?i reac?ii afective intense de teroare sau spaimã, stimulii rela?iona?i cu evenimentul incriminat pot genera un proces de condi?ionare operantã prin intermediul cãruia sã se constituie tabloul psihopatologic complex al PTSD. Studiul de fa?ã trece în revistã aspectele specifice ale prelucrãrii emo?iilor în PTSD eviden?iate de studiile recente, cu accent pe latura fenomenologicã a genezei acestei tulburãri, având în vedere cã dinamica PTSD este condi?ionatã de un anumit tip de evenimente de via?ã, un anumit context familial, socio-cultural ?i de dezvoltare ?i, mai ales, de prezen?a vulnerabilitã?ii individuale sub raportul tiparelor afective, comportamentale ?i cognitive rigide, disfunc?ionale, neadecvate la context ?i dezadaptative. Cuvinte cheie: tulburarea de stres posttraumatic, procesare emo?ionalã, fenomenologie, vulnerabilitate.

Upload: phamminh

Post on 13-Jul-2018

221 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN … · SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER ... features pf PTSD ... Of Emotional Processing In Posttraumatic

1

SPECIAL ARTICLES

SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER

1 1, 2Dana Cristina Her?a , Doina Cosman

Some authors regard basic emotions as the building blocks of emotional life, leading to more elaborate emotional processes through recombination (1). A range of basic emotions, universally true and with a socioculturally modulated expression, were identified – anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness and surprise (2), generated by stimuli with positive or negative valence. However, there is no consensus yet regarding the number and type of basic emotions, due to the diversity of distinctive definition criteria (1). Thus, basic emotions have been identified by a unique neural pattern (3), unique appraisal pattern (4), unique response tendency (5), unique pattern of physiological response (6), unique facial expression (7), or unique experiential trait (8).

Other studies dwell on the hypothesis that emotional life is based on a limited set of sub-emotional variables. Thus, six appraisal variables have been postulated (novelty, valence, relevance, congruence of the purpose, coping potential, instrumental traits) (9), structured as dimensional (10) or discrete variables (11, 12), hence the infinity of complex specific emotions generated by their combinations. Russell (13) emphasizes

in this respect the role of the dimensional variables valence (positive, negative) and arousal, specific to emotional experience and neurophysiological activity.

Affect, as a complex of phyisiological and motivational factors (1), refers to mood (lasting affective state, flexible and adaptive reflection of inner feelings to environmental context) and feelings (intense, transient, positively or negatively polarized affective states generated by specific experiences), and additionally to appraisals, attitudes and preferences (2). Emotions differ from basal mood through duration (brief for emotions, sustained for mood), intensity (high – emotions, and low – mood), and presence (in emotions) or absence (in mood) of a specific target, respectively (14).

It is noteworthy that emotional generation and/or regulation based on individual stimulus expectancies is

The plasticity of emotional processing throughout development, and the origin of its individual and inter-individual variations can be partly explained through the involvement of distinctive neural mechanisms in different aspects of emotional expression (15).

1 Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Medicine and Pharmacy “Iuliu Ha?ieganu” Cluj-Napoca.

2 rd 3 Psychiatric Clinic, Cluj County Emergency HospitalReceived October 03, 2011, Revised January 09, 2012, Accepted February 02, 2012.

rd Contact data: 3 Psychiatric Clinic Cluj-Napoca, Str. V. Babe? 43, Pavilion III, 400012, Romania; Tel/fax 0264590576; email: [email protected]

Abstract:Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) represents, from a clinical perspective, an entity characterized by a complex psychopathological pattern of symptoms triggered by the exposure to a traumatic event, while from a phenomenological perspective it entails a specific, dysfunctional pattern of maladjustive affective, cognitive and behavioral reactions to a specific type of events. If an interpretation of real or potential life threat, paired with helplessness and intense negative affect of fear or horror, is provided to negative or even neutral events, trauma – related stimuli may elicit an operant conditioning through which the complex clinical features pf PTSD develop. The current study is an overview of specific isuues of emotional processing in PTSD emerged from current research, with an emphasis on the phenomenology of this disorder, due to the fact that PTSD dynamic is conditioned by specific life events, a specific family, sociocultural and developmental context and, mostly, by the presence of individual vulnerability expressed by rigid, dysfunctional, maladjustive and context – inappropriate affective, behavioral and cognitive reaction patterns. Keywords: posttraumatic stress disorder, emotional processing, phenomenology, vulnerability.

