habitat use and community structure in an assemblage of cottid fishes

13
Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes Author(s): Stephen F. Norton Source: Ecology, Vol. 72, No. 6 (Dec., 1991), pp. 2181-2192 Published by: Ecological Society of America Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1941569 . Accessed: 16/07/2014 16:01 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . Ecological Society of America is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Ecology. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Upload: stephen-f

Post on 29-Jan-2017

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid FishesAuthor(s): Stephen F. NortonSource: Ecology, Vol. 72, No. 6 (Dec., 1991), pp. 2181-2192Published by: Ecological Society of AmericaStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1941569 .

Accessed: 16/07/2014 16:01

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range ofcontent in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new formsof scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

.

Ecological Society of America is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Ecology.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 2: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

Ecology, 72(6), 1991, pp. 2181-2192 © 1991 by the Ecological Society of America

HABITAT USE AND COMMUNITY STRUCTURE IN AN ASSEMBLAGE OF COTTID FISHES'

STEPHEN F. NORTON Fisheries Research Institute and Friday Harbor Laboratories, University of Washington,

620 University Road, Friday Harbor, Washington 98250 USA

Abstract. I applied an observational approach, using strip transects, to document the abundance and the spatial distributions of members of an assemblage of subtidal fishes (Scorpaeniformes: Cottidae) in the San Juan Islands, Washington, USA. I then assessed the extent to which species-specific distributions were correlated with habitat availability and the potential for interspecific competition to modify patterns of abundance and resource use. The six common cottid species were Artedius fenestralis, A. harringtoni, Asemichthys taylori, Chitonotus pugetensis, Icelinus borealis, and Jordania zonope. A comparison of their observed distributions with the distributions of microhabitats along the transects revealed that all species demonstrated significant affinities for or avoidance of many of the predominant substrates: clean rock, silt rock, shell sand, gravel, mud, and algae. Using a stepwise regression technique, I found that the densities of individual cottid species were significantly correlated with the percentage cover of these substrate types along the transects and with transect depth.

Several lines of evidence suggest that interspecific competition does not play a strong role in this assemblage. Species pairs that co-occurred on at least some transects did not show disjunct distributions. Microhabitat overlap of these species pairs was higher in sympatry (comparison of transects with both species present) than in allopatry (comparisons of transects with only one species present), counter to the expectation of niche divergence. Correlations of species densities after correcting for habitat heterogeneity with the Schoener- Crowell-Pimm technique revealed only one significant negative density correlation out of thirty potential correlations. Cottid diversity was not correlated with structural diversity along transects in spite of strong associations of each species with habitat structure. This suggests that the transect areas were undersaturated in the number of individuals and/or species of cottid fishes. The apparent lack of competition among subtidal cottids contrasts with studies on intertidal fish communities dominated by cottids for which there is ob- servational evidence of interspecific competition.

Key words: community structure; Cottidae; habitat selection; interspecific competition; microhab- itat overlap; observational approach; resource partitioning; species density; temperate fish communities; unsaturated habitats.

INTRODUCTION

A central goal of ecology is the identification of the roles that abiotic and biotic factors play in determining the distribution and abundance of species. In partic- ular, attention has been directed to the roles of these factors in determining patterns of resource use among closely related species. This interest stems from one of the central paradigms of ecology, the principle of com- petitive exclusion (Gause 1934). A major focus of eco- logical research has been to identify those conditions that allow coexistence of species using similar re- sources, e.g., predation (Paine 1974), non-equilibrial physical processes (Sale 1977, Wiens 1977, Sousa 1979) and non-linear competitive hierarchies (Buss and Jack- son 1979) or that reduce overlap in resource use, e.g., niche partitioning (Werner 1977, Hixon 1980, Pacala and Roughgarden 1982), character displacement (Grant

' Manuscript received 12 March 1990; revised 12 January 1991; accepted 14 January 1991.

and Schluter 1984), and disjunct distributions (Hair- ston 1980, Case 1983).

The term "resource partitioning" has been used in a general sense to describe any differences in resource use that exist between species and in a strict sense to describe only those changes in resource use due to com- petitors (Ebeling and Laur 1986). That species differ in resource use is a common ecological observation (see reviews in Schoener 1974a, Toft 1985, Ross 1986). More pertinent to the understanding of community dynamics is assessing to what extent these differences in resource use reflect inherent interspecific inequalities in the abilities to use and/or the preferences for using resources (autecological interactions) and to what ex- tent biotic processes, including predation and com- petition (synecological interactions) modify autecolog- ical differences (Strong 1983).

Both experimental and observational approaches have been used to investigate the factors influencing the distribution and abundance of guild members. The experimental approach, manipulation of a single factor

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 3: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

while holding others constant, is the stronger method, but is not without drawbacks (Cody 1974, Schoener

''_ (V Vancouver 1974a, Connell 1975, Underwood and Denley 1984, Diamond 1986). In the observational approach, cor- relations are made between the density or distribution

San Juan I s. of a species and the abundance or intensity of a biotic or abiotic factor as both vary spatially or temporally. The weakness of the observational approach is the leap

S e a t t I e from correlation to causation. Several studies have pro- vided examples in which experiments have refuted the importance of processes inferred by observed patterns

North or found that the effects were even reversed (see McGuinness 1988). Yet, in multispecies guilds, obser- vational studies can be valuable in generating hypoth- eses, especially in identifying subsets of factors or spe-

O ) 0 r c a s I s. cies that then may be subjected to experimental analysis (e.g., Brown et al. 1986).

This study uses an observational approach (multiple strip transects at several subtidal sites) to document habitat use and to identify possible mechanisms un-

(4^ ',9 ^^ t) <:J D derlying habitat partitioning among members of a guild of subtidal fishes in the family Cottidae. Ninety cottid

S anX vJ ua^ J n \ . species can be found between Baja California and the Aleutian Islands (Howe and Richardson 1978). Within

' tl9h" \ /7 v^ this geographic range cottids are often the dominant fishes in tidepool and nearshore subtidal habitats (Si- menstad et al. 1977, Yoshiyama et al. 1986). Many

L0 \ Lopez Is- ° studies have documented the importance of physical and biological factors, including competition, in de- termining the distribution of intertidal species (Morris

CO^-) i:,@1960, 1961, Graham 1970, Green 1971, Nakamura 1976a, b, Cross 1981, Richkus 1981, Yoshiyama 1981, Grossman 1982, 1986b, Wells 1986). Subtidal cottids

a - East Sound 1 e - Turn Is. are also quite abundant, but the factors determining b - East Sound 2 f - Eagle Point the distributions and abundances of these species are c - Bell Is. g - FHL Mud poorly known. In part, this reflects the small size, sed-

d - Point George h - Turn Rock entary behavior, and cryptic coloration of many spe-

FIG. 1. Diagram of the San Juan Islands, Washington, cies, which make them much less visible during un- showing the six major study sites (a-f) and the two minor derwater investigations (e.g., Burge and Schultz 1973, study sites (g and h). Ebeling et al. 1980, Larson and DeMartini 1984).

