increasing response rates to mail questionnaires: a review of inducement strategies

13
This article was downloaded by: [University of Prince Edward Island] On: 15 November 2014, At: 19:57 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK American Journal of Health Education Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ujhe20 Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies Keith A. King PhD, CHES a , Lisa N. Pealer PhD a & Amy L. Bernard PhD, CHES a a Health Promotion in the Health Promotion and Education Program , University of Cincinnati , ML 0002, 526 TC, Cincinnati , OH , 45221-0002 , USA Published online: 24 Feb 2013. To cite this article: Keith A. King PhD, CHES , Lisa N. Pealer PhD & Amy L. Bernard PhD, CHES (2001) Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies, American Journal of Health Education, 32:1, 4-15, DOI: 10.1080/19325037.2001.10609392 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19325037.2001.10609392 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http:// www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Upload: amy-l

Post on 17-Mar-2017

213 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

This article was downloaded by: [University of Prince Edward Island]On: 15 November 2014, At: 19:57Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

American Journal of Health EducationPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ujhe20

Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: AReview of Inducement StrategiesKeith A. King PhD, CHES a , Lisa N. Pealer PhD a & Amy L. Bernard PhD, CHES aa Health Promotion in the Health Promotion and Education Program , University ofCincinnati , ML 0002, 526 TC, Cincinnati , OH , 45221-0002 , USAPublished online: 24 Feb 2013.

To cite this article: Keith A. King PhD, CHES , Lisa N. Pealer PhD & Amy L. Bernard PhD, CHES (2001) Increasing ResponseRates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies, American Journal of Health Education, 32:1, 4-15, DOI:10.1080/19325037.2001.10609392

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19325037.2001.10609392

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) containedin the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of theContent. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, andare not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon andshould be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable forany losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoeveror howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use ofthe Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematicreproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in anyform to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Page 2: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

Keith A. King, Lisa N. Pealer, and Amy L. Bernard

AeSTRACT

Health education researchers frequently use mail questionnaires to collect data. One major limitation to mail survey research is nonresponse bias. W h e n the nonresponse rate is high, external validity is compromised. A number of studies have examined the efect of inducement strategies on response rate. Such studies have resulted in muck discrepancy and uncertainty as to the strategies that consistently increase response rate. This article provides survey researchers with efective inducement strategies based on a comprehensive review of the professional literature. Five areas of consideration regarding mail survey research are examined: (1) envelopes and postage, (2) cover letter, ( 3 ) incentives, (4) questionnaire characteristics, and (5) participant contact. The use of this information should strengthen mail survey research and minimize nonresponse bias.

Health surveys are a valuable source of information and a common method of research and evaluation in public health and health education. Mail survey research provides information for health care policy makers, health education and public health officials, and health consumers interested in planning, implementing, and evaluating health promotion programs, policies, and services. Mail surveys are generally an inexpensive survey technique, and if well designed can be simple, clear, and reach a large population (Aday, 1996; McDermott & Sarvela, 1999; Torabi, 1991). However, it is nearly impossible to receive a response from all participants in a sample, and nonresponse bias is a critical limitation to mail survey research. Nonetheless, error due to nonresponse in mail survey research can be offset by techniques designed to

increase participation (Fowler, 1984; Isaac & Michael, 1995; Rossi, Wright, & Anderson, 1983).

Nonresponse bias is the potential difference between those who respond to a specific survey and those who do not respond (Clausen & Ford 1947; Filion, 1975; Mangione, 1995; Worthen & Valcarce, 1985). External validity of research findings (the extent to which similar research findings can be expected in different populations) is compromised when the nonresponse rate of a study is high (Dodd & Markwiese, 1988). Research has shown that rejecting study results with response rates lower than 50% may be justifiable (Dolsen & Machlis, 1991). Therefore, researchers have examined various ele- ments critical to maximizing response rates. Unfortunately, however, the professional

literature and the common press report inconsistent findings regarding inducement strategies, which creates confusion as to what truly works for health education survey researchers.

In general, when conducting mail survey research consideration should be given to the following five areas: (1) enve- lopes and postage, ( 2 ) cover letter, (3) incentives, (4) questionnaire characteristics, and (5) participant contact. The purpose of this article is to provide survey researchers with effective inducement strategies based

Keith A. King, PhD, CHES; Lisa N. Pealer, PhD; and Amy L. Bernard, PhD, CHES are assistant professors of health promotion in the Health Promotion and Education Program, University of Cincinnati, M L 0002, 526 TC, Cincinnati, OH 45221 -0002; E-mail: [email protected].

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 3: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

on a comprehensive review of the pro- fessional literature. It is hoped that such information will strengthen mail sur- vey research by minimizing nonresponse bias, thus increasing the statistical power and external validity of studies. Recom- mendations for further research are also offered.

Envelopes and Postage An issue often overlooked in survey

research is that the targeted individual must first be motivated to open the envelope con- taining the cover letter and questionnaire.

with making decisions on the appearance of outgoing envelopes and postage, as well as whether to include a self-addressed, stamped, return envelope (Table I).

Outgoing Envelope and Postage Studies have found that personalization

of outgoing envelopes is important in increasing response rates (Kahle & Sales, 1978). One strategy used to personalize the outgoing envelope is to print the individual’s name on the envelope as opposed to using a blind respondent label (e.g., “respondent,” “occupant”). Dignan,

(1994) determined that outgoing envelopes using a person’s name and address yielded a significantly higher response rate than outgoing envelopes using“resident” or “occupant.” Similarly, Nederhof ( 1983a) found that using follow-up envelopes with personalized handwritten address labels led to a 6.5% greater response rate than using follow-up envelopes with personalized computer-printed labels. Regarding outgo- ing postage, certified mail, though more costly, has been shown to increase response rate by 16% over first-class postage (Del Valle, Morgenstern, Rogstad, Albright, & -

Survey researchers are therefore presented Michielutte, Jones-Lighty, and Bahnson Vichy , 1997).

T m 1. Envelope and Postage Inducement Strategies that Increase Response Rates

inducement Strategy

Comparison Group

Study % Increase of Response Rate

Outgoing Envelope Personalized typed Preprinted address labels Kahle & Sales ( 1 978) name and addresses “Resident” addressed labels Dignan et al. ( 1994)

1 o.oa -1-a

Outgoing Postage First-class postage

Certified mail First class postage

2nd, 3rd or bulk rate postage Metered postage

Fox et al. ( 1 988)a Fox et al. ( 1 988lb Del Valle et al. ( 1 997)

1.8 No difference

1 6.5a

Return Postage First-class postage No postage

Business reply postage

Business reply postage No postage Metered postage No postage Commemorative stamps No postage

Outgoing stamp different from return stamp

Regular paper stamps Outgoing stamp same as the return stamp

Choi et al. ( 1 990) Armstrong & Lusk ( 1 987ld Armstrong & Lusk ( 1 987)‘ Fox et al. ( 1 988)‘ Harris & Guffey ( 1 978) Choi et al. ( 1 990) Choi et al. ( 1 990) Choi et al. ( 1 990) Choi et at. ( 1 990) Hensley ( I 974)

1 3.8a 3.0 9.2a 6.2a 6.Ia 7.0a

1 1 .3a 1 7.3a 3.5 10.0”

Race-specific stamps Use of general stamps Price et al. ( 1996) , ?o difference

Follow-Up Envelopes Personalized Personalized computer labels Nederhoff 1 1 983a) handwritten addresses

6.5”

’p I .05 bResults based on meta-analysis of 5 studies ‘Results based on meta-analysis of 4 studies dResults based on meta-analysis of 6 studies ‘Results based on meta-analysis of 16 studies fResults based on meta-analysis of 9 studies 4- = Percentage increase not provided

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 4: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

Return Envelope and Postage Inclusion of a self-addressed, postage-

paid return envelope has been observed to increase response rates (Harris & Guffey, 1978; Yammarino, Skinner, & Childers, 1991). Stamped mail tends to yield higher response rates than metered or business reply mail (Fox, Crask, & Kim, 1988). The use of first-class postage on the return envelope has been found to yield a 9% greater increase in return rate than business reply postage (Armstrong & Lusk, 1987). Choi, Pak, and Purdham (1990) compared different types of stamps on return envelopes (i.e., no stamp, business-reply stamp, metered stamp, small regular stamp, and large commemorative stamp) and found that the use of paper stamps, especially large commemorative stamps, on return envelopes increased return rate and reduced response time. This finding tends to highlight the importance of envelope appearance. Respondents may perceive commemorative stamps to be indicators of increased time and consideration spent by researchers in developing the study. Thus, through the use of commemorative stamps, a more positive impression of the researcher may be gained, resulting in an increased response rate. Price, Easton, Kandakai, and Oden (1996) found that race-specific stamps on return envelopes did not increase the response rate of Afri- can-American women. Since some women may have immediately discarded the sur- veys prior to seeing the race-specific stamp, placement of such a stamp on the outgoing envelope may have increased response rate. Regardless of specific stamp type, research indicates that there is a greater response rate when the stamp on the outgoing envelope and the stamp on the return envelope are the same (Hensley, 1974). However, the pairing of a commemorative stamp on the outgoing envelope and meter postage on the return envelope yields a higher response rate than all other types of pairings.

Cover Letter Following the envelope, the survey cover

letter is often the next contact with poten-

tial participants and therefore is responsible for convincing participants to respond. The cover letter has been identified as an effec- tive strategy to elicit high response rates (Alreck & Settle, 1995; Dodd & Markwiese, 1986; Duncan, 1979; Forsgren, 1989; Foxet al., 1988; Gendall, Hoek, & Esslemont, 1995; Henley, 1976; Linsky, 1975; O’Rourke & O’Rourke, 1983; Wagner & O’Toole, 1985). Elements of the cover letter that continue to receive much attention and debate regarding their effect on response rate include sponsorship; personalization; type of appeal; notification of deadline; as surances of anonymity and/or confidenti- ality; signature; and postscript (Table 11). Sponsorship

Respondents are more likely to respond to questionnaires that they consider presti- gious (Brunner & Carroll, 1969; Houston & Nevin, 1977; Fox et al., 1988; Jones & Lang, 1980; Jones & Linda, 1978). The ef- fect of survey sponsorship on response rates has been widely examined. Among those sponsors studied (university, governmental, or commercial/private industry), university sponsorship has been found to yield the greatest increase in response rates, followed in order by governmental agency and com- mercial/private industry (Jones & Linda, 1978). Recognition of university or govern- ment sponsorship may increase respon- dents’ perceived importance of the study and abate feelings of marketing ploys.

Personalization A cover letter is considered personalized

when name and address individually iden- tify each participant and the salutation greets the person by name (Alreck & Settle, 1995; Carpenter, 1974; Forsgren, 1989). Research on the effect of personalization on response rates has been inconsistent and therefore somewhat disconcerting because a considerable investment might be required to personalize cover letters. Most reports find that a personal salutation does not significantly increase response rates when compared with an impersonal greeting (Forsgren, 1989; Kanuk & Berenson, 1975; Worthen &Valcarce, 1985).

However, typed salutations have been found to produce greater response rates than handwritten salutations (Moss & Worthen, 1991; Simon, 1967).

On the other hand, overpersonalization of the cover letter, such as inclusion of a picture of the researcher, has been shown to decrease response rate, because in- creased personalization may threaten ano- nymity or assurances of confidentiality (Rucker, Hughes, Thompson, Harrison, & Vanderlip, 1984). Linksy (1975) suggests considering the basis for a personal relation- ship with participants, and if questionnaire content requires maximum anonymity personalization might be contraindicated.

Type of Appeal Cover letter appeal, or message, has

been manipulated to determine influence on response rate. The types of appeals studied include the egoistic appeal and the altruistic appeal. The egoistic appeal emphasizes the importance of the participant’s opinions by including statements such as “your opinions are important to us” and “you can expect to be better served as a result” in the cover letter. Gendall et al. (1995) suggest the egoistic appeal poses the survey as an opportunity for participants to express their opinions. Altruistic appeals ask participants for their help (help-the-researcher or help-the-sponsor) and include statements such as “we need your help” in the cover letter. Altruistic appeals might also empha- size the benefits of the research to the par- ticipants’ population (social utility) in the cover letter by including statements such as “The information you provide will contrib- ute to understanding more about ...” (Champion & Sear, 1969; Forsgren, 1989; Gendall et al., 1995; Jones & Linda, 1978). An altruistic appeal has not proven effective in increasing response rate, yet the egoistic cover letter is likely to produce a higher response rate (Champion & Sear, 1969; Forsgren, 1989; Jones & Linda, 1978; O’Rourke & O’Rourke, 1983).

Researchers carefully note that “the effectiveness of any given appeal may be

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 5: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

situation specific” and that effectiveness of- ten is related to survey sponsorship (Duncan, 1979, p. 45; Gendall et al., 1995; Houston & Nevin, 1977; Jones & Linda, 1978).

Notification of Deadline No consistent evidence exists to confirm

that the inclusion of a deadline in the cover letter will increase response rate in mail

surveys (Duncan, 1979; Foxet al., 1988; Henley, 1976; O’Rourke & O’Rourke, 1983). However, compared with cover letters with no inclusion of a deadline, those with a deadline will likely yield a higher response rate in early responses (Duncan, 1979; Ferriss, 1951; Henley, 1976; O’Rourke & O’Rourke, 1983; Roberts, Mcrory, &

Forthofer, 1978). After the deadline has passed, fewer questionnaires will be returned. If no deadline is provided, researchers can anticipate a slower return rate, but will continue to receive question- naires for a longer time period, eventually equal to the response rate of a questionnaire that included a deadline in the cover letter

Inducement Strategy Comparison Study % increase of Group Response Rate

Sponsorship University Private agency Brunner & Carroll ( 1 969) 26 4”

10 0 6 3

Jones & Linda ( 1 978) Jones & Lang ( 1 980)

Government agency Jones & Linda [ 1978) 5 7 Government Private agency Jones & Linda ( I 978) 4 3

’ Typed Salutatfon Handwritten Moss & Worthen (1991) I2 0” Personalb Less personal type face Carpenter ( 1 974) 7 0”

Personalization

Personalb Impersonal form lettef Kanuk & Berenson [ 1975)” 7 0 Longworth ( 1 953) 5 0

Handwritten signature Facsimile Dodd & Markwiese [ 1986) 8 0 Kanuk & Berenson / 1 975ja

Personalized/hand signature Impersonal/facsimile signature Worthen & Valcarce ( 1 985) 4 8 Clausen & Ford ( I 947)

No details available

No difference

Type of Appeal Altruistic appeal Egoistic

Egoistic appeal Altruistic

Jones & Lang ( 1 980) Jones & Linda ( I 978) Gendall et al. [I 995) Champion & Sear ( I 969)

4.0 No difference

No details available” 3.6”

Notification of Deadline Deadline Expired deadline Ferriss / 1 95 1 ) 18.1 Dead I i ne No deadline given Roberts et al. ( 1 978) 24.0”

Henley ( 1 976) 7.6“

Confidentiality Anonymity Groves et al. ( 1 997) 4.4

Mason et al. / 196 I ) No difference Rosen ( 1 960) No difference

Maheux et al. ( 1 989) Kanuk & Berenson ( 1 9751”

*p 5.05 aResults based on a meta analysis of 4 experimental studies examining handwritten postscript versus no postscript. hPersonalized cover letters address participants by name, “Dear Mr. Smith” “mpersonal form letters address participants in a generic format, “Dear Ohioan.”

Handwritten postscript No postscript 8.8*

No details available

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 6: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

(Ferris, 1951; O’Rourke & O’Rourke, 1983; Roberts et al., 1978).

Anonymity and Confidentiality Research has examined whether anony-

mous or confidential surveys yield higher response rates. Groves, Price, Olsson, and King (1997) reported no significant differ- ence in return rates between the two groups, a finding supported in the literature (Mason, Dressel, & Bain, 1961; Rosen, 1960). Singer, von Thurn, and Miller (1995) found that to improve response rates for questionnaires containing sensitive items, a strong assurance of confidentiality was needed. However, if the questionnaire items are not sensitive, strong assurances of confidentiality may encourage skepticism and mistrustfulness in participants (Forsgren, 1989; Singer, Hippler, & Schwartz, 1992). Additional research conducted by Singer, Mathiowetz, and Couper (1995) identified participant “trust in the integrity of the data-collection agency” as being fundamental to the confidentiality debate rather than the “nature of the assurance given to the respon- dent” (p. 479). At times research requires anonymity to gain access to a sample, in which case double responses of mail surveys are a concern. Summers and Price (1997), however, found the rate of double responses to be low, less than 1%. Signature

Researchers have explored the effects of a handwritten signature versus a photo- copied or other facsimile signature. A personalized signature has not been shown conclusively to increase the response rate of mail surveys (Dodd & Markwiese, 1986; Kanuk & Berenson, 1975). McDermott and Sarvela (1999) recom- mend, however, that a personal signature using a colored ballpoint pen as evidence of a real signature demonstrates to poten- tial participants the researcher’s level of commitment to the questionnaire, a sugges- tion supported throughout the literature (Dillman, 1972). Postscript

Early survey research reported that handwritten notes (postscripts) requesting

participation increased response rates due to the effect of personalization (Fox et al., 1988; Longworth, 1958; Kanuk & Berenson, 1975). However, the effect of postscripts on response rate have been inconsistent, and moderately significant increases in response rate have been achieved (Andreasen, 1970; Forsgren, 1989; Fox et al., 1988). Incentives

The use of an incentive is the second most commonly addressed issue in studies examining mail survey response rates (Fox et al., 1988). It is clear from the literature that the use of an incentive does increase response rate (Brennan, Hoek, & Astridge, 1991; Church, 1993; Everett, Price, Bedell, & Telljohann, 1997; Fox et al., 1988; Goyder, 1982; Hare, Price, Flynn, & King, 1998; Heberlein & Baumgartner, 1978; Kanuk & Berenson, 1975; Linsky, 1975; Yammarino et al., 1991). Nevertheless, a number of issues should be adequately addressed when preparing to use incentives in a mail survey (Table 111). Amount of Incentive

The inclusion of a $.25, $.50, $1.00, or $2.00 incentive in the mailing has resulted in response rates that are 8.5, 8.9, 18.6, or 23.4% greater than controls, respectively (James & Bolstein, 1990). Similarly, Oden and Price (1999) found that a $1 .OO incen- tive resulted in an 81% response rate whereas no incentive resulted in a 66% response rate. Generally, the larger the incentive, the greater the response rate (Hopkins & Gullickson, 1992). However, Mizes, Fleece, and Roos (1984) found no significant difference on response rate between $1 and $5 incentives (each showed a 21% improvement over no incentive). Cost-fffectiveness of lncentives

Although incentives as low as $0.25 have been found to be effective at increas- ing response rates (Fox et al., 1988; James & Bolstein, 1990; Kanuk & Berenson, 1975), a $1.00 incentive is the most con- sistently effective incentive and has the highest codbenefit ratio (Easton, Price, Telljohann, & Boehm, 1997; Fox et al., 1988; James & Bolstein, 1990, 1992). In

fact, Everett and colleagues (1997) found that a $1.00 incentive for a questionnaire sent to physicians was more cost-effective than a third postcard reminder mailing. Although some research has shown a positive correlation between the amount of the incentive and response rate (Church, 1993; James & Bolstein, 1992; Linsky, 1975), others have demonstrated a point of diminishing returns. This point of diminishing returns is generally the point at which the amount of money is no longer considered a “thank you” (i.e., $ l ) , but instead a payment for services (i.e., $5) (Everett et al., 1997; Kanuk & Berenson, 1975; Mangione, 1995). Monetary Versus Nonmonetary lncen tives

The research shows that monetary incentives tend to have a greater impact on response rates than nonmonetary incentives (e.g., coffee packets; books; pens; key rings; golf balls; tie clips; donations to charity; and feedback on survey responses) (Church, 1993; Everett et al., 1997; James & Bolstein, 1990, 1992; Kanuk & Berenson, 1975; Linsky, 1975; Mangione, 1995; Yammarino et al., 1991). For instance, Easton et al. (1997) found that the inclusion of a $1.00 incentive led to a 15% higher response rate than the inclusion of an informational booklet incentive. Timing of Incentive Delivery

Regardless of amount of the monetary incentive, the research clearly shows that incentives provided in the initial mailing are much more effective at increasing response rate than promised rewards (Berry & Kanouse, 1987; Church, 1993; James & Bolstein, 1992; Linsky, 1975; O’Rourke, 1999). Hopkins and Gullickson (1992) employed a meta-analysis to show that the average response rate increased 19% when a gratuity was enclosed (prepaid) and only 7% when a gratuity was promised (contin- gent on the return of the questionnaire). A significant increase in response rate appears to be true regardless of whether the prepaid incentive is monetary or nonmon- etary (Church, 1993).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 7: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

Inducement Strategy Comparison Group

Study % Increase of Response Rate

Prepaid monetary incentives ( $ ) 40.00 No incentive 20.00 No incentive 10.00 No incentive 10.00 5 1 .oo

James & Bolstein ( 1992) 33.3" James & Bolstein 1992) 33.3*

23 .3* No difference

James & Bolstein ( 1 992) James & Bolstein ( 1 992)

5.00

5.00

No incentive Mizes et al. ( 1 984) 21.1* James & Bolstein ( 1 992) 5 1 .oo Mizes et al. ( 1 984) No difference

18.0"

I 2.00 No incentive James & Bolstein ( 1 990) 23.4'

1 .oo to 5 1.99 No incentive Hopkins & Gullickson 1 992Ia 25.1

1 .oo

1 .oo

No incentive Mizes et al. ( I 984) James & Bolstein ( 1 992) Brennan et al. ( I 99 1 ) James & Bolstein ( 1 990) Everett et al. ( I 997) Oden & Price ( I 999) Hare et al. ( 1 998) Hopkins et al. ( 1 988) Easton et al. ( 1 997) Informational booklet

21.1* 20 .0* 17.6" 18.6* 18.0* 15.0" 12.0* 1 o.o* 15.0"

.50 to .99 No incentive Hopkins & Gullickson ( 1 992Jb 20.0*

.50 No incentive Brennan et al. ( 1 99 I ) James & Bolstein ( 1 990)

21 .3* 8.9"

.50 or less No incentive Yammarino et al. ( 199 1 Ib 18.4" 7 .3* Hopkins & Gullickson ( 1 992Ic

.25 No incentive James & Bolstein ( 1 990) 8.5"

Timing of incentive Prepaid monetary incentive Incentive promised Berry & Kanouse ( 1 987) 12 o*

Hopkins & Gullickson ( 1992)( 1 1 9* $50 check promised James & Bolstein ( I 992) No difference No incentive Church ( 1 993)' 19 I *

Nonmonetary incentives Nonmonetary incentive No incentive Enclosed ballpoint pen No incentive

Church ( 1 993)' 7.9" Nederhof ( 1 98313) No difference

*p 5.05 'Results based on meta-analysis of 62 studies bResults based on meta-analysis of 115 studies 'Results based on meta-analysis of 38 studies

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 8: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

Questionnaire Characteristics Although incentives, cover letters, and

envelopes and postage are important issues to address when attempting to increase response rates, the questionnaire should not be overlooked. A questionnaire that is tedious, difficult to understand, and lacking in aesthetic appeal is less likely to be completed and returned (Dillman, Sinclair, & Clark, 1993; Fox et al., 1988). Therefore, questionnaire characteristics also must be examined prior to mailings (Table IV). Questionnaire Color

The color of the questionnaire has been shown to affect response rates (Pressley & Tullar, 1977; Pucel, Nelson, & Wheeler, 1971). Questionnaires that are a color other than white are less apt to be lost o r misplaced because they stand out on a respondents’desk (Mangione, 1995). Some evidence does exist to suggest that the use of a green questionnaire may yield higher response rates than the use of a white questionnaire (Fox et al., 1988; Gullahorn & Gullahorn, 1963). The effects

of other colors on response rates have not been widely reported in the literature. Questionnaire Length

Burchell and Marsh (1992) estimated that the response rate decreases by 0.4% for each additional page of a questionnaire. Therefore, these researchers recommended questionnaires be limited to 10 pages and 100 items. An experimental study of alternatives to the current U.S. decennial census questionnaire showed that shortening the questionnaire improved response (Dillman et al., 1993). Interest- ingly, Adams and Gale (1982) found that the response rate to a 3-page questionnaire was higher than that to a l-page or 5-page questionnaire. Perhaps participants may feel that a 3-page questionnaire is more important than a l-page question- naire and less burdensome than a 5-page questionnaire. However, researchers have suggested that length of questionnaire not be considered as the lone determin- ing factor of response rates (Burchell & Marsh, 1992; Mangione, 1995). Specific efforts should also be made to maintain

respondent motivation (Herzog & Bachman, 1981). Strategies to maintain high levels of motivation include ensuring respondents that their information is crucial to the suc- cess of the study, developing questionnaires examining interesting content, and format- ting questionnaires in an attractive and user- friendly manner. Ease and Interest in Completing the Survey

The research indicates that questionnaire length is much less important to increasing response rates than ease and interest in completing the survey (O’Rourke & O’Rourke, 1983). Questionnaires that address the most salient topics at the beginning tend to have higher response rates (Mullner, Levy, Byre, & Matthews, 1982). Quickly engaging a respondent’s interest and then maintaining that interest level is crucial (Mangione, 1995). Dillman et al. (1993) termed questionnaires that are easy to complete and avoid confusing items, “respondent -friendly.” Respondent-friendly questionnaires were found to increase response rates whereas questionnaires

Inducement Strategy Comparison Group

Study % Increase of Response Rate

Length 4 pages or less 3 pages 5 pages

4 pages or more

3 pages 1 Page 3 pages 1 Page 1 Page 5 pages 1 page [both sides) 8 pages

Yammarino et al. [ 199 1 J” 7.8* Adams & Gale ( 1 982) 25.1 *

5.9* Rudd & Maxwell ( 1 980) No difference Adams & Gale 1982) 19.2* Dillman et al. ( 1 993) 4.6*

Adams & Gale ( 1 982)

Color Green White Fox et al. ( 1 988)b 2.0

Gullahorn & Gullahorn [ I 9631 2 .o

At end of questionnaire At beginning of questionnaire Roberson & Sundstrom [ 1990) 8.0* Jensen ( 1 995) No difference

Demographic items present No demographic items present Giles & Feild ( 1 978) No difference

Location of Demographics

*p I .05 ‘Results based on meta-analysis of 115 experimental studies

bResults based on meta-analysis of 3 experimental studies

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 9: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

asking potentially difficult or objectionable questions (i.e., social security number) low- ered response rates (Dillman et al., 1993). Interestingly, the combined effect of a ques- tionnaire that was both respondent-friendly and short increased response rates by 8 to 10 percentage points. Location of Demographic Items

After reading a description of the ques- tionnaire in the cover letter, individuals who are perhaps looking forward to responding may be unpleasantly surprised that the first question(s) are about age, gender, race, or education. Such questions, in respon- dents’ minds, often are viewed as not directly pertaining to the health issue (Dillman, 2000). The most salient topics should be placed at the beginning to increase response rates (Mullner et al., 1982). For these reasons, Dillman (2000) recommends that questionnaires should seldom begin with demographic items. Roberson and Sundstrom (1990) found that placing demographic items at the end of the instrument increased response rate by 8%. Nevertheless, some recommend that “simple” and “easy-to-complete’’ items such as demographic items should be placed at the beginning of the questionnaire as a means to increase respondent confidence in completing the survey (Alreck & Settle, 1995). Jensen (1995) found no significant difference in response rate between questionnaires with demographic items placed in the beginning and questionnaires with demographic items placed at the end. Giles and Feild (1978) found no difference in response rate between ques- tionnaires with demographic items and questionnaires without demographic items. Thus, item placement should be considered in context with questionnaire content and target audience.

Participant Contact Participant contact can take two forms:

prenotification and follow-up (Table V). Prenotification is intended to alert partici- pants that the questionnaire will be arriv- ing and to establish the legitimacy of the

questionnaire. Follow-up contacts serve to positively reinforce participants who have returned the questionnaire and to remind those who have not to do so (Fox et al., 1988). Although all methods of contact have a positive impact on response rate, partici- pant contact via telephone or registered mail has been found to yield significantly higher rates than that via regular mail (Eckland, 1965; Champion & Sear, 1969; Gullahorn & Gullahorn, 1959; Heberlein & Baumgartner, 1978; Kanuk & Berenson, 1975; Linsky, 1975; Phillips, 1951; Slocum, 1956). The research clearly indicates that the number of participant contacts (either prenotification or follow-up) has a signifi- cant positive correlation with response rate (Heberlein & Baumgartner, 1978; Yammarino et al., 1991;Yu &Cooper, 1983). In a linear regression analysis, Heberlein and Baumgartner found that each contact increases the predicted response rate by 12%. However, most researchers agree that follow-up participant contacts are a better investment of resources than prenoti- fication contacts (Kanuk & Berenson, 1975; Linsky, 1975; Mangione, 1995; O’Rourke, 1999). Yammarino et al. found that although preliminary notification increased response rate by 28.5%, follow-up contacts increased response rates by over 30.6%. Dillman (1978) recommends three follow- ups after the initial mailing of the question- naire. He suggests first and third follow-ups should be postcard reminders and the second follow-up should include another copy of the questionnaire. Postcards have been shown to be equally effective as letters in follow-ups (Kanuk & Berenson, 1975; Linsky, 1975).

Conclusion Many of the inducement strategies

presented here are interdependent and interrelated in the execution of mail survey research. Decisions on each compo- nent of the mail survey process cannot be made independently of the others. There- fore, it may be impossible to establish a defined and rigid protocol for mail

survey research. It is imperative research- ers understand their potential respondents and tailor the cover letter and questionnaire to meet their unique interests and appeals.

Nevertheless, this review of the survey research literature suggests that mail survey researchers can increase response rates if (1) outgoing envelopes are personalized; (2) self-addressed stamped envelopes are provided; (3) university sponsorship is evident; (4) research benefits are empha- sized; ( 5 ) confidentiality is assured when asking sensitive questions; (6) up-front monetary incentives are provided (if funding allows); (7) colored, easy-to- complete questionnaires are employed; and (8) participants are contacted multiple times (Figure 1). Based on the findings of this review, the following recommenda- tions are offered to help direct future research in this area.

(1) Research studies in health education should be conducted that solely examine individuals’ beliefs of effective inducement strategies (as opposed to studies that exam- ine inducement strategies within the context of a larger, and topically different study).

(2) Research studies should seek to iden- tify nonrespondents to a particular survey and then examine their perceived barriers to responding (e.g., topic, format).

(3) Research studies should examine whether inducement strategies vary based on demographic characteristics of respon- dents (e.g., gender, education level, race/ ethnicity, age, socioeconomic status).

(4) Research studies should continue to examine the combined effects of induce- ment strategies on response rates as opposed to the individual effects of a lone inducement strategy.

(5) Attention should be given to the pro- fessional marketing literature as a means of identifying effective marketing concepts and integrating such concepts in health education survey research.

(6) Health education journals and conferences should continue to devote attention to maximizing mail survey response rates.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 10: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

References Aday, L. A. (1996). Designing and conduct-

ing health surveys. 2nd ed. San Francisco: Jossey- Bass.

Adams, L. L. M., & Gale, D. (1982). Solving the quandary between questionnaire length and response rate in educational research. Research in Higher Education, 17,231-240.

Alreck, P. L., & Settle, R. B. (1995). The sur- vey research handbook, 2nd ed. Boston: Irwin McGraw-Hill.

Andreasen, A. R. (1970). Personalizing mail questionnaire correspondence. Public ODinion

Armstrong, J. S., & Lusk, E. J. (1987). Return postage in mail surveys. Public Opinion Quar- terly, 51, 233-248.

Berry, S. H., & Kanouse, D. E. (1987). Physi- cian response to a mailed survey. An experiment in timing of payment. Public Opinion Quarterly,

Brennan,M.,Hoek, J., &Astridge,C. (1991). The effects of monetary incentives on the re- sponse rate and cost-effectiveness of a mail sur- vey. Journal of Marketing Research, 33, 229-241.

Brunner, A.G., & Carroll, S.J. (1969). Effect of prior notification on the refusal rate in

51, 102-114.

Research 9, 42-44. Burchell, B., & Marsh, C. (1992). The effect

of questionnaire length on survey response. Quality and Quantity, 26, 233-244.

Carpenter, E. H. (1974). Personalizing mail surveys: A replication ad reassessment. Public Opinion Quarterly, 38,614-620.

Champion, D. J. &Sear,A. (1969). Question- naire response rate: A methodological analysis. Social Forces, 47, 335-339.

Choi, B. K., Pak, A. W., & Purdham, J. T. (1990). Effects of mailing strategies on response rate, response time, and cost in a questionnaire

Quarterly, 34, 273-277. fixed address surveys. Journal of Advertising study among nurses. Epidemiology, 1,72-74.

Inducement Strategy Comparison Study % Increase of Group Response Rate

Preliminary Notification

By mail No preliminary notification Yammarino et al. ( 1 99 1 J a 28.5* 8 .1* 7 .7*

1 .o

Yu & Cooper ( I 983JC Fox et al. [ I 988Jb Kephart & Bressler ( 1958)

By personal contact No preliminary notification Lorei & Gore1 [ 1967) 4.9*

Follow-Up Notification ~ One contact follow-up NO fOllOW-up Yammarino et al. ( 199 1 )d 30.6*

19.9* 17.9*

Two contact follow-up One contact follow-up Heberlein & Baumgartner ( 1 97814 1 I . ? * 1 o.o*

Postcard follow-up NO fOllOW-up Eckland ( I 965) 9 .0*

Heberlein & Baumgartner ( 1 978If Yu & Cooper ( I 983)'

Heberlein & Baumgartner [ 1 978Jh Three contact follow-up Two contact follow-up Fox et al. ( 1 988)' 3 .5*

Telephone follow-up with Certified letter Eckland ( 1 965) 19 O* certified letter Telephone follow-up with Telephone follow-up Slocum ( I 956) 51 6 * CertlfJed letter Ph!hpS ( I 95 I J 38 O*

27 3* Letter via special delivery I 9 3"

Gullahorn & Gullahorn ( 1 959) Champion & Sear [ I 969) Letter via first class postage

*p < .05 'Results based on meta-analysis of 4 experimental studies hResults based on meta-analysis of 8 experimental studies cResults based on meta-analysis of 9 experimental studies dResults based on meta-analysis of 10 experimental studies 'Results based on meta-analysis of 14 experimental studies 'Results based on meta-analysis of 51 experimental studies gResults based on meta-analysis of 25 experimental studies hResults based on meta-analysis of 3 1 experimental studies 'Results based on meta-analysis of 6 experimental studies

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 11: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

Church, A.H. (1993). Estimating the effect of incentives on mail survey response rates: A meta-analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly,

Clausen, J. A., & Ford, R. N. (1947). Con- trolling bias in mail questionnaires. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 42,497-5 1 1.

Del Valle, M. L., Morgenstern, H., Rogstad, T. L., Albright, C., & Vickrey, B. G. (1997). A

57,62-79.

on response rate to a physician survey, and a cost-effectiveness analysis. Evaluation and the Health Professions, 20, 389-406.

Dignan, M. B., Michielutte, R., Jones-Lighty, D. D., & Bahnson, J. (1994). Factors influencing the return rate in a direct mail campaign. Public Health Reports, 109, 507-51 1.

Dillman, D. A. (2000). Mail and internet sur- veys. The tailored design method. New York John

randomized trial of the impact of certified mail Wiley & Sons.

Envelopes & Postage . Personlized out-going envelope . Self-addressed stamped envelope C I / I C’ Cover Letter

a University sponsorship a Personlization and appeal a Anonymity and confidentiality

I 1

I Incentives I A . Up - front . Monetary I I

I Questionnaire Characteristics I 9 Nonwhite

Respond en t-friend ly . Salient topic Not longer than 4 pages

1

Participant Contact

. Multiple mailings

. FOIIOW-UPS

a

Dillman, D. A. (1978). Mail and telephone surveys: T h e total design method. New York Wiley.

Dillman, D. A. (1972). Increasing mail questionnaire response in large samples of the general public. Public Opinion Quarterly, 36,

Dillman, D. A., Sinclair, M. D., & Clark, J. R. (1993). Effects of questionnaire length, respondent-friendly design, and a difficult ques- tion on response rates for occupant-addressed census mail surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 57,289-304.

Dodd, D. K., & Markwiese, B. J. (1986). Sur- vey response rate as a function of personalized signature on cover letter. Journal of Social Psy-

Dolsen, D. E., & Machlis, G. E. (1991). Re- sponse rates and mail recreation survey results: How much is enough? Journal of Leisure Re- search, 23, 272-277.

Duncan, J. W. (1979). Mail questionnaires in survey research: A review of response induce- ment techniques. Journal of Management , 5, 39-55.

Easton, A. N., Price, J. H., Telljohann, S. K., & Boehm, K. (1997). An informational versus monetary incentive in increasing physicians’ response rates. Psychological Reports, 81,

Eckland, B. K. (1965). Effects of prodding to increase mail-back returns. Journal ofApplied

Everett, S. A., Price, J. H., Bedell, A. W., & Telljohann, S. K. (1997). The effect of a mon- etary incentive in increasing the return rate of a survey to family physicians. Evaluation and the Health Professions, 20,207-214.

Ferris, A. L. (1951). A note on stimulating response to questionnaires. American Sociologi- cal Review, 16,247-249.

Filion, F. L. (1975). Estimating bias due to nonresponse in mail surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 39,482-492.

Forsgren, R. A. (1989). Increasing mail sur- vey response rates: Methods for small business researchers. Journal of Small Business Manage- ment, 27,61-66.

Fowler, F. J. (1984). Survey research methods. Beverly Hills, C A Sage.

Fox, R. J., Crask, M. R., & Kim, J. (1988). Mail

254-257.

chology, 127,97-98.

968-970.

Psychology, 49, 165-169.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 12: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

survey response rate: A meta-analysis of selected techniques for inducing response. Public Opin- ion Quarterly, 52,467-491.

Gendall, P., Hoek, J., & Esslemont, D. (1995). The effect of appeal, complexity and tone in a mail survey covering letter. Journal of the Mar- ket Research Society, 37,251-269.

Giles, W.F., & Feild, H.S., (1978). Effects of amount, format, and location of demographic information on questionnaire return rate and response to bias of sensitive and nonsensitive items. Personnel Psychology, 31,549-559.

Goyder, J. C. (1982). Further evidence on factors affecting response rates to mailed ques- tionnaires. American Sociological Review, 47,

Groves, B. W., Price, Olsson, R. H., & King, K. A. (1997). Response rates to anonymous ver- sus confidential surveys. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 665-666.

Gullahorn, J. E., & Gullahorn, J. T. (1963). An investigation of the effects of three factors on response to mail questionnaires. Public Opin- ion Quarterly, 27,294-296.

Gullahorn, J. T. & Gullahorn, J. F. (1959). Increasing returns from nonrespondents. Pub- lic Opinion Quarterly, 23, 119-121.

Hare, S., Price, J. H., Flynn, M. G., & King, K. A. (1998). Increasing return rates of a mail survey to exercise professionals using a modest monetary incentive. Perceptual and Motor Skills,

Harris, J. R., & Guffey, Jr., H. J. (1978). Ques- tionnaire returns: Stamps versus business reply envelopes revisited. Journal of Marketing Re- search, 15,290-293.

Heberlein, T. A., & Baumgartner, R. (1978). Factors affecting response rates to mailed ques- tionnaires: A quantitative analysis of the pub- lished literature. American Sociological Review,

Henley, J. R. (1976). Response rate to mail questionnaires with a return deadline. Public Opinion Quarterly, 40, 374-375.

Hensley, W. E. (1974). Increasing response rate by choice of postage stamps. Public Opin- ion Quarterly, 38,280-283.

Herzog, A. R., & Bachman, J. G. (1981). Ef- fects of questionnaire length on response qual- ity. Public Opinion Quarterly, 45, 549-559.

Hopkins, K. D., & Gullickson, A. R. (1992).

5 5 0-5 5 3.

86,217-218.

43,447-462.

Response rates in survey research: A meta-analy- sis of the effects of monetary gratuities. Journal of Experimental Education, 61,52-62.

Hopkins, K. D., Hopkins, B. R., & Schon, I. (1988). Mail surveys of professional popula- tions: The effects of monetary gratuities on re- turn rates. Journal of Experimental Education,

Houston, M. J., & Nevin, J. R. (1977). The effects of source and appeal on mail survey re- sponse patterns. Journal of Marketing Research,

Isaac, S. & Michael, W. B. (1995). Handbook in research and evaluation, 3rd ed. San Diego, CA: EdITS/Educational and Industrial Testing Services.

James, J. M., & Bolstein, R. (1990). The ef- fect of monetary incentives and follow-up mail- ings on the response rate and response quality in mail surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 54,

James, J. M. & Bolstein, R. (1992). Large monetary incentives and their effect on mail survey response rates. Public Opinion Quurterly,

Jenson, J. L. (1995). The effect of survey for- mat on response rate and patterns of responses. Dissertation Abstracts International Humanities Q Social Sciences, 55(9-A), 2787A.

Jones, W. H., & Lang, J. R. (1980). Sample composition bias and response bias in a mail survey: A comparison of inducement methods. Journal of Marketing Research, 17, 69-76.

Jones, W. H., & Linda, G. (1978). Multiple criteria effects in a mail survey experiment. Jour- nu1 of Marketing Research, 15,280-284.

Kahle, L. R., & Sales, B. D. (1978). Personal- ization of the outside envelope in mail surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 42, 547-550.

Kanuk, L., & Berenson, C. (1975). Mail Sur- veys and response rates: A literature review.Jour- nu1 of Marketing Research, 12,440-453.

Kephart, W.M., & Bressler, M. (1958) In- creasing the responses to mail questionnaires. Public Opinion Quarterly, 22. 123-132 .

Linsky, A. S. (1975). Stimulating responses to mailed questionnaires: A review. Public Opin- ion Quarterly, 39, 82-101.

Longworth, D.S. (1953) Use of mail ques- tionnaire. American Sociological Review, 18( 3),

57, 173-175.

14,374-377.

346-361.

56,442-453.

3 10-3 13.

Lorei, T., & Gurel , L. (1967). Composition of ex-mentl patient employment information obtained by mail and by interview, Journal of Counseling Psychology 14(5), 458-461.

Maheux, B. Legault, C., & Lambert, J. (1989). Increasing response rates in physicians mail sur- veys: An experimental study. American Journal of Public Health, 79(5), 638-639.

Mangione, T. W. (1995). Mail surveys. Im- proving the quality. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Martinez-Ebers, V. (1997). Using monetary incentives with hard-to-reach publications in panel surveys. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 9( l), 77-86.

Mason, W. S., Dressel, R. J., & Bain, R. K. (1961). An experimental study of factors affect- ing response to a mail survey of beginning teachers. Public Opinion Quarterly, 25,296-299.

McDermott, R. I., & Sarvela, P. D. (1999). Health education evaluation and measurement: A practitioner’s perspective, 2nd ed. New York WCBlMcGraw Hill.

Moss, V. D., & Worthen, B. R. (1991). Do personalization and postage make a difference on response rates to surveys of professional populations? Psychological Reports, 68,692-694.

Mullner, R. M., Levy, P. S., Byre, C. S., & Matthews, D. (1982). Effects of characteristics of the survey instrument on response rates to a mail survey of community hospitals. Public Health Reports, 97,465-469.

Oden, L., & Price, J. H. (1999). Effects of a small monetary incentive and follow-up mail- ings on return rates of a survey to nurse practi- tioners. Psychological Reports, 85, 1154-1 156.

O’Rourke, T. (1999). Increasing response rates: Specific applications. American Journal of Health Studies, 15,164-166.

O’Rouke, D. P., & O’Rourke, T. W. (1983). Improving response rates to health surveys us- ing mail questionnaires. Eta Sigma Gamman,

Phillips, W. M. (1951). Weaknesses of the mail questionnaire: A methodological study. Sociology and Social Research, 35,260-267.

Pressley, M. M., &Tullar, W. L. (1977).A fac- tor interactive investigation of mail survey re- sponse rates from a commercial population. Journal of Marketing Research, 14, 108-1 11.

Price, J. H., Easton, A., Kandakai, T. L., & Oden, L. (1996). Race-specific versus general

12-16.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 13: Increasing Response Rates to Mail Questionnaires: A Review of Inducement Strategies

stamps on African-American women’s survey return rates. Perceptual Q Motor Skills, 82,928- 930.

Pucel, D. J., Nelson, H. F., &Wheeler, D. N. (1971). Questionnaire follow-up returns as a function of incentives and responder character- istics. Vocational Guidance Quarterly, 19, 188- 193.

Roberson, M. R., & Sundstrom, E. (1990). Questionnaire design, return rates, and response favorableness in an employee attitude question- naire. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 75,354-357.

Roberts, R. E., McCrory, 0. F., & Forthofer, R. N. (1978). Further evidence on using a dead- line to stimulate responses to a mail survey. Pub- lic Opinion Quarterly, 42,407-410.

Rosen, N. A. (1960).Anonymity and attitude measurement. Public Opinion Quarterly, 24,

Rossi, P. H., Wright, J. D., &Anderson, A. B. (Eds.). (1983). Handbook of survey research. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

Rucker, M., Hughes, R., Thompson, R., Harrison, A., & Vanderlip, N. (1984). Personal- ization of mail surveys: Too much of a good thing? Educational and Psychological Measure- ment, 44,893-905.

Simon, R. (1967). Response to personal and form letters in mail survey. Journal ofAdvertis- ing Research, 7,28-30.

Singer, E., Hippler, H. J., & Schwarz, N. (1992). Confidentiality assurances in surveys: Reassurance or threat? International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 4,256-268.

Singer, E., Mathiowetz, N.A., & Couper, M.P. (1993). The impact of privacy and confidenti- ality concerns on survey participation: The case of the 1990 U.S. census. Public Opinion Quar- terly, 57(4), 465-482.

Singer, E., von Thurn, D. R., & Miller, E. R. (1995). Confidentiality assurances and re- sponses: A quantitative review of the literature. Public Opinion Quarterly, 59,66-77.

Slocum, W. L. (1956). Increasing response to questionnaires and structured interviews. American Sociological Review, 21,221-225.

Summers, J., & Price, J. H. (1997). Increas- ing return rates to a mail survey among health educators. Psychological Reports, 81,55 1-554.

Torabi, M. R. (1991). Factors affecting re- sponse rate in mail survey questionnaires.

675-679.

American Journal of Health Behavior, 15(5), 57- 59.

Wagner, W. G., & O’Toole, W. M. (1985). The effects of cover letter format on faculty response rate in mail survey research. Educational and Psychological Research, 5, 29-37.

Worthen, B. R., & Valcarce, R. W. (1985). Relative effectiveness of personalized and form covering letters in initial and follow-up mail surveys. Psychological Reports, 57,735-744.

Yammarino, F. J., Skinner, S. J., & Childers, T. L. (1991). Understanding mail survey re- sponse behavior. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55,

Yu, J., & Cooper, H. (1983). A quantitative review of research design effects on response rates to questionnaires. Journal of Marketing Research, 20, 36-44.

613-639.

Call for Papers on Mental Health

The American Journal of Health Education announces a call for original manu- scripts or Teaching Ideas for a special insert or issue on mental health. We invite ethicists, policy makers, practitioners, and researchers from health education or related disciplines to submit manuscripts that describe and/or analyze mental health issues of interest to health educators. Topics of interest would include but are not limited to the following:

Reducing violence in adolescents and/or adults Mental health promotion for at-risk groups Costs of mental illness to society and the role of health education Mental health provisions in public schools Suicide prevention for youths or other high-risk groups Stress; death and dying; eating disorders; etc.

Authors should follow the Guidelines for Authors found in issues of the

Manuscript submission deadline: March 30,200 1 American Journal of Health Education.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Pr

ince

Edw

ard

Isla

nd]

at 1

9:57

15

Nov

embe

r 20

14