abstracts of poster presentations

22
This article was downloaded by: [Western Kentucky University] On: 30 April 2013, At: 19:39 Publisher: Taylor & Francis Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Bioacoustics: The International Journal of Animal Sound and its Recording Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tbio20 Abstracts of Poster Presentations Published online: 16 Feb 2012. To cite this article: (2012): Abstracts of Poster Presentations, Bioacoustics: The International Journal of Animal Sound and its Recording, 21:1, 65-85 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2011.648773 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Post on 09-Dec-2016

218 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

This article was downloaded by: [Western Kentucky University]On: 30 April 2013, At: 19:39Publisher: Taylor & FrancisInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Bioacoustics: The International Journalof Animal Sound and its RecordingPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tbio20

Abstracts of Poster PresentationsPublished online: 16 Feb 2012.

To cite this article: (2012): Abstracts of Poster Presentations, Bioacoustics: The InternationalJournal of Animal Sound and its Recording, 21:1, 65-85

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2011.648773

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representationthat the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of anyinstructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primarysources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings,demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly orindirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Page 2: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

ABSTRACTS OF POSTER PRESENTATIONS

Time resolution in the auditory pathway of the cicada Tettigetta josei

D. Alves and P.J. Fonseca

Departamento de Biologia Animal e Centro de Biologia Ambiental, Faculdade de Ciencias,

Universidade de Lisboa, Bloco C2, Campo Grande, 1749-016, Lisboa, Portugal

[email protected]

Mate finding in the cicada Tettigetta josei is mediated by acoustic communication. The

species-specific male song is composed of complex phrases and the shortest pulse

periods vary between 4 and 8ms, depending on the body temperature. Here we

investigated if the central nervous system is able to resolve the fine time structure of

the song and the temperature dependence of these responses. Intracellular recordings of

auditory interneurons in the metathoracic-abdominal ganglionic complex and

simultaneous auditory nerve recordings were obtained. The experimental stimuli

were the species’ calling song pulses presented at different rates. Body temperature

was controlled with a Peltier element. PSTH and vector strength analysis indicate that

some interneurons were able to resolve pulse periods in the 3–4ms range when the

body temperature was above 24–268C (n ¼ 6). These cells were able to resolve pulse

periods of 6–8ms even when the body temperature was at 168C. The auditory

periphery does not seem to be a limiting factor as averaging analysis in the whole nerve

recordings shows that the pooled receptor response is able to resolve gaps of at least 1–

2ms in the whole temperature range tested (16–288C). Further studies will be

necessary in order to test whether this temporal resolution is maintained at the brain

level.

Lusitanian Toadfish song reflects male quality

M.C.P. Amorima, J.M. Simoesa, N. Mendoncab, N. Bandarrab, V.C. Almadaa and P.J.

Fonsecac

aUIEE - Unidade de Investigacao em Eco-etologia, Instituto Superior de Psicologia

Aplicada, Lisboa, Portugal; bHead of Nutrition Laboratory, IPIMAR, Portugal;cDepartamento de Biologia Animal, Faculdade de Ciencias, Universidade de Lisboa,

Portugal

[email protected]

During the breeding season, Lusitanian Toadfish males become territorial and emit an

advertisement call (the boatwhistle, BW) to attract mates. We test the hypothesis that

ISSN 0952-4622 print/ISSN 2165-0586 online

q 2012 Taylor & Francis

http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2011.648773

http://www.tandfonline.com

Bioacoustics

Vol. 21, No. 1, February 2012, 65–85

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 3: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

boatwhistles can convey information on individual quality by relating calling activity

and signal parameters with male physical features, namely male total length,

gonadosomatic index, sonic muscle index and male condition, measured by the

condition factor and muscle lipid content. We recorded 22 males in the Tagus River

estuary (Portugal) during the breeding season (2006 and 2007) for up to 8 days and for

an average of 35 h. Single males were kept inside closed artificial nests and groups of

6–8 individuals maintained a normal chorus activity. Acoustic activity varied greatly

among subject males. All males produced boatwhistles during the study period but

calling rate varied markedly among them. The average calling rate varied from 0.1 to

361 BWh-1 (overall mean ¼ 39.9 BWh-1) and the majority of time was spent in

silence (72.4% of the recorded hours). Boatwhistle emission rate and the fundamental

frequency of boatwhistles (equivalent to the sonic muscle contraction frequency during

sound production) were correlated with male condition (somatic and lipid content).

Boatwhistle emission rate was also correlated with sonic muscle index, i.e. with sonic

muscle hypertrophy. Calling rate variability also decreased with increased sonic

muscle and gonadosomatic indexes. These results suggest that only males with higher

somatic and reproductive condition and with higher sonic muscle mass were able to

sustain elevated acoustic activity and sonic muscle contraction rates during sound

production. In conclusion, boatwhistles seem to be able to signal male quality during

social interactions.

The song of Eugaster spinulosa (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae): description and

relationship between song parameters and temperature

A. Ariasa, D. Llusiaa, J.F. Beltranb, H. El Moudenc, T. Slimanic, M. Radic, A. Fattahc and

R. Marqueza

aFonoteca Zoologica, Dept. de Biodiversidad y Biologıa Evolutiva, Museo Nacional de

Ciencias Naturales (CSIC), C/ Jose Gutierrez Abascal 2, 28006, Madrid, Spain;bDepartamento de Fisiologıa y Zoologıa, Universidad de Sevilla, C/ Avda, Reina

Mercedes s/n. 41012, Sevilla, Spain; cLaboratory “Biodiversity and Dynamics of the

Ecosystems”, Department of Biology, Faculty of Science Semlalia. BP: 2390, Marrakech

40.000, Morocco

[email protected]

We describe quantitatively the song of Eugaster spinulosa (Johannson 1763),

Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae. To the best of our knowledge there are no previous

descriptions available of the song of any species in the genus. The description is based

on acoustic recordings of five males from the mountains of Jebilet, Marrakech

(Morocco). Recordings were made in a semi-anechoic chamber, in stereo, using

G.R.A.S. 40 BE (frequency range 10 Hz–100 kHz) and Sennheiser MKH 800 P48

(frequency range 30Hz–50 kHz) microphones, at a sampling rate of 192 kHz, on a

Sound Devices 722. The calling song consisted of sequences of a variable number of

echemes, each one of which comprised 2 to 11 broadband syllables, with the frequency

ranging from approximately 7 kHz to almost 80 kHz, but most energy concentrated

between 7 kHz and 27 kHz. We also investigated the relationship between song

parameters and temperature, and attempted to identify the static and dynamic

parameters of the song.

66 Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 4: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Stridulation organs and distress signals in various species of Trox F. (Coleoptera:

Trogidae)

J. Benes

Department of Zoology, Faculty of Science, Charles University in Prague, Czech Republic

[email protected]

Within the genus Trox the structure of the stridulation organs does not vary significantly

among species. I analysed distress signals in five species of the genus Trox F. (T. sabulosus,

T. cadaverinus, T. scaber, T. eversmannii and T. puncticollis). Each beetle was held with

pincers for a few seconds and then released, simulating an attempted predation by a bird.

Following this disturbance, the beetle would spontaneously stridulate for several minutes.

I was able to recognise various differences with regards to the length of chirps and

syllables of these distress signals. These differences appear to be species specific.

