gestalt 508

41
GESTALT USES IN THEORY AND PRACTICE: WEAVING A UNIVERSAL PERSPECTIVE By Rede Ballard Submitted to Marco Deyasi In Partial Fulfillment for Art 508, Sec 1 Philosophy and Art Art Department University of Idaho May 2009

Upload: rede-ballard

Post on 27-Jan-2015

146 views

Category:

Technology


9 download

DESCRIPTION

Art 508 paper for Marco Deyasi

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Gestalt 508

GESTALT USES IN THEORY AND PRACTICE:

WEAVING A UNIVERSAL PERSPECTIVE

ByRede Ballard

Submitted to Marco Deyasi In Partial Fulfillment for Art 508, Sec 1

Philosophy and ArtArt Department University of Idaho

May 2009

Page 2: Gestalt 508

Introduction

The major contention I would like to establish in this work is how gestalt, when

used affectively, is a more reliable connective psychological tool in visual

communication then the psychoanalytical approach that has dominated aesthetics over the

past century.

Gestalt has become the guiding influence in my work as well as in my teaching.

It is critical not only in terms of its practical design application to my work, but in my

understanding of what defines visual communication and teaching pedagogy as well;

what has become for me a psychology of connectedness. I feel that it is within most, if

not all, student or viewers to reason through a set of stimuli and diagnose a logical

individually relevant conclusion.

Many of Gestalt theories lay in direct conflict to the psychoanalytical approach to

aesthetics, which emphasizes an emotional nexus as the focus of aesthetic evaluation.

This approach has dominated the field of art criticism as well as production in recent

times and has, I feel, led artist away from the effective potential of their image making.

Gestalt is perhaps summed up best by Max Werthheimer’s quote that the

fundamental “formula” of Gestalt theory might be expressed in this way: “there are

wholes, the behavior of which is not determined by that of their individual elements, but

where the part-processes are themselves determined by the intrinsic nature of the whole.

It is the hope of gestalt theory to determine the nature of such wholes.” 1

Through it’s basic premise of constructing a universal experiential aesthetic

Gestalt allows for me a broader sense of the importance of the artist in human existence.

2

Page 3: Gestalt 508

We are the keepers of dialogue, our role as both mirror and interpreter make us the

guardians of human experience. It is through these theories and shared experience that I

am able to look back retrospectively through the history of art and see why it is that I

have felt connection to a particular number of works by others, and in turn borrowed

aspects of their solutions in an attempt to express a similar human story.

While my ability to fully comprehend the full potential of gestalt theory on my

work and teaching is not complete, it has been instrumental in my thinking about how,

and ultimately why, I, and my students produce art.

“Much more than mere perception, an image of humanity attaches to ordered

Perception. We perceive the bounty afforded by some things and the lack missing

In others.” 2

Historic Overview

The psychological theory of Gestalt began in Germany in the early 1900’s. It is

derived from investigations in psychology, logic, and epistemology. Gestalt has evolved

into a mixture of existential philosophy, phenomenology, holism, humanism, orthodox

and interpersonal psychoanalysis, and Eastern philosophies. It was developed by a group

of German psychologist most notable Max Wertheimer, Kurt Koffka, Wolfgang Kohler,

and Christian von Ehrenfels. Gestalt theory can be traced back to the romantic humanism

ideas of Spinoza. Harvard professor Rudolf Arnheim the leading expert on Gestalt and

its influences on art writes how “ Spinozistic was the notion that order and wisdom are

not imposed upon nature but are inherent in nature itself; of great interest also was

Spinoza’s idea that mental and physical existence are aspects of one and the same reality

and therefore reflections of each other.” 3It can then be asserted that in the Gestalt view

3

Page 4: Gestalt 508

the human mind and nature are bound by the laws of order, and that by applying this

notion there can exist a cohesive effective approach to visual communication.

Even in it’s infancy the influence of perception of external stimuli on a

psychological interpretation was evident. In 1890, Christian von Erenfels publish his

paper entitled “On Gestalt Qualities” where he pointed to the fact that a piece of music

can still be recognized even when played in different keys, or when notes where left out.

