daly-movement analysis r

Upload: george-baciu

Post on 13-Apr-2018

223 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    1/14

    Movement Analysis: Piecing Together the PuzzleAuthor(s): Ann DalyReviewed work(s):Source: TDR (1988-), Vol. 32, No. 4 (Winter, 1988), pp. 40-52Published by: The MIT PressStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1145888.

    Accessed: 11/02/2012 07:00

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at.http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

    JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of

    content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms

    of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    The MIT Pressis collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to TDR (1988-).

    http://www.jstor.org

    http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=mitpresshttp://www.jstor.org/stable/1145888?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/stable/1145888?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=mitpress
  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    2/14

    ovement

    nalysis

    Piecing Together

    the

    Puzzle

    Ann

    Daly

    Movement

    analysis

    is

    a blanket

    term

    covering

    methods as

    wide-

    ranging

    as Paul Ekman's

    Facial Affect

    Scoring

    Test,

    Laban

    Movement

    Analysis

    (LMA),

    and

    Ray

    L.

    Birdwhistell's kinesics. The field can

    be di-

    vided

    roughly

    in

    two. Nonverbal behavior

    research

    (or

    nonverbal

    com-

    munication

    research),

    which

    focuses

    mainly

    on

    the actions and

    structure

    of

    everyday

    life,

    has

    largely

    been

    created

    by psychologists,

    anthropologists,

    and

    ethologists.

    LMA,

    which

    deals with the

    qualities

    and

    dynamics

    of

    movement

    across the entire

    continuum of

    performance,

    has

    its roots

    in

    dance.

    The

    interdisciplinary

    field of

    movement

    analysis

    is

    like an unfin-

    ished

    jigsaw puzzle:

    some of the

    pieces

    are

    in

    place,

    some not.

    The

    object

    of this

    TDR issue

    is

    to

    put together

    the

    pieces

    we have

    while

    working

    toward

    finding

    those that

    are

    missing.

    My

    aim in

    editing

    this

    issue

    was

    also to

    demonstrate the

    applicability

    of

    movement

    analysis

    across the continuum of

    performance,

    from

    represen-

    tational

    to

    nonrepresentational,

    from

    trained to

    untrained:

    theatre,

    aes-

    thetic

    dance,1

    social

    dance,

    ritual,

    sports,

    aerobics,

    everyday

    behavior.

    The

    same

    elements

    of

    the

    body-in-motion

    apply

    to

    a

    ballet as

    they

    do to an

    assembly

    line,

    only

    the aesthetic

    performance

    is framed

    by

    additional

    lay-

    ers of

    convention

    (form,

    genre,

    choreographer, performer).

    As a

    whole,

    the

    articles

    explore

    the

    interpretive

    richness afforded

    by

    a

    close

    reading

    of

    movement in

    any

    situation. And

    they

    set

    in

    motion a

    discussion of

    the

    larger

    issues vital

    to the

    development

    of

    movement

    analysis

    as

    a

    perfor-

    mance

    studies

    methodology.

    Movement

    analysis

    is

    by

    no

    means new-it

    goes

    back at

    least to Charles

    Darwin

    (1872)

    and Francois Delsarte

    (Stebbins

    I902).

    Both

    men

    were

    fascinated

    by

    the

    relationship

    between movement and

    meaning,

    the

    former

    in

    the

    realm of

    natural

    history

    and the latter

    in

    the world

    of

    oratory.

    Later,

    in

    the

    I940s,

    psychologist

    Wilhelm

    Reich

    (I949)

    and

    anthropologist

    David

    Efron

    (1941)

    took

    up

    the issue in

    their

    respective

    disciplines.

    However,

    not

    until

    the

    I96os

    and

    '70s

    did the field

    accumulate the force of a

    movement

    movement. Nonverbal

    communication

    research was not an

    unlikely

    compatriate

    to

    the other

    movements

    of

    the era: social

    activism,

    encounter

    groups, the drug culture, and avant-garde performance. The emphasis was

    on

    process,

    and

    movement was

    process par

    excellence. The research of

    40

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    3/14

    Movement

    Analysis

    41

    scientists like

    Birdwhistell,

    Albert

    E.

    Scheflen,

    Edward

    T.

    Hall,

    and

    Daniel

    N.

    Stern was

    eagerly

    explored by performers

    and

    performance

    makers

    such as

    Robert Wilson

    (Brecht I978:32),

    Steve

    Paxton,

    and Yvonne

    Rainer

    (Schechner

    I973:I2I).

    Scholars,

    too,

    sought

    to

    adapt

    the

    findings

    of

    non-

    verbal

    communication

    research

    (see

    Schechner

    1973;

    Schechner with

    Mintz

    I973).

    The

    study

    of movement

    flourished

    during

    the late '6os and

    '70s

    because

    of a

    decisive

    shift in

    paradigms

    from text to

    performance,

    from

    speech

    to

    gesture,

    from

    passive reception

    to active

    perception/participation.

    The

    I98os

    have not

    sustained

    the

    momentum of the

    movement

    movement;

    performance

    studies

    theory

    and

    methodology

    is dominated

    by

    poststruc-

    turalist

    literary

    theory.

    As

    humanist

    thought

    once

    privileged language

    over

    the

    body

    as

    the source of truth

    and

    identity, poststructuralist

    thought

    currently

    privileges

    the remove of the

    spoken

    word over the now

    redefined

    and

    suspect literality

    of the

    performing body.

    The

    movement movement

    notwithstanding,

    nonverbal behavior has

    al-

    ways had a relatively low status in Western culture. Nonverbal behavior

    was

    considered

    only supplementary

    to

    language;

    it could

    only

    be defined as

    not verbal. As

    infant

    psychiatrist

    Stern

    (1985)

    pointed

    out,

    the nonver-

    bal

    portion

    of

    the human

    communication

    system

    remains

    unformalized

    and thus

    deniable;

    people

    are

    only

    held

    accountable for

    verbal

    messages.

    Among

    the

    most deniable

    aspects

    of

    movement are formal

    properties

    that

    are,

    in

    a

    sense,

    invisible and

    yet

    still observable.

    Spatial

    focus

    and

    interaction

    rhythms,

    for

    example,

    cannot be

    touched,

    yet

    they

    are

    palpably

    perceived.

    Movement

    (particularly

    dance

    movement)

    is

    both absence

    and

    presence,

    becoming only

    a

    trace of itself

    with the

    passage

    of time.

    There

    is

    nothing

    so

    powerful

    and

    extraordinary

    but at

    the same time so

    mundane

    and

    overlooked as

    movement.

    Just

    consider the

    equilibrium quickly nego-

    tiated

    in

    a

    crowded

    elevator,

    or an

    infant's tearful

    response

    to its

    mother's

    silent

    abruptness.