Agonistic context and acoustic communication in a cichlid: fine sound analysis and

first playback experiments

F. Bertuccci, M. Beauchaud, J. Attia and N. Mathevon

Laboratoire Ecologie et Neuro-Ethologie Sensorielles (EA3988) Universite Jean Monnet

23 Rue Dr Paul Michelon 42023 Saint Etienne cedex2, France

[email protected]

In Malawi cichlids of the Pseudotropheus complex, acoustic signals have been shown to

act on females during inter-specific mate recognition and, together with visual and

chemical cues, may thus play a role in the fast adaptive radiation of African cichlids. The

aim of our study was to investigate the role of the acoustic channel in an agonistic

context. We recorded emitted sounds and associated behaviours during interactions

between a resident and an intruder in both sexes of Pseudotropheus zebra. To assess the

importance of sound communication, fish were allowed to interact only via an acoustic

channel (playback experiment), only via a visual channel, or via both channels. The fine

acoustic structure of recorded sounds was analysed in both temporal and frequency

domains. Initial results reveal new information on sound structure and show that

behaviour-related and/or individual-related information could be coded by the emitted

sounds.

Calls of White-naped Crane reflect body mass of a bird

E.V. Bragina and I.R. Beme

Dept. of Vertebrate Zoology, Biological Faculty, Moscow State University

[email protected]

Sounds of passerine and non-passerine birds may provide data on body condition of a bird.

Information about organism quality may be encoded by different ways. For example, a

larger bird should have a larger syrinx that generates lower fundamental frequencies.

Bioacoustics 67

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 5: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Second, a longer trachea corresponds to smaller formant dispersion. More than 60 bird

species have a tracheal elongation i.e. loops of trachea inside of sternum. It has been

suggested that tracheal elongation is a way to imitate sounds of a larger animal due to

decreasing formant dispersion. We analysed correlation of body mass and sound

parameters in the calls of the White-naped Crane Grus vipio. We recorded calls of six

captive breeding pairs i.e. 12 adult cranes. We described the vocal repertoire of

White-naped Cranes as five types of single calls and a duet call – a stable sequence of

single calls. There are three tonal calls (guard call, bark and buzzing) and two non-tonal,

rhythmical calls (purring and quacking). Non-tonal rhythmical calls are more suitable for

highlighting of formants than tonal calls. We found that none of the parameters of tonal

calls correlated with body mass. At once, peak frequency of purring significantly

correlated with body mass. Frequency parameters of duet calls significantly correlated

with body mass. We suggest that sound duration should be connected with body condition:

a larger animal has a greater lung volume and can call for longer. Nevertheless, none of the

call types had a duration that correlated with body mass. Thus, (1) peak frequency of

purring correlated with body mass, thus supporting the hypothesis of vocal size

exaggeration and (2) the duet call of White-naped Crane provides information about body

mass.

Group signature in a songbird with a complex song: importance of syntax

E. Briefera, F. Rybakb and T. Aubinb

aSchool of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, Mile

End Road, London E1 4NS, UK; bUniversite Paris Sud, NAMC, CNRS-UMR8620, Equipe

communications acoustiques, Bat. 446, 91405 Orsay cedex, France

[email protected]

Bird songs are among the most complex sounds produced by animals. Song

complexity is thought to have evolved largely through intersexual selection. However,

this complexity may hinder vocal recognition, which plays an important role in a

variety of social contexts, including neighbour recognition during territory defence.

We investigated the influence of song complexity on neighbour recognition in a

territorial songbird with a highly complex song, the Skylark Alauda arvensis. This bird

is a territorial species of open landscape in which pairs settle in stable territories

during the breeding season. Due to the heterogeneity of the habitat, territories are

grouped in patches spaced by few kilometres. We first carried out a detailed analysis

of songs to describe song syntax (the organisation of sound units in sequences within

songs) and to measure song complexity. We showed that Skylark songs are among the

most complex acoustic signals compared to other songbird species, and that

geographical variation exists at the syntax level: in a given patch, males (neighbours)

share several sequences of syllables in their songs, whereas males settled in different

patches (strangers) have no sequences in common. We then tested neighbour

recognition using natural and artificially modified songs. Results showed that the

syntax of sequences shared by neighbours encodes the neighbourhood identity in the

complex song of the species and is used by birds to discriminate neighbours from

strangers. The ordering of syllables within songs is thus behaviourally salient and

carries a particular meaning for the birds.

68 Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 6: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Noise-dependent vocal plasticity in domestic fowl

H. Brumma and L. Schraderb

aMax Planck Institute for Ornithology, Communication and Social Behaviour Group,

Seewiesen, Germany; bFriedrich Loeffler Institute, Institute of Animal Welfare and Animal

Husbandry, Celle, Germany

[email protected]

Since acoustic communication is considerably constrained by environmental noise, some

animals have evolved adaptations to counteract its masking effects. We found in psycho-

acoustic experiments that domestic fowl (n ¼ 19) exhibited the Lombard effect, that is, a

regulation of vocal amplitude depending on the background noise level. This vocal

mechanism for communication in noise has also evolved in mammals and other bird

species. Previous studies have shown that, in addition to the Lombard effect, humans and

New World monkeys also increase the duration of brief vocalizations (below a few

hundred milliseconds) as the background noise level rises, a behaviour which increases the

detection probability of signals in noise by temporal summation. However, in contrast to

primates, the chickens tested did not regulate the duration of their brief call syllables in

relation to the level of masking background noise. This evidence for a lack of regulation of

syllable duration suggests that this signal parameter cannot be regulated to maintain signal

transfer in chickens because it is used to encode information or because this form of

noise-induced vocal plasticity is perhaps an autapomorphy of mammals or even primates.

Our findings indicate that the common problem of acoustic communication in noise has

led to the evolution of a common solution, the Lombard effect, but also to special

adaptations in different taxa.

Multiple sexually selected traits in the Serin: are colouration and song signalling the

same features?

M. Costa and P.G. Mota

Departamento de Antropologia, Universidade de Coimbra, 3000-056, Coimbra, Portugal

[email protected]

After many studies conducted in bird species, it is now understood that most sexually

selected traits are evolutionary signals that females use to select males with particular

characteristics. The presence of multiple traits, such as song and colouration, can either

signal different characteristics or be redundant. In the latter case, traits should be strongly

correlated between individuals. Previous studies conducted in our laboratory have shown

that both coloration and male song are sexually selected traits in the Serin Serinus serinus,

a small cardueline finch. The species is sexually dichromatic, with males presenting

intense yellow colouration, with high concentration of carotenoids. Also, male Serins sign

songs at a very fast rate of sound production, maintaining high frequency amplitude,

sustaining the production of very complex elements far beyond the Podos triangle. We

recorded songs of a group of males of a wild population during breeding and measured

their colouration by spectrophotometry in order to assess if the two traits were correlated

between individuals. We found that song variables and colouration were not correlated in

our sample, indicating that these two traits signal distinct things for their conspecifics.