This led to his argument that “if a melody and the notes that comprise it are so

independent, that a whole is not simply the sum of its parts, but a synergistic whole

effect, or gestalt”. 4

In a similar effect on a visual form of this theory Max Wertheimer, who studied

under Erenfels, experimented with the ideas of a motion picture in which he used a

zoetrope to view a single framed image in a running series much like a flipbook (Fig. 1).

It was Werthheimer’s assumption that when viewed in sequence, continuously, these

single images would combine to affect a common narrative. Rather a quant notion

nowadays considering advances in film technology, yet somewhat ground breaking in the

later half of the nineteenth century. These early works demonstrated that an individual

organizes his/her perceptions into meaningful sets. This principle of perception became a

basic concept in Gestalt Theory. These experiences with perception have influenced

other theorist and psychologist to link the potential of Gestalt theory to many other fields

including linguistics, art therapy, musicology, instructional design, human computer

interaction, architectural design, and visual communication. 5

It was after the establishment of Werthheimer’s “apparent movement” theory

based on the illusion of motion pictures, and the subsequent five-year period at the

4

Page 5: Gestalt 508

Psychological Institute in Frankfurt further investigating his theory, that he met with

psychologists Kurt Koffka and Wolfgang Kohler to form the nucleus of gestalt theory.

In 1923 Werthheimer published the “Theory of Form”, which became known as

the dot essay because it was illustrated with abstract patterns of dots and lines.

Werthheimer concluded in the work that certain gestalts are enhanced by our innate

tendency to constellate, or to see as “belonging together” elements that look alike (called

“Similarity grouping”), are close together (“proximity grouping”) or have structural

economy (“good continuation”). 6 That such tendencies are inborn, not learned, is

suggested by the cross-cultural effectiveness of sleight-of-hand magic and camouflage,

both of which work by subverting the laws described in Wertheimer’s paper. But the

interplay of such grouping tendencies is far from simple, because: (1) as the effective of

simultaneous contrast demonstrates, the appearance of parts is determined by wholes; (2)

judgments about similarity or proximity are always comparative; and (3) in compositions

as intricate as paintings, posters and page layouts, parts may be purposely made to

connect by one grouping tendency (similarity of color, for example, or differences of

shape, size or direction). 7

Many artist where directly influenced by these assertions including Vasily

Kandinsky and Piet Modrian. One artist who was directly influences by Werthheimer’s

dot essay was Paul Klee, who attended Werthheimer lectures and used some of its

diagrams in his paintings in the 1930’s (fig.2). 8 Most other artist have become familiar

with Werthheimer’s laws of visual organization through the two important texts:

Language of Vision (1944) by Gyorgy Kepes, and Art and Visual perception: A

Psychology of the Creative Eye (1954) by Rudolf Arnheim. It is Rudolf Arnheim who

5

Page 6: Gestalt 508

has had the most lasting influence. Arnheim, a German immigrant, has published 13

works on gestalt theory and art also became professor of the Psychology of Art at

Harvard University.

Arnheim

The Gestalt Psychology of Art

If we are to understand Rudolph Arnheims approach to gestalt aesthetics we must

first apply a definition of Psychology and it’s aims as well as look to how the gestalt

aesthetic method attempts to segregate itself from a psychoanalytical approach to

aesthetics.

It is Arnheim’s contention that if the psychology of art is to hold itself to a

scientific definition it must then have at its disposal methods of perception that are

scientifically arguable. This scientific reasoning is what Arnhiem’s points to as a

shortcoming in the psychoanalytical aesthetic model.

According to Ian Verstegen “What Arnheim would object to is a somewhat naïve

epistemological idea common to many psychoanalytic treatments that sees unconscious

drives as working blindly without feedback from the environment. Furthermore,

Arnheim has been disappointed with the inability of most psychoanalysis to respond to

the greatness of works of art”. 9 Over the past half a century or more it is this

psychoanalytical model of perceiving aesthetics through the unconscious that has

dominated approaches to not only production, critical observation and analysis as well.

Although the definition of psychoanalysis has changed from its Freudian roots it still

continues to emerge from the same epistemological standpoint and is thus not an

effective scientific method to be used it perceptual psychology.