    The

    movement

    qualities

    and

    dynamics

    that LMA

    addresses2 are

    espe-

    cially

    deniable. As a

    result,

    LMA

    has

    had to

    struggle

    to

    establish its

    le-

    gitimacy

    within

    the

    larger,

    scientifically

    oriented

    movement

    analysis

    community.

    LMA

    refers to

    any system

    developed by

    Rudolf

    Laban

    (I879-

    1958)

    or

    by

    his

    students,

    such as

    Warren

    Lamb's

    Action

    Profiling System

    and

    the

    Kestenberg

    Movement

    Profile.

    Rooted

    in

    Laban's

    theories

    of Ef-

    fort and

    Space

    Harmony,

    LMA

    flourishes in

    the

    Laban/Bartenieff

    Institute

    of

    Movement

    Studies

    (LIMS)

    in

    New

    York

    City

    and

    at

    colleges

    such as

    Ohio

    State

    University,

    Goldsmith's

    College

    in

    London,

    and

    the

    Depart-

    ment of Performance Studies at New York

    University.

    Laban's

    influence

    has

    been felt in

    many

    spheres:

    in

    choreography,

    through

    pupils

    such as

    Mary

    Wigman

    and

    Kurt

    Jooss;

    in

    dance

    criticism,

    especially

    the

    American

    descriptive

    school of

    the late

    '6os and

    '70s

    (see

    Dell

    1977);

    in

    dance

    preservation

    and

    reconstruction,

    through

    his

    notation

    sys-

    tem;3

    in

    performance

    scholarship, through

    certification

    programs

    in

    New

    York

    City

    and

    Seattle

    and in

    intensive

    study

    programs

    at the

    above

    named

    universities;

    in

    corporate

    management,

    through

    Warren

    Lamb's

    Action

    Profiling System

    (Lamb

    and

    Watson

    1979;

    Lamb

    and

    Turner

    1969);

    in

    human

    development,

    through

    the research

    of

    Judith

    Kestenberg

    (1965,

    1967);

    and in

    clinical

    psychology

    through

    the

    work

    of

    Irmgard

    Bartenieff

    (1980),

    MarthaDavis (1984), and Marion North (1972). At LIMS research

    also

    focuses on

    sports

    and

    dance

    therapy.

    Despite

    its

    considerable

    achievements

    within

    LIMS,

    the

    larger

    nonverbal

    behavior

    field has

    been

    reluctant to

    accept

    LMA

    without

    reliability

    data

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    4/14

    42

    Ann

    Daly

    (Rosenfeld

    I982:249).

    In order for a

    test

    or tool

    to be

    accepted

    in

    the

    sciences,

    its

    reliability

    must first be established

    empirically.

    A reliable

    in-

    strument is one that

    yields

    the same

    information for different observers

    and

    for the

    same observer across time. In order

    to

    establish the

    reliability

    of

    Effort-Shape Analysis,

    Davis

    (I987)

    assessed two kinds of

    reliability:

    re-

    peat reliabilityand interraterreliability. In the first part, she asked ratersto

    observe

    the

    same movement

    segments

    on

    separate

    occasions.

    In

    the sec-

    ond,

    she

    compared

    the

    responses

    of a number of raters to identical

    move-

    ment

    segments.

    Davis found

    that raters

    generally

    were

    individually

    consistent across

    time,

    and she found a

    high degree

    of

    consistency

    among

    raters on

    the

    occurrence of

    posture-gesture merging

    and

    on

    strong,

    direct,

    quick,

    and

    sustained

    qualities.

    Davis summarized that the LIMS

    Reliability project

    did

    produce

    evidence

    of

    the

    reliability

    of

    most

    of the basic

    concepts

    studied

    here. But

    more

    importantly,

    it

    generated

    methods

    and

    tools

    for

    facilitating

    ongoing

    assessment

    of

    observer

    agreement

    within LMA

    training

    programs

    and researchprojects (I987:I7). Similar reliability studies are being con-

    ducted for

    other Laban-based observational

    systems:

    the Action

    Profiling

    System

    (Winter

    1987),

    the

    Kestenberg

    Movement Profile

    (Sossin

    1987),

    and

    the Davis

    Nonverbal States Scales

    (Davis

    and Hadiks

    1987).

    Proof of

    reliability

    will

    be

    an

    important step

    toward

    LMA's full

    credibility

    and

    wider use

    by

    movement

    analysts.

    Beyond

    LMA

    reliability

    as an

    observational

    tool,

    movement

    analysis

    as

    a

    whole

    needs to turn its attention to the

    larger,

    semiotic issues

    concerning

    the

    relationship

    between movement

    and

    meaning.

    If

    movement

    analysis

    is

    to

    become a standard

    performance

    studies

    methodology, practitioners

    must address three

    basic

    questions:

    (I)

    does movement have its own mean-

    ing?;

    (2)

    if

    so,

    how does

    movement

    mean?;

    and

    (3)

    if

    so,

    where is the

    locus

    of

    meaning? Asking

    these

    questions

    will lead us to a better

    understanding

    of

    movement and also

    provoke

    the debate needed to

    demystify

    its role in

    performance.

    Does movement

    have its own

    meaning?

    A

    hundred

    years

    after Delsarte and

    Darwin,

    it

    would seem hard to

    believe that

    the

    very significance

    of movement is still at issue. And

    yet

    the

    fact

    that movement is so

    often left unconsidered

    in

    the

    study

    of

    perfor-

    mance

    suggests

    that the

    question

    is

    still

    unresolved. There was no doubt in

    the

    minds

    of

    nonverbal communication

    researchers

    n

    the

    I970s

    that move-

    ment had its own

    meaning;

    the

    question

    was, Whatkind?

    Today

    we can

    find

    in

    their

    work,

    especially

    in

    Scheflen's,

    the means to

    go

    further: to ask

    what

    social and

    political

    significance

    movement has

    in

    our

    culture and

    social

    order.

    One

    of

    the

    galvanizing

    issues of nonverbal

    communication in the

    I970s

    centered on

    epistemological paradigms

    and

    their attendant

    methodologies

    (see

    Skupien

    I982).

    On one

    side there were those such as

    Ekman who

    studied

    movement

    as

    individual

    expression.

    On

    the other side there

    were

    those,

    such as

    Birdwhistell and

    Scheflen,

    who insisted

    that

    behavior

    could

    not

    be

    studied as a

    sender-receiver

    phenomenon;

    they

    argued

    that behavior

    is

    a

    socially

    conditioned,

    patterned system

    of

    interactions that cannot be

    wrenched from its contexts. Movement, they insisted, communicated

    more than

    just

    discrete referential

    information.

    Scheflen,

    a

    visionary

    psychiatrist

    whose

    many

    contributions to

    the

    field

    of

    nonverbal

    communication have

    yet

    to be

    fully

    mined,

    helped

    to mend

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    5/14

    Movement

    Analysis

    43

    this

    seemingly impassable

    breach.

    Behavior,

    he

    asserted,

    can have

    many

    simultaneously

    different

    and sometimes

    contradictory

    kinds of

    meanings.

