misattribution of external speech in patients with hallucinations and delusions

11
Misattribution of external speech in patients with hallucinations and delusions Paul P. Allen * , Louise C. Johns, Cynthia H.Y. Fu, Matthew R. Broome, Goparlen N. Vythelingum, Philip K. McGuire Section of Neuroimaging, Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, P.O. Box 67, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK Received 1 August 2003; accepted 24 September 2003 Available online 26 November 2003 Abstract Background: One of the main cognitive models of positive symptoms in schizophrenia proposes that they arise through impaired self-monitoring. This is supported by evidence of behavioural deficits on tasks designed to engage self-monitoring, but these deficits could also result from an externalising response bias. We examined whether patients with hallucinations and delusions would demonstrate an externalising bias on a task that did not involve cognitive self-monitoring. Method: Participants passively listened (without speaking) to recordings of single adjectives spoken in their own and another person’s voice, and made self/nonself judgements about their source. The acoustic quality of recorded speech was experimentally manipulated by altering the pitch. Fifteen patients with schizophrenia who were currently experiencing hallucinations and delusions, 13 patients with schizophrenia not experiencing current hallucinations and delusions and 15 healthy controls were compared. Results: When listening to distorted words, patients with hallucinations and delusions were more likely than both the group with no hallucinations and delusions and the control group to misidentify their own speech as alien (i.e. spoken by someone else). Across the combined patient groups, the tendency to misidentify self-generated speech as alien was positively correlated with current severity of hallucinations but not with ratings of delusions or positive symptoms in general. Conclusions: These findings indicate that patients with hallucinations and delusions are prone to misidentifying their own verbal material as alien in a task which does not involve cognitive self-monitoring. This suggests that these symptoms are related to an externalising bias in the processing of sensory material, and not solely a function of defective self-monitoring. D 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Speech; Hallucination; Delusion 1. Introduction Auditory verbal hallucinations (AVHs) are a key feature of schizophrenia (Slade and Bentall, 1988) and are usually associated with delusions (Liddle, 1987). Most cognitive models of AVHs suggest that internally generated thoughts or images are mistaken for exter- nally generated events as a consequence of a malfunc- tion in either a central monitor (Hoffmann, 1986; Frith and Done, 1988) or in a reality discrimination process (Bentall, 1990). Reality or source monitoring tasks require participants to distinguish between memories of their self-generated material and externally generat- 0920-9964/$ - see front matter D 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.008 * Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (P.P. Allen). www.elsevier.com/locate/schres Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277 – 287

Upload: paul-p-allen

Post on 30-Nov-2016

216 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

www.elsevier.com/locate/schres

Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277–287

Misattribution of external speech in patients with hallucinations

and delusions

Paul P. Allen*, Louise C. Johns, Cynthia H.Y. Fu, Matthew R. Broome,Goparlen N. Vythelingum, Philip K. McGuire

Section of Neuroimaging, Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, P.O. Box 67, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK

Received 1 August 2003; accepted 24 September 2003

Available online 26 November 2003

Abstract

Background: One of the main cognitive models of positive symptoms in schizophrenia proposes that they arise through

impaired self-monitoring. This is supported by evidence of behavioural deficits on tasks designed to engage self-monitoring, but

these deficits could also result from an externalising response bias. We examined whether patients with hallucinations and

delusions would demonstrate an externalising bias on a task that did not involve cognitive self-monitoring.Method: Participants

passively listened (without speaking) to recordings of single adjectives spoken in their own and another person’s voice, and made

self/nonself judgements about their source. The acoustic quality of recorded speech was experimentally manipulated by altering

the pitch. Fifteen patients with schizophrenia who were currently experiencing hallucinations and delusions, 13 patients with

schizophrenia not experiencing current hallucinations and delusions and 15 healthy controls were compared. Results: When

listening to distorted words, patients with hallucinations and delusions were more likely than both the group with no

hallucinations and delusions and the control group to misidentify their own speech as alien (i.e. spoken by someone else). Across

the combined patient groups, the tendency to misidentify self-generated speech as alien was positively correlated with current

severity of hallucinations but not with ratings of delusions or positive symptoms in general. Conclusions: These findings indicate

that patients with hallucinations and delusions are prone to misidentifying their own verbal material as alien in a task which does

not involve cognitive self-monitoring. This suggests that these symptoms are related to an externalising bias in the processing of

sensory material, and not solely a function of defective self-monitoring.

D 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Speech; Hallucination; Delusion

1. Introduction Most cognitive models of AVHs suggest that internally

Auditory verbal hallucinations (AVHs) are a key

feature of schizophrenia (Slade and Bentall, 1988) and

are usually associated with delusions (Liddle, 1987).

0920-9964/$ - see front matter D 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.008

* Corresponding author.

E-mail address: [email protected] (P.P. Allen).

generated thoughts or images are mistaken for exter-

nally generated events as a consequence of a malfunc-

tion in either a central monitor (Hoffmann, 1986; Frith

and Done, 1988) or in a reality discrimination process

(Bentall, 1990). Reality or source monitoring tasks

require participants to distinguish between memories

of their self-generated material and externally generat-

P.P. Allen et al. / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277–287278

ed events. Patients who experience hallucinations are

more likely than controls and nonhallucinating patients

to misattribute self-generated items to an external

source (Bental et al., 1991; Morrison and Haddock,

1997). Furthermore, these source-monitoring errors

occur more often when the material is emotional as

opposed to neutral.