6

Page 7: Gestalt 508

Because Gestalt psychology attempts to explain many aspects of meaningful

perception at an elementary level, it promises to serve as an important fundamental level

of analysis. If, as Arnheim argued many years ago, “a round form represents for a child

‘thingness,’ it cannot be interpreted as representing some other idea that psychologists

might like to foist upon the child”. 10 Without a psychology sensitive to such factors, the

speculative psychologist as Verstegen sees it “will operate at a level of generality that is

too great to exhaust certain formal characteristics of the work that are necessary to

dispense with before further speculation is possible”. 11

If we now view the Arnhiem model of Gestalt as a perceptual model of

understanding it then has the advantage of not only demonstrating how the work of artist

can be dissected through design, it additionally presents us now with a contextual

framework for understanding the psychological emotional connectivity the work shares

with those who view it.

Gestalt theory represents what is seen as a weaving of realism and the formative

power of the human mind. Views stressing native abilities of rationalism along with the

importance of unlearned abilities and the sufficiency of stimulation for perception and

thought. This tying together of cognitive theories allows the gestalt model to demonstrate

how the viewer is able to intuitively accumulate sensory information in a given space and

then infer through reasoning a connective psychological thread.

A distinction exists her where the psychoanalytical approach which would view a

psychological response to these stimuli as emotive. The Gestalt view is a more

cognitively rational approach. Seeing the emotional response as an individualist

experiential reflexive response. Gestalt is thus universal not individual.

7

Page 8: Gestalt 508

Emotion and Cognitive Connection

To perceive insight into the internal character of a work through its outside

appearance and to then access an emotional origin is something Gestaltists have tried to

circumvent. The thing that Arnheim implied is that “metaphor is not birthed out of this

physiognomic perception rather the insight is derived out of metaphor built on structures

inherently in place in our psychological pre-constructs”. 12

For the Gestaltist there exists a structural connection of the senses that allows for

a unified frame of response when confronted with stimuli. This response is then

cognitively related to past experience and a qualitative metaphoric response is then

registered. The ensuing emotional response is then an individualized metaphorical

reaction. This effected response takes place at a level of abstracted cognitive reasoning

where two perceptual images are combined to form meaning. This structural metaphoric

reference taken from stimuli according to Arnheim “portrays things which are at a higher

level of abstractness than is the symbol itself”. 13

In this metacognitive approach Arnheim is attempting to weave together the

ability of the senses to contain universal or abstract information, along with the minds

ability to manipulate images for productive thinking. Arnheim is relating the two by

saying that “individual mental impressions, or our collective sense datum, already contain

abstract content.” 14 Through an engineered dissection of the symbols within a work of art

the artist and in turn the viewer are engaged in the task of problem solving for the act of

creation as well as interpretation. Arnheim pointed to this assertion in one of his pivotal

works “Perceptual Abstraction and Art”.

8

Page 9: Gestalt 508

Pragnanz

In asserting that perceiving has at its nature an abstracting sense Arnheim is predating

what is now taken for granted in Cognitive Psychology. In “Perceptual Abstraction and

Art” Arnheim considered the “intelligence” of the senses and asserted, through the gestalt

theory of pragnanz how this cognitive act takes place. The law of pragnanz is the

fundamental principle of gestalt perception, which says that we tend to order our

experience in a manner that is regular, orderly, symmetric, and simple. It is this that

allows gestalt theory to take care of abstraction because it provides us with a structural

framework with which the mind can economically organize stimuli, it creates the ability

to hold many examples under one rubric. As Arnheim says “all perception is the

perceiving of qualities, and since all qualities are generic, perception always refers to

generic qualities.” 15

Reception and Interpretation

What role then does how the viewer receives a work of art lead to an

understanding of interpretation in the Gestalt psychology of art? Gestaltists tried over the

years to describe the ways in which we see images differently because of our built

experiential framework yet similarly due to the nature of gestalt. What Arnheim

suggested is that because of the relativity of perception, “probably only a further shift of

the reality level is needed to make the Picassos, the Bracques, or the Klees look exactly

like the things they represent”. 16 What he was trying to say is that these artists and the

way in which there work has been received is based on our innate ability to organize

rationally all the given information and then contextually reason through it.

9

Page 10: Gestalt 508

This produces a functional approach to artist’s and art as well as interpretation.

What can be garnered from this approach is that art can be interpreted with the idea that

when all information is accumulated there is a unified level of reasoning that is universal

and applicable.

Defining Gestalt Design

1

Endnotes

? Max Wertheimer., “Gestalt Theory”, translation in a Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. Ed. Willis, D. Ellis. (New York: Harcourt Brace and Co., 1938), 1.

