cognitive neuroscience: cascading cognitive control

1
6 | JANUARY 2004 | VOLUME 5 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro HIGHLIGHTS Although it is clear that the prefrontal cortex is responsible for cognitive control — ensuring that our actions are appropriate not just in light of our sensory input but also for a given context or event — the organization of the prefrontal cortex is controversial. Koechlin et al. propose a new model for the functioning of the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) in which different areas form a hierarchy of cognitive control, from the premotor cortex to more anterior regions. The model is best understood by considering a well- known example of cognitive control. If a phone rings, a default response would be to answer it. However, if you are at a friend’s house (a different context), you would not usually answer the phone. But this contextual control can be overridden if, for example, your friend has gone out and has asked you to take any calls. In the model proposed by Koechlin and colleagues, there are three levels of control. The premotor cortex is responsible for ‘sensory control’ — answering a ringing phone. More anteriorly, the caudal LPFC carries out ‘contextual control’ — using cues that accompany the stimulus to tell you that it is inappropriate to answer your friend’s phone. And the rostral LPFC is responsible for ‘episodic control’, in which earlier cues (a conversation with your friend) tell you how you should respond during a given episode. In this hierarchy, signals flow from the most anterior parts of the LPFC to the more posterior parts and the premotor cortex, conveying top–down information to control our actions. The authors tested their model using behavioural studies and functional imaging — with rather more abstract stimuli — as well as information theory for quantifying cognitive control. Subjects were given tasks in which either sensory or contextual information could be varied, either alone or with episodic information. As predicted by the model, reaction times increased with stimulus, context and episode factors, and both stimulus and context effects were additive with the episode effect. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) allowed the authors to see whether different types of cognitive control were reflected in increased activity in different parts of the LPFC. Consistent with the cascading, top–down nature of the model, activity in the premotor cortex showed effects of stimulus, context and episode; caudal LPFC showed effects of context and episode, but not stimulus; and rostral LPFC only showed effects of episode. To test the prediction that the effects of context and episode in more posterior areas result from top–down control from the most anterior areas, the authors used a structural equation model to investigate the effective connectivity of the LPFC. The results also supported the three-tiered model of cognitive control. In this model, representations are distributed in the LPFC depending on their temporal structure, rather than their content or internal complexity. It joins a number of other models of prefrontal function and is unlikely to be the last, but it can also explain the pattern of results seen in a number of other studies in which rostral and caudal LPFC showed activity that depended on whether cognitive control was contextual or episodic. The story is far from over, but this study could be an important chapter. Rachel Jones References and links ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER Koechlin, E. et al. The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science 302, 1181–1185 (2003) FURTHER READING Wood, J. N. & Grafman, J. Human prefrontal cortex: processing and representational perspectives. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 139–147 (2003) | Duncan, J. An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2, 820–829 (2001) Cascading cognitive control COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE Neuromancy It was definitely the headline that got me reading. “Devices that read human thought now possible” reported the San Francisco Herald (10 November 2003), referring to studies presented at the past Society for Neuroscience meeting. Happily, my initial dismay at having missed the poster subsided when I discovered that the story was actually on brain–machine interfaces that harness neuronal activity to guide mechanical limbs. Such interfaces had already been used in monkeys, and the newspaper, prompted by preliminary results on patients with Parkinson’s disease, discussed the possibility of using them in people. Relieved by the fact that my secret thoughts will remain private for now, my attention turned to the financial twist of the story. On the one hand, Miguel Nicolelis, a leader in this field, is quoted as saying “I have no interests in any business … I want to have fun; I don’t want to make money. What I am very afraid of is that people who really want to make a buck out of this will be rushing into the clinical thing.” On the other, John Donaghue, another expert on this area, pointed out that he and his colleagues “realized the only way to fully exploit the technology was to form a company capable of raising the money needed to carry out very expensive clinical studies”. Cyberkinetics Inc. is the result of their realization. Regardless of whose side you are on, a growing number of neuroscientists are becoming aware of the financial implications of what they do in the lab, and spend more time with their colleagues over at the Technology Transfer Office. Although many of them are surely driven by their desire to foster technological progress, I suspect we would not need a mind-reading device to spot the phrase ‘lucrative patent’ in their thoughts. Juan Carlos López IN THE NEWS ©2004 Nature Publishing Group

Post on 26-Jul-2016

216 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

6 | JANUARY 2004 | VOLUME 5 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

H I G H L I G H T S

Although it is clear that the prefrontal cortex isresponsible for cognitive control — ensuring that ouractions are appropriate not just in light of our sensoryinput but also for a given context or event — theorganization of the prefrontal cortex is controversial.Koechlin et al. propose a new model for the functioningof the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) in whichdifferent areas form a hierarchy of cognitive control,from the premotor cortex to more anterior regions.