However, these tasks require participants to identify

the source of verbal material some time after presenta-

tion or generation. ‘Immediate’ source-monitoring

models propose that AVHs result from impaired moni-

toring of intended speech. Frith and Done’s model

proposes that AVHs and delusions of control result from

a breakdown in the awareness of self-generated action.

The theory is specific to current or the immediate

perception of all motor actions including the genera-

tion of speech. To distinguish between self-generated

and externally generated actions, we rely on a ‘feed

forward’ signal of our intentions to an internal monitor

(Blakemore et al., 2002). According to this model,

hallucinations and other positive symptoms can be

conceptualised as resulting from a breakdown in the

systemsmonitoring the current intention tomake actions

(including the generation of inner speech). Consequent-

ly, self-generated inner speech is misidentified as ‘alien’

and perceived as externally generated ‘voices’.

According to Frith’s model, self-monitoring applies

to all thoughts and motor actions (including verbal

actions) and impairments should effect the monitoring

of overt speech as well as inner speech. This model is

supported by data from studies that have engaged

verbal self-monitoring by experimentally manipulat-

ing auditory verbal feedback while patients spoke

aloud (Cahill et al., 1996; Johns et al., 2001). Altering

the acoustic characteristics of participants’ speech (by

distorting it) introduced a disparity between the

expected and perceived sound of the speech. Partic-

ipants were required to make self/nonself judgements

about the source of speech that was fed back to them.

In both studies, patients with schizophrenia who were

experiencing hallucinations and delusions were more

likely than controls to make errors of misattribution

(misidentify their own speech as alien) when it was

distorted. Moreover, they made more identification

errors when the words they read were derogatory as

opposed to neutral or positive (Johns et al., 2001).

While all these findings are consistent with Frith’s

model, they could equally be explained by an external-

ising response bias (Bentall, 1990; Brebion et al.,

2000) independent of impairments in verbal self-mon-

itoring. A response bias is apparent when participants

fail to recognise the source of a word or a thought and

misattribute it to an external speaker.

The aim of the present study was to measure

response bias independent of self-monitoring. Spe-

cifically, we examined whether the misidentification

of speech described by Cahill et al. (1996) and Johns

et al. (2001) would still be evident if their paradigm

was modified such that participants did not generate

speech, but made auditory judgements about the

source of recorded speech. Thus, participants pas-

sively listened to recordings of their own and an-

other person’s previously recorded speech, with the

speech distorted on a proportion of the trials to

increase uncertainty as to its source. The rationale

for the use of alien speech in the design of this study

was to test whether or not the hypothesised response

bias was specific to the subjects own speech, as

opposed to a general perceptual or voice discrimi-

nation failure that would affect both self-generated

and external speech.

We tested the hypothesis that, even in the absence

of a self-monitoring component, patients with halluci-

nations and delusions would still demonstrate a sig-

nificant externalising response bias.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

2.1.1. Patient groups

All patients were recruited through the South

London and Maudsley NHS Trust, met DSM-IV

criteria for schizophrenia and were on regular stable

doses of antipsychotic medication (as assessed from

the patient’s medication charts). Medication com-

prised of both typical and atypical antipsychotics.

Patients’ symptoms were assessed on the day of

testing using the Scale for the Assessment of Positive

Symptoms (SAPS; Andreason, 1984), the Scale for

Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS; Andrea-

son, 1984) and the Calgary Depression Scale (CDSS).

Reports of symptoms were corroborated with medical

notes. IQ was estimated using the NART 2nd edition

(Nelson and O’Connell, 1978).

nia Research 69 (2004) 277–287 279

2.1.2. Patients with hallucinations and delusions

Patients (n = 15) who were currently experiencing

auditory verbal hallucinations on a regular basis and

scored at least 3 on the SAPS hallucinations global

score. All of these patients had experienced halluci-

nations in the last month although none reported

experiencing hallucinations during the task. This

group also had other psychotic symptoms, particularly

delusions (see Table 1).

2.1.3. Patients not currently experiencing hallucina-

tions and delusions

Patients (n = 13) who were not currently experi-

encing hallucinations, as defined by a score of zero on

SAPS hallucinations global score. Their mean SAPS

global score for delusions was also significantly lower

than for the currently ill patients (Table 1). Eleven of

this group were outpatients and two were inpatients

awaiting discharge. All of these patients had a diag-

P.P. Allen et al. / Schizophre

Table 1

Group mean (standard deviations) for patient characteristics

Symptom ratings Patient groups

No hallucinations/

delusions

(N= 13)

Hallucinations

and delusions

(N= 15)

Analysis

(Mann–

Whitney

U-test)

Age of onset (age in

years at first

admission for

psychosis)

24.1 (5.0) 23.9 (3.8) ns

Duration of illness

(years since first

admission)

12.9 (12.1) 8.3 (9.6) ns

Auditory verbal

hallucinations*

0 3.8 (0.7) U = 0.00;

p= 0.00

Non-auditory

hallucinations*

0 2.1(1.9) U = 58;

p= 0.02

Delusions (global

scores)*

1.8 (1.6) 4.0 (1.3) U = 57;

p= 0.02

Other positive

symptomsa*

0.8 (1.1) 0.9 (1.0) ns

Negative symptomsb* 2.0 (1.4) 2.9 (1.13) ns

Attentional problems

(global score)

1.1 (1.4) 1.4(1.9) ns

CDSS total score 3.4 (4.6) 4.9 (3.4) ns

a Mean of global scores for bizarre behaviour and formal

thought disorder.b Mean of global scores for alogia, anhedonia, inappropriate

affect, avolation and affective flattening.