Bioacoustics 69

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 7: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Temporal patterns of North Atlantic Right Whale vocalizations during winter and

spring in the Northwest Atlantic, USA

A. Guerreiro da Silvaa, E. Vub, D. Rischb and S.M. Van Parijsb

aFaculdade de Ciencias, Universidade de Lisboa, Portugal; bNortheast Fisheries Science

Center, 166 Water Street, Woods Hole, Massachusetts 02543, USA

[email protected]

Visual and acoustic surveys have shown that endangered North Atlantic Right Whales

overwinter in the waters of the Northeast US. Detailed knowledge of this population’s

distribution is critical to reducing anthropogenic mortality events. Passive acoustic studies

have confirmed that Stellwagen Bank National Marine Sanctuary (SBNMS) and Jeffreys

Ledge are areas of high baleen whale acoustic activity during the winter. Right Whales

produce threemain types of calls, the up-call, gun-shot and tonal calls. However, no thorough

assessment has yet been made of which call type is the most appropriate one to use. SBNMS

has been part of an ongoing passive acoustic study since 2006 and provides an opportunity for

understanding the relationship between up-calls and gun-shots for this region. From 18

December 2007 to 28May 2008 an array of 10marine acoustic recording units was deployed

in SBNMS. An automated detector was used to determine seasonal occurrence of calls. Gun-

shots and upcalls occurred throughout the winter period, with gun-shots peaking in January,

and up-calls peaking inDecember. Comparative analyses of up-calls and gun-shots show that

they occur simultaneously around 17:00. Gun-shots primarily tended to occur between 16:00

and 4:00, while up-calls primarily occurred between 11:00 and 18:00. Average gun-shot

activity was 2.15 calls/min,while average up-call activitywas 3.45 calls/min. Future research

should be directed towards assessing occurrence of different call types between seasons, and

in the south-east US breeding grounds and northern foraging areas.

The effect of noise on acoustic communication in Cyprinella venusta

D.E. Holt and C.E. Johnston

Department of Fisheries and Allied Aquaculture, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama,

USA

[email protected]

Noise pollution is a serious problem that has increased dramatically with recent human

development. Despite their seemingly quiet underwater habitat, freshwater fishes are not

sheltered from elevated noise levels. Bridge and boat traffic, along with natural noise

sources including other organisms, waterfalls, rainfall, and water turbulence all contribute

to a noisy aquatic environment. Higher noise levels can result in elevated hearing

thresholds of hearing specialists, and decrease the signal-to-noise ratio of acoustic signals.

Because many fishes use acoustic signals during critical life history stages (such as

reproduction and territory defence), it is important to determine whether elevated noise

levels affect behaviour during these stages. The objectives of this study were to determine

the effect of elevated noise levels on nest choice, acoustic communication, and

reproductive and aggressive behaviours in the Black Tail Shiner Cyprinella venusta.

To determine how noise affected nest choice, two underwater speakers were suspended

over artificial crevice nests on opposite sides of a 1022 l tank. Bank limited white noise

70 Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 8: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

was played from one speaker, while the other speaker remained silent. The amount of time

spent and number of aggressive and reproductive behaviours performed by males at the

quiet nest was then compared to the noisy nest. To determine how elevated noise affected

acoustic communication, trials during which the sounds and associated behaviours of

C. venusta were recorded were performed in two tanks: one with elevated noise, and one

with silence. Acoustical parameters that were compared included amplitude, frequency,

call duration, and call frequency. Behavioural parameters compared included distance

between sender and receiver, and the number, duration, and escalation of aggressive and

reproductive behaviours.

Sonic muscle activity during territorial defence and distress calls in the Lusitanian

Toadfish Halobatrachus didactylus

J. Jordaoa, M.C.P. Amorimb and P.J. Fonsecaa

aDepartamento de Biologia Animal e Centro de Biologia Ambiental, Faculdade de

Ciencias, Universidade de Lisboa, Bloco C2, Campo Grande, 1749-016, Lisboa, Portugal;bUnidade de Investigacao em Eco-Etologia, ISPA, Rua Jardim do Tabaco, 34, Lisboa,

Portugal

[email protected]

Male Lusitanian Toadfish Halobatrachus didactylus are territorial and have a wide

acoustic repertoire. The sound production apparatus consists of a pair of intrinsic large

sonic muscles on the two lobes of the swimbladder. Sonic muscle activity and the

variability of the vocalizations were matched using electromyograms (EMGs). EMG

electrodes were surgically implanted on the sonic muscles of several territorial males.

After recovery, the subjects were released into an experimental tank with an available nest.

Sonic muscle activity was registered and the vocalizations were simultaneously recorded

using a hydrophone. Social context was manipulated by releasing different kinds of

intruders (male(s) or female(s)). This action elicited the production of boatwhistles, while

grunts were generated during fish disturbance. Preliminary analysis shows that, unlike

previously reported, the bilateral sonic muscles contract simultaneously during the

generation of both signals. The muscle contraction period is halved during the production

of the grunt relative to the boatwhistle. The EMG period matches the sound pulse period

and determines the fundamental sound frequency.

Vocal interactions between Lusitanian Toadfish males

J. Jordaoa, J.M. Simoesb, V.C. Almadab, P.J. Fonsecaa and M.C.P. Amorimb

aDepartamento de Biologia Animal e Centro de Biologia Ambiental, Faculdade de

Ciencias, Universidade de Lisboa, bloco C2, Campo Grande, 1749-016, Lisboa, Portugal;bUIEE- Unidade de Investigacao em Eco-etologia, ISPA, R. Jardim do Tabaco, 34 Lisboa,

Portugal

[email protected]

Males from the family Batrachoididae are thought to facilitate vocal activity from their

neighbours with their advertisement mating calls. As in other batrachoidids, Lusitanian

Bioacoustics 71

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 9: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Toadfish males emit an advertisement call (the boatwhistle) to attract mates. Here, we

investigate vocal interactions between males in small aggregations. We recorded groups

of six males that spontaneously occupied artificial concrete nests in an intertidal area of

the Tagus River estuary (Portugal). The number of boatwhistles produced by each male

was tallied for each 5-min interval of several periods of one hour. Boatwhistle

occurrences were not adjustable to a Poisson distribution, indicating that toadfish did not

call at random. Correlation analysis (non-parametric Gamma correlation) showed that

some males positively influenced other males to call while others suppressed the calling

activity of neighbours. Consistently, the number of boatwhistles produced per male was

not correlated with the number of calling males, contradicting the vocal facilitation

hypothesis. The effect of a neighbour’s calling activity was further tested with playback

experiments. Sound playback consisted of boatwhistles (BW) emitted at a rate of 5 or

20 BW/min or of white noise (20 sounds per min). Males (n ¼ 8) showed a tendency

to mimic the neighbours’ calling rate, when subjects were calling at a high rate (.12

BW/min). However, subject males were indifferent to low and inhibited by high

playback rate if they were initially calling at a low rate (,12 BW/min). These

preliminary results suggest that Lusitanian Toadfish males respond differentially to other

males depending on their own motivation (i.e. calling rate), indicating that vocal

interactions among Lusitanian Toadfish breeding males are more complex than initially

thought.

Vocal development via voice breaking in the Red-crowned Crane Grus japonensis

A. Klenovaa, I. Volodina,b, E. Volodinab and K. Postelnykhc

aDepartment of Biology, Moscow State University, Vorobiovi Gori, 119899, Russia;bScientific Research Department, Moscow Zoo, Russia; 3 Oka Biosphere State Nature

Reserve, Russia

[email protected]

Voice breaking is a process associated with puberty of human males and also accompanies

adolescence in some birds. This phenomenon is well studied in humans, but is still poorly

studied in birds, including cranes. Unanswered are questions about when do cranes start

and complete the process, what changes occur in time and frequency of vocal parameters

throughout the process, and how they are related to the sex, age and overall body growth.

Here we traced the vocal development in 31 cranes from hatching to 1.5-years old related

to increase of body mass and in comparison with voices of 13 conspecific adults. During

voice breaking, calls of both sexes contained two independent fundamental frequencies:

the high one, a retained juvenile frequency, and the low one, a newly appeared adult

frequency. Before voice breaking, calls contained only the high frequency, while after

voice breaking only the low frequency was recorded. Values of both frequencies did not

overlap and did not change during all ontogenesis. Cranes start voice breaking at 7 months

and complete it at 11.5 months on average. The onset of voice breaking coincided with

achieving the adult weight, while factors influencing its completion were not evident. We

did not find any effect of sex and age on the onset and completion of voice breaking. We

discuss that in nature the completion of voice breaking could be related to cessation of

parental care after the break of a parent-chick bond closely before the beginning of a new

breeding season.

72 Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 10: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Bioacoustic monitoring of a woodpecker community

M. Kocha, K.-H. Tauchertb and K.-H. Frommoltb

aHumboldt-Universitat zu Berlin, Institute of Biology, Invalidenstr. 43, D-10115 Berlin,

Germany; bMuseum fur Naturkunde – Leibniz Institute for Research on Evolution and

Biodiversity at the Humboldt University Berlin, Invalidenstr. 43, D-10115, Berlin,

Germany

[email protected]

Monitoring animal populations is of great importance for nature conservation. For birds

several standardized methods exist to estimate population sizes by mapping singing males

within their territories during the breeding season. In most woodpecker species these

surveys are done by mapping drumming males that in some cases are hard to localize by an

observer, especially if the population density is high and several individuals of different

species are drumming simultaneously. The idea of the study was to design and test a

feasible microphone setup that adopts the principle of established methods and enables

surveys to be conducted by locating drumming signals via acoustic recordings. Four-

channel-recordings with cardioid microphones in a cross configuration were conducted

within a woodpecker community in a natural preserve north of Berlin. Territories were

estimated by mapping the localization results of drumming signals obtained by

triangulation using cross correlation techniques and additional distance information from

sound pressure measurements. The findings showed good correlations with the field

mapping of drumming individuals by an ornithologist concerning species composition as

well as the individuals’ territories.

Effects of cover on loud trill-call and soft seet-call use in the Crested Tit Lophophanescristatus

T. Krama, I. Krams, A. Berzins and K. Igaune

Institute of Systematic Biology, Daugavpils University, Daugavpils LV-5401, Latvia

[email protected]

Predation is one of the main factors responsible for winter mortality in small birds.

Contact vocalizations of Crested Tits Lophophanes cristatus can be divided into two

categories: long- and short-range communication calls. The long-range calls are loud,

frequency-modulated trills, which are well suited for acoustic communication over

long distances. The short-range calls, in contrast, are soft, high-pitched tones, which

are strongly attenuated as they radiate through the environment. As the predation cost

in this species is mostly associated with the use of loud calls, we investigated whether

Crested Tits adjust the use of loud trill-calls and of soft seet-calls to changes in habitat

safety. We arranged two feeding sites that differed with respect to predicted safety, and

observed the utterance of loud trill-calls and soft seet-calls. Calling rates of the loud

trill-calls were highest when male Crested Tits foraged at the safe site. The loud trill-

calls were given significantly less often while visiting risky feeders placed just a few

metres away from the safe sites. The soft seet-calls were uttered both at risky and safe

feeders at similar rates. This study suggests that the long-range part of contact

communication in Crested Tits is strongly affected by the level of perceived predation

Bioacoustics 73

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 11: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

risk. In accordance with the data on hearing ability of predators, dominant male

Crested Tits decrease their exposure to predation in risky habitats by choosing a less

risky type of communication with high-pitched sounds.

Length and frequency in male Chiffchaff Phylloscopus collybita song: not all dare to

sing long songs

P. Linhart, P. Jaska and R. Fuchs

Department of Zoology, Faculty of Science, University of South Bohemia, Branisovska 31,

370 05 Ceske Budejovice, Czech Republic

[email protected]

In songbirds, both males and females can assess qualities of a singing male from features

of its song. However, different features may trigger different responses in males

assessing their rivals during male-male competition and in females searching for a mate.

In some cases, the same feature could be under the conflicting pressure of intra- and

intersexual selection. Chiffchaff males have simple songs consisting of several repeated

syllables. The song length in Chiffchaffs does not reflect strength in aggressive

interactions between males. But still, it could be important for females during mate

choice (e.g. as an indicator of a good territory). We found the song length to be

negatively correlated with the frequency of songs, a trait often used by males to assess

their rival’s fighting ability in many species. All males could potentially sing long songs

to attract females unless there was a mechanism preventing them to do so. We tested a

prediction that low quality males (with high frequency of songs) singing long songs

(probably attractive to females) are targeted with increased aggressive behaviour of the

other males. We studied responses of 48 Chiffchaff males to four types of stimuli: long

song/high frequency, long song/low frequency, short song/high frequency, short

song/low frequency. In general, longer songs elicited stronger responses. Moreover, the

response to long phrase/high frequency songs was the strongest according to our initial

prediction, suggesting that weak males could be ‘punished’ for displaying the trait they

cannot afford.

Acoustics of fish shelters

M. Lugli

Department of Evolutionary and Functional Biology, University of Parma, Italy

[email protected]

Many teleosts emit sounds from crevices, holes or cavities beneath stones and other

types of submerged objects. Yet, the acoustical properties of fish shelters are

virtually unexplored. This study investigates the resonance properties of shelters

commonly used by Mediterranean gobies as hiding places and/or nest sites in the

field, i.e. flat stones, shells belonging to five bivalve species (Tapes philippinarum,

Crassostrea gigas, Mytilus galloprovincialis, Scapharca inaequivalvis, Cerastoderma

edule) or within aquarium tanks (PVC tubes, concrete blocks, clay flower-pots cut

into halves). Both natural and artificial shelters were stimulated using a small

74 Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 12: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

underwater buzzer as sound source, placed inside or around the shelter to mimic a

fish calling from the nest site (source levels comparable to the those of the goby

sounds emitted under similar circumstances), and different types of driving stimuli

(white noise, pure tones and artificial pulse-trains). Results showed the presence of

remarkable amplitude gains (3–15 dB) at lower frequencies (100–150 Hz) in all

types of natural shelters but one (Mytilus), and in terracotta flowerpots and concrete

blocks. The gain was higher for stones and artificial shelters than for shells. The

experimental piling of sand on the shelter (mimicking a nest-related activity common

in sand gobies) increased gain in all types of shelter. Conclusions were verified by

performing analogous acoustical tests on natural shelters in the field. The

enhancement of low frequencies by stones and shells may explain the widespread

use of low-frequencies for acoustic communication by many teleosts calling from

enclosures. Our results draw attention to the use of suitable shelters for the recording

of sounds by fishes kept within laboratory aquaria.

