briefing_grammar_001_.pdf

Upload: ruqayarob3174

Post on 03-Apr-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/28/2019 Briefing_Grammar_001_.pdf

    1/8

    ONLINE DELTA

    MODULE TWO

    BRIEFING:

    TEACHING GRAMMAR

    How (or if) grammar is taught goes to the heart of the second

    language learning business, which, in a classroom setting, is a

    very demanding and frequently frustrating endeavour. It is our

    responsibility to facilitate this process and in doing so combine

    what we believe is the best approach - in a world of ideal

    students, resources etc. - with what our students can cope with,

    given their own expectations of learning in general and the

    language classroom in particular. The essential dichotomy is

    between a focus on form and an approach which is meaning-

    based, and we believe it is the integration of these polarities

    which constitutes the best way forward. For us the major

    question is how this integration is to be achieved.

    There is little research support for the wall-building approach

    to the teaching of grammar, in which discrete items are

    accumulated ("done") one at a time according to some

    hierarchy of difficulty or learnability, where the teaching mode

    is primarily one of transmission and the knowledge gained is

    largely declarative and rarely procedural. We view the

    acquisition of a second language as a process of linguistic

    metamorphosis in which structures interrelate and change each

    other, an organic growth which is unstable and cyclical. To

    foster this growth, we further believe that learners need

    exposure to a variety of text types in which they can see the

    interdependence of grammar, lexis and discourse. `Grammar`,

    for our purposes, does not include grammaticised lexis

    although teachers need to be aware of the overlap between the

    two as described by Lewis, 1993.

  • 7/28/2019 Briefing_Grammar_001_.pdf

    2/8

    The following questions arise:

    1.Should grammar be taught?

    Assuming that the answer is yes, then:

    2.Are there models or frameworks available for the teaching ofgrammar which we can use?

    1) Should grammar be taught?

    The short answer is yes. Even if one took an extreme, "natural"

    acquisition position, believing that sufficient exposure of the

    right kind and some interaction (i.e. an attempt to recreate

    mother-tongue learning conditions) will lead to the learning of

    grammar, there are other factors to consider, primarily the

    expectations of our students and the time they have to devote to

    learning English. A teacher's classroom practice should be

    based on a view of language and of teaching, as well as a view

    of learning. But is this (or any other) position tenable anyway,

    and to what extent does it make sense to talk about a single,

    unchanging point of view?

    Where do teachers, teacher trainers or course book writers look

    for guidance when deciding how to teach grammar? The

    obvious answer would seem to be from research into second

    language acquisition. (Of course, advice from colleagues,

    attendance at coferences etc. are also significant sources of

    guidance - as is personal experience: but we must remember

    that 20 years experience can be the same as 2 varied years

    repeated 10 times). Do the researchers have anything definitive

    to say which can help? The first problem here is that theories of

    second language acquisition (SLA) have multiplied so much

    that one leading academic in the field, Michael Long, has

    talked about the "culling" of theories. At the 1998 IATEFL

    conference, Rod Ellis was rather pessimistic about the help

    teachers could get from researchers:

    "SLA research does not and cannot tell us how to teach, but

    can help us make more informed decisions."

    He cited various experiments which have led to conclusions

    which contradict each other, and to others which contradict

    what many teachers believe they have learnt from their own

    experience. One experiment with tasks concluded that a two-

    http://intranetdev/pages/spf/curriculumDevelopment/papers/grammar.asp#taught#taughthttp://intranetdev/pages/spf/curriculumDevelopment/papers/grammar.asp#taught#taughthttp://intranetdev/pages/spf/curriculumDevelopment/papers/grammar.asp#taught#taughthttp://intranetdev/pages/spf/curriculumDevelopment/papers/grammar.asp#models#modelshttp://intranetdev/pages/spf/curriculumDevelopment/papers/grammar.asp#models#modelshttp://intranetdev/pages/spf/curriculumDevelopment/papers/grammar.asp#models#modelshttp://intranetdev/pages/spf/curriculumDevelopment/papers/grammar.asp#models#modelshttp://intranetdev/pages/spf/curriculumDevelopment/papers/grammar.asp#models#modelshttp://intranetdev/pages/spf/curriculumDevelopment/papers/grammar.asp#models#modelshttp://intranetdev/pages/spf/curriculumDevelopment/papers/grammar.asp#taught#taught
  • 7/28/2019 Briefing_Grammar_001_.pdf

    3/8

    way information gap produced more negotiation of meaning

    than one-way tasks, a conclusion that would surprise very few

    teachers - but another concluded that this was not the case.

    Another suggested that correction in the form of recasts (a more

    recent term for what was formerly known as caretaker languageor motherese) led to improved accuracy but was contradicted

    by other research.

    Allwright (1988) discusses the dilemma faced by the teacher

    trainer:

    ".the really conscientious teacher trainer will have no

    confidence left at all.perhaps he should find a way of avoiding

    prescription altogether since he will be unable to find research

    support for anything he says about teaching methods, about

    what a teacher should or should not do in the classroom."