*SAPS/SANS mean scores.

nosis of schizophrenia and had experienced past

psychotic episodes.

The ratio of typical to atypical antipsychotic med-

ication for the two patient groups was similar: 9:6 for

the Psychotic group and 8:7 for patients not experi-

encing hallucinations and delusions. Typical antipsy-

chotic medication included Haloperidol, Sulpiride,

Chlorpomazine, Depixol and Procyclidine. Atypical

medication included Clozapine, Olanzapine, Respiri-

done and Amisulperide.

2.1.4. Healthy volunteer (n=15)

Healthy volunteers with no history of psychiatric

illness. Volunteers were recruited to match the pa-

tients with respect to age, gender and pre-morbid IQ

(Table 2).

After reading a complete description of the study all

participants gave written informed consent to partici-

pate in the study, which was approved by the local

research ethical committee.

2.2. Materials

2.2.1. Word lists

Six lists of 32 personal adjectives were used (see

Appendix A). Three of the six lists were given to each

subject. These were randomly selected so that each

subject was given any three of the six possible lists.

The words on the lists were adjectives applicable to

people and were chosen because they were relevant to

the typical personal and emotive content of AVHs in

schizophrenia (Juninger and Frame, 1985). According

to Nayani and David (1996), voices experienced by

patients often have a sense of personal intimacy and

contain words of vilification and abuse. Although

AVHs often consist of phrases or sentences, previous

work on self-monitoring in patients with hallucinations

involved single adjectives, with emotional valence

varying with the nature of the word. The word lists

in the present study were intended to be as comparable

as possible with those used in these studies, and were

compiled from lists of adjectives used by Johns et al.

(2001) and McGuire et al. (1996b). The words com-

prised of negative, neutral and positive adjectives, as

defined by the above studies (see Johns et al., 2001).

The ratio with respect to emotional valence was 11

positive:11 negative:10 neutral in each of the six lists.

To assess the subjective rating of valence, after com-

Table 2

Participants’ mean age, mean NART (standard deviations) and gender ratio

Controls (N = 15) No hallucinations/delusions

(N = 13)

Hallucinations/delusions

(N= 15)

Analysis

Age in years 33.1 (10.45) 34.7 (8.71) 32.9 (9.78) F = 0.82; p= 0.37

NART 112 (7.11) 108 (6.13) 105 (8.8) F = 0.193; p= 0.19

Gender ratio M/F 13:2 12:1 13:2

P.P. Allen et al. / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277–287280

pletion of the task, participants rated each of the words

that had been presented on a scale of � 3 to + 3.

Words were considered negative if their mean rating

was between � 3 and � 1, neutral between � 0.9 and

+ 1 and positive between + 1.1 and + 3.

2.2.2. Auditory stimuli

Recorded speech was distorted by a DSP.FX digital

effects processor (Windows 95), which lowered the

pitch by 4 semitones (for words in the distorted

condition). This degree of pitch shift was chosen

because it made the speaker’s voice harder to recog-

nise without making the word incomprehensible.

Members of staff at the Institute of Psychiatry (one

male and one female) recorded the words for the

nonself condition (48 words in total). The rationale

for the use of alien speech in the design of this study is

to test whether or not the hypothesised response bias is

specific to external misattributions (i.e. self to other

misattributions) as opposed to a general perceptual or

voice discrimination failure. Purpose-built software

(programmed in visual basic V4) was used to present

the stimuli and to record the response of the subject.

2.3. Design

There were two sources of speech (self, alien) and

two levels of distortion (0, � 4 semitones). Thus, each

list of 32 words consisted of eight self, eight self distor-

ted, eight alien and eight alien distorted words. Three

lists were given to each subject making a total of 96

trials (24 for each experimental condition). The order of

conditions was randomised across the set of six lists.

2.3.1. Independent variables

Speech source (self, alien), distortion level (0, � 4

semitones), word type (negative, neutral, positive),

subject group (controls, patients not experiencing

hallucinations and delusions, patients with hallucina-

tions and delusions).

2.3.2. Dependent variables

Errors in identifying word source and the reaction

time for a response. This was therefore a mixed within

and between subjects repeated measures design.

2.4. Procedure

Participants were informed upon recruitment that

the experiment would be conducted over two ses-

sions. It was explained by the researcher that the first

session would be used to record their speech and the

second session would be used to administer the task.

2.4.1. First session

The participants were asked to read all 96 words

even though half would subsequently be presented

to them in an alien voice. This particular measure

was undertaken to ensure that participants could not

make judgements based on source information

when subsequently presented with the recorded

words. Participants were not instructed to try and

remember the words they had read. Thus, the task

relied on perceptual discrimination as opposed to

source memory.

Before the second session, the designated words on

each list were pitch shifted by � 4 semitones or

replaced with the alien version of the word. Care

was taken to ensure that the volume levels of both

the participants’ speech and the alien speech were as

close as possible to ensure that differential volume

could not subsequently be used to discriminate be-

tween the two types of speech. Three play lists (one for

each word list) were then constructed on the computer

for use in the task itself.

2.4.2. Second session

This took place 1–2 days after the first session.

This interval was the same for all participants. The

subject sat opposite the experimenter across a desk

on which the computer was placed. The computer

P.P. Allen et al. / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277–287 281

monitor could be clearly seen by the participants,

who sat approximately 30–40 cm from the com-

puter monitor and fixated on a central cross. To

minimise articulation of the words by participants

no words were presented on the computer screen

(only through the headphones). Participants wore

headphones and the volume was checked to ensure

that it was at a level sufficient for them to hear the

speech without difficulty. A button box connected

to the computer was placed in front of the subject.

Participants did not generate any speech and were

told to listen carefully to the stimuli presented in

the headphones. If they thought the speech they

heard was their own they were to press the button

marked ‘self’. If they were unsure of its source

they were to press the button marked ‘unsure’, and

if they thought the speech belonged to someone

else they were asked to press a button marked

‘other’. Participants were asked to make responses

as quickly as possible and not to talk during the

task.

After hearing each word participants had 4.5 s to

respond. The computer recorded the response

choice and the reaction time. When participants

made their response an ‘S’ ‘U’ or ‘O’ (for ‘self’/

‘unsure’/‘other’) appeared on the computer monitor

to confirm that the participants’ response choice

had been logged by the computer (this helped the

experimenter to assess if participants were under-

taking the task properly). There was 14 s between

the presentations of each word (a long ISI was

chosen so that the task could be used in a func-

tional magnetic resonance imaging study at a later

date). The total testing time was 40–45 min.

All participants successfully completed the three

word lists. None of the patients reported experiencing

hallucinations while attempting to complete the task.

The assessment of psychopathology and IQ testing

were carried out after completion of the task. The

entire second session took approximately 1 h for

controls and 1.5 h for patients (mainly due to the

SAPS/SANS interview).

2.4.3. Hypotheses

(1) Patients with hallucinations and delusions will

make more misattribution errors when identifying

their own distorted speech than the patients not

experiencing hallucinations and delusions and the

control group.

(2) Patients with hallucinations and delusions will be

more prone than the other groups to making errors

when the words are emotional (as opposed to

neutral).

2.4.4. Data analysis

The data were analysed using Excel V7 (prelimi-

nary analysis) and Statistical Package for Social Sci-

ence (SPSS).

2.4.5. Analysis of variance

Arcsine transformations were performed so that all

error data met the assumption of normal distribution.

Arcsine transformation is recommended for propor-

tional data (Howell, 1992). The transformed data were

analysed using an ANOVA for repeated measures. The

within-subjects factors were source of speech (self,

alien), distortion (0, � 4 semitones) and emotional

valence (negative, neutral, positive). The between

subjects factor was group (controls, patients not expe-

riencing hallucinations and delusions, patients with

hallucinations and delusions). Separate ANOVAs were

conducted with total errors, unsure responses, misat-

tribution errors and reaction times as the dependent

variable. Misattribution errors occurred when partic-

ipants misattributed the source of the speech (i.e.

responding with ‘other’ when the source of the speech

was self and vice versa), as opposed to errors when

participants responded with ‘unsure’ or made a null

response.

3. Results

3.1. Errors

The groups were compared in terms of (a) total

errors (unsure +misattributed responses), (b) unsure

responses and (c) misattribution errors. The mean

proportions of correct, unsure and misattributed

responses are shown in Tables 3 and 4.

3.1.1. Total errors

There was a significant main effect of source of

speech (F = 9.71 df = 1, 40 p = 0.003). Participants

generally made more errors during the self-trials than

Table 3

Mean untransformed proportions (standard deviations) for correct

responses, unsure responses and misattribution errors according to

condition and group

Self Self

Distorted

Alien Alien

Distorted

Controls

Correct response 0.92

(0.10)

0.33

(0.24)

0.84

(0.22)

0.62

(0.25)

Unsure responses 0.04

(0.06)

0.34

(0.3)

0.03

(0.06)

0.13

(0.12)

Misattributions 0.03

(0.05)

0.31

(0.29)

0.12

(0.21)

0.19

(0.27)

No hallucinations and delusions

Correct response 0.88

(0.16)

0.37

(0.24)

0.62

(0.18)

0.53

(0.23)

Unsure responses 0.05

(0.08)

0.35

(0.19)

0.18

(0.10)

0.31

(0.15)

Misattributions 0.04

(0.08)

0.24

(0.25)

0.16

(0.13)

0.11

(0.12)

Hallucinations and delusions patients

Correct Response 0.75

(0.28)

0.10

(0.08)

0.68

(0.32)

0.75

(0.28)

Unsure responses 0.06

(0.04)

0.14

(0.25)

0.16

(0.17)

0.14

(0.23)

Misattributions 0.14

(0.22)

0.68

(0.27)

0.15

(0.28)

0.10

(0.45)

Null responses are not included.

Table 4

Significant main effects and interactions from the ANOVA relating

to hypothesised group differences

ANOVA analysis factors F Significance ( p)

Total errors

Source 9.71 0.003

Source� group 7.17 0.002

Distortion 216.88 < 0.001

Valence 6.34 0.002

Source� distortion 36.52 < 0.001

Unsure responses

Distortion 33.68 < 0.001

Distortion� group 4.34 0.002

Misattribution errors

Source 11.02 0.002

Group 3.80 0.031

Source� group 6.97 0.002

Distortion 36.24 < 0.001

Source� distortion 35.52 < 0.001

Source� distortion� group 3.27 0.04

Reaction times

Source 4.47 0.04

Distortion 5.53 0.02

Distortion� group 5.53 0.02

Response (correct/error) 4.21 0.04

P.P. Allen et al. / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277–287282

they did during alien trials. There was also a significant

interaction between source of speech and group

(F = 7.17 df = 2, 40, p = 0.002). Patients with halluci-

nations and delusions made more errors on self-trials

than did control (t =� 3.71 df = 28, p = 0.01) and

patients not experiencing hallucinations and delusions

(t= 3.32 df = 26, p = 0.003).

There was a significant main effect of distortion

(F = 216.88 df = 2, 40, p < 0.001). All participants

made more errors when the words were distorted than

when they were not. There was a significant interac-

tion between distortion and source of speech

(F = 36.52 df = 1, 43, p < 0.001). When the words were

distorted, participants made significantly more errors

with words that they had recorded (self) than with

words spoken by someone else (alien). The interaction

between source of speech, distortion and group was

nonsignificant (F = 2.67 df= 2, 40, p = 0.08). Patients

with hallucinations and delusions did not make signif-

icantly more total errors than other groups in the self-

distorted condition.

There was a significant main effect for emotional

valence (F = 6.34 df = 2, 80, p = 0.002). Participants

generally made more total errors when the word

type was negative than they did when it was neutral

or positive. The interaction between valence and

group was nonsignificant ( F = 1.32 df = 4, 80,

p = 0.26).

3.1.2. Unsure response

The main effect for the source of speech was

nonsignificant (F = 0.23 df = 1, 40, p = 0.63). There

was a significant main effect for distortion (F = 33.68

df = 2, 40, p < 0.001); participants made more unsure

responses when the speech was distorted. There was

also a significant interaction between distortion and

group (F = 4.34 df = 2, 40, p = 0.002). The increase in

unsure responses when words were distorted was

significantly greater for controls than it was for

patients with hallucinations and delusions (t = 2.16,

Table 5

Mean reaction times (in milliseconds + standard deviation) for

correct and errors trials (standard deviation) by source condition

Self

correct

Self

error

Alien

correct

Alien

error

Controls 531

(58)

884

(100)

784

(94)

1021

(183)

No hallucinations

and delusions

667

(99)

974

(136)

1074

(405)

1158

(112)

Hallucinations

and delusions

945

(180)

772

(124)

1078

(145)

1075

(225)

P.P. Allen et al. / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277–287 283

p = 0.04). The comparison between the two patient

groups was nonsignificant (t = 1.22, p = 0.20). The

interaction between source of speech, distortion and

group was nonsignificant ( F = 0.55 df = 2, 40,

p = 0.58).

3.1.3. Misattribution errors

There was a significant main effect for source of

speech (F = 11.02 df = 1, 40, p = 0.002); participants

made significantly more misattribution errors when

hearing their own speech. There was a significant

main effect of distortion (F = 36.24 df = 1, 40,

p < 0.001). For all participants, misattribution errors

were significantly more common when the words

were distorted. There was a significant between-

subjects effect for group (F = 3.80 df = 1, 40,

p = 0.031). Also, there was a significant interaction

between source of speech, distortion and group

(F = 3.27 df = 2, 40, p = 0.04). Table 3 showed that

in the self-distorted condition patients with halluci-

nations and delusions made more misattribution

errors (selecting ‘other’ when hearing their own

voice) than both controls (t =� 3.6, p= 0.001) and

patients not experiencing hallucinations and delu-

sions (t =� 4.2, p < 0.001). There were no signifi-

cant variations between the groups in the other

three conditions (self undistorted, alien undistorted

or alien distorted). We predicted that the proportion

of errors that were misattributions as opposed to

unsure responses would be greater in the patients

with hallucinations and delusions. To test this the

proportion of misattribution errors was calculated

for only those trials on which participants had

made errors. Thus the types of errors made by

participants could be examined. The data was

analysed using a one-way ANOVA. There was a

significant difference between the groups when

listening to self distorted speech (F = 5.11 df = 2,

40, p = 0.01): 75% of errors made by patients with

hallucinations and delusions were misattribution

errors compared to 39% for patients not experienc-

ing hallucinations and delusions and 46% for con-

trols. The main effects and interactions for valance

were nonsignificant.

A bivariate correlational analysis was performed

to examine the association between misattribution

errors and symptom ratings other than hallucination

scores in the patient groups. Neither SAPS global

delusions score (r = 0.27 p = 0.20) nor SAPS total

scores (r = 0.27 p = 0.17) were significantly correlat-

ed with misattribution errors in the self-distorted

condition. As would be expected, SAPS global

hallucination scores were positively correlated with

errors in the self-distorted condition (r= 0.52 p =

0.009). There were no significant correlations be-

tween SAPS hallucination scores and any of the

other stimuli conditions.

3.2. Reaction times

To assess if task accuracy across groups was related

to response speed, mean reaction times were calculated

for correct and error trials and are shown in Table 5

(mean, as opposed to median, reaction times were used

as the data were not skewed). There was no main effect

of group across conditions ( F = 0.83 df = 1, 40,

p= 0.92). Patients were not generally slower or faster

than controls. For all participants, there was a main

effect for source of speech (F = 4.47 df = 1, 40,

p= 0.04): reaction times were significantly faster for

the participants’ own speech. There was also a signif-

icant main effect for distortion (F = 5.53 df= 1, 40,

p = 0.02): reaction times were significantly slower

when the words were distorted. There was an interac-

tion between distortion and group (F = 9.18 df = 2, 40,

p = 0.01). Controls had significantly faster reaction

times during nondistorted trials than both the patient

groups. Conversely they were significantly slower to

respond than both patient groups when the words were

distorted.

Reaction times for correct responses were signifi-

cantly faster than those for incorrect responses

(F = 4.21, df = 1, 40, p = 0.04). A multiple compari-

P.P. Allen et al. / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277–287284

sons test revealed that correct responses were signif-

icantly faster than unsure responses ( p < 0.001). How-

ever, the difference in reaction times between correct

responses and misattribution errors was nonsignificant

( p = 0.33). There was a trend for patients with hallu-

cinations and delusions to make incorrect decisions

faster than controls when responding in the self-

condition. However, this interaction just failed to

reach significance (F = 2.92 df = 1, 40, p = 0.06).

4. Discussion

This study examined whether the link between

hallucinations and delusions in schizophrenia and

the tendency to make external misattributions was

related to defective self-monitoring or alternatively,

could be explained in terms of an externalising re-

sponse bias.

Participants made judgements about the source

of pre-recorded speech, which was either their own

or somebody else’s, and was either distorted or

undistorted. The results indicated that patients with

schizophrenia currently experiencing hallucinations

and delusions were more likely to make external

misattributions about the source of their own dis-

torted speech than controls and patients not expe-

riencing these symptoms. This distinction between

patients with hallucinations and delusions and the

other groups was specific to the effect of distortion

on self-generated words, and was specific to errors

of attribution (i.e. misidentification of self-generated

speech as alien), as opposed to being unsure.

Patients with hallucinations and delusions did make

more total errors than the other groups for self

generated words; however, this was not specific to

the effects of distortion. The tendency to misattrib-

ute distorted self-generated speech to an external

source was particularly associated with hallucina-

tions as opposed to delusions or positive symptoms

in general.

Since the paradigm did not involve participants

generating verbal material, performance of the task

did not require verbal self-monitoring (although we

cannot exclude the possibility that subjects may

have been covertly generating the words as they

heard them). Cognitive models in which defective

self-monitoring is central to AVHs (Frith, 1987,

1992) would therefore predict that there would be

no difference (or less difference) between patients

with hallucinations and controls on this task. How-

ever, we found that patients with hallucinations and

delusions were prone to making external misattri-

butions about their own distorted speech. Indeed,

the results resembled those previously evident when

patients with hallucinations and delusions were

studied using the original version of the paradigm

in which participants read the words aloud (Cahill

et al., 1996; Johns et al., 2001). The present data

indicate that impaired self-monitoring cannot entire-

ly account for the tendency for patients with

hallucinations and delusions to make external attri-

butions, suggesting that another factor, such as an

externalising bias when processing unusual percep-

tual information (Bentall, 1990), might be operative.

A deficit in top-down processing which effected the

interpretation of auditory stimuli would produce

similar effects whether patients were evaluating

words that they spoke ‘on-line’ or that they were

listening to.

In all participants, the greatest proportion of total

errors was seen for negative words, although partic-

ipants were also prone to making more errors when

the words they heard were positive (rather than

neutral). This is consistent with previous studies

which have demonstrated that both psychiatric and

nonpsychiatric participants make more source moni-

toring errors with emotional rather than neutral mate-

rial (Morrison and Haddock, 1997). However, even

though patients with current hallucinations and delu-

sions tended to make more errors than controls and

patients not experiencing these symptoms when the

word type was negative this trend was nonsignificant.

This may have been due to the small group sizes:

previous research involving larger groups has found

that patients with current psychotic symptoms are

particularly likely to make more errors with negative

material, especially when the feedback is ambiguous

(Johns et al., 2001).

Analysis of the participants’ reaction times indi-

cated that all participants were significantly faster

when making correct as opposed to incorrect

responses. This trend was due to slower reaction

times for unsure responses rather than misattribution

errors. However, there was a tendency (just missing

significance) for patients with hallucinations and

P.P. Allen et al. / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277–287 285

delusions to respond more quickly than controls when

they made errors in the self-condition. Interestingly,

controls had the fastest reaction times in the non-

distorted condition but the slowest reaction times

when the speech was distorted, taking longer than

both patient groups (due to control participants’

tendency to make unsure responses in this condition).

The reaction times for the patient groups were similar

in both distortion conditions. Thus, when presented

with ambiguous stimuli, patients with schizophrenia

were faster at making judgements (whether correct or

incorrect) about the source of the speech than con-

trols. People with delusions may make probabilistic

judgements faster than healthy volunteers because

they ‘jump to conclusions’ (Hemsley, 1988). As all

our patients had delusions, it is possible that their

faster reaction times reflected the use of a similar

reasoning style in making judgements about the

source of ambiguous sensory stimuli (Fleminger,

1994).

As well as differing with respect to the severity

of delusions, patients not experiencing hallucina-

tions and delusions had lower levels of positive

symptoms in general than the currently ill patients,

raising the possibility that differences in task per-

formance were a function of the overall severity of

illness at the time of testing. Although the tendency

to misattribute self-generated speech was not corre-

lated with the severity of psychotic symptoms, this

might be better clarified by repeating the study with

patients matched more closely on the severity of

their illness. For example, patients could be

grouped according to specific symptoms (i.e.

patients with both hallucinations and delusions

compared to a group with delusions but not hallu-

cinations). When a group of subjects with delusions

but no hallucinations were studied using the ‘on-line’

version of the task (in which subjects did articulate the

words), their performance regarding misattribution

errors was intermediate between patients with both

hallucinations and delusions and a control group (Johns

et al., 2001).

Two other possibilities should be discussed regard-

ing the interpretation of these results as an external-

ising bias specific to patients with current psychotic

symptoms. Firstly, it is possible that the results reflect a

tendency of an individual to claim that an item was

presented from a specific source when they are actu-

ally unsure about its origin (Snodgrass and Corwin,

1988). In other words, when subjects were uncertain

regarding the source of the speech and ‘made a guess’

they may have been bias to respond with an external

attribution. However, in the present study, in addition

to the ‘self’ and ‘other’ responses, subjects were also

able to register an ‘unsure’ response. Thus, when

participants were in doubt about the source of the

speech, they were not obliged to make a forced choice

between ‘self’ and ‘other’, making it more likely that

when they did select either of these responses they did

so with some degree of confidence.

The results could also be interpreted in terms of

a more general problem with voice discrimination,

rather than a specific externalising bias. However,

one would assume that if patients had problems

with voice discrimination (as oppose to source

attribution) they would be more likely to make

‘unsure’ responses. However, this was not the case:

patients with hallucinations and delusions made

more misattribution errors than the other groups

but fewer unsure responses.

Moreover, one would expect a problem with

voice discrimination to have similar effects on self

and alien speech, but patients with hallucinations

and delusions were particularly likely to make

misattributions when it was their own speech (as

opposed to alien speech) that was distorted. This is

reflected in the currently ill patients’ increased

accuracy in the alien-distorted condition. This can

be considered an artefact of an externalising re-

sponse bias (i.e. if patients tend to answer ‘other’

when confronted with ambiguous stimuli they will

inevitably be correct when the speech is indeed that

of another). Finally, when presented with unambig-

uous speech (i.e. undistorted speech) patients with

hallucinations and delusions made correct responses

well above chance. Therefore, we believe that the

findings do not provide evidence for a voice

discrimination failure.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to The Wellcome Trust Travelling

Fellowship awarded to Cynthia H.Y. Fu and The

Medical Research Council UK Studentship to Paul P.

Allen.

d e f

careful (AD) brainy (A) scruffy (S)

hot (AD) unbiased (SD) blamed (SD)

Appendix (continued)

P.P. Allen et al. / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277–287286

Appendix. Adjectives used in the task

S = self undistorted word; SD= self distorted word;

A= alien undistorted word; AD= alien distorted word.

a b c

corrupt (A) gorgeous (AD) important (SD)

silent (AD) passable (SD) bold (A)

fabulous (S) gracious (S) stained (AD)

regular (S) stinking (A) precious (AD)

perplexed (SD) harmless (AD) shady (AD)

contaminated (S) helpless (S) attractive (S)

divine (SD) bruised (A) private (A)

imperfect (S) rapid (A) unfeeling (S)

worthless (A) unremarkable (S) vulgar (A)

nervous (AD) holy (A) local (AD)

creepy (AD) heartless (S) confused (SD)

tragic (SD) deranged (SD) tainted (A)

playful (A) selfish (SD) practical (S)

informal (SD) deplorable (AD) watchful (SD)

caring (AD) funny (SD) obnoxious (S)

balanced (A) evil (AD) defective (SD)

expert (A) placid (S) vague (A)

ordinary (S) suspicious (SD) oppressive (SD)

incredible (SD) lying (AD) splendid (AD)

slender (AD) despicable (A) punished (S)

fake (A) grubby (A) scabby (A)

painful (AD) horrid (AD) forlorn (AD)

unfortunate (S) famous (AD) usual (S)

tired (A) impartial (SD) fearful (AD)

conventional (SD) dynamic (S) informed (AD)

beloved (AD) miserable (SD) lighter (SD)

remarkable (S) regal (A) brilliant (S)

sorrowful (S) broad (AD) tormented (SD)

retarded (SD) pathetic (S) faithful (SD)

shy (A) loving (SD) hopeless (S)

sharp (SD) hateful (S) foolish (A)

dismal (AD) prepared (A) pure (A)

d e f

inoffensive (S) deviant (SD) chatty (AD)

loud (AD) crude (A) hurtful (A)

decayed (A) refined (SD) straight (SD)

revolting (SD) witty (AD) damaged (SD)

crooked (AD) knowing (A) distressed (S)

healthy (S) spiteful (S) vile (A)

infected (S) still (S) observant (S)

lazy (A) dreary (SD) selfless (A)

silly (S) dull (AD) massive (A)

repulsive (A) talkative (SD) crass (AD)

average (A) aware (AD) honourable (S)

sick (SD) sneaky (AD) elegant (SD)

familiar (SD) cowardly (SD) wretched (SD)

thoughtful (A) failed (A) charming (AD)

contented (SD) friendly (S) faulty (AD)

loathed (S) agitated (SD) afraid (A)

clumsy (SD) civil (AD) wakeful (S)

conscious (A) beaten (A) cherished (SD)

careless (AD) kind (S) wicked (AD)

truthful (A) innocuous (S) relaxed (AD)

ill (AD) sinister (SD) suitable (SD)

messy (A) odd (AD) handsome (S)

desirable (SD) impoverished (S) involved (A)

nasty (A) loyal (SD) clean (AD)

rejected (S) old (A) filthy (A)

wise (AD) proud (A) harassed (AD)

perverted (SD) good (AD) nauseating (S)

common (SD) fat (A) unkind (SD)

undamaged (S) slimy (AD) nice (A)

fantastic (A) awkward (SD) shabby (S)

References

Andreason, N.C., 1984. Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symp-

toms (SAPS) and Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symp-

toms (SANS). The University of Iowa, Iowa.

Bentall, R.P., 1990. The illusion of reality: a psychological model of

hallucinations. Psychological Bulletin 107, 82–95.

Bentall, R.P., Baker, G.A., Havers, S., 1991. Reality monitoring and

psychotic hallucinations. Br. J. Clin. Psychology 30, 213–222.

Blakemore, S.J., Wolpert, D.M., Frith, C.D., 2002. Abnormalities

in the awareness of action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6,

237–242.

Brebion, G., Amador, X., David, A., Malaspina, D., Sharif, Z.,

Gorman, J., 2000. Positive symptomology and source-monitor-

ing failure in schizophrenia—an analysis of symptom specific

effects. Psychiatry Research 95, 119–131.

Cahill, C., Silbersweig, D., Frith, C.D., 1996. Psychotic experiences

induced in deluded patients using distorted auditory feedback.

Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1, 201–211.

Fleminger, S., 1994. Top-down preconscious perceptual processing

and delusional misidentification in neuropsychiatric disorders.

In: David, A.S., Cutting, J.C. (Eds.), The Neuropsychology of

Schizophrenia. Psychology Press, UK, pp. 361–381.

Frith, C.D., 1987. The positive and negative symptoms of schizo-

phrenia reflect impairments in the perception and initiation of

action. Psychological Medicine 17, 631–648.

Frith, C.D., 1992. The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophre-

nia. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Hove.

Frith, C.D., Done, C.J., 1988. Towards a neuropsychology of schiz-

ophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 153, 437–443.

Hemsley, D.R., 1988. Psychological models of schizophrenia. In:

Miller, E., Cooper, P. (Eds.), Adult Abnormal Psychology.

Churchill Livingstone, London, pp. 101–127.

Hoffmann, R.E., 1986. Verbal hallucinations and language produc-

tion processes in schizophrenia. The Behavioral and Brain Sci-

ences 9, 503–548.

P.P. Allen et al. / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 277–287 287

Howell, D.C., 1992. Statistical Methods for Psychologists, 3rd ed.

Duxburry Press, Belmont, CA.

Johns, L.C., Rossell, S., Frith, C., Ahmad, F., Hemsley, D., Kuipers,

E., McGuire, P.K., 2001. Verbal self-monitoring and auditory

verbal hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia. Psycholog-

ical Medicine 31, 705–715.

Juninger, J., Frame, C.L., 1985. Self report of the frequency and

phenomenology of verbal hallucinations. Journal of Nervous

and Mental Diseases 73, 149–155.

Liddle, P.F., 1987. Schizophrenic syndromes, cognitive perform-

ance and neurological dysfunction. Psychological Medicine

17, 49–57.

McGuire, P.K., Silbersweig, D.A., Frith, C.D., 1996b. Functional

neuroanatomy of verbal self-monitoring. Brain 119, 907–917.

Morrison, A.P., Haddock, G., 1997. Cognitive factors in source

monitoring and auditory hallucinations. Psychological Medicine

27, 669–679.

Nayani, T.H., David, S., 1996. The auditory hallucination:

a phenomenological survey. Psychological Medicine 26,

177–189.

Nelson, H.E., O’Connell, A., 1978. Dementia: the estimation of

premorbid intelligence levels using the new adult reading test.

Cortex 14, 234–244.

Slade, P.D., Bentall, R.P., 1988. Sensory Deception: Towards a

Scientific Analysis of Hallucinations. Croom Helm, London.

Snodgrass, J.G., Corwin, J., 1988. Pragmatics of measuring recog-

nition memory: application to dementia and amnesia. Journal of

Experimental Psychology. General 117, 34–50.