    In How

    BehaviorMeans

    (I974)-an

    important

    volume

    for

    anyone

    interested

    in

    the

    past

    and the future of

    nonverbal communication

    research-Scheflen

    outlined four

    types

    of

    meaning:

    (I)

    denotative

    references;

    (2)

    the

    meaning

    derived from the immediate context of the interaction or transaction; (3)

    the

    cultural

    origins

    and

    personality

    of the

    speaker;

    and

    (4)

    meta-acts that

    comment on and

    qualify

    one's own

    behavior.

    Although

    these

    types

    can

    be

    separated

    for

    purposes

    of

    analysis,

    he

    warned,

    they

    are

    fully integrated

    in

    the act of

    communication.

    Scheflen

    readily

    conceded

    in

    the

    introduction to How BehaviorMeans

    that

    its focus on

    information

    was

    already

    somewhat limited

    and

    traditional;

    it is

    now clear that

    language

    and

    non-linguistic

    behavior

    have much

    broader

    roles

    in human

    affairs

    than

    simply

    the

    exchange

    of

    meaning

    and information.

    The

    com-

    municational system is a means of regulating transactionsof all types

    of behavior and

    of

    maintaining

    social

    order

    and social control

    (1974:4).

    His book

    Body Language

    and

    Social

    Order:

    Communication

    s BehavioralCon-

    trol

    (1972)

    took this next

    step,

    demonstrating

    with

    prose

    and

    photographs

    that behavior

    is also

    significant

    in that it

    maintains the dominant

    social

    order.

    Scheflen,

    in

    effect,

    went

    beyond

    description

    and

    methodology

    to

    social criticism. He concluded

    that:

    Any sweeping

    claim that communication

    has the

    purpose

    of

    individual

    expression

    or social

    change

    must be

    regarded

    as

    idealized

    myth-or

    else as a

    political

    gambit

    to

    give

    us the illusion

    of freedom

    we

    rarely

    attain.

    [.

    .

    .]

    On the

    basis of the data described

    in this

    book,

    we

    conclude that

    the usual

    purpose

    of kinesic and territorial

    systems

    is

    preservation

    of the

    existing

    order

    (1972:132).

    Scheflen's

    insights

    into the

    political implications

    of nonverbal communi-

    cation were

    not

    lost on

    the

    women's

    movement,

    which dovetailed with the

    movement movement

    in

    the

    I970s.

    A substantial amount of research

    into

    socialized

    gender

    differences

    in

    nonverbal behavior

    was undertaken

    (For-

    tier

    1977;

    Frances

    1979;

    Mayo

    and

    Henley

    1981;

    Weitz

    I976;

    Wex

    1979).

    For her book

    Body

    Politics:

    Power,

    Sex,

    and

    Nonverbal

    Communication

    1977)

    Nancy

    Henley investigated

    the rich store of movement

    analysis/nonverbal

    communication

    research to

    lear

    how movement communicates

    power

    and

    maintains

    asymmetrical

    relationships, especially

    in

    regard

    to

    gender.

    Gender is

    a

    doubly important

    issue

    in

    movement

    analysis,

    not

    only

    because

    movement

    style

    and

    interaction

    patterns

    are

    fundamental

    ingre-

    dients

    in

    the cultural

    construction

    of

    gender,

    but also because movement

    itself has

    traditionally

    been

    consigned

    to the realm of the

    feminine,

    set

    in

    opposition

    to

    male

    mastery

    over

    language.

    Gender

    dichotomy

    is

    still

    a

    very powerful underpinning

    of

    Western

    theatrical

    dance convention.

    As

    such,

    feminist

    inquiry

    into the

    way

    dance

    regulates

    cultural

    notions of feminine

    and masculine

    is

    dangerous

    to

    aesthetic dance as we know it. Bill T. Jones and Johanna Boyce are two

    choreographers

    who

    have

    endeavored

    to destabilize

    gender

    conventions on

    stage,

    experimenting

    with forms

    that are sometimes

    uncomfortably

    un-

    familiar. In this

    issue

    Jones

    and

    Boyce

    talk

    with Carol

    Martin

    and

    myself

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    6/14

    44

    Ann

    Daly

    about

    the

    significance

    of

    gender imagery

    on the

    stage.

    How do

    cultural

    conventions of

    movement

    regulate

    what

    is

    considered feminine

    and

    masculine in

    dance? How do

    we tease out these conventions and

    recon-

    struct

    them

    along

    less

    hierarchical lines? Can the

    purpose

    of

    nonverbal

    behavior-to

    preserve

    the dominant

    order,

    as Scheflen demonstrated-be

    overturned?

    How does

    movementmean?

    Once

    one

    accepts

    that movement

    possesses

    and communicates

    its

    own

    meanings,

    the

    next,

    more difficult

    question

    is: how

    does movement mean?

    There

    is no

    simple

    answer;

    the idea of

    a

    body language, though

    success-

    ful for

    the

    popular

    media,

    is

    an inaccurate reduction. Movement

    is

    a nexus

    of

    intersecting

    elements and

    systems-semantic, syntactic,

    formal,

    and

    contextual-clustered

    in

    infinitely

    complex

    and

    varying ways.

    Three basic

    principles coming

    from

    the

    systems paradigm

    of the

    I970S

    still hold fast.

    First,

    behavior

    is a

    patterned system.

    Scheflen's

    (1973)

    classic film

    analy-

    sis of a

    family

    therapy

    session

    demonstrated

    the

    elaborately cyclical

    nature

    of

    interaction,

    from

    gross-level postural

    shifts which

    demarcate

    distinct

    periods

    of

    behaviors to the

    most

    subtle shifts

    of

    the head and

    eye

    that

    serve

    as

    regulatory

    signals.

    Second,

    all behavior

    in an

    interaction

    is seen as

    functioning

    in its

    struc-

    ture and

    therefore as

    contributing

    to

    meaning

    as well.

    Whether

    consciously

    intentional

    or

    not,

    all

    behavior-postural

    shifts

    or

    the

    relinquishing

    of

    direct

    gaze,

    for

    example-potentially

    functions to

    establish,

    maintain,

    and

    terminate

    patterns

    of

    relationship

    between interactants.

    Third, meaning

    is

    created

    by

    the

    relationships

    between

    behavior

    and

    its

    many

    layers

    of

    context.

    A

    smile,

    for

    example,

    though

    it is

    generally

    con-

    sidered a

    discrete affect

    of

    pleasure,

    has no hard and

    fast

    meaning

    (Bird-

    whistell

    I970:29-39;

    Scheflen

    1978).

    Its

    manner

    of execution

    might

    signify

    deference,

    or

    discomfort,

    or even

    disdain,

    depending

    on the

    situation;

    at

    the same time it

    may

    also function in the flow of interaction. Its

    meaning

    also

    varies from culture

    to culture-a

    Japanese

    smile

    does not

    always

    mean what an American smile means. The

    significance

    of

    any given

    behavior

    can

    only

    be determined in its

    individual, interactional,

    institu-

    tional,

    and cultural

    contexts

    and in relation

    to

    behavioral

    expectations,

    for

    what is not

    done

    may

    be

    as

    significant

    as what is done.