2 ? Ian Verstegen., Gestalt and Art; A Psychological Theory. (Vienna: Springer, 2005), 2.3

? Rudolf Arnheim., Art and Visual Perception: A Psychology of the Creative Eye.

(Berkeley California: University of California Press, 1954, revised 1974), 37. 4 ? Mitchell Ash., Gestalt Psychology in German Culture: Holism and the Quest for Objectivity. (Cambridge University Press, 1998), 88. 5 ? Lisa Graham., “Gestalt Theory in Interactive Media Design”, Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences, 1, no 2, (2005), 1. 6 ? Max Wertheimer., “Gestalt Theory”, Translation in a Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. Ed. Willis, D. Ellis. (New York: Harcourt Brace and Co., 1938), 1.

10

Page 11: Gestalt 508

It is important to first address the physical vocabulary of Gestalt Design in order

to better understand its uses and effects on the psychology of the viewer.

Historically artist and designers using gestalt theory have focused on a handful of

perceptual laws to improve their effectiveness of image. Gestalt psychologist have

attempted to discover refinements of the law of pragnanz, these laws are theoretically

used to allow us to predict the interpretations of sensation, these are often called “gestalt

laws” They include proximity, similarity, continuity, closure, and equilibrium. It is

important to keep in mind that each principle does not operate independently but always

works in concert with every one of the others.

7 ? (Wertheimer 1938, 2)

8 ? Marianne Teuber “Blue Night by Paul Klee: Vision and Artifact” Henle M., ed.,New York: Springer, (1976) 9 ? (Verstegen 2005, 9)10

? (Verstegen 2005, 10) 11

? (Verstegen 2005, 10)12

? Rudolph Arnheim., New Essays on the Psychology of Art; Perceptual Abstraction and Art. Berkeley California: University of California Press, (1986), 36.

13 ? (Arnheim 1986, 37)14

? (Arnheim 1986, 38)15

? (Arnheim 1986, 38)16

? Rudolph Arnheim., Visual Thinking. Berkeley California: University of California Press, (1969), 37.

11

Page 12: Gestalt 508

Proximity: What is closest together unites. Elements that are near to one another join

together to form patterns or “groupings,” figures against the ground (fig.3). 17

Similarity: Visual elements that resemble another in some way (form or shape, size,

color, direction) unite to form a homogeneous group figure. Modern psychologists

believe that the orientation-or slope-of lines are the major factor in similarity grouping

(fig.4). 18

Continuity: Perceptual organization tends to move in one direction; thus we can follow

the path of a single line (or contour) even in a maze of many overlapping lines (fig.5).

Closure: We possess an innate tendency to close gaps and to make wholes. The result is

more visually stable. When we can achieve this, we are both physically and

psychologically rewarded. Things feel in harmony. If edges, contours, or masses are not

joined, our brain may connect them together if it can find evidence of continuation. The

fewer the interruptions or discontinuities, the more likely closure will occur. Such

“closed” areas are readily seen as a figure. Closure is a confirmation by the brain of a

preexisting idea; it is not a verification of knowledge (fig.6). 19

17 ? Jack Meyers, F,, The Language of Visual Art; Perception as a Basis for Design. Dryden Press: Saunders College Press, (1989), 54.

18 ? (Meyers 1989, 23)19

? (Meyers 1989, 24)

12

Page 13: Gestalt 508

Equilibrium: (Balance and orientation). Every visual field tends toward excellence or

precision-completeness-just as all physical activity is directed toward attaining physical

balance or achieving equity in opposing forces. The idea is reflected in the ancient

Chinese concept of the yin and the yang. Psychologically, we are very uncomfortable-

sometimes frightened-by anything perceived to be out of balance whether or not it is

something we are experiencing physically or perceiving visually. It is a condition that we

will not tolerate for very long. Nature strives for the most regular organization possible.

Evidence of this may be observed in a water droplet; stretched out as it starts, it gradually

changes to a spherical form as it falls. Our visual perception conforms to this concept.

We prefer or are attracted to, figures that exhibit the fewest alternative modes of

organization. The brain tends to see the stimulus as more correct than it really is by

mentally altering the configuration of any figure-object or thought perceived to fit (the

simplest interpretation). It is a process of stereotyping or model making. We assume that

objects viewed have a regular organization (fig.7). 20

Assimilation: This is a process by which a meaningful impression obtained by any one

or more of the senses are related to the vast storehouse of past experience and knowledge.

