factors relevant to adoption of cats in an animal shelter

8
This article was downloaded by: [University of Windsor] On: 13 November 2014, At: 17:28 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/haaw20 Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter Jacqueline M. Fantuzzi a , Katherine A. Miller b & Emily Weiss b a Department of Psychology , Hunter College b Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals , New York, New York Published online: 25 Mar 2010. To cite this article: Jacqueline M. Fantuzzi , Katherine A. Miller & Emily Weiss (2010) Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter, Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 13:2, 174-179, DOI: 10.1080/10888700903583467 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888700903583467 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

Upload: emily

Post on 16-Mar-2017

231 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter

This article was downloaded by: [University of Windsor]On: 13 November 2014, At: 17:28Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare SciencePublication details, including instructions forauthors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/haaw20

Factors Relevant to Adoption ofCats in an Animal ShelterJacqueline M. Fantuzzi a , Katherine A. Miller b &Emily Weiss ba Department of Psychology , Hunter Collegeb Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals ,New York, New YorkPublished online: 25 Mar 2010.

To cite this article: Jacqueline M. Fantuzzi , Katherine A. Miller & Emily Weiss (2010)Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter, Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science, 13:2, 174-179, DOI: 10.1080/10888700903583467

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888700903583467

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all theinformation (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform.However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness,or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and viewsexpressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, andare not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of theContent should not be relied upon and should be independently verified withprimary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for anylosses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages,and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly orindirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of theContent.

Page 2: Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan,sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone isexpressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found athttp://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

inds

or]

at 1

7:28

13

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 3: Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter

JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL WELFARE SCIENCE, 13:174–179, 2010

Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

ISSN: 1088-8705 print/1532-7604 online

DOI: 10.1080/10888700903583467

RESEARCH REPORT

Factors Relevant to Adoption ofCats in an Animal Shelter

Jacqueline M. Fantuzzi,1 Katherine A. Miller,2

and Emily Weiss2

1Department of Psychology, Hunter College2Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals, New York, New York

This study performed a multifactor analysis of the effects of the provision of toys,

cage location, and cat characteristics (activity level, age, sex, and coat color) on

111 cats available for adoption in a nonhuman animal shelter. The analysis revealed

a greater adopter viewing of cats housed at eye level and of those with toys—even

though the toys did not affect the cats’ behavior. Adopters viewed cats who were

active for longer periods of time. The active cats were more likely to be adopted

during the 16-week study than cats who were less active.

Recent studies in shelters for nonhuman animals suggest that a cat’s activity

level, playfulness, and cage enrichments (Gourkow & Fraser, 2006) can affect

the cat’s adoptability. Adopters’ perception of a shelter animal may even be

affected by the mere presence of toys in the animal’s cage, whether or not the

animal actually plays with them (Wells & Hepper, 2000). Surveys of adopters

and historical analysis of adoption records, however, suggest that coat color

and appearance take precedence over enrichment and behavior when a cat is

considered for adoption (Lepper, Kass, & Hart, 2002; Podberscek & Blackshaw,

1988). As in zoos, interest in particular animals may also be a function of ease

of observing the animal (Davey, 2005; Johnston, 1998).

Jacqueline M. Fantuzzi is now at the Animal Rescue League of Boston.

Correspondence should be sent to Jacqueline M. Fantuzzi, Animal Rescue League of Boston,

10 Chandler Street, Boston, MA 02116. Email: [email protected]

174

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

inds

or]

at 1

7:28

13

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 4: Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter

FACTORS RELEVANT TO CAT ADOPTION 175

This study examined the relationship between toy enrichment, cat activity

level, and adopter interest while taking cat characteristics such as coat color and

cage location into account. It was hypothesized that toys would both increase

cat activity and adopter interest and promote adoption.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects and Housing

Subjects were housed at the American Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to

Animals shelter in New York City. Cats included in the study were domestic

shorthair breeds between 12 and 39 months of age, with no known behavior

or health issues; the cats had resided in the shelter at least 3 but no more than

168 days. The study included 111 cats meeting these criteria (47 neutered males

and 64 spayed females; mean 22 months of age; mean 23 days in the shelter at

study commencement).