Rezumat:Tulburarea de stres posttraumatic (PTSD) reprezintã, din perspectivã clinicã, o entitate caracterizatã printr-un tablou simptomatologic complex, declan?at de expunerea la un eveniment traumatizant, iar din perspectivã fenomenologicã, un tipar specific, disfunc?ional, de reac?ie dezadaptat ivã afect ivã, cognit ivã ?i comportamentalã la o anumitã categorie de evenimente. Dacã evenimentelor negative sau chiar neutre li se furnizeazã o interpretare implicând risc vital real sau poten?ial asociat cu neputin?ã ?i reac?ii afective intense de teroare sau spaimã, stimulii rela?iona?i cu evenimentul incriminat pot genera un proces de condi?ionare operantã prin intermediul cãruia sã se constituie tabloul psihopatologic complex al PTSD. Studiul de fa?ã trece în revistã aspectele specifice ale prelucrãrii emo?iilor în PTSD eviden?iate de studiile recente, cu accent pe latura fenomenologicã a genezei acestei tulburãri, având în vedere cã dinamica PTSD este condi?ionatã de un anumit tip de evenimente de via?ã, un anumit context familial, socio-cultural ?i de dezvoltare ?i, mai ales, de prezen?a vulnerabilitã?ii individuale sub raportul tiparelor afective, comportamentale ?i cognitive rigide, disfunc?ionale, neadecvate la context ?i dezadaptative. Cuvinte cheie: tulburarea de stres posttraumatic, procesare emo?ionalã, fenomenologie, vulnerabilitate.

Page 2: SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN … · SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER ... features pf PTSD ... Of Emotional Processing In Posttraumatic

2

emotions or the explicit purpose of their regulation (16), in the absence of explicit instructions concerning these processes (11, 17).

as the role of emotions cannot be separated from the context of interpersonal relationships (19). Thus, the ability to recognize and share emotions represents the core of empathy and prosocial behaviors, facilitates social learning – that is, the ability of the human being to learn and build knowledge by observing emotional reactions

The cingulate – striatal – thalamic – cingulate circuit, formed by subgenual neurons in the anterior cingulate cortex projecting to nucleus accumbens (20), is involved in complex issues related to emotional processing: assessment of the intensity of emotions, syncronicity between emotions and the functioning of the autonomous nervous system, anticipatory reaction to an event with aversive emotional undertones, management of emotional conflicts (21).

Neurophysiology and neuroimagistics studies suggest that specific cortical areas, such as amygdala, insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, are crucial for emotional processing and integrating emotions with superior cognitive skills, with the purpose of adequate interpersonal functioning (22); however, other regions of the central nervous system (cerebellum, ventral striatum) also have an input (23 – 25).

Amygdala plays a key role in the perception of specific, emotionally-charged stimuli, and in pairing aversive stimuli with context – appropriate emotional response – ex. fear (26). Insular cortex, on the other hand, monitors somatic – visceral states (27). It was shown that dysfunctions in these areas lead to impaired reasoning and decision – making, with repercussions on social functioning (22).

Recent imagistic studies emphasized that reaction patterns generated in the amygdala are related to dopamine D2 receptor activity (28). Dopamine mediation in the amygdala has been involved both in emotional processing, and in the control of emotion–elicited behaviors (29, 30). On the other hand, D2 dopamine receptors and the DRD2 gene have been linked with the explicit emotion processing and emotion regulation supported by the prefrontal dorsolateral cortex (31).