    Each of

    these

    principles

    is

    applicable

    to

    representational

    performance:

    it,

    too,

    is

    patterned

    and

    gains

    meaning

    from

    everything

    within

    its

    frames.

    Form

    and

    structure are

    important

    expressive

    elements of

    any

    performance,

    whose

    meaning

    is

    greatly

    affected

    by

    its

    relation

    to the

    contexts of

    per-

    formance

    traditions,

    to other works

    by

    the same

    director

    or

    choreog-

    rapher,

    and

    even to its

    presenters,

    its

    physical

    environment,

    its

    spectators,

    and

    its

    patrons.

    Most

    nonverbal behavior

    research

    has,

    of

    necessity,

    focused on

    a

    single

    phenomenon

    such as interaction

    rhythm

    or facial

    expression

    or

    gesture.

    Though

    the

    legacy

    of

    the

    movement movement

    is

    indeed

    rich,

    the

    many

    different strains

    of research are

    rarely synthesized.

    Individual

    studies

    need

    to be

    integrated

    with each

    other

    and with LMA.

    The

    scientific

    preoccupa-

    tion with the what of movement and LMA's preoccupation with the

    how of

    movement need not be at

    odds

    with

    each other.

    They

    are com-

    plementary-like

    intersecting

    axes of a

    single

    grid.

    One without the other

    is an

    incomplete picture

    of how

    movement

    means. Eliot D.

    Chapple

    and

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    7/14

    Movement

    Analysis

    45

    Davis

    have

    made

    an

    important

    step

    toward

    completing

    the

    picture

    with

    Expressive

    Movement

    and

    Performance: Toward a

    Unifying Theory,

    their

    contribution to this issue of

    TDR. Their

    essay synthesizes

    cultural,

    neurophysiological,

    structural,

    and

    qualitative aspects

    of

    movement into

    a

    single

    model of

    what

    they

    call the

    body's

    infralanguage. Similarly,

    part

    of Susan Leigh Foster's project in ReadingDancing(1986), reviewed in this

    issue

    by

    Philip

    Auslander and Marcia B.

    Siegel,

    is

    to outline the factors

    that,

    working together,

    give

    rise to

    meaning

    in dance.

    The

    challenge

    is to

    devise

    a

    theory

    that

    takes into account the

    dynamic,

    complex

    nature of

    movement.

    If

    a word

    should be

    sought

    to

    denote the

    logic

    or

    harmony

    of

    movement,

    wrote

    Laban,

    it

    might

    be the term

    'confluence,'

    because it is

    the

    peculiar

    form

    of the

    flowing

    together

    of

    several

    movement

    constituents,

    which

    gives

    character

    to

    any

    meaningful

    dance-movement

    (Laban

    1971:3

    ).

    Birdwhistell's kinesics

    system

    has not

    proven

    useful as

    a

    means

    of

    analyzing

    the

    through-time

    phenomenon

    of

    movement because of

    its

    atomistic

    approach

    to behavior. Its

    roots

    in

    the

    unit-and-structure technique of structural linguistics tends to impose on

    movement

    an

    inappropriately

    static framework.

    One of the

    fundamental

    strengths

    of

    LMA is

    its

    ability

    to deal

    with

    the

    processual

    aspects

    of

    per-

    formance.

    Though

    the

    subject

    of

    Ekman's work is

    discrete

    categorical

    affects

    (facial

    expressions

    of

    happiness,

    disgust,

    surprise,

    sadness,

    anger,

    and

    fear)

    rather

    than

    full-body

    motion,

    his

    research

    into the links

    between

    emotion,

    facial

    expression,

    and

    the

    autonomic

    nervous

    system

    (Ekman,

    Levenson,

    and

    Friesen

    1983)

    cuts to the

    heart of

    essential

    questions

    performance

    scholars

    are

    asking

    about the

    nature of

    representation.

    In

    this

    issue Ekman

    raises

    some

    of

    these

    questions

    in his

    summary

    remarks

    made at

    a

    New York

    University symposium

    on his

    work

    and

    its application to performance

    studies.

    For

    example,

    what are

    the

    differences

    between

    performing

    a

    spon-

    taneous

    expression,

    a

    simulated

    expression,

    and

    an

    emblematic

    expression

    (Schechner

    1988)?

    How much

    can

    display

    rules-or

    performance

    conven-

    tions-tell us

    about

    a

    given

    culture

    or

    genre

    of

    performance?

    Which

    comes

    first,

    motion

    or

    emotion?

    Or

    rather,

    where do

    the two

    meet and

    form each

    other?

    Perhaps

    the

    most

    fundamental

    question

    of

    all,

    and

    one

    that remains

    dormant

    and

    scattered

    amidst

    the

    literature,

    is

    the

    principle

    of

    correspon-

    dence or

    rules of

    transformation

    linking

    movement to

    meaning.

    That

    is,

    what

    are

    movement's

    modes of

    representation?

    Is

    movement

    compatible

    with

    the

    traditional

    semiotic

    typology: symbol

    (arbitrary),

    icon

    (resem-

    blance),

    and

    index

    (cause-effect)?

    Of

    particular

    importance

    to

    movement

    analysis

    are the

    theory

    of

    iconism

    and the

    notion of

    intrinsic

    meaning.

    If

    iconic

    similitude is

    a

    matter of

    cultural

    convention,

    as

    Eco

    (I979)

    has

    convincingly

    argued,

    then

    by

    what

    process(es)

    are

    new

    conventions of

    resemblance

    achieved? How

    did

    Martha

    Graham's

    contraction-and-

    release,

    for

    example,

    come

    to look

    like

    inner

    turmoil

    rather than

    a torso

    spasm?

    And is

    there,

    as

    Davis

    (1975)

    has

    suggested,

    an

    intrinsic

    meaning

    to

    some

    formal

    aspects

    of

    movement? If

    so,

    how is

    the

    intrinsic

    aspect

    of a

    behavior

    negotiated

    with its

    situational

    and

    arbitrary

    characteristics

    n

    the

    making

    of

    meaning?

    Does this

    intrinsic

    meaning

    override

    context? Is it

    universal

    and

    innate? How

    might

    it be

    related

    to

    the

    universal

    categorical

    affects?

    The

    question

    of

    how

    movement means

    involves not

    just

    the

    phenome-

    non

    itself

    but also

    the

    observer as

    active

    meaning-maker.

    The

    challenge

    to

    LMA

    to

    establish

    empirical

    reliability bespeaks

    a

    skepticism

    not

    only

    about

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    8/14

    46

    Ann

    Daly

    the

    susceptibility

    of

    movement

    qualities

    and

    dynamics

    to

    replicable

    de-

    scription

    but

    also about the

    perceptual powers

    of its

    observers.