    (p.47)

    One is reminded of the bumble bee which, according to various

    laws of physics, does not have wings with a large enough

    surface area to carry it through the air, given the weight of the

    average bee. The bee is unaware of this and flies anyway.

    Language teaching is notoriously difficult to pin down and

    analyse in experiments which can be duplicated or even

    controlled to a satisfactory degree: there are just too many

    variables, most of them human. Nevertheless, we go about our

    business, as do our students, and, for whatever reason, learning

    goes on. Teachers have to be pragmatists and quickly acquire a

    repertoire of what appears to work in the classroom. They may

    be able to articulate some theoretical basis for what they do, but

    equally their eclecticism may only be subconsciously

    principled. But can some tendencies, at least, be discerned from

    SLA research and used by the teacher, to supplement the

    instincts honed by experience?

    "In a behaviour as complex as second language learning, we

    cannot play only the doubting game, we must temper our

    cautious doubts with a willingness to accept certain assertions

    until we can categorically rule them out." (p. 245)

    What assertions concerning the teaching of grammar appear to

    have some credibility at the moment? In 'How Languages are

  • 7/28/2019 Briefing_Grammar_001_.pdf

    4/8

    Learned' (Spada & Lightbown, OUP, 1993) the authors

    conclude that:

    "There is increasing evidence that learners continue to have

    difficulty with basic structures of the language in programswhich offer no form-focused instruction"(p.150).

    In the early 1980s Krashen"s Monitor Theory, as it came to be

    known, won many converts who found the idea of

    comprehensible input instinctively appealing, and for a short

    time grammar teaching either took a back seat or was forgotten

    altogether. This was derived in large part from studies in first

    and second language acquisition which revealed remarkable

    similarities between the two and also showed an unvarying

    sequence in the acquisition of certain morphemes. This led to

    the view that the learner's internal syllabus will always prevail

    and that attempts to grade syllabuses for teaching purposes

    according to a hierarchy of grammar were doomed to failure. It

    should be noted that teaching material and ideas for the

    implementation of a methodology based on this view were in

    conspicuously short supply. It was clear, nevertheless, that

    there is no support for the isolation, presentation and practice of

    individual features of grammar, as stated in the introduction.

    Subsequent studies have not changed this position, but have led

    to the rehabilitation of grammar teaching within a different

    framework. One approach, proposed by Michael Long and

    others in the early 1980s, developed by Legutke and Thomas

    (Process and Experience in the Language Classroom, Longman

    1991) and recently popularised by Jane Willis, is task-based

    learning. Another response to the conundrum of how to fit a

    focus on form into an approach which places primacy on

    meaning is grammatical consciousness raising (Rutherford,

    Second Language Grammar:Learning and Teaching, 1987).

    Long's research suggests that instruction can change the rate of

    grammar acquisition (but not the route) and can result in a

    higher level of achievement. It is also argued that it can lead to

    greater accuracy. Above all, however, it is the requirements and

    expectations of the learners (and, perhaps less obviously, of

    teachers and course book writers) which make grammar

    teaching in some guise essential. Adults have the cognitive

    apparatus which enables them to short-cut the hypothesis-

    making of the child, and while comprehensible input may help

  • 7/28/2019 Briefing_Grammar_001_.pdf

    5/8

    learners to discover rules, it cannot always help them to

    understand what is notgrammatically correct.

    Another major factor which complicates matters for the teacher

    is the need to be learner-centred. Having researched the latestthinking on the teaching of grammar, we find that many of our

    students prefer a more traditional approach, more akin to

    grammar translation than Communicative Language Teaching

    (CLT), and the teacher finds herself balancing what she

    believes to be the best approach with the need to satisfy the

    need (expressed at the beginning of the course when surveys of

    learning styles etc. are undertaken) felt by many students for

    overt grammar practice a la Murphy. On longer courses learner

    training can be of some help, but when time is short studentsgenerally want to get on with studying the language as soon

    and as much as possible, regardless of what we may tell them

    about the value of improving how they learn.

    In fact, when concluding that grammar must be included as a

    significant element in the syllabus, one must also conclude that

    there is very little that can be absolutely ruled out in terms of

    how we proceed in the classroom. This is because almost

    everything has some merit with some classes. Rote learning, for

    example, either via drilling or simply through asking students

    to go home and learn a list of vocabulary, to be tested at the end

    of the week, can be invaluable for certain learners: and it is also

    true to say that memorisation, however done, is a crucial part of

    foreign language learning.

    2) Are there models or frameworks available for the

    teaching of grammar which we can use?

    There are several and they have been the subject of (sometimes

    acrimonious) debate in recent years. PPP, (present, practise,

    produce), the father of them all in modern TEFL, refuses to

    take heed of the obituaries that have been written for it. ARC

    (authentic and restricted use of language, clarification of

    language - Scrivener) and ESA (engage, study, activate -

    Harmer) have been proposed: all three have some merit, though

    it should be said that the constant favour that PPP has enjoyed

    has more to do with security for the teacher, student and course

  • 7/28/2019 Briefing_Grammar_001_.pdf

    6/8

    book writer than theories about second language acquisition.