These data lead to the hypothesis that D2 receptor dopamine mediation shares a common genotype background, but with different, structure– and function–specific phenotype expressions, considering that amygdala is involved in emotion perception, while

On the other hand, it was demonstrated that the pathway and timing of key periods for the development of the affective functions depend on behavioral and expressive context. Increased insight into the genetic, neural, developmental context and individual affective experience allows a better understanding of the development of the emotion recognition process, but also of the manner emotional processing is integrated in interpersonal relationships and group-specific social behaviors (18),

of others and, last but not least, elicits moral and ethical behaviors (18).

INVOLVEMENT OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN THE DYNAMICS OF POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER

the prefrontal dorsolateral cortex is responsible of conscious, explicit appraisal of emotions (32). Emotional appraisal, regulation and experiencing are intricately liked with the activation patterns of the prefrontal dorsomedial cortex and rostral area of the dorsal cingulate cortex (17, 21, 22).

Emotions can be conceptualized as reflected and mentally reactivated somatic states, and the association between events and their affective background may i n f l u e n c e s u p e r i o r c o g n i t i v e p r o c e s s e s – decision–making, respectively, mostly in uncertain situation or when the subject has to chose between a great number of alternatives (35).

Operant conditioning theory provides an explanation to the pattern of association between the traumatic event (unconditional stimulus) generating unconditional emotional response (fear, horror, helplessness) and specific trauma-context stimuli, which may elicit intense conditioned emotional response by subsequent repeating after the event. Avoidance behaviors develop in this context in order to decrease the anxiety related to conditional stimuli. Cognitive processing of an unexpected or severe event and management of its impact are impaired in subjects developing PTSD, while traumatic experience, incompletely or ineffectively assimilated, lingers in the field of consciousness in the shape of intrusive recollections (33).

Dysfunctional conditional response pattern to fear in PTSD is reinforced through increased glutamate neurotransmission in amygdala, in the absence of ventromedial prefrontal cortex inhibition (20), while emotional processing in this context is modulated by a plethora of pretraumatic, traumatic (fragmented representations of trauma memories – somatic states, stimuli, feelings and cognitions reflecting intense fear, helplessness, discomfort, confusion) and posttraumatic factors (20, 33, 34).

Integration of information that would eventually modify the event appraisal, especially the issues related to danger degree, is required in order to decrease fear and extinguish maladjustive conditional response. Thus, by repeated exposure to a specific conditional stimulus with no negative consequences, ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus will send afferences to amygdala that will gradually inhibit fear response and decrease its intensity, although affective memory of trauma persists (20) – see Figure 1.

The emotional processing theory was advanced as a more flexible pattern of understanding the mechanisms involved in recovery after trauma, and in PTSD onset, respectively. The core concept of this model is fear, defined as a thriarchial structure – stimulus, meaning and response (physiological, behavioral, verbal), and complex interconnections between these elements provide the decisional base of conflict avoidance vs. danger confrontation (34). Sensory input is integrated in the amygdala with ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) and hippocampal afferences, thus triggering or suppressing the response to a stimulus appraised as threatening (20). This flexible fear reaction model is adaptive if based on a realistic appraisal of the danger degree and individual resources, but it may become maladjustive in the following contexts (34):

Dana Cristina Her?a, Doina Cosman Specificity Of Emotional Processing In Posttraumatic Stress Disorder:

Page 3: SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN … · SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER ... features pf PTSD ... Of Emotional Processing In Posttraumatic

3

Figure 1: Patterns of conditional learning and extinction of fear response (20)

-Stimulus unrealistically represented and appraised as threat -Physiological and behavioral flight response to neutral stimuli-Too intense/rash response to stimuli, interfering with adaptive behaviors-Unrealistic associations between stimulus and other response components

Basically, the specific features of this structure in subjects that develop PTSD are: association of stimuli with erroneous danger appraisal, and connection of exaggerated or dysfunctional response to stimuli with an erroneous appraisal of incompetence and helplessness, respectively (20, 21, 34).