Assimilation is responsible for a characteristic that psychologist call “isomorphic

correspondence.” This is the relationship between the appearance of a visual form and a

comparable response in human behavior. We know the glowing hot coils on an electric

stove will burn us; we will recoil and shout at a small child about to touch them. We can

feel the hurt. Seeing an image of broken glass or of sharp thorns may cause most viewers

20

? (Meyers 1989, 25)

13

Page 14: Gestalt 508

to shiver. We are reminded of a previous uncomfortable experience with these and

respond emotionally to the visual image. The image-sometimes the mere thought-of a

gun or a knife disturbs some persons because the function of guns and knives is well

understood. In the arts, isomorphic correspondence is an especially valuable concept.

When the correct groundwork is laid, the emotional response may be very strong (fig.8).

21

Selected Works

The first work I have chosen to isolate for this paper is Christina’s World by

Andrew Wyeth (fig.9). It has become for me an important example of effective use of

the laws of gestalt. It is my hope that my work will at some level achieve a similar form

of gestalt and create a connective thread with the viewer.

Through Wyeth’s use of proximity we can see the connectivity of actors in the

scene. The triangulation of Christina the house and the emotion of the scene are set in

motion through both this proximity or lack there-of. It becomes a play of the emotional

weight of distance. We as the viewer use the closure of a fixed line of Christina’s gaze to

become both Christina as well as Christina’s longing. Through assimilation we feel the

wind and temperature, the texture of the land under our skin the ache of her bones. It is a

perfect example of how the gestalt pragnanz can effect emotional connection.

I have used a similar approach in my recent figurative works based on my

children and their disabilities (fig.10). I am attempting for the same kind of triangulation

between the viewer, subject, and object. The walker or crutch (object) serves as a visual

cue, through closure or assimilation, to represent separation or difference with viewer.

21

? (Meyers 1989, 26)

14

Page 15: Gestalt 508

The proximity or physical relationships between viewer and subject, subject and object,

are intended to help create tension; back to viewer head turned in shame. The Gestalt set

in motion (fig.11).

Wyeth shared a similar sense of aesthetics with Edward hopper, whom I also

admire and have used as inspiration. In May of 1967 upon hearing of Hoppers death

Wyeth wrote,

“Even though Hopper wasn’t painting much, the fact that he was alive and still thinking….He never lost the large grasp, never got mixed up in a lot of picky young theories that don’t mean anything. The strange dignity of Hopper’s people and the way he has stripped everything away till there was nothing, till you are filling nothingness with emotion. He ended up painting a corner of a vacant room and a patch of sun. The whole world in a shaft of light”. 22

Hopper use of isolated figures within a field is something I have incorporated in

my figurative works. Through a lack of proximity of figures and the ensuing affect of

unbalanced viewer equilibrium Hopper is able to connect with an emotional response to

our feeling of aloneness. Through assimilation we feel the quite in an image like

Automate where we become in part, through pragnanz, the lone contemplative figure

(fig.12). I have also focused on solitary figures in my narrative works in hopes of

creating a similar effect of isolation and separation. My hope is that the viewer will feel,

as we do with Hopper, a sense of connection and quiet melancholy through Gestalt

(fig.13).

Another artists whose work I have been drawn to is Frida Kahlo. I have always

been attracted to the Two Fridas in particular (fig.14). The use of object as metaphor is

22

? Richard Meryman., Andrew Wyeth: A Secret Life. Harper Collins Publishers. (1998), 394.

15

Page 16: Gestalt 508

essential In Kahlo’s work as well as my own. The closure at work in the piece connects

the physical with the metaphysical and becomes one of its strongest tools. We connect

through similarity that the figures are the same. At the same time the offset of

equilibrium allows for a tension in the figures dissimilarity. The bleeding heart in it’s

fragile state, connecting through a common line the figures eternally struggle of identity,

give to us the ability to see how closure can enable metaphor to serve as a narrative

strand.