Sound production, spawning behaviour and parasitic nest intrusions in the

Canestrini’s Goby Pomatoschistus canestrinii

S. Malavasi, C. Valerio and P. Torricelli

Department of Environmental Sciences, University “Ca’ Foscari” of Venice, Campo della

Celestia, Castello 2737/b, 30122 Venice, Italy

The Canestrini’s Goby Pomatoschistus canestrinii (Ninni) is an endemic sand goby

occurring in lagoons and estuaries of the northern Adriatic Sea. As in other sand

gobies, reproduction involves nest acquisition and defence, mate attraction, and

repulsion of other male intruders that may try to penetrate the nest and perform

sneaky fertilisations of the eggs. Previous work has documented sound production in

this species, occurring both in reproductive and aggressive behavioural contexts. In

this paper, sound production and associated behaviours were assessed and compared

in pairs spawning in two different experimental situations: (1) isolated resident

males, and (2) resident males paired with a smaller male, and therefore exposed to

possible nest intrusions. The simultaneous video- and acoustical recording of the

spawning behaviour allowed us to quantify the intensity of sound production and

associated behaviours, which were then compared between the groups of males.

Results indicated that the spawning behaviour of the nest-holding male was affected

by the presence of a male intruder, as the spawning sequence was repeatedly

interrupted by the intrusion attempts. This was reflected in an intense aggression and

associated sound production of the nestholding male towards the intruder male, but

also in a significant increase of aggressive sounds and aggressive visual displays

towards the female. Furthermore, the frequency of a female visual display increased

significantly when the male was exposed to nest intruders. By contrast, the overall

frequency of pre-spawning sounds did not change between the two situations,

although the mean duration of the burst containing the highest number of sounds was

higher in the group of males exposed to male intruders that in the isolated spawning

males. On the whole, these results indicate that parasitic nest intrusions significantly

affect the spawning patterns of the nest-holding males, even in terms of sound rate.

Bioacoustics 75

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 13: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Cricket calling site selection and sound transmission in a tropical evergreen forest

J. Manjari and R. Balakrishnan

Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Bangalore, India

[email protected]

Acoustically communicating animal species often show preferential use of specific calling

sites or microhabitats from which to broadcast their calls. Site selection may be driven by

different factors, including predator avoidance, reducing acoustic interference or

minimising acoustic attenuation and degradation. We studied microhabitat selection in

13 species of crickets that form the major part of an acoustically communicating ensiferan

assemblage in a tropical evergreen forest in Southern India. We examined microhabitat

selection in these species by comparing proportional use with availability of microhabitats.

We found strong evidence for microhabitat selection by the different species. We then

investigated whether the selection for microhabitats confers sound transmission benefits to

these species by playing back their calls and re-recording them at different distances in both

native and non native microhabitats. The calls were examined for their attenuation and

degradation and compared between species and microhabitats.

Nestling call variation during early development in altricial colonial passerine

P.A.M. Marquesa,b and C.B. Araujoc

aCentro de Biologia Ambiental, Museu Nacional de Historia Natural, Universidade de

Lisboa, Portugal; bDepartamento de Biodiversidad y Biologıa Evolutiva, MNCN (CSIC),

Spain; cPPG Ecologia, IB, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Brasil

[email protected]

In the great majority of birds the nestling calls play a major role in parent-offspring

interactions, especially as a recognition signal or a signal of need. However, early in their

life cycle nestling birds undergo massive changes that are expected to influence the

development of their vocalizations. In this context we investigate how the nestling call

changes over time between hatching and fledgling and which call features are associated

with the change over time. In a second step we address how the changes affect the

discrimination between individuals at different ages. In this study we followed nine

Spanish Sparrow Passer hispaniolensis nestlings during their development, individually

recording their calls at 2 (n ¼ 8), 4 (n ¼ 9), 6 (n ¼ 8), 8 (n ¼ 5) and.8 (n ¼ 5) days old.

Our results show that we can discriminate calls produced by nestlings at different ages. A

post-hoc classification correctly classified the majority of the calls (76%). However, we

were not able to identify a single call parameter responsible for the discrimination. The

most discriminant parameter was the maximum frequency (seven out of nine nestlings),

call duration and maximum frequency time. Against what we expected, overall

discrimination between individuals was highly significant in all age classes. The post-hoc

classification correctly classified the majority of the calls (73%). The most important

parameters in discrimination were call duration, harmonic duration, maximum and

minimum frequencies. These results confirm the influence of development in the overall

call features. Surprisingly, it is possible to statistically discriminate between individuals

from a very early phase in their development.

76 Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 14: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Boing! Acoustic localization, characterization and comparison of Minke Whale songs

from the Hawaiian islands and other areas in the North Pacific Ocean

T. Norrisa, T. Yacka, J. Oswalda, S. Martinb, L. Thomasc and V. Janikc

aBio-Waves Inc., San Diego, California, USA; bSPAWAR Systems Center Pacific San

Diego, California, USA; cCentre for Research into Ecological and Environmental

Modelling and The Sea Mammal Research Unit, University of St Andrews, St Andrews,

Fife, KY16 9LZ, UK

[email protected]

The Minke Whale Balaenoptera acutorostrata is a small, elusive baleen whale that is

rarely sighted in tropical waters of the North Pacific Ocean. During winter and spring, they

produce songs, also known as ‘boings’, which are commonly detected at deep water

hydrophones located around the Hawaiian Islands. We acoustically monitored Minke

Whales using a fixed seafloor hydrophone array encompassing a large (.2000 km2), deep-

water area off the island of Kauai. Simultaneous visual-acoustic surveys of the same

region were conducted from a quiet motor-sailing vessel. The combination of the towed

and fixed hydrophone arrays allowed animals to be localized and tracked in near real-time.

Using both methods, we were able to visually confirm the location of a Minke Whale

initially detected and localized using the fixed hydrophone array, and later with the towed

hydrophone array. These data are being collected to help validate statistical methods that

are being developed to estimate densities of marine mammals using acoustic signals they

produce. In a related study, boings recorded in the Hawaiian Islands (central North Pacific)

were acoustically characterized and compared with boings recorded in the western and

eastern North Pacific. These results are discussed in relation to the behaviour and

population biology of this species. We provide recommendations for tracking, monitoring

behaviours and estimating the distribution and distribution of these vocally active but

visually elusive whales.

Close-range sound communication in Platypus quercivorus (Coleoptera: Platypodidae),a pest of oak trees in Japan

E. Ohya

Entomology Lab, Forestry and Forest Products Research Institute, Matsunosato 1,

Tsukuba 305-8687, Japan

[email protected]

The oak platypodid beetle Platypus quercivorus produces sounds by stridulating the

abdomen against the elytra. When a female was put onto the bark surface of a male-

infested log, she began to walk and produced an ‘approaching chirp’, searching for a

gallery entrance bored by a male. Upon finding one, she entered it, and the male pushed

her back. While they pushed against each other, the female made a ‘pre-mating buzz’

that lasted about 5–10 s. During this buzzing, the male backed out of the gallery in

order to allow her in. Females that had been silenced via surgery did not evoke this

male reaction; thus, males apparently identified females by their buzzing sound. The

male then followed the female into the gallery, and produced an ‘in-gallery chirp’ with

his posterior abdomen visible. After a while, both sexes backed out of the hole and

Bioacoustics 77

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 15: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

copulated at the entrance. Both sexes produced ‘stress chirps’ when confined inside a

cotton ball, and ‘spontaneous chirps’ when walking alone on the surface of an oak bark

piece. There are two local types in Japan and the inter-pulse-intervals in their buzzes

were different.

Effects of temperature on sound production and auditory sensitivity in the Raphael

Catfish Platydoras costatus

S. Papes and F. Ladich

University of Vienna, Department of Behavioural Biology, Althanstrasse 14, 1090 Vienna,

Austria

[email protected]

Hearing sensitivity and sound characteristics of poikilothermic animals are influenced

by ambient temperature. The present study investigates the influence of temperature on

the hearing ability and on sound characteristics in the neotropical Raphael Catfish

Platydoras costatus. Using the auditory evoked potential (AEP) recording technique,

the hearing thresholds of eight specimens were tested at six different frequencies from

100 to 4000 Hz. The temporal resolution was determined by analysing the minimum

resolvable click period (0.3–5ms). Hearing sensitivity and sound production were

measured after acclimation to 228C followed by acclimation to 308C for at least 3 weeks

each. Auditory sensitivity increased from 1 to 4 kHz with rising temperature, whereas

the temporal resolution showed no change. Catfishes produce their stridulation sound

by rubbing the pectoral spines in the shoulder girdle during abduction and adduction of

pectoral fins. Distress calls were recorded when fish were handheld. The stridulation

sounds produced during pectoral fin spine abduction and adduction became shorter at

the higher temperature. Maximum and minimum pulse period as well as pulse

number did not change with temperature, whereas the dominant frequency of sounds

was higher at the higher temperature. Our data indicate that constraints imposed on

hearing sensitivity at different temperatures cannot be compensated even by longer

acclimation. As sound characteristics also change with temperature, we suggest that the

ambient temperature directly affects acoustic communication in the neotropical catfish

P. costatus.

Wolf counting and individual acoustic discrimination by spectrographic analysis

D. Passilongoa, F. Dessi-Fulgheria, A. Gazzolab, M. Zaccaronia and M. Apolloniob

aDepartment of Evolutionary Biology University of Firenze via Romana 17 I - 50125,

Firenze, Italy; bDepartment of Zoology and Evolutionary Genetics 4 University of Sassari

5 via Muroni 25, I-07100 Sassari, Italy

[email protected]

Wolves are a threatened species in many countries around the world and one of the

main monitoring tools uses wolf howls. This study aimed to improve detection of

wolves by the howling technique using spectrographic analysis to count wolves and to

assign howls to the correct pack or individual. Chorus and solo howls from six

78 Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 16: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

different free ranging packs were elicited by playback from 2003 to 2007 on the

Tuscany Apennines (Italy); in all, 127 howls were analysed. A minimum number of

wolves were estimated considering contemporaneous howls in the chorus. We applied

the analysis at different points of the sound recordings to limit bias due to possible

biphonation and other non linear phenomena. Discrimination was performed by discriminant

function analysis (DFA).Wedetected27animals (mean4.5 individuals/pack).Cross-validated

discriminant analysis assigned 75.7% of the howls to the correct individual and 79.7% of the

howls to the correct pack. As regards DFA analysis, final and maximum fundamental

frequencies are the most important howl features for discriminating among individuals and

among packs. Individual discrimination could be a useful tool to validate the number of

animals detected by spectrographic visualization. Future investigations will be aimed towards

identification, thus associating howls with individuals over periods of time, a promising tool

for wolf conservation.

Isolation-induced call modification in Zebra Finches: an effect of stress of social

separation?

E.C. Pereza, J.E. Eliea, C. Soulageb, H. Soulab, N. Mathevona,c and C. Vignala

aUniversite de Lyon, Universite de Saint-Etienne, ENES EA 3988, France; bUniversite de

Lyon, RMND INSERM U870, INSA-Lyon, France; cUniversite Paris Sud, NAMC CNRS

UMR 8620, France

[email protected]

Perturbation of social interactions is one of the most effective stressors in animals.

Separation of affiliated individuals provokes behavioural perturbations and stress

hormone levels rise. Whereas this aspect of social affiliation has been extensively

described between monogamous partners or between mother and young, few studies

have dealt with potential social bonds between group mates. In the gregarious Zebra

Finch Taeniopygia guttata, we recently demonstrated that social isolation of males

modifies not only call rate in response to female calls, but also acoustic structure of

evoked calls. Here we hypothesize that these modifications in call response are stress-

induced and reveal the perturbation by isolation of existing social bonds with group-

mates. If so, the experimental increase of plasma corticosterone level of males in a

normal social context should mimic the effect of social isolation. After oral

administration of corticosterone, we measured males’ response to female calls of

varied salience (familiar and unfamiliar females) in the presence of a familiar audience.

We report that an increase in circulating stress hormone modifies male Zebra Finches

call response to female calls. We propose that modifications of call structure could carry

information about the emitter’s emotional state.

A combined neural network-statistics method for automated Sperm Whale clicks

detection

A. Sanchez-Garcıaa, A. Bueno-Crespob and J.L. Sancho-Gomezc

aS.A. de Electronica Submarina (SAES), Area Tecnica de Estudios Avanzados y

Tratamiento Digital de Senales, Carretera de la Algameca s/n, 30205-Cartagena,

Bioacoustics 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 17: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Spain; bUniversidad Catolica San Antonio. Departamento Informatica de Sistemas,

Campus de los Jeronimos s/n, Guadalupe 30107-Murcia, Spain; cUniversidad

Politecnica de Cartagena. Departamento de Tecnologıas de la Informacion y las

Comunicaciones, Campus Muralla del Mar. Antiguo Cuartel de Antigones s/n, 30202-

Cartagena, Spain

[email protected]

A new method for the automated detection of Sperm Whale clicks that combines neural

networks and statistical computations is presented. This method is intended to detect

regular clicks and creaks and can be broken down into two main processing stages. The

first stage works with the spectrogram output by computing the accumulated energy along

each time frame, extracting consecutive 2-seconds duration time windows, obtaining

statistical parameters characterizing these time windows and classifying them using a feed

forward neural network as either containing regular clicks, creaks or noise. In the final

stage, a dynamic energy-based criterion is applied to each classified time window based on

previously computed statistical parameters. The performance of the method has been

tested with three long recordings containing regular clicks and creaks and shows

significantly high percentages of correct detections (global score of 94.8%) with a reduced

computation time.