The hyerachical developmental model of emotional processing posits that sensations represent basic physiological elements of emotional awareness, through modulation, representation and refining which generates superior levels of conscious cognitive processing specific to individuals endowed with superior emotional intelligence and awareness, along with a degree of complexity of emotional experience that would facilitate affective regulation (36). Operating within this theoretical model, some authors suggested that subjects exposed to trauma and with no subsequent psychiatric disorders may be characterized by a higher degree of emotional awareness, compared to those that develop PTSD (37, 38). Moreover, it was suggested that both individuals that develop PTSD, and those with no subsequent posttraumatic psychopathology present levels of activation in the anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex during exposure to trauma – related stimuli, that appear associated with the degree of emotional awareness (38), assessed through the Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale (LEAS) (39).

Recent studies confirmed that PTSD subjects

have lower levels of emotional avareness and adaptability during the processing of emotionally – charged non-traumatic events, compared to healthy individuals (40, 41). Furthermore, high levels of emotional awareness in controls (ascertained by higher LEAS scores) were positively correlated with the activation of the ventral regions of the anterior cingulate and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, in contrast with PTSD subjects, in which LEAS scores were negatively correlated with the activation levels of the aforementioned areas (38). Also, in the same PTSD sample, the intensity of alexythymia was negatively correlated with the level of emotional awareness, while both negatively correlated with the degree of activation of the rostral area of the cingulate anterior cortex, which requires further clarifications (42).

A potential interpretation of this paradox may be that increased emotional awareness paired with low adaptive impact may be linked with an increased level of alexithymia; increased emotional awareness of a response entailing intense negative affect can become maladjustive (38, 42). Another potential explanation may be that individuals with impaired emotional awareness also exhibit impaired recognition of the elements of alexithymia (38), which is supported by the observation that subjects with alexithymia have extreme levels of emotional awareness – either very high, or very low (42). Therefore, both emotional awareness level and the adaptive impact of individual emotional response need to be considered in PTSD subjects (41, 42).

CLINICAL ISSUES IN POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a condition triggered by the exposure to a traumatic event which: generates feelings of helplessness, intense fear and horror; involves real or perceived life threat, and poses a threat to the physical integrity of the person or others. Core symptoms of PTSD are: re-experiencing of trauma – related issues, avoidance of trauma – related stimuli, arousal and numbing, while the time interval from the traumatic event to the onset of the disorder is also a diagnostic criterion (43).

PTSD onset involves a specific degree of severity of the event – unexpected/violent confrontation with death, severe trauma, threat posed on the integrity of the subject or others, paired with a specific individual reaction to the event – intense fear, helplessness, horror (33). As a number of negative life events are experienced by a significant proportion of persons throughout their existence without meeting the criteria of trauma that may potentially trigger PTSD, the definition of the traumatic event has undergone many changes in time, and a conceptual clarification of PTSD, as a complex entity reported to both the event per se, and the individual response to the event, was currently reached (44, 45). Etiopathogeny of PTSD entails complex interactions between the severity of the traumatic life event, individual vulnerability and peritraumatic environmental factors (33). Moreover, recent studies suggest that a heterogeneity of symptom patterns may develop, depending on the nature of the traumatic event (43).

PTSD diagnostic criteria include individual symptoms that can be ascertained in a variety of anxiety

Romanian Journal of Psychiatry, vol. XIV, No.1, 2012

Page 4: SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN … · SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER ... features pf PTSD ... Of Emotional Processing In Posttraumatic

4

patterns, irritability, hyperarousal, limited attention span, increased startle response (symptoms indicating the state of arousal), decreased interest for otherwise pleasant activities, aloofness, detached and constricted affect, limited projection in the future (33). What makes this clinical entity specific is the onset of the aforementioned symptoms consecutively to an extraordinary event that involves real or perceived threat to physical integrity, and generates intense negative affect – horror, helplessness, fear, thus supporting the classification as a distinctive disorder (46).

