Transcript
Page 1: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

1

Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

Dr. Sukhvinder S. Obhi

Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience

Wilfrid Laurier University

Page 2: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

2

Outline

• Historical Contextualisation

• Does Cognitive Psychology need the brain?

• Does neuroscience need cognitive psychology?

• Introduction to the study of Action

Page 3: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

3

Page 4: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

4

Epilepsy

• Epileptic seizures caused by spontaneous firing of neurons

• Can spread rapidly causing the body to convulse

• In extreme cases where people are suffering from recurrent seizures frequently, surgery may be considered

• The aim is to separate the source of the neuronal firing to stop it spreading

Page 5: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

5

Wilder Penfield (1954)

Awake patient: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=H4yhky5KIio

Page 6: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

6

Penfield

• Stimulation to the occipital lobe: “a star came down towards my nose”

• Stimulation of another (motor) region: “those fingers and my thumb gave a jump”

• Temporal lobe: “I heard a radio” (memory)

Page 7: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

7

Page 8: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

8

Contextualisation

• Somehow, functional processes are embedded into the mass of tissue that we call the brain

• Traditionally the study of cognition included such topics as thinking, perceiving, imagining, remembering, speaking, and planning

• Cognitive neuroscience considers the same issues, but also from a neural perspective

Page 9: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

9

Interdisciplinary Nature of CogNeuro

Cognitive Neuroscience

Anatomy

Neurophysiology

Psychology and Neuropsychology

BiologyPhysics

Computer Science

Neurology

Page 10: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

10

Cognitive Neuroscience• The study of the neural basis of cognition

Pinel (2006)

• The field of cognitive neuroscience concerns the scientific study of the neural mechanisms underlying cognition and action and is a branch of neuroscience. Cognitive neuroscience overlaps with cognitive psychology, and focuses on the neural substrates of mental processes and their behavioral manifestations. The boundaries between psychology, psychiatry and neuroscience have become quite blurred. Cognitive neuroscientists tend to have a background in experimental psychology, neurobiology, neurology, physics, and mathematics.

adapted by S.S. Obhi from wikipedia*

*(never use wikipedia as a single reference source – always cross check the information before citing it and provide other refs!!)

Page 11: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

11

Why interdisciplinary?

• Theory and methods of Cognitive Neuroscience are often very complex

• Individuals with different types of training collaborate to make it happen

• All good fMRI (brain imaging) centres have a Physics group that works with “experimental psychologists” to optimize the scanning methods for various types of research

• A great example of how researchers interested in a common topic can work together more usefully than working alone!

Page 12: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

12

Brain imaging webpage

Psychologist

Mathematician

MD

PhysicistNeurologist

Page 13: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

13

History

• During most of history humans were too busy to think about thought!

• “Thought about thought” may have come about at the stage in history where day-to-day survival did not occupy every hour of every day

• Probably linked to the beginnings of farming and domesticating animals etc

Page 14: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

14

Historical Perspective

• Long history of interest in how the brain creates our mental world

• Central question: how does the physical substance of the brain give rise to our feelings, thoughts, emotions and actions?

• Historically referred to as the mind-body problem

Page 15: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

15

Descartes & Dualism• Rene Descartes (1596-1650) was an

early proponent of dualism

• Mind non-physical and immortal, body physical and mortal

• They interact in the pineal gland in the centre of the brain

• According to Descartes, sensory stimulation would cause vibrations in the body/brain that would be picked up by the pineal gland which would generate a non-physical sense of awareness

Page 16: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

16

Critics of Descartes

• Spinoza (1623-1677)

• Mind and Brain were just two different levels of explanation for the same thing – but not two different things

• Termed Dual-Aspect Theory – still popular today

Page 17: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

17

Levels of Explanation

• This is a useful concept

• Can explain phenomena at the cellular level, at the neuronal population level and at the behavioural level

• One of the goals of the scientific study of mind and brain is to understand how things at one level are related to things at another

Page 18: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

18

Reductionism• A popular approach to the mind-body problem

• Cognitive, mind based concepts are currently useful but they will eventually be replaced by biological constructs (just as much of physical chemistry can be reduced to physics)

• So emotions, thoughts, desires etc, will be described simply as patterns of neuronal firing

• Many believe this is too extreme – there must be some validity in our own sense of “self”, perhaps over and above the physical working of the brain

• Pure reductionist view sometimes called “materialism”

Page 19: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

19

Cautionary Examples

• Understanding individual liver cells is not enough to gain an understanding of liver function

• Similarly, understanding atoms in a solid, does not tell you about the properties of the macroscopic solid

• Understanding the hardware of a computer is insufficient to understand the jobs a computer performs

• To believe fully in reductionism is to believe that low level processes always fully explain high level processes

Page 20: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

20

Scientific Approaches to Mind & Brain

• Thought started very early, Ancient Greece (if not before that!)

• Aristotle (384-322 BC) noted that the ratio of brain size to body size was greatest in more intellectually advanced species (humans)

• But wrongly thought that cognition was centered at the heart and that the brain was a cooling system

• Note: Roots of much of psychology (and therefore cognitive neuroscience) are in Philosophy

Page 21: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

21

Egyptian Contributions

• Ancient Egypt – scientists had access to condemned criminals

• There are reports that scientists actually vivisected these people (note: to vivisect is to dissect, cut or perform surgery on a living animal)!!

• In this age speculation that the brain controlled different behaviours emerged – but wrongly placed emphasis on the ventricles (cavities in the brain)

Page 22: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

22

Roman Contributions

• Galen – a surgeon who worked on gladiators

Page 23: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

23

Roman Contributions

• Galen (AD 130-201) thought that war and Gladiator games were the greatest school of surgery

• Identified many major structures in the brain

• Also credited for observing that nerves project to and from the brain

• But was misguided in thinking that mental experiences arose out of the ventricles – an idea that went unchallenged for 1500 years!

Page 24: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

24

Rene Descartes (1596-1650)

• Non-physical mind interacts with brain through pineal gland

• Often referred to the flow of “spirits” through nerves

• Compared to mechanical contraptions of his time

• Mind-body dualism

Page 25: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

25

Lack of interest in the cortex

• Old pictures of the brain are interesting since they tended to draw the ventricles in exquisite detail and then just crudely draw the cortex!

Page 26: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

26

Luigi Galvani (1737-98)

• Applied electrical currents to motor nerve and made frog’s legs move

• Linked electricity to the functions of the nervous system

• But wrongly thought that nerves and muscles contained an “electric fluid”

Page 27: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

27

Golgi & Cajal

• Battle between 2 neuro-anatomists in Italy & Spain

• Golgi (1843-1926) developed a staining technique that could stain individual neurons

• Cajal (1852-1934) went on to show that individual neurons were discrete entities – and that neural matter was not a continuous mass of processes

Page 29: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

29

Insights into Morphology

• Morphology = study of the structure of biological organisms

• Many early contributions were based on dissections and really attempted to inform knowledge of structure

Page 30: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

30

Shift to cortical interest & functions

• Gall & Spurzheim (1810) – their drawings started to focus on the cortex a bit more

• Gall was a proponent of phrenology – localization of “traits” in the brain

Page 31: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

31

The birth of Psychology

• Only really happened toward the end of the nineteenth century

• In a sense, it took the emphasis of the study of the mind away from its biological roots

• This was necessary since the topics of interest to early psychologists (James, Wundt etc) included such things as consciousness, attention and personality

• Biological study of the mind had, until this time, very little to say about these issues

Page 32: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

32

Early Psychologists

William James (USA)

Wilhelm Wundt (Germany)

Sigmund Freud (Austria)

Page 33: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

33

The gap between Psychology & Biology

• It is possible to make coherent and testable theories about cognition (mental processes) without referring to the brain

• Cognitive Psychologists have done this since the formation of the discipline - taking the “information processing approach” to cognition

• What are the processing steps underlying various mental phenomena?

• Use the computer metaphor in which the brain is analogous to a computer

Page 34: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

34

Computer Metaphor

• The brain is like a computer - popularized in the 1950s coincident with the invention of the computer

• Famous model that uses this framework is Broadbent’s model in which perceptual processes are followed by attentional processes which move information into short-term memory

Page 35: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

35

Box and Arrow models

Extraction of “physical” properties

Extraction of “abstract” properties

FilterIncoming

Stimuli

•A sequence of processing stages

•Implication of these models was that you could understand the steps needed to perform a certain task without reference to the brain

•Much as you can figure out how a computer program works without reference to the hardware

Page 36: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

36

Processing not always sequential

• Important concept in the cognitive approach is that it is possible to have parallel processing

• Stages of processing can be interactive meaning that higher level processes can begin before low level processes are completed

• E.g., Early feature extraction and two visual systems

• Back projections in vision

Page 37: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

37

Box and Arrows and Neural Nets

StimulusStimulus

identification

Response

selectionResponse

INPUT PROCESSING OUTPUT

Page 38: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

38

Top Down Processing

• These models also emphasize how higher level processes can influence lower level processes

• Many attentional cueing paradigms work on this idea

• A cue primes the lower level perceptual processes to expect something in particular

• Shows itself as a faster response in trials that had a cue versus trials that didn’t

Page 39: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

39

Example: Cueing Effects

Posner (1980)

Page 40: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

40

The Birth of Cognitive Neuroscience

• Much influenced by the development of imaging techniques which allow observations of the working brain

• In the past (phrenology, neuropsychology etc) had to wait until a person died to examine their brain

• Also largely correlated with the fall of behaviourism and the general acceptance that a search for cognitive processes had to be undertaken

• Gazzaniga, an influential proponent of Cognitive Neuroscience claims that the term “Cognitive Neuroscience” was coined in a New York City taxi in the late 1970s

Page 41: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

41

Methods

• Present day Cognitive Neuroscience is composed of a variety of methods

• Generally, the methods available to the cognitive neuroscientist can be placed on a number of dimensions

Page 42: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

42

Considerations

• Temporal resolution: refers to the accuracy with which one can measure when an event is occurring

• Spatial Resolution: refers to the accuracy with which one can measure where an event is occurring

• Invasiveness: refers to whether or not the equipment is located internally or externally

Page 43: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

43

Methods

Some cognitive neuroscience techniques

Page 44: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

44

Does Cognitive Psychology need the brain?

• My experience at a Cognitive Science conference – still a divide between strict “box modellers” and people who consider it important to know about the brain

• An important question asked by Coltheart (2004)

• “Has Cognitive Neuroscience, or if not, might it ever (in principle or even in practice) successfully use data from cognitive neuroimaging to make theoretical decisions entirely at the cognitive level (e.g., to adjudicate between competing information processing models of some cognitive system)?”

Page 45: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

45

Question:

• Does visual recognition of words and letters involve computing a representation that this independent of case?

Case example

Page 46: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

46

Different case priming experiment

Both reaction time and imaging measures are giving different information about the same process – useful (?)

Page 47: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

47

Does imaging just tell us where something happens?

• Many people would argue that it does

• But, the more we know about the systems that are engaged in the brain when performing different tasks, the more fully we will understand how the system works

Page 48: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

48

Furthermore

• One source of information will never give us the full picture

• In order to understand the processing mechanisms and their biological basis we need more information

• For example……

Page 49: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

49

• Inputs and outputs to the fusiform

• Cytoarchitechtonics* of the processing route

• Neurotransmitters in the processing route

*cellular structures

Page 50: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

50

Triangulation & Converging Evidence

• The use of multiple sources and methods to address a common issue

• By comparing results using different methodologies a holistic understanding of the phenomenon under study may be achieved

• This is very much in the spirit of understanding how to link evidence from cognitive neuroscience with theories and concepts from cognitive psychology - mutually informative and complimentary

• (Even though you will come across people who are in ‘one camp’ or another) - OPINIONS are allowed!!

Page 51: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

51

Example • Cognitive Psychology evidence favors parallel search in word

recognition, not serial

• But it does not give us any clues as to why – in fact computer programs can recognize words by both serial and parallel search

• The reason why might be linked to the relatively slow neural response time – we only know this because people have measured neural response time

• In this way, information from studies of the processing taking place in the brain can help to put biological constraints on otherwise unconstrained cognitive models

Page 52: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

52

Does Cognitive Neuroscience need Cognitive Psychology?

• We’ve known that things like perception, remembering, pain, mood and sexual desire are all products of processing going on in the brain for a long time (19th century)

• The methods of Cognitive Neuroscience make it possible to study these processes in vivo – this makes for tantalizing headlines in the media

Page 53: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

53

Media Frenzy due to in vivo observations

Page 54: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

54

Page 55: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

55

But

• Cognitive Neuroscience techniques by themselves are not useful without carefully designed experimental paradigms that have stood the test of time and the criticism of researchers

• These paradigms are often the paradigms developed by cognitive psychologists

• Without this knowledge gained over decades of research in cognitive psychology, imaging might not be anything more than the new phrenology

Page 56: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

56

What do you see?

• In an imaging experiment you typically observe changes in blood flow

• Your job however, as a cognitive neuroscientist is to relate these observations to the well established concepts of cognitive psychology

• For example, how does activity in the medial temporal lobes contribute to different types of memory as identified over many years by cognitive psychologists?

Page 57: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

57

Essential Reading

• Ward book Chapter 1 (relates to today)


Top Related