The cats were singly housed in the feline adoption ward of the shelter, which

consisted of 24 single cages within a 54 m2 area. Each cage (68 cm high �

62 cm deep and 66 to 91 cm wide) contained a shelflike perch, cardboard litter

box, metal water bowl, and a cat bed. Cage doors were clear glass with 14 to

22 small air holes. The cages were arranged in two tiers at 46 cm and 132 cm

above the floor. Five to 8 cats were observed at once. Cage location and tier

were balanced across treatment groups by evenly distributing study cats among

available cages in the room 18 hr before observations began. Nonstudy cats in

the room numbered between 16 and 19. Caretakers minimized interaction with

the cats during observations; otherwise, husbandry continued as usual. The cats

were fed at 05:30 and 17:30.

Apparatus and Materials

Cats were assigned to either the TOY (n D 55) or CONTROL (n D 56) group.

TOY cats were each given two toys 10 min before observations began: a yellow

plastic ping-pong ball and a “Cat Dancer™” type toy made of a 65 cm piece of

flexible yellow plastic strapping fed through a hole in a rubber stopper wedged

in the cage door. Fifteen centimeters of the strapping rested outside the cage,

enabling potential adopters to move the inner segment. However, very few did

so. CONTROL cats did not receive toys. None of the cats had prior experience

with the toys while in the shelter. Prior exposure to other toys was unknown.

Procedures

The study took place over 16 weekends, Day 1 (Saturday) and Day 2 (Sunday),

from March through July of 2007. For each weekend, 5 to 8 cats who met

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

inds

or]

at 1

7:28

13

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 5: Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter

176 FANTUZZI, MILLER, WEISS

the age, health, behavior, breed, and length-of-stay criteria and who had not

previously participated in the study were assigned to the two treatment groups

(balancing for color, sex, and age). The treatment condition remained in effect

over Days 1 and 2, and adoptions were tallied over both days. Observations of

cat behavior and of potential adopters, however, were conducted only on Day 1

from 11:00 (shelter opening time) until 17:30.

On Day 1, the observer (J.M.F.) sat quietly in the adoption ward recording

both the number and times each study cat was viewed and the duration of

each observation. Viewing was operationally defined as potential adopter moving

within 3 ft (0.914 m) of a cat’s cage and spending at least 3 s, face turned toward

the cat. Viewing ended when the adopter stepped away from the cage, face turned

away from the cage for at least 3 s. To observe the behavior of the study cats,

instantaneous scan samples were conducted at 30-min intervals from 11:00 to

17:30. Cats were recorded as being inactive (lying down, sleeping, or sitting

still) or active (any other behavior).

Data Analyses

The effect of toys, cat activity level (percentage of scans the cat was active),

cage location (top or bottom tier), and adoption status (whether the cat was

adopted during the study) on total viewing time and on number of viewings by

adopters was analyzed using the general linear model (GLM). Because the study

was conducted over 16 weekend replicates, study day was treated as a blocking

factor, as were coat color, age in months, sex, and number of days the cat had

been in the shelter at the start of the study. All factors were treated as additive,

fixed effects. Number of viewings was square-root transformed, average viewing

time was logarithmically transformed, and percentage of scans the cat was active

was angular transformed. The assumptions of parametric analysis were verified

through an inspection of residual plots versus the fitted values and a histogram

of the residuals.

The following GLM model was used: Y D treatment group C percentage

of scans the cat was active C cage location C adoption status C study day C

coat color C age C sex C days in shelter at start of study, where Y D either

number of viewings or average time viewed. Percentage of scans the cat was

active, age, and days in shelter at start of study were treated as covariates. Using

the resulting ANOVA tables, the partial correlation was calculated between the

percentage scans the cat was active and either the number of viewings or the

average viewing time by taking the square root of the ratio: adjusted sums of

squares for percentage of scans active divided by adjusted sums of squares for

percentage of scans active C adjusted sums of squares error.

The effect of treatment, number of viewings, cage location, and adoption

status on cat activity level was also analyzed using GLM. Blocking factors,

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

inds

or]

at 1

7:28

13

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 6: Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter

FACTORS RELEVANT TO CAT ADOPTION 177

transformations, and verification of the assumptions of parametric analysis were

completed as mentioned earlier. The GLM model was percentage of scans cat

was active D treatment group C number of viewings C cage location C adoption

status C study day C coat color C age C sex C days in shelter at start of study.

Covariates were number of viewings, age, and days in shelter at the start of the

study. Analyses were performed using Minitab Version 15 software (Minitab,

State College, PA). A statistic was considered significant if p < .05.

RESULTS

Some cats were viewed multiple times, sometimes by the same and sometimes

by different potential adopters. Even though the presence of toys did not affect

cats’ activity level, F(1, 83) D .77, p D .38), cats with toys were viewed more

times than cats without toys, F(1, 83) D 10.56, p D .002. Cats in the top-

tier cages were also viewed more times than cats in the bottom tier, F(1, 83) D

20.31, p < .001, and tended to be more active than those below, F(1, 83) D 3.15,

p D .08. Cats with higher activity levels tended to be viewed more times than

less active cats (partial r D .29, p D .058); cats who were adopted during the

study tended to have been viewed more times than cats who were not adopted,

F(1, 83) D 3.72, p D .057.

More active cats were viewed, on average, for a significantly longer duration

than were cats who were less active (partial r D .44, p < .001), as were cats

in the upper tier cages, F(1, 83) D 6.96, p D .01. Cats with toys tended to

be viewed for longer periods of time, F(1, 83) D 3.62, p D .06. There was

no difference between cats who were and were not adopted during the study

in terms of the average duration of viewing. Neither the number of times nor

the duration of adopter viewing of cats was affected by coat color, sex, or age

(.15 < p < .47).

Sixteen cats were adopted during the study: 6 CONTROL and 10 TOY cats.

Cats who were adopted during the study had a higher activity level than those

not adopted, F(1, 83) D 8.05, p D .006). Because only 16 cats were adopted

during the study, the effects of treatment, coat color, sex, or age on adoption

were not able to be analyzed. The color group with the highest proportion of

cats adopted was black cats (26%). Adopted cats were 62.5% male and 37.5%

female. Among the study cats, the median age at adoption was 18 months.

DISCUSSION

The presence of toys on shelter cats’ behavior and adopter interest was examined.

Even though the presence of toys in cages did not affect the cats’ behavior, cats

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

inds

or]

at 1

7:28

13

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 7: Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter

178 FANTUZZI, MILLER, WEISS

with toys were viewed more often than cats without toys. Adopters viewed cats

in the upper tier more times than cats in lower tiers, and active cats were viewed

longer than inactive cats. Adopted cats were more active and had been viewed

more times than nonadopted cats. Coat color, sex, and age did not affect viewing

by adopters.

Cats must be viewed before they can be adopted. In this study, cats who were

adopted tended to have been viewed more times than those not adopted. Despite

the toys’ failure to stimulate heightened cat activity, the toys did lead adopters

to view cats more times than those without toys, and 10 of the 16 cats adopted

were from the TOY group. Wells and Hepper (1992) found a similar effect of

toy presence on adopter interest in shelter dogs.

Gourkow and Fraser (2006) found that the addition of toys to shelter cats’

cages increased adoptions. In the present study, toys placed in the cats’ cages

resulted in cats being viewed for longer periods of time; however, due to the low

number of total adoptions, the relationship between toys and adoption could not

be statistically analyzed. We did find that more than half of the adoptions that

occurred were from the TOY group. In contrast to the Gourkow and Fraser study,

however, all the cages in the current investigation, even those in the CONTROL

group, were already somewhat enriched: all cats resided in a cage with a bed,

perch, semihidden area underneath the perch, litter box, and water bowl. In

addition, bird sounds and classical music were played ambiently throughout the

cat adoption area. In the Gourkow and Fraser study, the control cage featured

only a litter box, water bowl, and towel. Addition of toy enrichment to those

cages may have created an important contrast and therefore significantly affected

adoption.

Possibly due to visibility differences, cats in upper tiers in this study generated

more viewing by adopters than did cats in lower tiers. Unlike lower tier cats,

those in the upper tier were visible even if they were sleeping in a cat bed or

huddled under the perch. Davey (2005) reported such a link between visibility

and exhibit staying power in zoos; Johnston (1998) noted that zoo visitors viewed

exhibits for longer periods if the animals were in close proximity. In this study,

upper tier cats tended to be more active than lower tier cats, possibly because

they were being viewed more often and for longer duration than lower tier cats.

This attention may have, in turn, further heightened their activity and generated

even more adopter interest. Cats located in the upper tier cages may also have

demonstrated increased activity because cats often use elevated areas as vantage

points to assess their surroundings and the approach of people (Rochlitz, 2000).

Fearful and anxious cats, as a result of being elevated, may have been less

inclined to freezing and hiding due to a heightened ability to monitor their

surroundings. Overall, the strategic stocking of cages that provide the best

visibility of, and by, cats in an animal shelter may be a means to cost-effectively

generate adopter interest in cats who have proved harder to place.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

inds

or]

at 1

7:28

13

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 8: Factors Relevant to Adoption of Cats in an Animal Shelter

FACTORS RELEVANT TO CAT ADOPTION 179

CONCLUSION

Similar to studies of zoo animals (Bitgood, Patterson, & Benefield, 1986; John-

ston, 1998) and shelter dogs (Wells, 1996, as cited in Wells & Hepper, 2000),

this study demonstrated that adopters prefer to both view and adopt cats who are

active. Shelters may also find it beneficial, therefore, to find ways of generating

activity in their cat wards. Although the addition of toys in cages was not

effective in stimulating feline activity in this study, toys did attract observer

attention; this, in turn, led to adoption. Further studies, designed to identify cage

enrichments or other conditions that provoke the interest of both cats and po-

tential adopters, could help shelter management to establish an environment that

promotes adoption. In addition, because cat activity positively affects adopter

viewing behavior and adoption, research investigating means to increase cat

activity could have positive implications for the shelter field.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

We thank Graig Fantuzzi for his invaluable support in all aspects of this project

and Sheila Chase of Hunter College for her academic guidance and mentorship.

Thanks are also due to all of the staff at the American Society for the Prevention

of Cruelty to Animals for making this research possible.

REFERENCES

Bitgood, S., Patterson, D., & Benefield, A. (1986). Understanding your visitors: Ten factors that

influence visitor behavior. American Association of Zoological Park and Aquariums Annual

Proceedings, 726–743.

Davey, G. (2005). Relationships between exhibit naturalism, animal visibility and visitor interest in

a Chinese zoo. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 96, 93–102.

Gourkow, N., & Fraser, D. (2006). The effect of housing and handling practices on the welfare,

behavior and selection of domestic cats (Felis sylvestris catus) by adopters in an animal shelter.

Animal Welfare, 15, 371–377.

Johnston, R. J. (1998). Exogenous factors and visitor behavior: A regression analysis of exhibit

viewing time. Environment and Behavior, 30, 322–347.

Lepper, M., Kass, P. H., & Hart, L. A. (2002). Prediction of adoption versus euthanasia among dogs

and cats in a California animal shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 5, 29–42.

Podberscek, A. L., & Blackshaw, J. K. (1988). Reasons for liking and choosing a cat as a pet.

Australian Veterinary Journal, 65, 332–333.

Rochlitz, I. (2000). Feline welfare issues. In D. C. Turner & P. Bateson (Eds.), The domestic cat

(pp. 207–226). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Wells, D. L., & Hepper, P. G. (1992). The behaviour of dogs in a rescue shelter. Animal Welfare, 1,

171–186.

Wells, D. L., & Hepper, P. G. (2000). The influence of environmental change on the behaviour of

sheltered dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 68, 151–162.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

inds

or]

at 1

7:28

13

Nov

embe

r 20

14