The importance of co-morbidities in PTSD stems from the following observations: on one hand, increased rates of depression, anxiety disorders and substance-related disorders were found in patients with PTSD, which may involve a common etiological mechanism (47, 48). Moving beyond categorial outlook, it was also observed that subjects with subclinical PTSD can also present with severe functional impairment, high co-morbidity rates and even suicide ideation (46, 47). On the other hand, neurophysiological patterns of response are significantly different in PTSD subjects, compared to those diagnosed with the aforementioned disorders, which generates a vast research field i.e. the difference between normal and pathological response to trauma (27, 33, 34).

VULNERABILITY AND RESILIENCE IN THE PROCESS OF COPING WITH TRAUMA

Individual patterns of cognitive and affective response to trauma play a key role in coining the concept of PTSD (33). The need to include the subjective component of trauma – emotional participation (intense negative emotions experienced during the traumatic event) and its individual appraisal – in current PTSD research is supported by the finding that these factors influence the development of PTSD clinical features (43), along with the individual perception regarding the degree of controllability of the traumatic event (46), developmental individual vulnerability factors, individual coping styles and posttraumatic social support, prior history of trauma and family history of psychopatology (46, 49).A comprehensive stress-response model was advanced as an overview of biological and sociocultural individual vulnerability factors, as well as resilience and protective ones, capturing a developmental continuum of PTSD-specific symptoms, in stark contrast with the flexibility of adaptive reactions in resilient individuals (49) – see figure 2.Thus, in the early stage of acute exposure to the traumatic event, both resilient persons and subjects with individual neurobiological vulnerability will exhibit acute stress response associated with fear and anxiety feelings (33, 49). As far as the emotional impact of traumatic events is concerned, what distinguishes vulnerable subjects is the intensity of arousal and of emotions with negative valence, while negative cognitive appraisals and dissociation create the premises for the onset of PTSD symptoms (49).

In vulnerable subjects, recollections of the traumatic event, precipitated by internal or external stimuli resonating with the trauma symbols, may extend to recurring intrusions of the incriminated event in sleep,

perceptual ( i l lus ions , hal lucinat ions , dissociative flashbacks), emotional (intense feelings with negative valence) and behavioral (exhausting avoidance of all external and internal factors associated with trauma) re-experiencing of trauma, paired with intense physiological arousal (33).

Figure 2: Models of genesis and evolution of stress response (34)

Some persons with resilience resources (social support, internal neurobiological factors, effective coping mechanisms) may develop transient or subclinical symptoms from the depression, anxiety or PTSD spectrum in the immediate aftermath of trauma exposure (33, 46, 49). Nevertheless, resilient subjects have a common specific ability to provide a neutral cognitive appraisal to traumatic events (49).

Subjects with increased risk for PTSD can be identified based on specific factors, regarded as predictors of clinical severity and evolution of the aforementioned disorder, such as: degree of negative emotional reaction (fear, anger), dissociation and numbing, and also on the level of interpersonal distress inducted by the traumatic event (50).

Hyperarousal, superficial sleep and avoidance behaviors triggered by trauma-related stimuli may become necessary and adaptive responses of self-preservation in the context of a realistically appraised imminent life-threat; constricted affect may be justified if the uncensored expression of anger triggered the event; also, recollections of the event, even obsessive ones, may fit the pattern of a necessary, effective and adaptive process of meaningful reframing and appraising of the terrifying experience, its impact and consequences, in

Dana Cristina Her?a, Doina Cosman Specificity Of Emotional Processing In Posttraumatic Stress Disorder:

Page 5: SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN … · SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER ... features pf PTSD ... Of Emotional Processing In Posttraumatic

5

order for the resilient persons to incorporate those meanings into their history and draw lessons for their developmental future (49).