A big bird producing a high-pitched call: a vocal analysis of the Hazel Grouse

F. Sebea,b,c, P. Leonardb, T, Aubina, R. Isoardc and M. Montadertb

aNAMC CNRS UMR 8620, Universite Paris-Sud, 91405 Orsay Cedex, France; bONCFS

Centre national d’etudes et de recherche appliquees faune de montagne Micropolis, La

Berardie, 05000 Gap, France; cMaire d’Auzet 04140 France

[email protected]

The Hazel Grouse Bonasa bonasia, a sedentary bird species belonging to the grouse

family, breeds across northern Eurasia, central and eastern Europe in dense, mixed

coniferous woodland. While the territorial call is a good indicator of the grouse’s

presence, the role of acoustic cues is not clear. This study aimed to assess acoustic

characteristics of the Hazel Grouse call and differences between sexes. Recording and

playback experiments were performed in Alpes-de-Haute-Provence (France). The

territorial calls of males (n ¼ 8) and females (n ¼ 5) were recorded in the field. Analyses

of signals show that the male has a high-pitched ti-ti-ti-ti-ti call, and the female a liquid

tettettettettet call. The frequency of the peak of maximum amplitude of calls was very

high for such a large bird: 6841 Hz ^ 786 for the male and 6034Hz ^ 476 for the

female. After careful analyses, it turns out that the whole spectrum of the call was within

the audible domain. Moreover, within-individual variability, that allows encoding of the

individual identity, is higher in males (Cvb/Cvi ¼ 2.28) than in females

(Cvb/Cvi , 0.01). The characteristic of producing such high frequency calls may

reflect the necessity to improve localization efficiency in dense woodland, as high

frequencies are associated with better directionality. Moreover our results suggest that

acoustic characteristics of male calls have sufficient inter-individual variability to allow

the coding of individual identity.

80 Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 18: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Patterns of shelter usage and social aggregation by the vocal Lusitanian Toadfish

J.M. Simoesa, P.J. Fonsecab, V.C. Almadaa and M.C.P. Amorima

aUIEE - Unidade de Investigacao em Eco-etologia, ISPA, Rua Jardim do Tabaco,

34 Lisboa, Portugal; bDepartamento de Biologia Animal e Centro de Biologia Ambiental,

Faculdade de Ciencias, Universidade de Lisboa, bloco C2, Campo Grande, 1749-016,

Lisboa, Portugal

[email protected]

Marine fishes where males defend nests and provide parental care often migrate from

deeper to littoral waters to breed. In this study we describe seasonal patterns of shelter

occupation in estuarine shallow waters and assess social aggregation in the Lusitanian

Toadfish Halobatrachus didactylus (Batrachoididae), a vocal species with male parental

care. Occupation of intertidal shelters was restricted to the breeding season but adult fish

remained in the subtidal during the non-reproductive season, since they produced sounds

throughout the year. Intertidal shelters were aggregated with up to five shelters found per

2m segment of an 80m transect. This aggregation probably resulted from social attraction

since many segments were found unoccupied. Moreover, shelters contained on average

two fish (maximum of nine). Based on size, shelters with multiple fish seemed to contain

females, or possibly sneakers, but also more than one type I male, showing high social

tolerance for conspecifics.

Acoustic communication in a small benthic fish, Etheostoma crossopterum

P. Speares and C.E. Johnston

Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama, USA

[email protected]

Darters in the sub-genus Catonotus are small benthic fish that adopt a reproductive

strategy in which a male establishes a cavity and recruits multiple females to lay eggs

within his shelter. These nest guarding males have been shown to be vocal and acoustic

communication is thought to be critical in allowing these males to recruit females

(courtship vocalizations) and also ward off other males (aggressive vocalizations). One

such species, Ethesotoma crossopterum, is known to produce three types of

vocalizations including knocks and purrs which consist of pulses as well as a harmonic

component previously termed a ‘drum’. Although these vocalizations have been

previously studied, little is known about individual differences between males and how

these differences are related to the size of the male, or the physiological effect of these

vocalizations on other conspecifics. We examined the behaviours of the fish associated

with each of these vocalizations and examined the inter-individual differences in call

components. Recordings were conducted in the laboratory, using nest guarding males

which were exposed to both gravid females, and using intruder males. This poster will

examine the specific use of each of these vocalizations as well as associated behaviours,

in both aggressive and courtship contexts. Future planned research includes an

investigation of possible hormone modulation in response to playbacks of conspecific

vocalizations.

Bioacoustics 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 19: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Bioacoustic analysis for call characterization of two populations of green frog in Central

Italy

M.P. Spigonardi, G. La Porta and M. Mearelli

Department of Cellular and Environmental Biology, University of Perugia, Italy

[email protected]

Recent and important studies have pointed out that environmental stress presents a high risk

for amphibian populations in Europe and, in particular, for the Italian complex of water

frogs Rana (Pelophylax) lessonae. The analysis of vocalization in a single species can be a

useful cognitive tool to better understand this behaviour and an interesting starting point for

conservation strategies. In the last twenty years, an increasing amount of attention has been

dedicated to the study of vocalizations of Europeanwater frog populations (Rana ridibunda,

R. lessonae and their viable, fertile hybrid R. esculenta), in order to provide an additional

biogeographic survey tool, and to underline the similarities and differences between several

populations. Only one study has attempted to discuss the taxonomic question of the species

living in Central Italy using a bioacoustic approach, in synergy with other techniques.

In our work, we want to provide a methodological contribution for the bioacoustic

characterization of the green frog populations through a sampling protocol and a connected

technical analysis. Two green frog populations were investigated and more than 3200 calls

in the field recorded during three breeding seasons. The calls were performed at two sites

with different environmental features and with a comparable number of male specimens.

The results of this analysis highlighted the inter- and intra-population differences.

Furthermore, we provide a description of the individual sequences of vocalizations with

spectral and temporal features of mating and territorial calls.

A comparative study of bird song complexity measures

J.T. Tanttua, J. Turunena and P. Sirkiab

aTampere University of Technology, Pori P.O. Box 300, FIN-28101 Pori, Finland;bUniversity of Turku, Section of Ecology, Department of Biology, FIN-20014 Turku,

Finland

[email protected]

It is known that the song repertoire and versatility of the male Pied Flycatcher Ficedula

hypoleuca correlate significantly with the amount of previous experience (older males

have larger repertoire size), with the brightness of colour (differences between brown

and black males), body mass and physical condition and the spring arrival order. Thus

the species is an interesting test case for song complexity measures. Espmark and

Lampe (Bioacoustics, 1993, Vol. 5, pp. 33–65) analysed the song complexity of male

Pied Flycatcher with several different measures including number of elements and

number of element types in the phrase and repertoire size. The song element is defined

as the smallest (visually) separable entity in the spectrogram. Elements in turn are

building blocks of syllables. A string of syllables that occur together in a particular

pattern is called a phrase. In our study only phrase level complexity is considered. Da

Silva and Vielliard (Advances in Bioacoustics 2, pp. 185–204) claim that repertoire

82 Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 20: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

size is inadequate for measuring the degree of monotonity or diversity of the song and

propose the use of entropy or self-information as a complexity measure. We apply both

of these approaches in this paper and compare how well the measures predict the age

and physical condition of the singing male. The test data consists of male Pied

Flycatcher songs recorded in Ruissalo Island, Turku, Finland, by Paivi Sirkia with her

students in 2006 – 2008.

Monitoring of booming Bitterns Botaurus stellaris by acoustic triangulation

K.-H. Tauchert and K.-H. Frommolt

Museum fur Naturkunde – Leibniz Institute for Research on Evolution and Biodiversity at

the Humboldt University Berlin, Invalidenstr. 43, D-10115 Berlin, Germany

[email protected]

The Eurasian Bittern Botaurus stellaris is a characteristic bird of extensive reed beds.