The goal of this study was to document patterns of abundance and habitat use among members of the as- semblage of subtidal cottids, to correlate species-spe-

'ABLE 1. Distribution of transects and average cottid density on each transect by site. Habitat abbreviations: CR = clean rock, SR = silt rock, G = gravel, SS = shell sand, M = mud, A = algae. Species abbreviations: Ahar - Artedius harringtoni;

Total Cover of substrates (%) Number of area

Site transects (m2) CR SR G SS M A

Turn Island 6 85 52 13 15 8 5 7 Bell Island 8 106.5 59 0 0 40 0 1 East Sound 1 8 120 0 10 20 39 28 3 East Sound 2 5 67.5 0 40 0 57 0 2 Point George 12 170 75 6 1 12 3 3 Eagle Point 4 57.5 34 0 23 35 0 8 Friday Harbor ILaboratories 1 15 0 0 2 0 66 31 Turn Rock 1 15 98 0 1 0 0 1

Totals 45 636.5 42 10 8 28 8 4

2182 STEPHEN F. NORTON Ecology, Vol. 72, No. 6

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 4: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

COTTID COMMUNITY STRUCTURE

cific patterns of habitat use with patterns of habitat availability, and to assess the degree to which inter- specific competition appears to modify patterns of spe- cies abundance and resource use. I examined three patterns that would be expected if intraguild compe- tition is a strong force influencing the distribution and abundance ofcottid species: disjunct distributions, niche divergence, and inverse density relationships. Al- though other mechanisms besides competition may also be consistent with these patterns, invalidating these patterns would decrease the likelihood that intraguild competition was a strong mechanism influencing the distributions and abundances of these species.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study area

I conducted this study in the waters around the San Juan Islands of Washington state, USA. The San Juan Islands lie at the junction of Puget Sound, the Strait of Juan de Fuca, and Barkely Sound (Fig. 1). The complex topography of these islands (Mayers and Bennett 1973) and the mixed semidiurnal tides (Herlinveaux 1954) interact to create a variety of physical habitats (e.g., deep mud-bottomed fjords, current-swept rock reefs, sand flats) within a small area. These highly productive waters are well known for their rich marine life (MacPhee and Clemens 1962, Ellis 1971, Kozloff 1973, Foreman and Root 1975). Cottids are no exception; I have found in the San Juan Islands 31 of the 90 species described from the northeast Pacific (S. F. Norton, personal observation).

Fish and habitat sampling

Fish transects were conducted at six primary sites (4-12 transects/site) and two secondary study sites (one transect/site) (Fig. 1) in the summer of 1985. These sites are mosaics of several common habitat types in the nearshore subtidal in the San Juan Islands (Table 1). Most of the 45 transects were 30 m long, but several of the deeper transects were shorter due to time con- straints imposed by scuba-diving at these depths. Each transect was placed parallel to shore within a narrow depth range, and an effort was made to maximize the

diversity of substrates sampled by each transect. The influence of depth on cottid abundances was evaluated by running transects at several depths (between 7 and 30 m) at each primary site.

The initial pass along each transect sampled habitat use and density of cottids; a second sampled the avail- ability of different substrates along the transect. During the first pass I recorded the species and the substrate on which it was lying for each cottid individual en- countered within 0.5 m of the right side of the transect line. I carried a 0.5-m rod as a reference width for the transect. To accurately sample these cryptic sedentary fishes I swam slowly along the bottom at - 1.5 m/min. These data were recorded in 5-m blocks. During the second pass I sampled the availability of different sub- strates (microhabitats) by recording the substrate under the transect line every 10 cm. Observations from pre- liminary dives were used to identify six dominant sub- strates: (1) clean rock (CR), characteristic of high-cur- rent sites; (2) silt rock (SR), rock covered with a fine layer of silt, characteristic of low-current sites; (3) shell sand (SS), a complex of shell fragments and sand that collect below rockwalls; (4) gravel (Gr); (5) mud (M); and (6) foliose algae (A).

Data analysis

I examined the influence of biotic and abiotic factors on the distribution and abundance of cottids in two ways. The microhabitat analysis compared the distri- bution of individuals of each cottid species to the dis- tribution of substrates available along the transects. This analysis also examined the distributional patterns of species pairs for disjunct distributions and niche shifts. I defined a microhabitat as a physically uniform area of substrate (e.g., shell sand, mud, etc.) within which a cottid might spend part of the day. In the macrohabitat analysis I applied the regression tech- nique of Schoener (1974b) and Crowell and Pimm (1976) to examine the role that substrate abundance and the densities of other cottid species have on the density of each species along the transects. Each tran- sect was viewed as a macrohabitat containing a unique distribution of microhabitats.

Afen = A. fenestralis; Asem = Asemichthys taylori; Chit = Chitonotus pugetensis; Ice = Icelinus borealis; and Jord Jordania zonope.

Cottid density (Indsm -2 transect- ', X + 1 SD)

Ahar

0.81 + 0.17 0.68 ±+ 0.29

0 0

0.31 ± 0.42 0.37 + 0.42

0 2.60

0.41 ± 0.53

Afen

0 0

0.24 + 0.23 1.35 ± 0.98

0 0 0.13 0

0.20 + 0.52

Asem

0.07 ± 0.13 0.63 ± 0.52 0.13 + 0.18 0.96 + 0.47 0.09 + 0.11 0.40 + 0.20

0 0

0.31 + 0.42

Chit

0 0

0.29 ± 0.26 0.72 + 0.45

0 0 0 0

0.13 ± 0.29

Ice

0.38 ± 0.39 0.08 ±_ 0.11

0 0

0.73 + 0.56 1.63 ± 1.34

0.47 0

0.42 ± 0.67

Jord

0.51 ± 0.26 0.80 + 0.39

0 0.07 + 0.05 0.34 ± 0.22 0.75 + 0.57

0.13 0.53

0.39 + 0.39

December 1991 2183

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 5: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

STEPHEN F. NORTON

Microhabitat analysis

Intersite consistency in the microhabitat distribution of a species was measured using Kendall's coefficient of concordance and was tested for significance by Friedman's rank test (Kendall 1962). Because no cottid species was found at all sites, the rank tests for each species only included those sites where the species was observed. For both Artedius fenestralis and Chitonotus pugetensis, encountered only at the two East Sound sites, the coefficient of rank concordance and Fried- man's test were evaluated among transects.