    The other two models deserve greater consideration because

    they do not prescribe but merely seek to describe. What all

    three have in common is an understanding that at some point

    language forms need to be focused on, that students requireopportunities for practice and that "real world" use (or as near

    as we can come to it in the language classroom) should take

    place. For ARC and ESA there is no necessary sequence to the

    elements: PPP carried with it an understood sequence and

    assumed a very discrete item approach, although in truth there

    is no reason why it should not be more flexible, as evidenced

    by the test-teach-test adaptation.

    The inductive - deductive continuum has proved helpful whenattempting to place grammar teaching in the context of good

    practice. It is an assumption of this paper that attempts to

    understand (previously unknown) grammar by the student via a

    context which he can understand to a large extent -albeit with

    help from the teacher- is superior to an overtly deductive

    approach in which the teacher gives a rule from which

    examples may be generated. The need to adapt according to the

    nature of the students is an important rider to this position, as

    indicated above. Another significant variable is level. It maywell be that some advanced students can cope with the

    deductive teaching of grammar and find it more efficient to

    learn in this way (students studying for exams at FCE and

    above can also benefit from packaged, cut and dried shortcuts

    which they can memorise then forget as soon as the exam is

    over). Nevertheless, any methodology which demands deeper

    affective and cognitive processing by the student will make the

    consequent learning more personal, memorable and permanent.

    To a large extent, then, the teaching of grammar will be

    inextricably entwined with the teaching of skills and

    vocabulary. At times explicit grammatical features of a text -

    perhaps new, perhaps not - will be highlighted, rules of

    grammar made explicit: at other times students will be exposed

    to "new" grammar but it will not be the object of specific

    attention, rather it will be "noticed", feed the acquisition system

    and modify the interlanguage of the student the next time it is

    encountered. Thus we can hope to incorporate some of the

    suggestions from SLA research into our methodology, and

  • 7/28/2019 Briefing_Grammar_001_.pdf

    7/8

    combining these with our own understanding, based on

    experience, reading, conversations with colleagues etc., we can

    come up with some simple "do's" and "don'ts", e.g

    1.

    Allow processing time (although the "silent period" recommended forbeginners is rarely possible)

    2. Provide plenty of exposure to longer written and spoken texts3. Don't consistently isolate structures as this will prevent students from seeing

    how they interact with and modify each other

    4. In most cases, work from meaning to form5. Provide plenty of opportunities for interaction and negotiation of meaning

    (e.g. via the use of tasks)

    6. Accept that there is no one-to-one equivalence between teaching and learning7.

    Don't relentlessly demand accuracy in some structures which researchsuggests, and your own experience tells you, are late acquired, e.g. third

    person simple present 's' with low level students

    8. Allow time for repetition and revision so that elements of grammar canbecome "automatically" accessible, leaving processing time for higher level

    decisions.

    9. Try to precede a focus on form with a) some extensive skills work (whichincludes the item or items of "new" grammar) some days earlier which may

    encourage "noticing" and begin the process of interlanguage adjustment and b)

    within the same lesson, some receptive skills work with a focus on meaningbefore switching to form.

    10.Make grammar the explicit focus of some activities, particularly grammarwhich students have already encountered in the classroom, perhaps through

    grammatical consciousness raising.

    There is an assumption implicit in this list that "grammar" is a

    single, monolithic entity and these guidelines apply to any and

    all aspects of it, but this is not the case. There are, for example,

    features of grammar for which repetition and drilling can be

    more appropriate than others. These tend to be aspects of

    sentence-level grammar where there can be no debate about the

    requirement to use certain forms, such as question formation

    with auxiliary verbs. A great deal of grammar, however, is

    dependent on context, speaker intention, discourse-level

    meaning and even knowledge of the world. This is another

    variable the teacher needs to be aware of when planning

    classroom activities.

  • 7/28/2019 Briefing_Grammar_001_.pdf

    8/8

    Our approach to grammar teaching will, then, be informed by

    these (and other) provisional specifications gained from the

    study of instructed second language learning, by the teacher's

    understanding of syntactic and semantic significance but will

    be tempered greatly by the needs and learning styles of ourstudents, and by our own understanding gained through

    experience of how we can use a framework of the types

    described above to provide the security of a structure for

    ourselves and our students. Ultimately, however, it is highly

    likely that this security will come more from a course book that

    meets the teacher's practical and theoretical requirements with

    the minimum number of amendments.

    References

    Borg Teachers' Theories in Grammar TeachingELTJ 53/3

    Ellis, RSecond Language Acquisition Research - What's in it

    for teachers? IATEFL Conference 1998

    Harmer, J.How to Teach English Longman 1998

    Nunan, D.Language Teaching Methodology Prentice Hall

    1991

    Nunan, D. Teaching Grammar in Context, ELTJ 52/2

    Allwright, D. Observation in the Language Classroom

    (Longman, 1988) Brown, D. Principles of Language Learning

    and Teaching(Prentice Hall, 1987, 2nd. edition)

    Lewis, The Lexical Approach, Language Teaching

    Publications, 1993

    Scrivener, J.Learning TeachingMacmillan 1994