The presence of bitterns can most easily be detected by their characteristic booming

vocalizations. A booming sequence consists of a series of tonal calls with a fundamental

frequency of approximately 150Hz. The calls can be heard over distances ofmore than 1 km,

but despite their low frequency, it is difficult for an observer to estimate the exact position of

the caller. In addition, in areas with high population density it is difficult to discriminate

individual birds by the acoustic characteristics of their calls. We assessed the number of

booming bitterns in a large wetland area in the north-eastern part of Germany. A set of

four-channel-recorderswas used to record the vocalizations of the birds. The synchronization

of the recording unitswas realizedby recording a radio signal at each recorder at the beginning

of the recording session. The position of callers was estimated by the directional information

of each four-channel-recorder and the differences in the arrival time of the acoustic signal at

the different recording devices. Time differences were calculated using cross correlation

techniques. The method allowed us to distinguish between at least 14 booming bitterns in a

reedbed areaofonly two squarekilometres.Themain advantagesof acoustic triangulation are

that the method can be applied to large areas and the data acquisition can bemade by a single

observer. The study was supported by the Deutsche Bundesstiftung Umwelt.

The sounds of life on Earth

C. Tipp

Wildlife Section, British Library Sound Archive, 96 Euston Rd, London, NW1 2DB, UK

[email protected]

The British Library Sound Archive has one of the world’s largest and most comprehensive

collections of bioacoustic sound recordings. Founded in 1969, the collection now contains

more than 160,000 recordings ofwildlife and natural soundscape recordings,many ofwhich

have never been published. The wildlife collection was originally formed to serve as an

international resource for bioacoustics research, providing copies of recordings to

researchers and archiving field recordings for future generations to benefit from. Today

recordings are also provided for educational projects, exhibitions, private listening, artistic

sound installations, musical compositions, broadcasting and commercial publications.

Bioacoustics 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 21: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

SoundServer is an automated service that offers immediate onsite access to a growing

number of recordings. Visitors to the British Library’s Reading Rooms can either browse

the Sound Archive collections via a Jukebox service or pre-order a personal playlist

according to their specific needs. Remote access is available through the Archival Sound

Recordings (ASR) website. ASR is an online resource that provides access to thousands of

hours of recordings from across the Sound Archive. Three wildlife content packages are

currently available through this resource – British Wildlife Recordings, Natural

Soundscapes and Amphibians.

Ecoacoustical constraints shape sound communication in Lake Malawi Cichlids

M. Van Staadena, L.E. Wysockib and F. Ladichb

aDept of Biological Sciences, Bowling Green State University, OH 43402, USA; bDept of

Behavioural Biology, University of Vienna, Althanstrasse 14, 1090 Vienna, Austria

[email protected]

As with other African great lakes, Lake Malawi exhibits an explosive radiation of several

hundred cichlid species, most of which are endemics. Although visual communication

is widely considered the predominant force driving species recognition and mate choice

in this radiation, recent studies highlighted a potential role for acoustic signals.

To corroborate this proposition, we are analysing internal and external constraints on

acoustic communication in several species from different tribes. Here we focus on the

correlation between characteristics of species-specific vocalizations, auditory sensitivity,

and eco-acoustical constraints shaping this communication system. Ambient noise in Lake

Malawi is characterized by a steep increase to 800Hz and a slow decline up to several

kHz. This increase is particularly pronounced during windy conditions creating a low

frequency sound window. Interestingly, hearing sensitivity in Melanochromis johanni and

Metriaclima callainos, revealed by auditory-evoked potential measurements, is

maximally sensitive at 150–200Hz and lowest at 800–1000Hz. Moreover, this is true

whether sensitivity is measured in terms of acoustic pressure or particle acceleration. All

genera investigated to date produce pulsed sounds during agonistic and reproductive

interactions with energies concentrated between 200–300Hz. Thus, the main frequencies

for sound communication are concentrated both at the maximum auditory sensitivity, and

within the ambient noise window of the lake. These results indicate that sound

communication in Lake Malawi cichlids is facilitated by both internal sensory and eco-

acoustical conditions in the lake, and could play a major role in speciation.

Sexing of four monomorphic Dendrocygna whistling duck species by their loud

whistles

I. Volodina,b, V. Matrosovaa, M. Kaiserc, E. Volodinab and A. Klenovaa

aDepartment of Vertebrate Zoology, Faculty of Biology, Lomonosov Moscow State

University, Russia; bScientific Research Department, Moscow Zoo, Russia; cTierpark

Berlin-Friedrichsfelde GmbH, Berlin, Germany

[email protected]

84 Abstracts of Poster Presentations

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013

Page 22: Abstracts of Poster Presentations

We present an acoustic approach for the reliable sexing in four whistling ducks from

the genus Dendrocygna and compare it with molecular and cloacal inspection techniques.

In the four examined species, the White-faced Whistling Duck D. viduata (WF), Fulvous

Whistling duck D. bicolor (FU), Cuban Whistling Duck D. arborea (CU) and Red-billed

Whistling Duck D. autumnalis (RB), visual sexing is impossible, except when observing

copulation. However all the four species show strong sexual differences in the structure of

their species-specific loud whistles. In the WF and FU, the maximum fundamental

frequency of the loud whistles was always much lower in males than in females. In

contrast, in the CU, the maximum fundamental frequency of males was always higher than

in females. In the RB, the mean duration of notes of the end trill of a loud whistle was

always longer in males than in females. In all the four species, the values of the measured

acoustic parameters did not overlap between sexes. For the 59 examined birds, an

acoustic-based sexing showed 100% accordance to the DNA PCR analysis, while a cloacal

inspection showed only 89.8% accuracy (in six cases, males were mistakenly identified as

females). The results demonstrate that acoustic sexing represents a feasible alternative to

the two traditional methods as a non-invasive tool for the distant sexing of the four

whistling duck species both in captivity and in the wild.

The energetics of singing in noise – metabolic and respiratory costs of increasing

song amplitude

S.A. Zollingera, F. Gollerb and H. Brummc

aUniversity of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, UK; bUniversity of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT,

USA; cMax Planck Institute for Ornithology, Seewiesen, Germany

[email protected]

In an increasingly noisy world, animals that rely on acoustic communication are faced

with additional challenges in trying to make their signals heard. One response to a rise

in background noise is to make your signal louder. However, increasing vocal amplitude

to higher and higher levels may incur costs and is likely subject to physiological or

anatomic constraints. Previous studies in three different songbird species suggest that

the metabolic cost of song is fairly small relative to resting metabolic rate (requiring a

1.7–3.4-fold increase). Another study found that when subsyringeal air sac pressure was

experimentally reduced, song amplitude decreased. This suggests that singing louder

may require greater subsyringeal pressure, and potentially greater respiratory muscle

activity and/or greater volume of air than that needed to produce a quieter signal of equal

duration. Here we examine the potential costs of increasing song amplitude in Zebra

Finches Taeniopygia guttata singing in environments with different background noise

levels. For each 4 dB increase in background noise amplitude, birds significantly

increased their song amplitude. To test whether these amplitude increases required

associated increases in metabolic energy or subglottal air sac pressure, we measured

oxygen consumption, subsyringeal air sac pressure and song bout duration. We recorded

oxygen consumption by training birds to sing while wearing small, lightweight

respirometry helmets. Preliminary results suggest that oxygen consumption per motif

may increase with increasing amplitude, but within-bird variability for metabolic rate,

respiratory patterns and song duration in different noise conditions was high.

Bioacoustics 85

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Wes

tern

Ken

tuck

y U

nive

rsity

] at

19:

39 3

0 A

pril

2013