A view of the overall distribution of each species among subtidal microhabitats in the San Juan Islands was constructed by combining the data on the micro- habitat association of individuals over all transects. The hypothesis that individual species were distributed among microhabitats in proportion to the availabilities of these microhabitats was evaluated with G tests. Be- cause all species demonstrated non-random habitat use, additional tests were conducted to determine which microhabitats were significantly "preferred" or "avoided" by each species (i.e., over- and under-rep- resented). In these tests, the observed microhabitat dis- tributions on a single substrate and on the sum of the other substrates were compard to expected distribution of individuals on these two substrate classes if cottid distributions were based on microhabitat availability. Each microhabitat was singled out in turn (e.g., com- parison of the observed distribution of Artedius har- ringtoni on shell sand and all other substrates with the distribution expected if A. harringtoni used shell sand and the other substrates in the frequencies sampled on the transects). The critical G values were adjusted to reflect the a posteriori nature of these comparisons (Sokal and Rohlf 1981).

If competition among cottid species is influencing the microhabitat distribution, one might predict that species pairs would show checkerboard or disjunct dis- tributions, where one species excludes another from an area. This hypothesis was evaluated by totaling the number of 5-m transect sections (2.5 m2) where (1) both species of a pair were present, (2) both species were absent, and (3) only one species was present. Only transects where at least one member of a species pair was present were included, reducing the interdepen- dence of the comparisons (Toft et al. 1982). Indepen- dence in the distribution of species pairs among 5-m transect sections was evaluated using G tests.

Even if species pairs do not show checkerboard dis- tributions, competition by one member of a species pair may influence the microhabitat distribution of the other. If this occurs, one would expect the species pairs to show higher overlap in microhabitat use when in allopatry than when in sympatry (i.e., niche diver- gence). The potential of one species to alter the distri- bution of another was evaluated using G tests that com- pared the microhabitat distribution of one species in

those 5-m transect sections that also contained the po- tential competitor (sympatry, i.e., transects containing both members of a species pair) with the microhabitat distribution in 5-m transect sections that lacked the potential competitor (allopatry, i.e., transects contain- ing only one member of a species pair). An overlap index (Schoener 1970) was used to calculate micro- habitat overlap between members of species pairs in sympatry and in allopatry.

Under different scenarios of competition, resource overlap may vary directly or inversely with the inten- sity of competition (Lawlor 1980). One difficulty with traditional overlap measures as approximate measures of competition is that overlap measures based on pro- portional utilization confound the electivity of the con- sumer with the availability of resources in the envi- ronment (Lawlor 1980). Lawlor proposed that similarity of electivities between two species may be a better measure of potential competition. I used the asym- metric MacArthur-Levins equation to calculate both interspecific microhabitat overlap, based on propor- tional utilization, and microhabitat similarity, based on electivities determined using Manly's (1974) for- mula. The former measure is an estimate of overlap of the realized niches of the species pairs, while the latter should be a better indicator of overlap of the fundamental niche of these species.

Macrohabitat analysis

The method of Schoener (1974b) and Crowell and Pimm (1976), as modified by Rosenzweig et al. (1984), was used to examine the influence of substrate abun- dance and the density of potential competitors on cot- tid densities. In the Schoener-Crowell-Pimm (SCP) method, an initial multiple regression uses the density of a single species measured at several sites as the de- pendent variable and several habitat variables (e.g., rainfall, percentage cover of different substrates, tem- perature, etc.) as the independent variables. The re- gression coefficients provide estimates of the influence of the habitat variables on species abundance. The re- siduals of this regression should be estimates of species densities at each site that are largely free of habitat heterogeneity. A second set of multiple regressions uses the matrix of these residuals for all the species in the guild to assess the strength of interspecific interactions. Each species serves as the dependent variable and is regressed against the residuals of the other species. Neg- ative interspecific regression coefficients derived from the SCP technqiue have been invoked as evidence for competition (e.g., Crowell and Pimm 1976, Hallett and Pimm 1979, Dueser and Hallett 1980, Rosenzweig et al. 1984, Dueser and Porter 1986, but see Rosenzweig et al. 1985 and Abramsky et al. 1986). Simulation studies indicate that competition coefficients calculated by the SCP technique are robust to deviations of pop- ulations away from equilibrium (Hallett and Pimm

2184 Ecology, Vol. 72, No. 6

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 6: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

COTTID COMMUNITY STRUCTURE

TABLE 2. Rank correlation of microhabitats. Friedman's test, a non-parametric multisample rank-correlation statistic, was used to test for consistency of habitat use among sites or among transects (species designated with *). Kendall's W is indicative of the sample correlation coefficient.

No. of No. of sub-

Species sites strates W P

Artedius harringtoni 5 6 0.57 <.025 Asemichthys taylori 6 6 0.60 <.01 Icelinus borealis 4 6 0.43 <.25 Jordania zonope 6 6 0.51 <.01 Artedius fenestralis* 12 5 0.33 <.01 Chitonotus pugetensis* 12 5 0.48 <.001

1979) and for problems with the independence of the variables (Carnes and Slade 1988).

In this study the seven independent variables for the habitat-species regressions were the percentage cover estimates for the six substrate types on each transect and the depth of each transect. In the species-species regressions the residual densities of a single species from the habitat regressions served as the dependent variable and the residual densities of the other species were the independent variables. The distribution of percentage cover data was not significantly different from normal after arcsine square-root transformation. Similarly, the distribution of the density data was nor- mally distributed after square-root transformation.

In both stepwise regressions (habitat-species and species-species) a minimum significance level of 0.15 was required at each step before entry of any variable into the overall model. Other regression approaches (full regression models or models using the original cottid densities as independent variables in the species- species regressions) were explored as suggested by Ro- senzweig et al. (1984), but were poorer descriptors than the stepwise method; i.e., they accounted for less of the variation in cottid density. There were strong quali- tative similarities in the results regardless of the type of regression model. Examination of plots of the re- siduals vs. the independent variables of the habitat- species and species-species regressions revealed no ob- vious curvilinear relationships (i.e., no evidence for concave zero-isoclines; Schoener 1974b).

RESULTS

Cottids are abundant in the nearshore subtidal of the San Juan Islands; eight species totaling 1172 individ- uals were encountered during 45 transects. Cottid den- sity was 1.8 ± 0.16 individuals per square metre per transect (mean + 1 SE). The six common cottids were Artedius fenestralis, A. harringtoni, Asemichthys tay- lori, Chitonotus pugetensis, Icelinus borealis, and Jor- dania zonope (Table 1). Two other cottid species, Hemilepidotus hemilepidotus (N = 1) and Rhampho- cottus richardsoni (N = 8) were rare and are excluded in the analyses. Fishes sampled on the transects in- cluded both newly settled juveniles and adults of all

species. Other fishes encountered occasionally during the transects included Pholis clemensi, Ronquilus jot- dani, the rockfishes Sebastes auriculatus, S. caurinus, S. emphaeus, and S. flavidus, and the hexagrammids Ophiodon elongatus and Hexagrammos decagram- mus.

Selection of microhabitats by cottid fishes

The microhabitat distributions of cottid species were generally consistent among sites or transects (Table 2). None of the cottid species were found to use micro- habitats in proportion to their availability along the transects (Fig. 2; G tests, P < .001); each species showed significant microhabitat preferences and avoidances. Both J. zonope and Artedius harringtoni were found predominately on clean rock surfaces and avoided non- solid substrates. C. pugetensis and Asemichthys taylori were found almost exclusively on shell sand and avoid- ed clean rock. Encounters with A rtediusfenestralis were divided between shell sand and silt-covered rock. I. borealis was common on gravel substrates and rare on silt rock and shell sand.