Repeated exposure to emotionally – charged stimuli may generate a decrease in the amygdala activation pattern over time, i.e. a potential toning down of the difference between controls and PTSD subjects with amygdala hyperactivation patterns (53), which emphasizes the importance of the extent of exposure to stimuli with negative emotional valence (49).

FUNDING SUPPORT AND ROLE OF THE FUNDING SOURCE.The article is a part of a PhD research co-funded by the Social European Fund through the Operational

Posttraumatic stress disorder – state-of-the-science review of current research status

Although data from current neuroimagistic studies unravel relevant research directions in the field of PTSD, some limitations ought to be taken into account. Thus, the respective studies usually employed stimuli with negative emotional valence, while neural activation patterns for positive emotions in PTSD is insufficiently known. Also, standardized emotion–eliciting stimuli may have general relevance to some subjects, while others may appraise them as specific for the PTSD–generating event, given the individual specificity of the traumatic event (51, 52, 53).

Some neuroimagistic and functional anomalies observed in PTSD subjects may potentially represent family vulnerability factors, as opposed to consequences of exposure to trauma (33, 34, 41, 49).

Regarding research methods, transversal studies cannot ascertain whether the dysfunctional neural activation patterns described in PTSD existed prior to trauma or were generated by it – hence the comparative assessment of PTSD samples versus both healthy volunteers, and trauma – exposed subjects who did not subsequently develop PTSD (52). Results of veteran studies cannot be extrapolated to civilian populations, i.e. of both sexes and with a heterogenous distribution of age, nature of trauma, and pattern of exposure (55, 56).

Last but not least, Axis I and II co-morbid conditions, common in PTSD, may contribute to the neural activation patterns previously described (41, 48, 52, 53); however, excluding individuals with co-morbidities is not always an efficient solution, because resulting samples have a low significance from the viewpoint of the characteristics of the general PTSD population (41, 46, 53).

Exposure to trauma affectively and cognitively imprints individual and collective destiny, in the current context of globalization and acculturation. A plethora of psychopathological, neurobiological and sociocultural arguments confirm the key role of individual risk and resilience factors, intricated in a kaleidoscopic structure shaped by the subject's developmental dynamics. “It matters less how many times one falls, it matters more how many times one stands back up again”, therefore the efficiency of this structure is assessed through the appropriateness of context appraisal and the adaptive flexibility of individual response.CONFLICTS OF INTERESTS.The authors do not have any conflicts of interests to report.

Structural Programme for the Development of Human Resources (POSDRU) 2007 – 2013 „Invest in people! Doctoral Studies: through science to society”, Contract Code 6/1.5/S/3, University Babeº – Bolyai, Cluj-Napoca, Romania.

REFERENCES

1992;6: 47-53.

Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2007, 47-53.

11.Lazarus RS. Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. American Psychologist 1991;46(8):819-834.

New York: Oxford University Press, 2001, 3-34.

17.LeDoux JE. Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology 1995;46: 209-235.

22.Bar-On R, Tranel D, Denburg NL, Bechara A. Exploring the neurological substrate of emotional and social intelligence. Brain 2003;126: 1790-1800.

1.Moors A. Theories of emotion causation: a review. Cognition and emotion 2009;23(4):625-662.2.Oyebode F. Sims' Symptoms in the mind. An introduction to descriptive

thpsychopathology. 4 edition. Edinburgh: Saunders Elsevier, 2008, 315-338.3.Ekman P. An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion

4.Roseman IJ. Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. Cognition and Emotion 1991;5: 161-200.5.Frijda HN. What emotions might be? Comments on the comments. Social Science Information 2007;46: 433-443.6.Ekman P, Levenson RW, Friesen WV. Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science 1983;221(4616): 1208-1210.7.Ekman P. The directed facial action task. In: Coan JA, Allen JJB (eds). Handbook of emotional elicitation and assessment.

8.Oatley K, Johnson-Laird PN. Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and Emotion 1987;1: 29-50.9.Kuppens P. From appraisal to emotion. Emotion Review 2010;2(2): 157-158.10.Scherer KR. The dynamic architecture of emotion: evidence for the component process model. Cognition and Emotion 2009;23: 1307-1351.

12.Roseman IJ, Smith CA. Appraisal theory: Overview, assumptions, varieties, controversies. In: Scherer KR, Schorr A, Johnstone T (eds). Appraisal processes in emotion.

13.Cosman D. Psihologie medicalã. Ia?i: Editura Polirom, 2010, 56-101.14.Russell JA. Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review 2003;110: 145-172.

16.Moors A. Automatic constructive appraisal as a candidate cause of emotion. Emotion Review 2010;2(2): 139-156.

18.Leppanen JM. Neural and Developmental Bases of the Ability to Recognize Social Signals of Emotions. Emotion Review 2011;3(2): 179–188.19.Damasio AR. În cãutarea lui Spinoza. Cum explicã ?tiin?a sentimentele. Bucure?ti: Editura Humanitas, 2010.20.Stahl SM. Stahl's Essential Psychopharmacology. Neuroscientific

rdbasis and practical applications. 3 edition. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2008, 721-772.21.Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9: 242-249.

23.Christakou A, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Prefrontal cortical-ventral striatal interactions involved in affective modulation of attentional performance: implications for corticostriatal circuit function. Journal of Neuroscience 2004;24(4): 773-780.24.Lee GP, Meador KJ, Loring DW et al. Neural Substrates of Emotion as Revealed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2004;17(1): 9-17.25.Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher D, Aharon-Peretz J. Impaired "affective theory of mind" is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1): 55-67.26.Cheng DT, Knight DC, Smith CN, Helmstetter FJ. Human Amygdala Activity During the Expression of Fear Responses. Behavioral Neuroscience 2006;120(5): 1187–1195.27.Ohira H. The somatic marker revisited: brain and body in emotional decision-making. Emotion Review 2010;2(3): 245-249.28.LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 2000;23: 155–184.29.Salgado-Pineda P, Delaveau P, Blin O, Nieoullon A. Dopaminergic contribution to the regulation of emotional perception. Clin Neuropharmacol 2005;28: 228 –237.30.Pezze MA, Feldon J.Mesolimbic dopaminergic pathways in fear conditioning. Prog Neurobiol 2004;74: 301–320

15.Bastiaansen JACJ, Thioux M, Keysers C. Evidence for mirror systems in emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 2009;364: 2391–2404.

Romanian Journal of Psychiatry, vol. XIV, No.1, 2012

Page 6: SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN … · SPECIFICITY OF EMOTIONAL PROCESSING IN POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER ... features pf PTSD ... Of Emotional Processing In Posttraumatic

6

31.Seamans JK, Yang CR. The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Prog Neurobiol 2004;74: 1–58.32.Blasi G, Lo Bianco L, Taurisano P et al. Functional Variation of the Dopamine D2 Receptor Gene Is Associated with Emotional Control as well as Brain Activity and Connectivity during Emotion Processing in Humans. The Journal of Neuroscience 2009;29(47): 14812–14819.33.Kay J, Tasman A. Essentials of psychiatry. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons Ltd, 2006, 627-638.34.Yadin E, Foa EB. Cognitive behavioral treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder. In Kirmayer LJ, Lemelson R, Barad M (eds). Understanding trauma. Integrating biological, clinical and cultural perspectives. 35.Damasio AR. În cãutarea lui Spinoza. Cum explicã ?tiin?a sentimentele. Editura Humanitas: Bucure?ti; 2010, 29-131.36.Lane RD, Schwartz GE. Levels of emotional awareness: a cognitive-developmental theory ands its application to psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144: 133-43.37.Lane RD, Reiman EM, Axelrod B, Yun L-S, Holmes A, Schwartz GE. Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness: evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 1998;10: 525-35.38.Frewen P, Lane RD, Neufeld RWJ, Densmore M, Stevens T, Lanius R. Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness during trauma-script imagery in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine 2008;70: 27-31.39.Lane RD, Quinlan DM, Schwartz GE, Walker PA. The levels of emotional awareness scale: a cognitive-developmental measure of emotion. J Pers Assess 1990;55: 124-34.40.Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC et al. For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage 2004;23: 483–499.41.Nemeroff CB, Brenner JD, Foa EB, Mayberg HS, North CS, Stein MB. Posttraumatic stress disorder – a state-of-the-science review. J Psychiatr Res 2006;40: 709-29.42.Frewen PA, Dozois DJA, Neufeld RWJ, Lanius RA. Meta-analysis of alexithymia in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress 2008;21(2): 243-246.43.Semple D, Smyth R, Burns J, Darjee R, McIntosh A. Post-traumatic stress disorder (1) – diagnosis. In: Semple D, Smyth R, Burns J, Darjee R,

ndMcIntosh A (eds). Oxford Handbook of Psychiatry. 2 ed. New York: Oxford University Press, 2005, 368-9.

New York: Cambridge University Press, 2007, 178-193.

44.Adams CM. The Consequences of Witnessing Family Violence on Children and Implications for Family Counselors. The Family Journal 2006;14; 334.45.Teicher MH, Samson JA, Polcari A, McGreenery CE. Sticks, Stones, and Hurtful Words: Relative Effects of Various Forms of Childhood Maltreatment. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163: 993–1000.46.Benedek DM, Ursano, RJ. Posttraumatic stress disorder: from phenomenology to clinical practice. Focus 2009;7(2): 160-175.47.Baldwin DS, Hirschfeld RMA. Fast facts: Depression. 2nd edition. Oxford: Health Press, 2005, 43-48.48.Stein DJ, Hollander E. Anxiety disorders comorbid with depression. London: Martin Dunitz Ltd, 2002, 19-22.49.Konner M. Trauma, adaptation and resilience: a cross-cultural and evolutionary perspective. In: Kirmayer LJ, Lemelson R, Barad M (eds). Understanding trauma. Integrating biological, clinical and cultural perspectives. 50.Heinrichs M, Wagner D, Schoch W, Soravia LM, Hellhammer DH, Ehlert U. Predicting Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms From Pretraumatic Risk Factors: A 2-Year Prospective Follow-Up Study in Firefighters. Am J Psychiatry 2005;162: 2276–2286.51.Lanius RA, Williamson PC, Hopper J et al. Recall of emotional states in posttraumatic stress disorder: an fMRI investigation. Biol Psychiatry 2003;53:204-210.52.Rauch SL, van der Kolk BA, Fisler RE et al. A symptom provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron emission tomography and script-driven imagery. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996;53: 380-387.53.Phan KL, Taylor SF, Welsh RC, Decker LR, Noll DC, Nichols TE. Activation of the medial prefrontal cortex and extended amygdala by individual ratings of emotional arousal: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 2003;53: 211-215.24.Brady KT, Killeen TK, Brewerton T et al. Comorbidity of psychiatric disorders and posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 2000;61(suppl 7): 22-32.25.Simmons AD, Strigo IA, Mathhews SC, Paulus MP. Initial evidence of a failure to activate right anterior insula during affective set shifting in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine 2009;71: 373-377.56.Liberzon I, Taylor SF, Amdur R et al. Brain activation in PTSD in response to trauma-related stimuli. Biol Psychiatr 1999;45: 817-826.

New York: Cambridge University Press, 2007, 300-338.

***

Dana Cristina Her?a, Doina Cosman Specificity Of Emotional Processing In Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: