assessment of emotions: anxiety, anger, … · inventory (staxi), and the state-trait personality...

32
Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, Depression, and CuriosityCharles D. Spielberger* and Eric C. Reheiser University of South Florida, Tampa, USA Anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity are major indicators of psychological distress and well-being that require careful assessment. Measuring these psy- chological vital signs is of critical importance in diagnosis, and can facilitate treatment by directly linking intense emotions to the events that give rise to them. The historical background regarding theory and research on anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity is briefly reviewed, and the nature and assess- ment of these emotional states and personality traits are examined. The con- struction and development of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), the State-Trait Anger EXpression Inventory (STAXI-2), and the State-Trait Per- sonality Inventory (STPI) to assess anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity, and the major components of these emotional states and personality traits, are described in detail. Findings demonstrating the diverse utility and efficacy of these measures are also reported, along with guidelines for their interpretation and utilisation in research and clinical practice. Research with the STAI, STAXI and STPI over the last 40 years has contributed to understanding vitally important measurement concepts that are especially applicable to the assess- ment of emotions. These concepts included the state–trait distinction, item intensity specificity, and the importance of items that describe the presence or absence of emotions. Keywords: anger, anxiety, curiosity, depression, STAI, STAXI INTRODUCTION Emotions motivate behavior and have a significant impact on health and psychological well-being. According to the World Health Organization (2006), “Normally, emotions such as anxiety, anger . . . pain or joy interact to motivate a person to a goal-directed action. However, when certain emotions predominate and persist beyond their usefulness in motivating people for their goal-directed behaviour, they become morbid or pathological.” It is therefore essential to evaluate and monitor emotional states in diagnosis and * Address for correspondence: Charles D. Spielberger, Department of Psychology, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA. Email: [email protected] APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY: HEALTH AND WELL-BEING, 2009, 1 (3), 271–302 doi:10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01017.x © 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of Applied Psychology. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA.

Upload: duongdat

Post on 06-Aug-2018

251 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger,Depression, and Curiosityaphw_1017 271..302

Charles D. Spielberger* and Eric C. ReheiserUniversity of South Florida, Tampa, USA

Anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity are major indicators of psychologicaldistress and well-being that require careful assessment. Measuring these psy-chological vital signs is of critical importance in diagnosis, and can facilitatetreatment by directly linking intense emotions to the events that give rise tothem. The historical background regarding theory and research on anxiety,anger, depression, and curiosity is briefly reviewed, and the nature and assess-ment of these emotional states and personality traits are examined. The con-struction and development of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), theState-Trait Anger EXpression Inventory (STAXI-2), and the State-Trait Per-sonality Inventory (STPI) to assess anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,and the major components of these emotional states and personality traits, aredescribed in detail. Findings demonstrating the diverse utility and efficacy ofthese measures are also reported, along with guidelines for their interpretationand utilisation in research and clinical practice. Research with the STAI,STAXI and STPI over the last 40 years has contributed to understanding vitallyimportant measurement concepts that are especially applicable to the assess-ment of emotions. These concepts included the state–trait distinction, itemintensity specificity, and the importance of items that describe the presence orabsence of emotions.

Keywords: anger, anxiety, curiosity, depression, STAI, STAXI

INTRODUCTION

Emotions motivate behavior and have a significant impact on health andpsychological well-being. According to the World Health Organization(2006), “Normally, emotions such as anxiety, anger . . . pain or joy interact tomotivate a person to a goal-directed action. However, when certain emotionspredominate and persist beyond their usefulness in motivating people fortheir goal-directed behaviour, they become morbid or pathological.” It istherefore essential to evaluate and monitor emotional states in diagnosis and

* Address for correspondence: Charles D. Spielberger, Department of Psychology, Universityof South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA. Email: [email protected]

APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY: HEALTH AND WELL-BEING, 2009, 1 (3), 271–302doi:10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01017.x

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ,UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA.

Page 2: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

treatment just as physicians in medical examinations routinely measure pulserate, blood pressure, and temperature, the vital signs that provide essentialinformation about physical health.

When a physician detects an abnormal pulse or elevated blood pressureduring a medical examination, these symptoms may indicate a potentiallysignificant problem in the functioning of the cardiovascular system. Intenseanxiety and anger may be considered as analogous to elevations in pulse rateand blood pressure. The presence of a fever, as indicated by abnormally highbody temperature, can be considered as roughly analogous to depression.While running a high fever may indicate that the immune system is notprotecting the person from harmful viruses, symptoms of depression oftenreflect the presence of pervasive unresolved conflicts that contribute to anemotional fever.

Manifestations of anxiety, anger, and depression are critical psychologicalvital signs that are strongly related to an individual’s well-being. Variations inthe intensity and duration of these emotions provide essential informationabout a person’s mental health that help to identify recent events and long-standing conflicts that have a significant impact on the individual’s life.Assessing emotional vital signs and providing timely and meaningful feedbackwill also enhance awareness and understanding of a person’s feelings, and helpindividuals to recognise and cope more effectively with their emotions.

Symptoms of high anxiety are typically found in almost all emotionaldisorders. From a psychoanalytic perspective, Freud (1936, p. 85) regardedanxiety as the “fundamental phenomenon and the central problem of neuro-sis”. Observations of daily life and recent research findings indicate thatproblems with anger are also ubiquitous (Deffenbacher, 1992). Conse-quently, careful assessment of the experience, expression, and control ofanger is essential in psychological diagnosis and treatment planning (Sharkin,1988).

Depression has been described as “the common cold of mental healthproblems that strikes the rich and poor as well as the young and the old”(Rosenfeld, 1999, p. 10). The World Health Organization estimates that 340million people currently suffer from clinical depression, and that depressionwill become “the leading cause of disability and the 2nd leading contributorto the global burden of disease by the year 2020” (WHO, 2001). Theexperience of curiosity may be considered as a positive emotional vitalsign. As a motivator of exploratory behavior, curiosity often contributes toeffective personal adjustment and successful adaptation to environmentalstimuli.

The goals of this article are to briefly review the evolution of anxiety, anger,depression, and curiosity as emotional concepts, and to evaluate the role ofemotions as psychological vital signs. The construction and development ofthe State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), the State-Trait Anger Expression

272 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 3: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) formeasuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity, and theexperience, expression, and control of anger, will also be examined. In addi-tion, guidelines for interpreting and using these measures in research andclinical practice are described.

HISTORICAL EVOLUTION OF ANXIETY, ANGER,DEPRESSION, AND CURIOSITY AS EMOTIONAL CONCEPTS

Darwin (1872/1965) considered fear and rage to be universal characteristicsof both humans and animals that have evolved over time because theseemotions facilitated successful adaptation and survival (Plutchik, 2001).According to Darwin, “If we expect to suffer, we are anxious; if we have nohope of relief, we despair” (1872/1965, p. 176). In addition to relating anxietyto depression, Darwin (1872/1965, p. 81) regarded fear as “. . . the mostdepressing of all the emotions; and it soon induces utter, helpless prostra-tion”. Rage was also considered by Darwin (1872/1965, p. 74) to be a pow-erful emotion that motivated “. . . animals of all kinds, and their progenitorsbefore them, when attacked or threatened by an enemy” to fight and defendthemselves. He observed that rage was reflected in facial expression (e.g.reddened face, clenched teeth), accelerated heart-rate and muscular tension,and often resulted in violent behavior. For Darwin, anger was a state of mindthat differed “. . . from rage only in degree, and there is no marked distinctionin their characteristic signs” (1872/1965, p. 244). Thus, Darwin implicitlydefined anger as an emotional state that varies in intensity, from mild irrita-tion or annoyance to intense fury and rage.

Anxiety was defined by Freud (1924) as “something felt”, a specificunpleasant emotional state or condition that included apprehension, tension,worry, and physiological arousal. Freud (1936) equated objective anxietywith fear, which he considered to be an emotional reaction that was propor-tional in its intensity to a real danger in the external world. Consistent withDarwin’s evolutionary perspective, Freud’s “danger signal” theory empha-sised the adaptive utility of anxiety for motivating behavior that helped aperson cope more effectively with potentially harmful situations. Aggressionwas considered by Freud (1933/1959) to be an instinctual drive that moti-vated anger and aggressive behavior. When aggression cannot be directlyexpressed against external objects, it is turned back into the self, resulting indepression and other psychosomatic manifestations (Alexander & French,1948; Freud, 1936).

The origin of the concept of depression can be traced to the 5th century BCwritings of Hippocrates, the father of modern medicine (Jackson, 1986). TheGreek term, melancholia, which has the connotation of both anxiety anddepression, was used by Hippocrates to describe a “black mood” that

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 273

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 4: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

involved prolonged fear and sadness (Jackson, 1995). Galan’s restatement ofHippocrates’ description of melancholia as consisting of affective feelings,self-depreciating cognitions, and somatic symptoms prevailed for the next1,500 years.

In theory and research on psychopathology, anxiety and anger have beenrecognised as major contributors to depression for the last 100 years (Freud,1924, 1936; May, 1950/1977). The measurement of anxiety in diagnosis,treatment planning, and research has received significant attention for morethan half a century (Taylor, 1953; Spielberger, 1983). While the assessment ofanger has been relatively neglected, evidence from research on Type A behav-ior and heart disease (Friedman & Rosenman, 1974; Spielberger, Jacobs,Russell, & Crane, 1983; Spielberger & London, 1982), demonstrating thatanger was a major component of the Type A syndrome, has stimulated thedevelopment of measures of anger and hostility (Spielberger, 1976, 1980,1988, 1999).

In summary, fear (anxiety), anger (hostility, rage), and depression (melan-cholia) were considered by Darwin and Freud to be fundamental emotionalstates that had powerful effects on thoughts and behavior. Both Darwin andFreud also recognised that depression generally resulted from the interactionof anxiety and anger. Like anxiety and anger, symptoms of depression vary inseverity, from feeling sad or gloomy for a relatively short period of time, todeep long-lasting despair, extreme guilt, hopelessness, and thoughts of deaththat could result in suicide. Persistent depression as a personality trait canalso produce behavioral and physical symptoms such as fatigue, insomnia,impotence, frequent crying, chronic aches and pain, and excessive gain or lossin weight (Rosenfeld, 1999).

The insecurity caused by anxiety was also viewed by Freud (1936, 1959) asa major instigator of exploratory behavior, thus linking curiosity to anxiety.Although Freud did not directly address the origins of curiosity, he consid-ered exploratory behavior to be determined by instinctive biological urgesand ego mechanisms that served to reduce threat and insecurity (Aronoff,1962). William James (1890), who was strongly influenced by Darwin’s (1872/1965) views on evolution, also proposed an instinct theory of curiosity. Heconsidered attraction to a novel stimulus as adaptive because it facilitatedsurvival. Although the fear (anxiety) aroused by novel situations often inhib-ited curiosity, it was also adaptive because the novel stimulus might prove tobe dangerous. Thus, James, like Freud, recognised a potentially antagonisticrelationship between curiosity and fear, which often resulted in the simulta-neous arousal of these two emotions.

Curiosity can be broadly defined as a desire to acquire new knowledge andexperience that motivates exploratory behavior (Berlyne, 1949, 1950, 1954;Loewenstein, 1994; McDougall, 1921). While scientific interest in curiosityhas been reflected in the history of the discipline of psychology for more than

274 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 5: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

a hundred years (James, 1890), research on the assessment of curiosity hasreceived only passing attention (Spielberger & Starr, 1994). Of the smallnumber of researchers who have studied curiosity and exploratory behavior,Daniel Berlyne is clearly the most notable and influential contributor(Beswick, 2000). Berlyne (1954, 1960) identified two types of curiosity, whichhe labeled perceptual and epistemic. Perceptual curiosity was defined as “thecuriosity which leads to increased perception of stimuli” (Berlyne, 1954, p.180); epistemic curiosity was defined as “the drive to know” (p. 187). Twotypes of exploratory behavior were also differentiated by Berlyne (1960),which he labeled diversive and specific. Feelings of boredom stimulated adesire for stimulus variation that motivated diversive exploration; specificexploration was motivated by curiosity that stimulated acquiring new infor-mation about novel stimuli.

The fundamental importance of anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosityas emotional states that motivate a wide range of behaviors makes clear theneed to assess both the intensity of these emotions and how frequently theyare experienced. In early studies of human emotions, introspective reportswere used in efforts to discover and measure the qualitative feeling-states(“mental elements”) that were associated with different emotions (Titch-ener, 1897; Wundt, 1896). Feelings were considered by Wundt (1896) to bebasic, short-lived experiences, whereas emotions were more complex expe-riences that involved related feelings. It should be noted that Wundt dis-agreed with the James-Lange theory, which assumed that a person firstresponded cognitively to a situation and then experienced the emotion,including physiological arousal followed by behavioral reactions. Accord-ing to Wundt, introspection research demonstrated that the emotioncomes first, followed by cognition, physiological arousal, and behavioralconsequences.

With the advent of behaviorism, research on emotion shifted from theinvestigation of subjective feelings to the evaluation of physiological andbehavioral variables that could be objectively measured. The methodology ofbehaviorism required investigators to assess the impact of carefully definedand/or manipulated antecedent conditions on cognitive, physiological, andbehavioral responses that could be objectively measured. Beginning in the1960s, stimulated by a renaissance in cognitive psychology, the unique con-tributions of thoughts and feelings to emotional reactions was increasinglyrecognised. Emotions are now generally defined as complex psychobiologicalstates, consisting of affective feelings, cognitions, and physiological arousal(Spielberger, 1966). Since the quality and intensity of the feelings experiencedduring emotional arousal are the most unique and distinctive features of aparticular emotion, accurate assessment of emotions requires measurementof the intensity of qualitatively different emotional states as they fluctuateover time.

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 275

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 6: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

MEASURING ANXIETY WITH THE STATE-TRAIT ANXIETYINVENTORY (STAI)

The STAI was developed to provide reliable, relatively brief, self-report scalesfor assessing state and trait anxiety in research and clinical practice (Spiel-berger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970). Freud’s (1936) Danger Signal Theoryand Cattell’s concepts of state and trait anxiety (Cattell, 1966; Cattell &Scheier, 1958, 1963), as refined and elaborated by Spielberger (1966, 1972a,1972b, 1977, 1979a, 1983), provided the conceptual framework that guidedthe construction of the STAI. State anxiety (S-Anxiety) was defined as theintensity at a particular time of subjective feelings of tension, apprehension,nervousness, and worry, with associated activation (arousal) of the auto-nomic nervous system. Trait anxiety (T-Anxiety) was defined in terms ofrelatively stable individual differences in anxiety-proneness as reflected in thefrequency that anxiety states have been manifested in the past and the prob-ability that feelings of S-Anxiety will be experienced in the future.

When test construction for the STAI began in 1964, the initial goal wasto develop an inventory consisting of a single set of items that could beadministered with different instructions to assess both state and traitanxiety (Spielberger et al., 1970). A large pool of items was adapted fromexisting anxiety measures, and new items were written to assess either thepresence or absence of anxiety. The essential psychological content of eachitem was retained, but the format was modified so that the same item couldbe given with different instructions to assess either S-Anxiety or T-Anxiety.The state instructions required respondents to report the intensity of theirfeelings of anxiety, “right now, at this moment”. The trait instructionsasked subjects to report how they generally feel by rating the frequency thatthe anxiety-related feelings, cognitions, and symptoms described by eachitem were experienced.

A preliminary pool of more than 60 anxiety items was administered tolarge samples of university students and psychiatric patients, first with stateand then with trait instructions. In responding to the S-Anxiety items, sub-jects rated the intensity of their feelings (e.g. “I feel nervous”) on the following4-point scale: (1) Not at all; (2) Somewhat; (3) Moderately so; (4) Very muchso. In responding to the T-Anxiety items, subjects reported how they gener-ally feel by rating themselves on each item, using the following 4-pointfrequency scale: (1) Almost never; (2) Sometimes; (3) Often; (4) Almostalways. Based on extensive item-validity research with more than 2,000 studyparticipants, a final set of 20 items was selected for the preliminary form ofthe STAI (Spielberger et al., 1970).

Research with the preliminary STAI indicated that altering the instructionscould not overcome the strong state or trait psycholinguistic connotations ofkey words in a number of items (Spielberger et al., 1970). For example, when

276 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 7: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

given with trait instructions, “I worry too much” was stable over time andcorrelated highly with other T-Anxiety items, but scores on this item did notincrease in response to stressful circumstances, nor did they decrease underrelaxed conditions, as required for the construct validity of S-Anxiety items.In contrast, “I feel upset”, was a highly sensitive measure of S-Anxiety.Scores on this item increased markedly under stressful conditions and werelower under relaxed conditions. However, when given with trait instructions,the item scores were unstable over time and correlations with other T-Anxietyitems were relatively weak.

Given the difficulties encountered in measuring state and trait anxiety withthe same items, the STAI test-construction strategy was modified and sepa-rate sets of 20 items were selected to assess S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety. Whengiven with state instructions, items with the best construct validity, as indi-cated by substantially higher scores under stressful than non-stressful condi-tions, were selected for the 20-item STAI(Form X) S-Anxiety Scale. Whengiven with trait instructions, those items that were most stable over time withthe best concurrent validity, as indicated by the highest correlations with theTaylor (1953) Manifest Anxiety Scale (MAS) and Cattell and Scheier’s (1963)Anxiety Scale Questionnaire (ASQ), were selected for the 20-item STAI-(Form X) T-Anxiety Scale (Spielberger et al., 1970). Only five of the STAI-(Form X) items were the same in both the 20-item S-Anxiety and the 20-itemT-Anxiety scales. The remaining 15 items in each scale were relatively uniquemeasures of either state or trait anxiety.

Following the publication in 1970 of the STAI(Form X), insights gainedfrom more than a decade of research stimulated a major revision of thisinventory. In revising the STAI, the primary goal was to develop “purer”measures for assessing state and trait anxiety in adolescents and adults.Careful scrutiny of the content of the STAI items with the best psychometricproperties resulted in clearer conceptual definitions of the constructs of stateand trait anxiety, which then guided the construction of potential replace-ment items for the revised STAI(Form Y). The item selection and validationprocedures that resulted in the replacement of 30 per cent of the originalSTAI(Form X) items are described in detail in the STAI(Form Y) TestManual (Spielberger, 1983).

In the construction and validation of the STAI(Form Y), more than 5,000subjects were tested. Factor analyses of the Form Y items identified distincttrait and state anxiety factors (Spielberger, Vagg, Barker, Donham, & West-berry, 1980), which were generally consistent with the results of factor studiesof the STAI(Form X) (Gaudry, Spielberger, & Vagg, 1975). However, theSTAI(Form Y) T-Anxiety factors were stronger, more differentiated, andhad better simple structure then the corresponding Form X factors, reflectinga better balance between the number of T-Anxiety present and absent itemsin Form Y (Spielberger et al., 1980). The STAI(Form Y) State and Trait

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 277

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 8: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

Anxiety Scales are briefly described in Table 1, along with guidelines forinterpreting scores on these measures.

Reliability, Stability, and Internal Consistency of theSTAI(Form Y)Detailed psychometric information regarding the reliability and validity forthe STAI(Form Y) are reported in the Test Manual (Spielberger, 1983).Test–retest stability coefficients for the T-Anxiety scale over intervals of 20 to104 days were reasonably high for large groups of high school and collegestudents, ranging from .73 to .86. In contrast, the stability coefficients for theS-Anxiety scale were relatively low, with a median r of only .33. This lack oftest–retest stability for the S-Anxiety scale was both expected and considereddesirable because a valid state anxiety measure should reflect the influence ofunique situational factors at the time of testing.

Since anxiety states are expected to vary in intensity as a function ofperceived stress, measures of internal consistency such as alpha coefficientsprovide a more meaningful index of the reliability of S-Anxiety scales thantest–retest correlations. Alpha reliability coefficients for the STAI(Form Y)S-Anxiety scale, computed by Formula KR-20 as modified by Cronbach

TABLE 1Brief Description of the STAI(Form Y) State and Trait Anxiety Scales and

Guidelines for Interpreting High Scores on these Scales

STAI scalesNumberof items

Scorerange

Descriptions and guidelines for interpretingSTAI Anxiety Scale scores

State-Anxiety(S-Anxiety)

20 20–80 Measures the intensity of feelings of anxiety as anemotional state at a particular time.

Individuals with high S-Anxiety scores experiencedrelatively intense feelings of tension and anxiety at thetime the test was administered.

If S-Anxiety scores are elevated, the individual’s feelingsare influenced by situational factors that are interpretedas indicating present or anticipated danger, or bythoughts relating to traumatic past events associatedwith the present situation.

Trait-Anxiety(T-Anxiety)

20 20–80 Measures individual differences in anxiety proneness as apersonality trait as indicated by how often feelings ofanxiety (S-Anxiety) are experienced over time.

Persons high in T-Anxiety experience more frequent andintense feelings of S-Anxiety in situations perceived asdangerous or threatening, or when feeling inadequate ininterpersonal relationships.

278 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 9: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

(1951), were .86 or stronger for large, independent samples of male andfemale high school and college students, working adults, and militaryrecruits, with a median coefficient across groups of .93 (Spielberger, 1983).For these groups, the alpha coefficients of the T-Anxiety scale were alsouniformly high, with a median alpha of .90.

The distribution of scores for the STAI S-Anxiety scale when given underneutral conditions is positively skewed, and approaches a normal distributionunder stressful conditions. Consequently, the alpha coefficients may be some-what higher when this scale is given under conditions of greater psychologicalstress. For example, the alpha reliability for the STAI S-Anxiety scale was .94when administered to college males immediately after a distressing film withinstructions to report how they felt while watching the film. For the samesubjects, the alpha for the S-Anxiety scale was .89 when given following abrief period of relaxation training.

Content, Concurrent and Construct Validity of theSTAI(Form Y)Individual STAI items were required to meet stringent validity criteria at eachstage of the test development process (Spielberger, 1983; Spielberger &Gorsuch, 1966; Spielberger et al., 1970). Most of the original STAI itemswere selected on the basis of significant correlations with widely used mea-sures of anxiety at the time the inventory was developed (Spielberger et al.,1970). However, it was subsequently noted that the content of several itemsadapted from the Taylor (1953) MAS reflected depression rather than anxiety(e.g. “I cry easily”; “I feel blue”). In developing the revised STAI(Form Y),the conceptual definitions of state and trait anxiety were improved, and thenew items that were constructed in keeping with these definitions had betterpsychometric properties than the items with depressive content, which werereplaced (Spielberger, 1983).

Relatively high correlations between scores on the STAI(Form Y)T-Anxiety scale with the MAS and the ASQ, ranging from .73 to .85, provideevidence of concurrent validity, indicating that all three inventories measuretrait anxiety. It should be noted, however, that the 20-item STAI T-Anxietyscale requires less than half as much time to administer as the 50-item MASand the 43-item ASQ, and is less contaminated with depression than theMAS. The correlations of the STAI(Form Y) S-Anxiety scale with the MASand ASQ were less than .50, which were similar to the correlations betweenthe STAI(Form Y) State and Trait scales.

Evidence of the construct validity of the STAI(Form Y) T-Anxiety scale isreflected in the relatively high mean scores for various neuropsychiatric (NP)patient groups for whom anxiety is a major symptom (American PsychiatricAssociation, 1994). Except for patients with character disorders, all NP

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 279

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 10: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

diagnostic groups have substantially higher T-Anxiety scores than normalsubjects (Spielberger, 1983). General medical and surgical (GMS) patientswith psychiatric complications also have higher T-Anxiety scores than GMSpatients without such complications, indicating that the STAI T-Anxietyscale can help to identify non-psychiatric patients with emotional problems.

The construct validity of the STAI was demonstrated in findings that theS-Anxiety scale scores of college students were significantly higher duringexaminations and lower after relaxation training as compared to theirS-Anxiety scores when they were tested in relatively non-stressful classperiods (Spielberger, 1983). Further evidence of the construct validity of theS-Anxiety scale was observed in military recruits, whose S-Anxiety scoressubstantially increased while they participated in a highly stressful trainingprogram, and were much higher than those of high school and college stu-dents tested under relatively non-stressful classroom conditions. In construct-ing and validating the STAI, more than 10,000 adolescents and adults havebeen tested. Norms for high school and college students, working adults,military personnel, prison inmates, and psychiatric, medical, and surgicalpatients are reported in the STAI(Form Y) Test Manual (Spielberger, 1983).The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAIC), which was con-structed to measure anxiety in 9- to 12-year-old elementary school children(Spielberger, 1973), has been used in numerous studies of children withemotional or physical problems.

Since first being introduced more than 40 years ago (Spielberger &Gorsuch, 1966), the STAI has been adapted in 60 different languages anddialects, and cited in over 14,000 archival studies (Spielberger, 1989).Research with the STAI has included experimental investigations and clinicalstudies of stress-related psychiatric, psychosomatic, and medical disorders.The STAI has also been widely utilised in investigations of general psycho-logical processes, such as attention, memory, learning, and academic achieve-ment; the effects of anxiety on situation-specific phenomena, such as speechanxiety and sports competition; and as an outcome measure in research onthe effectiveness of biofeedback and behavioral and cognitive treatments.

ASSESSING THE EXPERIENCE, EXPRESSION, ANDCONTROL OF ANGER WITH THE STATE-TRAIT ANGER

EXPRESSION INVENTORY (STAXI)

The maladaptive effects of anger are traditionally emphasised as a majorcontributor to the etiology of the psychoneuroses and depression. Researchfindings also indicate that anger and hostility contribute to the pathogenesisof hypertension (e.g. Crane, 1981; Harburg, Blakelock, & Roeper, 1979) andcoronary heart disease (Friedman & Rosenman, 1974; Matthews, Glass,Rosenman, & Bortner, 1977; Spielberger & London, 1982). Although much

280 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 11: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

has been written about the negative impact of anger and hostility on physicaland psychological well-being, definitions of these constructs remain ambigu-ous and are sometimes contradictory, leading to conceptual confusion anda diversity of measurement operations of questionable validity (Biaggio,Supplee, & Curtis, 1981).

Given the substantial overlap in prevailing conceptual definitions of anger,hostility, and aggression, we have referred to them, collectively, as the AHA!Syndrome (Spielberger et al., 1985). The concept of anger usually refers to anemotional state that consists of feelings that vary in intensity, with associatedactivation or arousal of the autonomic nervous system. Although hostilitygenerally involves intense angry feelings, this concept also has the connota-tion of a complex set of attitudes and behaviors that include being mean,vicious, vindictive, and often cynical (Spielberger et al., 1985). Aggression asa psychological construct generally implies destructive or punitive behaviordirected towards other persons or objects in the environment (Buss, 1961). Itshould be noted, however, that most psychometric measures of anger-relatedconstructs tend to confound angry feelings with hostility and aggressivebehavior.

Measuring Anger with the State-Trait AngerScale (STAS)The State-Trait Anger Scale (STAS) was constructed to assess the intensity ofanger as an emotional state at a particular time, and to measure individualdifferences in anger proneness as a personality trait (Spielberger et al., 1983).The procedures for constructing the STAS were generally similar to thoseused in developing the STAI (Spielberger, 1980). State anger (S-Anger) wasdefined as a psychobiological state or condition, consisting of angry feelingsthat may vary in intensity, from mild irritation or annoyance to fury and rage,with associated activation of the autonomic nervous system. Trait anger(T-Anger) was defined in terms of individual differences in the frequency thatS-Anger was experienced over time. Guided by these working definitions, apool of items was constructed to assess the intensity of angry feelings(S-Anger) and individual differences in anger-proneness (T-Anger). Inresponding to the S-Anger and the T-Anger items, examinees rate the inten-sity of their angry feelings and the frequency that these feelings are experi-enced, using the same 4-point rating scales that are used in responding to theSTAI State and Trait Anxiety scales.

A preliminary form of the STAS, consisting of 15 S-Anger and 15 T-Angeritems, was administered to a large sample of university students (Spielbergeret al., 1983). The alpha coefficients for the 15-item preliminary STASS-Anger and T-Anger scales were .93 and .87, respectively, indicating a highlevel of internal consistency. The test–retest reliability coefficients for the

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 281

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 12: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

STAS T-Anger scale over a 2-week interval were .70 for males and .77 forfemales (Jacobs, Latham, & Brown, 1988). In contrast, the stability coeffi-cients for the STAS S-Anger scale were much lower (.27 for males, .21 forfemales), as would be expected for a measure of transitory anger.

Given the high internal consistency of the 15-item preliminary STASS-Anger and T-Anger scales, it was possible to reduce the length of each ofthese scales to 10 items without unduly weakening their psychometric prop-erties. The state and trait items with the largest item–remainder correlations(.50 or higher) were selected for the 10-item STAS S-Anger and T-Angerscales. Correlations between the 10- and 15-item S-Anger and T-Anger scalesfor college students and Navy recruits ranged from .95 to .99, indicating thatthe 10-item scales provided essentially the same information as the longerforms (Spielberger, 1988).

In factor analyses of the 10 items comprising the STAS S-Anger scale, onlya single underlying factor was identified for both males and females, indicat-ing that the S-Anger scale measures a unitary emotional state that varies inintensity. In contrast, factor analyses of the 10 STAS T-Anger items consis-tently identified two correlated factors, which were labeled Angry Tempera-ment (T-Anger/T) and Angry Reaction (T-Anger/R). The T-Anger/T itemsdescribed individual differences in the disposition to experience angerwithout specifying any provoking circumstance (e.g. “I am a hot headedperson”; “I have a fiery temper”). The T-Anger/R items described angryreactions in response to situations that involved frustration and/or negativeevaluations (e.g. “It makes me furious when I am criticised in front ofothers”).

The results of a study that compared hypertensive patients with medicaland surgical patients with normal blood pressure provided strong evidencedemonstrating that the two STAS T-Anger subscales measured differentfacets of trait anger (Crane, 1981). The finding that the T-Anger scores of thehypertensive patients were significantly higher than those of the medical andsurgical patients was due entirely to the substantially higher T-Anger/Rscores of the hypertensives. In contrast, the T-Anger/T scores of the hyper-tensives were essentially the same as those of the patients with normal bloodpressure. Crane (1981) also found that hypertensive patients had significantlyhigher S-Anger scores while performing on a mildly frustrating task thanpatients with normal blood pressure.

The STAS T-Anger scale was administered to large samples of collegestudents and Navy recruits (Spielberger et al., 1983), along with the Buss-Durkee (1957) Hostility Inventory (BDHI) and the Hostility (HO; Cook &Medley, 1954) and Overt Hostility (Hv; Schultz, 1954) scales. Moderatelyhigh correlations of the STAS T-Anger scale with all three hostility measuresfor males and females in both studies provide evidence of a strong relation-ship between T-Anger and hostility. Moderate correlations of the STAS

282 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 13: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

T-Anger scale were also found with the Neuroticism Scale of the EysenckPersonality Questionnaire (EPQ; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975) and theT-Anxiety scale of the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI; Spielberger,1979b). These findings are consistent with clinical observations that neuroticindividuals frequently experience guilt and anxiety in relation to their angryfeelings (Spielberger, 1988).

In a series of studies, Deffenbacher (1992) found that individuals with highSTAS T-Anger scores reported that they more frequently experienced intenseangry feelings across a wide range of provocative situations than persons withlow T-Anger scores. The high T-Anger individuals also reported experiencinganger-related physiological symptoms 2 to 4 times more often than those lowin T-Anger. When provoked, individuals with high T-Anger scores alsoreported stronger tendencies to both express and suppress their anger, andmore dysfunctional coping. Negative events, such as a failure experience, alsohad a more devastating (catastrophising) impact on high T-Anger individuals(Story & Deffenbacher, 1985).

Measuring the Expression and Control of Anger withthe Anger Expression (AX) ScaleAs our research on anger has progressed, the critical importance of differen-tiating between the experience of anger and the characteristic ways in whichanger was expressed became increasingly apparent (Spielberger et al., 1985).In previous research, anger expression was implicitly defined as a unidimen-sional, bipolar construct (Funkenstein, King, & Drolette, 1954; Gentry,Chesney, Gary, Hall, & Harburg, 1982), varying from extreme suppression orinhibition of anger to the frequent expression of anger in aggressive behavior.The Anger EXpression (AX) Scale was constructed to assess this dimension,guided by working definitions of anger-in and anger-out that were formu-lated on the basis of a review of relevant previous research (Spielberger et al.,1985). Anger-in was defined in terms of how often an individual experiences,but holds in (suppresses), angry feelings. Anger-out was defined as the fre-quency that an individual expresses angry feelings in verbal or physicallyaggressive behavior.

The content of the AX Scale items ranged from describing strong inhibi-tion or suppression of angry feelings (AX/In) to the extreme expression ofanger directed toward other persons or objects in the environment (AX/Out).The AX Scale also included items for assessing the middle range of thehypothesised unidimensional anger-in/anger-out continuum. The format forrating the AX Scale items was the same as that used with the STAS T-Angerscale, but the instructions differed markedly from those used in assessingT-Anger. Rather than simply asking respondents to rate themselves accord-ing to how they generally feel, they were informed that “Everyone feels angry

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 283

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 14: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

or furious from time to time . . . ”, and instructed to report “. . . how oftenyou generally react or behave in the manner described when you feel angry orfurious” (Spielberger, Reheiser, & Sydeman, 1995, p. 58). The following areexamples of AX Scale items:

“When angry or furious”:AX/In: I keep things in; I boil inside, but I don’t show it.AX/Out: I lose my temper; I strike out at whatever infuriates me.

Johnson (1984) administered the preliminary 33-item AX Scale to 1,060high school students. The results of separate factor analyses for both femalesand males clearly indicated that the AX Scale items were tapping two inde-pendent dimensions, which were labeled Anger/In and Anger/Out on thebasis of the content of the items with the strongest loadings on these factors.Given the strength and clarity of the Anger/In and Anger/Out factors and thestriking similarity of both factors for males and females, the items with thestrongest loadings on each factor were selected to form 8-item AX/In andAX/Out subscales (Spielberger et al., 1985). The items selected for the AX/Insubscale had uniformly high loadings for both sexes on the Anger/In factor(mdn. = .665) and negligible loadings on the Anger/Out factor(mdn. = -.045). Similarly, the 8 items selected for the AX/Out subscale hadhigh loadings for both sexes on the Anger/Out factor (mdn. = .59) and neg-ligible loadings on Anger/In (mdn. = -.01).

Pollans (1983) factored responses to the 33 prelimenary AX Scale items fora large sample of college students and found clearly defined anger-in andanger-out factors, with essentially zero correlations between the 8-itemAX/In and AX/Out subscales for both males and females. Similar findingshave subsequently been reported for other populations (Knight, Chisholm,Paulin, & Waal-Manning, 1988; Spielberger, 1988). In a study of the internalconsistency and reliability of the 8-item AX/In and AX/Out subscales, Jacobset al. (1988) found alpha coefficients ranging from .73 to .84, and test–retestreliability correlations of .64 to .86 over varying time periods. Thus, the8-item AX/In and AX/Out subscales are factorially orthogonal, empiricallyindependent, internally consistent, and relatively stable over time. Clearly,the AX/In and AX/Out subscales measure two distinct components ofanger-expression.

Pollans (1983) also found that three items (“Control my temper”; “Keepmy cool”; “Calm down faster”) included in the AX Scale to assess the middlerange of the hypothesised anger-in/anger-out continuum coalesced to formthe nucleus of a third factor, which stimulated the development of an AngerControl (AX/Con) subscale. Dictionary and thesaurus definitions and idiomspertaining to the control of anger guided the construction of additional angercontrol items. The resulting pool of anger control items was administered toa large sample of university students, along with the AX/In and AX/Out

284 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 15: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

subscale items. In separate factor analyses for males and females, a stronganger control factor was identified, along with clearly defined anger-in andanger-out factors. The five items with the strongest loadings on the anger-control factor for both males and females were added to the three originalanger control items to form the 8-item AX/Con subscale.

The 24-item AX Scale, which included 8-item AX/In, AX/Out andAX/Con subscales, was administered to a large sample of university students(Spielberger, Krasner, & Solomon, 1988). In factor analyses of responses tothe AX Scale items, anger-control consistently emerged as the strongestfactor for both males and females; all 8 AX/Con items had dominant salientloadings on this factor. Well-defined Anger/In and Anger/Out factors werealso found; all 8 AX/In and 8 AX/Out items had dominant salient loadings onthe appropriate factor. The AX/Con subscale correlated negatively with theAX/Out subscale for both males (r = -.59) and females (r = -.58). Correla-tions of the AX/In subscale scores with the AX/Out and AX/Con subscaleswere essentially zero for both sexes (Pollans, 1983; Spielberger et al., 1988).

Spielberger et al. (1988) found moderately high positive correlations of theAX T-Anger/T subscale with the AX/Out scale, suggesting that persons withan angry temperament are more likely to express their anger toward otherpeople or objects in the environment rather than to suppress it. Small positivecorrelations of the T-Anger/R subscale with the AX/Out and AX/In subscalesappear to indicate that individuals who frequently experienced anger whenfrustrated or treated unfairly were equally likely to express or suppress theiranger (Spielberger, 1988). Small but highly significant positive correlations ofthe AX/In and AX/Out subscales with the T-Anxiety scale of the State-TraitPersonality Inventory (STPI) suggest that persons who more often sup-pressed or expressed their anger tend to experience anxiety more frequentlythan those with low anger expression scores.

As previously noted, Harburg et al. (1979) and Gentry et al. (1982) foundthat individuals who suppressed their anger had higher systolic and diastolicblood pressure. Williams et al. (1980) reported that patients with high MMPIHostility scores were more likely to develop coronary artery disease. Simi-larly, Dembroski, MacDougall, Williams, and Haney (1985) observed thathigh ratings of “anger-in” or potential for hostility were positively associatedwith the severity of atherosclerosis. In keeping with these findings, a majorreason for constructing the AX Scale was to facilitate the investigation ofhow the expression and/or suppression of anger contributed to the etiology ofhypertension, atherosclerosis, and coronary heart disease.

Johnson (1984) investigated the relationship of AX Scale scores with sys-tolic (SBP) and diastolic (DBP) blood pressure for a large sample of highschool students (N = 1,114). The correlations of AX/In scores with SBP andDBP were positive and highly significant for both sexes; students with veryhigh AX/In scores had much higher SBP. After partialing out the influence of

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 285

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 16: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

a number of variables previously found to be related to BP (e.g. height,weight, dietary factors, racial differences, family history of hypertension), theAX/In scores continued to be positively and significantly associated withelevated SBP and DBP. In multiple regression analyses, Johnson found thatAX/In scores were better predictors of blood pressure than any other measurefor both males and females. In contrast, the correlations of AX/Out scoreswith blood pressure were quite small for both sexes.

Measuring the Experience, Expression, and Control ofAnger with the STAXIThe 20-item STAS and 24-item AX scales were combined to form the 44-itemState-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI), which was developed tomeasure the experience, expression, and control of anger (Spielberger, 1988).Fuqua et al. (1991) administered the STAXI to 455 undergraduate collegestudents, and factored their responses to the 44 items. The first six factorsidentified by Fuqua et al. (1991) were each defined by essentially the sameitems comprising the STAXI S-Anger, AX/In, AX/Out, and AX/Con scales,and the T-Anger Temperament and Reaction subscales. Almost all of the 44STAXI items had dominant salient loadings on the appropriate factor andnegligible loadings on the other factors. A small seventh factor, “Feel likeexpressing anger”, was also identified by Fuqua et al. (1991), which wasdefined primarily by strong loadings for three of the 10 STAS S-Anger items(Feel like . . . breaking things, banging on the table, hitting someone), whichalso had strong loadings on the Fuqua et al. S-Anger factor.

Van der Ploeg (1988) administered a Dutch adaptation of the 10-itemSTAS State Anger Scale to male military draftees in the Netherlands. Factoranalysis of responses to these items identified two S-Anger factors, indicatingthat “feeling angry” and “feel like expressing anger” were distinct compo-nents of anger as an emotional state. Subsequent factor analyses by Forgays,Forgays, and Spielberger (1997) of responses to the 44 STAXI items providedfurther evidence of two distinctive, relatively independent S-Anger factors.Based on the content of the items with dominant salient loadings, thesefactors were labeled Feeling angry (e.g. “I feel furious”, “I feel angry”) andFeel like expressing anger (e.g. “I feel like swearing”, “I feel like hittingsomeone”). The “Feel like expressing anger” factor was defined by highloadings on items that described either verbal or physical expression.

On the basis of research findings that consistently identified more than oneS-Anger factor, the original 44-item STAXI was revised and expanded. Therevised 57-item STAXI-2 comprises 42 of the 44 original STAXI items, plus15 new items that were constructed to measure three components of S-Anger(Spielberger, 1999). The 15-item STAXI-2 S-Anger scale includes three fac-torially derived 5-item subscales that assess: Feeling Angry (S-Anger/F), Feel

286 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 17: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

Like Expressing Anger Verbally (S-Anger/V), and Feel Like ExpressingAnger Physically (S-Anger/P). Brief descriptions of the STAXI-2 scales andsubscales, along with the number of items and the range of scores for eachscale, are noted in Table 2.

The 10-item STAXI-2 T-Anger scale and the two 4-item T-Anger subscalesthat assess Angry Temperament (T-Anger/T) and Angry Reaction(T-Anger/R) are unchanged from the original 44-item STAXI. The 8-itemSTAXI-2 AX/Out and AX/In scales are also the same as in the originalSTAXI. The STAXI-2 scale for measuring the control of anger-out(AX/Con-Out) comprises 7 of the 8 original STAXI AX/Con Scale items,plus 1 replacement item. An entirely new 8-item scale was constructed toassess the control of anger-in (AX/Con-In), i.e. calming down to reduce theintensity of suppressed anger. The STAXI-2 AX Index, which provides ameasure of total anger expression, is computed by the following formula; theconstant, 48, was included to eliminate possible negative numbers:

AX Index AX Out AX In AX Con-Out Ax Con-In= + − +( ) + 48.

Guidelines for Interpreting the STAXI Scale andSubscale ScoresThe STAXI has proved useful for assessing the experience, expression, andcontrol of anger in normal individuals, and in evaluating the anger experi-enced by patients with a variety of psychological and medical disorders,including alcoholism, hypertension, coronary heart disease, and cancer (Def-fenbacher, 1992; Moses, 1992; Spielberger, 1988, 1999). Comparing thescores on the STAXI-2 scales and subscales of individual males and femaleswith norms for high school and college students, working adults, psychiatricand medical patients, and other specific groups is an important first step intest interpretation. Norms for the STAXI-2 scales and subscales for male andfemale adolescents, adults, and psychiatric patients are reported in the TestManual (Spielberger, 1999), which includes percentile ranks and T-Scores foreach scale and subscale. Norms for the original STAXI are also available forgeneral medical and surgical patients, prison inmates, and military recruits.

Guidelines for interpreting scores on the STAXI-2 scales and subscales arereported in Table 3. Individuals with high anger scores (above the 75thpercentile) are more likely to experience and/or express angry feelings thatinterfere with optimal functioning. Persons with high anger-in and/or anger-out scores often have difficulties in interpersonal relationships, and maydevelop psychological and medical disorders. High AX/In scores when asso-ciated with high levels of anxiety have been found to be associated withelevated blood pressure and hypertension (Crane, 1981; Johnson, 1984).Individuals with high scores on both the AX/In and AX/Out scales (above the

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 287

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 18: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

TABLE 2Brief Description of the STAXI-2 Anger Experience, Expression, and Control

Scales and Subscales

STAXI-2scale/subscale

Numberof items

Scorerange

Description of the STAXI-2 scaleand subscale scores

State Anger(S-Anger)

15 15–60 Measures the intensity of angry feelings at aparticular time, and the extent to which aperson feels like expressing anger verballyor physically.

Feeling Angry(S-Ang/F)

5 5–20 Measures the intensity of the angry feelingsthat the respondent is currentlyexperiencing.

Feel Like ExpressingAnger Verbally(S-Ang/V)

5 5–20 Measures the intensity of current feelingsrelated to the verbal expression of anger.

Feel Like ExpressingAnger Physically(S-Ang/P)

5 5–20 Measures the intensity of current feelingsrelated to the physical expression of anger.

Trait Anger(T-Anger)

10 10–40 Measures individual differences in anger as apersonality trait in terms of how oftenangry feelings are experienced over time.

Angry Temperament(T-Ang/T)

4 4–16 Measures individual differences in thedisposition to experience anger withoutspecific provocation.

Angry Reaction(T-Ang/R)

4 4–16 Measures the frequency that angry feelingsare experienced in reaction to situationsthat involve negative evaluations or beingtreated badly.

Anger Expression-Out (AX/Out)

8 8–32 Measures how often angry feelings areexpressed in verbal or aggressive behaviordirected toward other persons or objects inthe environment.

Anger Expression-In(AX/In)

8 8–32 Measures how often angry feelings areexperienced and turned inward orsuppressed.

Anger Control-Out(AX/Con-Out)

8 8–32 Measures how often a person endeavors toprevent or control the expression of angryfeelings, e.g. by “keeping the lid on”.

Anger Control-In(AX/Con-In)

8 8–32 Measures how often a person attempts toreduce and control angry feelings bycalming down or cooling off.

Anger ExpressionIndex (AX Index)

32 0–96 Provides a general index of anger expressionbased on responses to the STAXI-2anger expression and control scales:AX Index = AX/Out + AX/In –(AX/Con-Out + AX/Con-In) + 48

288 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 19: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

90th percentile) may be at high risk for heart attacks and coronary arterydisease, especially those who also have high anger control scores (Spielberger,1999).

The STAS, AX, and the STAXI scales and subscales have been usedextensively in research on the relationship between anger and health (Brooks,

TABLE 3Guidelines for Interpreting High Scores on the STAXI-2 Scales and Subscales

ScaleCharacteristics of persons with high scores on the STAXI-2

scales and subscales

S-Anger: Individuals with high S-Anger scores experienced intense angry feelings atthe time the test was administered.

T-Anger: Individuals high in T-Anger frequently experience angry feelings, especiallywhen they believe they are treated unfairly by others. Whether personshigh in T-Anger express, suppress, or control their anger can be inferredfrom their scores on the AX/Out, AX/In and AX/Con scales.

T-Anger/T: Persons with high T-Anger/T scores frequently experience angry feelingswith little provocation, and are often impulsive if they have high AX/Outand low AX/Con scores. Individuals with high T-Anger/T scores who alsohave high AX/Con scores may be strongly authoritarian, and use anger tointimidate others.

T-Anger/R: Persons with high T-Anger/R scores are sensitive to criticism, perceivedaffronts, and negative evaluation by others, and frequently experienceintense feelings of anger under such circumstances. Those with highAX/Out scores are more likely to express their anger.

AX/Out: Persons with high AX/Out scores frequently express anger in physicallyaggressive acts, such as assaulting other persons, or verbally express angerin the form of criticism, sarcasm, insults, and the extreme use ofprofanity.

AX/In: Persons with high AX/In scores frequently experience intense angry feelings,which they suppress or inhibit, rather than expressing these feelings inaggressive behavior. Persons with high AX/In scores who also have highAX/Out scores may frequently express their anger in some situations,while suppressing it in others.

AX/Con-Out: Persons with high AX/Con-Out scores tend to invest a great deal of energyin monitoring and preventing the outward expression of anger. Whilecontrolling external manifestations of anger is generally desirable,over-control can lead to passivity, depression, and withdrawal. Personswith high AX/Con-Out, high T-Ang, and low AX/Out scores are likely toexperience chronic anger that they are not able to express.

AX/Con-In: Persons with high AX/Con-In scores invest a great deal of time and energy inendeavoring to calm down to reduce their anger. While the development ofinternal controls over the expression of anger is generally seen in a positivelight, it can reduce the person’s ability to respond with assertive behaviorthat might facilitate constructive solutions to frustrating situations.Persons with high AX/Con-In scores who also have high AX-Out andT-Anger scores have a higher risk for developing hypertension.

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 289

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 20: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

Walfish, Stenmark, & Canger, 1981; Johnson & Broman, 1987; Schlosser,1986; Vitaliano, 1984). As previously noted, elevations in scores on theSTAXI T-Anger and anger expression and control scales have been identifiedas important risk factors for elevated blood pressure and hypertension (e.g.Johnson, Spielberger, Worden, & Jacobs, 1987; Kearns, 1985; Schneider,Egan, & Johnson, 1986; Spielberger et al., 1988; Spielberger et al., 1985; vander Ploeg, van Buuren, & van Brummelen, 1988). The STAXI has also beenused to assess the experience, expression, and control of anger in research on:patients undergoing treatment for Hodgkins disease and lung cancer(McMillan, 1984); the role of anger in Type-A behavior (Booth-Kewley &Friedman, 1987; Janisse, Edguer, & Dyck, 1986; Krasner, 1986; Spielbergeret al., 1988); psychological factors that contribute to chronic pain (Curtis,Kinder, Kalichman, & Spana, 1988; Kinder, Curtis, & Kalichman, 1986); andthe use of marijuana (Stoner, 1988). James Moses (1992, p. 524) described theSTAXI as a “specific, sensitive, psychometric instrument . . . [with] . . . greatpotential . . . to significantly further our understanding of important stress-based and stress-influenced syndromes”.

MEASURING ANXIETY, ANGER, DEPRESSION, ANDCURIOSITY WITH THE STATE-TRAIT PERSONALITY

INVENTORY (STPI)

The 80-item State-Tait Personality Inventory (STPI, Form Y) compriseseight 10-item scales for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression,and curiosity (Spielberger, Ritterband, Sydeman, Reheiser, & Unger, 1995).The 10-item STPI S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety scales comprise the items withthe best psychometric properties that were selected from the 20-item STAI-(Form Y) State and Trait Anxiety scales (see Table 1). The 10-item STPIState and Trait Anger scales include the same items as in the original STAXIS-Anger and T-Anger scales (see Table 2). The construction and validation ofthe 10-item STPI state and trait depression and curiosity scales are describedin the following sections.

Measuring State and Trait Depression with the STPISymptoms of depression vary in severity, from feeling sad or gloomy for arelatively short period of time, to deep despair, hopelessness, extreme guilt,and thoughts of death that could result in suicide. Depressed persons oftenexperience high levels of anxiety, and intense anger, which is turned inward,resulting in low self-esteem. Persistent depression can also lead to behavioraland physical symptoms such as frequent crying, fatigue, insomnia, chronicpain, and excessive gain or loss in weight (Rosenfeld, 1999). Clearly, depres-sion is a complex, multifaceted syndrome with a number of underlyingdimensions.

290 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 21: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

The State-Trait Depression Scale (STDS) was constructed to assess theintensity of feelings of depression as an emotional state and individual dif-ferences in depression as a personality trait. In developing the STDS, a poolof 40 items that described the presence or absence of depressive feelings andcognitions was adapted from four widely used measures of depression: theBeck Depression Inventory (BDI: Beck & Steer, 1987; Beck, Steer, & Brown,1996); the Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale (ZUNG: Zung, 1965, 1986);the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D: Radloff,1977); and the Depression Scale of the Multiple Affect Adjective Check List(Zuckerman & Lubin, 1985). The content and wording of each STDS itemfollowed, as closely as possible, the description of depressive thoughts andfeelings in the item from which it was adapted.

The 40 STDS items were administered to a large sample of universitystudents, who responded to each item by rating themselves on the same4-point scales that are used to assess state and trait anxiety and anger with theSTAI and STAS. Study participants first rated the intensity of their depres-sive thoughts and feelings at a particular time, and then rated how often thesemanifestations of depression were experienced. Factor analyses of responsesto the STDS items identified strong state and trait depression-present anddepression-absent factors for both males and females. In selecting the bestdepression-present (dysthymia) and depression-absent (euthymia) items forthe 10-item STPI State and Trait Depression Scales, the state and trait itemswere evaluated in separate factor analyses. Those items with the strongestloadings on the state and trait depression-present and depression-absentfactors for both sexes were included in the 5-item STPI State Dysthymia(S-Dys), State Euthymia (S-Eut), Trait Dysthymia (T-Dys), and TraitEuthymia (T-Eut) subscales.

The STPI State and Trait Depression scales and subscales are brieflydescribed in Table 4, along with guidelines for interpreting these measures.The alpha coefficients were .81 or higher for both sexes (mdn. r = .90) for the10-item STPI State and Trait Depression scales, and the 5-item State andTrait Dysthymia and Euthymia subscales. The correlations of the STPIT-Dep scale with the BDI, ZUNG, and CES-D ranged from .72 to .85 (mdn.r = .78) for both females and males, which were somewhat higher than thecorresponding correlations of these measures with the STPI S-Dep scale(mdn. r = .655). Thus, the BDI, Zung, and CES-D assess both state and traitdepression (Ritterband & Spielberger, 1996), but appear to be somewhatbetter measures of more persistent trait-like depressive characteristics.

The STPI T-Dep scale and all three widely used depression measurescorrelated highly with the STPI T-Anxiety scale, reflecting a high degree ofcomorbidity of depression with anxiety (Gotlib & Cane, 1989; Mineka,Watson, & Clark, 1998). The STPI S-Dep scale also correlated substantiallywith T-Anxiety, but to a lesser degree. In addition, the STPI T-Dep and

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 291

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 22: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

S-Dep scales and the three widely used depression measures all correlatedpositively and significantly with the STPI T-Anger scale, but these correla-tions were substantially smaller than with T-Anxiety. Significant negativecorrelations of the STPI depression scales with the STPI T-Curiosity scale

TABLE 4Brief Description of the STPI State and Trait Depression and Curiosity Scales

and Subscales, and Guidelines for Interpreting High Scores on these Measures

STPI scalesNumber of

itemsScorerange

Descriptions and guidelines for interpretingthe STPI Depression and Curiosity Scales

State-Depression(S-Dep)

10 10–40 Measures the intensity of depressive feelings andcognitions at a particular time.

Individuals with high S-Dep scores experiencedrelatively intense feelings of sadness and gloom atthe time the test was administered.

S-Dysthymia(S-Dys)

5 5–20 Measures the intensity of depressive feelings andcognitions at a particular time.

S-Euthymia(S-Eut)

5 5–20 Measures the strength of positive feelings andcognitions experienced at a particular time thatindicate the absence of depression.

Trait-Depression(T-Dep)

10 10–40 Measures how often cognitions and feelings relatingto depression are experienced over time.

Persons with high T-Depression scores experiencemore frequent and intense cognitions and feelingsof depression, which may be reflected in deepdespair and hopelessness.

T-Dysthymia(T-Dys)

5 5–20 Measures how frequently cognitions and feelingsindicating the presence of depression areexperienced over time.

T-Euthymia(T-Eut)

5 5–20 Measures how often positive cognitions and feelingsindicating the absence of depression areexperienced over time.

State-Curiosity(S-Cur)

10 10–40 Measures the intensity of feelings and cognitionsrelating to curiosity and interest in exploratorybehavior at a particular time.

Individuals with high S-Curiosity scores experiencedincreased inquisitiveness and desire to engage inexploratory behaviors at the time the test wasadministered.

Trait-Curiosity(T-Cur)

10 10–40 Measures how often feelings of curiosity,inquisitiveness, and interest in exploring theenvironment are experienced over time.

Persons high in T-Curiosity experience morefrequent interest and desire to engage inexploratory behavior in a wide variety ofsituations.

292 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 23: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

suggested that feelings of depression inhibit curiosity, which is consistent withJames’s (1890) and Freud’s (1936) observations of an antagonistic relationbetween curiosity and fear.

Measuring State and Trait Curiosity with the STPICuriosity and exploratory behavior have been linked to a variety of motiva-tional constructs, including instincts, drives, and intrinsic motivation (Cofer& Appley, 1964; Dashiell, 1925; Dollard & Miller, 1950; Harlow, 1953; Voss& Keller, 1983). While this research has been guided by diverse theoreticalperspectives and contradictory empirical findings, it is generally agreed thatcuriosity has a significant impact on thoughts, feelings, and behavior, makingthe measurement of curiosity an essential requirement in the evaluation ofemotions and personality traits.

The State-Trait Curiosity Inventory (STCI) was developed to measure theintensity of curiosity as a transitory emotional state, and individual differencesin curiosity as a relatively stable personality trait (Spielberger & Butler, 1971;Spielberger, 1979b; Spielberger, Peters, & Frain, 1981). The STCI S-Curiosityscale requires respondents to report how they feel at a particular moment; theT-Curiosity scale instructs respondents to report how they generally feel.Subjects respond to the S-Curiosity and T-Curiosity scales by rating theintensity and the frequency of occurrence of their thoughts and feelingsrelating to curiosity and exploratory behavior on the same 4-point rating scalesthat are used to assess state and trait anxiety, anger, and depression.

Factor analyses of the STCI S-Curiosity and T-Curiosity items have con-sistently identified relatively independent state and trait curiosity factors(Spielberger & Starr, 1994). High levels of S-Curiosity reflect an intense desireto seek out, explore, and understand novel characteristics of the environment.The STCI T-Curiosity scale assesses individual differences in the dispositionto experience S-Curiosity as a reaction to novel or ambiguous stimuli.Persons high in T-Curiosity feel curious more frequently and with higherlevels of intensity than those who are low in T-Curiosity. The STCI State andTrait Curiosity scales were included in the 10-item STPI Curiosity scales.These scales are briefly described in Table 4, along with guidelines for inter-preting high scores on these measures.

Guidelines for Using the STPI Scales and SubscalesThe STPI can be rapidly and easily administered and scored to measureanxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity. If a patient is depressed or experi-encing intense anxiety or anger, it is imperative for the examiner or therapistto deal immediately and directly with these feelings. Intense emotions cangreatly interfere with judgment and reality testing, and can result in injuries

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 293

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 24: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

to the patient or other persons. Feedback concerning emotional vital signscan also help patients to recognise and report relationships between theirthoughts and feelings, and the events which give rise to them, thus facilitatingthe therapeutic process. The experience of curiosity may be considered as apositive emotional vital sign. As a motivator of exploratory behavior, curi-osity often contributes to effective personal adjustment and successful adap-tation to environmental stimuli.

Since the management of anxiety, anger, and depression are major con-cerns in counseling and psychotherapy, the continuous assessment of theseemotions can facilitate the treatment process (Novaco, 1979). Barlow (1985)has also consistently emphasised the importance of utilising measures thatdifferentiate between depression and anxiety during the course of treatment.While less attention has been given to the assessment of anger, Deffenbach-er’s (1992) research clearly demonstrates that anger can be readily measuredduring treatment, and that it is important to do so. Continuing to assessanxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity as emotional vital signs during thecourse of treatment can also provide objective information regarding theeffectiveness of the treatment process.

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS

The historical background relating to theory and research on anxiety, anger,depression, and curiosity has been briefly reviewed. Darwin observed fearand rage in facial expressions, and considered these to be universal manifes-tations of anxiety and anger as adaptive products of evolution that contrib-uted to survival. Similarly, Freud regarded anxiety and aggression asuniversal human characteristics, and both he and Darwin recognised that theinteraction of anxiety and anger contributed to depression. Freud also theo-rised that the insecurity caused by anxiety often motivated curiosity andexploratory behavior.

The construction, development, and validation of the State-Trait AnxietyInventory, the State-Trait Anger EXpression Inventory, and the State-TraitPersonality Inventory to assess anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity asemotional states and personality traits have been described in detail. Anxiety,anger, and depression are important indicators of psychological distress.These negative psychological vital signs should be carefully assessed in diag-nostic evaluations, and continuously monitored in counseling and psycho-therapy. Curiosity is a positive psychological vital sign that motivatesexploratory behavior, which may contribute to resolving problems. Measur-ing these emotions as psychological vital signs was considered to be of criticalimportance in diagnosis and treatment, and for providing clients and patientswith timely feedback for linking their intense emotions to the events andexperience that gave rise to them.

294 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 25: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

The construction of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory was guided by Cat-tell’s (1966; Cattell & Scheier, 1958, 1963) conceptual distinction betweenstate and trait anxiety. Separate state and trait instructions were developed inkeeping with our intention to use these instructions with the same items toassess the intensity of anxiety as an emotional state and individual differencesin anxiety proneness as a personality trait. However, our research on itemselection indicated that different state and trait instructions could notovercome the strong linguistic connotations of key words in several items(Spielberger et al., 1970). For example, “I feel upset” and “I am worried”were good measures, respectively, of state or trait anxiety, but were not validmeasures of both constructs. Similar limitations were encountered in theselection of items for measuring state and trait anger and depression.

In developing the revised STAI(Form Y), it was noted that the content ofseveral items in the original STAI(Form X) that were adapted from otheranxiety measures were more closely related to depression than anxiety (“I feellike crying”), or described overlapping characteristics of these emotions (“Iam regretful”). Replacing these by items with content that was clearly relatedto anxiety (e.g. “I feel frightened”, “I feel nervous and restless”) resulted inimproved scale psychometric properties. Additional items describing theabsence of anxiety were also included in the revised STAI, replacing severalanxiety-present items. These low intensity specificity items increased the sen-sitivity and range of measurement for the STAI state and trait anxiety scales.Items describing positive feelings indicating the absence of depression werealso included in the STPI, increasing the range and sensitivity of the Depres-sion scales.

The wording of each STAI, STAXI, and STPI item for measuring stateanxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity reflects different levels of the inten-sity of these emotional states. For example, in the measurement of anger,“furious” indicates a high level of item-intensity specificity, whereas“annoyed” reflects a much lower level. It was considered desirable to includeitems with varying degrees of item-intensity specificity in the STPI scales. Indeveloping the STPI, it was also considered important to eliminate items thatdescribed psychosomatic symptoms, which might be due to physical injury ormedical conditions such as cancer or heart disease.

In summary, a number of important points that were recognised in devel-oping the STAI, STAXI, and STPI are described in this paper. Theseincluded: (1) Taking the State–Trait distinction into account in developingmeasures of emotional states and personality traits; (2) Recognising that thewording of some items may have state or trait connotations that cannot beameliorated with instructions; (3) Items varying in intensity specificity mustbe included to assess the wide range in the intensity and frequency of emo-tional experience; and (4) Items indicating positive feelings or the absence ofnegative emotions should be included for assessing the full range of an

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 295

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 26: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

emotion. Finally, it was noted that physiological symptoms should not beincluded in measures of emotion since these might be due to unrelatedmedical conditions such as heart disease or cancer.

Newman, Ciarlo, and Carpenter (1999) have proposed detailed guidelinesfor psychological tests that are used in diagnosis, treatment, and outcomeassessment. These guidelines include clear instructions for administration andscoring, good psychometric properties, usable norms, and a history of suc-cessful use in research and clinical practice. The STAI, STAXI, and STPIappear to meet the criteria proposed by Newman et al. (1999). These mea-sures have been used extensively with normal adolescents and adults, anddiverse patient populations. The instructions for administration are brief andeasy to follow, and the scoring procedures are objective and relatively unin-fluenced by examiner bias. All three inventories have excellent psychometricproperties and yield results that can contribute to diagnosis, treatmentplanning, and outcome assessment.

REFERENCES

Alexander, F.G., & French, T.M. (Eds.) (1948). Studies in psychosomatic medicine: Anapproach to the cause and treatment of vegetative disturbances. New York: Ronald.

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual (DSM-IV)(4th edn., revised). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Aronoff, J. (1962). Freud’s conception of the origin of curiosity. Journal of Psychol-ogy, 54, 39–45.

Barlow, D.H. (1985). The dimensions of anxiety disorders. In A.H. Tuma & J.D.Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 479–500). Hillsdale, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Beck, A.T., & Steer, R.A. (1987). Beck Depression Inventory: Manual. New York:Psychological Corporation.

Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., & Brown, G.K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory: ManualBDI-II. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Berlyne, D.E. (1949). Interest as a psychological concept. British Journal of Psychol-ogy, 39, 184–185.

Berlyne, D.E. (1950). Novelty and curiosity as determinants of exploratory behavior.British Journal of Psychology, 41, 50–68.

Berlyne, D.E. (1954). A theory of human curiosity. British Journal of Psychology, 45,180–191.

Berlyne, D.E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.Beswick, D. (2000). (URL) www.beswick.inf/psychres/curiosityintro.htmBiaggio, M.K., Supplee, K.., & Curtis, N. (1981). Reliability and validity of four anger

scales. Journal of Personality Assessment, 45, 639–648.Booth-Kewley, S., & Friedman, H.S. (1987). Psychological predictors of heart disease:

A quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 343–362.Brooks, M.L., Walfish, S., Stenmark, D.E., & Canger, J.M. (1981). Personality vari-

ables in alcohol abuse in college students. Journal of Drug Education, 11, 185–189.

296 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 27: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

Buss, A.H. (1961). The psychology of aggression. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Buss, A.H., & Durkee, A. (1957). An inventory for assessing different kinds of

hostility. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 343–349.Cattell, R.B. (1966). Patterns of change: Measurement in relation to state-dimension,

trait change, lability, and process concepts. In R.B. Cattell (Ed.), Handbook ofmultivariate experimental psychology (pp. 1905–1998). Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.

Cattell, R.B., & Scheier, I.H. (1958). The nature of anxiety: A review of thirteenmultivariate analyses comprising 814 variables. Psychological Reports, 4, 351–388.

Cattell, R.B., & Scheier, I.H. (1963). Handbook for the IPAT Anxiety Scale (2nd edn.).Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.

Cofer, C.N., & Appley, M.H. (1964). Motivation: Theory and research. New York:John Wiley and Sons, Inc.

Cook, W.W., & Medley, D.M. (1954). Proposed hostility and pharisaic-virtue scalesfor the MMPI. Journal of Applied Psychology, 38, 414–418.

Crane, R.S. (1981). The role of anger, hostility, and aggression in essential hyperten-sion. (Doctoral dissertation, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, 1981)Dissertation Abstracts International, 42, 2982B.

Cronbach, L.J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests.Psychometrika, 16, 297–335.

Curtis, G., Kinder, B., Kalichman, S., & Spana, R. (1988). Affective differencesamong subgroups of chronic pain patients. Anxiety Research: An InternationalJournal, 1, 65–73.

Darwin, C. (1965 version, original publication 1872). The expression of emotions inman and animals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Dashiell, J.F. (1925). A quantitative demonstration of animal drive. Journal ofComparative and Physiological Psychology, 5, 205–208.

Deffenbacher, J.L. (1992). Trait anger: Theory, findings, and implications. In C.D.Spielberger & J.N. Butcher (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 9, pp.177–201). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Dembroski, T.M., MacDougall, J.M., Williams, R.B., & Haney, T.L. (1985). Com-ponents of Type A, hostility, and anger-in: Relationship to angiographic findings.Psychosomatic Medicine, 47, 219–233.

Dollard, J., & Miller, N.E. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy. New York:McGraw-Hill.

Eysenck, H.J., & Eysenck, S.B.G. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Ques-tionnaire. London: Hodder and Stroughton.

Forgays, D.G., Forgays, D.K., & Spielberger, C.D. (1997). Factor structure of theState-Trait Anger Expression Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment. 69,497–507.

Freud, S. (1924). Collected papers (Vol. 1). London: Hogarth Press.Freud, S. (1936). The problem of anxiety. New York: W.W. Norton.Freud, S. (1959). Why war? In J. Strachey (Ed.), Collected papers (Vol. 5). London:

Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1933.)Friedman, M., & Rosenman, R.H. (1974). Type A behavior and your heart. Green-

wich, CN: Fawcett Publications.

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 297

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 28: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

Funkenstein, D.H., King, S.H., & Drolette, M.E. (1954). The direction of angerduring a laboratory stress-inducing situation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 16, 404–413.

Fuqua, D.R., Leonard, E., Masters, M.A., Smith, R.J., Campbell, J.L., & Fischer,P.C. (1991). A structural analysis of the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory(STAXI). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51, 439–446.

Gaudry, E., Spielberger, C.D., & Vagg, P.R. (1975). Validation of the state–traitdistinction in anxiety research. Multivariate Behavior Research, 10, 331–341.

Gentry, W.D., Chesney, A.P., Gary, H.G., Hall, R.P., & Harburg, E. (1982). Habitualanger-coping styles: I. Effect on mean blood pressure and risk for essential hyper-tension. Psychosomatic Medicine, 44, 195–202.

Gotlib, I.H., & Crane, D.B. (1989). Self-report assessment of depression and anxiety,In P.C. Kendall & D. Watson (Eds.), Anxiety and depression: Distinctive andoverlapping features. Personality, psychopathology, and psychotherapy (pp. 131–169). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Harburg, E., Blakelock, E.H., & Roeper, P.J. (1979). Resentful and reflective copingwith arbitrary authority and blood pressure: Detroit. Psychosomatic Medicine, 3,189–202.

Harlow, H.F. (1953). Motivation as a factor in new responses. In M.R. Jones (Ed.),Current theory and research in motivation (pp. 24–49). Lincoln, NE: University ofNebraska Press.

Jacobs, G.A., Latham, L.E., & Brown, M.S. (1988). Test–retest reliability of theState-Trait Personality Inventory and the Anger Expression Scale. AnxietyResearch, 1, 263–265.

Jackson, S.W. (1986). Melancholia and depression: From Hippocratic times to moderntimes. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Jackson, S.W. (1995). A history of melancholia and depression. In G.H. Pollock &H.M. Visotsky (Eds.), Depression and Stress, 1, 3–42.

James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt.Janisse, M.P., Edguer, N., & Dyck, D.G. (1986). Type A behavior, anger expression,

and reactions to anger imagery. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 371–385.Johnson, E.H. (1984). Anger and anxiety as determinants of elevated blood pressure

in adolescents. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of South Florida,Tampa.

Johnson, E.H., & Broman, C.L. (1987). The relationship of anger expression to healthproblems among Black Americans in a national survey. Journal of BehavioralMedicine, 10, 103–169.

Johnson, E., Spielberger, C., Worden, T., & Jacobs, G. (1987). Emotional and familialdeterminants of elevated blood pressure in black and white adolescent males.Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 31, 287–300.

Kearns, W.D. (1985). A laboratory study of the relationship of mode of anger expres-sion to blood pressure. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of South Florida,Tampa.

Kinder, B., Curtis, G., & Kalichman, S. (1986). Anxiety and anger as predictors ofMMPI elevations in chronic pain patients. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50,651–661.

298 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 29: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

Knight, R.G., Chisholm, B.J., Paulin, J.M., & Waal-Manning, H.J. (1988). TheSpielberger Anger Expression Scale: Some psychometric data. Journal of ClinicalPsychology, 27, 279–281.

Krasner, S.S. (1986). Anger, anger control, and the coronary prone behavior pattern.Unpublished master’s thesis, University of South Florida, Tampa.

Loewenstein, G. (1994). The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation.Psychological Bulletin, 116, 75–98.

McDougall, W. (1921). An introduction to social psychology, Boston, MA: Luc.McMillan, S.C. (1984). A comparison of levels of anxiety and anger experienced by 2

groups of cancer patients during therapy for Hodgkin’s disease and small cell lungcancer. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of South Florida, Tampa.

Matthews, K.A., Glass, D.C., Rosenman, R.H., & Bortner, R.W. (1977). Competitivedrive, pattern A, and coronary heart disease: A further analysis of some data fromthe Western Collaborative Group Study. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 30, 489–498.

May, R. (1977 version, original publication 1950). The meaning of anxiety. New York:W.W. Norton.

Mineka, S., Watson, D., & Clark, L.A. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolarmood disorders. Annual Review of Psychology. 49, 377–412.

Moses, J.A. (1992). State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory, research edition. In D.J.Keyser & R.C. Sweetland (Eds.), Test critiques (Vol. IX, pp. 510–525). Austin, TX:PRO-ED.

Newman, F.L., Ciarlo, J.A., & Carpenter, D. (1999). Guidelines for selecting psy-chological instruments for treatment planning and outcome assessment. In M.E.Maruish (Ed.), The use of psychological testing for treatment planning andoutcomes assessment (Vol. 2, pp. 153–170). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumAssociates.

Novaco, R.W. (1979). The cognitive regulation of anger and stress. In P.C. Kendall &S.D. Hollon (Eds.), Cognitive behavioral interventions, theory, research, and proce-dures (pp. 241–285). New York: Academic Press.

Plutchik, R. (2001). The nature of emotions. American Scientist, July–August, 89.Pollans, C.H. (1983). The psychometric properties and factor structure of the Anger

EXpression (AX) Scale. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of South Florida,Tampa.

Radloff, L.S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research inthe general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.

Ritterband, L.M., & Spielberger, C.D. (1996). Construct validity of the Beck Depres-sion Inventory as a measure of state and trait depression in nonclinical popula-tions. Depression and Stress, 2(2), 123–145.

Rosenfeld, I. (1999). When the sadness won’t go away. Parade Magazine, 19 Septem-ber, 10.

Schlosser, M.B. (1986, August). Anger, crying, and health among females. Paperpresented at the 94th annual convention of the American Psychological Associa-tion, Washington, DC.

Schneider, R.H., Egan, B., & Johnson, E.H. (1986). Anger and anxiety in borderlinehypertension. Psychosomatic Medicine, 48, 242–248.

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 299

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 30: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

Schultz, S.D. (1954). A differentiation of several forms of hostility by scales empiri-cally constructed from significant items on the MMPI. Dissertation Abstracts, 17,717–720.

Sharkin, B.S. (1988). Treatment of client anger in counseling. Journal of Counselingand Development, 66, 361–365.

Spielberger, C.D. (1966). Theory and research on anxiety. In C.D. Spielberger (Ed.),Anxiety and behavior (pp. 3–20). New York: Academic Press.

Spielberger, C.D. (1972a). Anxiety as an emotional state. In C.D. Spielberger (Ed.),Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research (Vol. 1, pp. 24–49). New York:Academic Press.

Spielberger, C.D. (1972b). Current trends in theory and research on anxiety. In C.D.Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research (Vol. 1, pp. 3–19).New York: Academic Press.

Spielberger, C.D. (1973). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children.Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Spielberger, C.D. (1976). Stress and anxiety and cardiovascular disease. Journal of theSouth Carolina Medical Association, Suppl., 15, 72, 15–22.

Spielberger, C.D. (1977). Anxiety: Theory and research. In B.B. Wolman (Ed.),International Encyclopedia of Neurology, Psychiatry, Psychoanalysis, and Psychol-ogy (pp. 81–84). New York: Human Sciences Press.

Spielberger, C.D. (1979a). Understanding stress and anxiety. London: Harper andRow.

Spielberger, C.D. (1979b). Preliminary manual for the State-Trait Personality Inven-tory (STPI). Unpublished manuscript, University of South Florida, Tampa.

Spielberger, C.D. (1980). Preliminary manual for the State-Trait Anger Scale (STAS).Tampa, FL: University of South Florida, Human Resources Institute.

Spielberger, C.D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: STAI(FormY). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Spielberger, C.D. (1988). Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory(STAXI). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Spielberger, C.D. (1989). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: A comprehensive bibliography(2nd edn.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Spielberger, C.D. (1999). Professional manual for the State-Trait Anger ExpressionInventory-2 (STAXI-2). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources,.

Spielberger, C.D., & Butler, T.F. (1971). On the relationship between anxiety, curi-osity, and arousal: A working paper. Unpublished manuscript, Florida State Uni-versity, Tallahassee.

Spielberger, C.D., & Gorsuch, R.L. (1966). The development of the State-TraitAnxiety Inventory. In C.D. Spielberger & R.L. Gorsuch, Mediating processes inverbal conditioning. Final report to the National Institutes of Health, US PublicHealth Service on Grants MH-7229, MH-7446, and HD-947.

Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., & Lushene, R.D. (1970). STAI: Manual for theState-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Spielberger, C.D., Jacobs, G., Russell, S., & Crane, R. (1983). Assessment of anger:The State-Trait Anger Scale. In J.N. Butcher & C.D. Spielberger (Eds.), Advancesin personality assessment (Vol. 2, pp. 159–187). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.

300 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 31: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

Spielberger, C.D., Johnson, E.H., Russell, S.F., Crane, R.J., Jacobs, G.A., & Worden,T.J. (1985). The experience and expression of anger: Construction and validationof an anger expression scale. In M.A. Chesney & R.H. Rosenman (Eds.), Angerand hostility in cardiovascular and behavioral disorders (pp. 5–30). New York:Hemisphere/McGraw-Hill.

Spielberger, C.D., Krasner, S.S., & Solomon, E.P. (1988). The experience, expressionand control of anger. In M.P. Janisse (Ed.), Health psychology: Individual differ-ences and stress (pp. 89–108). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Spielberger, C.D., & London, P. (1982). Rage boomerangs: Lethal Type-A anger.American Health, 1, 52–56.

Spielberger, C.D., Peters, R.A., & Frain, F.J. (1981). Curiosity and anxiety. In H.G.Voss & H. Keller (Eds.), Curiosity research: Basic concepts and results (pp. 197–225). Weinheim, FRG: Beltz.

Spielberger, C.D., Reheiser, E.C., & Sydeman, S.J. (1995). Measuring the experi-ence, expression, and control of anger. In H. Kassinove (Ed.), Anger disorders:Definitions, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 49–76). Washington, DC: Taylor &Francis.

Spielberger, C.D., Ritterband, L.M., Sydeman, S.J., Reheiser, E.C., & Unger, K.K.(1995). Assessment of emotional states and personality traits: Measuring psycho-logical vital signs. In J.N. Butcher (Ed.), Clinical personality assessment: Practicalapproaches (pp. 42–58). New York: Oxford University Press.

Spielberger, C.D., & Starr, L.M. (1994). Curiosity and exploratory behavior. In H.F.O’Neil, Jr. & M. Drillings (Eds.), Motivation: Theory and research (pp. 221–243).Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Spielberger, C.D., Vagg, P.R., Barker, L.R., Donham, G.W., & Westberry, L.G.(1980). The factor structure of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. In I.G. Sarason& C.D. Spielberger (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 7, pp. 95–109). Washington,DC: Hemisphere.

Stoner, S.B. (1988). Undergraduate marijuana use and anger. Journal of Psychology,122, 343–347.

Story, D., & Deffenbacher, J.L. (1985, April). General anger and personality. Paperpresented at Rocky Mountain Psychological Association, Tucson, AZ.

Taylor, J.A. (1953). A personality scale of manifest anxiety. Journal of AbnormalSocial Psychology, 48, 285–290.

Titchener, E.B. (1879). An outline of psychology. New York: Macmillan.van der Ploeg, H.M. (1988). The factor structure of the State-Trait Anger Scale.

Psychological Reports, 63, 978.van der Ploeg, H.M., Van Buuren, E.T., & Van Brummelen, P. (1988). The role of

anger in hypertension. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 43, 186–193.Vitaliano, P.P. (1984). Identification and intervention with students at high risk for

distress in medical school. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wash-ington, Seattle.

Voss, H.G., & Keller, H. (1983). Curiosity and exploration: Theories and results. NewYork: Academic Press.

WHO (2001). (URL) www.who.int/mental_health/Topic_Depression/depression1.htm

ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONS 301

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.

Page 32: Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, … · Inventory (STAXI), and the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) for measuring state and trait anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity,

WHO (2006). (URL) www.who.int/en/Section1174/Section1199/Section1567/Section1826_8103.htm

Williams, R.B., Haney, T.L., Lee, K.L., Kong, Y., Blumenthal, J., & Whalen, R.E.(1980). Type A behavior, hostility, and coronary atherosclerosis. PsychosomaticMedicine, 42, 539–549.

Wundt, W. (1896). Outlines of psychology. New York: Dustav E. Stechert.Zuckerman, M., & Lubin, B. (1985). Manual for the Multiple Affect Adjective Check-

list. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.Zung, W.W.K. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of General Psychiatry,

12, 63–70.Zung, W.W.K. (1986). Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale and Depression Status

Inventory. In N. Sartorious & T.A. Ban (Eds.), Assessment of depression (pp.221–231). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

302 SPIELBERGER AND REHEISER

© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2009 International Association of AppliedPsychology.