Interspecific microhabitat overlap (based on realized overlap of resource use) and interspecific microhabitat similarity (based on overlap of resource electivities) varied greatly among the species pairs (Table 3; over- lap: mean ± 1 SD = 0.46 ± 0.42; similarity: mean + 1 SD = 0.40 ± 0.25). These measures of the overlap of actual and potential resource use were highly correlated (r2 = 0.58, F = 38.7, P < .001). Nine of 30 overlap coefficients and 4 of 30 similarity coefficients were in the "biologically significant" range, >0.60 (see refer- ences in Grossman 1986a). High overlap values were found for J. zonope and A. harringtoni, Asemichthys taylori and C. pugetensis, Artedius fenestralis and C. pugetensis (all reciprocal), and for J. zonope and A.

TABLE 3. Two measures of interspecific overlap in the spatial distributions of six common cottid species in the San Juan Islands. The table shows the overlap or similarity of the column species vs. the row species. Species abbreviations as in Table 1.

Ahar Afen Asem Chit Ice Jord

A. Asymmetrical interspecific microhabitat overlap (Mac- Arthur-Levins index). Ahar 0.091 0.123 0.081 0.557 1.076 Afen 0.156 1.068 0.943 0.290 0.116 Asem 0.102 0.518 0.843 0.219 0.058 Chit 0.090 0.609 1.122 0.236 0.043 Ice 1.238 0.376 0.585 0.475 1.296 Jord 0.925 0.058 0.060 0.034 0.501

B. Asymmetrical interspecific electivity similarity (MacAr- thur-Levins index). Ahar 0.428 0.238 0.169 0.499 1.134 Afen 0.367 0.507 0.462 0.336 0.404 Asem 0.134 0.333 0.696 0.284 0.081 Chit 0.131 0.420 0.965 0.349 0.115 Ice 0.502 0.394 0.507 0.450 0.452 Jord 0.793 0.329 0.101 0.103 0.315

December 1991 2185

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 7: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

STEPHEN F. NORTON

100 A. fenestralis

80 - 126 individuals

60 -

40 I-

20 - *

CR SR G A M SS

i(O 100 ** ** Jor~dania .Asemichthys

8 80( - 247 individuals ni

80 - 183 individuals

° 60- . 60 -

D 40O - 20- 20

60 c 60 I

° CRSRG AMSS | °CR SR G A M SS

I. borealis Chitonotus 80 - 260 individuals 80 - 87 individuals

60 60-

40 40

20- 20

CRSRG AMSS 0R SR G A M SS

100

80

60

40

0

CR SR G A M SS Substrate Type

FIG. 2. Microhabitat associations of six cottid species and the frequency of occurrence ofmicrohabitats along the transects. Significant positive or negative or negative substrate associations are indicated: * P .05, ** P < .01. Habitat abbreviations: CR = clean rock, SR = silt rock, G = gravel, A = algae, M = mud, SS = shell sand.

harringtoni on I. borealis, and Asemichythys taylori on Artedius fenestralis. High microhabitat similarities were found for J. zonope and A. harringtoni, and for Asem- ichthys taylori and C. pugetensis (each reciprocal). Both indices indicate that if substrate availabilities were lim- ited, competition was likely to occur among these cot- tid species pairs.

In contrast to the expectation of negative co-occur- rence patterns under the competition paradigm, six cottid species pairs showed neutral co-occurrence pat-

terns and three had positive co-occurrence patterns; none were negative (Fig. 3). Another possibility con- sistent with competition is that a species may alter its microhabitat use in the presence of a competitor (niche divergence). Although several species pairs did show significant differences in patterns of microhabitat use in the presence of potential competitors, interspecific overlap in microhabitat use was higher in sympatry than in allopatry, opposite to predictions of niche di- vergence under competition (Fig. 4).

1(00

80

60

40

20

2186 Ecology, Vol. 72, No. 6

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 8: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

COTTID COMMUNITY STRUCTURE

Species Co-occurrence

A. fenestralis ------------------- . Chitonotus

Asemichthys

Icelinus- | A. harringtoni

Jordania

A- -- positive -------- neutral

FIG. 3. Co-occurrence of species pairs in 5-m transect sections. Species pairs connected by arrows demonstrated significant positive co-occurrence patterns. Species pairs connected by dotted lines demonstrated independent patterns of occurrence.

Influence of habitat structure and heterospecifics on cottid densities

Densities of each species were significantly influ- enced by the relative abundances of particular habitat types in an area (Table 4). Stepwise regressions using the macrohabitat variables account for between 40 and 60% of the variance in species density (Table 4: R2 Habitat). As with microhabitat associations, the den- sity of each species demonstrated significant positive and negative correlations with areas dominated by par- ticular habitat types. Densities of Artedius fenestralis

and Asemichthys taylori were higher on transects with a high percentage cover of silt rock and shell sand. The density of Artedius harringtoni decreased with increas- ing depth and increased in areas dominated by clean rock surfaces. Abundance of C. pugetensis showed neg- ative relationships with clean rock, gravel, and algal abundances. The density of J. zonope was positively correlated with the percentage cover of clean rock and algae. Abundance of I. borealis increased with increas- ing depth and in areas dominated by gravel and algae, but this species avoided silt rock and shell sand.

Allopatric - Sympatric Overlap

A. fenestralis < 0.36 -0.67 > Chitonotus

Asemichthys

| Icelinus | 1 0.57 + 0.63 | ! A. harringtoni

I Jordania I

FIG. 4. Changes in microhabitat use between sympatry and allopatry and microhabitat overlap in allopatry (left value) and sympatry (right value) for cottid species pairs. Overlap measures were calculated using the index described in Schoener (1970). An arrow connecting two species indicates that the species at the head of the arrow demonstrates significant differences in its distribution among substrates when in sympatry vs. in allopatry on 5-m transect sections with the species at the tail of the arrow.

December 1991 2187

I

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 9: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

TABLE 4. Stepwise regression coefficients of cottid densities vs. habitat variables and of species vs. species residuals after removal of habitat effects. Species and habitat abbreviations as in Table 1.

Independent Dependent variables Independent

variables Ahar Afen Asem Chit Ice Jord

CR 9.133** -8.37** 8.25** SR 12.43** 3.84t -6.98* G -4.28* 6.60t SS 4.72* 11.00** -4.43t M A -14.16** 29.34** 10.40t Depth -0.07t G.19**

Ahar 0.32* Afen -0.40* 0.05** -0.29t Asem 0.38* 0.24t Chit 1.02** Ice -0.21t Jord 0.30t 0.35*

R2 Habitat 0.400 0.440 0.448 0.608 0.450 0.407 R2 Density 0.214 0.258 0.072 0.180 0.031 0.150 R2 Total 0.614 0.698 0.520 0.788 0.481 0.557

t Significant at P < .15; t significant at P < .10; * significant at P < .05; ** significant at P < .01.