    Because

    movement

    is

    so

    overlooked

    in our

    culture,

    movement

    analysts

    are

    some-

    times

    viewed as virtual

    mystics, divining

    information that the

    naked

    eye

    cannot

    see,

    despite

    the fact

    that movement

    analysts

    are

    merely

    capitalizing

    on an ability we all exercise everyday to get through even the most mun-

    dane interactions-an

    ability

    which

    is

    honed

    extremely

    early

    in

    life.

    According

    to

    Stern

    (I985),

    all humans

    show a

    basic

    capacity

    to

    perceive

    and

    interpret

    the

    most subtle of movement

    qualities

    during infancy.

    His

    empirically-based

    theories corroborate

    Laban's

    insistence on

    the

    primacy

    of

    movement

    qualities

    both in

    the creation

    of

    the self and

    in

    interpersonal

    communication. Like

    Laban,

    he

    emphasizes

    the

    importance

    of

    the

    way

    movement

    is

    performed.

    There are a thousand

    smiles,

    Stern

    wrote,

    a

    thousand

    getting-out-of-chairs,

    a thousand variations

    of

    performance

    of

    any

    and all

    behaviors

    (1985:56).

    His

    findings

    echo basic LMA

    principles,

    only

    Ster has

    behind

    him

    what

    Laban did

    not-the force

    of

    empirical

    research.

    Stern

    rejects

    the

    accepted

    psychoanalytic theory

    that infants

    start out in

    pre-verbal 4

    symbiosis

    with

    their mothers

    and

    progress

    toward the dif-

    ferentiation

    of

    selfhood that

    ultimately

    comes with

    the

    acquisition

    of

    lan-

    guage.

    Stern

    posits

    the

    opposite:

    a

    progression

    from

    differentiation to

    relatedness.

    Very

    early

    on an

    infant

    gains

    a sense of

    self

    through

    its own

    bodily

    experiences.

    One of the

    ways

    that

    the

    infant

    then

    begins

    to

    gain

    a

    subjective

    self is

    through vitality

    affects,

    a

    concept

    that echoes

    Laban's

    Effort

    theory.

    Vitality

    affect is

    Stern's

    term for the

    observable

    way

    behavior

    is

    enacted.

    Distinguished

    from

    discrete

    categorical

    affects,

    vitality

    affects are

    those

    dynamic,

    kinetic

    qualities

    of

    feeling

    that

    distinguish

    animate from

    inanimate and

    that

    correspond

    to the

    momentary

    changes

    in

    feeling

    states

    involved in

    the

    organic

    processes

    of

    being

    alive

    (1985:156).

    Like

    Laban

    before

    him,

    Stern

    points

    out that it is

    not

    just

    a

    smile

    that we

    interpret-it

    is the

    smile's

    impulsiveness,

    its

    explosiveness,

    or its

    reluctance that

    com-

    municates.

    The

    vitality

    affects that

    Stern

    describes-intensity,

    time,

    and

    shape-are

    strikingly

    similar

    to Laban's four

    Effort

    elements:

    weight,

    flow,

    time,

    and

    space. By

    attuning,

    or

    sharing,

    vitality

    affects,

    infants

    can

    interact as a

    self

    with others.

    They

    can

    share inner

    experiences

    on an

    almost

    continuous

    basis.

    For

    Stern,

    as

    for

    Laban who

    not

    only

    worked

    in

    the field of

    dance

    but

    also in

    industry

    as an

    analyst

    of

    work

    movement

    patterns,

    movement

    runs

    the continuum between art and behavior:

    It

    is

    inescapable

    that the

    infant and child

    first learn

    about

    vitality

    affects

    [.

    .

    .]

    from their

    interactions

    with their own

    behavior

    and

    bodily

    processes

    and

    by

    watching,

    testing,

    and

    reacting

    to

    the social

    behaviors

    that

    impinge

    on

    and

    surround

    them.

    They

    must also

    learn

    or

    somehow

    arrive at

    the

    realization

    that there

    are

    transformational

    means for

    translating

    perceptions

    of

    external

    things

    into internal

    feel-

    ings,

    besides

    those

    for

    categorical

    affects.

    These

    transformations

    from

    perception

    to

    feeling

    are

    first

    learned with

    spontaneous

    social

    behaviors.

    It

    seems that

    only

    after

    many years

    of

    performing

    these

    transformations and building up a repertoireof vitality affects is a

    child

    ready

    to

    bring

    this

    experience

    to

    the domain of

    art as

    some-

    thing

    that is

    externally

    perceived

    but

    transposed

    into felt

    experience

    (I985:I60).

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    9/14

    Movement

    Analysis

    47

    At this

    point,

    just

    what those rules

    of

    transformation

    are-how

    we

    get

    from

    describing

    an

    action

    as

    direct and

    quick

    to

    interpreting

    it as

    abrupt-

    remains

    undiscovered.

    Where

    s the

    locus

    of

    meaning?

    Movement

    analysis

    does not

    always stop

    at observation. LMA

    has

    been

    established as an

    objective

    observational

    tool,

    but the

    purpose

    of

    move-

    ment

    analysis

    in

    performance

    studies

    is

    to

    help yield

    meaning-an

    essen-

    tially

    subjective

    undertaking.

    As

    with

    any interpretive

    methodology,

    meaning

    is

    ultimately

    mediated

    by

    the observer and

    by

    the

    purpose

    of

    the

    study.

    As

    Paul

    Byers

    cautions,

    The

    word observation

    is a

    joker

    in the

    deck

    since we

    cannot

    ob-

    serve

    anything

    directly-only

    through

    our

    perceptions

    (scientific

    or

    other)

    which

    implies

    that some

    part

    of

    observation

    is

    always

    of

    our-

    selves

    (and

    what we find

    analytically

    is as much about our

    unrecog-

    nized

    premises

    as

    about that which

    we believe we are

    studying)

    (I986).

    Profound in

    any

    discipline,

    the

    question

    of

    where

    meaning

    is

    located

    is

    even

    more

    anxiously

    posed

    when

    the

    subject

    matter

    itself is so

    slippery,

    so

    unlocatable. How much of

    the

    meaning

    that

    analysts

    see in

    movement

    is

    really

    there,

    and how much is

    imposed

    on it? Where is

    the

    boundary

    between observation

    and

    interpretation?

    Is

    there

    a

    boundary?

    Is

    analysis

    always

    interpretive?

    These

    questions

    are

    particularly important

    to an-

    thropologists. Using

    Laban's

    Western-rooted

    principles,

    can we

    do

    justice

    to

    the

    performance

    of a

    non-Western culture? As

    questionable

    as

    Alan

    Lomax's conclusions about the choreometrics

    project

    may

    have

    been,

    the

    coding

    sheet that

    Irmgard

    Bartenieff and

    Forrestine

    Paulay

    developed

    as a

    framework for

    discerning

    cultural

    movement

    style

    is

    a

    valuable observa-

    tional

    tool

    (Lomax,

    Bartenieff,

    and

    Paulay

    I968).

    It is

    true that

    observation and

    interpretation

    are

    part

    of the same

    percep-

    tual

    package,

    but

    training

    enables

    analysts

    to

    reflexively

    discriminate be-

    tween

    the two.

    Being

    able to discern

    the

    elements of

    movement and

    understand

    how

    they

    are

    put

    together

    enables

    movement

    analysts

    to

    figure

    out

    why they

    make

    the

    interpretations

    they

    do

    and,

    if

    necessary,

    to

    recon-

    sider them.

    Movement

    analysis

    is

    a means of

    expanding

    our

    crosscultural

    understanding,

    not of

    embalming

    it.

    Anthropologist Allison Jablonko and movement analyst Elizabeth Ka-

    gan

    take

    up

    these issues in

    their

    article,

    An

    Experiment

    in

    Looking:

    Reexamining

    the

    Process

    of

    Observation.

    They suggest

    that

    using

    rather

    than

    suppressing

    one's

    cultural

    movement biases is

    a useful

    starting point

    for

    analyzing

    unfamiliar

    movement;

    in their

    observational

    project, they

    found

    that

    they

    could better

    discern what

    was

    present

    in

    Maring

    behavior

    through

    ts

    differences from

    their

    own

    culture's

    behavior,

    without

    necessar-

    ily

    reading

    meaning

    into

    those

    differences.

    Meaning,

    as

    Sally

    Ann

    Ness

    points

    out in

    her

    analysis

    of

    the

    film Tro-

    briand

    Cricket:An

    Ingenious

    Response

    o

    Colonialism,

    cannot be

    determined

    through

    movement

    analysis

    alone.

    By

    itself,

    she

    writes,

    it

    cannot

    go

    beyond descriptive interpretations into explanatory statements of origin,

    function,

    and

    meaning.

    Nevertheless,

    movement

    analysis

    can

    yield

    valu-

    able

    insights

    that,

    in

    turn,

    serve

    as

    the

    basis for

    a

    more

    extended and

    integrated

    interpretive

    analysis.

    In

    Looking

    at

    Movement as

    Culture,

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    10/14

    48

    Ann

    Daly

    Cynthia

    J.

    Novack

    illustrates

    the

    ambiguity

    as

    well

    as the

    clarity

    of move-

    ment

    as a cultural

    phenomenon:

    certain movement

    qualities appear

    through

    time,

    yet

    meanings suggested

    by

    these

    qualities

    subtly

    shift;

    con-

    trasting

    movement

    styles

    exist

    simultaneously,

    sometimes

    embodying

    the

    same

    meanings

    and

    sometimes

    opposite

    meanings.

    The

    importance

    of movement-in the

    production

    of the self and

    in

    the

    production

    of culture-can no

    longer

    be denied. Movement

    analysis,

    which

    is

    already

    gathering

    outward momentum

    through

    efforts such as the

    LIMS LMA

    Theory

    Network,5

    the

    American Association

    of Laban

    Move-

    ment

    Analysts

    (AALMA),

    and

    the

    Dance and Movement

    Analysis

    section

    of the

    American Folklore

    Society,

    is faced with

    an

    opportunity

    to

    join

    what seems to be

    a

    growing

    interest

    in

    the humanities

    for

    the

    study

    of

    the

    body.6

    Movement

    analysis

    needs to connect with these other

    ways

    of

    looking

    at the

    body;

    it is

    only through

    engagement

    with other

    methodologies

    and theories that movement

    analysis

    can

    make

    its own

    contribution.

    Notes

    I.

    In

    this

    article use the term aesthetic ance o

    distinguish

    t from social

    dance;

    however,

    other

    authors

    n

    the field and

    in this

    issue

    use terms such as

    stage

    dance

    or

    theatredance.

    2.

    Laban'sEffort

    heory

    dealswith the

    dynamic

    tructure nd

    rhythm

    of movement

    (see

    Bartenieff,Davis,

    and

    Paulay

    1972).

    He identified he

    irreducible ormal

    elementsof movement

    (what

    he called

    Efforts )

    as

    weight,

    time,

    flow,

    and

    space.

    His

    theory

    investigating patial

    structure nd

    relationships

    s

    known as

    SpaceHarmony

    or Choreutics.Laban's

    oncept

    of the

    affinity

    betweenthe ef-

    fort,

    or

    energy,

    nvested

    n

    movementand ts

    spatialunfolding

    was

    developedby

    Warren

    Lamb nto the Americanmethod known as

    Effort-ShapeAnalysis.

    Laban's

    heory

    of

    movement

    successfully

    voids

    reductiveness.

    eraMaletic's

    Body, Space, Expression:

    The

    Developmentof Rudolf

    Laban'sMovement

    and

    Dance

    Concepts

    akesclearhis holistic

    approach,

    here

    ust

    in termsof Effort:

    The

    significance

    f

    particular ualities

    merge

    within

    the

    macrostructure,

    the context of a

    movement

    sequence,

    and these

    qualities ain meaning

    when

    they

    are related

    o what

    precedes

    and

    what

    follows. Thus the se-

    quencing,phrasing,

    or the

    rhythm

    of effort

    sequences

    s identifiablerom

    several

    points

    of view:

    a)

    how

    particular

    ualities

    build

    up

    with

    regard

    o the

    optimalsequence

    of attention space), ntention weight),decision time),andprecisionor

    progression

    flow).

    b)

    how effort

    qualities hange

    nto another

    quality;

    hese mutations an

    occur

    either

    gradually

    with the

    change

    of one

    element,

    more

    surprisingly

    with the

    change

    of

    two,

    or

    they

    can be contrastedwith the

    change

    of all

    elements.

    c)

    what the

    compositionalpattern

    of

    the

    sequence

    s,

    such as

    repetition,

    rebounding,

    ontrasts,

    variations,

    development.

    d)

    whether

    he effort

    sequence

    s

    performed

    with

    the whole

    body being

    attuned o it or whetherdifferent

    qualities

    are

    performed

    t the same time

    by

    different

    body

    parts

    (I987:I03-04).

    3. Laban'sheoriesof Effortand SpaceHarmonyas well as his notationsystem,

    now called

    Labanotation,

    ere

    meantto have both

    descriptive

    nd

    prescriptive

    applications.

    The laws

    of

    movementhe uncovered

    were not

    only

    to

    help

    us to

    better

    observe,

    analyze,

    and recordmovement

    but also

    to

    help performers

    x-

    pand

    their

    personal ange

    of

    movement.

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    11/14

    Movement

    Analysis

    49

    Laban's

    concept

    of movement

    as

    expression

    coincided

    with that of

    early

    mod-

    ern

    dancers

    like

    Wigman

    and

    Jooss,

    who embraced his

    theory

    for the

    purpose

    of

    choreography.

    Though expression

    is no

    longer

    the dance

    paradigm

    it was

    then,

    choreographers

    still use Laban's theories to

    generate

    movement. William

    For-

    sythe,

    director of Frankfurt

    Ballet,

    for

    example,

    uses Laban's

    principles

    of

    Space

    Harmony

    to

    generate

    movement for dances such as

    Same

    Old

    Story (1987).

    This issue deals with the

    usefulness of

    LMA

    to the

    performance

    scholar

    rather

    than to the

    performance

    maker.

    Notation,

    too,

    falls outside the

    scope

    of this

    issue

    (see

    Topaz

    1988).

    4.

    The term

    pre-verbal

    has

    always

    been

    a

    subtle

    way

    of

    marginalizing

    move-

    ment: of

    relegating

    it to the

    negative

    role of other in

    a world

    supposedly

    constructed

    solely

    in

    language.

    Stern's theoretical move is an

    important

    one to a

    politics

    of

    movement

    analy-

    sis. His

    argument

    defuses the rhetoric of the

    pre-verbal by

    pointing

    out that

    (I)

    the

    infant does

    experience

    a

    sense

    of

    self before

    learning

    to

    talk,

    (2)

    the

    infant

    does

    relate to

    others

    through

    movement

    before

    learning

    to

    talk,

    and

    (3)

    these

    bodily

    senses of self

    and means of

    interpersonal

    communication

    persist

    even

    after

    the

    acquisition

    of

    language.

    Nonverbal

    communication, then,

    is not

    pre-verbal

    at

    all. Movement and

    language

    share in the

    process

    of

    creating

    the self and com-

    municating

    with

    others.

    5.

    The

    LIMS LMA

    Theory

    Network

    has

    gathered

    a

    bibliography

    (which

    will

    be

    annually

    updated)

    of

    unpublished

    manuscripts

    that deal with

    LMA

    theory.

    It

    was

    published

    in the

    AALMA

    February

    1988

    Newsletter.

    6.

    More

    and more

    scholarship

    is

    exploring

    how the

    body

    is

    inscribed in

    various

    institutions

    and social

    discourse:

    medicine and

    sexuality

    (Foucault

    1978;

    Gal-

    lagher

    and

    Laqueur

    I987);

    psychoanalysis;

    art

    and

    photography

    (Banta

    1987;

    Dijkstra I986);

    patriarchy

    (Suleiman

    1986);

    fashion

    (Banner

    I983);

    health,

    fitness,

    and

    sports

    (Green

    1986);

    postmodernism

    (Foster

    1986;

    Auslander

    forthcoming;

    and

    Auslander's

    review

    of

    Foster's

    Reading Dancing

    in

    this

    issue);

    and

    perfor-

    mance

    (Steinman

    I986).

    References

    Auslander,

    Philip

    (forthcoming) Vito

    Acconci

    and the

    Politics of

    the

    Body

    in

    Postmodern

    Perfor-

    mance. In

    Postmodernism,

    dited

    by

    Gary

    Shapiro.

    Albany,

    NY:

    SUNY Press.

    Banner,

    Lois

    W.

    1983

    American

    Beauty.

    Chicago

    and

    London: The

    University

    of

    Chicago

    Press.

    Banta,

    Martha

    1987 ImagingAmerican Women:Idea and Ideals in Cultural History. New

    York:

    Columbia

    University

    Press.

    Bartenieff,

    Irmgard,

    with

    Dori

    Lewis

    1980

    Body

    Movement:

    Coping

    with the

    Environment.New

    York:

    Gordon

    and

    Breach

    Science

    Publishers,

    Inc.

    Bartenieff,

    Irmgard,

    Martha

    Davis,

    and

    Forrestine

    Paulay

    1972

    Four

    Adaptations

    of

    Effort

    Theory

    in

    Research nd

    Teaching.

    New

    York:

    Dance

    Notation

    Bureau.

    Birdwhistell,

    Ray

    L.

    1970

    Kinesicsand

    Context.

    Philadelphia:

    University

    of

    Pennsylvania

    Press.

    Brecht,

    Stefan

    1978 The Theatre

    of

    Visions: Robert Wilson. Frankfurt:

    Suhrkamp

    Verlag

    Frankfurt am Main.

    Byers,

    Paul

    I986

    Personal

    correspondence,

    I5

    December. New

    York

    City.

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    12/14

    50

    Ann

    Daly

    Darwin,

    Charles

    [I872]

    I965

    The

    Expressionof

    the Emotions n Man and

    Animals.

    Chicago:

    Univer-

    sity

    of

    Chicago

    Press.

    Reprint.

    Davis,

    Martha

    1975

    Towards

    Understanding

    he Intrinsic

    n

    Body

    Movement.New York: Arno

    Press.

    1984

    Nonverbal Behavior and

    Psychotherapy:

    Process

    Research. In Non-

    verbal Behavior:

    Perspectives,

    Applications,

    Intercultural

    Insights,

    edited

    by

    Aaron

    Wolfgang,

    203-228.

    Gottingen:

    C.J.

    Hogrefe.

    1987

    Steps

    to

    Achieving

    Observer

    Agreement:

    The LIMS

    Reliability Proj-

    ect. MovementStudies: Observer

    Agreement

    2:7-19.

    Davis, Martha,

    and

    Dean

    Hadiks

    1987

    The Davis

    Nonverbal States Scales

    for

    Psychotherapy

    Research: Re-

    liability

    of LMA-Based

    Coding.

    Movement

    Studies: Observer

    Agree-

    ment

    2:29-34.

    Dell,

    Cecily

    1977 A PrimerforMovementDescriptionUsingEffort-Shape ndSupplementary

    Concepts.

    New York: Dance Notation Bureau Press.

    Dijkstra,

    Bram

    1986

    Idols

    of Perversity:

    Fantasies

    of

    Feminine Evil in Fin-de-SiecleCulture.

    New

    York and Oxford: Oxford

    University

    Press.

    Eco,

    Umberto

    1979

    A

    Theory of

    Semiotics.

    Bloomington:

    Indiana

    University

    Press.

    Efron,

    David

    [I94I]

    I972

    Gesture,

    Race

    and

    Culture.

    The

    Hague,

    Paris: Mouton.

    Ekman, Paul,

    Robert

    W.

    Levenson,

    and

    Wallace

    V.

    Friesen

    1983 Autonomic Nervous System Activity Distinguishes Among Emo-

    tions.

    Science221:1208-1210.

    Fortier,

    Cheryl

    E.

    1977

    Male-Female

    Differences in Movement.

    Dance

    Therapy

    Research:

    Seven Pilot

    Studies,

    edited

    by

    C. Schmais

    and

    F.

    Orleans,

    115-I38.

    New York: Hunter

    College

    Publication.

    Foster,

    Susan

    Leigh

    1986

    Reading

    Dancing:

    Bodies and

    Subjects

    n

    Contemporary

    merican

    Dance.

    Berkeley:

    University

    of California Press.

    Foucault,

    Michel

    1978

    The

    History of

    Sexuality.

    Translated

    by

    Robert

    Hurley.

    New

    York:

    Pantheon

    Books.

    Frances,

    Susan

    J.

    I979

    Sex

    Differences in

    Nonverbal Behavior. Sex Roles

    5

    (no. 4):

    519-

    535.

    Gallagher,

    Catherine,

    and

    Thomas

    Laqueur,

    eds.

    I987

    The

    Making of

    the

    Modem

    Body:

    Sexuality

    and

    Society

    in the

    Nineteenth

    Century.

    Berkeley:

    University

    of California

    Press.

    Green,

    Harvey

    I986

    Fitfor

    America:

    Health,

    Fitness,

    Sport

    and American

    Society.

    New York:

    Pantheon

    Books.

    Henley, Nancy

    M.

    1977

    Body

    Politics:

    Power, Sex,

    and Nonverbal

    Communication.

    Englewood

    Cliffs,

    NJ:

    Prentice-Hall,

    Inc.

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    13/14

    Movement

    Analysis

    51

    Kestenberg,

    Judith

    S.

    1965

    The Role of Movement

    Patterns in

    Development:

    II-Flow

    of

    Ten-

    sion and Effort.

    Psychoanalytic

    Quarterly34:5I7-563.

    1967

    The

    Role

    of Movement

    Patterns in

    Development:

    III--The

    Control

    of

    Shape. Psychoanalytic

    Quarterly

    36:356-409.

    Laban, Rudolf

    197I

    Rudolf

    Laban

    Speaks

    about

    Movementand Dance.

    Edited

    by

    Lisa

    Ull-

    mann.

    Addlestone,

    Surrey:

    Laban Art of Movement

    Centre.

    Lamb,

    Warren,

    and

    Elizabeth

    Watson

    1979

    Body

    Code:

    The

    Meaning

    in Movement. Boston:

    Routledge

    &

    Kegan

    Paul.

    Lamb, Warren,

    and

    David

    Turner

    1969

    Management

    Behavior.

    New

    York: International

    Universities Press.

    Lomax,

    Alan,

    Irmgard

    Bartenieff,

    and Forrestine

    Paulay

    I968

    Dance

    Style

    and

    Culture.

    In Folk

    Song Style

    and

    Culture,

    edited

    by

    Alan

    Lomax,

    222-273.

    Washington,

    DC: American Association

    for

    the Advancement of Science.

    Maletic,

    Vera

    1987

    Body, Space, Expression:

    the

    Developmentof Rudolf

    Laban's Movement

    and Dance

    Concepts.

    New York: Mouton de

    Gruyter.

    Mayo,

    Clara,

    and

    Nancy Henley,

    eds.

    1981

    Genderand Nonverbal

    Behavior. New York:

    Springer-Verlag.

    North,

    Marion

    1972

    Personality

    Assessment

    through

    Movement.

    London: Macdonald & Evans.

    Reich,

    Wilhelm

    1949

    Character

    Analysis.

    New

    York:

    Farrar,

    Straus & Giroux.

    Rosenfeld,

    Howard M.

    1982

    Measurement of

    Body

    Motion and Orientation.

    In

    Handbook

    of

    Methods

    of

    Nonverbal

    Behavior

    Research,

    dited

    by

    Klaus R.

    Scherer

    and

    Paul

    Ekman,

    199-286.

    Cambridge: Cambridge University

    Press.

    Schechner,

    Richard

    1973

    On

    Kinesic

    Analysis:

    A Discussion with

    Daniel

    N.

    Stern. The

    Drama

    Review

    17,

    no.

    3

    (T59):II4-126.

    1988

    Magnitudes

    of

    Performance.

    In

    Performance heory,

    251-288.

    New

    York:

    Routledge.

    Schechner,

    Richard,

    with

    Cynthia

    Mintz

    1973

    Kinesics

    and

    Performance.

    The

    Drama Review

    17,

    no.

    3

    (T59):Io2-

    io8.

    Scheflen,

    Albert

    E.

    1973

    Communicational tructure.

    Bloomington:

    Indiana

    University

    Press.

    1974

    How Behavior

    Means.

    Garden

    City,

    NJ:

    Anchor

    Press/Doubleday.

    1978

    Susan Smiled:

    On

    Explanation

    in

    Family

    Therapy.

    Family

    Pro-

    cess:59-68.

    Scheflen,

    Albert

    E.,

    with Alice

    Scheflen

    1972

    Body

    Language

    and Social

    Order:

    Communication s Behavioral

    Control.

    Englewood

    Cliffs,

    NJ:

    Prentice-Hall,

    Inc.

    Skupien,

    Janet

    1982

    Behavior as

    Sign.

    Unpublished manuscript.

    Sossin,

    K. Mark

    1987

    Reliability

    of

    the

    Kestenberg

    Movement Profile.

    MovementStudies:

    Observer

    Agreement

    :23-28.

  • 7/26/2019 Daly-Movement Analysis R

    14/14

    52

    Ann

    Daly

    Stebbins,

    Genevieve

    [1902]

    I977

    Delsarte

    System of

    Expression.

    New York:

    Dance Horizons.

    Reprint.

    Steinman,

    Louise

    1986

    The

    Knowing

    Body:

    Elements

    of

    Contemporary

    erformance

    nd Dance.

    Boston:

    Shambhala.

    Stern,

    Daniel N.

    1985

    The

    Interpersonal

    World

    of

    the

    Infant:

    A View

    from

    Psychoanalysis

    nd

    Developmental

    Psychology.

    New York: Basic

    Books,

    Inc.

    Suleiman,

    Susan Rubin

    1986

    The

    Female

    Body

    in

    WesternCulture.

    Cambridge,

    MA:

    Harvard

    Uni-

    versity

    Press.

    Topaz,

    Muriel,

    ed.

    1988

    The

    Notation Issue.

    Choreography

    nd

    Dance: An

    InternationalJour-

    nal

    I

    (no.

    i).

    Weitz,

    Shirley

    1976 Sex Differences in Nonverbal Communication. Sex Roles2 (no.

    2):

    I75-84.

    Wex,

    Marianne

    1979

    Let's

    Take

    Back

    Our

    Space :

    Female

    and

    Male

    Body

    Language

    as

    a

    Result

    of

    Patriarchal

    Structures.

    Hamburg:

    Frauenliteraturverlag

    Her-

    mine Fees.

    Winter,

    Deborah Du

    Nann

    1987

    Field

    Studies of Action

    Profiling Reliability.

    Movement

    Studies:

    Ob-

    server

    Agreement

    :21-22.

    Ann

    Daly

    is a

    contributing

    editor to

    TDR. She has

    written dance

    criticism

    for

    TDR,

    High

    Performance,

    Ballett

    International,

    and Women

    and

    Perfor-

    mance.