An additional artist whose work with object as metaphor that has been

inspirational to me is Renee Magritte. I have been impressed with the effectiveness of

two of the laws in particular. In a work like Son of Man Magritte has emphasized

through proximity the relationship between the apple and the face of the businessman

(fig.15). In doing so he is able to challenge human assumptions about the unknown.

There is tension that is created for the viewer with a loss of continuity and equilibrium in

what is otherwise a “normal” existence, this tension is what I have been trying to mimic

in my recent Will series.

In this recent Will series I have tried to employ a similar use of object as

metaphor. The series is based on readings of Carlos Castaneda a historic anthropologist

doing work in the late 60’s based on the use of psychotropic drugs for medicinal use.

The series is based in particular on the Yaqui Indian belief that there are places on the

human body where life forces emanate, in this case will. In the work the transparent lines

become metaphor for what is at times partially or completely unseen. Similar to the Two

Fridahs and the conflict of identity I want the idea of struggle of will to be hinted at by

the physical evidence while at the same time creating questions of causality in the viewer.

16

Page 17: Gestalt 508

I wanted to disrupt the equilibrium by placing an unexpected object within a normalized

figurative environment causing a Magrettesque challenge of the assumed unknown

(fig.16).

In the landscape monotype series I have been extensively working on I have been

trying to imitate various gestalt tools I have observed throughout the work of the

impressionist. It is their application methods that are most important in terms of effective

use of gestalt. In particular the work of Monet and Cezanne have most impacted my

work. In both artists work there exists the profound use of similarity and closure. In

Monet’s Ruen Cathedral series the viewer is meant to see structure through subtle

changes in light and value, in actuality the surface is a blinding abstraction of dabs of

paint, yet for the viewer it becomes a sublime affect of color and the definition is physical

reality (fig.17). Through closure we are allowed to see the world our experiences tell us

exist. In Cezanne’s work we can apply the same understanding with a more simplified

yet effective approach in use of gestalt. His work challenges the viewer even more to use

closure and similarity to identify object and subject yet the results are no less apparent.

In the work he is able to coax the viewer into a relationship of perception. We view and

recognize the information, make association through the senses, and the magic is kept

alive (fig.18).

There have been additional artist whose work has used similar effects that have

extended my understanding of this form of Gestalt. The work of Richard Schmidt and

Russell Chatham have had influence on my work as well, each accomplishes, through

different routs of execution, to do what the earlier impressionist works have done to

connect the viewer to the scene. In particular I have used the abstract edge quality of

17

Page 18: Gestalt 508

Richard Schmid to push the boundaries of the closure of realism through abstraction

(fig.19). This occurs in the work of Chatham as well, his ability to blur the edges of

subject, control subtleties of tone, and blur the line of realism have been of particular

importance in my work (fig.20).

I have tried in these landscapes to use elements of both the impressionists as well

as the modern landscape artist to produce a hybrid mixture of sensibilities heavily routed

in the same practices. My aim has been to use effects similar to the artists mentioned to

engage the viewer in the play of experience and psychology of art through the principles

of gestalt. I have tried to implement an abstract sense through brush work like that of

Cezanne or Monet adding marks of color and tone each combining to become sky, land,

structure (fig.21). I have played around with abstract edges to balance the boarder

between abstraction and realism like Schmidt, and controlled tone and subject edge

similar to Chatham, to blur the separation between what is perceived as actual and what

our innate gestalt implies (fig.22).

Implications for Teaching

The Gestalt principles have many applications in the field of education. I have

tried to weave its influences throughout my productive, as well as theoretical, curriculum

at the secondary and collegiate level.

My aim has always been to encourage the native inherent abilities in students to

make connection through what is not given. “There’s to need to beat the viewer over the

head with your purpose you might as well paint arrows” is a typical expression students

have heard over the years. It has always been my intent to teach them to engage the

18

Page 19: Gestalt 508

viewer in a dialogue of common experience. “Allow the viewer to become an active

member in the dissection of what you set in motion, let them finish your equation.” At

the same time I am pushing for use of pragnanz in design, I am coaxing a realization of

connection through aesthetic evaluation of historical works. “See the process be the

process.”

19

Page 20: Gestalt 508

My hope has always been that when our time together is finished the students

leave with at least an initial understanding and appreciation for the connective threads of

gestalt.

CONCLUSION

While not existing in the mainstream of conceptual aesthetics Gestalt for me is the

philosophical cornerstone of my productive life. It has woven itself into my conscious

beliefs on what art and educational pedagogy should be. I am a pragmatist. While I

understand the philosophical stance of the modernist aesthetic, I feel it has somehow

focused attention on that which is perpetually functionally, irrelevant. Not that questions

of the subconscious shouldn’t be asked, but perhaps we shouldn’t continue to expect an

answer. Even for Freud sometimes a “cigar is just a cigar”.

20

Page 21: Gestalt 508

“What happens when a problem is solved, when one suddenly “sees the point”? Common as this experience is, we seek in vain for it in the textbooks of psychology. Of things arid, poor, and inessential there is an abundance, but that which really matters is missing. Instead we are told of formation of concepts, of abstraction and generalization, of class concepts and judgments, perhaps of associations, creative fantasy, intuitions, talents-anything but an answer to our original problem. And what are these last words but names for the problem? Where are the penetrating answers? Psychology is replete with terms of great potentiality-personality, essence, intuition, and the rest. But when one seeks to grasp their concrete content, such terms fail.”23

23 ? (Wertheimer 1938, 1)

ImagesFigure 1……………………………………….Zoetrope

21

Page 22: Gestalt 508

Bibliography

Arnheim, R., Art and Visual Perception: A Psychology of the Creative Eye. Berkeley California: University of California Press, 1954, revised 1974.

Arnheim, R., New Essays on the Psychology of Art; Perceptual Abstraction and Art. Berkeley California: University of California Press, 1986.

Arnheim, R., Visual Thinking. Berkeley California: University of California Press, 1969.

Ash, M., Gestalt Psychology in German Culture: Holism and the Quest for Objectivity. Cambridge University Press, 1998.

Graham Lisa., “Gestalt Theory in Interactive Media Design”, Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences, 1, no 2, 2005.

Marianne Teuber “Blue Night by Paul Klee: Vision and Artifact” Henle M., ed.,New York: Springer, 1976.

22

Page 23: Gestalt 508

Richard Meryman., Andrew Wyeth: A Secret Life. Harper Collins Publishers. 1998.

Meyers, J, F,, The Language of Visual Art; Perception as a Basis for Design. Dryden Press: Saunders College Press, 1989.

Verstegen, I., Gestalt and Art; A Psychological Theory. Vienna: Springer, 2005.

Wertheimer, M., “Gestalt Theory”, translation in a Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. Ed. Willis, D. Ellis. New York: Harcourt Brace and Co., 1938.

23

Page 24: Gestalt 508

Circa 1900’s Figure 2……………………………………….Paul KleeHeroic Roses

1938 Figure 3……………………………………….

Proximity Figure 4……………………………………….

24

Page 25: Gestalt 508

SimilarityFigure 5………………………………………

Continuity Figure 6………………………………………

25

Page 26: Gestalt 508

Closure Figure 7………………………………………

Equilibrium Figure 8………………………………………

26

Page 27: Gestalt 508

Assimilation Figure 9………………………………………Andrew Wyeth, Christina’s World,

1948 Figure 10……………………………………..Rede Ballard, Logan’s Place,

27

Page 28: Gestalt 508

2009 Figure 11……………………………………..Rede Ballard, Taylor,

2009 Figure 12……………………………………..Edward Hopper, Automate,

28

Page 29: Gestalt 508

1927 Figure 13……………………………………..Rede Ballard, Taylor’s fife

2009 Figure 14……………………………………..Frida Kahlo, The Two Fridas,

29

Page 30: Gestalt 508

1939 Figure 15……………………………………..Renee Magrite, Son of Man,

1964 Figure 16……………………………………..Rede Ballard, Braydn’s Will,

30

Page 31: Gestalt 508

2008 Figure 17……………………………………..Claude Monet, Cathedrale de Rouen,

1893 Figure 18……………………………………..Paul Cezanne, Mount Sainte Victoire,

31

Page 32: Gestalt 508

1893 Figure 19……………………………………..Richard Schmid, Cumbrian Waterfall,

1988 Figure 20……………………………………..Russell Chatham, Fall Evening,

1980 Figure 21……………………………………..

32

Page 33: Gestalt 508

Rede Ballard, Teton Valley,

2009 Figure 22……………………………………..Rede Ballard, The Long Road,

33

Page 34: Gestalt 508

2008

34