From the results of the second series of regressions, the subtidal cottid community in the San Juan Islands shows little evidence of being structured by competi- tion for space. Of 30 possible interspecific coefficients, only 10 passed the initial significance cutoff (P < .15) in the stepwise regression models; 6 of these are sig- nificant at the .05 level or better (Table 4). Only one of these six coefficients is negative (A. fenestralis on A. harringtoni). The interspecific regression models ac- count for between 3 and 26% of the total variation in cottid densities (Table 4: R2 Density). Together the habitat and interaction regressions account for between 48 and 79% of the variation in the initial densities of the cottid species (Table 4: R2 Total).

Diffuse competition (the aggregate influence on one species by the other members of a guild, MacArthur 1972) does not appear to be important in this guild. I conducted a regression analysis for each species in which the dependent variable was the residual density of the single species and the independent variable was the sum of the residual densities of all the other species after habitat effects had been removed. None of the regressions indicated a negative relationship as pre- dicted under a diffuse competition model (Table 5). On the contrary, abundances of both Asemichthys tay- lori and J. zonope were significantly higher in areas with high densities of other cottids.

DISCUSSION

The results from this study indicate that subtidal cottids in the San Juan Islands are distributed differ- entially among six common structural habitats. The patterns of distributions and the abundances of these species are more consistent with mechanisms based on autecological responses of species to spatial heteroge- neity in the distribution of habitats (the individualistic concept, Gleason 1926) than they are with synecolog-

ical mechanisms. Under the individualistic concept, differences in resource use among guild members (re- source partitioning sensu lato, e.g., Schoener 1974a, Ross 1986) reflect the unique adaptations and evolu- tionary histories of each species, and not coevolution- ary interactions among guild member (resource par- titioning sensu stricto, Roughgarden 1976). The individualistic concept has been invoked to explain niche differences in a variety of systems (Strong 1983), e.g., terrestrial plant communities (Gleason 1926), aquatic snails (Aho et al. 1981), shrub-steppe birds (Wiens and Rotenberry 1981), and temperate fishes (Grossman et al. 1982, Ebeling and Laur 1986).

Influence of habitat factors

The influence of habitat on species distributions may change when examined over different spatial scales (e.g., Wiens and Rotenberry 1981, Morris 1987, Snyder and Best 1988). Within the San Juan Islands, most cottid species demonstrated significant rank consistency in the use of substrates at different sites or among tran- sects. On a broader scale, the habitat distributions of subtidal cottids in the San Juan Islands are similar to qualitative observations of their distributions in other

TABLE 5. Diffuse competition: influence of the aggregate density of other cottid species on the density of single spe- cies after correction for habitat effects.

Regres- sion

Species coefficient F P

Artedius harringtoni 0.025 0.14 >.70 Artedius fenestralis -0.061 1.31 >.25 Asemichthys taylori 0.245 19.73 <.001 Chitonotus pugetensis 0.004 0.02 >.90 Icelinus borealis -0.069 1.34 >.25 Jordania zonope 0.118 7.13 <.05

2188 STEPHEN F. NORTON Ecology, Vol. 72, No. 6

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 10: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

COTTID COMMUNITY STRUCTURE

parts of their ranges (MacPhee and Clemens 1962, Wilkie 1963, Burge and Schultz 1973, Peden and Wil- son 1976, Moulton 1977, Leaman 1980, Yoshiyama et al. 1986).

In this study, habitat structure had similar effects in the micro- and macrohabitat distributions of cottids. The densities of a cottid species were enhanced in those habitats dominated by substrates for which it had high microhabitat electivities. Of the 21 significant micro- habitat associations and 10 significant macrohabitat correlations (i.e., P < .05), seven pairs of habitat effects were shared across the two scales of analysis, e.g., pos- itive microhabitat association and positive macrohab- itat correlation of Artedius harringtoni and clean rock. Conflicts existed for only one species: on a microhab- itat scale Asemichthys taylori was rarely observed on silt rock, but on a macrohabitat scale its overall density was enhanced on transects with a high percentage cover of this substrate. This likely reflects the positive as- sociation of these substrates in East Sound transects. At this low-current site, the source of shell fragments is from barnacle tests from the silt-covered rockwalls.

Studies of intertidal cottid communities have doc- umented a similar strong role of habitat structure on their distributions. Important environmental features correlating with distributional patterns of intertidal cottids include the predominant substrates in the tide- pool and the degree of wave exposure, tidal height, and pool size (Green 1971, Nakamura 1976a, b, Cross 1981, Yoshiyama 1981, Yoshiyama et al. 1986). As with subtidal cottids, the associations of intertidal cottids with particular substrates are relatively consistent throughout their geographic ranges (up to 1000 km) in spite of changes in the composition of the intertidal fish community (e.g., Yoshiyama et al. 1986). Tests of microhabitat choice have shown that the associations of several intertidal species with particular environ- mental features in the field can be replicated in the laboratory and do not change in the absence of poten- tial competitors (Nakamura 1976a, Richkus 1981). Marliave (1977) demonstrated that the substrate pref- erences of several intertidal fishes, including two cot- tids, appear to be set at the time of larval settlement.

Influence of interspecific competition

Three lines of evidence indicate that interspecific competition does not influence the distribution of sub- tidal cottids. First, there is no evidence that some spe- cies excluded others from an area (disjunct distribu- tions). Second, although the presence of other species was associated with changes in the microhabitat as- sociations of several species, the microhabitat overlap between all species pairs was higher in sympatry (tran- sects containing both species) than in allopatry (tran- sects with one species absent), opposite to the expec- tation under typical competitive models. This reflects some convergence of species onto the predominant substrate type in sympatric areas. Finally, using the

Schoener-Crowell-Pimm (SCP) method, only 1 of 30 interspecific interaction coefficients was negative, in- dicating a lack of strong negative correlation of species' densities.

Recently, the SCP method has come under intense scrutiny regarding the mechanics of the technique and the experimental corroboration of its results (Rosenz- weig et al. 1984, Pimm 1985, Schoener 1985, Abram- sky et al. 1986, Cames and Slade 1988). The SCP meth- od has been applied repeatedly to examine the influence of competition in small mammal communities (Crow- ell and Pimm 1976, Dueser and Hallett 1980, Hallett 1982, Porter and Dueser 1982, Rosenzweig et al. 1984); a consistent result of these studies has been that an overwhelming predominance of significant and nega- tive interspecific interactions remain after removal of habitat effects. This pattern has been interpreted to be the result of strong competition among members of these communities, but experimental verification of these putative competitive interactions has yet to ap- pear.

In the one experimental test of the SCP method, it failed to detect consistently competition between two bee species that were shown experimentally to be strong competitors (Abramsky et al. 1986). The predictions of the SCP method were upheld when the experimental manipulations of bee populations were conducted over a range of densities similar to those used in the re- gressions, but failed when the experiments were con- ducted at densities far beyond the range used in the regression analysis. Pimm (1985) has argued that the "failure" of this second test may less reflect inadequa- cies of the SCP method than inherent difficulties with linear extrapolations beyond the range of observed val- ues, especially if competitive effects are non-linear as predicted in several theoretical treatments (Rosenz- weig 1981, Schoener 1985).

The SCP method for calculating interspecific inter- action coefficients may be viewed as a conservative test of competition. It assumes that habitat effects on species distributions have priority over interspecific effects. As originally formulated, this arrangement was necessary to adjust species densities at multiple sites that differed in the equilibrium densities of potential competitors due to differences in the distribution of habitats (one of the major drawbacks of the observa- tional approach) before comparing species densities (Schoener 1974b). One result of this arrangement is that some of the variation in cottid densities attributed to habitat in the initial habitat-species stepwise re- gression may actually be niche partitioning caused by competitive interactions among species. This would reduce the ability of the SCP technique to detect sig- nificant negative species interactions.

On the other hand, some of the variation in cottid densities attributed to species interactions in the spe- cies-species regressions may be due to covariance be- tween environmental factors not included in the initial

December 1991 2189

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 11: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

STEPHEN F. NORTON

habitat regressions (e.g., food supply, substrate relief, etc.) and species densities. The effects of these "hidden" habitat factors on species densities would not be "cor- rected" when calculating the residual densities of some species. The positive associations between several pairs ofcottids (Table 4) are probably due to such covariance and not to mutualistic interactions between species.

Although mindful of the drawbacks of the SCP ap- proach, it has fulfilled one of the advantages accrued when using an observational approach to examine re- source partitioning within complex assemblages, i.e., identification of specific physical factors that are most likely to influence the distribution and abundance of a cottid species and identification of the species pairs that show inverse density relationships consistent with interspecific competition. These results predict that ex- perimental manipulations of most cottid species are not likely to reveal strong competitive interactions. The one negative interaction coefficient, Artedius fe- nestralis on A. harringtoni, may be an artifact due to covariance with an environmental variable not includ- ed in the original analysis (e.g., current speed), or may be due to strong competitive interactions between closely related species that have similar diets (Norton 1989). Experimental manipulations of these two spe- cies are necessary to distinguish between these possi- bilities.

Another line of evidence consistent with a general lack of interspecific competition among subtidal cot- tids is the lack of a correlation between cottid diversity and habitat diversity (Shannon-Weiner index, H') along the subtidal transects, despite strong association of in- dividual cottid species with habitat variables (r2 = 0.047, F = 3.784, P > .05). Other studies have documented a positive relationship between complexity of the phys- ical environment and species diversity (see references in Gorman and Karr 1978, Boecklen 1986). This ob- servation implies that the subtidal cottid community may be undersaturated; some transects lack either the full complement of species and/or individuals that one would expect given the available habitats. Undersatu- ration may be due to mortality caused by predators (e.g., Paine 1974) or by physical processes (Sousa 1979, Grossman et al. 1982), or to recruitment limitations (Shulman 1985, Victor 1986).

Comparison of intertidal and subtidal cottid communities

Several authors have concluded that the distribu- tions of rocky intertidal fishes are strongly structured by competition for space (Yoshiyama 1981, Cross 1981, Yoshiyama et al. 1986). These conclusions rest on ob- servations of apparent resource partitioning via niche complementarity (Cross 1981, Yoshiyama 1981), of reciprocal patterns of abundance (Cross 1981), of in- creased microhabitat breadth in areas of decreased spe- cies richness (Cross 1981), and of high microhabitat overlap (Yoshiyama 1981). Results from experiments

that manipulated the densities of putative competitors (Yoshiyama 1981) or the abundance of spatial re- sources (Cross 1981) have been inconclusive. How- ever, Grossman (1986a) has argued that strong com- petition does exist among intertidal fishes, but that the limiting resource is food, not space.

In light of the apparent importance of competition among members of the intertidal assemblage, it is sur- prising that the subtidal cottid community of the San Juan Islands shows little evidence of competitive reg- ulation, especially in the absence of other structuring forces (e.g., predation or physical disturbance). Ex- amination of the diets of other potential predators and competitors (e.g., rockfishes and hexagrammids) in- dicates that these fishes rely primarily on prey sizes and types that are not utilized by cottid fishes, and cottids are not normally encountered in the diets of these species (Moulton 1977; F. S. McEuen, personal communication, S. F. Norton, personal observation). Also, physical disturbance is much less pronounced in the subtidal than in the intertidal (Cross 1981, Levings et al. 1983).

The densities of subtidal cottids may be kept below levels necessary to saturate the available resources by recruitment processes, especially when compared to the densities of intertidal species. The transport mech- anisms (the strong currents in the San Juan Islands and a larval period of several weeks or months for cottids) seem sufficient to ensure access of potential recruits to all intertidal and subtidal habitats. However, the sup- ply of recruits of subtidal species may be limited by heavy predation on benthic eggs in the subtidal by invertebrates and by other fishes (DeMartini 1978).

In conclusion, habitat partitioning by subtidal cottid species is more likely the result of inherent micro- and macrohabitat preferences of individual species for par- ticular substrates rather than responses to the presence ofheterospecifics. The lack of any relationship between species diversity and habitat structural diversity de- spite strong habitat preferences implies that subtidal cottid populations are below environmental carrying capacity. In the absence of great physical disturbance and predation pressure, population regulation may be due to recruitment limitation, especially through egg predation.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I would like to thank my dive partners, T. Miller, D. Hol- lister, and S. Herbert, for their assistance in collecting the transect data. Lab space and diving facilities were provided by Friday Harbor Laboratories. P. Raimondi, A. Ebeling, and D. Reed provided helpful comments on the analysis. The comments of A. Ebeling, S. Sweet, R. Warner, M. Carr, and G. Grossman greatly improved the manuscript. This research was supported by grants from Sigma Xi, the Theodore Roo- sevelt Fund, the Lerner Fund, the Raney Fund, and from the Graduate Division of UCSB.

LITERATURE CITED

Abramsky, Z., M. A. Bowers, and M. L. Rosenzweig. 1986. Detecting interspecific competition in the field: testing the regression method. Oikos 47:199-204.

2190 Ecology, Vol. 72, No. 6

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 12: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

COT TID COMMUNITY STRUCTIURE

Aho, J., E. Ranta, and J. Vourinen. 1981. Species compo- sition of freshwater snail communities in lakes of south and west Finland. Annales Zoologici Fennici 18:233-242.

Boecklen, W. J. 1986. Effects of habitat heterogeneity on the species-area relationships of forest birds. Journal of Biogeography 13:59-68.