The model is best understood by considering a well-known example of cognitive control. If a phone rings, adefault response would be to answer it. However, if youare at a friend’s house (a different context), you wouldnot usually answer the phone. But this contextualcontrol can be overridden if, for example, your friendhas gone out and has asked you to take any calls.

In the model proposed by Koechlin and colleagues,there are three levels of control. The premotor cortex isresponsible for ‘sensory control’ — answering a ringingphone. More anteriorly, the caudal LPFC carries out‘contextual control’ — using cues that accompany thestimulus to tell you that it is inappropriate to answeryour friend’s phone. And the rostral LPFC isresponsible for ‘episodic control’, in which earlier cues(a conversation with your friend) tell you how youshould respond during a given episode. In thishierarchy, signals flow from the most anterior parts ofthe LPFC to the more posterior parts and the premotorcortex, conveying top–down information to controlour actions.

The authors tested their model using behaviouralstudies and functional imaging — with rather moreabstract stimuli — as well as information theory forquantifying cognitive control. Subjects were given tasksin which either sensory or contextual informationcould be varied, either alone or with episodic

information. As predicted by the model, reaction timesincreased with stimulus, context and episode factors,and both stimulus and context effects were additivewith the episode effect.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)allowed the authors to see whether different types ofcognitive control were reflected in increased activity indifferent parts of the LPFC. Consistent with thecascading, top–down nature of the model, activity inthe premotor cortex showed effects of stimulus,context and episode; caudal LPFC showed effects ofcontext and episode, but not stimulus; and rostralLPFC only showed effects of episode. To test theprediction that the effects of context and episode inmore posterior areas result from top–down controlfrom the most anterior areas, the authors used astructural equation model to investigate the effectiveconnectivity of the LPFC. The results also supportedthe three-tiered model of cognitive control.

In this model, representations are distributed in theLPFC depending on their temporal structure, ratherthan their content or internal complexity. It joins anumber of other models of prefrontal function and isunlikely to be the last, but it can also explain thepattern of results seen in a number of other studies inwhich rostral and caudal LPFC showed activity thatdepended on whether cognitive control was contextualor episodic. The story is far from over, but this studycould be an important chapter.

Rachel JonesReferences and links

ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER Koechlin, E. et al. The architecture ofcognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science 302, 1181–1185(2003)FURTHER READING Wood, J. N. & Grafman, J. Human prefrontal cortex:processing and representational perspectives. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4,139–147 (2003) | Duncan, J. An adaptive coding model of neural function inprefrontal cortex. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2, 820–829 (2001)

Cascading cognitive control

C O G N I T I V E N E U R O S C I E N C E

NeuromancyIt was definitely the headlinethat got me reading. “Devicesthat read human thought nowpossible” reported the San Francisco Herald(10 November 2003),referring to studies presentedat the past Society forNeuroscience meeting.Happily, my initial dismay athaving missed the postersubsided when I discoveredthat the story was actually onbrain–machine interfaces thatharness neuronal activity toguide mechanical limbs.Such interfaces had alreadybeen used in monkeys, andthe newspaper, prompted bypreliminary results on patientswith Parkinson’s disease,discussed the possibility ofusing them in people.

Relieved by the fact that mysecret thoughts will remainprivate for now, my attentionturned to the financial twist ofthe story. On the one hand,Miguel Nicolelis, a leader inthis field, is quoted as saying“I have no interests in anybusiness … I want to havefun; I don’t want to makemoney. What I am very afraidof is that people who reallywant to make a buck out ofthis will be rushing into theclinical thing.” On the other,John Donaghue, anotherexpert on this area, pointedout that he and hiscolleagues “realized the onlyway to fully exploit thetechnology was to form acompany capable of raisingthe money needed to carryout very expensive clinicalstudies”. Cyberkinetics Inc. isthe result of their realization.

Regardless of whose sideyou are on, a growingnumber of neuroscientistsare becoming aware of thefinancial implications of whatthey do in the lab, and spendmore time with theircolleagues over at theTechnology Transfer Office.Although many of them aresurely driven by their desire tofoster technologicalprogress, I suspect we wouldnot need a mind-readingdevice to spot the phrase‘lucrative patent’ in theirthoughts.

Juan Carlos López

IN THE NEWS

© 2004 Nature Publishing Group