Brown, J. H., D. W. Davidson, J. C. Munger, and R. S. Inouye. 1986. Experimental community ecology: the desert grani- vore system. Pages 41-61 in J. Diamond and T. J. Case, editors. Community ecology. Harper & Row, New York, New York, USA.

Burge, R. T., and S. A. Schultz. 1973. The marine environ- ment in the vicinity of Diable Cove with special reference to abalones and bony fishes. Marine Resources Technical Report Number 19, California Department of Fish and Game, Sacramento, California, USA.

Buss, L. W., and J. B. C. Jackson. 1979. Competitive net- works: nontransitive competitive relationships in cryptic coral reef environments. American Naturalist 113:223-234.

Carnes, B. A., and N. A. Slade. 1988. The use of regression for detecting competition with multicollinear data. Ecology 69:1266-1274.

Case, T. J. 1983. Niche overlap and the assembly of island lizard communities. Oikos 41:427-433.

Cody, M. L. 1974. Competition and the structure of bird communities. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey, USA.

Connell, J. H. 1975. Some mechanisms producing structure in natural communities; a model and evidence from field experiments. Pages 460-490 in M. L. Cody and J. Dia- mond, editors. Ecology and evolution of communities. Belknap, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA.

Cross, J. N. 1981. Structure of a rocky intertidal fish assem- blage. Dissertation. College of Fisheries, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington, USA.

Crowell, K. L., and S. L. Pimm. 1976. Competition and niche shifts of mice introduced onto islands. Oikos 27:251- 258.

DeMartini, E. E. 1978. Spatial aspects of reproduction in buffalo sculpin, Enophrvs bison. Environmental Biology of Fishes 3:331-336.

Diamond, J. 1986. Overview: laboratory experiments, field experiments, and natural experiments. Pages 3-22 in J. Diamond and T. J. Case, editors. Community ecology. Harper & Row, New York, New York, USA.

Dueser, R. D., and J. H. Hallett. 1980. Competition and habitat selection in a forest-floor small mammal fauna. Oikos 35:293-297.

Dueser, R. D., and J. H. Porter. 1986. Habitat use by insular small mammals: relative effects of competition and habitat structure. Ecology 67:195-201.

Ebeling, A. E., R. J. Larson, and W. S. Alevizon. 1980. Habitat groups and island-mainland distribution of kelp- bed fishes off Santa Barbara, California. Pages 403-431 in D. M. Powers, editor. Multidisciplinary symposium on the California islands. Santa Barbara Museum of Natural His- tory, Santa Barbara, California, USA.

Ebeling, A. E., and D. R. Laur. 1986. Foraging in surfperch- es: resource partitioning or individualistic responses? Pages 123-133 in C. A. Simenstad and G. M. Cailliet, editors. Contemporary studies in fish feeding. Dr. W. Junk, The Hague, The Netherlands.

Ellis, D. V. 1971. A review of marine infaunal community studies in the Strait of Georgia and adjacent inlets. Syesis 4:3-9.

Foreman, R. E., and J. Root. 1975. Macrophyte studies in the Strait of Georgia. Benthic ecology research program report 75-2. Department of Botany, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.

Gause, G. F. 1934. Struggle for existence. Hafner, New York, New York, USA.

Gleason, H. A. 1926. The individualistic concept of plant association. Bulletin of the Torrey Botanical Club 53:7-26.

Gorman, 0. T., and J. R. Karr. 1978. Habitat structure and stream fish communities. Ecology 59:507-515.

Graham, J. B. 1970. Temperature sensitivity of two species of intertidal fishes. Copeia 1970:49-56.

Grant, P., and D. Schluter. 1984. Interspecific competition inferred from patterns of guild structure. Pages 201-233 in D. R. Strong, D. Simberloff, L. G. Abele, and A. B. Thistle, editors. Ecological communities: conceptual issues and the evidence. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jer- sey, USA.

Green, J. N. 1971. Local distribution of Oligocottus maculo- sus Girard and other tidepool cottids of the west coast of Vancouver Island, British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology 49:1111-1128.

Grossman, G. D. 1982. Dynamics and organization of a rocky intertidal fish assemblage: the persistence and resil- ience of taxocene structure. American Naturalist 119:611- 637.

1986a. Food resource partitioning in a rocky inter- tidal fish assemblage. Journal of Zoology, London 1:317- 355.

· 1986b. Long term persistence in a rocky intertidal fish assemblage. Environmental Biology of Fishes 15:315- 317.

Grossman, G. D., P. B. Moyle, and J. 0. Whitaker, Jr. 1982. Stochasticity in structural and functional characteristics of an Indiana stream fish assemblage: tests of community the- ory. American Naturalist 120:423-454.

Hairston, N. G. 1980. Evolution under interspecific com- petition: field experiments on terrestrial salamanders. Evo- lution 34:409-420.

Hallett, J. G. 1982. Habitat selection and the community matrix of a desert small-mammal fauna. Ecology 63:1400- 1410.

Hallett, J. G., and S. L. Pimm. 1979. Direct estimation of competition. American Naturalist 113:593-600.

Herlinveaux, R. H. 1954. Tidal currents in the Juan de Fuca Strait. Journal of the Fisheries Research Board of Canada 11:799-805.

Hixon, M. A. 1980. Competitive interactions between Cal- ifornia reef fishes in the genus Embiotica. Ecology 61:918- 931.

Howe, K. M., and S. L. Richardson. 1978. Taxonomic re- view and meristic variation in marine sculpins (Osteich- thys: Cottidae) of the northeast Pacific Ocean. Final Report, NOAA-NMFS contract number 03-78-M02-120. National Marine Fisheries Service, Seattle, Washington, USA.

Kendall, M. G. 1962. Rank correlation methods. Charles Griffin, London, England.

Kozloff, E. N. 1973. Seashore life of Puget Sound, the Strait of Georgia, and the San Juan Archipelago. University of Washington Press, Seattle, Washington, USA.

Larson, R. J., and E. E. DeMartini. 1984. Abundance and vertical distribution of fishes in a cobble-bottom kelp forest off San Onofre, California. Fishery Bulletin 82:37-53.

Lawlor, L. R. 1980. Overlap, similarity, and competition coefficients. Ecology 61:245-251.

Leaman, B. M. 1980. The ecology of fishes in British Co- lumbia kelp beds. Fisheries Development Report Number 22, Ministry of Environment, Vancouver, British Colum- bia, Canada.

Levings, C. D., R. E. Foreman, and V. J. Tunnicliffe. 1983. Review of the benthos of the Strait of Georgia and contig- uous fjords. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 40:1120-1141.

MacArthur, R. H. 1972. Geographical ecology. Patterns in

December 1991 2191

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 13: Habitat Use and Community Structure in an Assemblage of Cottid Fishes

STEPHEN F. NORTON

the distribution of species. Harper & Row, New York, New York, USA.

MacPhee, C., and W. A. Clemens. 1962. Fishes of the San Juan Archipelago, Washington. Northwest Science 36:27- 38.

Marliave, J. B. 1977. Substratum preferences of settling lar- vae of marine fishes reared in the laboratory. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 27:47-60.

Manly, B. F., Jr. 1974. A model for certain types of selection experiments. Biometrics 30:281-294.

Mayers, I. R., and L. C. Bennett, Jr. 1973. Geology of the Strait of Juan de Fuca. Marine Geology 15:89-117.

McGuinness, K. A. 1988. Short-term effects of sessile or- ganisms on colonization of intertidal boulders. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 116:159-175.

Morris, D. W. 1987. Ecological scale and habitat use. Ecol- ogy 68:362-369.

Morris, R. W. 1960. Temperature, salinity, and southern limits of three species of Pacific cottid fishes. Limnology and Oceanography 5:175-179.

. 1961. Distribution and temperature sensitivity of some eastern Pacific cottid fishes. Physiological Zoology 34:217-227.

Moulton, L. L. 1977. An ecological analysis of fishes in- habiting the rocky nearshore regions of northern Puget Sound, Washington. Dissertation. University of Washing- ton, Seattle, Washington, USA.

Nakamura, R. 1976a. Experimental assessment of factors influencing microhabitat selection by the two tidepool fish- es, Oligocottus maculosus and 0. snyderi. Marine Biology 37:97-104.

1 976b. Temperature and the vertical distribution of two tidepool fishes (Oligocottus maculosus, 0. snyderi). Copeia 1976:143-152.

Norton, S. F. 1989. Constraints on the foraging ecology of subtidal cottid fishes. Dissertation. Department of Biolog- ical Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Cal- ifornia, USA.

Pacala, S., and J. Roughgarden. 1982. Resource partitioning and interspecific competition in two two-species insular Anolis lizard communities. Science 217:444-446.

Paine, R. T. 1974. Intertidal community structure: experi- mental studies on the relationship between a dominant competitor and its principle predator. Oecologia (Berlin) 15:93-120.

Peden, A. E., and D. E. Wilson. 1976. Distribution of in- tertidal and subtidal fishes of northern British Columbia and southeastern Alaska. Syesis 9:221-248.

Pimm, S. L. 1985. Estimating competition coefficients from census data. Oecologia (Berlin) 67:588-590.

Porter, J. H., and R. D. Dueser. 1982. Niche overlap and competition in an insular small mammal fauna: a test of the niche overlap hypothesis. Oikos 39:228-236.

Richkus, W. A. 1981. Laboratory studies of intraspecific behavioral interactions and factors influencing tidepool se- lection of the wooly sculpin, Clinocottus analis. California Fish and Game 67:187-195.

Rosenzweig, M. L. 1981. A theory of habitat selection. Ecol- ogy 62:327-355.

Rosenzweig, M. L., Z. Abramsky, and S. Brand. 1984. Es- timating species interactions in heterogeneous environ- ments. Oikos 43:329-340.

Rosenzweig, M. L., Z. Abramsky, B. Kotler, and W. Mitchell. 1985. Can interaction coefficients be determined from cen- sus data. Oecologia (Berlin) 66:194-198.

Ross, S. T. 1986. Resource partitioning in fish assemblages: a review of field studies. Copeia 1986:352-388.

Roughgarden, J. 1976. Resource partitioning among com-

peting species-a coevolutionary approach. Theoretical Population Biology 9:388-424.

Sale, P. F. 1977. Maintenance of high diversity in coral reef fish communities. American Naturalist 111:337-359.

Schoener, T. W. 1970. Non-synchronous spatial overlap of lizards in patchy habitats. Ecology 51:408-418.

1974a. Resource partitioning in ecologial com- munities. Science 185:27-39.

1974b. Some methods for calculating competition coefficients from resource-utilization spectra. American Naturalist 108:332-340.

1985. On the degree of consistency expected when different methods are used to estimate competition coeffi- cients from census data. Oecologia (Berlin) 67:591-592.

Shulman, M. J. 1985. Variability in recruitment of coral reef fishes. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 89:205-219.

Simenstad, C. A., B. S. Miller, J. N. Cross, K. L. Fresh, S. N. Steinfort, and J. C. Fegley. 1977. Nearshore fish and mac- roinvertebrate assemblages along the Strait of Juan de Fuca including food habits of nearshore fish. Marine Ecosystems Analysis (MESA) Puget Sound Project, Seattle, Washing- ton,USA.

Snyder, E. J., and L. B. Best. 1988. Dynamics of habitat use by small mammals in prairie communities. American Mid- land Naturalist 119:128-136.

Sokal, R. R., and F. J. Rohlf. 1981. Biometry. W. H. Free- man, San Francisco, California, USA.

Sousa, W. P. 1979. Disturbance in marine intertidal boulder fields: the non-equilibrium maintenance of species diver- sity. Ecology 60:1225-1239.

Strong, D. R. 1983. Natural variability and the manifold mechanisms of ecological communities. American Natu- ralist 122:636-660.

Toft, C. A. 1985. Resource partitioning in amphibians and reptiles. Copeia 1985:1-21.

Toft, C. A., D. L. Trauger, and H. W. Murdy. 1982. Tests for species interactions: breeding phenology and habitat use in subarctic ducks. American Naturalist 120:586-613.

Underwood, A. J., and E. J. Denley. 1984. Paradigms, ex- planations, and generalizations in models for the structure of intertidal communities on rocky shores. Pages 151-180 in D. R. Strong, D. Simberloff, L. G. Abele, and A. B. Thistle, editors. Ecological communities: conceptual issues and the evidence. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey, USA.

Victor, B. C. 1986. Larval settlement and juvenile mortality in a recruitment-limited coral reef fish population. Ecolog- ical Monographs 56:145-160.

Wells, A. W. 1986. Aspects of ecology and life history of the woolly sculpin, Clinocottus analis, from southern Cal- ifornia. California Fish and Game 72:213-226.

Werner, E. E. 1977. Species packing and niche complemen- tarity in three sunfishes. American Naturalist 111:553-578.

Wiens, J. A. 1977. On competition and variable environ- ments. American Scientist 65:590-597.

Wiens, J. A., and J. T. Rotenberry. 1981. Habitat associa- tions and community structure of birds in shrubsteppe en- vironments. Ecological Monographs 51:21-41.

Wilkie, D. W. 1963. Occurrence of the longfin sculpin, Jor- dania zonope Starks 1985, in British Columbia. Copeia 1963:588-590.

Yoshiyama, R. M. 1981. Distribution and abundance pat- terns of rocky intertidal fishes in central California. Envi- ronmental Biology of Fishes 6:315-332.

Yoshiyama, R. M., C. Sassaman, and R. N. Lea. 1986. Rocky intertidal fish communities of California: temporal and spa- tial variation. Environmental Biology of Fishes 17:23-40.

2192 Ecology, Vol. 72, No. 6

This content downloaded from 109.154.88.16 on Wed, 16 Jul 2014 16:01:59 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions